29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Saturday Afternoon Papers 257–262<br />

more quickly, indicating they did not consider their falsity, but this<br />

only held for statements noninformative when false. Thus, semantic<br />

properties of statements determine whether people consider their<br />

falsity.<br />

Discourse Processing<br />

Shoreline, Saturday Afternoon, 3:30–5:30<br />

Chaired by Gary E. Raney, University of Illinois, Chicago<br />

3:30–3:45 (257)<br />

Transfer Across Seemingly Unrelated Narratives: A Similarity-<br />

Based Explanation. GARY E. RANEY, FRANCES DANIEL,<br />

JOANNA C. BOVEE, FIONA LYNCH, & TOM VADAKARA, University<br />

of Illinois, Chicago—According to Raney (2003), repetition<br />

effects and transfer benefits across texts will be larger if the situation<br />

models of the texts overlap than if they do not overlap. Klin, Ralano,<br />

and Weingartner (in press) tested this aspect of Raney’s model. <strong>The</strong>y<br />

had readers read two supposedly unrelated stories that shared a sentence<br />

and measured transfer (processing time of target sentences in the<br />

second story). <strong>The</strong>y concluded that transfer was found across unrelated<br />

stories. To make this claim, Klin et al. must demonstrate that<br />

their story pairs are unrelated. We present passage similarity ratings<br />

from 4 studies supporting the conclusion that their story pairs are related.<br />

Similarity ratings were largest in those conditions where Klin<br />

et al. found fastest processing times and were smallest where Klin<br />

et al. found slowest processing times. We discuss these findings in relation<br />

to other research that proposes evidence of transfer across unrelated<br />

passages.<br />

3:50–4:05 (258)<br />

Verb Aspect and Event Simulations. CAROL J. MADDEN, Erasmus<br />

University Rotterdam, & DAVID J. THERRIAULT, University of<br />

Florida—According to embodied frameworks of language comprehension,<br />

pictures of instruments in use (open umbrella, uncapped pen)<br />

should better match readers’ simulations of described events than instruments<br />

not in use (closed umbrella, capped pen). This effect may<br />

be stronger for events described as ongoing (imperfective aspect)<br />

rather than completed (perfect aspect). Participants read imperfective<br />

(was playing) and perfect (had played) event descriptions, word by<br />

word in a rebus paradigm, with the critical instrument word replaced<br />

by a picture of that instrument in use or not in use. Keypresses to advance<br />

through in-use pictures were faster than keypresses for not inuse<br />

pictures in both imperfective and perfect sentences. Furthermore,<br />

keypresses on the two words following the picture, as well as sentencefinal<br />

sensibility judgments also showed the in-use advantage, although<br />

only for imperfective sentences. <strong>The</strong>se findings suggest that<br />

there are aspectual differences in the time course of perceptual simulations<br />

of events.<br />

4:10–4:25 (259)<br />

Cognitive and Neurological Bases of Inferential Processes During<br />

Reading. PAUL VAN DEN BROEK, ALEX MERKLE, & ELISSAIOS<br />

KARAGEORGIOU, University of Minnesota, PANAYIOTA KENDEOU,<br />

McGill University, & ART LEUTHOLD & APOSTOLOS GEORGOP-<br />

OULOS, University of Minnesota—How do we understand what we<br />

read? Reading comprehension is a complex process that involves the<br />

interpretation of unfolding text information using prior knowledge<br />

and the construction of a coherent representation of the text in the<br />

reader’s mind. In the present set of studies, we begin to explore the<br />

neurological bases of this complex process using magnetoencephalography<br />

(MEG). First, we use the Landscape Model theory of<br />

reading comprehension to make predictions about the specific<br />

40<br />

moment-by-moment comprehension activities during the reading of<br />

various texts. Next, we collect MEG data during reading for comprehension<br />

of these texts. Finally, we draw connections between the<br />

model and behavioral data to better understand the cognitive and neurological<br />

bases of reading comprehension.<br />

4:30–4:45 (260)<br />

Memory for Functional Relations in Text and Problem Solving.<br />

JOHN B. BLACK, Teachers College, Columbia University—This research<br />

investigated the relationship between participants’ memories<br />

for various kinds of information in expository texts and the participants’<br />

abilities to solve problems using that information. Of particular<br />

interest were participants’ memories for functional relations that<br />

describe how one entity changes as a function of changes in another<br />

entity, how integrated the memory for these functional relations is,<br />

and how well the participants can use this information to solve “whatif<br />

” problems (i.e., problems where a change is described and the participants<br />

have to figure out what other changes will occur). College<br />

students read texts describing systems of interacting entities, then recalled<br />

the texts, drew pictures of the text contents, and then solved the<br />

“what-if ” problems. <strong>The</strong> relationships between memory for the functional<br />

relations, the integration of those memories, and the problem<br />

solving performance were examined.<br />

4:50–5:05 (261)<br />

Age-Related Changes in Spoken Discourse Comprehension.<br />

MITCHELL S. SOMMERS, SANDRA HALE, JOEL MYERSON, &<br />

NATHAN S. ROSE, Washington University, & NANCY TYE-MURRAY<br />

& BRENT SPEHAR, Washington University Medical School—Previous<br />

research on adult age-related differences in speech comprehension has<br />

focused primarily on lower levels in the language comprehension<br />

process (i.e., phoneme and word perception, and, to a lesser extent,<br />

sentence comprehension). In contrast, the present study examined potential<br />

age-related differences in spoken discourse comprehension. To<br />

this end, we measured spoken discourse comprehension for three different<br />

passage types (lectures, interviews, and oral histories) using<br />

three types of comprehension questions (information, integration, and<br />

inference). In addition, we measured auditory sensitivity as a potential<br />

mediator of age-related differences. Consistent with previous<br />

studies, we found that spoken language comprehension declines as a<br />

function of age during adulthood. Importantly, we also found that taking<br />

into account normal age-related changes in auditory function did<br />

not substantially reduce this deficit. Instead, our results indicate that<br />

age-related differences in cognitive processes are primarily responsible<br />

for the deficits in spoken discourse comprehension.<br />

5:10–5:25 (262)<br />

Bill Clinton on the Middle East: Perspective in Media Interviews.<br />

CAMELIA SULEIMAN, Florida International University, &<br />

DANIEL C. O’CONNELL, Georgetown University (read by Daniel C.<br />

O’Connell)—Four TV and two radio interviews given by Bill Clinton<br />

after the publication of his memoirs in June 2004 are examined for expressions<br />

of perspective on the Middle East. Bill Clinton’s personal perspective<br />

is clearly reflected in his choice of referencing terms for parties<br />

in the Middle East conflict: <strong>The</strong> Israeli and Palestinian points of<br />

view are designated by first-person and third-person pronominals, respectively.<br />

A similar relationship is found for his references to the USA<br />

and Iraq. His perspective is manifested in his references to countries,<br />

people, and individual political leaders. Such references thus become<br />

expressions of a more general frame of power relationships in discourse,<br />

in which a progressive, advanced world is compared with a primitive,<br />

violent world. Implications for a theory of personal perspective in discourse<br />

and for the dialogic nature of perspective are discussed.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!