17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Chapter</strong> 6 - During Contact 229<br />

assimilation, and control systems analysis. Our point here is that like<br />

the eye, the sensory and motor functions of the hand are intertwined<br />

and indivisible, as the hand is moving to grasp, feeling to grasp,<br />

grasping to feel and grasping to move objects. This ability, called<br />

active touch or haptics, is highly developed in the human sensorimotor<br />

system.<br />

Active touch, or touching was distinguished from passive touch,<br />

or being touched by Gibson (1962), who noted that with active touch,<br />

the impression on the skin is brought about by the perceiver. Active<br />

touch, our concern here, is an active exploratory sense. Through<br />

exploratory movements, tactile scanning can occur, providing<br />

information about object properties.<br />

6.<strong>2.</strong>1 Wielding a stick: feeling object length<br />

In an extensive analysis of perception of length, weight, and<br />

centre of mass in lifted rods, Hoisington (1920) showed that for non-<br />

visual perception of length of a rod lifted or held in palm opposition,<br />

the underlying essential condition is a ratio between two opposing<br />

pressures, held together by directional and temporal relations; in<br />

addition to the two opposed pressures are the absolute intensity of the<br />

impressions, their temporal course, the pressure gradient formed and<br />

the muscle and strain sensations from the hand and arm. Center of<br />

mass of the object bears a constant direct relation to the ratio of<br />

opposing pressures, but is removed from the experienced perception.<br />

Perceptions of length reachable by hand-held rods, partial and<br />

total rod lengths have been evaluated by Burton, Solomon, and<br />

Turvey who suggested that the moments of inertia provided the kinetic<br />

bases for judgements of rod length (Solomon & Turvey, 1988;<br />

Solomon, Turvey & Burton, 1989; Burton & Turvey, 1991). To hold<br />

stationary a rod held in the hand, the forces and torques that must be<br />

applied are a function of the type of opposition (in this case, palm<br />

opposition), virtual finger mapping, hand configuration, hand<br />

placement on the rod, and the physical dimensions of the object.<br />

Placing added weights and the hand at different positions along the<br />

length of the rod altered perceived length in predictable ways<br />

according to the center of mass, and moment arm length, consistent<br />

with the earlier statements of Hoisington (1920) on ratios of opposing<br />

pressures and intensities in the hand and arm. Burton and Turvey<br />

(1991) address the questions of corresponding deformation patterns in<br />

the hand and how prior intents are functionally equivalent to physical<br />

constraints that harness dynamics to form task-specific mechanisms

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!