17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Chapter</strong> 8 - Constraints on Human <strong>Prehension</strong> 317<br />

While these are merely anecdotal evidence for such effects on<br />

prehension, kinesiologists and psychologists have begun to explore<br />

the functional issues. Movement constraints act within and around the<br />

intentions of the performer, which delineate goals for a movement.<br />

An example would be the 'sherry-glass response' (Traub, Rothwell &<br />

Marsden, 1980). Subjects were asked to maintain their thumb and<br />

index finger a few millimeters from the rim of a full glass of sherry. A<br />

perturbation was made to the wrist sufficient to cause the hand to hit<br />

the glass and knock it over. A short-latency grab response (50 msec)<br />

occurred in the thumb muscle in response to the perturbation, thereby<br />

saving the glass from being knocked over. However, the response<br />

was observed only when the subject's intent was to prevent the glass<br />

from falling over. Traub et al. argued that this suggests the presence<br />

of a grab reflex, where the digits respond within a time too short to be<br />

voluntary, flexing in order to maintain contact but only if contact is the<br />

person's in tent.<br />

In a formal empirical situation, intent emerges from the<br />

experimenter's request to the subject to perform a certain task. In<br />

MacKenzie et al. (1987) for example, the subjects were asked to point<br />

with a stylus 'as quickly and as accurately as possible' to a target of<br />

varying size and at varying distances. The question being asked was<br />

whether there was a reliable kinematic measure of the speed and<br />

accuracy requirements of the task. In this case, Fitts' Law (Fitts<br />

1954) was used, which states that movement time (MT) is directly<br />

proportional to the index of difficulty (ID) of the task, or MT = a + b x<br />

ID where the ID is the log2(2A/W), A is the amplitude of movement<br />

(an extrinsic object property), and W is the width of target, or target<br />

tolerance (an intrinsic object property). When plotting MT against ID,<br />

a linear relationship is seen. MacKenzie et al. (1987) measured the<br />

MT of the tip of the stylus, its time to peak resultant velocity, and the<br />

percentage of movement time after peak resultant velocity. They<br />

found a differential effect of target size and amplitude on these<br />

parameters. For each target size, the acceleration time increased as the<br />

ID (amplitude) increased. For each amplitude, there is no effect of<br />

target size on the acceleration time. When the data was normalized,<br />

the percentage of MT after peak resultant velocity (the deceleration<br />

phase) increased for each amplitude as the ID increased (target size<br />

decreased). These results indicate that the resultant velocity profile is<br />

not symmetrical. Fitts' Law argues that the MT will increase as the<br />

target size decreases; the reason that the MT increases is the result of a<br />

relatively longer deceleration phase. As well, for longer movements,<br />

there is a longer acceleration phase. The results show that the time

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!