17.12.2012 Views

Program & Abstract Book - EPFL Latsis Symposium 2009

Program & Abstract Book - EPFL Latsis Symposium 2009

Program & Abstract Book - EPFL Latsis Symposium 2009

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>EPFL</strong>-LATSIS <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong><br />

Lausanne, Switzerland<br />

understanding<br />

February 11 – 13 th <strong>2009</strong><br />

VIOLENCE<br />

Recent advances in biology, sociology and modeling<br />

<strong>Program</strong> &<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> <strong>Book</strong>


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

Scientific Committee<br />

Organizer:<br />

Carmen Sandi<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong>, Switzerland<br />

Co-organizers:<br />

Andreas Meyer-Lindenberg<br />

Mel Slater<br />

Pierre Magistretti<br />

FONDATION LATSIS<br />

Internationale<br />

Sponsors<br />

Local Organizers<br />

Jorge Castro<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong>, Switzerland<br />

Ioana Steinmann<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong>, Switzerland<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

3


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

Practical Information<br />

Meeting Logistics and Organization<br />

Prof. Carmen Sandi<br />

E-mail: info-latsis<strong>2009</strong>@epfl.ch<br />

Media Relations<br />

Dr. Eng Ariane Vlérick<br />

Email: ariane.vlerick@epfl.ch<br />

Webmaster<br />

M. Olivier Burri<br />

E-mail: webmaster.burri@gmail.com<br />

4<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

Emergency telephone during the<br />

Conference<br />

+41 (0)21 693 82 02


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

Credits<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

Recall that participation to this meeting allows<br />

you to obtain Continuous Education Credits<br />

from the following institutions:<br />

Swiss Society for Pediatry<br />

SSP-SGP<br />

Swiss Neurological Society<br />

Swiss Society for Neuropediatry<br />

SGN<br />

Genetic Medicine<br />

Swiss Cantonal Veterinary<br />

Association (ASCV)<br />

10 Credits<br />

3 Credits<br />

2 Credits<br />

2 Credits (Continuous Education)<br />

1 Day Credit Recognition<br />

Remember to request a certificate<br />

of your participation as you leave the<br />

conference and return your badge<br />

5


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

6<br />

8:00-8:45 Registration<br />

Understanding VIOLENCE:<br />

Recent advances in biology, sociology, and modeling<br />

WEDNESDAY, FEBRUARY 11th, <strong>2009</strong><br />

8:45-9:00 Opening Remarks<br />

Prof Dusan Sidjanski (<strong>Latsis</strong> Foundation)<br />

Prof Giorgio Margaritondo (Vice-president <strong>EPFL</strong>)<br />

Session 1 Are individuals born violent? Can we talk of ‘genetic’ determinism?<br />

Chair Carmen Sandi<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

9:00-9:45 Richard E Tremblay<br />

Developmental trajectories of human physical violence and their determinants: From genes to<br />

social policy (or vice versa)<br />

9:45-10:20 Klaus Peter Lesch<br />

The neurobiology of impulsivity and aggression in ADHD<br />

10:20-10:50 Coffee break<br />

10:50-11:25 Stephen J. Suomi<br />

Aggression, serotonin, and gene X environment interactions in primates<br />

11:25-12:00 Caroline Blanchard<br />

The evolution and adaptive functions of anger, aggression and violence<br />

12:00-12:35 Discussion - Didier Trono<br />

LUNCH<br />

Session 2 Are hormones key to understanding aggression?<br />

Chair John Archer<br />

14:00-14:45 Richard Ebstein<br />

Nonapeptide social hormones, oxytocin and arginine vasopressin, the flip side of violence?<br />

14:45-15:20 Andreas Meyer-Lindenberg<br />

Neurogenetic and neurohormonal mechanisms of prosocial and aggressive behavior<br />

15:20-15:55 Jozsef Haller<br />

Types of aggression and types of brain mechanisms. The role of glucocorticoids<br />

