29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Papers 29–34 Friday Morning<br />

duction”? NATE KORNELL & ROBERT A. BJORK, UCLA (read by<br />

Robert A. Bjork)—Learning about a new concept or category by observing<br />

examples—that is, inductive learning—happens constantly,<br />

from a baby learning a new word to a doctor classifying x-rays. What<br />

influence, though, does the spacing of such exemplars have on the efficiency<br />

of induction? We designed two experiments to test Ernst Z.<br />

Rothkopf’s assertion that “spacing is the friend of recall, but the<br />

enemy of induction.” In both experiments participants studied paintings<br />

by 12 different artists. A given artist’s paintings were presented<br />

either consecutively (massed) or interleaved with other artists’ paintings<br />

(spaced). Memory was tested using a cued-recall task in Experiment<br />

1 and a recognition task in Experiment 2. To our surprise, inducing<br />

artists’ styles profited from spacing, not massing. Massed<br />

practice, however, apparently created a sense of fluent induction: Participants<br />

rated massing as more effective than spacing for learning an<br />

artist’s style even after their own test performance had demonstrated<br />

the opposite.<br />

8:40–8:55 (29)<br />

Learning Algebra by Exploration. ANGELA BRUNSTEIN &<br />

JOHN R. ANDERSON, Carnegie Mellon University—A major component<br />

of learning a skill like algebra is knowing which operators are<br />

appropriate for what situations. This study investigated learning to<br />

solve algebra-isomorphic problems either by exploration or by a stepby-step<br />

instruction. Instructed participants performed better on early<br />

problems, whereas explorers performed better on solving related<br />

problems later on. Explorers were less likely to acquire shallow operators<br />

that only applied in the original learning situation. Exploring<br />

problems on their own also enhanced participants’ capabilities for debugging<br />

and acquiring new operators. More importantly, exploration<br />

also changed the way they perceived new situations: Although these<br />

situations were new to all participants, explorers were better able to<br />

focus on task relevant aspects. <strong>The</strong>refore, they were more likely to<br />

choose the correct operators in new situations.<br />

9:00–9:15 (30)<br />

What Does It Take to Link a Cue With a Behavior in Human Skill<br />

Acquisition? HAL PASHLER, University of California, San Diego,<br />

EDWARD VUL, MIT, & TIMOTHY C. RICKARD, University of<br />

California, San Diego—Much has been learned about when mental<br />

linkages are (and are not) formed in Pavlovian conditioning and in<br />

contingency judgments, but far less is known about what principles<br />

govern the acquisition of cue–behavior linkages in human skill acquisition.<br />

In several experiments, we had subjects make speeded responses<br />

based upon an instructed stimulus–response mapping. Additional<br />

redundant cues were also presented at certain times within each<br />

training trial, reliably predicting the appropriate response. In some<br />

cases these cues did not appear until it was too late to use them. <strong>The</strong><br />

results (assessed in various types of transfer-test blocks) shed light<br />

upon whether acquisition of cue–behavior linkages requires (1) actual<br />

experience using the cue to select a response during training, (2) perceiving<br />

the cue prior to selecting the response on the training trial, or<br />

(3) merely having the cue appear, correlated with the response, even<br />

if it was not useful or timely.<br />

9:20–9:35 (31)<br />

How Conceptualizing Music Affects Novice Trumpeters’ First Sound<br />

Attempts. MATTHEW STEINFELD, MICHAEL F. SCHOBER, &<br />

MICHELLE F. LEVINE, New School for Social Research (read by<br />

Michael F. Schober)—To what extent do novice musicians need to<br />

conceptualize music as they acquire musical skill? We tested novice<br />

trumpeters (individuals who could not read written music and who had<br />

never played a wind instrument before) in their first encounter with a<br />

trumpet, under three different conditions: (1) simply following fingered<br />

instructions; (2) hearing the to-be-achieved pitches before following<br />

the fingered instructions; and (3) hearing and singing the tobe-achieved<br />

pitches before following the fingered instructions. If<br />

conceptualizing the music and hearing it internally matter, as McPher-<br />

5<br />

son (2005) has proposed, then novice trumpeters who hear and sing<br />

the pitches should be able to play the desired pitches more often and<br />

more accurately than when they simply follow fingered instructions.<br />

<strong>The</strong> results reflect this pattern, and are consistent with a model of musical<br />

production that suggests working from internally represented<br />

sound to action is more effective than acting without musical conceptualization.<br />

