29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Posters 3112–3118 Friday Evening<br />

(3112)<br />

Capacity Limits of Spatial Updating: Online and Offline Updating.<br />

ERIC HODGSON & DAVID WALLER, Miami University (sponsored<br />

by Leonard S. Mark)—Keeping track of changing self-to-object spatial<br />

relations is generally considered to be capacity limited. However,<br />

the limits of updating seem to depend on whether objects are updated<br />

online (i.e., in real time) or offline (i.e., reconstructed after movement).<br />

Previous results (Hodgson & Waller, 2006) indicate that spatial<br />

updating through a 135º rotation involves offline processing and<br />

is unaffected by the number of targets. In contrast, participants in the<br />

present experiment were required to keep track of the locations of between<br />

1 and 10 objects through a smaller, 45º rotation (putatively requiring<br />

only online processing). Set size effects were found in both<br />

error and latency, indicating a capacity limit of online updating around<br />

6 items. An additional experiment showed that updating capacity depends<br />

both on the number of updated objects and on the magnitude of<br />

the movement, such that the further one rotates, the fewer objects one<br />

can update online.<br />

(3113)<br />

Can People Determine Parallel and Perpendicular Paths in Active<br />

Navigation? ELIZABETH R. CHRASTIL & WILLIAM H. WARREN,<br />

JR., Brown University—<strong>The</strong>ories of path integration and cognitive map<br />

construction often assume that metric information about distances and<br />

angles is acquired during walking. Moreover, a map with Euclidean<br />

or affine structure must preserve parallelism. In the present study, we<br />

test whether traversed angles can be used to determine parallel and<br />

perpendicular paths in an ambulatory virtual environment. Participants<br />

performed a shortcut task in which they walked down a main<br />

path, took a 90º turn onto a side path, then a variable angle turn, and<br />

finally were asked to walk perpendicular or parallel to the main path.<br />

Participants were expected to exhibit large variable errors and a 90º<br />

bias, over-turning acute angles and under-turning obtuse angles. This<br />

task dissociates angle from distance, which are confounded in triangle<br />

completion tasks. <strong>The</strong> results can be used to predict path integration<br />

performance and have implications for the accuracy and geometric<br />

structure of cognitive maps.<br />

(3114)<br />

Route Angularity Effects on Distance Estimation in a Virtual Environment.<br />

ADAM T. HUTCHESON, DOUGLAS H. WEDELL, &<br />

DAVID E. CLEMENT, University of South Carolina—Research has<br />

shown that increasing the number of turns a route takes through the<br />

environment increases estimates of distance, known as the route angularity<br />

effect. Two studies examined mental processes responsible<br />

for the occurrence of the route angularity effect in virtual settings.<br />

Study 1 tested the plausibility of a scaling explanation of the route angularity<br />

effect using a paradigm that has produced the effect in the<br />

past. <strong>The</strong> route angularity effect was found, but the data did not support<br />

the scaling hypothesis. Study 2 examined the role of memory in<br />

the route angularity effect, with memory load manipulated at encoding<br />

or interference manipulated prior to retrieval. <strong>The</strong> results demonstrated<br />

that the route angularity effect was significantly increased by<br />

a memory load at encoding but that it was unaffected by the inference<br />

task prior to retrieval. Both studies show a greater magnitude of underestimation<br />

as the actual path length increases.<br />

• WORKING MEMORY •<br />

(3115)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Impact of Perception–Action Incompatibility on Auditory Serial<br />

