29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Saturday Morning Papers 1<strong>65</strong>–171<br />

University of Virginia—Traditionally, researchers in perception and<br />

cognition have overlooked the fact that brains exist within bodies. Recent<br />

research, however, suggests that the body helps to define how<br />

people see and understand the outside world. <strong>The</strong> goal of this symposium<br />

is to provide an integrated overview of state of the art research<br />

on embodied perception and cognition. <strong>The</strong> speakers represent a diversity<br />

of approaches and techniques directed at the study of how having<br />

a body and moving that body changes the way people perceive,<br />

represent, and understand their physical and social environments. <strong>The</strong><br />

central topics in this symposium include perception–action coupling,<br />

spatial perception, attention, motion perception, action understanding,<br />

and representational momentum. <strong>The</strong> presentations will be designed<br />

for a wide audience, and thus, will be of interest to experts and nonexperts<br />

alike.<br />

10:00–10:15 (1<strong>65</strong>)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Perception of Action-Scaled Affordances. BRETT FAJEN,<br />

Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute—What kinds of theoretical ideas are<br />

needed to do justice to the inextricable link between perception and<br />

action that is characteristic of so many routine and skilled behaviors?<br />

I will argue that the tight linkage between perception and action is<br />

neatly captured by the theory of affordances, according to which<br />

movements are guided by perceiving possibilities for action. My focus<br />

will be on action-scaled affordances—that is, possibilities for action<br />

that are constrained by limitations on actors’ ability to move, such as<br />

how fast one can run, how sharply one can turn, and how hard one can<br />

decelerate. To perceive action-scaled affordances is to perceive the<br />

world in units that are directly related to one’s action capabilities. I<br />

will show how the perception of action-scaled affordances plays a fundamental<br />

role in performing tasks such as steering, braking, and intercepting,<br />

for which information in optic flow and movement are<br />

tightly coupled.<br />

10:20–10:35 (166)<br />

How Motor Constraints and Experience Influence Perception.<br />

GÜNTHER KNOBLICH, Rutgers University, Newark—Do our action<br />

capabilities influence how we perceive our own actions and those of<br />

others? Cognitive psychologists and neuroscientists have long assumed<br />

that perception and action are clearly separated. Contrary to this supposition,<br />

I will defend the view that perception and action are closely<br />

linked. I will present two recent studies supporting this view. <strong>The</strong> first<br />

study demonstrates that Fitts’s law does not only constrain how we<br />

move ourselves but that it also constrains what we perceive as doable<br />

for others. <strong>The</strong> second study addressed the role of expertise in perception.<br />

It demonstrates that keyboard actions affect pitch perception<br />

in pianists but not in novices. Such results strongly support the assumption<br />

that the motor system plays a crucial role in perception.<br />

10:40–10:55 (167)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Bio-Energetic Scaling of Perceived Space. DENNIS PROF-<br />

FITT, University of Virginia—<strong>The</strong> visual perception of spatial layout<br />

relates people’s body and goals to the opportunities and bio-energetic<br />

costs of acting in the environment. Spatial perceptions are influenced,<br />

not only by optical and ocular-motor information, but also by people’s<br />

purposes and physiological state. For example, under constant viewing<br />

conditions, the apparent incline of hills increases when people are<br />

tired or encumbered by wearing a heavy backpack. Hills also appear<br />

steeper to people who are experiencing blood glucose depletion, are<br />

in poor physical condition, or are elderly and in declining health, compared<br />

with those who are young, healthy, and fit. Apparent distances<br />

are similarly influenced by the bio-energetic state of the body; reduced<br />

bio-energetic potential results in an increase in apparent distances.<br />

In essence, the perceptual system scales spatial layout to the<br />

bio-energetic costs of intended actions.<br />

11:00–11:15 (168)<br />

Influences of the Body on Spatial Attention. CATHERINE L. REED<br />

& JOHN P. GARZA, University of Denver—<strong>The</strong> study of spatial at-<br />

