29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Friday Afternoon Papers 86–91<br />

people often experience intrusive remindings, and make efforts to exclude<br />

the unwanted memory from awareness. Prior work has established<br />

that suppressing retrieval in this way recruits executive control<br />

regions in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) that downregulate<br />

activation in the hippocampus, impairing later retention.<br />

Here, we investigate the neurobiological mechanisms underlying the<br />

intrusion experience. In an adapted version of the “think/no think”<br />

procedure, subjects reported after each suppression trial whether the<br />

unwanted memory had intruded into awareness. <strong>The</strong> results show that<br />

although subjects initially report many intrusions (~50%), these intrusions<br />

decline with practice, with later memory impairment predicted<br />

by this decline. Moreover, intrusions were associated with decreased<br />

engagement of the DLPFC and elevated activity in the<br />

hippocampus. <strong>The</strong>se findings suggest that intrusive memories reflect<br />

a failure to engage DLPFC during suppression, and that successful<br />

down-regulation of hippocampal activation is an essential component<br />

of memory control.<br />

2:10–2:25 (86)<br />

Independent Cues Are Not Independent. GINO CAMP, DIANE<br />

PECHER, HENK G. SCHMIDT, & RENÉ ZEELENBERG, Erasmus<br />

University Rotterdam—Retrieval practice with particular items from<br />

memory (e.g., orange) can result in forgetting of related items (e.g.,<br />

banana) that are associated to the same cue (e.g., FRUIT). This retrievalinduced<br />

forgetting effect can be explained by both interference and inhibitory<br />

processes. <strong>The</strong> independent cue technique has been developed<br />

to differentiate between the contributions of interference and<br />

inhibition to the forgetting effect. Independent cues are not associated<br />

to the practiced item (e.g., monkey–b___ for banana). Inhibition theory<br />

predicts forgetting with this type of cue. However, interference<br />

theory predicts forgetting only with the studied category (FRUIT) and<br />

does not predict forgetting with independent cues. <strong>The</strong>refore, forgetting<br />

with independent cues is seen as an empirical criterion for inhibition.<br />

However, in three experiments, we demonstrate experimentally<br />

that, even when participants are only cued with independent cues,<br />

studied categories are activated at test. Thus, independent cues may<br />

not differentiate effectively between interference and inhibition.<br />

2:30–2:45 (87)<br />

Long-Term Episodic Inhibition and Sleep. MIHALY RACSMANY,<br />

Hungarian Academy of Sciences, MARTIN A. CONWAY, University<br />

of Leeds, & GYULA DEMETER, Budapest University of Technology<br />

and Economics (sponsored by Martin A. Conway)—Studies of retrieval<br />

inhibition have persistently found that retrieval practice of selected<br />

category items from a previously studied list reduces recall of<br />

studied unpracticed items from the same category. Past research suggests<br />

that this effect is short-term. Two experiments investigated the<br />

long-term effect of retrieval practice and its relationship to sleep. In<br />

both experiments two groups of subjects undertook a selective practice<br />

paradigm with a 12-h delay between practice and recall. One<br />

group (sleepers) had a full night’s sleep during delay, while the other<br />

group (nonsleepers) recalled at the end of the day. Both groups<br />

showed highly reliable retrieval-induced forgetting. In the second experiment<br />

a surprise free recall procedure was used. A significant inhibitory<br />

effect for the sleepers was again found but there was no reliable<br />

effect for the nonsleepers. <strong>The</strong>se findings are considered in terms<br />

of consolidation of the contents of episodic memories.<br />

Word Processing<br />

Seaview, Friday Afternoon, 1:30–3:30<br />

Chaired by Julie E. Boland, University of Michigan<br />

1:30–1:45 (88)<br />

Contextual Modulation of Dominant Homophone Meaning in the<br />

Visual World. LILLIAN CHEN & JULIE E. BOLAND, University<br />

of Michigan (read by Julie E. Boland)—In a visual world eye tracking<br />

study, participants heard homophones in either neutral or subordinate-<br />

14<br />

biased contexts (e.g., NEUTRAL, Amanda didn’t supervise her little<br />

brother, who ended up dropping the glasses; BIASED, <strong>The</strong> dishwashers<br />

