30.12.2012 Views

Handedness in Pedophilia and Hebephilia - Laboratory for the ...

Handedness in Pedophilia and Hebephilia - Laboratory for the ...

Handedness in Pedophilia and Hebephilia - Laboratory for the ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Archives of Sexual Behavior, Vol. 34, No. 4, August 2005, pp. 447–459 ( C○ 2005)<br />

DOI: 10.1007/s10508-005-4344-7<br />

<strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Pedophilia</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Hebephilia</strong><br />

James M. Cantor, Ph.D., 1,5 Philip E. Klassen, M.D., 1,2 Robert Dickey, M.D., 1,2<br />

Bruce K. Christensen, Ph.D., 2,3 Michael E. Kuban, M.A., M.Sc., 1 Thomas Blak, B.A., 1<br />

Natasha S. Williams, M.A., 1,4 <strong>and</strong> Ray Blanchard, Ph.D. 1,2<br />

INTRODUCTION<br />

Men with primary erotic <strong>in</strong>terests ei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>for</strong> prepubescent<br />

children or pubescent children show poorer<br />

per<strong>for</strong>mance on <strong>in</strong>telligence <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r neuropsychological<br />

tests than do men with a primary erotic <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>for</strong><br />

1Law <strong>and</strong> Mental Health Program, Centre <strong>for</strong> Addiction <strong>and</strong> Mental<br />

Health, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.<br />

2Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario,<br />

Canada.<br />

3Schizophrenia Program, Centre <strong>for</strong> Addiction <strong>and</strong> Mental Health,<br />

Toronto, Ontario, Canada.<br />

4Adler School of Professional Psychology, Chicago, Ill<strong>in</strong>ois.<br />

5To whom correspondence should be addressed at Law <strong>and</strong> Mental<br />

Health Program, Centre <strong>for</strong> Addiction <strong>and</strong> Mental Health, 250<br />

College Street, Toronto, Ontario M5T 1R8, Canada; e-mail: james<br />

cantor@camh.net.<br />

Received May 21, 2004; revision received July 15, 2004; accepted July 15, 2004<br />

A sample of 404 adult men underwent assessment follow<strong>in</strong>g illegal or cl<strong>in</strong>ically significant sexual<br />

behaviors or <strong>in</strong>terests. Patients’ assessments <strong>in</strong>cluded: adm<strong>in</strong>istration of a modified version of <strong>the</strong><br />

Ed<strong>in</strong>burgh <strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong> Inventory; record<strong>in</strong>g of patients’ phallometric (penile) responses to erotic<br />

stimuli depict<strong>in</strong>g adults, pubescent children, <strong>and</strong> prepubescent children of both sexes; <strong>and</strong> a tabulation<br />

of <strong>the</strong> numbers of patients’ victims, ages 0–11, 12–14, 15–16, <strong>and</strong> 17 <strong>and</strong> older, of both sexes. In<br />

Study 1, patients’ right-h<strong>and</strong>edness scores correlated negatively with <strong>the</strong>ir phallometric responses<br />

to stimuli depict<strong>in</strong>g prepubescent children <strong>and</strong> positively with stimuli depict<strong>in</strong>g adults, replicat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>the</strong> pattern described <strong>in</strong> a previous report (Cantor et al., 2004). Unlike <strong>the</strong> previous study, however,<br />

patients’ h<strong>and</strong>edness scores did not significantly correlate with <strong>the</strong>ir numbers of prepubescent victims.<br />

To explore this discrepancy, Study 2 comb<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>the</strong> patients from this replication sample with those <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> previously reported sample, categoriz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m by <strong>the</strong> sex <strong>and</strong> age group of greatest erotic <strong>in</strong>terest<br />

to <strong>the</strong>m. The odds of non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>in</strong> men offend<strong>in</strong>g predom<strong>in</strong>antly aga<strong>in</strong>st prepubescent<br />

children were approximately two-fold higher than that <strong>in</strong> men offend<strong>in</strong>g predom<strong>in</strong>antly aga<strong>in</strong>st adults<br />

<strong>and</strong> three-fold higher after elim<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g those men with <strong>in</strong>trafamilial (i.e., <strong>in</strong>cest) offenses. <strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong><br />

differences between men erotically <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> males versus females were not statistically significant.<br />

These results <strong>in</strong>dicate that <strong>the</strong> rates of non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>in</strong> pedophilia are much larger than<br />

previously suggested <strong>and</strong> are comparable to <strong>the</strong> rates observed <strong>in</strong> pervasive developmental disorders,<br />

such as autism, suggest<strong>in</strong>g a neurological component to <strong>the</strong> development of pedophilia <strong>and</strong> hebephilia.<br />

KEY WORDS: h<strong>and</strong>edness; laterality; neuropsychology; pedophilia; phallometry; sexual abuse; sex offenders.<br />

447<br />

adult sexual partners (e.g., Cantor et al., 2004). We refer<br />

to erotic <strong>in</strong>terests <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong>se age groups as pedophilia (von<br />

Krafft-Eb<strong>in</strong>g, 1886/1965), hebephilia (Glueck, 1955),<br />

<strong>and</strong> teleiophilia (Blanchard, et al. 2000), respectively.<br />

Cognitive per<strong>for</strong>mance appears to relate more strongly to<br />

pedophilic <strong>and</strong> hebephilic <strong>in</strong>terest than to <strong>the</strong> propensity<br />

to commit sexual offenses <strong>in</strong> general or to <strong>the</strong> propensity<br />

to commit offenses of a nonsexual nature. Unlike samples<br />

of men show<strong>in</strong>g evidence of erotic <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> children,<br />

samples of sexual offenders aga<strong>in</strong>st adults have not<br />

as consistently shown neuropsychological test scores<br />

lower than control groups or test norms (e.g., Qu<strong>in</strong>sey,<br />

Arnold, & Pruesse, 1980; see also Blanchard, Cantor,<br />

& Robichaud, <strong>in</strong> press, <strong>for</strong> a review). Similarly, when<br />

compared with sexual offenders aga<strong>in</strong>st children, men<br />

who have committed only nonsexual crimes typically<br />

score higher (e.g., Wormith, 1986).<br />

0004-0002/05/0800-0447/0 C○ 2005 Spr<strong>in</strong>ger Science+Bus<strong>in</strong>ess Media, Inc.


448 Cantor et al.<br />

One possible explanation <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> association between<br />

erotic <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> children <strong>and</strong> poorer cognitive ability is<br />

that <strong>the</strong>y both reflect an underly<strong>in</strong>g bra<strong>in</strong> dysfunction,<br />

one that prevented <strong>the</strong> development of more typical <strong>in</strong>tellectual<br />

<strong>and</strong> sexual characteristics, as we have previously<br />

proposed (Blanchard et al., 2002). Alternately possible<br />

is that <strong>the</strong> observed group differences on cognitive<br />

neuropsychological tests resulted from an ascerta<strong>in</strong>ment<br />

bias: Sexual offenders aga<strong>in</strong>st children could be more<br />

likely to be apprehended if <strong>the</strong>y have poorer cognitive<br />

abilities, <strong>and</strong> sexual offenders with greater cognitive<br />

abilities could be more likely to be well employed, to<br />

af<strong>for</strong>d superior legal counsel, <strong>and</strong> to escape conviction<br />

(Blanchard et al., 2002; Cantor et al., 2004). Thus,<br />

sexual offenders aga<strong>in</strong>st children might score lower<br />

on tests of <strong>in</strong>tellectual function merely because such<br />

men are more likely to become available to research<br />

studies.<br />

Developmental neuropathologies manifest <strong>in</strong> many<br />

cognitive <strong>and</strong> behavioral characteristics, one of which is<br />

an <strong>in</strong>creased probability of non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

evaluation of h<strong>and</strong>edness comprises a st<strong>and</strong>ard component<br />

of neuropsychological assessment. Non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness<br />

occurs <strong>in</strong> approximately 8–15% of <strong>the</strong> general adult<br />

population (see Hardyck & Petr<strong>in</strong>ovich, 1977 <strong>for</strong> a<br />

review), but 1.5–3.0 times more frequently <strong>in</strong> populations<br />

with any of several neurological disorders. Such disorders<br />

<strong>in</strong>clude Down’s Syndrome (e.g., Ba<strong>the</strong>ja & McManus,<br />

1985), epilepsy (e.g., Schachter et al., 1995), autism (e.g.,<br />

Soper et al., 1986), learn<strong>in</strong>g disabilities <strong>and</strong> dyslexia<br />

(e.g., Cornish & McManus, 1996), <strong>and</strong> mental retardation<br />

(e.g., Grouios, Sakadami, Poderi, & Alevriadou, 1999).<br />

The association of h<strong>and</strong>edness with pedophilia <strong>and</strong><br />

hebephilia can thus <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>m several issues regard<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>the</strong> etiology of erotic age preference. First, elevated<br />

rates of non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness would argue aga<strong>in</strong>st <strong>the</strong><br />

a<strong>for</strong>ementioned ascerta<strong>in</strong>ment bias explanation of <strong>the</strong><br />

poorer neuropsychological function<strong>in</strong>g among <strong>the</strong>se men.<br />

Although it seems plausible to assert that men with poorer<br />

cognitive skills are more likely to be apprehended <strong>and</strong><br />

convicted, it is much less plausible to posit that h<strong>and</strong>edness<br />

would substantially affect rates of apprehension<br />

(over <strong>and</strong> above any effects of poor cognitive function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

itself). An elevated rate of non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>in</strong> pedo<strong>and</strong><br />

hebephilic men relative to that rate <strong>in</strong> teleiophilic<br />

men would <strong>in</strong>stead suggest an association between erotic<br />

age preference <strong>and</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> function. That is, non-righth<strong>and</strong>edness<br />

would be serv<strong>in</strong>g as a marker of an underly<strong>in</strong>g<br />

neurological difference between teleiophilic <strong>and</strong> nonteleiophilic<br />

groups.<br />

Researchers differentiate natural left-h<strong>and</strong>ers (who<br />

may have <strong>in</strong>herited an <strong>in</strong>creased probability of s<strong>in</strong>istrality<br />

from <strong>the</strong>ir parents) from pathological left-h<strong>and</strong>ers, <strong>for</strong><br />

whom s<strong>in</strong>istrality resulted from a compensatory reaction<br />

of <strong>the</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g bra<strong>in</strong> to some trauma (e.g., Bishop,<br />

1990). When one hemisphere of <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> suffers damage<br />

dur<strong>in</strong>g development, <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r may take on additional<br />

functions, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g those expressed through h<strong>and</strong>edness<br />

(Bakan, 1971; Bakan, Dibb, & Reed, 1973). Because one<br />

cerebral hemisphere (<strong>the</strong> left) achieves functional dom<strong>in</strong>ance<br />

<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> great majority of humans, any perturbations<br />

sufficient to alter hemispheric dom<strong>in</strong>ance <strong>and</strong> equally<br />

likely to occur <strong>in</strong> ei<strong>the</strong>r will exaggerate <strong>the</strong> frequency of<br />

<strong>the</strong> less common outcome (Satz, 1973). That is, changes <strong>in</strong><br />

cerebral dom<strong>in</strong>ance from left-to-right will be much more<br />

common than changes <strong>in</strong> dom<strong>in</strong>ance from right-to-left<br />

because many more cases start out with dom<strong>in</strong>ance <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

left hemisphere to beg<strong>in</strong> with (<strong>for</strong> reviews, see Bishop,<br />

