S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

psychonomic.org
from psychonomic.org More from this publisher
29.01.2013 Views

Saturday Morning Papers 179–186 11:20–11:35 (179) Dynamic Processes in Visual Word Recognition: A New Theoretical Challenge. SHANNON O’MALLEY & DEREK BESNER, University of Waterloo (read by Derek Besner)—New findings illustrate how the effect of word frequency in reading aloud is context sensitive in a way unanticipated to date. These findings are taken to suggest that the underlying processing is dynamic in a way largely unanticipated to date. Several theoretical accounts are considered. 11:40–11:55 (180) On the Biological and Computational Plausibility of Grandmother Cells. JEFFREY S. BOWERS, University of Bristol—Advocates of the PDP approach often highlight the similarity between the distributed representations learned in connectionist models and the neural networks implemented in the brain. Models with localist coding, including localist network models that learn, are often rejected on the basis of their biological implausibility. In this talk I review a variety of singlecell recording studies that undermine the common assumption that neuroscience data are more consistent with the PDP approach. Indeed, the field of neurophysiology is predicated on the (well-demonstrated) fact that the activity of single neurons correlates with perception and action. This observation is problematic for the PDP assumption that knowledge is coded in a distributed manner. I also challenge some of the common functional (computational) criticisms that are raised against models that include localist representations. Visual Attention Beacon B, Saturday Morning, 10:00–12:00 Chaired by Raymond M. Klein, Dalhousie University 10:00–10:15 (181) Two Flavors of Inhibition of Return. ANA CHICA & JUAN LUPIÁÑEZ, University of Granada, & TRACY L. TAYLOR & RAY- MOND M. KLEIN, Dalhousie University (read by Raymond M. Klein)— Inhibition of return (IOR) refers to slower reaction times to targets presented at previously stimulated or inspected locations. Taylor and Klein (2000) showed that IOR can affect both input and output processing, depending on whether the oculomotor system is in a quiescent or in a prepared state, respectively. If the motoric flavor of IOR is truly nonperceptual and nonattentional, no IOR should be observed when the responses to targets are not explicitly spatial. When the eyes moved to the peripheral cue and back to center before the target appeared (to emphasize the motoric component), IOR was generated in a detection task (for which motor preparation is important) but not in a color discrimination task. This same discrimination task showed IOR when the motoric component was not activated, by preventing eye movements to the cue. Thus, the motoric flavor of IOR, elicited by oculomotor programming is restricted to output processing. 10:20–10:35 (182) Inducing Inhibition of Return in One Person Based on the Actions of Another. GEOFF G. COLE & PAUL A. SKARRATT, University of Durham, & ALAN KINGSTONE, University of British Columbia— Welsh et al. (2005) reported a highly novel inhibition of return (IOR) effect in which the phenomenon is induced in one person through the observation of another person’s actions. They showed that participants were slower to make reaching responses to a location that had previously been responded to by another person (compared to a location where no previous response had been made). The present study examined whether this socially modulated IOR effect occurs when the observer only knows where the other person has responded, rather than actually seeing the response being made. The results showed the presence of IOR both when the reaching response was visible and when the response location was only inferred. This suggests that IOR can be induced on the basis of a higher order inference of the biological behavior of another individual. 