29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Papers 144–150 Saturday Morning<br />

sults by the simultaneous use of task and temporal context representations<br />

to probe memory.<br />

8:40–8:55 (144)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Zero-Sum Trade-Off and Scallop Effects in Spacing Designs.<br />

PETER F. DELANEY, University of North Carolina, Greensboro, &<br />

PETER P. J. L. VERKOEIJEN, Erasmus University Rotterdam—<strong>The</strong><br />

present work explored two systematic within-list serial position effects<br />

that arise from including repeated presentations of items within<br />

a list: the zero-sum trade-off effect and the scallop effect. A zero-sum<br />

trade-off effect occurs when the height in the primacy region of the<br />

serial position function is inversely correlated with the height in the<br />

rest of the list. Unequal zero-sum trade-off effects across experimental<br />

conditions can be mistaken for other effects if primacy and recency<br />

regions are discarded from recall, or if ceiling effects occur in the primacy<br />

region. A scallop effect occurs when people use repeated long<br />

presentations to consolidate the preceding short presentations, as<br />

when several massed items follow several spaced items. <strong>The</strong> scallop<br />

effect results in some spaced items being better recalled than others,<br />

which can be confused with lag effects.<br />

9:00–9:15 (145)<br />

Exploring the Limits of Memory Access Control and Binding<br />

Mechanisms. MICHAEL S. HUMPHREYS, KRISTA L. MURRAY,<br />

& ANGELA M. MAGUIRE, University of Queensland—An analogue<br />

to the problem of remembering where you parked today, in the face of<br />

interference from prior parking memories, was instantiated by having<br />

participants learn a long list before learning a series of short lists.<br />

After each short list they received two cues. Both cues could refer to<br />

the short list (no-switch trials) or the first cue could refer to the long<br />

list and the second to the short list (switch trials). Each experiment<br />

made the task of focusing retrieval progressively more difficult. In the<br />

first experiment switch and no-switch trials occurred in blocks and in<br />

the second and third they were randomly intermixed. In addition, in<br />

the third experiment the same words were used in the long and short<br />

lists (an AB ABr paradigm, which requires a 3-way binding). In all<br />

three experiments participants were able to flexibly focus their retrieval<br />

efforts though it came with a cost.<br />

9:20–9:35 (146)<br />

Divided Attention and Repetition at Study: Effects on Group Recall.<br />

LUCIANE P. PEREIRA-PASARIN & SUPARNA RAJARAM,<br />

Stony Brook University (read by Suparna Rajaram)—Collaborative inhibition<br />

refers to the phenomenon that a group that works together<br />

performs worse than a nominal group where an equal number of individuals<br />

work alone and their nonredundant responses are pooled. We<br />

asked whether collaborative inhibition can be reduced under conditions<br />

where poorer or stronger individual memory changes the<br />

strength of individual organization. Participants studied categorized<br />

word lists and performed a free recall test in groups of 3 (collaborative)<br />

or individually (nominal). In Experiment 1, divided attention at<br />

study predictably reduced recall and weakened the individual subjective<br />

organization (ARC scores) of information. Critically, and as a result,<br />

collaborative inhibition also reduced following divided attention<br />

encoding. In Experiment 2, repetition at study predictably improved<br />

recall and strengthened individual subjective organization. Consequently,<br />

this opposite effect on organization also reduced collaborative<br />

inhibition. We discuss when collaboration can benefit or hurt individual<br />

performance and the role of individual organization in<br />

modulating this process.<br />

9:40–9:55 (147)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Ins and Outs of Memory: Written Versus Spoken Recall.<br />

