29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Saturday Noon Posters 4021–4027<br />

(4021)<br />

Neighbor Frequency Effects in English: Parallels With Lexical<br />

Ambiguity Resolution? TIMOTHY J. SLATTERY, KEITH RAYNER,<br />

& ALEXANDER POLLATSEK, University of Massachusetts,<br />

Amherst—Two eye movement experiments were conducted to investigate<br />

whether the processing of a word can be affected by the word’s<br />

higher frequency neighbor. In Experiment 1, target words with (birch)<br />

or without (spruce) higher frequency neighbors were embedded into<br />

two types of sentence frames: one in which the higher frequency<br />

neighbor (birth) could fit given the prior sentence context and one in<br />

which it could not. Experiment 2 used the same design with target<br />

words that were both neighbors and homonyms (beech, beach) to investigate<br />

the role of phonology. <strong>The</strong> results suggest that sentence context<br />

can affect the processing of word neighbors in much the same way<br />

it affects lexical ambiguity resolution. <strong>The</strong> separate meanings of a lexically<br />

ambiguous word, then, may be thought of as extreme forms of<br />

word neighbors (word roommates). Implications for models of word<br />

recognition and lexical ambiguity resolution will be discussed.<br />

• COGNITIVE CONTROL •<br />

(4022)<br />

Error Repetitions and Attention: Examination Within the Older<br />

Adults Group. ETSUKO T. HARADA, Hosei University, & SATORU<br />

SUTO, Chuo University—<strong>The</strong> phenomenon of error repetition—that<br />

elder users of IT-based equipment often show continual repetitions of<br />

the same erroneous operations in daily lives and the usability testing<br />

labs—was investigated with a simple kanji selection task, and we<br />

found that it was related to attention, especially the function of maintaining<br />

multiple goals in both age groups, younger and older adults<br />

(Harada & Suto, 2006). In this study, we examined this hypothesis further<br />

within the older adults group, by adding another simple taskswitching<br />

task, the figure selection task. Both of the following results<br />

supported the hypothesis: (1) reexamination of preceding experimental<br />

results about the relation between the kanji selection task and the<br />

figure selection task, and (2) the experiment in which good and poor<br />

older groups divided with the figure selection task were compared directly<br />

in terms of error repetition frequency. <strong>The</strong> implication of this<br />

hypothesis to human-error research will be also discussed.<br />

(4023)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Source of Enhanced Cognitive Control in Bilinguals: Evidence<br />

From Bimodal-Bilinguals. GIGI LUK, York University, JENNIE E.<br />

PYERS, Wellesley College, KAREN EMMOREY, San Diego State University,<br />

& ELLEN BIALYSTOK, York University—Bilinguals often<br />

outperform monolinguals on nonverbal tasks that require resolving<br />

conflict from competing alternatives. <strong>The</strong> regular need to select a target<br />

language is argued to enhance cognitive control. To investigate<br />

whether this enhancement stems from a general effect of bilingualism<br />

(the representation of two languages) or from an articulatory constraint<br />

that forces language selection, we compared the performance<br />

of 12 bimodal-bilinguals (fluent in a signed and spoken language, and<br />

hence free from articulatory conflict) with that of 15 monolinguals<br />

and 15 unimodal-bilinguals on a computerized cognitive control task<br />

(a flanker paradigm). <strong>The</strong>re was no difference between the groups in<br />

accuracy, but unimodal-bilinguals were faster in response time than<br />

the other groups; bimodal-bilinguals did not differ from monolinguals.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se results trace the bilingual advantage in cognitive control<br />

to the unimodal-bilingual experience where the production demand on<br />

a single output system creates an ongoing conflict that must be resolved<br />

through attention and selection.<br />

(4024)<br />

Effects of Bilingualism and Aging in Multitasking. MYTHILI<br />

VISWANATHAN & ELLEN BIALYSTOK, York University—Previous<br />

research has shown that bilingualism offsets age-related losses in executive<br />

processes (Bialystok et al., 2004). <strong>The</strong> present study investigated<br />

