17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

316 CONSTRAINTS AND PHASES<br />

level fine adjustments. These data suggest physiological limitations on<br />

prehensile movements, specifically on the underlying sensory and<br />

motor pathways.<br />

Other types of damage to different neural substrates constrains<br />

available grasping behaviors. It has been shown that cutting the<br />

pyramidal tract, which is a direct pathway between various cortical<br />

areas and the motoneurons in the spinal cord, results in the loss of<br />

fractionated finger movements (Lawrence & Kuypers, 1968a,b).<br />

Primates can still perform collective hand grasping movements<br />

because less direct pathways are still intact, but are unable to oppose<br />

thumb and fingers. One other aspect of the nervous system lies in the<br />

issue of sensorimotor representations (c.f. Iwamura et al., 1981).<br />

While the motor and somatosensory cortical representation for the<br />

hand is quite large, the type of processing is still not known.<br />

In summary, human prehensile behavior is further constrained by<br />

anatomical and physiological aspects of the body and nervous system,<br />

just as robots and their computer controllers are constrained by their<br />

construction and interfaces. However, without a ‘reason’ for the<br />

movement, which in itself can constrain the formation and modulation<br />

of the movement, there would be no need for this complex machinery.<br />

Therefore, higher level goals are discussed in the next subsection.<br />

8.3 High level constraints<br />

At the highest level, four classes of constraints in living systems<br />

can be grouped together. These constraints are imposed for a variety<br />

of reasons. The importance is that there are probably large individual<br />

differences. They are labeled here as follows: semiotic/informational,<br />

motivational, social/cultural, and functionalhntentional.<br />

Semiotic, symbolic, or emotional information may be conveyed in<br />

the way an object is grasped (Nespoulous & Lecours, 1986). For<br />

example, one might hold a cigarette in a suggestive manner. Anxiety<br />

or anger may be communicated by gripping an object tightly.<br />

Motivations, such as needs being satiated, may add an urgency to the<br />

movement. As well, sociocultural constraints create boundaries on<br />

acceptable behavior. Refined behavioral interactions with objects in<br />

one culture might be taboo in another. For example, sticking out the<br />

little finger as one grasps a teacup may be a sign of good upbringing in<br />

a culture (see Section 8.2 for a possible anatomical reason). On the<br />

other hand, sticking out one’s elbow while reaching might be an<br />

invasion of someone else’s extrapersonal space.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!