29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Papers 317–322 Sunday Morning<br />

<strong>The</strong> Goodman–Kruskal gamma coefficient, G, has been proposed as<br />

a “nonparametric” measure of accuracy in perceptual and memory<br />

discrimination tasks as well as in metacognitive judgments. This measure<br />

is widely used, particularly in tasks that assess metamemory performance.<br />

We demonstrate that G is not free of distributional assumptions<br />

after all, and that the computed value of G systematically<br />

deviates from its actual value under realistic conditions. This finding<br />

implies that caution is needed when using G as a measure of accuracy<br />

and an alternative measure is recommended. Application of this alternative<br />

measure requires no change in how data are typically collected<br />

in metacognition experiments.<br />

11:40–11:55 (317)<br />

Type I Error Rates and Power Analyses for Single-Point Sensitivity<br />

Measures. CAREN M. ROTELLO, University of Massachusetts,<br />

Amherst, MICHAEL E. J. MASSON, University of Victoria, &<br />

MICHAEL F. VERDE, University of Plymouth—Experiments often<br />

produce a hit rate and a false-alarm rate in each of two conditions.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se response rates are summarized into a single-point sensitivity<br />

measure such as d′ and t tests are conducted to test for experimental<br />

effects. Using large-scale Monte Carlo simulations, we evaluate this<br />

research strategy with four commonly used single-point measures (d′,<br />

A′, percent correct, and gamma) and a newly proposed measure (gammaC).<br />

We ask two questions: (1) Assuming two conditions that differ<br />

only in response bias, what is the Type I error rate for each measure?<br />

(2) Assuming two conditions that differ in true sensitivity, what is the<br />

power of each measure to detect that effect? Our simulations indicate<br />

that power is similar for these measures, but that the Type I error rates<br />

are often unacceptably high. Type I errors are minimized when the selected<br />

sensitivity measure is theoretically appropriate for the data.<br />

Memory in the Real World<br />

Seaview, Sunday Morning, 10:00–12:00<br />

Chaired by Ruth S. Day, Duke University<br />

10:00–10:15 (318)<br />

Memory Failures in the Real World: What Were Those Side Effects?<br />

RUTH S. DAY, Duke University—Both doctors and patients must remember<br />

key information about medications, especially their potential<br />

side effects. Otherwise both may fail to recognize serious side effects<br />

and take appropriate actions when they occur. Doctors should be able<br />

to remember side effects well, since they have considerable medical<br />

knowledge and experience. However, this research shows that they are<br />

no better than laypersons. Participants studied medication information<br />

then we tested them using several memory tasks. Overall memory for<br />

side effects was very poor, for both doctors and laypersons. However<br />

both groups improved dramatically when the same information was<br />

presented in new displays, designed to enhance the “cognitive accessibility”<br />

of the information. Cognitive accessibility trumped many<br />

factors, including prior knowledge, memory load, and motivation.<br />

Cognitive accessibility may play an important role in many memory<br />

experiments, independent of their stimulus content.<br />

10:20–10:35 (319)<br />

Remembering Products, Not Faces: “Refreshing Recollection” of<br />

Eyewitnesses in Product Liability Situations. J. TRENT TERRELL<br />

& CHARLES A. WEAVER III, Baylor University (read by Charles A.<br />

Weaver III)—Eyewitness identification usually involves witnesses of<br />

a crime selecting suspects from a lineup. Eyewitness memory is also<br />

critical in product liability cases, where a person identifies particular<br />

products (usually containing asbestos) suspected of causing current<br />

illnesses (usually cancer). Due to the diseases’ latency periods, the illness<br />

often occurs decades after possible exposure. To assist witnesses<br />

in identifying products, attorneys often “refresh” their memories by<br />

showing them product photographs prior to identification. We showed<br />

witnesses a video containing various products. After 10-min or 1-week<br />

delays, subjects viewed photographs of products and told these “may”<br />

49<br />

help them in later tests. Subjects selected the products shown in photographs<br />

