29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Papers 263–269 Sunday Morning<br />

Task Switching<br />

Regency ABC, Sunday Morning, 8:00–9:40<br />

Chaired by Catherine M. Arrington, Lehigh University<br />

8:00–8:15 (263)<br />

Stimulus–Task Binding in Voluntary Task Switching: <strong>The</strong> Role of<br />

Prior Experience in Task Choice. CATHERINE M. ARRINGTON<br />

& RACHEL L. PAUKER, Lehigh University—Stimulus–task binding<br />

has been hypothesized to underlie some performance deficits on<br />

switch trials in task-switching paradigms. We examined the effect of<br />

individual stimuli on task choice in a voluntary task-switching (VTS)<br />

paradigm where subjects are allowed to choose which task to perform<br />

on each trial. Subjects performed large/small or living/nonliving<br />

judgments on single-word stimuli under instructions to perform the<br />

tasks equally often in a random order. Stimulus–task binding was<br />

measured as a match to the initial categorization of an item in all subsequent<br />

presentations of that stimulus. When subjects voluntarily<br />

choose the initial categorization, the probability of match to initial<br />

was substantial (.669). This effect was reduced (.522), but still significant,<br />

when the initial categorization was determined by the experimenter.<br />

<strong>The</strong> roles of prior experience with a stimulus and stimulus<br />

identity in VTS suggest that task choice is a complex process<br />

involving interactions of internal and external factors.<br />

8:20–8:35 (264)<br />

Strategic Control Over Task-Set Inhibition. ULRICH MAYR, University<br />

of Oregon, & STEPHAN STEGT, University of Bonn—We<br />

(Mayr & Bell, 2006) recently suggested that rate of switching in a voluntary<br />

switching situation depends on whether individuals adopt a<br />

“continuous-flow” or a “discrete-event” strategy. <strong>The</strong>oretically, the<br />

discrete-event strategy is characterized by inhibition after each trial,<br />

thereby clearing the slate for an unbiased choice of the next-trial task.<br />

In Experiment 1, we instructed subjects to either treat trials as a stream<br />

of interdependent stimulus–response instances or as distinct events.<br />

As expected, discrete-event subjects showed higher switch rates. In<br />

addition, they exhibited longer no-switch and faster switch RTs than<br />

did continuous-flow subjects, which is consistent with the use of inhibition<br />

to segregate consecutive events. In Experiment 2, we tested<br />

the inhibition prediction directly, by assessing the effect of strategy on<br />

the backward-inhibition effect (Mayr & Keele, 2000). As expected,<br />

we found substantial backward inhibition after discrete-event, but<br />

none after continuous-flow instruction. Thus, inhibition seems to be<br />

under some strategic control.<br />

8:40–8:55 (2<strong>65</strong>)<br />

Response Effects in Voluntary Task Switching. ANDRE VAN-<br />

DIERENDONCK, JELLE DEMANET, & BAPTIST LIEFOOGHE,<br />

Ghent University—In voluntary task switching, participants are free<br />

to select the task to perform, as long as they perform both tasks an approximately<br />

equal number of times and perform the tasks in a random<br />

order. Previous research (Mayr & Bell, 2006) demonstrated an influence<br />

of sequences of stimuli on this pattern of alternation. Following<br />

this up, we investigated the role of response sequences by manipulating<br />

the compatibility of the response layout in task selection and task<br />

execution. We were interested in the effects of the intertrial interval<br />

on the response used to indicate the subsequent task selection. Three<br />

experiments demonstrated that task selection latencies were affected<br />

by these response variations, whereas the task-repetition probabilities<br />

and task selection randomness only varied with the intertrial interval.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se results show that while response effects influence task-level<br />

processes, they do not come into play in the process of task selection.<br />

9:00–9:15 (266)<br />

Proactive and Concurrent Interferences in Task-Switching.<br />

MYEONG-HO SOHN, E. ZITA PATAI, & REBECCA B. WELDON,<br />

George Washington University—Cognitive control is required to minimize<br />

the interference effect on the performance of the goal-relevant<br />

41<br />

task. In this study, we examined the effects of proactive and concurrent<br />

interferences. <strong>The</strong> proactive interference occurs when the current<br />