28 10:40–10:55 (183) Effects of Exogenous and Endogenous Orienting of Attention on Change Detection. GARY C.-W. SHYI & KIT-MAN YUEN, National Chung Cheng University—A large number of studies now show that people are surprisingly poor at detecting significant changes in visually presented scenes. In the present study, we conducted three experiments to investigate the relationship between orienting attention and change detection. In Experiments 1 and 2 we demonstrated, respectively, that exogenous and endogenous orienting can influence allocation of attention, which in turn can affect participants’ performances on change detection. In Experiments 3, we compared and contrasted the relative dominance of exogenous versus endogenous orienting as a function of time course. The results showed that when both exogenously and endogenously attended items were provided, the timing of onset appearance was critical to determine whether exogenous or endogenous control would be activated, which in turn may enhance the performance of detecting changes. The implications of our findings for the role of attention in change blindness and change detection are discussed. 11:00–11:15 (184) Masking and Decay in the Attentional Blink for Detection and Orientation Discrimination. BRADLEY J. WOLFGANG & PHILIP L. SMITH, University of Melbourne—The attentional blink (AB) is a transient perceptual deficit that is characterized by a reduction in second target performance following the correct identification of a first target presented between 100 and 500 msec earlier. The AB is thought to occur when inattention combines with a limitation in the availability of second target information. Theoretically, such a limitation could be (1) induced by visual masking, or (2) occur through passive stimulus decay in unmasked displays. We investigated the masking requirements for the detection and discrimination of two near-threshold Gabor patch stimuli. For detection, the AB was only found when stimuli were backward masked; for discrimination, an AB was obtained in both masked and unmasked displays. The decay-based AB for discrimination was additionally shown to depend on spatial frequency. We suggest that the attentional demands of perceptual processing interact with masking and stimulus decay to determine the strength of stimulus representations in visual short-term memory. 11:20–11:35 (185) Whole Versus Partial Report: The Attentional Blink in RSVP Sentences. MARY C. POTTER, MIT, MARK NIEUWENSTEIN, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, & NINA STROHMINGER, University of Michigan—An attentional blink in RSVP target search (errors in reporting T2 at an SOA of about 200 msec) disappears when all items must be reported; instead, performance drops over serial position (Nieuwenstein & Potter, 2006). In contrast to unrelated items such as letters, words that form a sentence are easily remembered in RSVP (Potter, 1984). Would target words in sentences escape an attentional blink? Subjects either reported two red words (T1 and T2) or the whole sentence. There was a blink for T2 at an SOA of 187 msec in partial report, but in whole report T1 and T2 were easily remembered. When the sentence was scrambled, whole report dropped but partial report was unaffected: T1 was now better in partial than in whole report, but T2 (at an SOA of 187 msec) was again worse. The attentional blink is not due to memory processing of T1, but to selection. 11:40–11:55 (186) Hold Everything! Hand Position Alters Vision. RICHARD A. ABRAMS, FENG DU, CHRISTOPHER C. DAVOLI, & WILLIAM H. KNAPP, Washington University—The present study explored the manner in which hand position may affect visual perception. We studied three classic visual attention tasks (visual search, inhibition of return, and attentional blink) during which the participants held their hands either near the stimulus display, or far from the display. Remarkably, the hands altered visual processing: People shifted their at-

