29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Sunday Morning Papers 297–302<br />

parietal lobule (SPL) subserves goal-directed orienting. Here, we investigated<br />

the independence of these attentional subsystems in patients<br />

with attentional deficits following an acquired lesion to the dorsal<br />

(SPL) or ventral (TPJ) parts of the attentional network. Patients<br />

completed a goal-directed attentional shifting task and a visual contingent<br />

capture task. Patients with lesioned SPL but spared TPJ exhibited<br />

difficulties with goal-directed attentional orienting as well as<br />

exhibited a “hyper-capture” phenomenon. Patients with lesioned TPJ<br />

but spared SPL performed normally on the goal-directed orienting<br />

task, while remaining immune to capture. <strong>The</strong>se findings suggest that<br />

in the normal functioning system, the two types of attentional orienting<br />

are not entirely independent, but, rather, goal-directed attentional<br />

control (SPL) carries information regarding what is important to the<br />

task at hand and thus constrains bottom-up attentional capture (TPJ).<br />

10:40–10:55 (297)<br />

Action Relations Reduce Spatial Extinction Through Configural<br />

Coding. JANE RIDDOCH & GLYN W. HUMPHREYS, University of<br />

Birmingham (sponsored by Glyn W. Humphreys)—Patients with parietal<br />

lesions have shown recovery from extinction when pairs of objects<br />

share an action relation (e.g., a bottle pouring into a glass) relative to<br />

when they do not (e.g., a bottle pouring away from a glass) (Riddoch<br />

et al., 2003, 2006). We investigated the visual constraints on this effect.<br />

We show that the advantage for objects sharing an action relation<br />

is significantly reduced when stimuli are inverted, whereas inversion<br />

does not affect unrelated objects. Similarly changing the size of the objects<br />

relative to one another reduces the effects of action relations on<br />

extinction. We suggest that the effects of action relation are based on<br />

high-level configural grouping of stimuli, which reduces spatial competition<br />

between the stimuli in patients with parietal damage.<br />

11:00–11:15 (298)<br />

Word Fragmentation on the Stroop Task: An Event-Related Potential<br />

Study. SOLEDAD BALLESTEROS, JOSÉ MANUEL REALES,<br />

& FRANCISCO MUÑOZ, UNED—Fifteen adults were presented<br />

with congruent, incongruent, and neutral stimuli at four levels of fragmentation<br />

while performing the Stroop task. Reaction times and<br />

event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded. An early negative<br />

component with a peak-amplitude at 85 msec was found that was<br />

larger for the more degraded stimuli at occipital-parietal areas. At<br />

200 msec, a negative potential larger for the most fragmented stimuli<br />

in incongruent condition reflected the Stroop effect. From 250 to<br />

400 msec, congruent stimuli showed a larger positive deviation relative<br />

to both neutral and incongruent conditions. This bilateral positive<br />

potential was larger for the more degraded stimuli. Congruence resolution<br />

signalled by a positive ERP and larger for the more fragmented<br />

stimuli occurred before the incongruent condition. A late larger negativity<br />

for the most fragmented stimuli than for the completed words<br />

peaked at 450 msec and was sensitive to the congruent–incongruent<br />

conflict. This ERP effect displays a centro-parietal topography.<br />

11:20–11:35 (299)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Shifting Sands of Conflict Processing. ROBERT WEST, Iowa<br />

State University—Studies using functional neuroimaging methodologies<br />

reveal that conflict processing is associated with recruitment of<br />

anterior and lateral prefrontal cortex, parietal cortex, and anterior cingulate.<br />

However, there is striking variation in the pattern of recruitment<br />

that is observed across studies using, PET, fMRI, and ERP methods.<br />

In this study I examined the influence of two factors (level of<br />

practice and trial pacing) on ERPs elicited during the Stroop task.<br />

When there was 500 msec between trials, practice led to a shift from<br />

medial frontal recruitment early in the task to lateral frontal and parietal<br />

recruitment later in the task. When there was 2,000 msec between<br />

trials, the shift in recruitment over time was much less pronounced.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se results are consistent with current theories of cognitive control<br />

and indicate that investigators need to consider both of these factors<br />

when interpreting the results of studies of conflict processing.<br />

46<br />

11:40–11:55 (300)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Cognitive Control and Representation Systems of the Human<br />

