S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

psychonomic.org
from psychonomic.org More from this publisher
29.01.2013 Views

Friday Afternoon Papers 86–91 people often experience intrusive remindings, and make efforts to exclude the unwanted memory from awareness. Prior work has established that suppressing retrieval in this way recruits executive control regions in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) that downregulate activation in the hippocampus, impairing later retention. Here, we investigate the neurobiological mechanisms underlying the intrusion experience. In an adapted version of the “think/no think” procedure, subjects reported after each suppression trial whether the unwanted memory had intruded into awareness. The results show that although subjects initially report many intrusions (~50%), these intrusions decline with practice, with later memory impairment predicted by this decline. Moreover, intrusions were associated with decreased engagement of the DLPFC and elevated activity in the hippocampus. These findings suggest that intrusive memories reflect a failure to engage DLPFC during suppression, and that successful down-regulation of hippocampal activation is an essential component of memory control. 2:10–2:25 (86) Independent Cues Are Not Independent. GINO CAMP, DIANE PECHER, HENK G. SCHMIDT, & RENÉ ZEELENBERG, Erasmus University Rotterdam—Retrieval practice with particular items from memory (e.g., orange) can result in forgetting of related items (e.g., banana) that are associated to the same cue (e.g., FRUIT). This retrievalinduced forgetting effect can be explained by both interference and inhibitory processes. The independent cue technique has been developed to differentiate between the contributions of interference and inhibition to the forgetting effect. Independent cues are not associated to the practiced item (e.g., monkey–b___ for banana). Inhibition theory predicts forgetting with this type of cue. However, interference theory predicts forgetting only with the studied category (FRUIT) and does not predict forgetting with independent cues. Therefore, forgetting with independent cues is seen as an empirical criterion for inhibition. However, in three experiments, we demonstrate experimentally that, even when participants are only cued with independent cues, studied categories are activated at test. Thus, independent cues may not differentiate effectively between interference and inhibition. 2:30–2:45 (87) Long-Term Episodic Inhibition and Sleep. MIHALY RACSMANY, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, MARTIN A. CONWAY, University of Leeds, & GYULA DEMETER, Budapest University of Technology and Economics (sponsored by Martin A. Conway)—Studies of retrieval inhibition have persistently found that retrieval practice of selected category items from a previously studied list reduces recall of studied unpracticed items from the same category. Past research suggests that this effect is short-term. Two experiments investigated the long-term effect of retrieval practice and its relationship to sleep. In both experiments two groups of subjects undertook a selective practice paradigm with a 12-h delay between practice and recall. One group (sleepers) had a full night’s sleep during delay, while the other group (nonsleepers) recalled at the end of the day. Both groups showed highly reliable retrieval-induced forgetting. In the second experiment a surprise free recall procedure was used. A significant inhibitory effect for the sleepers was again found but there was no reliable effect for the nonsleepers. These findings are considered in terms of consolidation of the contents of episodic memories. Word Processing Seaview, Friday Afternoon, 1:30–3:30 Chaired by Julie E. Boland, University of Michigan 1:30–1:45 (88) Contextual Modulation of Dominant Homophone Meaning in the Visual World. LILLIAN CHEN & JULIE E. BOLAND, University of Michigan (read by Julie E. Boland)—In a visual world eye tracking study, participants heard homophones in either