15:55-16:30 Discussion - Ron de Kloet<br />

16:30-18:30 Poster Session with Coffee and refreshments


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

8:30-9:00 Registration<br />

THURSDAY, FEBRUARY 12th, <strong>2009</strong><br />

Session 3 What are the social, contextual and evolutionary causes of violence?<br />

Chair Pierre Magistretti<br />

9:00-9:45 Richard Wilkinson<br />

The violence of inequality<br />

9:45-10:20 Stefan Klusemann<br />

Micro-situational antecedents of violent atrocity<br />

10:20-10:55 Mark Broom<br />

Balancing risks and rewards: the logic of violence<br />

10:55-11:25 Coffee break<br />

11:25-12:10 John Archer<br />

Sexual selection as an explanation of human sex differences in aggression<br />

12:10-12:45 Discussion - Alexander Butchart<br />

LUNCH<br />

Session 4 What works differently in the brain of violent individuals? What makes<br />

psychopaths different? Can we reverse, treat aggression?<br />

Chair Andreas Meyer-Lindenberg<br />

<strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>Program</strong><br />

14:00-14:45 Kent Kiehl<br />

Psychopathy, violence and brain<br />

14:45-15:20 Sheilagh Hodgings<br />

Getting the phenotypes right: the essential ingredient to understanding aetiological mechanisms<br />

and developing effective treatments<br />

15:20-15:55 Joshua W. Buckholtz<br />

Multimodal characterization of genetic risk mechanisms for impulsive aggression<br />

15:55-16:30 Discussion - Olaf Blanke<br />

16:30-17:00 Coffee break<br />

7


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

8<br />

17:00-18:30 Round Table: “TACKLING VIOLENCE”<br />

Panelists Hans-Peter Schmoll-Flockerzie<br />

AHBasel Krisentintervention für Jugendliche, Basel<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

Stephen Thomas<br />

Security Officer for UEFA and Assistant Chief Constable British Transport Police<br />

Bruno Gravier<br />

Head of the Prison Medicine and Psychiatry Service, CHUV, Lausanne<br />

Gregoire Rubovszky<br />

Head of Emergency Psychiatry Service, HCUGE, Geneva<br />

Christopher Mikton<br />

Technical Officer, Prevention of Violence Unit, WHO<br />

Jakob Lund<br />

Director of the SMART programs in Scandinavia, Art-of-Living<br />

Marie-Claude Hofner<br />

Violence Medical Unit - University Center of Legal Medicine Western Switzerland<br />

19:00 Banquet – Departure Bus


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

8:30-9:00 Registration<br />

FRIDAY, FEBRUARY 13th, <strong>2009</strong><br />

<strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>Program</strong><br />

Session 5 Are individuals ‘made’ violent? Does stress shape individual’s aggression?<br />

Chair Stephen J. Suomi<br />

9:00-9:45 Francois Ansermet and Pierre Magistretti<br />

Neuronal plasticity, homeostasis and violence<br />

9:45-10:20 Inga D. Neumann<br />

Abnormal social behaviour and aggression: Link to anxiety<br />

10:20-10:55 Jaap M. Koolhaas<br />

From aggression to violence in rats and mice: role of individual behavioral and neurobiological<br />

characteristics<br />

10:55-11:25 Coffee break<br />

11:25-12:00 Carmen Sandi<br />

Stress as a generator of social imbalance<br />

12:00-12:35 Discussion - Daniel Schechter<br />

LUNCH<br />

Session 6 Do people know how they would behave in an unexpected violent situation?<br />

Chair Mel Slater<br />

How can virtual reality and modelling approaches help for treatment?<br />

14:00-14:35 Mark Levine<br />

Groups, bystanders and the informal regulation of violence<br />

14:35-15:10 Mel Slater<br />

Virtual reality in the study of extreme situations<br />

15:10-15:45 Lee Alan Dugatkin<br />

Winners, losers and bystanders: the structure of dominance hierarchies<br />

15:45-16:15 Coffee break<br />

16:15-17:00 Albert Rizzo<br />

Reexposure to combat in virtual reality as a treatment for PTSD<br />

17:00-17:35 Discussion - Philippe Cotter<br />

17:35-17:45 Concluding Remarks<br />

17:45 Farewell<br />

9


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

Speakers<br />

Richard E. Tremblay<br />

10<br />

Professor, Research Unit on Children’s Psychosocial<br />

Maladjustment University of Montreal, Canada<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

