29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Saturday Noon Posters 4061–4068<br />

(4061)<br />

Reasoning Over Isomorphic and Nonisomorphic Logic Structures.<br />

JOHN BEST, Eastern Illinois University—A variety of dual-process<br />

theories claim that reasoning is usually accomplished by a heuristic<br />

system that may at times be overridden by a deductively competent analytic<br />

system. One alternative states that all reasoning is accomplished<br />

by one system whose representations are strengthened with experience.<br />

In this study, subjects solved 16 conditional reasoning problems<br />

and then 3 “logic table” problems that either were isomorphs of each<br />

other, or did not share an underlying logic structure. Crossed with this<br />

condition, half of the participants were given instructions and an answer<br />

grid that emphasized the role of the analytic system. Contrary to<br />

the predictions of dual-process but consistent with single-process alternatives,<br />

the subjects in each condition solved the third logic table<br />

problem much more quickly than they did the first (mean reduction =<br />

38%), with no corresponding fall-off in deductive accuracy for any of<br />

the four groups.<br />

(4062)<br />

Cross-Cultural Cognitive Differences in Analogical Reasoning: A<br />

Computational Account. ROBERT G. MORRISON, Northwestern<br />

University, LEONIDAS A. A. DOUMAS, Indiana University, &<br />

LINDSEY E. RICHLAND, University of California, Irvine—We previously<br />

reported that when children can identify the critical relations<br />

in a scene analogy problem, development of their ability to reason<br />

analogically interacts with both relational complexity and featural distraction<br />

(Richland, Morrison, & Holyoak, 2006). Here, we report that<br />

unlike 3- to 4-year-old American children, Hong Kong children are sensitive<br />

to featural distraction, but not to relational complexity. Differences<br />

in the linguistic structure of Chinese and English task versions<br />

suggest that this result is driven by variations in children’s knowledge<br />

representations. Performance differences are eliminated when Hong<br />

Kong children perform under a working memory dual task. We present<br />

computer simulations in LISA (Hummel & Holyoak, 1997,<br />

2003) explaining these results as trade-offs between inhibition in<br />

working memory and the sophistication of relational knowledge representations.<br />

Specifically, we show that changes in inhibition can best<br />

explain the overall developmental progression; however, differences<br />

in knowledge representation best account for cultural differences.<br />

(4063)<br />

Can Domain Knowledge Improve Causal Reasoning? MICHELLE R.<br />

ELLEFSON & CHRISTIAN D. SCHUNN, University of Pittsburgh—<br />

Grasping scientific phenomena includes more than the mere memorization<br />

of scientific facts. It requires the creation of appropriate models<br />

of the relations among important aspects of scientific phenomena.<br />

Many of these relations are causal. <strong>The</strong> present study explored students’<br />

ability to reason about simple and complex causes, both within<br />

and outside of the scientific domain that they were studying. <strong>The</strong> participants<br />

were urban high school students in biology, chemistry, and<br />

physics courses (n = 99). Overall, the results suggest that reasoning<br />

about interactions among two or more causal variables is more difficult<br />

than reasoning about the main effect of one causal variable. Students<br />

did not achieve higher accuracy for reasoning questions in the<br />

domain that they were studying. <strong>The</strong>refore, because the complexity of<br />

the questions influenced performance more than domain knowledge<br />

did, the results suggest that domain knowledge is neither necessary<br />

nor sufficient for complex reasoning in introductory science courses.<br />

(4064)<br />

Thinking Dispositions, Metacognition, and Syllogistic Reasoning.<br />

JAMIE A. PROWSE TURNER, ERIN L. BEATTY, & VALERIE A.<br />

THOMPSON, University of Saskatchewan—We studied the relationship<br />

between three measures of thinking dispositions and (1) metacognitive<br />

ability and (2) reasoning performance on a syllogistic reasoning<br />

task. <strong>The</strong> measures of thinking dispositions included the general<br />