neutral or subordinate- 14 biased contexts (e.g., NEUTRAL, Amanda didn’t supervise her little brother, who ended up dropping the glasses; BIASED, The dishwashers washed the plates, the silverware, and finally the glasses). At homophone onset, four pictures appeared: subordinate homophone meaning (drinking glasses), dominate meaning shape competitor (handcuffs, for eyeglasses), and two unrelated pictures. The shape competitor was used to index subliminal activation of the dominant meaning. Context influenced the proportion of fixations on both the subordinate picture and the shape competitor, beginning with the second fixation after the pictures appeared. Nonetheless, the shape competitor attracted more fixations than expected by chance, even in the subordinate-biased condition. This pattern is similar to the subordinatebias effect reported in reading experiments (Binder & Rayner, 1998): Context modulates the activation of homophone meanings, but the dominate meaning remains partially activated in subordinate-biased contexts. 1:50–2:05 (89) Selection of Homograph Meaning: Implicit Memory Effects in Episodic Memory. DAVID S. GORFEIN, Adelphi University, VIN- CENT R. BROWN, Hofstra University, & EMILY EDWARDS, University of Texas, Arlington—Traditional research in meaning selection for ambiguous words (homographs/homophones) has focused on semantic tasks such as the selection of a meaning for the ambiguous word in a biasing context and for the same word presented later in a similar or different semantic context. We have previously demonstrated that an ambiguous word presented along with a picture consistent with its secondary meaning induces a bias which is maintained across relatively long time intervals, many intervening trials, changes in task context, and changes in response requirements. These findings are now extended to transfer from the initial secondary-meaning picture priming task to an episodic memory task, free recall. The data support the interpretation that the picture priming task alters the meaning representation of the words, thereby affecting the frequency and pattern of recall not only for the ambiguous words themselves, but for words related to the ambiguous words’ initially dominant and secondary meanings. 2:10–2:25 (90) Bricks Don’t Breathe: Semantic Decisions Are Made by Weighing Evidence For and Against Category Membership. DIANE PECHER & RENÉ ZEELENBERG, Erasmus University Rotterdam, & ERIC- JAN WAGENMAKERS, University of Amsterdam—In the animal decision task words are categorized as animals or nonanimals. Deadline models propose that decisions are based on activation of an animal feature in combination with a temporal deadline (Carreiras, Perea, & Grainger, 1997; Forster & Hector, 2002). Instead, we propose that the decision process weighs several features that provide partial evidence for either an animal or a nonanimal decision. If this weighing provides strong evidence for either response, performance is better than if the weighing provides only weak evidence for either response. In Experiment 1 performance was worse for words having features correlated with the opposite response (e.g., organic for a nonanimal) than for words not having such features. In Experiment 2 nonanimal responses were faster than animal responses if the proportion of nonanimals was high. These results are problematic for a deadline model but consistent with a model combining evidence from different features. 2:30–2:45 (91) An REM Model for Animal Decision. ERIC-JAN WAGENMAKERS, University of Amsterdam, & DIANE PECHER & RENÉ ZEELEN- BERG, Erasmus University Rotterdam—In animal decision, people have to quickly decide whether or not words such as “mountain” or “parrot” refer to an animal. This task requires that people access both orthographic and semantic information. Previous research has shown that (1) activation of semantic information occurs before orthographic processing is complete; (2) performance for typical animals (e.g., dog) and nonanimals (e.g., computer) is better than performance for

