17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Chapter</strong> 8. Constraints on Human <strong>Prehension</strong><br />

"Our hands become extensions of the intellect, for by hand<br />

movements the dumb converse, with the specialized fingertips<br />

the blind read; and through the written word we learn from the<br />

past and transmit to the future.''<br />

--Sterling Bunnell(l944)<br />

305<br />

The dextrous and versatile performance of human prehension can<br />

be viewed as emerging from a large multidimensional constraint space.<br />

Roboticists and experimentalists seem to be at odds currently in terms<br />

of the important variables examined in the quantification of motor<br />

control. It has been argued (Nelson 1983; MacKenzie & Marteniuk<br />

1985; Marteniuk et al. 1987) that interactions are occurring among<br />

multiple performance constraints. Nelson (1983) argued that dynamic<br />

models, such as that of Hollerbach (1980), are inadequate for<br />

explaining human movement unless they also include both the<br />

performance constraints and the objectives affecting the neural and<br />

neuromuscular inputs. MacKenzie and Marteniuk (1985) described<br />

two types of constraints: those variables that constrain the use of<br />

feedback and those due to structural limitations that affect the<br />

preparation and execution of goal directed movement. Under the first<br />

category are included issues such as how fast feedback available, the<br />

types of feedback that can be used, and the experience of the<br />

performer in learning sensorimotor manipulations. In the second<br />

category are limitations from anatomical and neurological structure,<br />

from the structure of communicated information, the structure of<br />

environmental information, and constraints due to context and<br />

intentions. Marteniuk et al. (1987) emphasized the interaction between<br />

knowledge (which includes a person's experience and movement<br />

objectives) and the biological structure. Rewording the list of<br />

MacKenzie and Marteniuk (1985), Marteniuk et al. (1987) made even<br />

more explicit the notions of task constraints and conditions of speed<br />

and accuracy. Taking a task-specific view of movement planning and<br />

control, a task is defined as the interaction of the performer with the<br />

environment under given movement goals. This view predicts that<br />

movement is optimized and specific to the unique requirements that<br />

arise from an individual interacting with the environment.<br />

In order to study the complex interaction between movement<br />

goals, object properties, environmental characteristics, and the

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!