17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

318 CONSTRAINTS AND PHASES<br />

spent in the deceleration phase was predicted by ID as well or better<br />

than MT. This was not the case for acceleration time. Only movement<br />

amplitude affected the time to peak velocity. Thus, amplitude and<br />

target size effects were dissociable in that the shape of the tangential<br />

velocity profile was a function of target size (accuracy), and the peak<br />

speed along the path of the trajectories was scaled according to<br />

movement amplitude.<br />

In grasping and aiming tasks (Marteniuk et al. 1987), precision<br />

requirements were varied, showing how intention, context, and object<br />

properties affect timing parameters of prehensile movements. The first<br />

experiment varied goal (pointing to a target or grasping a disk); the<br />

second one varied object properties (grasping compliant tennis ball or<br />

fragile light bulb); the third one varied movement intent (placing or<br />

throwing an object. Using Fitts’ Law to equalize the ID’S, it was<br />

observed that the grasping took longer than pointing. The percentage<br />

of time in the deceleration phase was longer for grasping than<br />

pointing, for grasping the light bulb than grasping the tennis ball, and<br />

for placing than throwing. They argued that all these effects could be<br />

due to the requirement for ‘precision’. Fitts’ Law predicts that MT<br />

increases with aiming precision requirements, but they demonstrate<br />

this increase is due to the lengthening of the deceleration phase<br />

disproportionately to the rest of the movement. As well, they show<br />

other influences, such as object properties and task intent. Less<br />

variability between conditions was seen in the early or acceleration<br />

phase of the movement, and more variability during the deceleration<br />

phase. They argued that the early part of the movement is more likely<br />

to be directly influenced by central stereotyped movement planning or<br />

programming, while the later part of the movement, during the<br />

deceleration phase, uses more sensorimotor adjustments for<br />

controlling the movement, causing more variability. Increasing<br />

precision requirements of a task may induce subjects to use more<br />

sensory information, particularly in the ‘homing in’ part of the task.<br />

This relates to Jeannerod (1984), where it was argued that an initial<br />

ballistic phase places the hand into the vicinity of the target, and a<br />

second phase using feedback guides the hand to the target. It is<br />

interesting to note that in pointing tasks, where subjects pointed to<br />

imaginary targets in space, velocity profiles are symmetrical (Atkeson<br />

& Hollerbach, 1985).<br />

Another dimension to task requirements, in contrast to precision,<br />

is the anticipated forces acting in the task. In Iberall et al. (1986), an<br />

informal study is reported to observe the effect of anticipated forces on<br />

the posture chosen to grasp a vertically standing dowel. Subjects were

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!