Papers 92–98 Friday Afternoon less typical animals (e.g., shrimp) and nonanimals (e.g., tree); (3) “nonanimal” decisions can be faster than “animal” decisions. Here, we introduce a REM model to account for these findings. In the model, orthographic and semantic information accumulate over time. Semantic information from a particular lexical representation is weighted by the posterior probability that the representation matches the stimulus orthographically. The present research supports the notion of parallel access to a feature-based semantic representation for composite concepts such as “animalness.” 2:50–3:05 (92) Semantic and Emotional Effects in Lexical Decision, Naming, and Perceptual Identification. LEE H. WURM & SEAN R. SEAMAN, Wayne State University—Previous research shows that the subjective danger and usefulness of words affect recognition times. Usually an interaction is found: Increasing danger predicts faster RTs for words low on usefulness, but increasing danger predicts slower RTs for words high on usefulness. We explore the nature of this interaction using three experimental tasks. The interaction is found in all tasks. It holds for nouns, verbs, and adjectives. It is significant over and above effects of variables such as frequency, length, concreteness, age of acquisition, imageability, and familiarity. The interaction cannot be characterized as a speed versus accuracy trade-off. We believe its origin is an approach/withdraw response conflict induced by stimuli that are both dangerous and useful. It may be a manifestation of the rapid evaluation effects pervasive in the literature. Post hoc analyses also show that danger and usefulness explain as much variance as Valence and Arousal, or Evaluation, Potency, and Activity. 3:10–3:25 (93) Anticipatory Effects in Delayed Naming: The Role of the Hazard Function Probability. MICHAEL J. CORTESE, University of Nebraska, Omaha—Two experiments examined anticipatory effects in delayed naming performance by varying the hazard function probability (HFP) of the response signal. In Experiment 1, critical stimuli were named at a 1,300-msec delay. The HFP equaled .33 in one condition and 1.0 in another condition. When the HFP equaled 1.0, naming latencies were 36 msec faster than when the HFP equaled .33. Experiment 2 compared naming latencies for three delays (900 msec, 1,100 msec, and 1,300 msec) that were distributed uniformly in one block of trials (i.e., the HFP increased systematically across the delay intervals) and exponentially in another block of trials (i.e., the HFP remained constant at .50 across the delay intervals). Naming latencies decreased along with delay interval only in the uniform distribution condition. Based on these results, researchers who use the delayed naming task should employ an exponential distribution schedule in order to guard against anticipatory effects. Spatial Cognition Shoreline, Friday Afternoon, 1:30–3:50 Chaired by Amy Lynne Shelton, Johns Hopkins University 1:30–1:45 (94) Place and Response Mechanisms in Human Environmental Learning. AMY LYNNE SHELTON, STEVEN A. MARCHETTE, & NAOHIDE YAMAMOTO, Johns Hopkins University—Place learning and response learning reflect two spatial learning systems. Less is known about the role these systems play in environmental learning in humans than in rats. In behavioral and neuroimaging experiments, we investigated whether these different learning systems could be observed using (1) task differences within individuals and (2) individual differences within tasks. For task differences, a comparison of environmental learning from ground-level and aerial perspectives revealed patterns of brain activation and subsequent memory performance that suggested distinct but overlapping learning systems. Focusing on individual differences within a given perspective, we found differences in the brain activation and the subsequent memory per- 15 formance as a function of gender and spatial skills. The differential patterns of brain activation were localized to regions typically associated with place and response learning. Taken together, we assert that humans, like other animals, have multiple systems that may be preferentially engaged across tasks and individuals. 1:50–2:05 (95) Spatial Memory: Categorical and Metric Encoding of Location in Complex Scenes. THOMAS F. SHIPLEY, MARK P. HOLDEN, LONGIN J. LATECKI, NORA S. NEWCOMBE, & SHANNON L. FITZHUGH, Temple University—How do we remember object locations? Intuitively, we remember that our keys are on a table, and roughly 5 inches to left of the center. A Bayesian combination of categorical and metric information offers an optimal memory strategy under uncertainty, and prior research (e.g., Huttenlocher, Hedges, & Duncan, 1991) supports the use of such a strategy for simple figures. We report the results of several studies that confirm the use of a combination of categorical and coordinate information to estimate location in complex scenes. Subjects’ errors in recall indicate they encoded the location of single objects categorically, as within a bounded region. However, rather than encoding the location relative to the center of the category, the metric encoding appears to locate objects within a region relative to the spine (the line of local symmetry) of the region. 2:10–2:25 (96) Toward an Embodied Account of Spatial Memory: Egocentric Experience Trumps Intrinsic Structure. DAVID WALLER, NATHAN GREENAUER, & CATHERINE MELLO, Miami University—Mou and McNamara’s (2002) theory of human spatial representation holds that memory is organized by intrinsic reference systems (those based on the spatial structure of a configuration of objects, and not on the surroundings or on the observer) and codes interobject spatial relationships. However, the degree to which perceptually salient intrinsic axes of a layout of objects are used to organize spatial memory is an open question. We show that salient intrinsic axes in a layout of objects are neither necessary nor sufficient for people to use a nonegocentric reference system in organizing spatial memory. Additional experiments demonstrated that when people used nonegocentric reference systems, the selection of a reference direction was influenced by their action possibilities during encoding. We suggest that what are often interpreted as nonegocentric reference systems may be better conceptualized as imagined egocentric reference systems because they are so closely tied to potential personal (embodied) experience. 2:30–2:45 (97) Do Humans Have an Internal Compass Like Other Mammals Do? M. JEANNE SHOLL, Boston College—A head-direction (HD) cell fires when the animal’s head points in the cell’s preferred allocentric direction, irrespective of the animal’s location in the environment. The cells have been studied extensively in rats, and although their role in navigation is not fully understood, they are undoubtedly the neural basis of a rat’s “sense of direction.” While there is neuropsychological and neuroimaging data consistent with a human allocentric-heading system, human HD cells have yet to be discovered. The present paper will describe some behavioral evidence from our laboratory showing that people can recover from a pictured scene, the allocentric direction their body faced in the actual environment when looking at the scene straight on. Perhaps revealingly, people differ widely in their ability to do this, and these differences correlate with self-reported sense of direction. The behavioral findings will be discussed in the context of theoretical models of HD-system function in rats. 2:50–3:05 (98) Longer Segments of Slopes Are Overestimated More Than Short Segments. BRUCE BRIDGEMAN, MERRIT HOOVER, ERIC M. CHIU, & JOSHUA R. QUAN, University of California, Santa Cruz—