http://www.psy.umontreal.ca/employes/TREMBLAYRichardE.html<br />

Richard Tremblay started his clinical career treating dangerous<br />

mentally offenders. For the past 25 years he has conducted<br />

a program of longitudinal and experimental studies to understand<br />

the physical, cognitive, emotional characteristics of humans<br />

from conception onward, in order to gain a better understanding<br />

of the development and prevention of physically<br />

violent behaviors.<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: De v e l o p m e n t a l t r a j e c t o r i e s o f h u m a n p h y s i c a l v i o l e n c e a n D t h e i r<br />

D e t e r m i n a n t s : fr o m g e n e s t o s o c i a l p o l i c y (o r v i c e v e r s a)<br />

Klaus-Peter Lesch<br />

Professor of Psychiatry and Vice Chair of the Department of<br />

Psychiatry, Psychsomatics, and Psychotherapy and Director of<br />

the ADHD <strong>Program</strong> of the University of Würzburg, Germany<br />

http://www.psychobiologie.uni-wuerzburg.de/<br />

Klaus-Peter Lesch’s work has led to new insights into the psychobiology<br />

of personality, behavior, and psychiatric disorders.<br />

He has been pioneer in discovering genes linked to aggression<br />

in different animal species. He is strongly committed to bridge<br />

the sizeable gap between basic molecular and clinically applicable<br />

research. His work attempts to elucidate neural mechanisms<br />

involved in the pathophysiology of psychiatric disorders<br />

with the goal to identify final common pathways which could<br />

be targeted by novel treatments.<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: th e n e u r o b i o l o g y o f impulsivity a n D a g g r e s s i o n in aDhD<br />

Stephen J. Suomi<br />

Chief of the Laboratory of Comparative Ethology, the National<br />

Institute of Child Health & Human Development (NICHD),<br />

National Institutes of Health (NIH), Bethesda, Maryland<br />

http://www.nichd.nih.gov/about/staff/bio.cfm?nih_id=0010152932<br />

Prof. Suomi has received international recognition for his extensive<br />

research on biobehavioral development in rhesus monkeys<br />

and other primate species, with a particular focus on aggressive<br />

behaviors. His research at Wisconsin led to his election<br />

as Fellow in the American Association for the Advancement of<br />

Science “for major contributions to the understanding of social<br />

factors that influence the psychological development of nonhuman<br />

primates.”<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: ag g r e s s i o n, s e r o t o n i n , a n D g e n e X e n v i r o n m e n t i n t e r a c t i o n s


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

Speakers, Discussants and Round Table Panelists<br />

Caroline Blanchard<br />

Professor at the Pacific Biosciences Research Center, and Dept.<br />

of Genetics and Molecular Biology, John A. Burns School of<br />

Medicine, University of Hawaii<br />

http://www2.hawaii.edu/~blanchar/home.html<br />

Caroline Blanchard’s work has focused on the functions and<br />

biology of “negative” emotion patterns such as aggression<br />

and defense, across mammalian species and, more recently,<br />

on more “positive” emotions, examining behaviors comprising<br />

sociality and affiliative emotions. Her approach characterizes<br />

by using research based on lower order mammals to analyze<br />

both the expression of these emotions in people, and the brain<br />

systems involved in potentially related human emotional psychopathologies.<br />

She recently (2004-2006) served as President<br />

of the International Society for Research on Aggression.<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: th e ev o l u t i o n a n D aD a p t i v e fu n c t i o n s o f an g e r , ag r e s s i o n a n D<br />

