17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>Chapter</strong> 6 - During Contact 271<br />

very small objects, where side opposition is used and movement is<br />

generated by the index finger and while the thumb remains stationary.<br />

In twiddling, this is reversed (thumb moves, index stationary) so that<br />

alternating between pad and side opposition is exhibited. The rock<br />

movement is for larger objects, such as turning jar lids or coins. The<br />

squeeze motion, occuring in the x axis, does not cause movement of<br />

the opposition vector; instead, it changes the length of it, as in<br />

squeezing a syringe or a rubber ball. Finally, regrasping can occur if<br />

the fingers must be repositioned to repeat the movement in a sequential<br />

or phased way.<br />

Elliott and Connolly (1984) also observed dynamic combinations<br />

of oppositions. The ulnar fingers could affix the object, or part of the<br />

object, in the hand, in palm opposition, while the radial fingers could<br />

perform a manipulation using pad opposition. An example is holding<br />

a string and tying a knot in it. Another combination is what they call<br />

the ‘solo’ use of a finger: an object, such as a cigarette lighter, is held<br />

in palm opposition, while the thumb, as a virtual finger three, strikes<br />

the lighter. One interesting pattern of dynamic movements that they<br />

observed is the palmar slide (see Table 6.6). Using palm opposition to<br />

hold part of the object, translation of the other part of the object occurs<br />

in the x-y plane using either pad opposition or a combination of pad<br />

and side opposition.<br />

Table 6.6 could be viewed from a different perspective, so that<br />

the focus is not on the movement names, but instead on the<br />

functionality. This is similar to what was done in Appendix B for<br />

static postures. Besides the oppositions used and virtual finger<br />

mappings seen in the appendix, the dynamic function table for human<br />

hands would include the type of opposition vector transformation,<br />

direction of movement, and relative use of fingers, as outlined in Table<br />

6.6. These, along with the length of the opposition vector, are a<br />

dynamic way to relate the motions needed in the task to the motions<br />

available to the hand, demonstrating Napier’s conjecture that power<br />

and precision task requirements are met by the hand’s power and<br />

precision capabilities (Napier, 1956).<br />

Johansson and colleagues (Johansson et al., 1992a,b,c) made a<br />

contrast beween two types of purposeful grasping activities,<br />

manipulating ‘passive’ objects (to move or reposition mechanically<br />

predictable, stable objects like a mug) or ‘active’ objects (to manipulate<br />

mechanically unpredictable objects, like my dog’s leash, when the dog<br />

is attached). They suggested that for mechanically predictable objects,<br />

we rely on sensorimotor memories, and motor output is based on<br />

anticipatory parameter control, i.e., we are using robust but flexible

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!