29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Saturday Noon Posters 4035–4040<br />

formance in auditory lexical decision, but whether its influence extends<br />

to other tasks such as shadowing remains controversial. In a first experiment,<br />

we show that the emergence of the consistency effect depends<br />

on the degree of lexical involvement in shadowing auditory words. Consistency<br />

effects also emerged in two additional experiments using the<br />

semantic and the gender categorization tasks. A last shadowing experiment<br />

indicates that the magnitude of the orthographic effects reduces<br />

when words are embedded in sentence contexts. Simulation works lead<br />

us to propose that orthography does not necessarily mediate lexical access,<br />

but that learning to read and write might cause phonological representations<br />

to reflect the orthographic properties of words.<br />

(4035)<br />

Additional Evidence for Role of Focus Prosody in Silent Reading.<br />

JENNIFER GROSS, Grand Valley State University, BRIAN BARTEK,<br />

University of Michigan, & SAMANTHA RIECK & KYLE HAMPTON<br />

BREDELL, Grand Valley State University—<strong>The</strong> implicit prosody hypothesis<br />

(Fodor, 2002) contends that silent readers have a prosodic<br />

“inner voice.” To test this, participants read short paragraphs, in which<br />

select words were capitalized. For example, in the sentence, “Sam fell<br />

out of the canoe,” either “SAM” or “FELL” appeared in caps. <strong>The</strong><br />

paragraph in which this sentence appeared manipulated whether<br />

“Sam” or “fell” was new information (not previously mentioned), and<br />

therefore in prosodic focus, or old information furnished in the story.<br />

Participants judged (on a 5-point scale) the helpfulness of capped<br />

words, under the ruse of helping an editor determine intentional from<br />

nonintentional caps (caused by a computer virus). Experimental conditions—capitalized<br />

words and congruency of context—were fully<br />

crossed. Replicating and extending our previous work, participants<br />

rated the “capped” words as more helpful, only when these target<br />

words were “new” (i.e., prosodically focused) rather than “given,”<br />

consistent with Selkirk’s (1995) theory of focus projection.<br />

• DISCOURSE PROCESSES •<br />

(4036)<br />

An Electrophysiological Examination of Temporal Cue Use During<br />

Discourse Comprehension. TALI DITMAN & PHILLIP J. HOL-<br />

COMB, Tufts University, & GINA R. KUPERBERG, Tufts University<br />

and MGH (sponsored by Phillip J. Holcomb)—To examine the time<br />

course for the integration of temporal information into readers’ situation<br />

models, the present study recorded event-related brain potentials<br />

(ERPs) as participants read short scenarios with temporal continuities<br />

(after one second), small shifts (after one minute), or large shifts (after<br />

one year). Difficulty integrating temporal shifts into the present model<br />

was examined by N400 amplitude at the point of the shift<br />

(second/minute/year). Difficulty accessing information preceding the<br />

shift was examined by N400 amplitude to a repeated noun-phrase<br />

anaphor immediately following the shift. This type of anaphor is not<br />

preferred as a referent for a salient antecedent. <strong>The</strong> results demonstrated<br />

that participants had increased difficulty integrating large temporal<br />

shifts into the preceding discourse model as evidenced by the<br />

largest amplitude N400. Processing large temporal shifts also decreased<br />

the accessibility of preceding information, making it appropriate<br />

to employ a repeated noun-phrase anaphor, as evidenced by the<br />

smallest amplitude N400.<br />

(4037)<br />

Before We Begin: <strong>The</strong> Consequences of Introductions on Processing<br />

of Explanations. MICHAEL MENSINK, University of Minnesota,<br />

DAVID N. RAPP, Northwestern University, PANAYIOTA<br />

KENDEOU, McGill University, & R. BROOKE LEA, Macalester<br />

College—Topics can be introduced in a variety of ways, but do particular<br />

introductions influence how individuals process those topics?<br />

We assessed the influence of introductions on participants’ reading of<br />

brief scientific explanations. Participants read sixteen texts describing<br />

basic scientific processes (e.g., mitosis, reflexes). Texts included<br />

3-sentence introductions that (1) provided an engaging narrative in<br />

110<br />

which a character needed to provide the explanation or (2) described<br />

a technical rationale for why the explanation is worth reading about.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se manipulations were intended to emulate the engaging introductions<br />

