17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

204 THE PHASES OF PREHENSION<br />

about the state of interaction with the object during the task in order to<br />

ensure grasping and manipulative stability. As an effector, it applies<br />

task appropriate forces for grasping and manipulative stability, using<br />

the muscles (somatically innervated sensorimotor system) in parallel<br />

with the eccrine sweat glands (autonomically innervated sudomotor<br />

system’), given the inherent ‘passive’ structural characteristics of the<br />

hand (e.g., the compliant pads of the fingers and palm). The object<br />

has characteristics relevant for stable grasping which may be assessed<br />

with varying degrees of accuracy through the visual, kinesthetic, or<br />

tactile systems. As well, physical characteristics of the object’s<br />

structure determine the nature of the interaction in stable grasping: for<br />

example, the object and the hand surfaces together determine the<br />

coefficient of friction.<br />

Some object properties are found through direct interaction with<br />

the object, or hapticallyz. Grasping activates complex patterns of<br />

cutaneous, muscle and joint receptors. Gibson (1962) suggested that<br />

the covariance of cutaneous and articular motion provides information<br />

for haptic form perception. For example, Iwamura and Tanaka (1978)<br />

identified types of units in Area 2 of the parietal lobe which did not<br />

respond to ordinary passive cutaneous stimulation of the hand, nor<br />

with manipulation of wrist or finger joints by the experimenter. These<br />

neurons were selectively responsive to properties of the object being<br />

grasped. Gibson (1962) suggested that some movements are<br />

‘exploratory’ and can be distinguished from ‘performatory’ hand<br />

movements.<br />

<strong>Prehension</strong> is defined as the application of functionally effective<br />

forces by the hand to an object for a task, given numerous constraints.<br />

Key questions are how the hand can effect these forces and what is the<br />

nature of the forces arising in the task. This chapter addresses these<br />

questions by looking at how the hand meets the functional demands of<br />

applying forces to match the anticipated forces in the task, imparting<br />

motion to the object, and gathering sensory information about the state<br />

of the interaction with the object. First, as a key to the interface<br />

between object and hand, the skin and its properties are discussed.<br />

Then touch and active touch are addressed through a discussion of<br />

cutaneous sensors and sensorimotor integration. Force application is<br />

Sudomotor refers to the motor innervation of sweat glands. It is derived from<br />

Latin, sudor, sudoris, meaning sweat.<br />

2Haptics is a perceptual system that uses both cutaneous (including thermal) and<br />

kinesthetic inputs to derive information about objects, their properties, and their<br />

spatial layout (Loomis and Lederman, 1986).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!