decision-making style instrument (GDMS; Scott & Bruce, 1995), the<br />

114<br />

actively open-minded thinking scale (AOT; Stanovich & West, in<br />

press), and the cognitive reflection test (CRT; Frederick, 2005). We<br />

measured metacognitive accuracy in a number of ways, including asking<br />

for item-by-item estimates of performance and several posttask estimates<br />

of performance. Regression equations established that different<br />

sets of variables predicted accuracy on the reasoning task, the<br />

item-by-item estimates, and the posttask estimates of performance.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se findings are consistent with the hypothesis that item-by-item<br />

estimates and posttask estimates tap different processes (Stankov,<br />

2000). We plan a follow-up study that includes measures of cognitive<br />

ability and performance on belief-biased materials.<br />

(4066)<br />

Premise Versus Conclusion Believability in Categorical Reasoning.<br />

SHARON LEE ARMSTRONG, La Salle University—Previous studies<br />

have shown that neutral content produces superior performance over<br />

emotional or abstract content in evaluating conditional arguments and<br />

categorical syllogisms. Furthermore, consistent with theories of motivated<br />

reasoning, it is often suggested that it is prior beliefs about<br />

conclusions that drive validity judgments. However, my previous research<br />

found that it was the believability of the first premise, rather<br />

than the believability of the conclusion, that played the primary role<br />

in predicting correct validity judgments in conditional reasoning<br />

tasks. To determine whether this finding is peculiar to conditional reasoning,<br />

the present study investigated the role of the believability of<br />

different argument statements in the evaluation of categorical syllogisms.<br />

Comparisons of the patterns of validity judgments across the<br />

two types of logic problems are discussed with regard to current theories<br />

of reasoning.<br />

(4067)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Acquisition of Causal Variables. NOAH D. GOODMAN &<br />

JOSHUA B. TENENBAUM, MIT—Standard theories of causal learning<br />

identify causal relations between preexisting variables, such as the<br />

nodes in a Bayesian network. But how are these variables acquired?<br />

How does the continuous flux of perception become symbolically<br />

coded in variables that support appropriate causal relations? We argue<br />

that whereas some variables are innate, others must be learned, in<br />

ways that depend on both bottom-up perceptual coherence and the<br />

functional role that variables play in the learner’s causal model of the<br />

world. We present a Bayesian framework for explaining how causal<br />

variables and relations can be learned simultaneously, in the form of<br />

a grounded causal model: a system of event concepts connected by<br />

causal relations and explicitly grounded in perception. We also present<br />

empirical studies showing that participants can learn grounded<br />

causal models from dynamic perceptual evidence. Several factors<br />

modulate learning difficulty as predicted by the Bayesian model, including<br />

perceptual distinctiveness and causal contrast.<br />

(4068)<br />

Similarity Falls Out of a Model of Analogy. ERIC G. TAYLOR &<br />

JOHN E. HUMMEL, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign (sponsored<br />

by John E. Hummel)—<strong>The</strong> concept of “similarity” plays a central<br />

role in cognition. Existing models of similarity capture either “featural”<br />

or “structural” aspects of similarity, but typically not both. We<br />

attempted to capture both by fitting similarity judgment data with the<br />

LISA model of relational reasoning (Hummel & Holyoak, 1997, 2003).<br />

LISA simulates both structural effects on similarity, such as violations<br />

of the metric axioms and the dominance of alignable over nonalignable<br />

differences in similarity judgments, and the relation between structural<br />

and featural aspects as embodied by the combined effects of MIPs and<br />

MOPs (featural matches “in” or “out of place”; Goldstone, 1994) on<br />

similarity judgments. LISA’s ability to capture both featural and structural<br />

aspects of similarity derives from its knowledge representations,<br />

which are simultaneously symbolic, by virtue of dynamically binding<br />

relational roles to their arguments, and semantically rich, by virtue of<br />

their distributed representations of those roles and arguments.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!