26<br />

tention traditionally focuses on how it is influenced by the location of<br />

objects within the visual environment. However, a primary function<br />

of spatial attention is to plan physical actions. When events occur in<br />

the world, we need to integrate visual information with our current<br />

body position to help us prepare effective responses to these events.<br />

Furthermore, our current actions can subsequently influence further<br />

deployments of attention. Thus, spatial attention must be considered<br />

within the context of the body. In this talk, we will present research<br />

demonstrating that one’s own body can influence spatial attention<br />

mechanisms: Hand position, function, and action are shown to be important<br />

influences on the prioritization of functional space near the<br />

body and the direction of attention. Together this work emphasizes a<br />

need for an embodied theory of spatial attention.<br />

11:20–11:35 (169)<br />

Seeing Yourself in Others. MAGGIE SHIFFRAR, Rutgers University,<br />

Newark—Effective social behavior requires the accurate and rapid<br />

perception of other people’s actions. When a nearby person moves toward<br />

you, your visual system analyzes that movement so that you can<br />

determine whether that person is reaching out to shake your hand, to<br />

fix your tie, or to punch you. Psychophysical studies of the perception<br />

of human movement indicate that observers use their own bodily<br />

experience to interpret the actions of other people. For example, observers<br />

demonstrate greater visual sensitivity to point-light depictions<br />

of their own actions than to the actions of other people. This sensitivity<br />

enhancement is independent of visual experience. Indeed, enhanced<br />

visual sensitivity to self-generated motion occurs even when<br />

the observer’s past actions appear on someone else’s body. Finally, a<br />

study of hand perception by individuals born without hands further<br />

supports the hypothesis that observers use internal representations of<br />

their own body to perceive movement.<br />

11:40–11:55 (170)<br />

Predicting Movements That Are Hard to Perform: Biomechanics<br />

and Representational Momentum. MARGARET WILSON &<br />

JESSY LANCASTER, University of California, Santa Cruz—Human<br />

bodies are perceived differently than other stimuli—specifically, biomechanical<br />

knowledge of one’s own body is recruited in perceiving<br />

others’ bodies (e.g., Kourtzi & Shiffrar, 1999). Wilson and Knoblich<br />

(2005) propose more specifically that motor knowledge is used for<br />

perceptual prediction, to cast forward into the immediate future and<br />

simulate probable outcomes of others’ movements. We support this<br />

claim using the phenomenon of representational momentum (RM), by<br />

showing that observers are sensitive to biomechanical constraints in<br />

generating these simulations. RM of human bodies is maximized for<br />

downward movements, and movements shown in the direction that<br />

they were originally filmed. In contrast, upward and backward movements<br />

exert a “drag” on the forward projection of RM. All the shown<br />

movements are biomechanically possible, but some are more natural<br />

and easy than others. This appears to affect observers’ ability to generate<br />

robust simulations of these movements.<br />

12:00–12:15 (171)<br />

Tool Use and Intention Affect Perceived Distance. JESSICA WITT,<br />

Purdue University—Near space is defined relative to the extent of<br />

arm’s reach, and thus, near space is scaled by reachability distance.<br />

Tool use extends reaching extent, and in so doing, it rescales spatial<br />

perceptions within near space. Compared to when no tool is held, objects<br />

that are beyond arm’s reach appear closer when people intend to<br />

reach with a tool that allows the objects to be touched. This is one of<br />

several examples demonstrating that behavioral potential affects perception.<br />

Little is known, however, about what underlying mechanisms<br />

might be responsible for such action-specific influences on perception.<br />

Experiments are presented that implicate motor simulation as a<br />

mediator of these effects. When people view a surface with an intention<br />

to act, people perform a motor simulation of the intended action,<br />

and thereby, scale perceived space in terms of the simulated action and<br />

its outcome.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!