Papers 187–193 Saturday Morning tention between items more slowly when their hands were near the display. The same results were observed for both visible and invisible hands. This enhancement in vision for objects near the hands reveals a mechanism that could facilitate the detailed evaluation of objects for potential manipulation, or the assessment of potentially dangerous objects for a defensive response. False Memory Seaview, Saturday Morning, 10:00–12:00 Chaired by Ken McRae, University of Western Ontario 10:00–10:15 (187) Schema-Based Gist and Backward Associations in False Memories. DAVID R. CANN, KEN MCRAE & ALBERT N. KATZ, University of Western Ontario (read by Ken McRae)—We used recall and recognition tasks to examine predictions of two major theories of false memories. We manipulated backward associative strength, a central factor in activation monitoring theory, and gist strength, a central factor in fuzzy trace theory. We used common word lists from Deese/Roediger–McDermott (DRM) studies, and developed a new type of list (Gist lists). In recognition, the Gist lists, which are strong in terms of schema-based gist but low on backward associative strength, produced a similar proportion of false memories as DRM lists which had slightly lower gist strength, but far greater backward associative strength. Furthermore, DRM lists equated with the Gist lists on backward associative strength but differing on gist strength produced a lower proportion of false recognition memories. Due to differences in source monitoring, the recall results were more consistent with activation monitoring theory. Thus, this research provides evidence for aspects of both theories. 10:20–10:35 (188) Gist Effects of Emotion on True and False Memory. CHARLES J. BRAINERD, Cornell University, L. M. STEIN, PUCRS, MARK L. HOWE, Lancaster University, VALERIE F. REYNA, Cornell University, & G. ROHENKOHL & R. FAVARIN DO SANTOS, PUCRS—We report a series of experiments that dealt with how the emotional content of study materials influences true and false memory. The object was to determine, using conjoint-recognition methodology, whether emotion effects were due to variations in the quality of verbatim or gist memories. The basic procedure was the same in all experiments: Subjects studied “emotional” DRM lists (i.e., list words varied in emotional valence and/or arousal). The major effect was that negative valence substantially elevated false memory and that it moderately elevated true memory. When the data were analyzed with the conjointrecognition model, negative valence appeared to be a pure gist effect: The meaning-similarity parameters for false memory and true memory were both elevated. In addition to pinpointing gist memory as the locus of the valence effect, the model analyses ruled out some other hypotheses (e.g., that negative emotion increases false memory by disrupting true memories). 10:40–10:55 (189) Source Judgments for True and False Memories Are Based on Memory Construction Rather Than Memory Strength. IRA E. HYMAN, REID A. NELSON, & CHRISTOPHER L. SHARP, Western Washington University—We investigated possible distinctions between true and false memories. Using DRM word lists, we presented participants sets of words presented in six colors. Each set contained items related and unrelated to a critical lure. On a recognition test, participants labeled a target old or new, rated their confidence, and were asked to state the color in which it had been presented. Contrary to a Single Strength Signal Detection Theory prediction, the percentage of words for which a color was claimed did not vary as a function of confidence. Instead claims of source information were based on the ability to construct source information, lending some support for Source Monitoring theory. 29 11:00–11:15 (190) The Role of Metacognitive Strategies in the Rejection of False Memories. DAVIDE BRUNO & PHILIP A. HIGHAM, University of Southampton, & TIMOTHY J. PERFECT, University of Plymouth (sponsored by Timothy J. Perfect)—The production and rejection of false memories has been a topic of interest for a long time in the field of recognition memory. In particular, many studies have focused on how memorability influences acceptance and rejection of false alarms. If an item is very memorable (e.g., one’s own name) and such item is employed as a distractor at test, participants will tend to reject it very confidently. The present research is concerned with evaluating the metacognitive strategies participants employ to reject distractors. Experiments are presented where memorability is manipulated as a function of item strength (repetition) and of type of item (regular words, regular nonwords, and irregular nonwords). Also, participants’ confidence in accepting or rejecting distractors is assessed with the aid of type-2 signal detection interpretations 11:20–11:35 (191) Generalizing the fSAM Model: Simulation of Core False Recall Effects. DANIEL R. KIMBALL & TROY A. SMITH, University of Texas, Arlington—Kimball, Smith, and Kahana (Psychological Review, in press) demonstrated that the fSAM model—the first fully specified quantitative model of false recall—could simulate several core findings involving false recall of DRM lists, including developmental patterns, specific list effects, association strength effects, and true–false correlations. Participants frequently intrude an unstudied critical word while recalling a DRM list comprising its strongest semantic associates. The fSAM model assumes that, during encoding, an unstudied word’s associations to list context strengthens in proportion to its joint strength of semantic association to all co-rehearsed words. During retrieval, words receive preference in proportion to their joint strength of semantic association to recently recalled words. We now report results showing that the fSAM model generalizes to other DRM false recall effects, including the effects of repeated study, repeated testing, repeated study–test trials, presentation rate, levels of processing, critical word presentation, and blocked versus random presentation of multiple lists. Adjustments in Speech Recognition Shoreline, Saturday Morning, 10:20–12:00 Chaired by Lynne C. Nygaard, Emory University 10:20–10:35 (192) Perceptual Adaptation to Systematic Variation in Speech. JESSICA E. DUKE & LYNNE C. NYGAARD, Emory University, & JENNIFER S. QUEEN, Rollins College (read by Lynne C. Nygaard)—The present study investigated the time course of perceptual learning of talker- and accent-specific properties of spoken language. Adult native English speakers were exposed to Spanish-accented English for either 1 or 3 days of training and were tested both on the following day and six weeks after initial training. During training, listeners transcribed words and received feedback on their performance. At test, listeners transcribed novel words spoken by familiar and unfamiliar accented talkers. Overall, listeners trained on Spanish-accented speech for 1 or 3 days of training performed better than untrained controls. However, the benefits of additional training on performance and long-term retention may depend on the amount of variability encountered during training. The results suggest that exposure to accented speech allows listeners to perceptually adapt to systematic variation in the speech signal and to generalize accent-specific learning to novel words produced by both familiar and unfamiliar talkers. 10:40–10:55 (193) The Effect of Experience on the Perception of Dialectal Variation. MEGHAN SUMNER & ARTHUR G. SAMUEL, Stony Brook University—Dialectal variation complicates the task of recognizing spo-

