S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

psychonomic.org
from psychonomic.org More from this publisher
29.01.2013 Views

Saturday Morning Papers 194–196 ken words. When living in a new dialect region, however, processing difficulties associated with dialectal variation dissipate over time. Through a series of priming tasks (form priming, semantic priming, and long-term repetition priming), we investigate the role of experience in the perception and representation of dialectal variation. The main questions addressed are: (1) How are cross-dialect variants recognized and stored, and (2) how are these variants accommodated by listeners with different levels of exposure to a particular dialect? Three claims are made based on the results: (1) Dialect production is not representative of dialect perception and representation, (2) experience is linked with a listener’s ability to recognize and represent spoken words, and (3) there is a general benefit for having the status as the “ideal” variant, even if this variant is not the most common one. 11:00–11:15 (194) First Impressions and Last Resorts: How Listeners Adjust to Speaker Variability. TANYA KRALJIC, University of California, San Diego, & ARTHUR G. SAMUEL & SUSAN E. BRENNAN, Stony Brook University—The study of speech perception is fundamentally the search for processes that support perceptual constancy— the perception of a variable physical signal as relatively constant. Little reliable acoustic invariance exists in speech. We propose that speech perception processes recover invariants not about the signal, but about the source that produced the signal, and examine the evidence the system can use to do so. We find perceptual learning for properties of speech resulting from idiosyncratic speaker characteristics; the same properties are not learned when they can be attributed to incidental factors. In the absence of explicit attribution, the perceptual system may rely on episodic order (a “first-impressions” heuristic), but this heuristic can be overridden when relevant pragmatic information is available (e.g., if the listener sees that the speaker has a pen in her mouth). Perceptual learning is a powerful mechanism for coping with variability, but is applied flexibly and conservatively. 11:20–11:35 (195) Exploring the Relationship Between Face Familiarity and Speechreadbility. KAREN LANDER, University of Manchester— 30 Theories of face perception suggest independence between identity and visual speech processing (Bruce & Young, 1986). In two experiments we explore how face familiarity influences speechreadability. In Experiment 1, we compare the speechreading performance from participants who are, or are not, artificially familiarized with the speaker. First, we measured baseline speechreading performance. Next, participants either view the speaker telling a story (familiarized group) or complete a pen and paper puzzle (control group). Speechreading performance is then measured again. Finally, participants view another story or complete another puzzle before speechreading performance is re-measured. The results suggest that speechreading performance improves faster when the participant is more familiar with the speaker. In Experiment 2 we explore this effect by comparing speechreading performance from a personally familiar face and an unfamiliar one. The results are discussed with regard to the independence of facial identity and visual speech processing and the factors that influence speechreading. 11:40–11:55 (196) Penetrability of Speech Segmentation Strategies by Attentional Factors. SVEN L. MATTYS, University of Bristol—Although common in everyday listening conditions, attentional loads have been largely ignored in the literature on spoken-word recognition and segmentation. Here, we investigated whether listeners’ segmentation strategies are affected by a concurrent attentional load [the penetrability hypothesis] and, if so, whether the nature of the load (lexical-semantic vs. acoustic-phonetic) interacts with the nature of the segmentation strategy (knowledge-driven vs. signal-driven) (the domain-specificity hypothesis). Participants performed a segmentation task in one ear while performing either semantic categorization or phoneme monitoring on unrelated stimuli in the other ear. Compared to baseline, both loads were found to increase knowledge-driven segmentation. The results show that speech-processing mechanisms are penetrable by attentional factors and that this effect is not domain specific: Reliance on meaning is elevated whenever a processing load is encountered, regardless of the nature of the load. This result is discussed relative to theories of segmentation that give primacy to knowledgedriven strategies.

