S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

psychonomic.org
from psychonomic.org More from this publisher
29.01.2013 Views

Friday Afternoon Papers 99–104 Observers overestimate the slopes of hills with verbal measures, but are more accurate with body-based motor matching techniques, an example of contrast between cognitive and sensorimotor visual information. We replicated this effect using arm posture measured with digital photography rather than a tilt board for the motor measure, eliminating body contact with the hardware. Judged slopes are too steep at long distances, measured by having observers estimate segments of slopes between themselves and markers; at 16 m an 11º hill is verbally estimated at nearly 30º; the motor estimate is much lower. At 1 m, however, estimates are more accurate with both measures. What happens when observers traverse the slope before estimating it, giving them short-distance, presumably more accurate perceptual information at every point on the slope? Their overestimates are just as great as those of observers who did not traverse the slope. Appearance dominates knowledge of the terrain. 3:10–3:25 (99) The Perception of Four-Dot Configurations. MARY C. PORTILLO, CARL HAMMARSTEN, SHAIYAN KESHVARI, STEPHEN W. JEWELL, & JAMES R. POMERANTZ, Rice University (read by James R. Pomerantz)—Perceivers see stars in the night sky configured into dozens of nameable constellations. With simpler stimuli, twopoint configurations are organized into a straight line, and three points into a triangle. How do we perceive configurations of four points, as when four coins are tossed randomly on the floor: as quadrilaterals, as straight lines, curves, Y patterns, Ls, Ts, or yet others? We presented 328 patterns that systematically sampled the space of all possible 4-dot arrangements (ignoring size scale, orientation, and reflections) for subjects to free-classify based on perceived similarity. We then cluster-analyzed their responses. The structure of their classifications was captured by a hierarchy of 14 clusters of patterns. The first bifurcation occurred between patterns having 3 dots in a straight line versus those that did not. Further branches suggest an ordered set of rules for grouping dot patterns, including grouping by proximity, linearity, parallelism, and symmetry. 3:30–3:45 (100) Spationumerical Associations Between Perception and Semantics. PETER KRAMER, IVILIN STOIANOV, CARLO UMILTÀ, & MARCO ZORZI, University of Padua (sponsored by Johannes C. Ziegler)—Stoianov, Kramer, Umiltà, and Zorzi (Cognition, in press) found an interaction, between visuospatial and numerical information, that is independent of response selection effects (e.g., the SNARC effect). This Spatial-Numerical Association between Perception and Semantics (SNAPS) emerges when a spatial prime follows (backward priming), but not when it precedes (forward priming), a numerical target. Here, we investigate the time course and nature of the SNAPS effect. We used nonspatial, verbal parity judgments and number comparisons and, to dissociate the SNAPS effect from other numerical effects, we compared conditions with and without priming. The results show that the SNAPS effect is inhibitory and peaks when the prime follows the target by about 100 msec. Moreover, we observed a main effect of number size even in the parity judgment task, contrary to earlier claims. This latter finding has important implications for current models of the representation of numerical magnitude. Selective Attention Regency ABC, Friday Afternoon, 4:10–5:30 Chaired by Zhe Chen, University of Canterbury 4:10–4:25 (101) Implicit Perception in Object Substitution Masking. ZHE CHEN, University of Canterbury, & ANNE TREISMAN, Princeton University—Object substitution masking (OSM; Enns & Di Lollo, 1997) refers to reduced target discrimination when the target is surrounded by a sparse mask that does not overlap with the target in space but 16 trails it in time. In two experiments, we used a novel approach to investigate the extent of processing of a masked target in OSM. We measured response compatibility effects between target and mask, both when the offsets were simultaneous and when the mask offset was delayed. Participants made a speeded response to the mask followed by an accuracy only response to the target, then categorizing their responses to the target as “see” or “guess.” Targets and masks matched or differed at a feature level in Experiment 1 and at a categorical level in Experiment 2. Evidence for OSM as well as a dissociation between perception and awareness was found in both experiments. 