Papers 187–193 Saturday Morning<br />

tention between items more slowly when their hands were near the display.<br />

<strong>The</strong> same results were observed for both visible and invisible<br />

hands. This enhancement in vision for objects near the hands reveals<br />

a mechanism that could facilitate the detailed evaluation of objects for<br />

potential manipulation, or the assessment of potentially dangerous objects<br />

for a defensive response.<br />

False Memory<br />

Seaview, Saturday Morning, 10:00–12:00<br />

Chaired by Ken McRae, University of Western Ontario<br />

10:00–10:15 (187)<br />

Schema-Based Gist and Backward Associations in False Memories.<br />

DAVID R. CANN, KEN MCRAE & ALBERT N. KATZ, University<br />

of Western Ontario (read by Ken McRae)—We used recall and recognition<br />

tasks to examine predictions of two major theories of false<br />

memories. We manipulated backward associative strength, a central<br />

factor in activation monitoring theory, and gist strength, a central factor<br />

in fuzzy trace theory. We used common word lists from<br />

Deese/Roediger–McDermott (DRM) studies, and developed a new<br />

type of list (Gist lists). In recognition, the Gist lists, which are strong<br />

in terms of schema-based gist but low on backward associative<br />

strength, produced a similar proportion of false memories as DRM<br />

lists which had slightly lower gist strength, but far greater backward<br />

associative strength. Furthermore, DRM lists equated with the Gist<br />

lists on backward associative strength but differing on gist strength<br />

produced a lower proportion of false recognition memories. Due to<br />

differences in source monitoring, the recall results were more consistent<br />

with activation monitoring theory. Thus, this research provides<br />

evidence for aspects of both theories.<br />

10:20–10:35 (188)<br />

Gist Effects of Emotion on True and False Memory. CHARLES J.<br />

BRAINERD, Cornell University, L. M. STEIN, PUCRS, MARK L.<br />

HOWE, Lancaster University, VALERIE F. REYNA, Cornell University,<br />

& G. ROHENKOHL & R. FAVARIN DO SANTOS, PUCRS—We<br />

report a series of experiments that dealt with how the emotional content<br />

of study materials influences true and false memory. <strong>The</strong> object<br />

was to determine, using conjoint-recognition methodology, whether<br />

emotion effects were due to variations in the quality of verbatim or<br />

gist memories. <strong>The</strong> basic procedure was the same in all experiments:<br />

Subjects studied “emotional” DRM lists (i.e., list words varied in<br />

emotional valence and/or arousal). <strong>The</strong> major effect was that negative<br />

valence substantially elevated false memory and that it moderately elevated<br />

true memory. When the data were analyzed with the conjointrecognition<br />

model, negative valence appeared to be a pure gist effect:<br />

<strong>The</strong> meaning-similarity parameters for false memory and true memory<br />

were both elevated. In addition to pinpointing gist memory as the<br />

locus of the valence effect, the model analyses ruled out some other<br />

hypotheses (e.g., that negative emotion increases false memory by disrupting<br />

true memories).<br />

10:40–10:55 (189)<br />

Source Judgments for True and False Memories Are Based on Memory<br />

Construction Rather Than Memory Strength. IRA E. HYMAN,<br />

REID A. NELSON, & CHRISTOPHER L. SHARP, Western Washington<br />

University—We investigated possible distinctions between true<br />

and false memories. Using DRM word lists, we presented participants<br />

sets of words presented in six colors. Each set contained items related<br />

and unrelated to a critical lure. On a recognition test, participants labeled<br />

a target old or new, rated their confidence, and were asked to<br />

state the color in which it had been presented. Contrary to a Single<br />

Strength Signal Detection <strong>The</strong>ory prediction, the percentage of words<br />

for which a color was claimed did not vary as a function of confidence.<br />

Instead claims of source information were based on the ability<br />

to construct source information, lending some support for Source<br />

Monitoring theory.<br />

29<br />

11:00–11:15 (190)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Role of Metacognitive Strategies in the Rejection of False<br />