Papers 197–203 Saturday Afternoon SYMPOSIUM: Toward a Cognitive Psychology of Collective Memory: Methods, Data, and Theories Regency ABC, Saturday Afternoon, 1:30–3:50 Chaired by Amanda J. Barnier, Macquarie University 1:30–1:45 (197) Toward a Cognitive Psychology of Collective Memory: Methods, Data, and Theories. AMANDA J. BARNIER, Macquarie University— In recent years, across many disciplines, there has been increasing interest in the relationships among individuals remembering alone, individuals remembering in a group, and the group itself remembering. Cognitive psychologists have contributed a range of clever paradigms to this area, which map the reciprocal influences of individuals and their social groups on both remembering and forgetting. In this symposium, presenters describe experiments that used a range of paradigms (including collaborative recall, social contagion, and adaptations of retrieval-induced forgetting), that focused on a range of target material (from relatively simple material to more complex, emotional, personal memories), and that collected data from a range of participants (including younger adults or older adults, from laboratory settings and applied settings). Based on these methods and data, the presenters develop and discuss theoretical interpretations that will best capture the cognitive psychology of individual and collective memory. 1:50–2:05 (198) Individual and Group Retrieval Efforts Modulate Later Memory Outcomes. HELENA M. BLUMEN & SUPARNA RAJARAM, Stony Brook University (read by Suparna Rajaram)—Group retrieval is a common social practice and one that is often emphasized in educational settings (e.g., study groups). Yet, the effects of group retrieval on later individual memory remain poorly understood. To understand this relationship, we compared three hypotheses—the individual-strategy hypothesis, the combined-strategy hypothesis and the group-strategy hypothesis—and tested the roles of two opposing cognitive processes, namely, anticipated benefits from re-exposure to study material during collaboration and losses from retrieval disruption that can occur because of other group members’ output. Participants studied unrelated words and completed three successive recall trials in one of four conditions—III (individual–individual–individual), ICI (individual– collaborative–individual), CII (collaborative–individual–individual), and CCI (collaborative–collaborative–individual). The results support the group-strategy hypothesis and the individual-strategy hypothesis by showing that integrating other group members’ output through repeated group recalls (CCI) and securing individual retrieval organization prior to group recall (ICI) are important for benefiting from reexposure during group collaboration. 2:10–2:25 (199) Collaboration Among Older Adults: The Role of Retrieval Manipulations. MICHELLE L. MEADE, Montana State University, & HENRY L. ROEDIGER III, Washington University—Age differences in collaborative inhibition were examined across repeated retrieval attempts. Subjects studied categorized word lists and were then given two consecutive recall tests and a final recognition test. On the first recall test, subjects were given cued or forced recall instructions and recalled the lists either alone or in collaboration with another subject of the same age group. Collaborative inhibition was found for both young and older adults on cued and forced recall tests. Of interest was how this initial recall influenced subsequent memory performance. The subsequent cued recall test and the final recognition test were each taken individually. No lasting effect of collaborative inhibition was obtained when young and older adults were subsequently tested on an individual cued recall test or when subjects were tested on a final recognition test. 2:30–2:45 (200) The Mystery of Stimulated Cognition in Collaborative Recall. 31 VERLIN B. HINSZ, North Dakota State University—People tend to believe that other people facilitate their recall of temporarily forgotten material. This belief is akin to socially cued recall and is consistent with the notion of stimulated cognition in that social interaction creates new solutions and responses. In contrast, combinations-ofcontributions theory holds that group interaction cannot be truly creative and that group performance cannot exceed an optimal combination of the member contributions (e.g., memories). These contrasting perspectives are examined in a laboratory experiment involving the recall of information from a murder mystery. The recall performance of interacting dyads is compared to relevant individual conditions. Consistent with earlier research, dyads were found to achieve higher levels of recall performance, but did not exceed that expected from a direct combination of dyad member recollections. Moreover, the results demonstrated collaborative interference rather than socially cued recall. The results from a number of recall measures are inconsistent with the stimulated cognition perspective. 2:50–3:05 (201) Interpersonal Source Monitoring of Eyewitness Evidence. D. STEPHEN LINDSAY, C. A. ELIZABETH BRIMACOMBE, LEORA C. DAHL, & MELISSA A. BOYCE, University of Victoria—When one person listens to another’s memory report, the auditor evaluates (if only implicitly) the veracity of the speaker’s report. Such evaluations may be based on beliefs regarding the speaker’s expertise and/or on supposed cues to reliability in the speaker’s behavior (e.g., fluency, amount of detail, apparent confidence). My colleague Elizabeth Brimacombe, our students, and I have been exploring interpersonal metamemory in the context of simulated forensic investigations in which subject/investigators interview a witness to a video-taped crime, search a data base of potential suspects, rate the probability that their suspect is the culprit (p[S = C]), administer a lineup identification test to the witness, and re-rate p[S = C]. We find that undergraduate subject/investigators put far too much stock in witnesses, especially under conditions that lead witnesses to perform poorly, and that subject/investigators are as swayed by unreliable witnesses as by reliable ones. I’ll review some of these experiments and discuss their implications. 3:10–3:25 (202) Socially Shared Retrieval-Induced Forgetting of People’s Flashbulb Memories of September 11. WILLIAM HIRST & ALIN COMAN, New School for Social Research—Something as commonplace as a conversation can shape and reshape memories. Although most work has focused on memory implantation, Cuc, Koppel, and Hirst (2007) recently demonstrated that silences in a conversation—specifically, a speakers’ failure to recall aspects of a memory shared by a listener— will induce the listener to forget the unmentioned material more than would be expected if the conversation had never taken place. We extend this finding of socially shared retrieval-induced forgetting to emotionally intense material and to conversations about different, but related memories. We collected in a structured interview flashbulb memories of September 11, formed pairs, and then asked each pair to share their memories. A final recognition test for the information collected in the structured interview followed. Response times were recorded. Socially shared retrieval-induced forgetting was observed. Even when one talks about different emotionally intense events, conversations can alter memory. 3:30–3:45 (203) Social Contagion of Autobiographical Memories. AMANDA J. BARNIER, Macquarie University, TASNEEM KHAN, University of New South Wales, & CELIA B. HARRIS & JOHN SUTTON, Macquarie University—Roediger and Meade’s social contagion paradigm illustrates the impact of collaboration on individual memory. To examine its impact on more complex, self-relevant material, we adapted the procedure to autobiographical memories. In Session 1, participants individually recalled recent events such as their 18th birthday.

