29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Posters 4028–4034 Saturday Noon<br />

ken message of the navigation system corresponded. Moreover, inferior<br />

performance was found in older drivers but no stronger age-related<br />

compatibility effects. <strong>The</strong> compatibility effect completely vanished in<br />

older professional drivers, indicating that expertise reduces age-related<br />

compatibility effects in critical driving situations.<br />

(4028)<br />

Cognitive Control in Sense of Time. MARIA GRAZIA CARELLI &<br />

TIMO MÄNTYLÄ, University of Umeå—A number of patient studies<br />

suggest that impairments in executive/frontal functions are associated<br />

with disorders in temporal information processing. One implication<br />

of these findings is that subjective experience of time should be related<br />

to executive functions regardless of etiology. We examined sense<br />

of time in relation to components of executive functioning in healthy<br />

children and adults. Sense of time was based on tasks of varying complexity,<br />

including a time reproduction task in which participants completed<br />

discrete stimulus durations and a multitiming task in which participants<br />

reported observed temporal patterns with different onset and<br />

offset times. Individual differences in executive functioning were related<br />

to multitiming performance, rather than to time reproduction<br />

performance, in healthy participants. <strong>The</strong>se findings suggest that the<br />

psychophysical paradigms of interval timing are not easily translated<br />

to complex goal-directed tasks, and that clinical assessment of temporal<br />

processing should reflect complexities of everyday cognition.<br />

(4029)<br />

Measures of Working Memory Capacity That Work. JAMES M.<br />

BROADWAY & RANDALL W. ENGLE, Georgia Institute of Technology—We<br />

administered three types of immediate memory test<br />

(complex span, running span, and simple span) to assess their differential<br />

prediction of general fluid (gF) abilities. We also manipulated<br />

rate of presentation in running span, producing fast and slow versions,<br />

since previous researchers have proposed that fast and slow running<br />

span reflect different processes. <strong>The</strong> results indicated that the majority<br />

of predicted variance in gF was due to variance shared among the<br />

three span measures, with running and complex spans each making<br />

unique incremental contributions to prediction and simple span making<br />

little. Additionally, there was little support for the notion that fast<br />

and slow running span tap distinct psychological constructs or abilities<br />

that are differentially related to gF. We propose that running span<br />

is a useful and valid measure of working memory capacity because it<br />

requires subjects to cope with information overload and to discriminate<br />

among internally generated retrieval cues.<br />

• PSYCHOLINGUISTICS •<br />

(4030)<br />

Does Lexical Activation Flow From Word Meanings to Word Sounds<br />

During Spoken Word Recognition? EILING YEE & SHARON L.<br />

THOMPSON-SCHILL, University of Pennsylvania—During spoken<br />

word recognition, lexical activation flows from word sounds to word<br />

meanings, whereas during production, activation flows in the opposite<br />

direction. If the production and comprehension systems share the<br />

same architecture, then during comprehension, some amount of activation<br />

might also flow from meaning to sound. We investigate this<br />

question by monitoring eye movements during spoken word recognition.<br />

Participants heard a word while viewing a four-object array. In<br />

critical trials, the name of one of the objects sounded similar to a word<br />

related in meaning to the heard word (e.g., for the heard word “key,”<br />

there was a picture of “logs,” because “logs” is related in sound to<br />

“lock,” which is related in meaning to “key”). We found that participants<br />

preferentially fixate on the object whose name sounds similar<br />

to a word related in meaning to the heard word, suggesting that information<br />

flows from meaning to sound during comprehension.<br />

(4031)<br />

Subliminal Speech Priming for Words and Nonwords: Evidence<br />

From Eyetracking. LARA TAGLIAPIETRA & HOLGER MITTERER,<br />

109<br />

Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics (sponsored by James M.<br />