1990; Coren & Halpern, 1991). This property permits<br />

elevated rates of s<strong>in</strong>istrality to denote perturbation(s)<br />

occurr<strong>in</strong>g dur<strong>in</strong>g bra<strong>in</strong> development.<br />

Although elevated rates of non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>in</strong><br />

pedo- <strong>and</strong> hebephilia would argue <strong>for</strong> a neurological<br />

contribution to <strong>the</strong> etiology of those conditions, it would<br />

also argue aga<strong>in</strong>st any simple, focal-lesion model of<br />

that contribution. Some <strong>in</strong>vestigators have described case<br />

studies of <strong>in</strong>dividuals who suffered bra<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>jury or disease<br />

<strong>in</strong> adulthood <strong>and</strong> subsequently engaged <strong>in</strong> sexual offenses,<br />

often aga<strong>in</strong>st children (e.g., Mendez, Chow, R<strong>in</strong>gman,<br />

Twitchell, & H<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>, 2000). Based on <strong>the</strong> characteristics<br />

of <strong>the</strong> lesions, some authors concluded that <strong>the</strong> sexually<br />

offend<strong>in</strong>g behaviors were <strong>the</strong> result of a specific, localized<br />

neuropathology (e.g., Casanova, Mannheim, & Kruesi,<br />

2002). Elevated rates of non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>in</strong> large<br />

samples of pedophiles, however, would <strong>in</strong>dicate that a<br />

neurological explanation of pedophilia based on specific<br />

bra<strong>in</strong> sites is <strong>in</strong>complete at best. Except <strong>for</strong> gross deficits <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> motor control of <strong>the</strong> preferred h<strong>and</strong>, h<strong>and</strong>edness does<br />

not change follow<strong>in</strong>g bra<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>jury <strong>in</strong> adults. Elevated levels<br />

of non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness are, however, associated with<br />

biological stresses occurr<strong>in</strong>g pre- <strong>and</strong> per<strong>in</strong>atally, achiev<strong>in</strong>g<br />

frequencies of non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness comparable to<br />

those <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> a<strong>for</strong>ementioned pervasive developmental<br />

disorders (e.g., Searleman, Cunn<strong>in</strong>gham, & Goodw<strong>in</strong>,<br />

1988). Such pre- <strong>and</strong> per<strong>in</strong>atal stressors <strong>in</strong>clude premature<br />

birth (e.g., Marlow, Roberts, & Cooke, 1989; Ross,<br />

Lipper, & Auld, 1992), tw<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> multiple births<br />

(e.g., Coren, 1994; Davis & Annett, 1994; Williams,<br />

Buss, & Eskenazi, 1992), <strong>and</strong> low birth weight (e.g.,<br />

O’Callaghan et al., 1987; Powls, Bott<strong>in</strong>g, Cooke, &<br />

Marlow, 1996). Notably, three-fold <strong>in</strong>creases <strong>in</strong> rates of<br />

non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness occur <strong>in</strong> some such samples even <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> absence of any dramatic difference <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> groups’ mean<br />

IQs.


<strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Pedophilia</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Hebephilia</strong> 449<br />

Two reports have suggested elevated rates of nonright-h<strong>and</strong>edness<br />

<strong>in</strong> pedophilia, although nei<strong>the</strong>r report<br />

provided an adequate estimate of <strong>the</strong> actual proportion of<br />

pedophilic or hebephilic men who are non-right-h<strong>and</strong>ed.<br />

Bogaert (2001) reanalyzed h<strong>and</strong>edness data archived by<br />

<strong>the</strong> K<strong>in</strong>sey Institute <strong>for</strong> Sex, Gender, <strong>and</strong> Reproduction. A<br />

sample of men who had committed sexual offenses <strong>and</strong> a<br />

sample of men who had committed no known offenses had<br />

been asked by <strong>the</strong> K<strong>in</strong>sey Institute researchers to <strong>in</strong>dicate<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir h<strong>and</strong>edness as right-h<strong>and</strong>ed, left-h<strong>and</strong>ed, ambidextrous,<br />

or left- <strong>and</strong> retra<strong>in</strong>ed to right-h<strong>and</strong>-use. Collaps<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>the</strong> non-right-h<strong>and</strong>ed responses toge<strong>the</strong>r, Bogaert (2001)<br />

reported a small difference <strong>in</strong> non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness between<br />

<strong>the</strong> sample of controls (11.5% non-right-h<strong>and</strong>ed,<br />

n = 4706) <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> subsample of sexual offenders whose<br />

victims <strong>in</strong>cluded at least one extrafamilial (i.e., non<strong>in</strong>cest)<br />

child of ei<strong>the</strong>r sex, age 11 or younger (15.7% nonright-h<strong>and</strong>ed,<br />

n = 286). This group difference achieved<br />

statistical significance prior to partial<strong>in</strong>g out differences<br />

<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> men’s level of education (e B = 1.61, p = .030), but<br />

less so after (e B = 1.66, p = .054).<br />

The K<strong>in</strong>sey sample may have underestimated <strong>the</strong><br />

actual rate of non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>in</strong> pedophilia. An<br />

unreported proportion of <strong>the</strong> sexual offenders aga<strong>in</strong>st<br />

children also committed sexual offenses aga<strong>in</strong>st adults,<br />

obfuscat<strong>in</strong>g those participants’ erotic age preferences.<br />

Moreover, at least some sexual offenders aga<strong>in</strong>st children<br />

are actually teleiophilic (e.g., Freund, Watson, & Dickey,<br />

1991). The K<strong>in</strong>sey database does not <strong>in</strong>clude results of<br />

objective tests of sexual <strong>in</strong>terest, such as psychophysiological<br />

tests of penile responses to erotic stimuli (i.e.,<br />

phallometry), which would have permitted verification<br />

of participants’ erotic age preference. Thus, it rema<strong>in</strong>s<br />

unknown what proportion of that sample was actually<br />

pedophilic, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>clusion of non-pedophiles <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

sample of sexual offenders aga<strong>in</strong>st children would have<br />

served to reduce <strong>the</strong> group differences observed.<br />

Cantor et al. (2004) recorded from a large sample<br />

of sexually anomalous men: h<strong>and</strong>edness on a n<strong>in</strong>e-item<br />

<strong>in</strong>ventory, phallometric responses to erotic stimuli <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g<br />

ei<strong>the</strong>r males or females <strong>in</strong> three age groups (adults,<br />

pubescent children, or prepubescent children), <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

numbers of sexual victims <strong>and</strong> consent<strong>in</strong>g sexual partners<br />

<strong>in</strong> each of several age groups (ages 17 or older, ages 15–<br />

16, ages 12–14, or ages 11 or younger). Study participants<br />

were undergo<strong>in</strong>g assessment follow<strong>in</strong>g ei<strong>the</strong>r a sexual<br />

offense <strong>for</strong> which <strong>the</strong>y were charged or atypical sexual<br />

<strong>in</strong>terests <strong>for</strong> which <strong>the</strong>y or <strong>the</strong>ir health care providers<br />

requested more <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation. Scores on <strong>the</strong> h<strong>and</strong>edness<br />

<strong>in</strong>ventory correlated significantly with <strong>the</strong> number of<br />

victims ages 11 or younger <strong>and</strong> with <strong>the</strong> magnitude of<br />

<strong>the</strong> genital response to stimuli depict<strong>in</strong>g prepubescent<br />

children. Greater non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness predicted more<br />

victims <strong>and</strong> a greater genital response, both be<strong>for</strong>e <strong>and</strong><br />

after partiall<strong>in</strong>g out participants’ estimated IQ scores <strong>and</strong><br />

ages at test<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

In that report, however, we did not provide <strong>the</strong><br />

actual proportion of its samples exhibit<strong>in</strong>g non-righth<strong>and</strong>edness.<br />

This prevents direct comparison of its f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

with those of <strong>the</strong> h<strong>and</strong>edness literature. Moreover,<br />

although <strong>the</strong> association between h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>dicators of pedophilia achieved statistical significance,<br />

<strong>the</strong> magnitudes of <strong>the</strong> correlations were small, <strong>in</strong> absolute<br />

terms. The report drew no dist<strong>in</strong>ction between men<br />

who offended aga<strong>in</strong>st <strong>the</strong>ir own children or step-children<br />

(i.e., <strong>in</strong>trafamilial offenders) <strong>and</strong> extrafamilial offenders;<br />

because <strong>in</strong>trafamilial offenders may be less likely to have<br />

genu<strong>in</strong>e erotic <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> children (e.g., Freund et al.,<br />

1991), <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>clusion of <strong>in</strong>trafamilial offenders might have<br />

decreased <strong>the</strong> magnitude of <strong>the</strong> association observed.<br />

These f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>the</strong>re<strong>for</strong>e pose several questions:<br />

(1) because both Bogaert (2001) <strong>and</strong> Cantor et al.<br />

(2004) found h<strong>and</strong>edness to correlate with pedophilia<br />

only modestly, <strong>the</strong> general determ<strong>in</strong>ation of whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>re<br />

exists an association between h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>and</strong> pedophilia<br />

bears repeat<strong>in</strong>g. (2) Because <strong>the</strong> sampl<strong>in</strong>g method of<br />

Bogaert (2001) could have <strong>in</strong>cluded teleiophilic men <strong>in</strong><br />

its sample of offenders aga<strong>in</strong>st children, <strong>and</strong> because<br />

Cantor et al. (2004) did not provide h<strong>and</strong>edness data<br />

<strong>in</strong> a dichotomous <strong>for</strong>m, <strong>the</strong> actual proportions of nonright-h<strong>and</strong>edness<br />

<strong>in</strong> conservatively diagnosed samples of<br />

pedophiles <strong>and</strong> hebephiles rema<strong>in</strong> to be determ<strong>in</strong>ed. (3)<br />

Whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>re is an association of h<strong>and</strong>edness with <strong>the</strong><br />

sex of <strong>the</strong> victims also rema<strong>in</strong>s unknown. Bogaert (2001)<br />

collapsed offenders aga<strong>in</strong>st female children with offenders<br />

aga<strong>in</strong>st male children <strong>in</strong> his analysis, <strong>and</strong> Cantor et al.<br />

(2004) provided only equivocal results regard<strong>in</strong>g this<br />

question. (4) Also unknown is whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> detection of<br />

an association of h<strong>and</strong>edness with pedo- <strong>and</strong> hebephilia<br />

was h<strong>in</strong>dered by <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>clusion of <strong>in</strong>trafamilial offenders.<br />

The Bogaert (2001) sample excluded men whose offenses<br />

were limited to <strong>in</strong>trafamilial victims, but <strong>in</strong>cluded men<br />

who had both <strong>in</strong>trafamilial <strong>and</strong> extrafamilial victims. The<br />

Cantor et al. (2004) sample collapsed <strong>in</strong>to a s<strong>in</strong>gle group<br />

men with ei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>in</strong>trafamilial or extrafamilial victims.<br />

We undertook Study 1 to address question (1), that<br />

is, to confirm our prior f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g of an association between<br />

non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>and</strong> erotic age preference, us<strong>in</strong>g a<br />

non-overlapp<strong>in</strong>g sample of sexually atypical male patients<br />

that <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong>dividuals with pedophilic, hebephilic,<br />

or teleiophilic <strong>in</strong>terests. Study 2, to follow, addressed<br />

questions (2)–(4). For each analysis, we adjusted <strong>for</strong><br />

any extraneous effects on h<strong>and</strong>edness from participants’<br />

chronological age <strong>and</strong> level of <strong>in</strong>tellectual function.


