29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Sunday Morning Papers 270–276<br />

items in new pairings), inverted-format pairs (studied pairs in which<br />

the picture–word format of each item was switched), half-old pairs (1<br />

studied item, 1 unstudied item), and new pairs (2 unstudied items). Patients,<br />

particularly those with confabulation, committed more false<br />

alarms to inverted-format pairs than did controls. In contrast, false<br />

alarms to rearranged pairs and other lures did not differ between<br />

groups. <strong>The</strong>se findings suggest that AR and SM tap distinct processes,<br />

with only the latter depending on ventromedial prefrontal cortex.<br />

8:40–8:55 (270)<br />

Cognitive Functioning Mediates the Use of Distinctive Processing<br />

in Reducing Memory Illusions. AYANNA K. THOMAS, Tufts University,<br />

& MARK A. MCDANIEL, Washington University—Recent<br />

studies suggest that distinctive processing can reduce errors of commission<br />

(Arndt & Reder, 2003). <strong>The</strong> present study examines the relationship<br />

between two aspects of distinctive processing (item-specific<br />

and relational) in younger adults, high frontal lobe functioning (FLF)<br />

older adults, and low FLF older adults. Variables presumed to affect<br />

both relational and item-specific processing were manipulated. Younger<br />

adults and high FLF older adults showed reductions in errors of commission<br />

when item-specific processing was executed. Furthermore,<br />

item-specific processing did not necessarily lead to impoverished relational<br />

processing; however, low FLF older adults showed reductions in<br />

errors of commission only when relational processing was impoverished.<br />

<strong>The</strong> results suggest that item-specific and relational processing<br />

may be performed in parallel and may not compete for finite resources.<br />

However, cognitive decline did affect the use of item-specific and relational<br />

information. <strong>The</strong>se findings suggest a model that accounts for<br />

distinctive processing as a function of age-related cognitive decline.<br />

9:00–9:15 (271)<br />

Adaptive Memory: Is Survival Processing Special? JAMES S.<br />

NAIRNE & JOSEFA N. S. PANDEIRADA, Purdue University—<br />

Nairne, Thompson, and Pandeirada (2007) recently proposed that our<br />

memory systems might be “tuned” to remember information that is<br />

processed for survival, perhaps as a result of fitness advantages accrued<br />

in our ancestral past. Across several experiments participants<br />

showed superior memory when unrelated words were rated for survival,<br />

at least when compared with several “deep” processing control<br />

conditions. <strong>The</strong> present experiments extend this “survival effect” to<br />

related lists containing survival-relevant categories. Rating words for<br />

survival in a categorized list, compared to rating the same words for<br />

pleasantness, produced the best recall, even though individual-item<br />

processing (e.g., rating for pleasantness) in a related list is thought to<br />

be an optimal procedure for enhancing subsequent recall. We also report<br />

the results of a category-sorting experiment in which items were<br />

sorted into the same four categories, but those categories were labeled<br />

as either survival-relevant or not.<br />

9:20–9:35 (272)<br />

Differential Effects of Emotional Content in Working Memory and<br />

Recognition. SUSANNE M. JAEGGI, MARTIN BUSCHKUEHL, &<br />

WALTER J. PERRIG, University of Bern (sponsored by Rudolf<br />

Groner)—Emotional content enhances memory performance. In general,<br />

there is a positive recall bias and sometimes, there is a slight advantage<br />

for negative material, which is explained by attentional<br />

processes. Only a few studies investigate the impact of emotional content<br />

on both working memory performance and long-term memory<br />

within subjects. We used a working memory task with neutral, happy,<br />

and angry faces as emotional stimuli. In a surprise recognition task 1 h<br />

after task completion, participants had to indicate which faces were<br />

presented in the working memory task before. We found differential<br />

effects of emotional content in the working memory task and recognition:<br />

Whereas in the working memory task, performance was best for<br />

the angry faces, participants had the longest reaction times to the angry<br />

faces in the recognition task, a dissociation which is interpreted by automatic<br />

and attention-related processes in the working memory condition,<br />

and by mood-repair processes in the recognition task.<br />

42<br />

9:40–9:55 (273)<br />

Inhibition of Eye Movements to Memorized Locations. ARTEM V.<br />

BELOPOLSKY & JAN THEEUWES, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam—<br />