Papers 197–203 Saturday Afternoon<br />

SYMPOSIUM: Toward a Cognitive Psychology of Collective<br />

Memory: Methods, Data, and <strong>The</strong>ories<br />

Regency ABC, Saturday Afternoon, 1:30–3:50<br />

Chaired by Amanda J. Barnier, Macquarie University<br />

1:30–1:45 (197)<br />

Toward a Cognitive Psychology of Collective Memory: Methods,<br />

Data, and <strong>The</strong>ories. AMANDA J. BARNIER, Macquarie University—<br />

In recent years, across many disciplines, there has been increasing interest<br />

in the relationships among individuals remembering alone, individuals<br />

remembering in a group, and the group itself remembering.<br />

Cognitive psychologists have contributed a range of clever paradigms<br />

to this area, which map the reciprocal influences of individuals and<br />

their social groups on both remembering and forgetting. In this symposium,<br />

presenters describe experiments that used a range of paradigms<br />

(including collaborative recall, social contagion, and adaptations<br />

of retrieval-induced forgetting), that focused on a range of target<br />

material (from relatively simple material to more complex, emotional,<br />

personal memories), and that collected data from a range of participants<br />

(including younger adults or older adults, from laboratory settings<br />

and applied settings). Based on these methods and data, the presenters<br />

develop and discuss theoretical interpretations that will best<br />

capture the cognitive psychology of individual and collective memory.<br />

1:50–2:05 (198)<br />

Individual and Group Retrieval Efforts Modulate Later Memory<br />

Outcomes. HELENA M. BLUMEN & SUPARNA RAJARAM, Stony<br />

Brook University (read by Suparna Rajaram)—Group retrieval is a<br />

common social practice and one that is often emphasized in educational<br />

settings (e.g., study groups). Yet, the effects of group retrieval on later<br />

individual memory remain poorly understood. To understand this relationship,<br />

we compared three hypotheses—the individual-strategy<br />

hypothesis, the combined-strategy hypothesis and the group-strategy<br />

hypothesis—and tested the roles of two opposing cognitive processes,<br />

namely, anticipated benefits from re-exposure to study material during<br />

collaboration and losses from retrieval disruption that can occur<br />

because of other group members’ output. Participants studied unrelated<br />

words and completed three successive recall trials in one of four<br />

conditions—III (individual–individual–individual), ICI (individual–<br />

collaborative–individual), CII (collaborative–individual–individual),<br />

and CCI (collaborative–collaborative–individual). <strong>The</strong> results support<br />

the group-strategy hypothesis and the individual-strategy hypothesis<br />

by showing that integrating other group members’ output through repeated<br />

group recalls (CCI) and securing individual retrieval organization<br />

prior to group recall (ICI) are important for benefiting from reexposure<br />

during group collaboration.<br />

2:10–2:25 (199)<br />

Collaboration Among Older Adults: <strong>The</strong> Role of Retrieval Manipulations.<br />

MICHELLE L. MEADE, Montana State University, &<br />

HENRY L. ROEDIGER III, Washington University—Age differences<br />

in collaborative inhibition were examined across repeated retrieval attempts.<br />