vi o l e n c e<br />

Richard J Ebstein<br />

Director, Department of Research – Herzog Hospital Director,<br />

Scheinfeld Center for Human Behavioral Genetics Hebrew<br />

University Professor<br />

http://ebsteinlab.huji.ac.il/index.html<br />

Richard Ebstein has made major contributions to human behavioral<br />

genetics, especially in the emerging field of personality<br />

genetics. His catalyzing role in personality genetics was<br />

initiated with a seminal report in Nature Genetics in 1996 that<br />

first showed an association between a common polymorphism<br />

and a specific temperament (Ebstein et al 1996). He has published<br />

more than 200 scientific articles.<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: a r o l e f o r a r g i n i n e v a s o p r e s s i n a n D o X y t o c i n in s h a p i n g h u m a n s o c i a l<br />

b e h a v i o r<br />

Andreas Meyer-Lindenberg<br />

Professor, Chair of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University<br />

of Heidelberg and Director, Central Institute of Mental Health,<br />

Mannheim, Germany<br />

http://snp.nimh.nih.gov/<br />

Prof. Meyer-Lindenberg is an expert in the fields of neural<br />

mechanisms of psychopathology, imaging genetics, social neuroscience<br />

and systems neuro-science in humans. Using mathematical<br />

tools, he investigates complex inter-actions between<br />

genetic variants and their influence on the human brain. He<br />

combines studies of genetic indicators of mental illness with<br />

neuroimaging.<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: ne u r o g e n e t i c a n D n e u r o h o r m o n a l m e c h a n i s m s o f p r o s o c i a l a n D a g g r e s s i v e<br />

b e h a v i o r<br />

11


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

12<br />

Jozsef Haller<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

Professor, Head of the Behavioural Neurobiology Department<br />

Institute of Experimental Medicine Budapest, Hungary<br />

http://www.koki.hu<br />

Jozsef Haller’s research interests focus on the understanding<br />

of brain mechanisms involved in violent forms of aggression.<br />

By comparing the brain mechanisms of violent aggression induced<br />

by different means (e.g. genetic selection, social deprivation,<br />

glucocorticoid deficiency), he has made major contributions<br />

to the field. Working in rodents, he has identified main<br />

components of the brain circuitry underlying violent forms of<br />

aggression.<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: ty p e s o f a g g r e s s i o n a n D t y p e s o f b r a i n m e c h a n i s m s . th e r o l e o f<br />

g l u c o c o r t i c o i D s<br />

Richard Wilkinson<br />

Professor of Social Epidemiology, University of Nottingham<br />

Medical School, UK<br />

Visiting Professor, International Centre for Health and Society,<br />

University College London<br />

http://www.nottingham.ac.uk/cps/index.php?page=2.0.0.40<br />

Richard Wilkinson’s research career focusses on health inequalities<br />

and the social determinants of health. Working in<br />

the field for 30 years, Richard has played a formative role in research<br />

and public awareness of health inequalities and the social<br />

determinants of health. Since persuading the UK Secretary<br />

of State to set up the working party which in 1980 produced<br />

the Black Report on Health Inequalities, he has worked particularly<br />

on the health and social effects of income inequality.<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: th e vi o l e n c e o f in e q u a l i t y<br />

Stefan Klusemann<br />

Scientist at Sociology MA, University of Pennsylvania, USA<br />

Stefan Klusemann works at the University of Pennsylvania,<br />

USA, on violence, armed groups, historical sociology of political<br />

and cultural change, and sociological theory, Stefan Klusemann<br />

work studies rituals, violence and the organization of force in<br />

situations of state breakdown. He received an M.A. in Sociology<br />

from the University of Pennsylvania and a Diploma in Sociology,<br />

Economics, and Law from the Free University of Berlin.<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: mi c r o-s i t u a t i o n a l a n t e c e D e n t s o f v i o l e n t a t r o c i t y


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

Speakers, Discussants and Round Table Panelists<br />

Mark Broom<br />

Professor, Head Department of Mathematics University of<br />

Sussex, UK<br />

http://www.sussex.ac.uk/maths/profile27849.html<br />

Mark Broom’s research focuses on the application of game theory<br />

to the mathematical modelling of biology, especially using<br />

the concept of the Evolutionarily Stable Strategy (ESS). He<br />

works in modelling a number of biological phenomena, most<br />

notably including the areas of dominance hierarchies and of<br />

food-stealing. Recent work involves the modelling of simple<br />

evolution processes and more complex games on structured<br />

populations, especially graphs.<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: ba l a n c i n g r i s k s a n D r e w a r D s: t h e l o g i c o f v i o l e n c e<br />