450 Cantor et al.<br />

Remov<strong>in</strong>g chronological age accounts <strong>for</strong> effects potentially<br />

<strong>in</strong>troduced by <strong>the</strong> established association between<br />

h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>and</strong> age (e.g., Ashton, 1982; Porac, Coren,<br />

& Duncan, 1980); left-h<strong>and</strong>edness appears to be related<br />

to shorter life expectancies (Coren & Halpern, 1991).<br />

Remov<strong>in</strong>g IQ accounts <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> a<strong>for</strong>ementioned association<br />

between <strong>in</strong>creased rates of non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>and</strong><br />

lower <strong>in</strong>telligence.<br />

STUDY 1<br />

Method<br />

Participants<br />

We recruited study participants from <strong>the</strong> Kurt Freund<br />

<strong>Laboratory</strong> at <strong>the</strong> Centre <strong>for</strong> Addiction <strong>and</strong> Mental Health<br />

(Toronto, Ontario, Canada), which provides evaluation<br />

services to male patients referred as a result of illegal or<br />

cl<strong>in</strong>ically significant sexual behaviors. The primary source<br />

of referrals to <strong>the</strong> facility was parole <strong>and</strong> probation officers,<br />

with some physicians <strong>and</strong> lawyers provid<strong>in</strong>g o<strong>the</strong>rs.<br />

As detailed <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>the</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard assessment of<br />

<strong>the</strong> <strong>Laboratory</strong> consists of a psychophysiological (phallometric)<br />

assessment of <strong>the</strong> patients’ erotic preferences,<br />

a semi-structured <strong>in</strong>terview, a brief neuropsychological<br />

evaluation that <strong>in</strong>cludes h<strong>and</strong>edness, <strong>and</strong> a review of<br />

supplementary psychiatric <strong>and</strong> legal documents supplied<br />

by <strong>the</strong> referral source. Upon <strong>the</strong> completion of his<br />

evaluation, each patient was <strong>in</strong>vited to permit his cl<strong>in</strong>ical<br />

data to be used <strong>for</strong> research purposes.<br />

The replication sample consisted of <strong>the</strong> 404 consecutive<br />

male patients of <strong>the</strong> Kurt Freund <strong>Laboratory</strong><br />

who met <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>clusion/exclusion criteria <strong>and</strong><br />

who completed assessment between February 1, 2002<br />

<strong>and</strong> December 31, 2003. This time frame immediately<br />

followed that <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> sample reported <strong>in</strong> Cantor et al.<br />

(2004). The replication sample excluded an additional<br />

42 persons assessed <strong>in</strong> that time period who could not<br />

participate <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> complete neuropsychological battery<br />

(because of deafness, <strong>in</strong>sufficient English-language skills,<br />

etc.), who decl<strong>in</strong>ed to consent to <strong>the</strong> use of <strong>the</strong>ir cl<strong>in</strong>ical<br />

results <strong>for</strong> research, or <strong>for</strong> whom <strong>the</strong>re was no sexual<br />

behavior or <strong>in</strong>terests <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation available o<strong>the</strong>r than<br />

self-report.<br />

The sample showed mean <strong>and</strong> median ages of<br />

37.7 years (SD = 13.3) <strong>and</strong> 38.0 years, respectively.<br />

The mean <strong>and</strong> median educational levels were 11.7<br />

(SD = 2.73) <strong>and</strong> 12.0 years, respectively. The patients<br />

were predom<strong>in</strong>antly of European descent, with 77.5%<br />

describ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>mselves as White, 2.5% as Asian, 7.4%<br />

as Black, 4.0% as Sou<strong>the</strong>ast Asian, 3.0% as Aborig<strong>in</strong>al<br />

Canadian, 1.0% as Filip<strong>in</strong>o or Pacific Isl<strong>and</strong>er, <strong>and</strong> 4.5%<br />

as “o<strong>the</strong>r,” which <strong>in</strong>cluded mixed ancestry. For one patient<br />

(0.2%), this <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation was unknown.<br />

Of <strong>the</strong> replication sample, 47.8% committed a<br />

sexual offense aga<strong>in</strong>st one or more victims ages 11 or<br />

under, 26.7% aga<strong>in</strong>st one or more victims ages 12–14,<br />

14.1% aga<strong>in</strong>st one or more victims ages 15–16, <strong>and</strong><br />

30.4% aga<strong>in</strong>st one or more victims ages 17 or over;<br />

14.6% of <strong>the</strong> sample had no known victims of any<br />

sexual offenses. These latter patients received assessments<br />

follow<strong>in</strong>g charges of possession of child pornography<br />

or because of <strong>the</strong> patient’s concern regard<strong>in</strong>g his own<br />

sexual <strong>in</strong>terests, etc. The characteristics of <strong>the</strong> victims of<br />

additional 19 patients (4.7%) were not yet verified at <strong>the</strong><br />

time of <strong>the</strong> present <strong>in</strong>vestigation <strong>and</strong> are <strong>in</strong>cluded only <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> phallometric analyses. The sum of <strong>the</strong>se percentages<br />

exceeds 100% due to some offenders hav<strong>in</strong>g victims <strong>in</strong><br />

more than one age category. As <strong>in</strong> Cantor et al. (2004),<br />

no dist<strong>in</strong>ction was made <strong>in</strong> this analysis between <strong>in</strong>trafamilial<br />

offenses (i.e., <strong>in</strong>cest offenses) <strong>and</strong> extrafamilial<br />

offenses.<br />

Measures<br />

<strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong>. Patients <strong>in</strong>dicated <strong>the</strong> h<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>y prefer<br />

to use (right, left, or no preference) <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g<br />

activities: writ<strong>in</strong>g, draw<strong>in</strong>g, throw<strong>in</strong>g, strik<strong>in</strong>g a match,<br />

open<strong>in</strong>g a box, <strong>and</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g scissors, a toothbrush, a knife,<br />

<strong>and</strong> a spoon. This comprised a modified version of<br />

<strong>the</strong> Ed<strong>in</strong>burgh <strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong> Inventory (Oldfield, 1971;<br />

Williams, 1986). The arithmetic difference between <strong>the</strong><br />

number of <strong>the</strong> “right” responses <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> number of<br />

“left” responses, divided by <strong>the</strong> arithmetic sum of <strong>the</strong>se<br />

two numbers yields <strong>the</strong> h<strong>and</strong>edness quotient; that is,<br />

quotient h<strong>and</strong>edness = (right − left)/(right + left). To maximize<br />

<strong>the</strong> normality of <strong>the</strong> result<strong>in</strong>g J-shaped distribution,<br />

<strong>the</strong> h<strong>and</strong>edness quotients were arcs<strong>in</strong> trans<strong>for</strong>med, <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong>n were reflected <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>verted, as recommended by<br />

Tabachnick <strong>and</strong> Fidell (1989).<br />

Phallometric Measurement of Erotic Gender–Age<br />

Preferences. Blanchard, Klassen, Dickey, Kuban, <strong>and</strong><br />

Blak (2001) described <strong>the</strong> phallometric procedure <strong>and</strong><br />

data h<strong>and</strong>l<strong>in</strong>g technique <strong>in</strong> detail. Briefly, a computer<br />

records an exam<strong>in</strong>ee’s penile blood volume while <strong>the</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>ee<br />

observes a st<strong>and</strong>ardized set of stimuli that depict a<br />

variety of activities <strong>and</strong> persons of potential erotic <strong>in</strong>terest<br />

to <strong>the</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>ee. Changes <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>ee’s penile blood<br />

volume (i.e., his degrees of penile erection) <strong>in</strong>dicate his<br />

relative erotic <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> each class of stimuli. Cl<strong>in</strong>icians<br />

<strong>and</strong> researchers employ phallometry to quantify <strong>the</strong> erotic


<strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Pedophilia</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Hebephilia</strong> 451<br />

<strong>in</strong>terests of sexual offenders aga<strong>in</strong>st children (e.g., Howes,<br />

1995), <strong>and</strong> meta-analytic review of 61 studies <strong>in</strong>dicated<br />

that such procedures represented <strong>the</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle most reliable<br />

predictor of which men will commit additional sexual<br />

offenses after release <strong>in</strong>to society (Hanson & Bussière,<br />

1998). The specific protocol <strong>in</strong> use at <strong>the</strong> Kurt Freund<br />

<strong>Laboratory</strong> over <strong>the</strong> course of <strong>the</strong> present <strong>in</strong>vestigation<br />

reliably dist<strong>in</strong>guishes pedophilic from teleiophilic men<br />

(Blanchard et al., 2001).<br />

The stimuli used <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> phallometric test were<br />

audiotaped narratives presented through headphones <strong>and</strong><br />

accompanied by slides. There were seven categories of<br />

narratives. They describe sexual <strong>in</strong>teractions with ei<strong>the</strong>r<br />

female children, female pubescents, female adults, male<br />

children, male pubescents, or male adults, or erotically<br />

neutral (i.e., solitary, nonsexual) activities. The accompany<strong>in</strong>g<br />

slides depicted nude models correspond<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> age<br />

<strong>and</strong> sex to <strong>the</strong> topic of <strong>the</strong> narrative. Neutral narratives<br />

accompanied slides of l<strong>and</strong>scapes.<br />

The data reduction process yielded seven category<br />

scores, one to reflect each of <strong>the</strong> six comb<strong>in</strong>ations of<br />

<strong>the</strong> age group <strong>and</strong> sex of <strong>the</strong> stimuli, plus <strong>the</strong> neutral<br />

category. For <strong>the</strong> present <strong>in</strong>vestigation, three scores were<br />

of <strong>in</strong>terest: <strong>the</strong> response to prepubescent children overall<br />

(i.e., <strong>the</strong> reaction to prepubescent male stimuli or to<br />

prepubescent female stimuli, whichever was greater), <strong>the</strong><br />

response to pubescent children overall, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> response<br />

to adult stimuli overall. Subjects who did not produce<br />

a valid result on <strong>the</strong> phallometric test (e.g., those who<br />

did not respond to any category) were dropped from <strong>the</strong><br />

phallometric analyses.<br />

Sexual History. A st<strong>and</strong>ardized <strong>for</strong>m was used by<br />

<strong>the</strong> phallometric laboratory staff to record each patient’s<br />

history of sexual offenses. The <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation came primarily<br />

from documents that accompanied <strong>the</strong> patient’s<br />

referral, such as reports from police, probation, or parole<br />

officers. Some patients reported additional <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation<br />

<strong>the</strong>mselves, regard<strong>in</strong>g offenses that were not <strong>in</strong>cluded<br />

<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir files <strong>and</strong> <strong>for</strong> which <strong>the</strong>y had not been <strong>for</strong>mally<br />

charged.<br />

For <strong>the</strong> present analyses, four sexual history variables<br />

were of <strong>in</strong>terest: <strong>the</strong> patient’s numbers of victims ages 11<br />

or under (regardless of <strong>the</strong>ir sex), victims ages 12–14<br />

(regardless of <strong>the</strong>ir sex), victims ages 15–16 (regardless<br />

of <strong>the</strong>ir sex), <strong>and</strong> victims ages 17 or older (regardless of<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir sex). For <strong>the</strong> patients with any victims <strong>in</strong> a given age<br />

category, <strong>the</strong> modal number of victims was one; however,<br />

some patients had very many victims, produc<strong>in</strong>g highly<br />

skewed distributions. Moreover, some patients were only<br />

able to provide estimates of <strong>the</strong>ir numbers of victims,<br />

ra<strong>the</strong>r than precise quantities. There<strong>for</strong>e, <strong>the</strong> scores on<br />

