29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Saturday Noon Posters 4008–4013<br />

Normal University, & PING LI, University of Richmond—Zevin and<br />

Seidenberg (2002) suggested that there may be a lasting advantage for<br />

early learned words in Chinese because of the more arbitrary nature<br />

of the script. We investigated age of acquisition (AoA) effects with behavioral,<br />

electrophysiological, and brain imaging methods on Chinese<br />

single-character lexical decision. Experiment 1 used a factorial manipulation<br />

and found AoA effects when other variables were controlled.<br />

Experiment 2 confirmed these results using multiple regression<br />

methods. In Experiment 3, EEG data confirmed wave amplitude<br />

differences between early and late acquired items. In Experiment 4,<br />

event-related fMRI revealed that early and late acquired items activated<br />

different brain regions including frontal and precuneus cortices.<br />

We argue that AoA effects exist in the recognition of characters in<br />

Chinese, which is compatible with the arbitrary mapping hypothesis.<br />

(4008)<br />

Late Semantic Age of Acquisition Effects in Reading: Evidence<br />

From Eye Movements. BARBARA J. JUHASZ, Wesleyan University,<br />

& MARGARET GULLICK, Dartmouth College—Words that are acquired<br />

early in life are recognized faster than words acquired later in<br />

life. Previous studies have observed age of acquisition (AoA) effects<br />

on fixation durations during reading (Juhasz & Rayner, 2003, 2006).<br />

<strong>The</strong> present study explored the locus of this AoA effect. Balanced ambiguous<br />

words (tick) were selected for which both meanings of the<br />

word (clock sound, insect) were equally frequent, but one meaning<br />

was rated as being acquired earlier. <strong>The</strong>se ambiguous words were embedded<br />

in sentence contexts that disambiguated the meaning of the<br />

ambiguous word; readers’ eye movements were then recorded. Meaning<br />

AoA affected late processing measures on the ambiguous word,<br />

including second-pass reading time. <strong>The</strong> effect of meaning AoA also<br />

spilled over onto the posttarget region. A comparison of these late semantic<br />

AoA effects with previous studies suggests that AoA affects<br />

multiple stages in the lexical processing system. Implications for<br />

models of word recognition will be discussed.<br />

(4009)<br />

Hemispheric Differences in Processing Early and Late AoA Words<br />

for Left- and Right-Handed Adults. J. MICHAEL BOWERS &<br />

SHELIA M. KENNISON, Oklahoma State University (sponsored by<br />

Shelia M. Kennison)—<strong>The</strong> research extended prior studies showing<br />

that there are hemispheric differences in the processing of words<br />

learned early in childhood (early AoA) versus words learned later in<br />

life (late AoA) (Bowers & Kennison, 2007). <strong>The</strong>se prior studies<br />

showed that there is greater right hemisphere involvement in the processing<br />

of early AoA words than of late AoA words. <strong>The</strong> present research<br />

compared the hemispheric differences in the processing of early<br />

and late AoA words for right-handed and left-handed adults. Three<br />

groups of participants were compared: (1) left-handed adults; (2) righthanded<br />

adults with only right-handed biological relatives; and<br />

(3) right-handed adults with some left-handed biological relatives. Participants<br />

viewed early and late AoA words presented briefly to either<br />

the left or the right visual field and carried out a lexical decision task.<br />

<strong>The</strong> results showed that there were significant hemispheric differences<br />

in the processing of late AoA words by left- and right-handed adults.<br />

(4010)<br />

<strong>The</strong>re Are Many Ways to Be Rich: Effects of Three Measures of<br />

Semantic Richness on Visual Word Recognition. PENNY M. PEX-<br />

MAN & IAN S. HARGREAVES, University of Calgary, PAUL D.<br />

SIAKALUK, University of Northern British Columbia, & GLEN E.<br />

BODNER & JAMIE POPE, University of Calgary—Previous studies<br />

have reported facilitatory effects of semantic richness on word recognition<br />