John Archer<br />

Professor, Director of Research The Aggression Research Group<br />

University of Central Lancashire, UK<br />

http://www.uclan.ac.uk/facs/science/psychol/research/people/Archer.htm<br />

Prof. Archer’s research interests are the study of human aggression,<br />

and bereavement and loss. He is the former President<br />

of the International Society for Research on Aggression<br />

(ISRA), and is on the editorial board of the journal Aggressive<br />

Behavior. He is the author of several books and has published<br />

over 100 articles, with a total citation count of over 3,500. John<br />

Archer studies topics such as young men’s physical aggression<br />

from an evolutionary perspective, including cross-national<br />

comparisons, partner violence and stalking, bullying and aggression<br />

in prisons and other secure institutions, testosterone<br />

and aggression, and people’s attributions about their aggression.<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: se X u a l s e l e c t i o n a s a n e X p l a n a t i o n o f h u m a n s e X D i f f e r e n c e s in<br />

a g g r e s s i o n<br />

Kent Kiehl<br />

Professor of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of New<br />

Mexico; Director, Mobile Imaging Core and Clinical Cognitive<br />

Neuroscience, Mind Research Network, New Mexico<br />

http://www.mrn.org/kkiehl<br />

Prof. Kiehl conducts clinical neuroscience research of major<br />

mental illnesses, with special focus on criminal psychopathy,<br />

substance abuse, and psychotic disorders. His group uses noninvasive<br />

techniques for measuring brain function, including<br />

event-related potentials (ERPs) and functional magnetic resonance<br />

imaging (fMRI). He has authored over 70 articles and<br />

book chapters and he currently directs four major NIH projects<br />

in the areas of adolescent psychopathy, adult psychopathy,<br />

substance abuse, and early stage psychosis.<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: criminal ps y c h o p a t h y : as s e s s m e n t, vi o l e n c e , a n D ne u r o s c i e n c e<br />

13


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

14<br />

Sheilagh Hodgins<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

Head of Forensic Mental Health Sciences King’s College<br />

London, UK<br />

Professor at the Institute of Psychiatry King’s College,<br />

University of London UK<br />

http://www.iop.kcl.ac.uk/staff/profile/?go=10161<br />

Sheilagh Hodgins has authored numerous papers, book chapters,<br />

and books on antisocial, violent, and criminal behaviours<br />

among persons with mental disorders. Her current work aims<br />

to identify causal mechanisms leading to early-onset antisocial<br />

behaviour that remain stable over the life-span among persons<br />

who develop schizophrenia, as well as the factors that maintain<br />

these unwanted behaviours. She also works on the development<br />

of treatments to reduce violent behaviour.<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: ge t t i n g t h e p h e n o t y p e s r i g h t : th e e s s e n t i a l i n g r e D i e n t t o u n D e r s t a n D i n g<br />

a e t i o l o g i c a l m e c h a n i s m s a n D D e v e l o p i n g e f f e c t i v e t r e a t m e n t s<br />

Joshua W. Buckholtz<br />

Scientist at the Neuroscience Vanderbilt Brain Institute<br />

Department of Psychology Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN<br />

USA<br />

http://www.vanderbilt.edu/psychological_sciences/people/buckholtz<br />

Joshua Buckholtz uses functional, structural, and receptor imaging<br />

techniques to characterize the influence of genetic risk<br />

factors for antisocial behavior and addiction on neural circuitry<br />

for affect regulation, motivation, and social cognition. He obtained<br />

his bachelor of science degree from the University of<br />

Wisconsin – Madison and then received training in genetics<br />

and neuroimaging in the intramural program of the National<br />

Institute of Mental Health.<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: mu l t i m o D a l c h a r a c t e r i z a t i o n o f g e n e t i c r i s k m e c h a n i s m s f o r impulsive<br />