Recent research has shown a direct link between spatial working memory<br />

and activation of the eye movement system. It was argued that<br />

keeping a location in working memory may be nothing else than the<br />

preparation of an eye movement. <strong>The</strong> present study further examined<br />

this question. Participants were asked to maintain a location in memory<br />

and on half of the trials to make a saccade in response to a central<br />

cue. On half of the trials, the location of the saccade target coincided<br />

with a location kept in memory. We found that participants were<br />

slower in making a saccade to a location kept in spatial working memory<br />

relative to other locations. This inhibition was significantly<br />

greater than inhibition of return caused by the mere presentation of<br />

the cue. <strong>The</strong> results suggest strong involvement of oculomotor system<br />

in maintenance of spatial working memory.<br />

Concept Learning<br />

Beacon A, Sunday Morning, 8:00–10:00<br />

Chaired by Carol A. Seger, Colorado State University<br />

8:00–8:15 (274)<br />

Four Lessons From Cognitive Neuroscience for Category Learning.<br />

CAROL A. SEGER, Colorado State University—First, abstraction and<br />

generalization processes in category learning are situational and limited.<br />

Some neural systems can generalize to new stimuli; others cannot.<br />

Second, cognitive processes recruited during a categorization task that<br />

researchers often assume to be inconsequential in actuality affect<br />

learning. For example, small differences in the characteristics of the<br />

feedback signal in category learning tasks have significant effects on<br />

neural recruitment and resulting learning. Third, categorization is<br />

procedural (that is, reliant on motor systems) to a greater degree than<br />

generally appreciated. Neural systems underlying motor processing<br />

are recruited during categorization, and category knowledge is often<br />

specific to a particular associated motor response. Fourth, the same<br />

neural systems are often recruited regardless of whether category<br />

learning is implicit or explicit. <strong>The</strong>refore, the accessibility of learning<br />

to conscious awareness may not be a fundamental distinction that<br />

can be used to differentiate between learning systems.<br />

8:20–8:35 (275)<br />

Mental Models and Boolean Concept Learning. GEOFFREY P.<br />

GOODWIN & PHILIP N. JOHNSON-LAIRD, Princeton University—<br />

A common source of new concepts is a Boolean combination of existing<br />

concepts—for example, pregnant or nursing women, and not on<br />

medication. As this example illustrates, any Boolean concept can be<br />

defined solely from a combination of properties using negation (not),<br />

conjunction (and), and inclusive disjunction (or). A common intuition<br />

underlying theories of the learning of these concepts is that individuals<br />

attempt to simplify the information presented to them; for example,<br />

they seek a parsimonious decision tree or, in a recent account, a<br />

minimal description of the concept. Here, we show that the intuition is<br />

correct, but that what is minimized is neither a decision tree nor a description,<br />

but the mental models of the concept. A single, theoretically<br />

derived parameter—number of mental models—predicts the difficulty<br />

of learning Boolean concepts better than more elaborate previous accounts.<br />

<strong>The</strong> results of a new experiment also corroborate the theory.<br />

8:40–8:55 (276)<br />

Task Switching in Category Learning: Evidence for Stimulus-<br />

Dependent Representation. MICHAEL A. ERICKSON, University<br />

of California, Riverside—ATRIUM, a multiple-system model of category<br />

learning, posits that within a single category-learning task people<br />

can learn to utilize different systems with different representations<br />

to classify different stimuli. This is referred to as stimulus-dependent<br />

representation (SDR). <strong>The</strong> use of SDR implies that people are switching<br />

from task to task as trials demand. Thus, the use of SDR can be

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!