29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Posters 4097–4103 Saturday Noon<br />

generation pill users, but they performed worse than OC nonusers.<br />

(3) <strong>The</strong> hormone that influences visuospatial task performance is different<br />

for OC users and nonusers. Our results show that the androgenic<br />

component in OCs mediates visuospatial task performance in OC<br />

users. On the other hand, visuospatial task performance in naturally<br />

cycling females is influenced by naturally fluctuating estrogen levels.<br />

(4097)<br />

Reorienting When Cues Conflict: Using Geometry and Features Following<br />

Landmark Displacement. KRISTIN R. RATLIFF & NORA S.<br />

NEWCOMBE, Temple University (sponsored by Kristin R. Ratliff)—<br />

Three experiments were conducted to test the idea that use of features<br />

during reorientation is highly dependent upon their perceived usefulness<br />

and prior experience as proposed by the adaptive combination<br />

view rather than by a modular view wherein geometric information always<br />

dominates behavior. When featural landmarks are placed in conflict<br />

with the geometry of an enclosed space, we found that (1) adults<br />

favor geometric information only in a small room and favor features<br />

in a larger room, (2) large room training increases use of features in<br />

a small room, and (3) using multiple landmarks that move dependently<br />

increases feature use in a small room. Although geometric information<br />

is important during reorientation, its supposed dominance over<br />

featural information is limited to small, fully enclosed spaces with<br />

only one feature. Flexible reorientation, encompassing all available<br />

spatial cues, is characteristic in larger spaces or when learning history<br />

and experience have established the usefulness of features.<br />

(4098)<br />

Transfer of Orthogonal and Parallel Incompatible Mappings to<br />

the SNARC Effect. GI YEUL BAE, JONG MOON CHOI, & YANG<br />

SEOK CHO, Korea University, & ROBERT W. PROCTOR, Purdue<br />

University (sponsored by Yang Seok Cho)—Three experiments examined<br />

the effect of prior practice on performance of a subsequent<br />

parity judgment task. In Experiment 1, a magnitude judgment task<br />

preceded the parity judgment task. A reversed SNARC effect was obtained<br />

when participants practiced with incompatible mapping<br />

(large–left/small–right), indicating that new associations between<br />

number magnitude codes and response codes, which were created<br />

when participants performed the magnitude judgment task, were<br />

maintained throughout the subsequent task. In Experiment 2, a parallel<br />

stimulus–response compatibility task was performed as practice.<br />

A regular SNARC effect was obtained regardless of whether participants<br />

practiced with compatible or incompatible mapping. However,<br />

in Experiment 3, in which participants practiced an orthogonal compatibility<br />

task, the SNARC effect was smaller when participants practiced<br />

with up–left/down–right mapping than with up–right/down–left.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se results suggest that the SNARC effect is due to polarity correspondence<br />

between stimulus and response codes, rather than to spatial<br />

correspondence.<br />

(4099)<br />

Horizontal and Vertical Preparation in Right–Left Prevalence. AKIO<br />

NISHIMURA & KAZUHIKO YOKOSAWA, University of Tokyo<br />

(sponsored by Kazuhiko Yokosawa)—Horizontal stimulus–response<br />

compatibility (SRC) effect is often larger than vertical SRC effect<br />

when stimulus and response simultaneously vary in both dimensions.<br />

We investigated the role of horizontal and vertical set preparation in<br />

this right–left prevalence effect by switching the tasks on the basis of<br />

horizontal or vertical dimensions. In Experiment 1, we manipulated<br />

the preparation time for the upcoming task dimension by varying the<br />

cue–target SOA. A right–left prevalence effect was observed only<br />

when participants prepared for the upcoming task. <strong>The</strong> absence of the<br />

right–left prevalence effect with 0-msec SOA was replicated in Experiment<br />

2. In Experiment 3, the right–left prevalence effect emerged<br />

with simultaneous presentation of task cue and the target when participants<br />

prepared for each dimension on the basis of its frequency. We<br />

discuss the findings in terms of the speed and the activation level of<br />

horizontal and vertical spatial codes.<br />

119<br />

• ASSOCIATIVE LEARNING •<br />

(4100)<br />

Does My Lie Have to Imply? <strong>The</strong> Role of Implication in Eyewitness<br />