a g g r e s s i o n<br />

Stephen J. Suomi<br />

Chief of the Laboratory of Comparative Ethology, the National<br />

Institute of Child Health & Human Development (NICHD),<br />

National Institutes of Health (NIH), Bethesda, Maryland<br />

http://www.nichd.nih.gov/about/staff/bio.cfm?nih_id=0010152932<br />

Prof. Suomi has received international recognition for his extensive<br />

research on biobehavioral development in rhesus monkeys<br />

and other primate species, with a particular focus on aggressive<br />

behaviors. His research at Wisconsin led to his election<br />

as Fellow in the American Association for the Advancement of<br />

Science “for major contributions to the understanding of social<br />

factors that influence the psychological development of nonhuman<br />

primates.”<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: ag g r e s s i o n, s e r o t o n i n , a n D g e n e X e n v i r o n m e n t i n t e r a c t i o n s


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

Speakers, Discussants and Round Table Panelists<br />

Francois Ansermet<br />

Head Service of psychiatry of children and adolescents Geneva<br />

University Hospital<br />

Professor of Psychiatry at the University of Geneva Faculty of<br />

Medicine<br />

http://spea.hug-ge.ch/<br />

Prof. Ansermet’s research has focused on perinatal stress, and,<br />

specifically, on risks for and consequences of prematurity. He<br />

has also explored the psychological and ethical implications<br />

of new advances in perinatal biotechnology, in particular, assisted<br />

reproductive therapy, genetic counselling, and gender<br />

attribution in cases of ambiguous genitalia. Moreover, Prof.<br />

Ansermet is interested in the link between neuroscience and<br />

psychoanalysis and the role of neuronal plasticity in the psychoanalytic<br />

process.<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: ne u r o n a l p l a s t i c i t y, h o m e o s t a s i s a n D v i o l e n c e<br />

Pierre Magistretti<br />

Professor of Neuroscience - Director, Brain Mind Institute, <strong>EPFL</strong><br />

Director of the Center for Psychiatric Neuroscience, University<br />

of Lausanne Medical School and Hospital (CHUV)<br />

http://bmi.epfl.ch/page61215.html<br />

Prof. Magistretti’s laboratory has discovered some of the cellular<br />

and molecular mechanisms that underlie the coupling between<br />

neuronal activity and energy consumption by the brain.<br />

He is strongly engaged in public understanding of neuroscience<br />

and in establishing links betweens neuroscience and psychoanalysis.<br />

He has occupied an International Chair 2007-08<br />

of the Collège de France, Paris. He is member of Academia Europea<br />

and of the Swiss Academy of Medical Sciences. He is the<br />

Past-President of the Federation of European Neuroscience Societies<br />

(FENS) and Vice-Chairman of the European Dana Alliance<br />

for the Brain (EDAB).<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: ne u r o n a l p l a s t i c i t y, h o m e o s t a s i s a n D v i o l e n c e<br />

Inga D. Neumann<br />

Professor, Head Department of Behavioural and Molecular<br />

Neuroendocrinology University of Regensburg, Germany<br />

http://www.biologie.uni-regensburg.de/Zoologie/Neumann/index.htm<br />

Her main research interest focuses on uncovering molecular,<br />

neuronal and neuroendocrine parameters linked to complex social<br />

and emotional behaviours, including intermale aggression,<br />

depression- and anxiety-related behaviours, social phobia and<br />

social cognition as well as maternal care and aggression. Using<br />

animal models, she studies interactions between genetic<br />

and early environmental events shaping adult stress-related<br />

behaviours.<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: ge n e t i c a n D e a r l y e n v i r o n m e n t a l f a c t o r s s h a p i n g a D u l t m a l e a g g r e s s i o n<br />

