06.09.2021 Views

Mind, Body, World- Foundations of Cognitive Science, 2013a

Mind, Body, World- Foundations of Cognitive Science, 2013a

Mind, Body, World- Foundations of Cognitive Science, 2013a

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

evaluation, interpretation, and description” (Leman, 2008, p. 4). This suggests that<br />

cross-modal interactions may be critical determinants <strong>of</strong> musical experience.<br />

Some research on musical cognition is beginning to explore this possibility. In<br />

one study (Vines et al., 2006) subjects were presented with performances by two<br />

clarinetists. Some subjects only heard, some subjects only saw, and some subjects<br />

both heard and saw the performances. Compared to the first two groups <strong>of</strong> subjects,<br />

those who both heard and saw the performances had very different experiences.<br />

The visual information altered the experience <strong>of</strong> tension at different points, and<br />

the movements <strong>of</strong> the performers provided additional information that affected the<br />

experienced phrasing as well as expectations about emotional content. “The auditory<br />

and visual channels mutually enhance one another to convey content, and . . .<br />

an emergent quality exists when a musician is both seen and heard” (p. 108).<br />

In a more recent study, Vines et al. (2011) used a similar methodology, but they<br />

also manipulated the expressive style with which the stimulus (a solo clarinet piece<br />

composed by Stravinsky) was performed. Subjects were presented with the piece<br />

in restrained, standard, or exaggerated fashion. These manipulations <strong>of</strong> expressive<br />

style only affected the subjects who could see the performance. Again, interactions<br />

were evident when performances were both seen and heard. For instance, subjects<br />

in this condition had significantly higher ratings <strong>of</strong> “happiness” in comparison to<br />

other subjects.<br />

The visual component <strong>of</strong> musical performance makes a unique contribution to the<br />

communication <strong>of</strong> emotion from performer to audience. Seeing a musician can<br />

augment, complement, and interact with the sound to modify the overall experience<br />

<strong>of</strong> music. (Vines et al., 2011, p. 168)<br />

Of course, the embodied approach to music makes much stronger claims than<br />

that there are interactions between hearing and seeing; it views cognition not as a<br />

medium for planning, but instead as a medium for acting. It is not surprising, then,<br />

to discover that embodied musical cognition has studied the relationships between<br />

music and actions, gestures, and motion in a variety <strong>of</strong> ways (Gritten & King, 2011).<br />

One <strong>of</strong> the most prominent <strong>of</strong> these relationships involves the exploration<br />

<strong>of</strong> new kinds <strong>of</strong> musical instruments, called digital musical instruments. A digital<br />

musical instrument is a musical instrument that involves a computer and in<br />

which the generation <strong>of</strong> sound is separate from the control interface that chooses<br />

sound (Marshall et al. 2009). This distinction is important, because as Marshall et<br />

al. (2009) pointed out, there are many available sensors that can register a human<br />

agent’s movements, actions, or gestures. These include force sensitive resistors,<br />

video cameras, accelerometers, potentiometers, and bend sensors, not to mention<br />

buttons and microphones.<br />

The availability <strong>of</strong> digital sensors permits movements, actions, and gestures<br />

to be measured and used to control the sounds generated by a digital musical<br />

304 Chapter 6

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!