SYMPOSIUM: Mechanisms of Cognitive Development:<br />

Domain-General Learning or Domain-Specific Constraints?<br />

Regency ABC, Friday Morning, 9:40–12:00<br />

Chaired by Vladimir M. Sloutsky, Ohio State University<br />

9:40–9:55 (32)<br />

Mechanisms of Cognitive Development: Domain-General Learning<br />

or Domain-Specific Constraints? VLADIMIR M. SLOUTSKY,<br />

Ohio State University—How do children come to acquire language,<br />

to individuate objects, to navigate in space, to use numbers, and to<br />

have concepts? For the past two decades, the prevailing answer has<br />

been that the environmental input is fundamentally indeterminate and<br />

therefore learning is organized by a set of innate domain-specific constraints.<br />

However, a growing body of evidence indicates that the input<br />

is more systematic than previously believed and that there are powerful<br />

domain-general learning mechanisms capable of exploiting these<br />

regularities and building sophisticated domain-specific knowledge.<br />

Although no single comprehensive learning account of cognitive development<br />

has been offered yet, there are accounts of how infants and<br />

children extract regularities from the input, to acquire language, to<br />

learn to navigate in space, and to form concepts. This symposium will<br />

focus on domain-general learning and its role in language learning<br />

(Christiansen, Smith), conceptual development (McClelland, Sloutsky),<br />

and spatial navigation (Newcombe). Successes and limitations<br />

of the domain-general learning account will be discussed (Goldstone).<br />

10:00–10:15 (33)<br />

Self-Organizing “Constraints” on Word Learning. LINDA B.<br />

SMITH, Indiana University, & HANAKO YOSHIDA, University of<br />

Houston—Growing evidence suggests that children learn how to learn<br />

language, creating language learning mechanisms that fit the learning<br />

task, as they proceed. One line of evidence supporting this view<br />

concerns cross-linguistic differences—not just in what children know<br />

about their language, but in the very processes and strategies they use<br />

to learn language. This talk will present evidence on the developmental<br />

emergence of cross-linguistic differences in language learning<br />

in 12- to 36-month-old children learning Japanese or English as their<br />

first and only language. <strong>The</strong> evidence shows emerging differences in<br />

strategies of noun learning, verb learning, and in such important distinctions<br />

as animate and inanimate.<br />

10:20–10:35 (34)<br />

Statistical Learning and Language: In Search of Underlying Neural<br />

Constraints. MORTEN H. CHRISTIANSEN, Cornell University,<br />

CHRISTOPHER M. CONWAY, Indiana University, & LUCA ONNIS,<br />

Cornell University—Over the past decade, statistical learning—the<br />

discovery of structure by way of statistical properties of the input—<br />

has emerged as an important paradigm for studying domain-general<br />

constraints on language acquisition. <strong>The</strong> basic assumption of this research<br />

is that statistical learning and language are subserved by the<br />

same mechanisms, an assumption for which there is little neural evidence.<br />

We therefore used event-related potentials (ERPs) to investigate<br />

the distribution of brain activity while adults performed (1) a natural<br />

language reading task and (2) a statistical learning task involving<br />

sequenced stimuli. <strong>The</strong> same positive ERP deflection, the P600 effect,<br />

typically linked to difficult or ungrammatical syntactic processing,<br />

was found for structural incongruencies in both natural language as<br />

well as statistical learning and had similar topographical distributions.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se results suggest that the same neural mechanisms are recruited

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!