4:30–4:45 (102) Intertrial Biasing of Selective Attention Leads to Blink-Like Misses in RSVP Streams. ALEJANDRO LLERAS & BRIAN LEVINTHAL, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign—When participants are required to report the case of the color-oddball letter in a single-target RSVP stream, their ability to do so is modulated by the position of the target in the RSVP stream, and more crucially, by the match or mismatch between the current target color and the color of distractors in the prior RSVP stream. When the target is presented in the color of the distractors in the previous trial RSVP stream, participants very often miss the target (performance is at chance) when the target is presented early on in the RSVP stream. Performance recovers for later target positions in the RSVP stream. The pattern of performance (cost and recovery) is strongly reminiscent of the attentional blink, even though only one target is to be detected on any given trial and more than 2 sec have elapsed since the end of the previous trial. 4:50–5:05 (103) The Time Course of Goal-Driven Saccadic Selection. WIESKE VAN ZOEST & MIEKE DONK, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam (sponsored by Mieke Donk)—Four experiments were performed to investigate goal-driven modulation in saccadic target selection as a function of time. Observers were presented with displays containing multiple homogeneously oriented background lines and two singletons. Observers were instructed to make a speeded eye-movement to one singleton in one condition and the other singleton in another condition. Simultaneously presented singletons were defined in different dimensions (orientation and color in Experiment 1), or in the same dimension (i.e., orientation in Experiment 2, color in Experiments 3 and 4). The results showed that goal-driven selectivity increased as a function of saccade latency and depended on the specific singleton combination. Yet, selectivity was not a function of whether both singletons were defined within or across dimensions. Instead, the rate of goal-driven selectivity was related to the similarity between the singletons; when singletons were dissimilar, accuracy as a function of time increased more rapidly than when they were similar. 5:10–5:25 (104) Tracking of Visual Objects Containing Textual Information. LAURI OKSAMA & JUKKA HYÖNÄ, University of Turku (sponsored by Jukka Hyönä)—Do properties of textual identity information associated with moving targets influence visual tracking? In realworld visual environments, such as air traffic displays, targets to be tracked contain textual information (e.g., call signs). In the present study, the textual information appeared within rectangles that moved around the computer screen. Four factors were manipulated in the experiments: (1) number of targets (2–6), (2) length of textual information (5 vs. 10 character words), (3) familiarity of textual information (existing words vs. pronounceable pseudowords), and (4) speed of object movement. We observed that performance accuracy decreased as a function of target set-size, text length, word unfamiliarity, and target speed. We argue that the results are consistent with the recently proposed serial model of dynamic identity–location binding (MOMIT), which states that identity–location bindings for multiple moving objects become more difficult when target identification consumes more time (e.g., as text length increases).