task is different from the most recent task. <strong>The</strong> concurrent interference<br />

occurs when the current target is accompanied with a distractor.<br />

Participants performed two perceptual classification tasks and two semantic<br />

classification tasks in a task-switching paradigm. In general,<br />

participants were more fluent with perceptual than with semantic<br />

tasks. When switching between different tasks, the latency of semantic<br />

tasks was slower when preceded by perceptual tasks. In addition,<br />

the semantic tasks were performed slower when the concurrent distractor<br />

was associated with another semantic task. <strong>The</strong> perceptual<br />

tasks were not affected by the types of distractors or transitions. <strong>The</strong>se<br />

results suggest that the interference effect depends on the strength of<br />

the relevant and irrelevant tasks.<br />

9:20–9:35 (267)<br />

Hierarchical Plans Can Produce Nonhierarchical Behavior. GOR-<br />

DON D. LOGAN & DARRYL W. SCHNEIDER, Vanderbilt University—Hierarchical<br />

plans structure behavior by dividing a set of tasks<br />

into subsets and specifying which subset to perform at which time.<br />

Usually, the sequence of tasks in the plan mirrors the sequence of<br />

tasks the person performs—the plan says “do A, then B” and the person<br />

does A then B—making it hard to determine whether the observed<br />

evidence of hierarchical control reflects plan-level processing or tasklevel<br />

processing. We separated these levels of processing by having<br />

subjects memorize two task sequences and then perform a randomly<br />

cued task at one of the serial positions in a sequence (e.g., “alpha 3”<br />

meant “perform task three from list alpha”). Response times were<br />

longer when subjects switched between sequences than when they repeated<br />

sequences, indicating hierarchical plan-level processing. We<br />

present a model that illustrates how structured plans control sequences<br />

of responses that do not mirror the sequence in the plan.<br />

Memory<br />

Regency DEFH, Sunday Morning, 8:00–10:00<br />

Chaired by Moshe Naveh-Benjamin<br />

University of Missouri, Columbia<br />

8:00–8:15 (268)<br />

Is Older Adults’ Associative Memory Deficit Mediated by Age-<br />

Related Sensory Decline? MOSHE NAVEH-BENJAMIN, ANGELA<br />

KILB, & YOKO HARA, University of Missouri, Columbia—Numerous<br />

studies show age-related decline in episodic memory. One of the<br />

explanations for this decline is an associative deficit hypothesis,<br />

which attributes part of older adults’ declining episodic memory performance<br />

to their difficulties encoding and retrieving separate features<br />

of episodes as cohesive entities (e.g., Naveh-Benjamin, 2000).<br />

Here, we evaluate the degree to which this deficit is partially mediated<br />

by sensory loss associated with increased age. In two experiments,<br />

young adults were presented with visually and auditorily degraded<br />

word pairs. <strong>The</strong>ir memory for both the components and the<br />

associations of these degraded pairs was then tested using word and<br />

associative recognition tests, respectively. We also used groups of<br />

older adults with nondegraded stimuli. <strong>The</strong> results and their theoretical<br />

implications will be discussed.<br />

8:20–8:35 (269)<br />

Ventromedial Prefrontal Damage Impairs Source but Not Associative<br />

Memory. ELISA CIARAMELLI, Rotman Research Institute, &<br />

JULIA SPANIOL, Ryerson University (read by Julia Spaniol)—Both<br />

associative recognition (AR) and source memory (SM) are thought to<br />

capture episodic binding. However, whether these tasks tap the same<br />

cognitive and neural systems is unclear. Here, we show a dissociation<br />

between AR and SM in patients with lesions in ventromedial prefrontal<br />

cortex. Thirteen patients, including 6 with spontaneous confabulation,<br />

and 13 controls studied picture–word pairs and received<br />

an old–new recognition test. Lures included rearranged pairs (studied

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!