or technical remarks that often preface explanatory materials.<br />

Participants took longer to read technical in comparison with narrative<br />

introductions as they unfolded. However, participants took<br />

longer to begin reading explanatory material following narrative in<br />

comparison with technical introductions. Introductory content did not<br />

differentially influence participants’ expectations as to their understanding<br />

of the texts. Introductions may influence reader attention to<br />

explanatory material without concomitant awareness of their effects.<br />

(4038)<br />

Three Information Functions of Headings. ROBERT F. LORCH, JR.,<br />

University of Kentucky, JULIE LEMARIÉ, University of Toulouse, Le<br />

Mirail, & WHITNEY C. O’ROURKE & SHAWN CANTY, University<br />

of Kentucky—A recent theory of signaling distinguishes six “information<br />

functions” performed by signaling devices. Signals may:<br />

(1) demarcate structural boundaries in a text; (2) emphasize content;<br />

(3) establish the function of a text subsection; (4) identify the topic of<br />

a subsection; (5) provide a label for a subsection; and/or (6) communicate<br />

organizational information. Three experiments isolated and<br />

tested three of the hypothesized information functions using a simple<br />

text search task. In Experiment 1, search times to locate target sentences<br />

were faster if the text contained topic-identifying headings than<br />

if the headings did not identify topics. In Experiment 2, search times<br />

were faster if the questions identified the subsections in which the targets<br />

were located. In Experiment 3, search times were faster if the<br />

questions identified the numbers of the relevant subsections. Thus, the<br />

experiments validate the information functions of topic identification,<br />

labeling, and organization.<br />

(4039)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Influence of Reading Goals on Text Recall. JONATHAN<br />

SCHUSTER & RUTHANN ATCHLEY, University of Kansas—This<br />

research examined the manner in which reading goals and organizational<br />

signals might act independently or interact with each other while reading<br />

to influence text recall. Participants read texts, which contained<br />

no signals, half signals, or full signals, and were assigned the goal of<br />

either reading for school or reading for pleasure. Significant reading<br />

goal differences were found, and there was a significant interaction<br />

that involved organizational signals and reading goal for overall recall.<br />

School goal participants recalled more information overall when<br />

signals were present than did pleasure goal participants; both groups<br />

were similar when signals were absent. Subsequently, we have also investigated<br />

the possible relationship between reading goals and working<br />

memory span to determine whether working memory span impacts<br />

what reading goals are available and how they are utilized.<br />

(4040)<br />

Monitoring Situational Content and Continuity While Reading.<br />

SARA GILLIAM & PETER W. FOLTZ, New Mexico State University,<br />

& JOSEPH P. MAGLIANO, Northern Illinois University (sponsored<br />

by Peter W. Foltz)—According to Zwaan, Langston, and Graesser’s<br />

(1995) event-indexing model, readers monitor changes in a series of<br />

critical dimensions: space, time, protagonist, causality, and goal.<br />

However, before readers can monitor for continuities across dimensions,<br />

they have to extract the situation model information that is present.<br />

<strong>The</strong> goal of this study was to explore the extent to which readers<br />

monitor both situational content and continuities while reading naturalistic<br />

discourse. Participants read four naturalistic texts, and sentence<br />

reading times were recorded. Discourse analyses were developed<br />

to determine the situational content and continuity of each<br />

sentence for characters, time, space, causality, and goals. <strong>The</strong> results<br />

suggest that reading times are influenced more by situational continuity<br />

than by content. Furthermore, consistent with prior research,<br />

causality is the dimension that is most closely monitored. Although<br />

readers may build representations based on situational content, mon-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!