06.09.2021 Views

Mind, Body, World- Foundations of Cognitive Science, 2013a

Mind, Body, World- Foundations of Cognitive Science, 2013a

Mind, Body, World- Foundations of Cognitive Science, 2013a

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

In Lovecraft’s (1933) story, the witch Keziah “might have had excellent reasons<br />

for living in a room with peculiar angles; for was it not through certain angles that<br />

she claimed to have gone outside the boundaries <strong>of</strong> the world <strong>of</strong> space we know?”<br />

Shannon’s (1938) scholarly paper led to astonishing conclusions for similar reasons:<br />

it detailed equivalence between the formal and the physical. It proved that electric<br />

circuits could be described in two very different vocabularies: one the physical<br />

vocabulary <strong>of</strong> current, contacts, switches and wires; the other the abstract vocabulary<br />

<strong>of</strong> logical symbols and operations.<br />

2.4 Multiple Procedures and Architectures<br />

According to a Chinese proverb, we all like lamb, but each has a different way to cook<br />

it. This proverb can be aptly applied to the circuits <strong>of</strong> switches for which Shannon<br />

(1938) developed a logical interpretation. Any <strong>of</strong> these circuits can be described as<br />

defining a logical function that maps inputs onto an output: the circuit outputs a<br />

current (or not) depending on the pattern <strong>of</strong> currents controlled by one or more<br />

switches that flow into it. However, just like lamb, there are many different ways to<br />

“cook” the input signals to produce the desired output. In short, many different circuits<br />

can be constructed to compute the same input-output function.<br />

To illustrate this point, let us begin by considering Shannon’s (1938) selective<br />

circuit, which would be <strong>of</strong>f when 0 or 2 <strong>of</strong> its 4 relays were closed, but which would<br />

be on when any other number <strong>of</strong> its relays was closed. In Shannon’s original formulation,<br />

20 components—an arrangement <strong>of</strong> 20 different switches—defined a circuit<br />

that would behave in the desired fashion. After applying logical operations to simplify<br />

the design, Shannon reduced the number <strong>of</strong> required components from 20<br />

to 14. That is, a smaller circuit that involved an arrangement <strong>of</strong> only 14 different<br />

switches delivered the same input-output behaviour as did the 20-switch circuit.<br />

Reflecting on these two different versions <strong>of</strong> the selective circuit, it’s clear that<br />

if one is interested in comparing them, the result <strong>of</strong> the comparison depends on the<br />

perspective taken. On the one hand, they are quite different: they involve different<br />

numbers <strong>of</strong> components, related to one another by completely different patterns <strong>of</strong><br />

wiring. On the other hand, in spite <strong>of</strong> these obvious differences in details, at a more<br />

abstract level the two designs are identical, in the sense that both designs produce<br />

the same input-output mapping. That is, if one built a truth table for either circuit<br />

that listed the circuit’s conductivity (output) as a function <strong>of</strong> all possible combinations<br />

<strong>of</strong> its 4 relays (inputs), the two truth tables would be identical. One might<br />

say that the two circuits use markedly different procedures (i.e., arrangements <strong>of</strong><br />

internal components) to compute the same input-output function. They generate<br />

the same behaviour, but for different reasons.<br />

32 Chapter 2

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!