<strong>the</strong>se variables were capped at 10.<br />

IQ <strong>and</strong> Age at Test<strong>in</strong>g. A six subtest short-<strong>for</strong>m<br />

of <strong>the</strong> WAIS–R (In<strong>for</strong>mation, Similarities, Digit Span,<br />

Arithmetic, Picture Completion, <strong>and</strong> Block Design) permitted<br />

estimation of each patient’s level of <strong>in</strong>tellectual<br />

function<strong>in</strong>g. IQ scores were estimated from <strong>the</strong> age-scaled<br />

subtest scores by <strong>the</strong> method detailed by Tellegen <strong>and</strong><br />

Briggs (1967), us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tercorrelations among those<br />

subtests <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> WAIS–R st<strong>and</strong>ardization sample. Each<br />

participant’s age was recorded as his age at his last<br />

birthday.<br />

Results<br />

The sample sizes varied <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g analyses<br />

due to miss<strong>in</strong>g data: Some patients failed to provide valid<br />

phallometric test results <strong>and</strong> appeared only <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sexual<br />

history analyses. Similarly, <strong>the</strong> <strong>for</strong>ensic files of some<br />

patients had not been verified at <strong>the</strong> time of <strong>the</strong> present<br />

study, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>se patients appeared only <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> phallometric<br />

analyses.<br />

Greater right-h<strong>and</strong>edness was significantly associated<br />

with patients’ greater age at test<strong>in</strong>g, r(402) = .112,<br />

p = .025, two-tailed, <strong>and</strong> with higher IQ, r(402) = .114,<br />

p = .022, two-tailed, as consistent with <strong>the</strong> literature.<br />

Table I shows <strong>the</strong> partial correlations between <strong>the</strong> h<strong>and</strong>edness<br />

quotients <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> phallometric responses <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> laboratory<br />

to each age category, <strong>and</strong> between <strong>the</strong> h<strong>and</strong>edness<br />

quotients <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> patients’ numbers of victims <strong>in</strong> each age<br />

category. Each entry represents a partial correlation; that<br />

is, <strong>the</strong> effects of estimated IQ <strong>and</strong> age at test<strong>in</strong>g already<br />

have been removed. To facilitate comparison, <strong>the</strong> table<br />

also <strong>in</strong>cludes <strong>the</strong> orig<strong>in</strong>al results from Cantor et al. (2004)<br />

as well as <strong>the</strong> results from comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> samples.<br />

These partial correlations were very close to <strong>the</strong><br />

zero-order correlations of h<strong>and</strong>edness quotient with phallometric<br />

responses <strong>and</strong> numbers of victims (i.e., <strong>the</strong><br />

correlations be<strong>for</strong>e partiall<strong>in</strong>g out effects of IQ <strong>and</strong> age).<br />

Right-h<strong>and</strong>edness scores correlated with phallometric<br />

responses to prepubescents, pubescents, <strong>and</strong> adults at<br />

rs(339) =−.161, .007, <strong>and</strong> .134, ps = .003, .902, <strong>and</strong><br />

.013, respectively. Right-h<strong>and</strong>edness did not correlate<br />

significantly with numbers of victims ages 11 or under,<br />

ages 12–14, ages 15–16, <strong>and</strong> ages 17 or older, rs(380) =<br />

.009, −.046, .057, <strong>and</strong> −.009, ps = .867, .372, .264, <strong>and</strong><br />

.860, respectively.<br />

Discussion<br />

This replication sample demonstrated <strong>the</strong> same pattern<br />

of associations between h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>and</strong> phallometric


452 Cantor et al.<br />

Table I. Partial Correlations of <strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong> with Phallometric Responses <strong>and</strong> Numbers of Victims<br />

Sample<br />

Indicator of erotic age preference Orig<strong>in</strong>al sample Replication sample Comb<strong>in</strong>ed samples<br />

Phallometric response n = 377 n = 341 n = 718<br />

Pedophilic stimuli −.13 ∗ −.16 ∗∗ −.15 ∗∗∗<br />

Hebephilic stimuli .04 −.00 .02<br />

Teleiophilic stimuli .07 .13 ∗ .10 ∗<br />

Number of victims n = 455 n = 384 n = 839<br />

Ages 11 <strong>and</strong> under −.13 ∗ .01 −.07<br />

Ages 12–14 −.04 −.05 −.04<br />

Ages 15–16 −.06 .06 −.00<br />

Ages 17 <strong>and</strong> older .06 −.01 −.00<br />

Note. Entries represent <strong>the</strong> partial correlations between h<strong>and</strong>edness quotients on a modified version of <strong>the</strong><br />

Ed<strong>in</strong>burgh <strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong> Inventory (Oldfield, 1971; Williams, 1986) <strong>and</strong> each phallometric age category<br />

or number of victims, with IQ <strong>and</strong> age at test<strong>in</strong>g as covariates. Phallometric responses are quantified as<br />

ipsative z scores, based only on <strong>the</strong> patient’s own data. Numbers of victims are capped at 10. Data <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

orig<strong>in</strong>al sample are from Cantor et al. (2004).<br />

∗ p ≤ .05, two-tailed. ∗∗ p ≤ .005, two-tailed. ∗∗∗ p ≤ .0005, two-tailed.<br />

responses as did <strong>the</strong> orig<strong>in</strong>al sample <strong>in</strong> Cantor et al. (2004).<br />

Phallometric responses to <strong>the</strong> youngest age category<br />

correlated negatively with h<strong>and</strong>edness scores (<strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

less right-h<strong>and</strong>edness), <strong>and</strong> phallometric responses<br />

to <strong>the</strong> oldest age category correlated positively with<br />

h<strong>and</strong>edness scores (<strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g more right-h<strong>and</strong>edness),<br />

<strong>the</strong> latter association be<strong>in</strong>g statistically significant <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> replication sample only. For each of <strong>the</strong> replication,<br />

orig<strong>in</strong>al, <strong>and</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ed samples, h<strong>and</strong>edness correlated<br />

with <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>termediate age category, hebephilia, at an<br />

<strong>in</strong>termediate level, not significantly different from zero.<br />

The lack of such a correlation, however, does not necessarily<br />

<strong>in</strong>dicate that h<strong>and</strong>edness is irrelevant to hebephilia.<br />

Ra<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong> uncorrelated scores may reflect <strong>the</strong> status of<br />

<strong>the</strong> hebephilic stimuli as an <strong>in</strong>termediate class among<br />

three ord<strong>in</strong>al <strong>and</strong> related characteristics. A man will show<br />

his greatest phallometric response to <strong>the</strong> class of stimuli<br />

that <strong>in</strong>terest him <strong>the</strong> most <strong>and</strong> will show his second<br />

greatest response to <strong>the</strong> next closest age category. That<br />

is, a man most erotically <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> adults will show<br />

his greatest response to adults, but his second greatest<br />

responses to pubescents (Freund, 1967; Freund, Langev<strong>in</strong>,<br />

Cibiri, & Zajac, 1973). Similarly, men most erotically<br />

<strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> children will react <strong>the</strong> most to children<br />

<strong>and</strong> show <strong>the</strong>ir second greatest responses to <strong>the</strong> next<br />

closest age category, <strong>in</strong> this case to pubescents, aga<strong>in</strong><br />

(Freund, Langev<strong>in</strong>, Wescom, & Zajac, 1975). Thus, <strong>the</strong><br />

magnitude of teleiophilic responses predicts greater righth<strong>and</strong>edness,<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> magnitude of pedophilic responses<br />

predicts greater non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness, but <strong>the</strong> magnitude<br />

of hebephilic responses <strong>in</strong>discrim<strong>in</strong>ately comb<strong>in</strong>es <strong>the</strong><br />

secondary responses from both o<strong>the</strong>r types of men<br />

toge<strong>the</strong>r with those from men with genu<strong>in</strong>e hebephilia,<br />

mask<strong>in</strong>g any underly<strong>in</strong>g association. As a test of this<br />

possibility, samples of hebephilic men could be compared<br />

with <strong>in</strong>dependent samples of pedophilic <strong>and</strong> teleiophilic<br />

men. We explore this fur<strong>the</strong>r <strong>in</strong> Study 2.<br />

For nei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> replication nor comb<strong>in</strong>ed samples<br />

did patients’ numbers of victims <strong>in</strong> any of <strong>the</strong> various age<br />

categories show any systematic association with h<strong>and</strong>edness<br />

scores, unlike with <strong>the</strong>ir phallometric responses.<br />

Although phallometric response <strong>and</strong> sexual history both<br />

attempt to capture <strong>the</strong> same underly<strong>in</strong>g construct—erotic<br />

age preference—sexual history may be more subject<br />

to misclassification. A man can sometimes suppress<br />

his phallometric responses dur<strong>in</strong>g test<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> controlled<br />

conditions, yield<strong>in</strong>g an undifferentiat<strong>in</strong>g profile. Patients<br />

produc<strong>in</strong>g such profiles, however, are typically dropped<br />

from analysis, thus produc<strong>in</strong>g samples of demonstrably<br />

pedophilic men, demonstrably hebephilic men, <strong>and</strong><br />

demonstrably teleiophilic men. Men unclassifiable by<br />

phallometric profile represent miss<strong>in</strong>g data <strong>for</strong> phallometric<br />

analysis, but still provide usable data <strong>for</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

analyses. Victim history data have <strong>the</strong> empirical advantage<br />

of not requir<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> loss of <strong>the</strong> data from men who<br />

produce no valid phallometric profile, but suffer o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

disadvantages: (1) Victim history only reflects <strong>the</strong> victim<br />

history known to <strong>the</strong> researchers, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>re may exist<br />

unknown victims <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r age/sex categories unavailable<br />

to <strong>the</strong> categorization procedure applied to <strong>the</strong> victim<br />

history data. (2) Victim history can be <strong>in</strong>fluenced by <strong>the</strong><br />

types of victims available to <strong>the</strong> offender <strong>and</strong> thus, aga<strong>in</strong>,<br />

fail to reflect his actual erotic preferences. This would<br />

be particularly true of men with a s<strong>in</strong>gle (known) victim.


<strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Pedophilia</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Hebephilia</strong> 453<br />

(3) Cantor et al. (2004) <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> present analysis comb<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

offenders with <strong>in</strong>trafamilial <strong>and</strong> extrafamilial victims,<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>trafamilial offenders appear to be less likely to be<br />

genu<strong>in</strong>ely pedophilic, as already mentioned. Assortment<br />

by phallometric response would separate teleiophilic<br />

offenders aga<strong>in</strong>st <strong>in</strong>trafamilial children from pedophilic<br />

offenders aga<strong>in</strong>st <strong>in</strong>trafamilial children; assortment by<br />

victim age group, however, would comb<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong>m. In his<br />

sample, Bogaert (2001) elim<strong>in</strong>ated men who committed<br />

solely <strong>in</strong>trafamilial offenses; however, an <strong>in</strong>trafamilial<br />

offender would still have been <strong>in</strong>cluded if he also had<br />

an extrafamilial victim. Thus, nei<strong>the</strong>r study <strong>in</strong>cluded a<br />

sample of offenders aga<strong>in</strong>st extrafamilial children only.<br />

Given <strong>the</strong> reliability of <strong>the</strong> association between<br />

h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>and</strong> erotic age preference <strong>in</strong> general, we<br />

undertook Study 2 to answer questions (2)–(4) from <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>troduction. To identify any differences between patients<br />

most <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> male versus female victims <strong>and</strong> to<br />

identify any differences between patients most <strong>in</strong>terested<br />

<strong>in</strong> prepubescent children versus pubescent children versus<br />

adults, men were assigned to non-overlapp<strong>in</strong>g groups,<br />

each represent<strong>in</strong>g one of <strong>the</strong> six comb<strong>in</strong>ations of <strong>the</strong><br />

two sex <strong>and</strong> three age groups. Group assignments were<br />

carried out first us<strong>in</strong>g patients’ phallometric test results<br />

to <strong>in</strong>dicate <strong>the</strong>ir preferred category <strong>and</strong> aga<strong>in</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong><br />

predom<strong>in</strong>ant sex <strong>and</strong> age group from <strong>the</strong> patients’ actual<br />

victim history. The rates of non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness of each<br />

group appear as proportions to permit comparison of<br />

<strong>the</strong>se proportions with those <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> published literature. To<br />

determ<strong>in</strong>e whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>clusion of <strong>in</strong>trafamilial offenders<br />

reduced group differences, analyses were repeated first<br />

<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>n exclud<strong>in</strong>g patients who committed<br />

<strong>in</strong>trafamilial sexual offenses. To provide sufficient numbers<br />

of observations <strong>for</strong> reliable analyses of offender<br />

subtypes, <strong>the</strong>se analyses comb<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>the</strong> replication sample<br />

just described with <strong>the</strong> orig<strong>in</strong>al sample first described <strong>in</strong><br />

Cantor et al. (2004).<br />

STUDY 2<br />

Methods<br />

Measures<br />

Phallometric Group Assignment. Phallometric data<br />

were acquired as already described, <strong>and</strong> each patient<br />

received group assignment accord<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> stimulus<br />

category which elicited from him <strong>the</strong> greatest genital<br />

response: male adults, male pubescents, male children,<br />

female adults, female pubescents, or female children. That<br />

category provided <strong>the</strong> sex (male or female) <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> age<br />

group (children, pubescents, or adults) of greatest erotic<br />

<strong>in</strong>terest to him. The analysis dropped patients who did not<br />

provide a valid phallometric profile.<br />

Sexual History Group Assignment. The numbers of<br />

victims <strong>for</strong> which a patient was charged or <strong>for</strong> which<br />

he admitted contact were acquired as already described.<br />

Each patient was assigned to whichever sex <strong>and</strong> age group<br />

was his predom<strong>in</strong>ant victim type. In order to match <strong>the</strong><br />

number of categories available to <strong>the</strong> phallometrically<br />

determ<strong>in</strong>ed groups (six), <strong>the</strong> numbers of victims ages 12–<br />

14 <strong>and</strong> victims ages 15–16 were collapsed <strong>in</strong>to a s<strong>in</strong>gle<br />

variable represent<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> number of victims ages 12–16.<br />

The scores on this new variable were capped at 10, to<br />

ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> consistency with <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r variables which were<br />

already capped at 10. Thus, each case was categorized<br />

accord<strong>in</strong>g to his numbers of victims <strong>in</strong> six categories:<br />

females ages 17 or older, females ages 12–16, females<br />

ages 11 or younger, males ages 17 or older, males ages<br />

12–16, <strong>and</strong> males ages 11 or younger.<br />

We operationally def<strong>in</strong>ed predom<strong>in</strong>ant victim type<br />

conservatively. To be assigned to a category, a patient first<br />

had to have had more victims <strong>in</strong> that category than <strong>in</strong><br />

any of <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r five categories. This rule <strong>the</strong>re<strong>for</strong>e had<br />

<strong>the</strong> additional <strong>and</strong> beneficial effect of remov<strong>in</strong>g from <strong>the</strong><br />

analyses those patients whose preferred type of victim<br />

was <strong>in</strong>determ<strong>in</strong>ate because of equal numbers of victims<br />

<strong>in</strong> more than one category. (Moreover, this rule dropped<br />

patients with zero victims, who necessarily would have <strong>the</strong><br />

same, zero, number of victims <strong>in</strong> each category.) Although<br />

it is relatively <strong>in</strong>tuitive that a patient with multiple victims<br />

of a particular sex <strong>and</strong> age group is erotically <strong>in</strong>terested<br />

<strong>in</strong> such persons, it is not at all clear whe<strong>the</strong>r a patient<br />

with a s<strong>in</strong>gle victim is most <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> that particular<br />

sex <strong>and</strong> age group. That is, some unknown number of<br />

men committ<strong>in</strong>g sexual offenses did so aga<strong>in</strong>st a person<br />

not of <strong>the</strong>ir genu<strong>in</strong>ely preferred sex <strong>and</strong> age category,<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> risk of misclassification is greatest among men<br />

with s<strong>in</strong>gle victims. To m<strong>in</strong>imize such misclassifications,<br />

we <strong>the</strong>re<strong>for</strong>e applied <strong>the</strong> additional criterion that <strong>the</strong><br />

number of victims <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> predom<strong>in</strong>ant category had to<br />

be greater than <strong>the</strong> number of victims <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> next most<br />

frequent category plus one. Thus, <strong>for</strong> example, someone<br />

with victims number<strong>in</strong>g 1, 1, 3, 0, 0, <strong>and</strong> 0 across <strong>the</strong><br />

six categories would be assigned to <strong>the</strong> group <strong>in</strong>dicated<br />

by <strong>the</strong> category with three victims, but someone with<br />

victims number<strong>in</strong>g 1, 1, 2, 0, 0, <strong>and</strong> 0 or with victims<br />

number<strong>in</strong>g 0, 0, 0, 1, 0, <strong>and</strong> 0 would rema<strong>in</strong> unassigned<br />

(<strong>and</strong> dropped from analysis), lack<strong>in</strong>g a predom<strong>in</strong>antly<br />

preferred category of victim.<br />

Assignment to one of <strong>the</strong> categories (homosexual<br />

teleiophilia) required h<strong>and</strong>l<strong>in</strong>g by an alternate method.<br />

The Kurt Freund <strong>Laboratory</strong> receives only very few cases


454 Cantor et al.<br />

of persons with any sexual offenses aga<strong>in</strong>st adult males.<br />

The <strong>Laboratory</strong> does, however, conduct assessments of<br />

non-crim<strong>in</strong>al men seek<strong>in</strong>g evaluation to help <strong>the</strong>m identify<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir own sexual orientation as straight or gay men. Thus,<br />

a person was also classified as predom<strong>in</strong>antly <strong>in</strong>terested<br />

<strong>in</strong> adult males if his number of consent<strong>in</strong>g, adult male<br />

sexual partners outnumbered his number of consent<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

adult female sexual partners <strong>and</strong> if <strong>the</strong> patient had no<br />

sexual contacts after his 16th birthday with anyone who<br />

was both younger than 15 <strong>and</strong> more than 5 years younger<br />

than <strong>the</strong> patient.<br />

The category to which each patient was assigned<br />

thus provided <strong>the</strong> sex (male or female) <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> age group<br />

(children, pubescents, or adults) of greatest erotic <strong>in</strong>terest<br />

to him. The numbers of <strong>in</strong>trafamilial <strong>and</strong> extrafamilial<br />

victims were recorded separately.<br />

<strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong>. Because <strong>the</strong> cl<strong>in</strong>ical database records<br />

patients’ <strong>in</strong>dividual responses to each item on <strong>the</strong><br />

Ed<strong>in</strong>burgh <strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong> Inventory, patients could be dichotomously<br />

categorized accord<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong>ir response to<br />

<strong>the</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle item, “Which h<strong>and</strong> do you write with?” The<br />

follow<strong>in</strong>g analyses classified cases as simply right-h<strong>and</strong>ed<br />

or non-right-h<strong>and</strong>ed; patients <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g no preference <strong>for</strong><br />

writ<strong>in</strong>g h<strong>and</strong> were scored as non-right-h<strong>and</strong>ed.<br />

IQ <strong>and</strong> Age at Test<strong>in</strong>g. The IQ <strong>and</strong> age at most recent<br />

birthday were acquired as already described.<br />

Data Analysis<br />

Logistic regression provided <strong>the</strong> significance test<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> ma<strong>in</strong> effects of preferred sex of victim <strong>and</strong><br />

preferred age group of victim on writ<strong>in</strong>g h<strong>and</strong>, both be<strong>for</strong>e<br />

<strong>and</strong> after enter<strong>in</strong>g IQ <strong>and</strong> age at test<strong>in</strong>g as covariates. Analyses<br />

were conducted first with all cases classifiable with<br />

<strong>the</strong> preced<strong>in</strong>g methods <strong>and</strong> aga<strong>in</strong> after elim<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g cases<br />

who had any <strong>in</strong>trafamilial victims, to determ<strong>in</strong>e whe<strong>the</strong>r<br />

<strong>in</strong>trafamilial offenders masked underly<strong>in</strong>g associations,<br />

as already described.<br />

Results<br />

Sort<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>trafamilial <strong>and</strong> extrafamilial offenders<br />

by <strong>the</strong> category of <strong>the</strong>ir greatest phallometric response<br />

classified 727 cases: 325 cases responded <strong>the</strong> most to<br />

female adults, 242 to female pubescents, 41 to female<br />

children, 38 to male adults, 40 to male pubescents, <strong>and</strong> 41<br />

to male children.<br />

Panel A of Fig. 1 <strong>in</strong>dicates <strong>the</strong> proportions of each of<br />

<strong>the</strong>se six categories who used <strong>the</strong>ir right h<strong>and</strong> <strong>for</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

The non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness rates of both of <strong>the</strong> teleiophilic<br />

categories—men who phallometrically respond <strong>the</strong> most<br />

to ei<strong>the</strong>r adult women, 10.5%, or to adult men, 7.9%—<br />

fell with<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> range of <strong>the</strong> general population. The men<br />

who responded <strong>the</strong> most to <strong>the</strong> pedophilic categories,<br />

however, showed non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness at approximately<br />

triple those rates. Logistic regression of writ<strong>in</strong>g h<strong>and</strong> onto<br />

<strong>the</strong> sex <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> age group of cases’ most preferred category<br />

<strong>in</strong>dicated a significant association, χ 2 (3,N = 727) =<br />

15.07, p = .002; <strong>the</strong> model add<strong>in</strong>g IQ <strong>and</strong> age at test<strong>in</strong>g as<br />

covariates was also significant, χ 2 (5,N = 727) = 28.45,<br />

p = .00003. Table II provides <strong>the</strong> logistic regression<br />

coefficients <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir st<strong>and</strong>ard errors, <strong>the</strong> odds ratios<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir 95% confidence <strong>in</strong>tervals, <strong>the</strong> Wald statistics,<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> reliability <strong>for</strong> each predictor <strong>and</strong> covariate.<br />

The age group, but not <strong>the</strong> sex, represented by each<br />

patient’s <strong>for</strong>emost erotic <strong>in</strong>terest significantly predicted<br />