Memory. DEBORAH K. EAKIN & JESSICA WOOD, Mississippi<br />

State University—Exposure to misleading postevent information typically<br />

impairs memory for the original event, a finding known as the<br />

misinformation effect (e.g., Eakin, Schreiber, & Sergent-Marshall,<br />

2003). One component of the typical misinformation paradigm is that<br />

most of the postevent information corresponds with the event information,<br />

implying that the information in which the misleading information<br />

is embedded is a reiteration of the original event. We manipulated<br />

implication by presenting misleading items in a narrative that<br />

differed from the slide show; both the location of the event and the<br />

persons described varied from the original event (no-implication condition).<br />

Memory for event information was compared between this<br />

condition and one for which the narrative reiterated the slide show<br />

(implication condition). We obtained a significant misinformation effect<br />

regardless of implication condition. Even when a relationship between<br />

the original and postevent was not implied, memory was impaired<br />

by the presence of misleading postevent information.<br />

(4101)<br />

Adaptation to Temporal Covariations. ANNIKA WAGENER &<br />

JOACHIM HOFFMANN, University of Würzburg—<strong>The</strong>re is ample<br />

evidence demonstrating covariation learning between identity and location<br />

of targets. We explored whether temporal information (length<br />

of foreperiod) also enhances target processing when it covaries, either<br />

with target identity or target location. In our experiments, either a short<br />

or a long foreperiod preceded one of two targets, occurring typically<br />

at one of two locations. Participants’ task was to identify the presented<br />

targets by pressing a left or a right button as quickly as possible. <strong>The</strong><br />

results revealed an adaptation to the employed covariations: <strong>The</strong>re<br />

were processing benefits for those targets and for those locations<br />

which were likely for a particular foreperiod. We attribute the observed<br />

temporal validity effects to acquired associations between representations<br />

of temporal information and representations of target<br />

identities and target locations.<br />

(4102)<br />

Examining the Retrieval Process in Mediated Learning Using<br />

Priming Effects. JAMES A. KOLE & ALICE F. HEALY, University<br />

of Colorado—<strong>The</strong> covert mediation model posits that mediators connect<br />

cue and target and are automatically activated during retrieval,<br />

even after extended retrieval practice. Three experiments tested this<br />

model using mediated priming effects. In each experiment, participants<br />

used the keyword method to learn French vocabulary and subsequently<br />

completed a modified lexical decision task in which they<br />

first translated a French word, then made a lexical decision on either<br />

a semantic associate of the keyword, a semantic associate of the<br />

English translation, a semantically unrelated word, or a nonword. <strong>The</strong><br />

amount of practice translating the French words was varied before<br />

completing the lexical decision task, and participants were tested for<br />

explicit memory of keywords with a cued recognition test. A mediated<br />

priming effect was found only when no additional translation<br />

practice was administered. However, explicit memory for the keyword<br />

was high even with translation practice. <strong>The</strong> results argue against the<br />

covert mediation model.<br />

(4103)<br />

Subject-Controlled Feedback in a Verbal Learning Task. ASHLEY<br />

N. D. MEYER & JESSICA M. LOGAN, Rice University—Giving subjects<br />

control over the feedback they receive during learning may improve<br />

feedback effectiveness and later memory performance. Such<br />

subject-controlled feedback has been found to be more effective than<br />

experimenter-controlled feedback in the motor learning domain, but<br />

little is known about its effectiveness in the verbal domain. Using foreign<br />

language vocabulary (German–English word pairs), we exam-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!