Papers 92–98 Friday Afternoon<br />

less typical animals (e.g., shrimp) and nonanimals (e.g., tree);<br />

(3) “nonanimal” decisions can be faster than “animal” decisions.<br />

Here, we introduce a REM model to account for these findings. In the<br />

model, orthographic and semantic information accumulate over time.<br />

Semantic information from a particular lexical representation is<br />

weighted by the posterior probability that the representation matches<br />

the stimulus orthographically. <strong>The</strong> present research supports the notion<br />

of parallel access to a feature-based semantic representation for<br />

composite concepts such as “animalness.”<br />

2:50–3:05 (92)<br />

Semantic and Emotional Effects in Lexical Decision, Naming, and<br />

Perceptual Identification. LEE H. WURM & SEAN R. SEAMAN,<br />

Wayne State University—Previous research shows that the subjective<br />

danger and usefulness of words affect recognition times. Usually an<br />

interaction is found: Increasing danger predicts faster RTs for words<br />

low on usefulness, but increasing danger predicts slower RTs for<br />

words high on usefulness. We explore the nature of this interaction<br />

using three experimental tasks. <strong>The</strong> interaction is found in all tasks.<br />

It holds for nouns, verbs, and adjectives. It is significant over and<br />

above effects of variables such as frequency, length, concreteness, age<br />

of acquisition, imageability, and familiarity. <strong>The</strong> interaction cannot be<br />

characterized as a speed versus accuracy trade-off. We believe its origin<br />

is an approach/withdraw response conflict induced by stimuli that<br />

are both dangerous and useful. It may be a manifestation of the rapid<br />

evaluation effects pervasive in the literature. Post hoc analyses also<br />

show that danger and usefulness explain as much variance as Valence<br />

and Arousal, or Evaluation, Potency, and Activity.<br />

3:10–3:25 (93)<br />

Anticipatory Effects in Delayed Naming: <strong>The</strong> Role of the Hazard<br />

Function Probability. MICHAEL J. CORTESE, University of Nebraska,<br />

Omaha—Two experiments examined anticipatory effects in<br />

delayed naming performance by varying the hazard function probability<br />

(HFP) of the response signal. In Experiment 1, critical stimuli<br />

were named at a 1,300-msec delay. <strong>The</strong> HFP equaled .33 in one condition<br />

and 1.0 in another condition. When the HFP equaled 1.0, naming<br />

latencies were 36 msec faster than when the HFP equaled .33. Experiment<br />

2 compared naming latencies for three delays (900 msec,<br />

1,100 msec, and 1,300 msec) that were distributed uniformly in one<br />

block of trials (i.e., the HFP increased systematically across the delay<br />

intervals) and exponentially in another block of trials (i.e., the HFP<br />

remained constant at .50 across the delay intervals). Naming latencies<br />

decreased along with delay interval only in the uniform distribution<br />

condition. Based on these results, researchers who use the delayed<br />

naming task should employ an exponential distribution schedule in<br />

order to guard against anticipatory effects.<br />

Spatial Cognition<br />

Shoreline, Friday Afternoon, 1:30–3:50<br />

Chaired by Amy Lynne Shelton, Johns Hopkins University<br />

1:30–1:45 (94)<br />

Place and Response Mechanisms in Human Environmental<br />

Learning. AMY LYNNE SHELTON, STEVEN A. MARCHETTE, &<br />

NAOHIDE YAMAMOTO, Johns Hopkins University—Place learning<br />

and response learning reflect two spatial learning systems. Less is<br />

known about the role these systems play in environmental learning in<br />

humans than in rats. In behavioral and neuroimaging experiments, we<br />

investigated whether these different learning systems could be observed<br />

using (1) task differences within individuals and (2) individual<br />

differences within tasks. For task differences, a comparison of environmental<br />

