29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Saturday Noon Posters 4048–4054<br />

that, although listeners tend to not ignore salient irrelevant properties,<br />

experience and knowledge both have an effect on the use of specific<br />

relevant properties. <strong>The</strong> results will be discussed in terms of the degree<br />

to which individual properties can be altered.<br />

(4048)<br />

Brief Meditation Training and Its Effects on Pain Perception.<br />

FADEL Z. ZEIDAN, NAKIA S. GORDON, & PAULA GOOLKASIAN,<br />

University of North Carolina, Charlotte—Long-term meditation practice<br />

attenuates responses to experimental pain (Kaikigi et al., 2005).<br />

Meditation may reduce pain perception by serving as a distraction; inducing<br />

a relaxation response (Lazar et al., 2000); or enhancing ability<br />

to cognitively shift attention from pain (Orme-Johnson et al.,<br />

2006). <strong>The</strong>se issues have been explored largely in adept meditators<br />

(Kabat-Zinn et al., 1985). We examined the effects of 3 consecutive<br />

days of 20-min mindfulness meditation training on pain perception to<br />

electrical stimulation. A comparison group tested under relaxation<br />

and arithmetic distraction conditions was also included. <strong>The</strong> results<br />

demonstrated that brief meditation intervention significantly reduced<br />

pain perception in comparison with relaxation and distraction. <strong>The</strong>se<br />

findings suggest that pain perception can be attenuated after only<br />

60 min of meditation instruction. <strong>The</strong> cognitive aspect of meditation<br />

practice may play a role in this effect.<br />

(4049)<br />

Presentation Order of Comparison and Standard Stimulus Affects<br />

the Difference Threshold. EINAT LAPID & ROLF ULRICH, University<br />

of Tübingen, & THOMAS RAMMSAYER, University of Bern—<br />

In determining the difference threshold (i.e., the just noticeable difference)<br />

in temporal discrimination tasks, subjects are typically asked<br />

to compare a constant standard stimulus with a variable comparison<br />

stimulus. This task allows one to establish a psychometric function<br />

from which the difference threshold is calculated. <strong>The</strong> psychophysical<br />

results of our experiments showed that the presentation order of<br />

the comparison and the standard could strongly influence the size of<br />

this threshold. A mathematical model can account for this order effect.<br />

It assumes that subjects average stimulus information across trials<br />

to establish a stable internal representation of the standard. This<br />

model also accounts for our finding that difference thresholds determined<br />

by a two-alternative forced-choice task are larger than thresholds<br />

determined by the classical method of constant stimuli.<br />

(4050)<br />

Does Norwich’s Entropy <strong>The</strong>ory of Perception Actually Generate<br />

the Stevens’ and Weber–Fechner Laws? IFTIKHAR R. NIZAMI—<br />

Norwich’s “entropy equation,” supposedly derived from information<br />

theory, relates sensation strength to stimulus strength. At “low”<br />

strengths, the relation is approximately logarithmic—“the Weber–<br />

Fechner law”—and at “high” strengths, approximately a power function—“Stevens’<br />

law.” “Low” and “high” must be established through<br />

curve fitting, because the entropy equation has three unknowns, including<br />

a power exponent (y), allegedly “Stevens’ exponent” (x). To<br />

test whether y = x, the logarithmic forms of the entropy equation and<br />

Stevens’ Law were regression-fitted to 64 published plots of logarithm(magnitude-estimate)<br />

versus logarithm(stimulus-strength).<br />

y versus x is broadly scattered; 62/64 times, y � x. In theory, the fitted<br />

entropy equation allows calculation of the transmitted information.<br />

Constraining the fit to yield 2.5 bits/stimulus narrows the scatterplot<br />

so that, roughly, y = 1.7x. Neither the Weber–Fechner nor the<br />

Stevens approximations fully describe any examined sensationgrowth<br />

plot. Quantification of sensation growth by “physical entropy”<br />

(Norwich, Acta Biotheoretica, 2005) repeats these errors.<br />

• EVENT COGNITION •<br />

(4051)<br />

Cognitive Biases in Estimating the Similarity of Environmental<br />

Sounds. BRUNO L. GIORDANO, STEPHEN MCADAMS, & JOHN<br />

112<br />

MCDONNELL, McGill University—Relevant auditory features for<br />

the perception of sound events are often studied using similarity ratings.<br />

Acoustical features for perception are inferred from the association<br />

between similarities and acoustical parameters. This approach<br />

might lead to mistaken conclusions if participants are influenced by<br />

the similarity of the knowledge structures activated by the recognition<br />

of the sound source (e.g., train whistling, train wheels). <strong>The</strong> comparative<br />