<strong>the</strong> patient’s h<strong>and</strong>edness. Although both <strong>the</strong> pedophilic<br />

<strong>and</strong> hebephilic samples showed higher rates of nonright-h<strong>and</strong>edness<br />

than <strong>the</strong> teleiophilic sample, only <strong>the</strong><br />

pedophilic sample was significantly so.<br />

The criteria already described <strong>for</strong> sort<strong>in</strong>g cases by<br />

predom<strong>in</strong>ant victim type classified 295 cases: 101 cases<br />

were most erotically <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> female adults, 44 <strong>in</strong><br />

female pubescents, 71 <strong>in</strong> female children, 19 <strong>in</strong> male<br />

adults, 22 <strong>in</strong> male pubescents, <strong>and</strong> 38 <strong>in</strong> male children.<br />

Panel B of Fig. 1 <strong>in</strong>dicates <strong>the</strong> proportions of men <strong>in</strong> each<br />

category who use <strong>the</strong> right h<strong>and</strong> <strong>for</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g. Although<br />

<strong>the</strong> profile of <strong>the</strong> proportions <strong>for</strong> each group somewhat<br />

resembled that from sort<strong>in</strong>g by phallometric responses,<br />

<strong>the</strong> overall model was not significant, χ 2 (3,N = 295) =<br />

2.58, p = .46. The model <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> two covariates<br />

approached significance, χ 2 (5,N = 295) = 10.03, p =<br />

.074, but this reflected <strong>the</strong> association of h<strong>and</strong>edness with<br />

<strong>the</strong> covariates, age <strong>and</strong> IQ (see Table III).<br />

To determ<strong>in</strong>e whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> failure of sexual history<br />

to reveal any group differences was attributable, at least<br />

<strong>in</strong> part, to misclassifications of men with <strong>in</strong>trafamilial<br />

victims, <strong>the</strong> preced<strong>in</strong>g categorizations <strong>and</strong> analyses were<br />

repeated, elim<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g cases with any history of <strong>in</strong>trafamilial<br />

victims. Sort<strong>in</strong>g only <strong>the</strong> extrafamilial offenders by<br />

phallometric responses now yielded 251 cases respond<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>the</strong> most to female adults, 183 to female pubescents,<br />

32 to female children, 28 to male adults, 37 to male<br />

pubescents, <strong>and</strong> 39 to male children. The removal of cases<br />

with any <strong>in</strong>trafamilial victims from <strong>the</strong> phallometrically<br />

derived categories produced virtually <strong>the</strong> same pattern<br />

of results as did <strong>the</strong> phallometrically derived categories<br />

that <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong>trafamilial offenders (cf., panels A <strong>and</strong> C<br />

of Fig. 1). <strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong> significantly related to preferred<br />

category, χ 2 (3,N = 570) = 8.89, p = .03 <strong>and</strong> rema<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

significant after <strong>the</strong> addition to <strong>the</strong> model of <strong>the</strong> two<br />

covariates, χ 2 (5,N = 570) = 17.72, p = .003. The age<br />

group of stimulus category was <strong>the</strong> most reliable predictor<br />

<strong>in</strong> each model (see Table IV).


<strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Pedophilia</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Hebephilia</strong> 455<br />

Table II. Logistic Regression of Writ<strong>in</strong>g H<strong>and</strong> onto Sex <strong>and</strong> Age Group of Phallometric Category Elicit<strong>in</strong>g Maximum Response<br />

Predictor B SEB Wald statistic Odds ratio (e B ) CI of odds ratio p<br />

Model one<br />

Sex of category 0.33 0.32 1.11 1.40 0.75–2.60 .29<br />

Age group of category 15.84 .0004<br />

Adult vs. child 1.30 0.33 15.83 3.68 1.94–6.98 .00007<br />

Adult vs. pubescent<br />

Model two<br />

0.39 0.24 2.53 1.47 0.91–2.37 .11<br />

Patient IQ −0.01 0.01 2.91 0.99 0.97–1.00 .09<br />

Patient age at test<strong>in</strong>g −0.03 0.01 8.28 0.98 0.96–0.99 .004<br />

Sex of category 0.26 0.32 0.64 1.29 0.69–2.44 .43<br />

Age group of category 14.25 .0008<br />

Adult vs. child 1.26 0.34 14.25 3.54 1.84–6.81 .0002<br />

Adult vs. pubescent 0.38 0.25 2.40 1.47 0.90–2.38 .12<br />

Note. N = 727; SEB = st<strong>and</strong>ard error of <strong>the</strong> regression coefficient; CI = 95% confidence <strong>in</strong>terval.<br />

Table III. Logistic Regression of Writ<strong>in</strong>g H<strong>and</strong> onto Sex <strong>and</strong> Age Group of Predom<strong>in</strong>ant Victim Category<br />

Predictor B SEB Wald statistic Odds ratio (e B ) CI of odds ratio p<br />

Model one<br />

Sex of category 0.00 0.39 0.00 1.00 0.46–2.16 1.00<br />

Age group of category 2.36 .31<br />

Adult vs. child 0.61 0.42 2.09 1.83 0.81–4.16 .15<br />

Adult vs. pubescent<br />

Model two<br />

0.57 0.47 1.46 1.77 0.70–4.46 .23<br />

Patient IQ −0.02 0.01 3.15 0.98 0.96–1.00 .08<br />

Patient age at test<strong>in</strong>g −0.02 0.01 2.84 0.98 0.95–1.00 .09<br />

Sex of category −0.12 0.40 0.09 0.89 0.40–1.96 .77<br />

Age group of category 2.83 .24<br />

Adult vs. child 0.63 0.43 2.20 1.88 0.82–4.35 .14<br />

Adult vs. pubescent 0.70 0.48 2.14 2.02 0.79–6.19 .14<br />

Note. N = 295; SEB = st<strong>and</strong>ard error of <strong>the</strong> regression coefficient; CI = 95% confidence <strong>in</strong>terval.<br />

Table IV. Logistic Regression of Writ<strong>in</strong>g H<strong>and</strong> onto Sex <strong>and</strong> Age Group of Phallometric Category Elicit<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Maximum Response—Exclud<strong>in</strong>g Patients with Intrafamilial Victims<br />

Predictor B SEB Wald statistic Odds ratio (e B ) CI of odds ratio p<br />

Model one<br />

Sex of category 0.30 0.34 0.80 1.35 0.70–2.60 .37<br />

Age group of category 9.20 .01<br />

Adult vs. child 1.10 0.36 9.16 2.99 1.47–6.08 .002<br />

Adult vs. pubescent<br />

Model two<br />

0.36 0.27 1.87 1.44 0.85–2.42 .17<br />

Patient IQ −0.02 0.01 3.92 0.99 0.97–1.00 .048<br />

Patient age at test<strong>in</strong>g −0.02 0.01 3.24 0.98 0.97–1.00 .07<br />

Sex of category 0.19 0.34 0.32 1.21 0.62–2.37 .57<br />

Age group of category 7.54 .02<br />

Adult vs. child 1.01 0.37 7.52 2.75 1.34–5.66 .006<br />

Adult vs. pubescent 0.33 0.27 1.50 1.39 0.82–2.36 .22<br />

Note. N = 570; SEB = st<strong>and</strong>ard error of <strong>the</strong> regression coefficient; CI = 95% confidence <strong>in</strong>terval.


456 Cantor et al.<br />

Fig. 1. Proportions of patients <strong>in</strong> each group who use <strong>the</strong> left h<strong>and</strong> <strong>for</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g. The six groups<br />

represent <strong>the</strong> sex (female or male) <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> age group (adult, pubescent child, or prepubescent<br />

child) of primary erotic <strong>in</strong>terest to <strong>the</strong> patient. Panels A <strong>and</strong> B represent both extrafamilial <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>trafamilial (i.e., <strong>in</strong>cest) offenses; Panels C <strong>and</strong> D represent only those patients who committed<br />

extrafamilial <strong>and</strong> no <strong>in</strong>trafamilial offenses. For Panels A <strong>and</strong> C, patients were grouped accord<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

<strong>the</strong> stimuli that elicited <strong>the</strong>ir greatest genital (phallometric) response. For Panels B <strong>and</strong> D, patients<br />

were grouped accord<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> sex <strong>and</strong> age group of <strong>the</strong>ir predom<strong>in</strong>ant victim type. See text <strong>for</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> exact classification procedures. Results of <strong>the</strong> logistic regressions of <strong>the</strong> h<strong>and</strong>edness rates <strong>in</strong><br />

Panels A–D appear <strong>in</strong> Tables II–V, respectively.<br />

Re-apply<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> group<strong>in</strong>g criteria <strong>for</strong> sexual history<br />

after remov<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> cases with <strong>in</strong>trafamilial victims resulted<br />

<strong>in</strong> 96 patients most erotically <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> female<br />

adults, 31 <strong>in</strong> female pubescents, 48 <strong>in</strong> female children,<br />

19 <strong>in</strong> male adults, 22 <strong>in</strong> male pubescents, <strong>and</strong> 31 <strong>in</strong><br />

male children. After exclud<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>trafamilial offenders,<br />

<strong>the</strong> proportions of non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness among <strong>the</strong> six<br />

categories of extrafamilial offenders much more closely<br />

resembled those shown by <strong>the</strong> phallometrically derived<br />

categories (cf., panels D, A, <strong>and</strong> C of Fig. 1). Logistic<br />

regression <strong>in</strong>dicated that h<strong>and</strong>edness related to <strong>the</strong> set<br />

of predictors after <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g IQ <strong>and</strong> chronological age,<br />

χ 2 (5,N = 247) = 14.26, p = .01, but only marg<strong>in</strong>ally<br />

so be<strong>for</strong>eh<strong>and</strong>, χ 2 (3,N = 247) = 7.06, p = .07. Once<br />

aga<strong>in</strong>, <strong>the</strong> age group, but not <strong>the</strong> sex, of <strong>the</strong> categories<br />

significantly related to h<strong>and</strong>edness (see Table V). In this


<strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Pedophilia</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Hebephilia</strong> 457<br />

Table V. Logistic Regression of Writ<strong>in</strong>g H<strong>and</strong> onto Sex <strong>and</strong> Age Group of Predom<strong>in</strong>ant Victim<br />

Category—Exclud<strong>in</strong>g Patients with Intrafamilial Victims<br />

Predictor B SEB Wald statistic Odds ratio (e B ) CI of odds ratio p<br />

Model one<br />

Sex of category 0.26 0.42 0.39 1.30 0.57–2.96 .54<br />

Age group of category 6.42 .04<br />

Adult vs. child 1.07 0.46 5.45 2.93 1.19–7.21 .02<br />

Adult vs. pubescent<br />

Model two<br />

1.07 0.50 4.51 2.92 1.09–7.85 .03<br />

Patient IQ −0.03 0.01 5.17 0.97 0.95–1.00 .02<br />

Patient age at test<strong>in</strong>g −0.01 0.02 0.82 0.99 0.96–1.02 .36<br />

Sex of category 0.13 0.44 0.09 1.14 0.49–2.68 .76<br />

Age group of category 6.37 .04<br />

Adult vs. child 1.06 0.47 5.13 2.90 1.15–7.28 .02<br />

Adult vs. pubescent 1.13 0.52 4.79 3.09 1.13–8.46 .03<br />

Note. N = 247; SEB = st<strong>and</strong>ard error of <strong>the</strong> regression coefficient; CI = 95% confidence <strong>in</strong>terval.<br />

analysis, both <strong>the</strong> pedophilic <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> hebephilic samples<br />

differed significantly from <strong>the</strong> teleiophilic sample, show<strong>in</strong>g<br />

odds of non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness approximately triple<br />

that of offenders aga<strong>in</strong>st adults.<br />

Discussion<br />

These analyses confirmed <strong>the</strong> association between<br />

h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>and</strong> erotic age preference. Study 1 <strong>in</strong>dicated<br />

that <strong>the</strong> procedures of Cantor et al. (2004) provided a<br />

reliable, albeit small, correlation between <strong>the</strong>se characteristics.<br />