learning from ground-level and aerial perspectives<br />

revealed patterns of brain activation and subsequent memory performance<br />

that suggested distinct but overlapping learning systems. Focusing<br />

on individual differences within a given perspective, we found<br />

differences in the brain activation and the subsequent memory per-<br />

15<br />

formance as a function of gender and spatial skills. <strong>The</strong> differential<br />

patterns of brain activation were localized to regions typically associated<br />

with place and response learning. Taken together, we assert that<br />

humans, like other animals, have multiple systems that may be preferentially<br />

engaged across tasks and individuals.<br />

1:50–2:05 (95)<br />

Spatial Memory: Categorical and Metric Encoding of Location in<br />

Complex Scenes. THOMAS F. SHIPLEY, MARK P. HOLDEN,<br />

LONGIN J. LATECKI, NORA S. NEWCOMBE, & SHANNON L.<br />

FITZHUGH, Temple University—How do we remember object locations?<br />

Intuitively, we remember that our keys are on a table, and<br />

roughly 5 inches to left of the center. A Bayesian combination of categorical<br />

and metric information offers an optimal memory strategy<br />

under uncertainty, and prior research (e.g., Huttenlocher, Hedges, &<br />

Duncan, 1991) supports the use of such a strategy for simple figures.<br />

We report the results of several studies that confirm the use of a combination<br />

of categorical and coordinate information to estimate location<br />

in complex scenes. Subjects’ errors in recall indicate they encoded<br />

the location of single objects categorically, as within a bounded<br />

region. However, rather than encoding the location relative to the center<br />

of the category, the metric encoding appears to locate objects<br />

within a region relative to the spine (the line of local symmetry) of<br />

the region.<br />

2:10–2:25 (96)<br />

Toward an Embodied Account of Spatial Memory: Egocentric Experience<br />

Trumps Intrinsic Structure. DAVID WALLER, NATHAN<br />

GREENAUER, & CATHERINE MELLO, Miami University—Mou<br />

and McNamara’s (2002) theory of human spatial representation holds<br />

that memory is organized by intrinsic reference systems (those based<br />

on the spatial structure of a configuration of objects, and not on the<br />

surroundings or on the observer) and codes interobject spatial relationships.<br />

However, the degree to which perceptually salient intrinsic<br />

axes of a layout of objects are used to organize spatial memory is an<br />

open question. We show that salient intrinsic axes in a layout of objects<br />

are neither necessary nor sufficient for people to use a nonegocentric<br />

reference system in organizing spatial memory. Additional experiments<br />

demonstrated that when people used nonegocentric<br />

reference systems, the selection of a reference direction was influenced<br />

by their action possibilities during encoding. We suggest that<br />

what are often interpreted as nonegocentric reference systems may be<br />

better conceptualized as imagined egocentric reference systems because<br />

they are so closely tied to potential personal (embodied) experience.<br />

2:30–2:45 (97)<br />

Do Humans Have an Internal Compass Like Other Mammals Do?<br />

M. JEANNE SHOLL, Boston College—A head-direction (HD) cell<br />

fires when the animal’s head points in the cell’s preferred allocentric<br />

direction, irrespective of the animal’s location in the environment. <strong>The</strong><br />

cells have been studied extensively in rats, and although their role in<br />

navigation is not fully understood, they are undoubtedly the neural<br />

basis of a rat’s “sense of direction.” While there is neuropsychological<br />

and neuroimaging data consistent with a human allocentric-heading<br />

system, human HD cells have yet to be discovered. <strong>The</strong> present paper<br />

will describe some behavioral evidence from our laboratory showing<br />

that people can recover from a pictured scene, the allocentric direction<br />

their body faced in the actual environment when looking at the<br />

scene straight on. Perhaps revealingly, people differ widely in their<br />

ability to do this, and these differences correlate with self-reported<br />

sense of direction. <strong>The</strong> behavioral findings will be discussed in the<br />

context of theoretical models of HD-system function in rats.<br />

2:50–3:05 (98)<br />

Longer Segments of Slopes Are Overestimated More Than Short<br />

Segments. BRUCE BRIDGEMAN, MERRIT HOOVER, ERIC M.<br />

CHIU, & JOSHUA R. QUAN, University of California, Santa Cruz—

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!