29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Saturday Noon Posters 4104–4110<br />

ined how learning was influenced by three feedback conditions: no<br />

feedback, automatic feedback given after every response, and subjectcontrolled<br />

feedback in which subjects decided whether they needed<br />

to see the correct answer after a recall attempt. Similar to findings in<br />

the motor learning domain, any feedback resulted in improved cued<br />

recall performance as compared with the absence of feedback. Unlike<br />

in the motor learning domain, subject- and experimenter-controlled<br />

feedback resulted in similar cued recall performance. <strong>The</strong> results have<br />

implications for further understanding the role that metacognition<br />

plays in the effectiveness of feedback during learning.<br />

(4104)<br />

Associative History Modulates Human Learning: Evidence From<br />

Implicit Tasks. THOMAS BEESLEY & MIKE E. LE PELLEY, Cardiff<br />

University (sponsored by Andrew Delamater)—It has been shown in<br />

both animal and human learning that the predictive history of cues<br />

modulates the new associations that are formed with novel outcomes.<br />

Models which allow for changes in the “associability” of cues have<br />

been developed in the associative learning literature, yet whether similar<br />

cue-processing effects can be observed in implicit learning is unknown.<br />

We explored this possibility using the serial reaction time task.<br />

During pretraining, some cues consistently predicted outcomes,<br />

whereas others were relatively nonpredictive. During this stage, participants’<br />

responses reflected the differences in predictiveness of trials.<br />

When all cues were then consistently paired with new locations,<br />

participants learned about cues that were previously good predictors<br />

faster than they learned about cues that were previously poor predictors.<br />

<strong>The</strong> results suggest that the prior predictiveness of cues modulates<br />

implicit sequence learning and that associability effects in humans<br />

are not necessarily a product of rational inference.<br />

(4105)<br />

Holographic Neural Networks. MICHAEL N. JONES, Indiana University,<br />

Bloomington—Holographic neural networks (HNNs) have been<br />

enormously successful in engineering but have seen limited attention<br />

as cognitive models. An HNN has no fixed set of connections but,<br />

rather, represents a node as a complex enfolding of weights in a Riemann<br />

space. Complex-valued weights are computed deterministically,<br />

effectively giving a single holographic node the computational power<br />

of an entire traditional connectionist network (TCN). HNNs have several<br />

appealing characteristics when compared with TCNs, including<br />

greater storage capacity with fewer nodes, no need for error feedback<br />

or iterative learning, the ability to represent complex stimulus spaces<br />

(e.g., XOR) with a single layer, and intrinsic response confidence estimates.<br />