washed the plates, the silverware, and finally the glasses). At homophone<br />

onset, four pictures appeared: subordinate homophone meaning<br />

(drinking glasses), dominate meaning shape competitor (handcuffs,<br />

for eyeglasses), and two unrelated pictures. <strong>The</strong> shape<br />

competitor was used to index subliminal activation of the dominant<br />

meaning. Context influenced the proportion of fixations on both the<br />

subordinate picture and the shape competitor, beginning with the second<br />

fixation after the pictures appeared. Nonetheless, the shape competitor<br />

attracted more fixations than expected by chance, even in the<br />

subordinate-biased condition. This pattern is similar to the subordinatebias<br />

effect reported in reading experiments (Binder & Rayner, 1998):<br />

Context modulates the activation of homophone meanings, but the<br />

dominate meaning remains partially activated in subordinate-biased<br />

contexts.<br />

1:50–2:05 (89)<br />

Selection of Homograph Meaning: Implicit Memory Effects in<br />

Episodic Memory. DAVID S. GORFEIN, Adelphi University, VIN-<br />

CENT R. BROWN, Hofstra University, & EMILY EDWARDS, University<br />

of Texas, Arlington—Traditional research in meaning selection<br />

for ambiguous words (homographs/homophones) has focused on semantic<br />

tasks such as the selection of a meaning for the ambiguous<br />

word in a biasing context and for the same word presented later in a<br />

similar or different semantic context. We have previously demonstrated<br />

that an ambiguous word presented along with a picture consistent<br />

with its secondary meaning induces a bias which is maintained<br />

across relatively long time intervals, many intervening trials, changes<br />

in task context, and changes in response requirements. <strong>The</strong>se findings<br />

are now extended to transfer from the initial secondary-meaning picture<br />

priming task to an episodic memory task, free recall. <strong>The</strong> data<br />

support the interpretation that the picture priming task alters the<br />

meaning representation of the words, thereby affecting the frequency<br />

and pattern of recall not only for the ambiguous words themselves, but<br />

for words related to the ambiguous words’ initially dominant and secondary<br />

meanings.<br />

2:10–2:25 (90)<br />

Bricks Don’t Breathe: Semantic Decisions Are Made by Weighing<br />

Evidence For and Against Category Membership. DIANE PECHER<br />

& RENÉ ZEELENBERG, Erasmus University Rotterdam, & ERIC-<br />

JAN WAGENMAKERS, University of Amsterdam—In the animal decision<br />

task words are categorized as animals or nonanimals. Deadline<br />

models propose that decisions are based on activation of an animal<br />

feature in combination with a temporal deadline (Carreiras, Perea, &<br />

Grainger, 1997; Forster & Hector, 2002). Instead, we propose that the<br />

decision process weighs several features that provide partial evidence<br />

for either an animal or a nonanimal decision. If this weighing provides<br />

strong evidence for either response, performance is better than if the<br />

weighing provides only weak evidence for either response. In Experiment<br />

1 performance was worse for words having features correlated<br />

with the opposite response (e.g., organic for a nonanimal) than for<br />

words not having such features. In Experiment 2 nonanimal responses<br />

were faster than animal responses if the proportion of nonanimals was<br />

high. <strong>The</strong>se results are problematic for a deadline model but consistent<br />

with a model combining evidence from different features.<br />

2:30–2:45 (91)<br />

An REM Model for Animal Decision. ERIC-JAN WAGENMAKERS,<br />

University of Amsterdam, & DIANE PECHER & RENÉ ZEELEN-<br />

BERG, Erasmus University Rotterdam—In animal decision, people<br />

have to quickly decide whether or not words such as “mountain” or<br />

“parrot” refer to an animal. This task requires that people access both<br />

orthographic and semantic information. Previous research has shown<br />

that (1) activation of semantic information occurs before orthographic<br />

processing is complete; (2) performance for typical animals (e.g.,<br />

dog) and nonanimals (e.g., computer) is better than performance for

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!