29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Saturday Afternoon Papers 244–250<br />

In several experiments, memory was impaired for new contextual information<br />

presented with an emotional harbinger when the anticipated<br />

negative or positive emotional outcome did not occur. Furthermore,<br />

there was retrograde amnesia for the emotional harbinger itself as well<br />

as forgetting of neutral information following it. This emotional harbinger<br />

effect may help shed light on the mechanisms of disorders involving<br />

impaired emotional expectations, such as anxiety, phobias,<br />

and compulsive gambling.<br />

5:10–5:25 (244)<br />

Quantitative Review of 25 Years of Research on Prospective Memory.<br />

BOB UTTL & KIMBERLY BALTIMORE, Red Deer College—We<br />

rely upon prospective memory proper (ProMP) to bring back to awareness<br />

previously formed plans and intentions at the right place and<br />

time—for example, a plan to buy groceries en route home. ProMP is<br />

distinguished from other subdomains of prospective memory (ProM)<br />

such as vigilance/monitoring and habitual ProM. Our meta-analysis<br />

of several hundred articles accumulated over 25 years of research on<br />

ProM shows that (1) many studies suffer from widespread methodological<br />

problems (e.g., ceiling effects, small sample sizes) and conceptual<br />

confusions (e.g., failure to distinguish between ProM subdomains),<br />

(2) many aspects of ProM have received no or only minimal<br />

attention (e.g., event cued ProMP examined in natural settings), and<br />

(3) the ProM instruction to ongoing task start delay as well as a number<br />

of ProM cues encountered by each participant influence performance<br />

on ProM tasks, supporting the distinction between vigilance/<br />

monitoring and ProMP.<br />

Categorization<br />

Beacon B, Saturday Afternoon, 3:30–5:30<br />

Chaired by Donald A. Schumsky, University of Cincinnati<br />

3:30–3:45 (245)<br />

Response-Time Approach to Contrasting Rule and Exemplar Models<br />

of Classification. ROBERT M. NOSOFSKY & MARIO FIFIC,<br />

Indiana University, Bloomington—We collect response-time data in<br />

tasks of speeded classification to contrast the predictions of serialprocessing<br />

rule-based models, parallel-processing rule-based models,<br />

and exemplar models. <strong>The</strong> paradigm builds upon the “systems-factorialtechnology”<br />

used by researchers for identifying alternative mentalprocessing<br />

architectures. Initial validation tests show that in conditions<br />

involving separable-dimension stimuli and in which observers<br />

are given explicit instructions for use of a serial rule-based strategy,<br />

the technology recovers the use of this strategy. Ongoing tests contrast<br />

the predictions from the models in situations involving integraldimension<br />

stimuli and in situations in which observers learn category<br />

structures via induction over training exemplars. Beyond contrasting<br />

rule and exemplar models, the research aims to unite mentalarchitecture<br />

and random-walk approaches to modeling choice and response<br />

times.<br />

3:50–4:05 (246)<br />

Similarity and Categorization, Single or Multiple Processes? An<br />

Application of the Reverse Association Test. TAKASHI YAMAUCHI,<br />

Texas A&M University—<strong>The</strong> reverse association test (Dunn & Kirsner,<br />

1988) is a powerful procedure to assess the link between underlying<br />

cognitive processes and task performance. It offers strong evidence for<br />

the presence of multiple processes/systems underlying two related<br />

tasks. However, this test has not been fully utilized. <strong>The</strong> test often requires<br />

a large scale single study employing at least 6 independent manipulations.<br />

Furthermore, the statistical procedure to verify reverse association<br />

has not been clearly delineated. This article examines the<br />

relationship between categorization and similarity judgments and presents<br />

practical solutions for these problems. First, a meta-analytic procedure<br />

comparing multiple studies is proposed. Second, a contrast<br />

analysis for the detection of reverse association is illustrated in a case<br />

study. This case study reveals reverse association in categorization and<br />

38<br />

similarity judgments, providing additional evidence for the view that<br />

categorization and similarity rely on separable cognitive processes.<br />

4:10–4:25 (247)<br />

Promoting the Use of Less Likely Category Alternatives in Property<br />

Induction With Uncertain Categories. BRETT K. HAYES &<br />

BEN R. NEWELL, University of New South Wales—Three experiments<br />

examined how people make property inferences about instances<br />

whose category membership is uncertain. Participants were shown<br />

two categories and a novel instance with a feature that indicated the<br />

instance was more likely to belong to one category (target) than to the<br />

other (nontarget). Participants then made categorization decisions and<br />

property inferences about the novel instance. In some conditions property<br />

inferences could only be made by considering both target and<br />

nontarget categories. In other conditions predictions could be based<br />

on both categories or on the target category alone. Like previous studies<br />

(e.g., Murphy & Ross, 1994, 2005), we found that many people<br />

made predictions based on consideration of only the target category.<br />

However, the prevalence of such single-category reasoning was reduced<br />

by highlighting the costs of neglecting nontarget alternatives<br />

and by asking for inferences before categorization decisions. <strong>The</strong> implications<br />

for process models of uncertain inference are discussed.<br />

4:30–4:45 (248)<br />

Judging Discrete and Continuous Quantity: In Large Sets, Number<br />

Counts. HILARY C. BARTH, Wesleyan University—Recent studies<br />

suggest that interference from automatically extracted irrelevant quantitative<br />

dimensions affects adults’ judgments of discrete and continuous<br />

quantity. Four experiments provide evidence against this idea.<br />

Adults made judgments of the total continuous quantity present in a set<br />

of elements. Continuous amount was not rapidly and automatically extracted;<br />

comparisons of possible models of individual performance<br />

demonstrate that participants arrived at estimates of total continuous<br />

amount through at least four different unconscious computations. A<br />

fourth experiment required participants to make judgments of the number<br />

of elements in a set. Comparisons of possible models of individual<br />

performance show that a single model can explain each participant’s responses,<br />

and that all participants based their judgments on discrete<br />

number. <strong>The</strong>se findings demonstrate large qualitative differences between<br />

adults’ routes to discrete and continuous quantity: the computation<br />

of total continuous quantity is inaccurate and idiosyncratic, unlike<br />

the quick and effective extraction of discrete numerical information.<br />

4:50–5:05 (249)<br />

Absolute Frequency Identification: How a Sequence of Events Can<br />

Affect People’s Judgments. PETKO KUSEV & PETER AYTON,<br />

City University London, PAUL VAN SCHAIK, University of Teesside,<br />

& NICK CHATER, University College London (sponsored by James<br />

Hampton)—<strong>The</strong>ories of absolute identification and categorization established<br />

over past decades have revealed people’s inability to classify<br />

or judge perceived objects independently of their context. Our series<br />

of experiments shows that judged frequencies of sequentially encountered<br />

stimuli are affected by certain properties of the sequence<br />

configuration: Representations of a category do not only depend on<br />

(1) the number of stimuli/chunks in the sequence (2) the relation of<br />

the current stimulus to the immediately preceding stimulus, and (3) relations<br />

between stimuli further back versus recent stimuli but also on<br />

(4) simple sequence characteristics. Specifically, a first-run effect occurs<br />

whereby people overestimate the frequency of a given category<br />

of event when that category is the first repeated category to occur in<br />

the sequence. While current theories of absolute identification and<br />

categorization cannot account for these results, the simplicity framework,<br />

suggested in this paper, accounts for this and other context effects<br />

on judgment tasks.<br />

5:10–5:25 (250)<br />

Evolutionary Models of Color Categorization Based on Discrimination.<br />

KIMBERLY A. JAMESON, NATALIA L. KOMAROVA, LOUIS

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!