nearly 90% of the time, even if those photographs were misleading.<br />

In subsequent experiments, subjects viewed photographs of<br />

nonexistent products created using Photoshop (“Smucker’s barbeque<br />

sauce”). Subjects later identified these fictitious products approximately<br />

half the time. Implications for “refreshing recollection” of eyewitnesses<br />

are discussed.<br />

10:40–10:55 (320)<br />

Discriminating Fact From Fiction in Recovered Memories of<br />

Childhood Sexual Abuse. ELKE GERAERTS, Harvard University,<br />

D. STEPHEN LINDSAY, University of Victoria, HAROLD MERCK-<br />

ELBACH & LINSEY RAYMAEKERS, Maastricht University,<br />

MICHELLE M. ARNOLD, University of St Andrews, & JONATHAN<br />

S. SCHOOLER, University of British Columbia—Are individuals<br />

able to forget and then later accurately recover episodes of traumatic<br />

events such as childhood sexual abuse? According to some investigators,<br />

many recovered memory experiences are the products of overly<br />

suggestive therapy rather than reflecting true memory recovery. According<br />

to other investigators, such experiences can be genuinely forgotten<br />

and recovered quite accurately later in life. In this talk, I present<br />

research focusing on individuals who report having recovered a<br />

memory of childhood sexual abuse, examining two classes of people:<br />

those who gradually recovered childhood abuse during suggestive<br />

therapy, and those who spontaneously recovered being abused. In both<br />

groups, we assessed whether the abuse could be independently corroborated<br />

as well as cognitive markers believed to be sensitive to<br />

memory suggestibility and metamemory. This research reveals striking<br />

differences in these two types of memory recovery, both in terms<br />

of corroboration and memory ability. Possible mechanisms underlying<br />

genuine recovered memory experiences will be discussed.<br />

11:00–11:15 (321)<br />

Guided Cognition of Unsupervised Learning: Designing Effective<br />

Homework. Part 2. WILLIAM B. WHITTEN II, MITCHELL RABIN-<br />

OWITZ, & SANDRA E. WHITTEN, Fordham University—Guided<br />

Cognition structures study tasks to engage students in specific, observable<br />

cognitive events that elicit underlying cognitive processes.<br />

We identified cognitive events that commonly occur in classrooms<br />

and that have correlates in the experimental literature, then designed<br />

some into homework. Last year we reported that quiz performance<br />

was better after Guided Cognition homework than after Traditional<br />

homework on Shakespeare’s Macbeth, for average and advanced English<br />

literature students. Subsequent experiments using Conrad’s “<strong>The</strong><br />

Secret Sharer” and Anouilh’s Becket determined this advantage was<br />

due neither to differences in time spent on the two forms of homework,<br />

nor to teaching that preceded the homework. Recent experiments<br />

using Shakespeare’s Macbeth determined that these results are<br />

not due to novelty and that Guided Cognition experience can influence<br />

thinking during subsequent unguided learning. Detailed analysis indicates<br />

that visualizing, role-playing, brainstorming, divergent thinking,<br />

and relating to prior experience may each contribute to the<br />

Guided Cognition learning advantage.<br />

11:20–11:35 (322)<br />

Applying the Principles of Testing and Spacing to Long-Term Retention<br />

of Course Information. SHANA K. CARPENTER, University<br />

of California, San Diego, NICHOLAS J. CEPEDA, York University,<br />

& DORIS ALVAREZ & HAL PASHLER, University of California,<br />

San Diego—Spaced repetition and testing have been shown to enhance<br />

memory in short-term laboratory studies. <strong>The</strong>ir utility for promoting<br />

long-term retention in classroom situations is not clear, however. We<br />

explored 8th grade students’ memory for U.S. history facts (e.g.,<br />

Ulysses S. Grant became president of the U.S. in 1869), depending on<br />

whether the facts were reviewed after 3 days, versus 3 months. Students<br />

reviewed some facts through testing (Who became president of<br />

the U.S. in 1869?) followed by feedback (Ulysses S. Grant), and they<br />

reviewed other facts by simply rereading them. Nine months later, stu-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!