Papers 105–112 Friday Afternoon Applications of Source Monitoring Regency DEFH, Friday Afternoon, 3:50–5:30 Chaired by Linda A. Henkel, Fairfield University 3:50–4:05 (105) Repetition and Source Memory in an Eyewitness Context. CALEB E. NANNES & KRISTI S. MULTHAUP, Davidson College (read by Kristi S. Multhaup)—We will review the literature on source memory in eyewitness contexts, focusing on the effect of misinformation repetition. Zaragoza and Mitchell (1996) reported the intriguing finding that misinformation repetition increased source memory errors while simultaneously increasing correct responses (i.e., identifying questiononly items as from a video and as from questions, respectively). We extended this work into the traditional misinformation paradigm in which misinformation was presented in narratives rather than in questions. Misinformation was presented in 0 (control), 1, or all 3 narratives. Once-presented misinformation increased reports of seeing the detail in both the narratives (correct) and the video (errors) compared with control items. Repetition effects remained for the correct source (saying the narrative-only items came from the narratives), but not for the source errors (saying narrative-only items came from the video). We will discuss how procedural factors affect the likelihood of repetition increasing errors, including the implications for everyday memory. 4:10–4:25 (106) How Photographs Distort Our Memories: Source Confusions After Seeing Pictures of Imagined Events. LINDA A. HENKEL, Fairfield University—Viewing photographs can lead people to misremember details about events and even falsely remember events that never occurred. Two experiments investigated whether photographs increase people’s false claims of performing actions they did not actually do. Participants performed and imagined performing actions that could leave behind evidence the action had been completed (e.g., tear the card in half, crumple the paper). Later they were exposed to photographs 0, 1, or 3 times showing some of the imagined actions in a state of completion, and later had to indicate which actions they had actually performed. The presentation of photographs was found to increase source errors: Participants were more likely to falsely claim to have performed actions that were in fact imagined when photographic evidence of the completed actions had been shown, and this was related to how many times the photo had been viewed. Findings are interpreted via the source monitoring framework. 4:30–4:45 (107) Interpersonal Source Monitoring in Simulated Investigative Interviews of Eyewitnesses. D. STEPHEN LINDSAY, C. A. ELIZABETH BRIMACOMBE, LEORA C. DAHL, & MELISSA A. BOYCE, University of Victoria, & JOHN TURTLE, Ryerson University—We monitor the sources of one another’s memory reports. Such interpersonal source monitoring is usually tacit/unconscious, and it is not very accurate, but people routinely make inferences as to the provenance and reliability of others’ reports. Our research group has been exploring such inferences in the context of simulated police investigations. In our procedure, undergraduates role-playing as police officers interview a witness, search a data base of potential suspects, and collect eyewitness identification evidence. Participant-investigators tend to put too much stock in participant-witnesses’ eyewitness identification judgments (unless viewing conditions lead to very high accuracy rates, in which case participant-investigators’ reliance on participantwitnesses is justified). Also, participant-investigators show only very limited sensitivity to participant-witnesses’ accuracy. 4:50–5:05 (108) Social and Emotional Aspects of Source Monitoring. NANCY FRANKLIN, SARAH J. BARBER, & SHANNA ALLEN, Stony Brook University—Three recent lines of research in our lab speak to social and emotional phenomena impacting source judgments. (1) We have 17 found that although self-relevance improves memory in standard paradigms, it also leads to predictable self-serving distortions. (2) We have extended a classic finding, that source discriminability improves source memory, to the realm of social intelligence. Namely, people with greater perspective-taking skills are worse at discriminating selfgenerated from other-generated items in memory. (3) Previous work shows that mood can produce both selective memory and judgment effects. Our work suggests that some of these effects may be attributable to source biases, and is consistent with the argument that angry people, operating on “hot cognition,” base their behavior on more heuristic processing, whereas sad people think more analytically. All of these findings will be discussed inside a larger theoretical framework for understanding source memory and the functions it serves. 5:10–5:25 (109) Are Age-Related Episodic Memory Deficits Caused by High- Confidence Misrecollections? CHAD S. DODSON, University of Virginia—I will review work showing that older adults frequently make high-confidence errors when recollecting details about past events. When matched with younger adults on overall accuracy, older adults more often make high-confidence errors on source identification (i.e., who-said-what) and cued-recall tests but not on old–new recognition and general knowledge tests. Moreover, these highconfidence errors cannot be explained by age-related differences in the use of the confidence rating scale. These high-confidence misrecollections, however, are consistent with a misrecollection account of cognitive aging. This account proposes that all age-related episodic memory deficits are caused—in part—by older adults’ propensity to experience compelling misrecollections. Animal Cognition Beacon A, Friday Afternoon, 3:50–5:30 Chaired by Robert G. Cook, Tufts University 3:50–4:05 (110) Temporal Control of Mental States in Pigeons. ROBERT G. COOK & HARA A. ROSEN, Tufts University—We have recently discovered a phenomenon in pigeons involving the temporal anticipation of complex rule changes or task switches in a single discrimination setting. In a two-alternative conditional discrimination task involving a single pair of stimuli, three pigeons had to discriminate among a matching rule applicable to the first half of a session and an oddity rule that was enforced over the second half of the session. All birds successfully learned this ordered discrimination and the results of four tests suggested an internal clock mediated their task switching behavior. Implications for the serial organization of behavior are considered. 4:10–4:25 (111) Evidence of Base-Rate Neglect by Pigeons in Matching-to-Sample. THOMAS R. ZENTALL, University of Kentucky, TRICIA S. CLEMENT, Stanford University, & KELLY A. DIGIAN, REBECCA A. SINGER, & HOLLY C. MILLER, University of Kentucky—In conditional discrimination learning (matching-to-sample) by pigeons, (comparison) choice is hypothesized to depend on (1) the conditional probability of reinforcement given a conditional (sample) stimulus and (2) the overall probability of reinforcement for comparison choice in the absence of a sample. We show that, independent of these two probabilities, sample frequency also affects comparison choice. The implication of this finding is that sample frequency is dissociated (stored separately) from conditional probability. Thus, in the absence of memory for the sample, pigeons are biased to choose comparisons based on overall sample frequency and not just on the probability of reinforcement associated with the two comparison stimuli. 4:30–4:45 (112) Dissociation of “Uncertainty” Responses and “Middle” Responses in Capuchin Monkeys. MICHAEL J. BERAN, Georgia State University,