McQueen)—Kouider and Dupoux (2005) obtained masked auditory<br />

repetition priming for words, but not for nonwords, in lexical decision.<br />

We tested whether this pattern is task specific. Participants heard unmasked<br />

word and nonword targets, which were preceded (with either<br />

a 0- or a 400-msec delay) by identical or unrelated masked primes.<br />

Eye movements were tracked as participants looked at a display of<br />

four printed letter strings (2 words and 2 nonwords), which included<br />

the target to be clicked on and the unrelated prime. Looks to both word<br />

and nonword targets occurred earlier after identical primes than after<br />

unrelated primes. Unrelated primes also triggered looks to their visual<br />

counterparts. This effect occurred for words and nonwords when target<br />

presentation was delayed by 400 msec, but only for words when<br />

targets immediately followed the primes. <strong>The</strong>se results demonstrate<br />

masked auditory priming for nonwords and suggest that lexicality effects<br />

appear under particular experimental circumstances.<br />

(4032)<br />

Directed Imitation in Conversational Interaction. JENNIFER S.<br />

PARDO & ISABEL CAJORI JAY, Barnard College, & ALEXANDRA<br />

C. SUPPES & ROBERT M. KRAUSS, Columbia University—Previous<br />

research found that paired talkers converged in acoustic–phonetic<br />

forms over the course of a single conversational interaction. However,<br />

convergence was subtle and asymmetrical, leading to the conclusion<br />

that speech perception does not reflexively elicit imitation in speech<br />

production. In the present study, one member of a pair of talkers was<br />

explicitly instructed to try to imitate his/her partner in a paired conversational<br />

task. Twenty-four talkers provided speech before, during,<br />

and after task performance. Repeated phrases from the corpus comprised<br />

materials for AXB similarity tests performed by separate listeners.<br />

<strong>The</strong> listeners judged similarity in pronunciation of consonants<br />

and vowels, similarity in vowels alone, and imitation. Overall, talkers<br />

became more similar in phonetic forms, and the degree of similarity/imitation<br />

was influenced by imitation instruction, task role, and the<br />

talker’s sex. <strong>The</strong>se findings demonstrate the importance of nonlinguistic<br />

factors in speech production and perception.<br />

(4033)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Time Course of Pragmatic Context Integration in Homophone<br />

Ambiguity Resolution. DANIEL MIRMAN, JAMES S. MAGNUSON,<br />

TED STRAUSS, & JAMES A. DIXON, University of Connecticut<br />

(sponsored by James A. Dixon)—Syntactic, thematic, pragmatic, and<br />

visual contexts all help listeners recognize ambiguous words, such as<br />

homophones. In interactive theories, context is integrated continuously,<br />

but in autonomous theories, there is an initial, encapsulated<br />

stage of processing protected from context. We examined whether a<br />

strong pragmatic expectation for concrete nouns could mediate ambiguity<br />

resolution. We tracked eye movements as subjects saw displays<br />

of four images of concrete objects and selected one that was<br />

named in a spoken instruction. <strong>The</strong> entire time course of processing<br />

showed greater activation of semantic associates of contextually appropriate<br />

(i.e., noun) meanings than contextually inappropriate (i.e.,<br />

verb) meanings. <strong>The</strong> early, continuous, and graded pattern of context<br />

effects and the consequent effect on semantic associates rule out<br />

decision-level integration accounts of these results. Rather, these results<br />

indicate that context acts directly as a graded constraint on meaning<br />

activation.<br />

(4034)<br />

Does Orthography Matter in Auditory Word Recognition? Converging<br />

Evidence From Different Tasks. RONALD PEEREMAN &<br />

IMANE BENBOUSSELHAM, University of Bourgogne, SOPHIE<br />

DUFOUR, University of Geneva, & PATRICK BONIN, LAPSCO/CNRS<br />

and Université Blaise Pascal—Although many studies indicate that<br />

phonology shapes visual word recognition, it is currently unclear<br />

whether reciprocal influences of orthography occur during auditory<br />

word recognition. Previous data indicate that the consistency of the<br />

mappings between phonological and orthographic units modulates per-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!