the role of bilingualism and aging in a dual-task classification para-<br />

108<br />

digm. Younger (30–40) and older (60–80) monolinguals and bilinguals<br />

performed a visual classification task in which they designated pictures<br />

to one of two categories by a manual response at the same time<br />

as performing an auditory classification task with a verbal response.<br />

Younger adults and bilinguals (in both age groups) were faster in responding<br />

to both tasks, but older monolinguals often stopped performing<br />

the auditory task because of processing overload. <strong>The</strong> RT difference<br />

between responding to each task was calculated to determine<br />

how responses were executed. Bilinguals were more likely to respond<br />

to both tasks simultaneously, whereas monolinguals were more likely<br />

to switch back and forth. <strong>The</strong>se data point to differences in executive<br />

control in a multitasking context for bilinguals.<br />

(4025)<br />

Does Musical Expertise Enhance Executive Functioning? ANNE-<br />

MARIE DEPAPE & ELLEN BIALYSTOK, York University, &<br />

TAKAKO FUJIOKA & FERGUS I. M. CRAIK, Rotman Research Institute<br />

(sponsored by Ellen Bialystok)—Following evidence for bilingual<br />

advantages in cognitive control tasks, the purpose of this study<br />

was to determine whether musical expertise provided parallel benefits<br />

in executive functioning. <strong>The</strong> sample included 95 young adults<br />

(24 years) who were identified by self-report as being monolingual<br />

speakers, bilingual speakers, monolingual instrumentalists, or monolingual<br />

singers. Participants completed two executive control tasks in<br />

which they resolved visual (Simon arrows task) or auditory (auditory<br />

Stroop task) conflict. In the Simon arrows task, the bilinguals and both<br />

musician groups outperformed the monolinguals, with no differences<br />

between the first three groups. In the auditory Stroop task, both musician<br />

groups outperformed the monolinguals and bilinguals in the<br />

pitch condition, whereas no differences were found between these<br />

groups in the word condition. <strong>The</strong> findings indicate that extensive musical<br />

training can promote enhanced executive functioning, both for<br />

domain general (visual Simon task) and domain specific (auditory<br />

Stroop task) performance.<br />

(4026)<br />

Cross-Task Correspondence Effects in Driving. JOCHEN MÜS-<br />

SELER, RWTH Aachen University, & GISA ASCHERSLEBEN &<br />

KATRIN ARNING, University of the Saarland—In a previous study,<br />

we demonstrated cross-task correspondence effects between response<br />

location in a primary task and the location at which stimuli are presented<br />

in a secondary task. In the present experiments, this cross-task<br />

effect is examined in a critical driving situation: <strong>The</strong> secondary task<br />

required listening to the spoken message of the navigation system<br />

(“turn left [right]”), whereas the primary task required reacting with<br />

a left or right steering wheel response to a pedestrian suddenly entering<br />

the street. Findings denoted that when the pedestrian enters from<br />

the right (left) side, a right (left) message from the navigation system<br />

impairs performance as it conflicts with the left (right) steering wheel<br />

response to avoid hitting the pedestrian. In other words, superior performance<br />

is observed when spoken message and direction of the steering<br />

wheel response correspond than when they do not.<br />

(4027)<br />

Influence of Age and Expertise on Cross-Task Compatibility in<br />

Driving. KATRIN ARNING & GISA ASCHERSLEBEN, University<br />

of the Saarland, & JOCHEN MÜSSELER, RWTH Aachen University<br />

(sponsored by Jochen Müsseler)—Dual-task conditions impose high<br />

demands on older drivers, especially in critical traffic situations. Factors<br />

influencing driving performance such as dual-task demands, crosstask<br />

compatibility, age, and driving expertise (nonprofessional vs. professional<br />

drivers) were analyzed. Participants performed a driving task<br />

(primary task), in which a collision with a pedestrian suddenly entering<br />

the street from the side (from the left/right) had to be avoided with a<br />

steering wheel response (to the left/right). <strong>The</strong> secondary task was to<br />

listen to the spoken message (“turn left [right]”) of a navigation system.<br />

<strong>The</strong> results showed superior driving performance in nonprofessional<br />

drivers when the directions of the steering wheel response and the spo-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!