Study 2, us<strong>in</strong>g both <strong>the</strong> <strong>for</strong>mer <strong>and</strong> a new<br />

sample, <strong>in</strong>dicated that <strong>the</strong> rate of non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness<br />

<strong>in</strong> pedophilic men was nearly triple that <strong>in</strong> teleiophilic<br />

men. With an odds ratio of approximately three, after<br />

account<strong>in</strong>g <strong>for</strong> group differences <strong>in</strong> IQ <strong>and</strong> chronological<br />

age, <strong>the</strong> association of pedophilia with h<strong>and</strong>edness equals<br />

or exceeds that of several major neurological disorders,<br />

<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g Down’s syndrome <strong>and</strong> autism. The elevated<br />

rates of non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness seem to be associated<br />

specifically with pedophilia <strong>and</strong> not with sexual offend<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>in</strong> general; men who sexually offended aga<strong>in</strong>st two or<br />

more female adults—<strong>and</strong> aga<strong>in</strong>st no children—showed<br />

rates of non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness clearly with<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> range<br />

shown by <strong>the</strong> general population.<br />

Be<strong>for</strong>e account<strong>in</strong>g <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> covariates, <strong>the</strong> patients<br />

with erotic <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> males showed 1.30–1.40 greater<br />

odds of be<strong>in</strong>g non-right-h<strong>and</strong>ed than did patients with<br />

erotic <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> females (with <strong>the</strong> exception of <strong>the</strong> analyses<br />

shown <strong>in</strong> Table III). Although <strong>the</strong>se odds ratios did not<br />

achieve statistical significance, <strong>the</strong>y were very close both<br />

<strong>in</strong> direction <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> magnitude to a prior meta-analytic<br />

comparison of heterosexual men with homosexual men<br />

(all presumably teleiophilic), which found an odds ratio<br />

of 1.34, with male homosexuality show<strong>in</strong>g greater odds<br />

of be<strong>in</strong>g non-right-h<strong>and</strong>ed be<strong>for</strong>e controll<strong>in</strong>g <strong>for</strong> age<br />

(Lalumière, Blanchard, & Zucker, 2000). This suggests,<br />

<strong>the</strong>re<strong>for</strong>e, not that hetero-/homosexuality is unrelated to<br />

h<strong>and</strong>edness, but that <strong>the</strong> present sample did not have as<br />

much statistical power as did <strong>the</strong> meta-analysis to detect<br />

a difference of similar magnitude.<br />

These data provided only partial evidence <strong>for</strong> a difference<br />

<strong>in</strong> rates of non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness between hebephilia<br />

<strong>and</strong> teleiophilia. Although <strong>the</strong> hebephilic sample scored<br />

midway between <strong>the</strong> pedophilic <strong>and</strong> teleiophilic samples<br />

<strong>in</strong> most analyses, <strong>the</strong> contrast between <strong>the</strong> hebe- <strong>and</strong><br />

teleiophilic samples did not always achieve statistical<br />

significance, <strong>and</strong> nei<strong>the</strong>r phallometric responses to<br />

pubescents nor number of victims 12–14 nor 15–16 correlated<br />

with h<strong>and</strong>edness scores. As already discussed, <strong>the</strong><br />

correlations may reflect, not a lack of an association, but an<br />

association that becomes masked by responses to related<br />

erotic categories of stimulus. The rates of non-righth<strong>and</strong>edness<br />

among hebephilic men <strong>in</strong>termediate between<br />

<strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r groups is consistent with this <strong>in</strong>terpretation. It<br />

would be worthwhile to repeat <strong>the</strong> group analyses with a<br />

sample of hebephiles <strong>and</strong> teleiophiles only.<br />

The relative difficulty <strong>in</strong> identify<strong>in</strong>g a significant<br />

h<strong>and</strong>edness difference between hebephilic <strong>and</strong> teleiophilic<br />

groups may serve as a caution to future <strong>in</strong>vestigators.<br />

As used here, <strong>the</strong> age range of victims used to <strong>in</strong>dicate<br />

hebephilia was 12–16, while <strong>the</strong> victim age range taken<br />

to <strong>in</strong>dicate pedophilia was 0–11 years. Thus, one would<br />

expect <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g difficulty <strong>in</strong> detect<strong>in</strong>g group differences<br />

<strong>in</strong> h<strong>and</strong>edness (<strong>and</strong>, perhaps, <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r characteristics)<br />

with <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g mean age of victims. That is, samples<br />

of sexual offenders aga<strong>in</strong>st children may be less likely


458 Cantor et al.<br />

to demonstrate group differences <strong>in</strong> h<strong>and</strong>edness (<strong>and</strong><br />

perhaps <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r characteristics) when those samples<br />

<strong>in</strong>clude larger proportions of sexual offenders aga<strong>in</strong>st<br />

older children.<br />

The present results argue aga<strong>in</strong>st IQ <strong>and</strong> chronological<br />

age as explanations of <strong>the</strong> association between<br />

h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>and</strong> erotic age preference, because <strong>the</strong> association<br />

rema<strong>in</strong>ed significant after remov<strong>in</strong>g any effects of<br />

IQ <strong>and</strong> age. Societal ef<strong>for</strong>ts to change one’s h<strong>and</strong>edness<br />

can also be ruled out as an explanation. Ef<strong>for</strong>ts to<br />

alter h<strong>and</strong>edness perta<strong>in</strong> to one type of change only:<br />

tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g natural left-h<strong>and</strong>ers to use <strong>the</strong>ir right h<strong>and</strong>. Such<br />

unidirectional ef<strong>for</strong>ts would serve to reduce any group<br />

differences <strong>in</strong> h<strong>and</strong>edness. Thus, detection of a significant<br />

group difference <strong>in</strong> rates of non-right-h<strong>and</strong>edness should<br />

be <strong>in</strong>terpreted as hav<strong>in</strong>g occurred despite any pressures to<br />

change h<strong>and</strong>edness, ra<strong>the</strong>r than because of it.<br />

The cl<strong>in</strong>ical data available <strong>for</strong> study did not permit<br />

any dist<strong>in</strong>ction between natural left-h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>and</strong><br />

pathological left-h<strong>and</strong>edness. Thus, <strong>for</strong> some unknown<br />

proportion of <strong>the</strong> present sample, left-h<strong>and</strong>edness developed<br />

<strong>for</strong> reasons unrelated to any pathological process.<br />

The l<strong>in</strong>k between h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>and</strong> pedophilia suggests<br />

that pedophilic samples (<strong>and</strong>, possibly, <strong>the</strong> hebephilic<br />

samples) would conta<strong>in</strong> an excess specifically of pathological<br />

left-h<strong>and</strong>ers <strong>and</strong> not natural left-h<strong>and</strong>ers. Conversely,<br />

<strong>the</strong> teleiophilic left-h<strong>and</strong>ers would be more likely to<br />

be natural ra<strong>the</strong>r than pathological left-h<strong>and</strong>ers. If true,<br />

<strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> better an <strong>in</strong>vestigation is able to dist<strong>in</strong>guish<br />

pathological from natural left-h<strong>and</strong>edness, <strong>the</strong> greater <strong>the</strong><br />

group difference <strong>in</strong> h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigation would<br />

detect. Some researchers have used <strong>the</strong> left-h<strong>and</strong>edness of<br />

one or both of a patient’s parents as a marker suggest<strong>in</strong>g<br />

that <strong>the</strong> patient is more likely to have developed lefth<strong>and</strong>edness<br />

from a normal, genetic route ra<strong>the</strong>r than<br />

from a pathological factor (e.g., Bradshaw-McAnulty,<br />

Hicks, & K<strong>in</strong>sbourne, 1984); however, familial s<strong>in</strong>istrality<br />

can serve only as an approximation (see Bishop,<br />

1990).<br />

The three-fold <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> rate of non-righth<strong>and</strong>edness<br />

<strong>in</strong> pedophilia relative to that <strong>in</strong> teleiophilia detected<br />

here is consistent with hypo<strong>the</strong>ses of a relationship<br />

between pedophilia <strong>and</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> organization on par with<br />

o<strong>the</strong>r major neurological conditions. Although numerous<br />

psychosocial explanations of child molestation have been<br />

proffered (see Araji & F<strong>in</strong>kelhor, 1985 <strong>for</strong> a review),<br />

none predicts a group difference <strong>in</strong> h<strong>and</strong>edness. Although<br />

<strong>the</strong>re may exist psychosocial factors that contribute to <strong>the</strong><br />

development or <strong>the</strong> expression of erotic age preference,<br />

any complete <strong>the</strong>ory must account <strong>for</strong>, not only <strong>the</strong><br />

differences <strong>in</strong> h<strong>and</strong>edness, but also <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> differences<br />

<strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r neuropsychologically relevant traits, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g<br />

lower mean IQs (e.g., Cantor et al., 2004) <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>creased<br />

frequencies of head <strong>in</strong>juries <strong>in</strong> childhood but not <strong>in</strong> adulthood<br />

(Blanchard et al., 2002, 2003). Research regard<strong>in</strong>g<br />

psychosocial factors <strong>in</strong> pedophilia may advance more<br />

readily by pursu<strong>in</strong>g data—not to reveal how psychosocial<br />

factors might cause pedophilia—but how psychosocial<br />

factors might <strong>in</strong>teract with predispos<strong>in</strong>g biological factors<br />

or how <strong>the</strong> psychosocial differences <strong>the</strong>mselves reflect<br />

pre-exist<strong>in</strong>g biological differences.<br />

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS<br />

This research was supported by Canadian Institute<br />

<strong>for</strong> Health Research Grant 94205 to Ray Blanchard <strong>and</strong><br />

by a postdoctoral fellowship award from <strong>the</strong> Centre <strong>for</strong><br />

Addiction <strong>and</strong> Mental Health Foundation <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> Ontario<br />

M<strong>in</strong>istry of Health to James M. Cantor. We thank S<strong>and</strong>ra<br />

Belfry, Meredith Chivers, Lisa Couperthwaite, <strong>and</strong> Lori<br />

Robichaud, <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir assistance <strong>in</strong> data collection <strong>and</strong><br />

comments on an earlier version of this article.<br />

REFERENCES<br />

Araji, S., & F<strong>in</strong>kelhor, D. (1985). Explanations of pedophilia: Review of<br />

empirical research. Bullet<strong>in</strong> of <strong>the</strong> American Academy of Psychiatry<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> Law, 13, 17–37.<br />

Ashton, G. C. (1982). <strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong>: An alternative hypo<strong>the</strong>sis. Behavior<br />

Genetics, 12, 125–147.<br />

Bakan, P. (1971). <strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong> <strong>and</strong> birth order. Nature, 229, 195.<br />