(e.g., Buchanan et al., 2001; Pexman et al., 2003). We compared<br />

the ability of three richness measures to account for reaction time (RT)<br />

and error variance in lexical decision and semantic categorization<br />

tasks. <strong>The</strong> measures were: (1) number of semantic neighbors (NSN;<br />

the number of words appearing in similar linguistic contexts),<br />

(2) number of features (NoF; the number of attributes listed for a<br />

106<br />

word’s referent), and (3) contextual dispersion (CD; the distribution<br />

of a word’s occurrences across contexts). NoF and CD accounted for<br />

unique variance in both tasks, whereas NSN accounted for unique<br />

variance only in the lexical decision task. Moreover, each measure<br />

showed a different pattern of relative contribution across the tasks.<br />

Our results provide new clues about how words are represented, and<br />

favor word recognition models that incorporate both lexical–semantic<br />

and episodic influences.<br />

(4011)<br />

Multiple Semantic Associates for a Single Individual With Free<br />

Association. LANCE W. HAHN, Western Kentucky University—<br />

Traditionally, free association tasks have acquired a single response<br />

for each cue word across several participants, and the results have been<br />

treated as a set of weighted associations for an idealized participant.<br />

One disadvantage of this approach is that it obscures individual differences.<br />

Monte Carlo simulations suggest a second disadvantage:<br />

Collecting single responses may undersample secondary associations.<br />

<strong>The</strong> presence of a strong associate can diminish the likelihood of a<br />

second moderate associate being produced in a single-response free<br />

association task. Single-response and multiple-response free association<br />

tasks were used to create a set of associations. <strong>The</strong> results suggest<br />

that the multiple-response task produces larger association strengths<br />

than does the single-response task for secondary associations. Additionally,<br />

the multiple-response task can be used to measure individual<br />

and group differences in semantic associations.<br />

(4012)<br />

Does Prior Knowledge Affect Distraction? <strong>The</strong> Effects of Aging<br />

and Music Expertise. ELIZABETH R. GRAHAM & GABRIELLE<br />

OSBORNE, Claremont Graduate University, & DEBORAH M.<br />

BURKE, Pomona College—Visual distractors semantically related to<br />

a text slow reading time more for older than young adults. We investigated<br />

whether these effects reflect generic age-related inhibition<br />

deficits or older adults’ enriched semantic network. Young and older<br />

adults with low or high knowledge about music read passages about<br />

music or a control topic; passages contained related, unrelated, or no<br />

distractors. Older adults’ reading times were slowed more by distractors<br />

than were young adults’. Reading times for music passages with<br />

related distractors were slower for related than for unrelated distractors<br />

for older adults but only for high-knowledge young adults.<br />

Recognition memory was better for targets than for distractors, with<br />

a larger effect for young than for older adults for the control passage.<br />

For music passages, however, superior recognition of targets occurred<br />

for low- but not for high-knowledge participants across age. Thus,<br />

prior knowledge modifies age differences in distraction, suggesting<br />

the importance of richer semantic networks for performance.<br />

(4013)<br />

Behavioral and Anatomical Correlates of Corpus Callosum Size.<br />

CHRISTINE CHIARELLO & SUZANNE E. WELCOME, University<br />

of California, Riverside, LAURA K. HALDERMAN, University of<br />

Pittsburgh, STEPHEN TOWLER, University of Florida, Gainesville,<br />

RONALD OTTO, Riverside Imaging LLC, & CHRISTIANA M.<br />

LEONARD, University of Florida, Gainesville—<strong>The</strong> corpus callosum<br />

varies widely in size and shape across individuals. We investigated<br />

anatomical and behavioral correlates of this variation in 200 young<br />

adults. Size of the total corpus callosum and seven subregions was<br />

measured from structural MRI scans, and the participants were tested<br />

in seven divided visual field lexical tasks. Contrary to prior findings<br />

with smaller samples, there was no relationship between corpus callosum<br />

size and either behavioral or anatomical asymmetries. <strong>The</strong>re<br />

were no overall sex differences in corpus callosum anatomy. However,<br />

we did obtain a sex � handedness interaction in a subsample of<br />

matched left- and right-handers: <strong>The</strong> splenium and isthmus were<br />

larger in left-handers than in right-handers, only for males. Variations<br />

in corpus callosum anatomy may not be associated with simple measures<br />

of structural or behavioral asymmetries.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!