17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Chapter</strong> 5 - Movement Before Contact 119<br />

Jeannerod, 1988). The reaching movements are inaccurate and much<br />

slower, having lower peak velocities and longer deceleration phases.<br />

Thus, the posterior parietal cortex is implicated in disruption of the<br />

convergence of visual and proprioceptive maps for visually guided<br />

reaching.<br />

Other evidence has been observed in behavioral pointing studies<br />

for a mapping between extrinsic and intrinsic coordinate frames (step<br />

2). In Soechting and Terzuolo (1990), subjects were asked to remem-<br />

ber a target’s location, and then asked to point to that remembered spot<br />

in the dark. The distance and direction of the target were the extrinsic<br />

measures while intrinsic measures were the shoulder and elbow joint<br />

angles in terms of angular elevation and yaw. Subjects were able to<br />

point in the correct direction but there were errors in the radial distance<br />

from the shoulder, ranging from 6.7 to 11.7 cm average root mean<br />

square total error in each subject. Soechting and Terzuolo argued that<br />

the errors were due to the transformation between extrinsic and<br />

intrinsic coordinates, and suggested that the problem is solved only<br />

approximately, thus leading to predictable distortions. Soechting and<br />

Flanders (1991) showed that for pointing tasks, a spatial location is<br />

first transformed from head centered to shoulder centered spatial<br />

coordinates. The location is then redefined in an intrinsic kinematic<br />

framework by two separate channels that convert target azimuth<br />

(horizontal direction) into intended yaw angles of arm in sagittal plane<br />

when the hand is at the target, and target elevation and distance into<br />

arm segment elevation angles. The difference between current and<br />

intended arm angles is then transformed into a motor command<br />

signaling direction of hand movement. The error patterns suggest that<br />

the CNS uses linear approximations of the exact nonlinear relation<br />

between limb segment angles and target location7.<br />

Separate encoding of distance and direction was obtained in motor<br />

memory research in the 1970s, using linear or lever positioning tasks.<br />

In such tasks, blindfolded humans grasped a handle using palm, pad<br />

or side opposition, and moved to a preselected or experimenter-<br />

defined endpoint. Then, either immediately or after some filled or<br />

unfilled retention interval, the individuals were asked to reproduce the<br />

distance or location, from a new or different starting location.<br />

Evidence supported a qualitatively different memory for location than<br />

distance (subjects are better at reproducing locations than distances),<br />

and that there were different patterns of forgetting of location and dis-<br />

7Separate planning channels may also exist for direction, amplitude and force<br />

(Favilla et al., 1989; Riehle & Requin, 1989; Rosenbaum, 1980).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!