06.09.2021 Views

Torts - Cases, Principles, and Institutions Fifth Edition, 2016a

Torts - Cases, Principles, and Institutions Fifth Edition, 2016a

Torts - Cases, Principles, and Institutions Fifth Edition, 2016a

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Witt & Tani, TCPI 1. Introduction<br />

973 F. Supp. at 548. The Restatement authors agree with Judge Black. The substantial certainty<br />

doctrine, the Restatement asserts, should be limited to cases in which “the defendant has<br />

knowledge to a substantial certainty that the conduct will bring about harm to a particular victim<br />

or to someone within a small class of potential victims within a localized area.” RESTATEMENT<br />

(THIRD) OF TORTS: PHYS. & EMOT. HARM § 1 cmt. e (2010) (emphasis added). Why limit the tort<br />

of battery in this way?<br />

3. Is intentionality intuitive? The famed Supreme Court justice Oliver Wendell Holmes, Jr.,<br />

once claimed that “even a dog knows the difference between being kicked <strong>and</strong> being stumbled<br />

over.” OLIVER WENDELL HOLMES, JR., THE COMMON LAW 3 (1881). Yet research from<br />

developmental psychology paints a more complicated picture.<br />

It is true that the ability to distinguish intentional from unintentional action is a<br />

foundational skill that emerges early in development. Chimpanzees, orangutans, <strong>and</strong> eightmonth-old<br />

human infants can reliably discriminate between purposeful <strong>and</strong> accidental action.<br />

Josep Call & Michael Tomasello, Distinguishing Intentional from Accidental Actions in<br />

Orangutans (Pongo Pygmaeus) <strong>and</strong> Chimpanzees (Pan Troglodytes) <strong>and</strong> Human Children, 122 J.<br />

COMP. PSYCHOL. 192 (1998). This faculty plays an essential role in helping children acquire<br />

language <strong>and</strong> develop interpersonal skills. In fact, preschoolers whose ability to infer others’<br />

intentions is disrupted, often due to maltreatment or physical abuse, are at heightened risk for later<br />

social maladjustment <strong>and</strong> behavioral problems. See Kenneth A. Dodge, John E. Bates & Gregory<br />

S. Petit, Mechanisms in the Cycle of Violence, 250 SCIENCE 1678 (1990). Indeed, some have<br />

argued that our ability to identify unintentional harms as accidents <strong>and</strong> to respond with<br />

forgiveness is what allows for the evolution of cooperative societies in which individuals<br />

coordinate their behavior to maximize collective welfare. E.g., Drew Fudenberg, David G. R<strong>and</strong><br />

& Anna Dreber, Slow to Anger <strong>and</strong> Fast to Forgive: Cooperation in an Uncertain World, 102<br />

AM. ECON. REV. 720 (2012).<br />

Yet while the distinction between intentional <strong>and</strong> accidental behavior is intuitive for apes<br />

<strong>and</strong> babies alike, we do not always use this distinction when forming moral judgments. For<br />

instance, the celebrated developmental psychologist Jean Piaget observed in 1932 that young<br />

children consider it morally worse (“naughtier”) for a child to make a large ink stain accidentally<br />

than to make a small stain intentionally. Older children, he noticed, felt just the opposite. Since<br />

Piaget’s discovery, this developmental trajectory has been extensively documented. Preschoolers<br />

tend to judge the moral valence of an act by focusing on its outcome <strong>and</strong> largely ignoring the<br />

agent’s beliefs <strong>and</strong> intentions. By around age 6, children begin to condemn actions on the basis of<br />

the agent’s mental states such as intent to harm. In other words, children shift from outcomebased<br />

moral judgment to intent-based moral judgment as they age. Fiery Cushman, Rachel<br />

Sheketoff, Sophie Wharton & Susan Carey, The Development of Intent-Based Moral Judgment,<br />

127 COGNITION 6 (2013).<br />

Under some circumstances, adults will also exhibit a tendency to focus on outcomes rather<br />

than intentions when judging actions that result in harm. Research from cognitive psychology<br />

demonstrates that adults who are placed under time pressure <strong>and</strong> asked to make hurried moral<br />

judgments show a systematic bias toward judging actions as intentional. In their haste, these<br />

decision-makers will say that harms were intentional. Given more time to make their decisions,<br />

they revert to saying that harms were unintentional. Evelyn Rosset, It’s No Accident: Our Bias<br />

23

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!