Bakan, P., Dibb, G., & Reed, P. (1973). <strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong> <strong>and</strong> birth stress.<br />

Neuropsychologia, 11, 363–366.<br />

Ba<strong>the</strong>ja, M., & McManus, I. C. (1985). <strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> mentally<br />

h<strong>and</strong>icapped. Developmental Medic<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong> Child Neurology, 27,<br />

63–68.<br />

Bishop, D. V. M. (1990). <strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong> <strong>and</strong> developmental disorder.<br />

Ox<strong>for</strong>d: Blackwell Scientific.<br />

Blanchard, R., Barbaree, H. E., Bogaert, A. F., Dickey, R., Klassen,<br />

P., Kuban, M. E., et al. (2000). Fraternal birth order <strong>and</strong> sexual<br />

orientation <strong>in</strong> pedophiles. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 29, 463–<br />

478.<br />

Blanchard, R., Cantor, J. M., & Robichaud, L. K. (<strong>in</strong> press). Biological<br />

factors <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> development of sexual deviance <strong>and</strong> aggression <strong>in</strong><br />

males. In H. E. Barbaree & W. L. Marshall (Eds.), The juvenile sex<br />

offender (2nd ed.). New York: Guil<strong>for</strong>d.<br />

Blanchard, R., Christensen, B. K., Strong, S. M., Cantor, J. M., Kuban,<br />

M. E., Klassen, P., et al. (2002). Retrospective self-reports of<br />

childhood accidents caus<strong>in</strong>g unconsciousness <strong>in</strong> phallometrically<br />

diagnosed pedophiles. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 31, 511–516.<br />

Blanchard, R., Klassen, P., Dickey, R., Kuban, M. E., & Blak, T. (2001).<br />

Sensitivity <strong>and</strong> specificity of <strong>the</strong> phallometric test <strong>for</strong> pedophilia <strong>in</strong><br />

nonadmitt<strong>in</strong>g sex offenders. Psychological Assessment, 13, 118–<br />

126.<br />

Blanchard, R., Kuban, M. E., Klassen, P., Dickey, R., Christensen, B.<br />

K., Cantor, J. M., et al. (2003). Self-reported head <strong>in</strong>juries be<strong>for</strong>e<br />

<strong>and</strong> after age 13 <strong>in</strong> pedophilic <strong>and</strong> nonpedophilic men referred <strong>for</strong><br />

cl<strong>in</strong>ical assessment. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32, 573–581.<br />

Bogaert, A. F. (2001). <strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong>, crim<strong>in</strong>ality, <strong>and</strong> sexual offend<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Neuropsychologia, 39, 465–469.


<strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Pedophilia</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Hebephilia</strong> 459<br />

Bradshaw-McAnulty, G., Hicks, R. E., & K<strong>in</strong>sbourne, M. (1984).<br />

Pathological left-h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>and</strong> familial s<strong>in</strong>istrality <strong>in</strong> relation<br />

to degree of mental retardation. Bra<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> Cognition, 3, 349–356.<br />

Cantor, J. M., Blanchard, R., Christensen, B. K., Dickey, R., Klassen,<br />

P. E., Beckstead, A. L., et al. (2004). Intelligence, memory, <strong>and</strong><br />

h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>in</strong> pedophilia. Neuropsychology, 18, 3–14.<br />

Casanova, M. F., Mannheim, G., & Kruesi, M. (2002). Hippocampal<br />

pathology <strong>in</strong> two mentally ill paraphiliacs. Psychiatry Research<br />

Neuroimag<strong>in</strong>g, 115, 79–89.<br />

Coren, S. (1994). Tw<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g is associated with an <strong>in</strong>creased risk of<br />

left-h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>verted writ<strong>in</strong>g h<strong>and</strong> posture. Early Human<br />

Development, 40, 23–27.<br />

Coren, S., & Halpern, D. F. (1991). Left-h<strong>and</strong>edness: A marker <strong>for</strong><br />

decreased survival fitness. Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 109, 90–106.<br />

Cornish, K. M., & McManus, I. C. (1996). H<strong>and</strong> preference <strong>and</strong> h<strong>and</strong><br />

skill <strong>in</strong> children with autism. Journal of Autism <strong>and</strong> Developmental<br />

Disorders, 26, 597–609.<br />

Davis, A., & Annett, M. (1994). <strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong> as a function of tw<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

age, <strong>and</strong> sex. Cortex, 30, 105–111.<br />

Freund, K. (1967). Erotic preference <strong>in</strong> pedophilia. Behaviour Research<br />

<strong>and</strong> Therapy, 5, 339–348.<br />

Freund, K., Langev<strong>in</strong>, R., Cibiri, S., & Zajac, Y. (1973). Heterosexual<br />

aversion <strong>in</strong> homosexual males. British Journal of Psychiatry, 122,<br />

163–169.<br />

Freund, K., Langev<strong>in</strong>, R., Wescom, T., & Zajac, Y. (1975). Heterosexual<br />

<strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> homosexual males. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 4, 509–<br />

518.<br />

Freund, K., Watson, R., & Dickey, R. (1991). Sex offenses aga<strong>in</strong>st female<br />

children perpetrated by men who are not pedophiles. The Journal<br />

of Sex Research, 28, 409–423.<br />

Glueck, B. C. (1955). F<strong>in</strong>al report: Research project <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> study<br />

<strong>and</strong> treatment of persons convicted of crimes <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g sexual<br />

aberrations. June 1952 to June 1955. New York: New York State<br />

Department of Mental Hygiene.<br />

Grouios, G., Sakadami, N., Poderi, A., & Alevriadou, A. (1999). Excess<br />

of non-right h<strong>and</strong>edness among <strong>in</strong>dividuals with <strong>in</strong>tellectual disability:<br />

Experimental evidence <strong>and</strong> possible explanations. Journal<br />

of Intellectual Disability Research, 43, 306–313.<br />

Hanson, R. K., & Bussière, M. T. (1998). Predict<strong>in</strong>g relapse: A<br />

meta-analysis of sexual offender recidivism studies. Journal of<br />

Consult<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Psychology, 66, 348–362.<br />

Hardyck, C., & Petr<strong>in</strong>ovich, L. F. (1977). Left-h<strong>and</strong>edness. Psychological<br />

Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 84, 385–404.<br />

Howes, R. J. (1995). A survey of plethysmographic assessment <strong>in</strong> North<br />

America. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research <strong>and</strong> Treatment, 7,<br />

9–24.<br />

Lalumière, M. L., Blanchard, R., & Zucker, K. J. (2000). Sexual<br />

orientation <strong>and</strong> h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>in</strong> men <strong>and</strong> women: A meta-analysis.<br />

Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 126, 575–592.<br />

Marlow, N., Roberts, B. L., & Cooke, R. W. I. (1989). Laterality <strong>and</strong><br />

premature birth. Archives of Diseases <strong>in</strong> Childhood, 64, 1713–<br />

1716.<br />

Mendez, M. F., Chow, T., R<strong>in</strong>gman, J., Twitchell, G., & H<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>, C. H.<br />

(2000). <strong>Pedophilia</strong> <strong>and</strong> temporal lobe disturbances. Journal of<br />

Neuropsychiatry <strong>and</strong> Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Neuroscience, 12, 71–76.<br />

O’Callaghan, M. J., Tudehope, D. I., Dugdale, A. E., Mohay,<br />

H., Burns, Y., & Cook, F. (1987, May 16). <strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong> <strong>in</strong><br />

children with birthweights below 1000 g. Lancet, 329(8542),<br />

1155.<br />

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment <strong>and</strong> analysis of h<strong>and</strong>edness: The<br />

Ed<strong>in</strong>burgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.<br />

Porac, C., Coren, S., & Duncan, P. (1980). Life-span age trends <strong>in</strong><br />

laterality. Journal of Gerontology, 35, 715–721.<br />

Powls, A., Bott<strong>in</strong>g, N., Cooke, R. W. I., & Marlow, N. (1996).<br />

<strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong> <strong>in</strong> very-low-birthweight (VLBW) children at 12 years<br />

of age: Relation to per<strong>in</strong>atal <strong>and</strong> outcome variables. Developmental<br />

Medic<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong> Child Neurology, 38, 594–602.<br />

Qu<strong>in</strong>sey, V. L., Arnold, L. S., & Pruesse, M. G. (1980). MMPI profiles<br />

of men referred <strong>for</strong> a pretrial psychiatric assessment as a function<br />

of offense type. Journal of Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Psychology, 36, 410–416.<br />

Ross, G., Lipper, E., & Auld, P. A. M. (1992). H<strong>and</strong> preference, prematurity<br />

<strong>and</strong> developmental outcome at school age. Neuropsychologia,<br />

30, 483–494.<br />

Satz, P. (1973). Left-h<strong>and</strong>edness <strong>and</strong> early bra<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>sult: An explanation.<br />

Neuropsychologia, 11, 115–117.<br />

Schacter, S. C., Boulton, A., Manoach, D., O’Connor, M., We<strong>in</strong>traub,<br />

S., Blume, H., et al. (1995). <strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong> <strong>in</strong> patients with <strong>in</strong>tractable<br />

epilepsy: Correlations with side of temporal lobectomy <strong>and</strong> gender.<br />

Journal of Epilepsy, 8, 190–192.<br />

Searleman, A., Cunn<strong>in</strong>gham, T. F., & Goodw<strong>in</strong>, W. (1988). Association<br />

between familial s<strong>in</strong>istrality <strong>and</strong> pathological left-h<strong>and</strong>edness:<br />

A comparison of mentally retarded <strong>and</strong> nonretarded subjects.<br />

Journal of Cl<strong>in</strong>ical <strong>and</strong> Experimental Neuropsychology, 10, 132–<br />

138.<br />

Soper, H. V., Satz, P., Ors<strong>in</strong>i, D. L., Henry, R. R., Zvi, J. C., &<br />

Schulman, M. (1986). <strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong> patterns <strong>in</strong> autism suggest<br />

subtypes. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 16, 155–<br />

167.<br />

Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1989). Us<strong>in</strong>g multivariate statistics<br />

(2nd ed.). New York: HarperColl<strong>in</strong>s.<br />

Tellegen, A., & Briggs, P. F. (1967). Old w<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong> new sk<strong>in</strong>s: Group<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Wechsler subtests <strong>in</strong>to new scales. Journal of Consult<strong>in</strong>g Psychology,<br />

31, 499–506.<br />

von Krafft-Eb<strong>in</strong>g, R. (1965). Psychopathia sexualis: A medico-<strong>for</strong>ensic<br />

study (H. E. Wedeck, Trans.). New York: Putnam. (Orig<strong>in</strong>al work<br />

published 1886)<br />

Williams, C. S., Buss, K. A., & Eskenazi, B. (1992). Infant resuscitation<br />

is associated with an <strong>in</strong>creased risk of left-h<strong>and</strong>edness. American<br />

Journal of Epidemiology, 136, 277–286.<br />

Williams, S. M. (1986). Factor analysis of <strong>the</strong> Ed<strong>in</strong>burgh <strong>H<strong>and</strong>edness</strong><br />

Inventory. Cortex, 22, 325–326.<br />

Wormith, J. S. (1986). Assess<strong>in</strong>g deviant sexual arousal: Physiological<br />

<strong>and</strong> cognitive aspects. Advances <strong>in</strong> Behavior Research <strong>and</strong> Therapy,<br />

8, 101–107.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!