HNNs also display associative commutativity: A network<br />

trained on S–R mappings can produce R–S mappings without inversion<br />

or additional training. Furthermore, HNNs have architectural<br />

similarities to biological neurons and are related to quantum models,<br />

which are gaining popularity as models of cognition. I will compare<br />

HNNs and TCNs on pattern classification and present extensions to<br />

models of complex cognition.<br />

(4106)<br />

Aging and Updating Causal Beliefs. SHARON A. MUTTER,<br />

ANTHONY R. ATCHLEY, & LESLIE F. PLUMLEE, Western Kentucky<br />

University—In a causal learning task, unrelated food cues presented<br />

in compound were followed by an allergic reaction (e.g., AB+), then<br />

single food cues from these compounds were revalued (A�). Young<br />

and older adults learned causal relationships between the compound<br />

and single cues and the outcome, but unlike young adults, older adults<br />

did not retrospectively revalue the causal effectiveness of the absent<br />

cues (B). Older adults’ within-compound associations for the unrelated<br />

foods were weaker than those of young adults, suggesting that<br />

they were unable to update the causal value of the absent cues because<br />

the single food cues did not effectively activate the representations of<br />

these cues. However, a second experiment showed that older adults did<br />

not revalue absent cues even when foods in the compound cues had<br />

strong preexisting associations. This suggests that changes in older<br />

120<br />

adults’ inferential reasoning processes also contribute to age differences<br />

in updating causal beliefs.<br />

• RECOGNITION MEMORY •<br />

(4107)<br />

Distinguishing Among Remembered, Familiar, and Just Known<br />

Memories: An Experimental Analysis. SARAH J. BARBER &<br />

SUPARNA RAJARAM, Stony Brook University, & ELIZABETH J.<br />

MARSH, Duke University (sponsored by Suparna Rajaram)—How<br />

does the shift from episodic to semantic memory take place? Although<br />

this change in memory awareness over time has been demonstrated in<br />

real-world classrooms (e.g., Conway, Gardiner, Perfect, Anderson, &<br />

Cohen, 1997), the present experiment demonstrates a decrease in remember<br />

and an increase in just-know responses in a controlled laboratory<br />

paradigm. Participants read short stories containing easy and hard<br />

facts. On an immediate, a delayed, or a delayed-repeated general knowledge<br />

test, participants provided remember, just-know, and familiar responses.<br />

Our results indicate that recalled responses are associated more<br />

often with remember responses on an immediate test but with just-know<br />

responses on a delayed test. Furthermore, this shift occurs for easy but<br />

not for difficult factual information. Our results also show that familiar<br />

responses (similar to the know response in the traditional remember–<br />

know paradigm) show a different pattern from just-know responses.<br />

(4108)<br />

Processing Fluency Affects Subjective Claims of Recollection.<br />

BRIAN P. KURILLA & DEANNE L. WESTERMAN, Binghamton<br />

University—Previous studies that have used the remember–know paradigm<br />

to investigate subjective awareness in memory have shown that<br />

fluency manipulations impact knowing but not remembering (e.g., Rajaram,<br />

1993), a finding typically accounted for by invoking inferential<br />

processing in judgments of familiarity but not recollection. However,<br />

in light of criticisms of this procedure, as well as findings that fluency<br />

affects a variety of subjective judgments, the present study was conducted<br />

to investigate whether recollection might also be inferential and<br />

not solely the product of conscious retrieval. When using the standard<br />

remember–know procedure, manipulations of perceptual fluency increased<br />

know responses but had no effect on remember responses, replicating<br />

previous findings. However, when employing an independent ratings<br />

method (Higham & Vokey, 2004), manipulations of perceptual and<br />

conceptual fluency increased claims of both familiarity and recollection,<br />

suggesting that the conclusion that fluency affects only know responses<br />

may be an artifact of the standard remember–know procedure.<br />

(4109)<br />

How Does Response Time Inform Recollection-Based Retrieval<br />

Strategies? DAVID A. GALLO & MICHAEL HAWKINS, University<br />

of Chicago—On recognition tests, familiar lures can be rejected if they<br />

fail to cue expected recollections, or if they successfully cue the recollection<br />

of inconsistent information. We designed a task to measure<br />

these two rejection processes. Subjects studied unrelated words, with<br />

some followed by a corresponding picture. On the picture recollection<br />

test, subjects could reject familiar lures if they failed to cue picture<br />

recollections (distinctiveness heuristic), whereas on the picture exclusion<br />

test, subjects could reject familiar lures if they successfully<br />

cued picture recollections (recall-to-reject). False recognition patterns<br />

were consistent with these different processes. We hypothesized that<br />

the distinctiveness heuristic should be less strategic than recall-toreject,<br />

and hence faster. Although response latencies were faster on<br />

the picture recollection test, experimentally speeding response times<br />

reduced accuracy on each test to the same degree. Latencies may be<br />

a better index of strategy differences than experimentally manipulating<br />

response time, which appears to reduce recollection overall.<br />

(4110)<br />

Recollection in Semantic Decisions. LILLIAN PARK, Rotman Research<br />

Institute, MARY PAT MCANDREWS, Toronto Western Hospi-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!