Subjects studied categorized word lists and were then given<br />

two consecutive recall tests and a final recognition test. On the first<br />

recall test, subjects were given cued or forced recall instructions and<br />

recalled the lists either alone or in collaboration with another subject<br />

of the same age group. Collaborative inhibition was found for both<br />

young and older adults on cued and forced recall tests. Of interest was<br />

how this initial recall influenced subsequent memory performance.<br />

<strong>The</strong> subsequent cued recall test and the final recognition test were<br />

each taken individually. No lasting effect of collaborative inhibition<br />

was obtained when young and older adults were subsequently tested<br />

on an individual cued recall test or when subjects were tested on a<br />

final recognition test.<br />

2:30–2:45 (200)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Mystery of Stimulated Cognition in Collaborative Recall.<br />

31<br />

VERLIN B. HINSZ, North Dakota State University—People tend to<br />

believe that other people facilitate their recall of temporarily forgotten<br />

material. This belief is akin to socially cued recall and is consistent<br />

with the notion of stimulated cognition in that social interaction<br />

creates new solutions and responses. In contrast, combinations-ofcontributions<br />

theory holds that group interaction cannot be truly creative<br />

and that group performance cannot exceed an optimal combination<br />

of the member contributions (e.g., memories). <strong>The</strong>se contrasting<br />

perspectives are examined in a laboratory experiment involving the recall<br />

of information from a murder mystery. <strong>The</strong> recall performance of<br />

interacting dyads is compared to relevant individual conditions. Consistent<br />

with earlier research, dyads were found to achieve higher levels<br />

of recall performance, but did not exceed that expected from a direct<br />

combination of dyad member recollections. Moreover, the results<br />

demonstrated collaborative interference rather than socially cued recall.<br />

<strong>The</strong> results from a number of recall measures are inconsistent<br />

with the stimulated cognition perspective.<br />

2:50–3:05 (201)<br />

Interpersonal Source Monitoring of Eyewitness Evidence. D.<br />

STEPHEN LINDSAY, C. A. ELIZABETH BRIMACOMBE, LEORA C.<br />

DAHL, & MELISSA A. BOYCE, University of Victoria—When one<br />

person listens to another’s memory report, the auditor evaluates (if<br />

only implicitly) the veracity of the speaker’s report. Such evaluations<br />

may be based on beliefs regarding the speaker’s expertise and/or on<br />

supposed cues to reliability in the speaker’s behavior (e.g., fluency,<br />

amount of detail, apparent confidence). My colleague Elizabeth<br />

Brimacombe, our students, and I have been exploring interpersonal<br />

metamemory in the context of simulated forensic investigations in<br />

which subject/investigators interview a witness to a video-taped<br />

crime, search a data base of potential suspects, rate the probability that<br />

their suspect is the culprit (p[S = C]), administer a lineup identification<br />

test to the witness, and re-rate p[S = C]. We find that undergraduate<br />

subject/investigators put far too much stock in witnesses, especially<br />

under conditions that lead witnesses to perform poorly, and that<br />

subject/investigators are as swayed by unreliable witnesses as by reliable<br />

ones. I’ll review some of these experiments and discuss their<br />

implications.<br />

3:10–3:25 (202)<br />

Socially Shared Retrieval-Induced Forgetting of People’s Flashbulb<br />

Memories of September 11. WILLIAM HIRST & ALIN COMAN,<br />

New School for Social Research—Something as commonplace as a<br />

conversation can shape and reshape memories. Although most work<br />

has focused on memory implantation, Cuc, Koppel, and Hirst (2007)<br />

recently demonstrated that silences in a conversation—specifically, a<br />

speakers’ failure to recall aspects of a memory shared by a listener—<br />

will induce the listener to forget the unmentioned material more than<br />

would be expected if the conversation had never taken place. We extend<br />

this finding of socially shared retrieval-induced forgetting to<br />

emotionally intense material and to conversations about different, but<br />

related memories. We collected in a structured interview flashbulb<br />

memories of September 11, formed pairs, and then asked each pair to<br />

share their memories. A final recognition test for the information collected<br />

in the structured interview followed. Response times were<br />

recorded. Socially shared retrieval-induced forgetting was observed.<br />

Even when one talks about different emotionally intense events, conversations<br />

can alter memory.<br />

3:30–3:45 (203)<br />

Social Contagion of Autobiographical Memories. AMANDA J.<br />

BARNIER, Macquarie University, TASNEEM KHAN, University of<br />

New South Wales, & CELIA B. HARRIS & JOHN SUTTON, Macquarie<br />

University—Roediger and Meade’s social contagion paradigm<br />

illustrates the impact of collaboration on individual memory. To examine<br />

its impact on more complex, self-relevant material, we adapted<br />

the procedure to autobiographical memories. In Session 1, participants<br />

individually recalled recent events such as their 18th birthday.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!