15


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

16<br />

Jaap M. Koolhaas<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

Professor in Behavioral Physiology, University of Groningen,<br />

The Netherlands<br />

http://www.onderzoekinformatie.nl/en/oi/nod/onderzoeker/PRS1237036/<br />

His research aims to unravel the behavioral and physiological<br />

mechanisms by which animals (rodents) adapt to challenges<br />

in their social environment. Specifically, his group studies<br />

both the causes and the consequences of intermale aggressive<br />

behavior. The research tries to understand the interaction<br />

between social environmental demands and the individual<br />

capacity to cope with these demands in terms of behavior and<br />

physiology (coping style). The results of his studies are relevant<br />

for understanding the interaction between personality<br />

and environmental factors in the development of violence and<br />

depression.<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: ag g r e s s i o n a n D vi o l e n c e in a n i m a l s; r o l e o f p e r s o n a l i t y f a c t o r s<br />

Carmen Sandi<br />

Professor, Head of Laboratory of Behavioral Genetics<br />

Brain Mind Institute <strong>EPFL</strong>, Switzerland<br />

http://lgc.epfl.ch/<br />

Carmen Sandi is interested in understanding how stress affects<br />

brain function, behavior and cognition. Her work has implicated<br />

stress hormones and associated pathways in the modulation of<br />

learning and memory. Her recent work focuses on the impact<br />

of stress on social dominance hierarchies and aggressive behaviour.<br />

Her lab is also devoted to develop cognitive enhancers<br />

and new drug treatments for stress-related mood and anxiety<br />

disorders.<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: st r e s s a s a g e n e r a t o r o f s o c i a l i m b a l a n c e<br />

Carmen Sandi, M. Isabel Cordero, Cristina Marquez, Marjan Timmer<br />

Mark Levine<br />

Professor of Psychology at the Lancaster University, UK<br />

http://www.psych.lancs.ac.uk/people/MarkLevine.html<br />

Mark Levine’s research, in the behaviour of bystanders in emergencies<br />

has integrated classic work on bystander intervention<br />

with more recent developments in the social psychology of<br />

group behaviour. His research uses a variety of quantitative<br />

and qualitative methods to explore bystander behaviour at the<br />

group level, with a particular interest on analysing ‘real life’<br />

behavioural data, or at least trying to study behaviour in ‘real<br />

time’. Recently, he has been working on an analysis of CCTV<br />

footage of violent incidents in the night-time economy.<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: gr o u p s , by s t a n D e r s a n D t h e in f o r m a l re g u l a t i o n o f vi o l e n c e


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

Speakers, Discussants and Round Table Panelists<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: ??<br />

Mel Slater<br />

Professor of Virtual Environments Department of Computer<br />

Science University College London, UK<br />

http://www.event-lab.org<br />

Mel Slater carries out research in immersive virtual reality -<br />

systems that incorporate a participant into a virtual space that<br />

is created by computer displays. His research covers both the<br />

technical aspects, such as creating new display methods, and<br />

new forms of interaction, and also the scientific side, in order<br />

to understand how people respond to being inside a virtual reality.<br />

His main application interests are in the use virtual reality<br />

in aspects of neuroscience and psychotherapy. In 2005 he<br />

was awarded the Virtual Reality Career Award by IEEE Virtual<br />

Reality.<br />

Lee Alan Dugatkin<br />

Professor and Distinguished University Scholar, Department of<br />

Biology, University of Louisville, USA<br />

http://louisville.edu/~laduga01/dugatkin.htm<br />

Prof. Lee Alan Dugatkin’s main area of research interest is the<br />

evolution of social behavior. He is currently studying the evolution<br />

of cooperation, the evolution of aggression, the interaction<br />

between genetic and cultural evolution, the evolution<br />

of disease virulence, the evolution of antibiotic resistance and<br />

the evolution of risk-taking behavior. He is the author of seven<br />

books on evolution and behavior, and has published over 120<br />

articles in highly regarded scientific journals.<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: wi n n e r s , l o s e r s a n D b y s t a n D e r s : t h e s t r u c t u r e o f D o m i n a n c e h i e r a r c h i e s<br />

Albert Rizzo<br />

Professor, Institute for Creative Technologies University of<br />

Southern California, USA<br />

http://www.usc.edu/schools/medicine/departments/cell_neurobiology/research/isnsrrizzo_pres.htmll<br />

