06.09.2021 Views

Mind, Body, World- Foundations of Cognitive Science, 2013a

Mind, Body, World- Foundations of Cognitive Science, 2013a

Mind, Body, World- Foundations of Cognitive Science, 2013a

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

In contrast, expert workers were much more likely to scaffold this problem solving<br />

by working directly from the visual appearance <strong>of</strong> cases, as illustrated in a very different<br />

protocol:<br />

I walked over and I visualized. I knew the case I was looking at had ten out <strong>of</strong> it, and I<br />

only wanted eight, so I just added two to it. I don’t never count when I’m making the<br />

order, I do it visual, a visual thing you know. (Scribner & Tobach, 1997, p. 303)<br />

It was also found that expert workers flexibly alternated the distribution <strong>of</strong> scaffolded<br />

and mental arithmetic, but did so in a systematic way: when more mental<br />

arithmetic was employed, it was done to decrease the amount <strong>of</strong> physical exertion<br />

required to complete the order. This led to Scribner postulating a law <strong>of</strong> mental<br />

effort: “In product assembly, mental work will be expended to save physical work”<br />

(Scribner & Tobach, 1997, p. 348).<br />

The law <strong>of</strong> mental effort was the result <strong>of</strong> Scribner’s observation that expert<br />

workers in the dairy demonstrated marked diversity and flexibility in their solutions<br />

to work-related problems. Intelligent agents may be flexible in the manner in<br />

which they allocate resources between sense-act and sense-think-act processing.<br />

Both types <strong>of</strong> processes may be in play simultaneously, but they may be applied in<br />

different amounts when the same problem is encountered at different times and<br />

under different task demands (Hutchins, 1995).<br />

Such flexible information processing is an example <strong>of</strong> bricolage (Lévi-Strauss,<br />

1966). A bricoleur is an “odd job man” in France.<br />

The ‘bricoleur’ is adept at performing a large number <strong>of</strong> diverse tasks; but, unlike<br />

the engineer, he does not subordinate each <strong>of</strong> them to the availability <strong>of</strong> raw materials<br />

and tools conceived and procured for the purpose <strong>of</strong> the project. His universe <strong>of</strong><br />

instruments is closed and the rules <strong>of</strong> his game are always to make do with ‘whatever<br />

is at hand.’ (Lévi-Strauss, 1966, p. 17)<br />

Bricolage seems well suited to account for the flexible thinking <strong>of</strong> the sort described<br />

by Scribner. Lévi-Strauss (1966) proposed bricolage as an alternative to formal, theoretical<br />

thinking, but cast it in a negative light: “The ‘bricoleur’ is still someone who<br />

works with his hands and uses devious means compared to those <strong>of</strong> a craftsman”<br />

(pp. 16–17). Devious means are required because the bricoleur is limited to using<br />

only those components or tools that are at hand. “The engineer is always trying to<br />

make his way out <strong>of</strong> and go beyond the constraints imposed by a particular state <strong>of</strong><br />

civilization while the ‘bricoleur’ by inclination or necessity always remains within<br />

them” (p. 19).<br />

Recently, researchers have renewed interest in bricolage and presented it in<br />

a more positive light than did Lévi-Strauss (Papert, 1980; Turkle, 1995). To Turkle<br />

(1995), bricolage was a sort <strong>of</strong> intuition, a mental tinkering, a dialogue mediated by<br />

228 Chapter 5

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!