Recall. ROBERT W. HUGHES, JOHN E. MARSH, & DYLAN M.<br />

JONES, Cardiff University (sponsored by Fabrice Parmentier)—<strong>The</strong><br />

talker-variability effect in serial recall—the impairment of auditory–<br />

verbal serial recall when successive to-be-remembered items are spoken<br />

in different voices—is typically explained by recourse to item loss<br />

through decay within a bespoke short-term store. We provide evidence<br />

that the effect can be reattributed to the incompatibility between the<br />

103<br />

automatic auditory perception of order and the action requirement to<br />

reproduce the veridical order of the to-be-remembered items: Factors<br />

that promote the automatic perceptual encoding of order by voice<br />

rather than according to the true temporal order of the items exacerbates<br />

the effect. Moreover, whereas tasks that require memory for<br />

order (serial recall and probe tasks) are susceptible, tasks that call only<br />

for item memory (missing-item task) are not. We suggest that the<br />

talker variability effect can be added to a growing list of short-term<br />

“memory” phenomena that may be reconstrued as reflecting the constraints<br />

on perceptual and gestural-planning processes.<br />

(3116)<br />

Phonological Similarity Effects in Verbal Working Memory and Language<br />

Production Tasks. DANIEL J. ACHESON & MARYELLEN C.<br />

MACDONALD, University of Wisconsin, Madison—Similar patterns<br />

of errors have been observed in language production and verbal working<br />

memory (WM) tasks under conditions of phonological similarity<br />

(Ellis, 1980). Such errors have been attributed to item-ordering<br />

processes in the WM literature but to phoneme ordering processes in<br />

language production. <strong>The</strong> nature of serial ordering mechanisms was<br />

investigated with a tongue-twister (TT) paradigm. Lists were composed<br />

of four nonwords, with ±TT status varying only through nonword<br />

ordering (e.g., TT list: tiff deeve diff teeve; non-TT list: tiff teeve<br />

diff deeve). Memory demands varied across four levels: simple reading<br />

(Experiment 1), immediate serial recall, immediate serial typing,<br />

and serial recognition (Experiments 2, 3 and 4, respectively). Robust<br />

effects of TT ordering were observed despite constant phonological<br />

overlap across items. Relative to item serial-position analyses,<br />

phoneme error analyses provided insight into the serial ordering<br />

process shared by language production and verbal WM. Implications<br />

for theories of WM are discussed.<br />

(3117)<br />

Improving Strategy Choice in Individuals With Low Working Memory<br />

Capacity. MELANIE CARY, University of Wisconsin, La Crosse,<br />

& MARSHA C. LOVETT, Carnegie Mellon University—How do individuals<br />

with low versus high working memory capacity differ in<br />

their strategy choices? Prior research (Cary & Lovett, 2004) examined<br />

this question for an income-calculation task where the two primary solution<br />

strategies were both viable but differed in working memory demands.<br />

<strong>The</strong> results showed that high-capacity individuals were more<br />

likely to adapt their choices toward the less-demanding strategy,<br />

whereas low-capacity individuals were more likely to stick with their<br />

initial strategy, even when it was the more demanding one. In the present<br />

study, two experiments explored alternative approaches to improving<br />

the strategy choices made by low-capacity individuals. One experiment<br />

examined the impact of providing low-capacity participants<br />

with a subtle instructional bias toward the less-demanding strategy.<br />

<strong>The</strong> other experiment examined the impact of providing low-capacity<br />

and high-capacity participants with example problems to illustrate the<br />

use of both strategies. Our analyses focus on participants’ strategy<br />

choice and adaptation in each case.<br />

(3118)<br />

Individual Differences in Interference From Auditory Distractors:<br />

<strong>The</strong> Role of Working Memory Capacity. EMILY M. ELLIOTT,<br />

JILL A. SHELTON, & SHARON D. LYNN, Louisiana State University,<br />

& CANDICE C. MOREY, University of Missouri, Columbia—Irrelevant<br />

sounds are pervasive, and can disrupt cognitive performance. <strong>The</strong><br />

goals of the present study were to investigate individual differences<br />

in performance in the presence of auditory distractors and to assess<br />

the relationship of these differences with multiple indices of working<br />

memory capacity. Our findings suggest that goal maintenance and the<br />

ability to avoid distraction from irrelevant sounds were both important<br />

in determining the relationship between working memory and<br />

performance in a cross-modal version of the Stroop task. We found<br />

that out of three working memory measures, only operation span, a<br />

storage-plus-processing task emphasizing attentional control, showed

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!