Memories. DAVIDE BRUNO & PHILIP A. HIGHAM, University of<br />

Southampton, & TIMOTHY J. PERFECT, University of Plymouth<br />

(sponsored by Timothy J. Perfect)—<strong>The</strong> production and rejection of<br />

false memories has been a topic of interest for a long time in the field<br />

of recognition memory. In particular, many studies have focused on<br />

how memorability influences acceptance and rejection of false<br />

alarms. If an item is very memorable (e.g., one’s own name) and such<br />

item is employed as a distractor at test, participants will tend to reject<br />

it very confidently. <strong>The</strong> present research is concerned with evaluating<br />

the metacognitive strategies participants employ to reject distractors.<br />

Experiments are presented where memorability is manipulated as a<br />

function of item strength (repetition) and of type of item (regular<br />

words, regular nonwords, and irregular nonwords). Also, participants’<br />

confidence in accepting or rejecting distractors is assessed with the<br />

aid of type-2 signal detection interpretations<br />

11:20–11:35 (191)<br />

Generalizing the fSAM Model: Simulation of Core False Recall<br />

Effects. DANIEL R. KIMBALL & TROY A. SMITH, University of<br />

Texas, Arlington—Kimball, Smith, and Kahana (Psychological Review,<br />

in press) demonstrated that the fSAM model—the first fully<br />

specified quantitative model of false recall—could simulate several<br />

core findings involving false recall of DRM lists, including developmental<br />

patterns, specific list effects, association strength effects, and<br />

true–false correlations. Participants frequently intrude an unstudied<br />

critical word while recalling a DRM list comprising its strongest semantic<br />

associates. <strong>The</strong> fSAM model assumes that, during encoding,<br />

an unstudied word’s associations to list context strengthens in proportion<br />

to its joint strength of semantic association to all co-rehearsed<br />

words. During retrieval, words receive preference in proportion to<br />

their joint strength of semantic association to recently recalled words.<br />

We now report results showing that the fSAM model generalizes to<br />

other DRM false recall effects, including the effects of repeated study,<br />

repeated testing, repeated study–test trials, presentation rate, levels of<br />

processing, critical word presentation, and blocked versus random<br />

presentation of multiple lists.<br />

Adjustments in Speech Recognition<br />

Shoreline, Saturday Morning, 10:20–12:00<br />

Chaired by Lynne C. Nygaard, Emory University<br />

10:20–10:35 (192)<br />

Perceptual Adaptation to Systematic Variation in Speech. JESSICA<br />

E. DUKE & LYNNE C. NYGAARD, Emory University, & JENNIFER<br />

S. QUEEN, Rollins College (read by Lynne C. Nygaard)—<strong>The</strong> present<br />

study investigated the time course of perceptual learning of talker- and<br />

accent-specific properties of spoken language. Adult native English<br />

speakers were exposed to Spanish-accented English for either 1 or 3<br />

days of training and were tested both on the following day and six<br />

weeks after initial training. During training, listeners transcribed<br />

words and received feedback on their performance. At test, listeners<br />

transcribed novel words spoken by familiar and unfamiliar accented<br />

talkers. Overall, listeners trained on Spanish-accented speech for 1 or<br />

3 days of training performed better than untrained controls. However,<br />

the benefits of additional training on performance and long-term retention<br />

may depend on the amount of variability encountered during<br />

training. <strong>The</strong> results suggest that exposure to accented speech allows<br />

listeners to perceptually adapt to systematic variation in the speech<br />

signal and to generalize accent-specific learning to novel words produced<br />

by both familiar and unfamiliar talkers.<br />

10:40–10:55 (193)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Effect of Experience on the Perception of Dialectal Variation.<br />

MEGHAN SUMNER & ARTHUR G. SAMUEL, Stony Brook University—Dialectal<br />

variation complicates the task of recognizing spo-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!