17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Chapter</strong> 4 - Planning of <strong>Prehension</strong> 77<br />

measured while the posture was maintained for a few seconds. In this<br />

pad opposition task, mean grip size correlated positively with object<br />

size. Error in matching was computed for each subject as the<br />

difference between the grip size used during normal grasping and the<br />

person’s measured grip size. While object size estimation was shown<br />

to be linearly related to the actual object size, errors ranged between<br />

f1.5 cm with very little consistency (of the six subjects, two subjects<br />

always underestimated while the rest tended to overestimated). This<br />

experiment demonstrates the ability of the CNS to plan an opposition<br />

space, but with qualifications. The visual perception of object<br />

properties is an estimation (visual scaling) which is shown to be<br />

highly accurate. Transforming this visual information into motor<br />

commands (visuomotor scaling) causes errors, which Jeannerod and<br />

Decety argue could stem from the change from one coordinate system<br />

to another. Visual objects are encoded in retinal coordinates, whereas<br />

hand configurations are encoded in some other frame. Retinal cues<br />

related to object size are not sufficient for determining grip size,<br />

particularly in a precision task using pad opposition. They suggest<br />

that for accuracy, visual feedback is needed to improve the planned<br />

hand posture for correcting visuomotor biases and reducing motor<br />

variability.<br />

In a similar experiment, Chan, Carello, and Turvey (1990) asked<br />

subjects to use their other hand to evaluate the size of the object. An<br />

object was placed in front of the subject, who then used the thumb and<br />

index finger of the left hand to indicate the size of the object. Subjects<br />

were able to do this accurately in a linear fashion for objects below a<br />

10 cm width. Above that, there were non-linearities due to a ceiling<br />

effect; that is, there were biomechanical constraints on the hand.<br />

Using the thumb and middle finger removed these non-linearities.<br />

The speed with which object properties are perceived is important.<br />

Klatzky et al. (1987, 1990) analyzed the two-dimensional perception<br />

of objects, arguing that the projection of a three dimensional object on<br />

the retina can be used for making decisions about interacting with the<br />

object. By varying the area and depth of the two-dimensional<br />

projection of an object, they showed an effect on the chosen grasp.<br />

As seen in Figure 4.6, subjects pinched small things and clenched<br />

larger things. Since object size is a critical planning parameter, it is<br />

noteworthy that a simple two-dimensional representation of the object<br />

can be used for choosing a grasp posture. This representation is<br />

available at the level of the retina, thus quickly accessible to the CNS.<br />

Intrinsic object properties can be perturbed at movement onset in<br />

order to observe how long it takes to perceive object properties, or at

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!