Brain. WALTER S. SCHNEIDER, MICHAEL W. COLE, ROBERT<br />

GOLDBERG, & SUDIR PATHAK, University of Pittsburgh—A fundamental<br />

distinction of human cognition and cortical processing is the<br />

interaction of representation specific areas (e.g., visual object, motor<br />

action) and domain general control systems (e.g., attention, decision,<br />

response mapping, affect coding). We detail the function, anatomy,<br />

and interaction of these cortical systems based on behavioral, fMRI,<br />

anatomical (DTI), and functional connectivity methods. We relate the<br />

cortical systems view to automatic and control processing as well as<br />

semantic representation. <strong>The</strong> representation areas are involved in representation<br />

specific encoding, recognition, similarity judgment,<br />

episodic recall, and automatic coding. <strong>The</strong> control system components<br />

are tightly coupled, showing differential activity based on comparison<br />

decision (anterior cingulate cortex and presupplementary motor<br />

area), goal processing (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex), stimulus filtering<br />

(inferior frontal junction), arousal (anterior insular cortex), attentional<br />

control (posterior parietal cortex), and affect assessment<br />

(amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex).<br />

<strong>The</strong> Nature of Thinking<br />

Regency DEFH, Sunday Morning, 10:20–12:00<br />

Chaired by Bradley C. Love, University of Texas, Austin<br />

10:20–10:35 (301)<br />

Putting the Psychology Back Into Psychological Models: Mechanistic<br />

Versus Rational Approaches. BRADLEY C. LOVE & MATT<br />

JONES, University of Texas, Austin, & YASUAKI SAKAMOTO,<br />

Stevens Institute of Technology—Two basic approaches to explaining<br />

the nature of the mind are the rational and mechanistic approaches.<br />

Rational analyses attempt to characterize the environment and the behavioral<br />

outcomes that humans seek to optimize, whereas mechanistic<br />

models attempt to simulate human behavior using processes and<br />

representations analogous to those used by humans. We compared<br />

these approaches on their accounts of how humans learn the variability<br />

of categories. <strong>The</strong> mechanistic model departs in subtle ways from<br />

rational principles, due to its incremental process of updating category<br />

representations. <strong>The</strong> model holds that people adjust their estimates of<br />

a category’s mean and variance through error-driven learning, based<br />

on discrepancies between new category members and the current representation<br />

of the category. <strong>The</strong> model yields a prediction, which we<br />

verify, regarding the effects of order manipulations that the rational<br />

approach does not anticipate. We suggest that psychological advances<br />

are primarily driven by consideration of process and representation,<br />

and that rational accounts trail these breakthroughs.<br />

10:40–10:55 (302)<br />

Smarter Than We Think: Conflict Monitoring in Decision Making.<br />

WIM DE NEYS, University of Leuven, OSHIN VARTANIAN, University<br />

of Toronto, VINOD GOEL, York University, & GÉRY D’YDE-<br />

WALLE, University of Leuven (sponsored by Géry d’Ydewalle)—<br />

Human thinking has been characterized as an interplay between an<br />

intuitive and logical reasoning process. Although monitoring the output<br />

of the two systems for conflict is crucial to avoid decision making<br />

errors there are some widely different views on the efficiency of<br />

the process. Kahneman (2002) claims that the monitoring of the<br />

heuristic system is typically quite lax whereas others such as Sloman<br />

(1996) claim it is flawless and people typically experience a struggle<br />

between what they “know” and “feel” in case of a conflict. We present<br />

a set of behavioral and fMRI studies that contrasted these views. Implicit<br />

behavioral conflict detection measures and the activation pattern<br />

of the lateral and medial (ACC) prefrontal cortex during decision<br />

making both point to the flawless nature of the monitoring: Although<br />

people frequently fail to inhibit illogical intuitive beliefs they do detect<br />

that their responses are not fully warranted.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!