Papers 105–112 Friday Afternoon<br />

Applications of Source Monitoring<br />

Regency DEFH, Friday Afternoon, 3:50–5:30<br />

Chaired by Linda A. Henkel, Fairfield University<br />

3:50–4:05 (105)<br />

Repetition and Source Memory in an Eyewitness Context. CALEB E.<br />

NANNES & KRISTI S. MULTHAUP, Davidson College (read by<br />

Kristi S. Multhaup)—We will review the literature on source memory<br />

in eyewitness contexts, focusing on the effect of misinformation repetition.<br />

Zaragoza and Mitchell (1996) reported the intriguing finding<br />

that misinformation repetition increased source memory errors while<br />

simultaneously increasing correct responses (i.e., identifying questiononly<br />

items as from a video and as from questions, respectively). We<br />

extended this work into the traditional misinformation paradigm in<br />

which misinformation was presented in narratives rather than in questions.<br />

Misinformation was presented in 0 (control), 1, or all 3 narratives.<br />

Once-presented misinformation increased reports of seeing the<br />

detail in both the narratives (correct) and the video (errors) compared<br />

with control items. Repetition effects remained for the correct source<br />

(saying the narrative-only items came from the narratives), but not for<br />

the source errors (saying narrative-only items came from the video).<br />

We will discuss how procedural factors affect the likelihood of repetition<br />

increasing errors, including the implications for everyday memory.<br />

4:10–4:25 (106)<br />

How Photographs Distort Our Memories: Source Confusions After<br />

Seeing Pictures of Imagined Events. LINDA A. HENKEL, Fairfield<br />

University—Viewing photographs can lead people to misremember<br />

details about events and even falsely remember events that never occurred.<br />

Two experiments investigated whether photographs increase<br />

people’s false claims of performing actions they did not actually do.<br />

Participants performed and imagined performing actions that could<br />

leave behind evidence the action had been completed (e.g., tear the<br />

card in half, crumple the paper). Later they were exposed to photographs<br />

0, 1, or 3 times showing some of the imagined actions in a<br />

state of completion, and later had to indicate which actions they had<br />

actually performed. <strong>The</strong> presentation of photographs was found to increase<br />

source errors: Participants were more likely to falsely claim to<br />

have performed actions that were in fact imagined when photographic<br />

evidence of the completed actions had been shown, and this was related<br />

to how many times the photo had been viewed. Findings are interpreted<br />

via the source monitoring framework.<br />

4:30–4:45 (107)<br />

Interpersonal Source Monitoring in Simulated Investigative Interviews<br />

of Eyewitnesses. D. STEPHEN LINDSAY, C. A. ELIZABETH<br />

BRIMACOMBE, LEORA C. DAHL, & MELISSA A. BOYCE, University<br />

of Victoria, & JOHN TURTLE, Ryerson University—We monitor<br />

the sources of one another’s memory reports. Such interpersonal<br />

source monitoring is usually tacit/unconscious, and it is not very accurate,<br />

but people routinely make inferences as to the provenance and<br />

reliability of others’ reports. Our research group has been exploring<br />

such inferences in the context of simulated police investigations. In<br />

our procedure, undergraduates role-playing as police officers interview<br />

a witness, search a data base of potential suspects, and collect<br />

eyewitness identification evidence. Participant-investigators tend to<br />

put too much stock in participant-witnesses’ eyewitness identification<br />

judgments (unless viewing conditions lead to very high accuracy<br />

rates, in which case participant-investigators’ reliance on participantwitnesses<br />