RONALD T. KELLOGG, St. Louis University—Explicit memory is<br />

known to depend on input modality, with recent events showing a<br />

strong advantage when presented aurally rather than visually. <strong>The</strong> effects<br />

of output modality are less clear, but a writing deficit is plausible.<br />

In contrast to speaking, writing uses both phonological and or-<br />

23<br />

thographic representations that must be maintained in working memory<br />

for relatively long intervals due to slow motor output. It is also<br />

less practiced and automatic compared with speaking. Because writing<br />

makes greater demands on working memory storage and executive<br />

attention, it may reduce retrieval effort as a consequence. <strong>The</strong> evidence<br />

from word list recall is mixed, with some data even supporting<br />

the opposite conclusion of a writing superiority effect. However, recalling<br />

texts, complex visual events, autobiographical events, and category<br />

exemplars all show a writing deficit. <strong>The</strong>se tests all require a<br />

significant degree of retrieval effort that writing appears to disrupt.<br />

Cognitive Control<br />

Beacon B, Saturday Morning, 8:00–9:40<br />

Chaired by Keith A. Hutchison, Montana State University<br />

8:00–8:15 (148)<br />

Attentional Control, Relatedness Proportion, and Target Degradation<br />

Effects on Semantic Priming. KEITH A. HUTCHISON, Montana<br />

State University—Past research has demonstrated that target degradation<br />

effects (degradation � priming interactions) only occur when<br />

participants receive a stimulus list with many related items (i.e., a relatedness<br />

proportion, RP, of .50 or more). In this study, participants<br />

completed both an attentional control battery (Ospan, Antisaccade,<br />

and Stroop tasks) and a semantic priming task in which target words<br />

were presented either clearly or visually degraded and RP was manipulated<br />

within participants, within blocks using prime color to indicate<br />

the probability (.78 or .22) a to-be-named target would be related.<br />

Replicating Hutchison (2007), significant RP effects occurred<br />

and linearly increased with attentional control, indicating participants’<br />

flexible use of an effortful conscious expectancy process. However,<br />

overall target degradation effects occurred that were not dependent<br />

upon conditional RP or levels of attentional control, suggesting<br />

that degradation effects are not under participants’ conscious control<br />

and are a function of overall, rather than conditional, RP<br />

8:20–8:35 (149)<br />

Cognitive Control, Task Goals, and Memory. FREDERICK VER-<br />

BRUGGEN, Ghent University and Vanderbilt University, & GOR-<br />

DON D. LOGAN, Vanderbilt University—Cognitive control theories<br />

attribute control to executive processes that adjust and control behavior<br />

online. <strong>The</strong>ories of automaticity attribute control of skilled behavior<br />

to memory retrieval (MR). We contrasted online adjustments<br />

(OA) with MR, elucidating their roles in controlling performance in<br />

the stop-signal paradigm. We found evidence of short-term OA after<br />

unsuccessful stopping. In addition, we found that MR can slow responses<br />

for 1–10 trials after successful inhibition, suggesting the automatic<br />

retrieval of task goals. Based on these findings, we concluded<br />

that cognitive control can rely on both MR and OA.<br />

8:40–8:55 (150)<br />

Postconflict Slowing Depends on Relative Frequency of Conflict.<br />

WIM NOTEBAERT, WIM GEVERS, WIM FIAS, & TOM VERGUTS,<br />

Ghent University—Cognitive control processes impose behavioral<br />

adaptations after the detection of conflict (e.g., incongruent Stroop<br />

stimuli) and errors. <strong>The</strong> literature reveals that adaptation after conflict<br />

differs from adaptation after errors; after an error, reaction times increase,<br />

while this is not observed after conflict trials. An important<br />

difference between errors and conflict trials is the frequency of their<br />

occurrence: Whereas conflict typically occurs on 50% of the trials, errors<br />

occur with a much lower frequency. In this study, we investigated<br />

whether the frequency of conflict trials determines the occurrence of<br />

postconflict slowing. When conflict occurred on 25% of the trials,<br />

postconflict slowing was observed, but when conflict occurred on<br />

75% of the trials, slowing was observed after nonconflict (congruent)<br />

trials. This suggests that slowing is not triggered by conflict (or error),<br />

but rather by the detection of unexpected events. <strong>The</strong> results are discussed<br />

in terms of conflict monitoring theory.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!