17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Chapter</strong> 5 - Movement Before Contact 123<br />

in the motor cortex, providing evidence that the motor cortex may be<br />

viewed as a computational map. At the same time, some features of<br />

limb movements may not be explicitly planned by supraspinal sys-<br />

tems. It is possible that the CNS is using simplifying rules to generate<br />

close but inexact approximations (or ballparks) of an intended mov-<br />

ment. The hand path, for example, which tends to be in straight lines<br />

and exhibit bell-shaped velocity profiles, may not be being planned,<br />

but may emerge as a result of the CNS transformations from goal lo-<br />

cation to intrinsic joint angles or muscle lengths and/or from the me-<br />

chanical properties of the limb.<br />

5.3.2 Symmetric vs asymmetric velocity profiles<br />

A pointing study performed by Atkeson and Hollerbach (1985)<br />

brought out important features about unrestrained arm movements.<br />

Subjects started with their hand in one of six locations in space, and<br />

were asked to move their arm within a plane to another location which<br />

was lit with a light-emitting diode. In order to collect these data,<br />

Atkeson and Hollerbach used a SELSPOT system, allowing them to<br />

track the subject’s index finger tip, wrist, elbow, and shoulder move-<br />

ments over time. Small, light-emitting diodes were attached to the<br />

body part to be tracked, and solid state cameras captured the kinemat-<br />

ics of the movement. In terms of the path of the wrist, some paths<br />

were straight-line, some were curved, depending on the location of the<br />

movement in the workspace. Subjects were required to carry a load in<br />

their hands for some of the trials. For a given direction, there was<br />

path invariance with speed and load. Atkeson and Hollerbach found<br />

that practice did not influence the movements. They showed that the<br />

tangential velocity profile, scaled both in time and amplitude, was<br />

invariant for all subjects, speeds, loads and directions (shown in<br />

Figure 5.4).<br />

Other investigators have not always found symmetric velocity<br />

profiles in pointing tasks. MacKenzie, Marteniuk, Dugas, Liske, and<br />

Eickmeier (1987) replicated the conditions from Paul Fitts’ study on<br />

discrete aiming (Fitts 1954, Experiment 2), and found systematic<br />

effects of target size on the degree of asymmetry in the tangential<br />

velocity profiles. Subjects were asked to point with a stylus ‘as<br />

quickly and as accurately as possible’ to a target of varying size and at<br />

varying distances. The question being asked concerned whether there<br />

was a reliable kinematic measure of the precision requirements of the<br />

task. In this case, Fitts’ Law (Fitts, 1954) was used, which states

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!