is justified). Also, participant-investigators show only very<br />

limited sensitivity to participant-witnesses’ accuracy.<br />

4:50–5:05 (108)<br />

Social and Emotional Aspects of Source Monitoring. NANCY<br />

FRANKLIN, SARAH J. BARBER, & SHANNA ALLEN, Stony Brook<br />

University—Three recent lines of research in our lab speak to social<br />

and emotional phenomena impacting source judgments. (1) We have<br />

17<br />

found that although self-relevance improves memory in standard paradigms,<br />

it also leads to predictable self-serving distortions. (2) We<br />

have extended a classic finding, that source discriminability improves<br />

source memory, to the realm of social intelligence. Namely, people<br />

with greater perspective-taking skills are worse at discriminating selfgenerated<br />

from other-generated items in memory. (3) Previous work<br />

shows that mood can produce both selective memory and judgment<br />

effects. Our work suggests that some of these effects may be attributable<br />

to source biases, and is consistent with the argument that angry<br />

people, operating on “hot cognition,” base their behavior on more<br />

heuristic processing, whereas sad people think more analytically. All<br />

of these findings will be discussed inside a larger theoretical framework<br />

for understanding source memory and the functions it serves.<br />

5:10–5:25 (109)<br />

Are Age-Related Episodic Memory Deficits Caused by High-<br />

Confidence Misrecollections? CHAD S. DODSON, University of<br />

Virginia—I will review work showing that older adults frequently<br />

make high-confidence errors when recollecting details about past<br />

events. When matched with younger adults on overall accuracy, older<br />

adults more often make high-confidence errors on source identification<br />

(i.e., who-said-what) and cued-recall tests but not on old–new<br />

recognition and general knowledge tests. Moreover, these highconfidence<br />

errors cannot be explained by age-related differences in<br />

the use of the confidence rating scale. <strong>The</strong>se high-confidence misrecollections,<br />

however, are consistent with a misrecollection account<br />

of cognitive aging. This account proposes that all age-related episodic<br />

memory deficits are caused—in part—by older adults’ propensity to<br />

experience compelling misrecollections.<br />

Animal Cognition<br />

Beacon A, Friday Afternoon, 3:50–5:30<br />

Chaired by Robert G. Cook, Tufts University<br />

3:50–4:05 (110)<br />

Temporal Control of Mental States in Pigeons. ROBERT G. COOK<br />

& HARA A. ROSEN, Tufts University—We have recently discovered<br />

a phenomenon in pigeons involving the temporal anticipation of complex<br />

rule changes or task switches in a single discrimination setting.<br />

In a two-alternative conditional discrimination task involving a single<br />

pair of stimuli, three pigeons had to discriminate among a matching<br />

rule applicable to the first half of a session and an oddity rule that was<br />

enforced over the second half of the session. All birds successfully<br />

learned this ordered discrimination and the results of four tests suggested<br />

an internal clock mediated their task switching behavior. Implications<br />

for the serial organization of behavior are considered.<br />

4:10–4:25 (111)<br />

Evidence of Base-Rate Neglect by Pigeons in Matching-to-Sample.<br />

THOMAS R. ZENTALL, University of Kentucky, TRICIA S.<br />

CLEMENT, Stanford University, & KELLY A. DIGIAN, REBECCA A.<br />

SINGER, & HOLLY C. MILLER, University of Kentucky—In conditional<br />

discrimination learning (matching-to-sample) by pigeons,<br />

(comparison) choice is hypothesized to depend on (1) the conditional<br />

probability of reinforcement given a conditional (sample) stimulus<br />

and (2) the overall probability of reinforcement for comparison choice<br />

in the absence of a sample. We show that, independent of these two<br />

probabilities, sample frequency also affects comparison choice. <strong>The</strong><br />

implication of this finding is that sample frequency is dissociated<br />

(stored separately) from conditional probability. Thus, in the absence<br />

of memory for the sample, pigeons are biased to choose comparisons<br />

based on overall sample frequency and not just on the probability of<br />

reinforcement associated with the two comparison stimuli.<br />

4:30–4:45 (112)<br />

Dissociation of “Uncertainty” Responses and “Middle” Responses in<br />

Capuchin Monkeys. MICHAEL J. BERAN, Georgia State University,

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!