17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

178 THE PHASES OF PREHENSION<br />

books and papers: Goodwin and Darian-Smith (1985); Humphrey<br />

(1979); Hyvarinen (1982); and Phillips (1986).<br />

5.4.5 Restricted sensory conditions<br />

One way to gather information to answer the coupling question is<br />

to vary the available amount of sensory information, and see what the<br />

effects are on the various movement components. To recall, Wing et<br />

al. (1986) had subjects reach to grasp a vertically standing dowel un-<br />

der conditions where visual feedback and speed of movement varied.<br />

Focussing on the effects of the sensory manipulations, the peak grip<br />

aperture was larger in movements faster than normal and in move-<br />

ments where subjects had their eyes closed. There was more variabil-<br />

ity at peak aperture than at contact. At the frame just before contact<br />

(pre-contact), they noted more variability in the blind and fast condi-<br />

tions than in control conditions. This suggests the possibility of a<br />

very conservative Reshape controller, opening the hand wider when<br />

reliable visual information is not available. Recalling Jeannerod' s ex-<br />

periments (1981, 1984), he noted that transport movements fell short<br />

of the target under conditions in which subjects saw only the objects,<br />

not the hand; in contrast, movements with vision had a longer MT and<br />

low-velocity phase. However, he did not observe a difference in<br />

maximum grip aperture and precontact grip aperture for the various vi-<br />

sual manipulations.<br />

While the importance of visual information has been demonstrated,<br />

the type and resolution of visual information can also have an effect.<br />

Normal vision is a combination of central vision (the central lo" of the<br />

visual field) and peripheral vision. Research has shown that central<br />

vision is functionally specialized for responding to spatial patterns,<br />

while peripheral vision responds to movement stimuli. In the Paillard<br />

(1982b) model seen in <strong>Chapter</strong> 3, it was suggested that movement and<br />

location cues from peripheral vision were used for transporting the<br />

arm, and size and shape cues from central vision were used for form-<br />

ing the hand. Interestingly, if the eyes are focused on the object, then<br />

at the end of the reach, both the object and the hand are in central vi-<br />

sion.<br />

Sivak (1989; Sivak 8z MacKenzie, 1990, 1992) performed ma-<br />

nipulations of visual information in grasping tasks, using pad opposi-<br />

tion. Infrared markers were placed on the wrist, index finger, and<br />

thumb in order to study the kinematics of the grasping and transport<br />

components using a WATSMART system. In the first experiment, a<br />

subject sat with the head stationary in a chin rest and was instructed to

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!