relevance of acoustical and conceptual information to the estimation<br />

of the similarity of animate and inanimate environmental<br />

sounds was measured. Similarities were measured using the agglomerative<br />

hierarchical sorting technique. Participants judged either identification<br />

labels, or the acoustical properties of the sounds, or the<br />

sounds without further specification of the response criteria (conceptual,<br />

acoustical, and unbiased conditions, respectively). Unbiased estimation<br />

of the similarity of animate and inanimate sound events resembled<br />

more closely conceptual and acoustical similarities,<br />

respectively. Methodological and theoretical consequences are<br />

discussed.<br />

(4052)<br />

Using Autobiographical Memory Questionnaires As a Form of Exposure<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy for Stressful Events. LISA M. HATHAWAY &<br />

ADRIEL BOALS, University of North Texas—<strong>The</strong> present study investigated<br />

whether completing the autobiographical memory questionnaire<br />

(AMQ) for a stressful event results in a reduction in the emotionality<br />

and vividness of the event. At Time 1, 190 participants<br />

nominated a negative event from their lives. Participants in the experimental<br />

group completed the AMQ in reference to their nominated<br />

event, whereas the control group nominated six other events. Six<br />

weeks later, participants in both groups completed the AMQ in regards<br />

to the negative event nominated at Time 1. Participants in the<br />

experimental group reported significantly less emotional intensity and<br />

vividness of the event at Time 2, in comparison with the control group.<br />

No such pattern of group differences was observed for a second nominated<br />

stressful event also completed during Time 2. <strong>The</strong>se results<br />

suggest that completing the AMQ for a stressful memory results in a<br />

decrease in the emotional intensity and vividness of the event.<br />

(4053)<br />

Actor Similarity and Binding Errors in Event Memory. JULIE L.<br />

EARLES, ALAN W. KERSTEN, EILEEN S. CURTAYNE, &<br />

JONATHAN G. PERLE, Florida Atlantic University—Mistakes in<br />

eyewitness identification can occur when an eyewitness incorrectly<br />

associates a familiar actor with the actions of another person. <strong>The</strong><br />

present research demonstrates that actors do not need to be similar in<br />

appearance for such binding errors to occur. <strong>The</strong> actors can in fact be<br />

very different in appearance, even different sexes. Participants attempted<br />

to remember a series of brief events, each involving an actor<br />

performing a simple action. Participants were tested one week later<br />

on their recognition memory for the events. Increases in actor similarity<br />

led to increases in binding errors, in which participants falsely<br />

recognized a familiar actor performing a familiar action that had actually<br />

been performed by someone else. Binding errors often occurred<br />

even when the familiar actor was of a different sex than the original<br />

actor, however, providing strong evidence that these binding errors are<br />

not due solely to actor similarity.<br />

(4054)<br />

Freshman Flashbulbs: Exploring Flashbulb Memory Mechanisms in<br />

a Collegiate Sample. JENNIFER M. TALARICO, Lafayette College—<br />

Pilot data from the class of 2010 suggests that memories of distinctive,<br />

emotional, and personally significant events concerning the transition<br />

to college (e.g., meeting one’s roommate or attending one’s first<br />

collegiate class) satisfy the criteria for “flashbulb memory” status;<br />

participants remembered where they were, what they were doing, and<br />

had vivid visual images for these memories. In addition, they were<br />

quite confident that the event occurred as they remembered it. This<br />

allows for the exploration of potential mechanisms for the flashbulb

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!