Albert Rizzo conducts research on virtual applications that use<br />

360 Degree Panoramic video for exposure therapy (social phobia),<br />

role-playing applications, anger management, etc.), journalism<br />

studies and digital media art creation. He also conducts<br />

research to study of how virtual reality simulation technology<br />

can be usefully applied to serve the needs of the user/client/<br />

patient in a manner that goes beyond what is available with<br />

traditional 20th Century tools and methods. His latest project<br />

has focused on an exposure therapy application for combatrelated<br />

PTSD with Iraq War veterans.<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong> Title: re-e X p o s u r e t o co m b a t in vi r t u a l re a l i t y a s a tr e a t m e n t f o r ptsD<br />

17


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

Discussants<br />

Didier Trono<br />

18<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

Professor, Dean of Life Science Faculty <strong>EPFL</strong>, Switzerland<br />

http://tronolab.epfl.ch/<br />

His research focuses on interactions between viral pathogens<br />

and their hosts, and on the exploration of genetics from both<br />

fundamental and therapeutic perspectives. More recently, he<br />

has become interested on the study of epigenetic mechanisms<br />

regulating brain function and behavior and has started a research<br />

programme addressed to ascertain genomic and epigenomic<br />

mechanisms linked to differences in personality traits<br />

and stress vulnerability. For the last four years, has been serving<br />

as deputy director of the Swiss National Science Foundation<br />

“Frontiers in Genetics” Center of Excellence.<br />

Ronald de Kloet<br />

Professor of Medical Pharmacology, Leiden University The<br />

Netherlands<br />

http://www.medicalpharmacology.leidenuniv.nl/<br />

Ron de Kloet is a work leader in the field of neuroendocrinology<br />

of stress and adaptation. He received the 2008 Lifetime<br />

Achievement Award of the International Society of Psychoneuroendocrinology,<br />

the 2007 Award of the European College<br />

of Neuropsychopharmacology and the 2005 Geoffrey Harris<br />

Award of the European Federation of Endocrine Societies. In<br />

2004 he was awarded the prestigious Academy Professorship<br />

of the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences, one<br />

of the highest scientific accolades in The Netherlands. He has<br />

published more than 550 articles on the topic and 6 books.<br />

Alexander Butchart<br />

Prevention of Violence Coordinator, WHO, Switzerland<br />

http://www.who.int/violence_injury_prevention/violence/en/<br />

Dr. Alexander Butchart’s responsibilities at WHO include coordinating<br />

the Global Campaign for Violence Prevention, the development<br />

of policy for the prevention of interpersonal violence,<br />

preparation of guidelines for the prevention of specific types of<br />

interpersonal violence, and the coordination of research into<br />

various aspects of interpersonal violence and its prevention.<br />

Prior to joining WHO, he worked mainly in Southern and East<br />

Africa, where he was lead scientist in the South African Violence<br />

and Injury Surveillance Consortium, and participated in<br />

training violence and injury prevention workers from a number<br />

of African countries.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

Speakers, Discussants and Round Table Panelists<br />

Olaf Blanke<br />

Professor, Head Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience Brain<br />

Mind Institute <strong>EPFL</strong>, Switzerland<br />

http://lnco.epfl.ch/<br />

Olaf Blanke’s research focuses on cognitive and social neuroscience<br />

and the role of different sensory systems and the motor<br />

systems in bodily self-consciousness. Projects rely on the investigation<br />

of healthy subjects as well as neurological patients<br />

combining psychophysical and cognitive paradigms with state<br />

of the art neuroimaging techniques such as intracranial electroencephalography<br />

(EEG), surface EEG, and functional magnetic<br />

resonance imaging. His most recent research on bodily<br />

self consciousness has integrated full-body tracking and other<br />

technologies from the field of virtual reality with cognitive science<br />

and neuroimaging.<br />

Daniel Schechter<br />

Professor, Department of Psychiatry Columbia University<br />

Medical Center<br />

http://asp.cumc.columbia.edu/facdb/profile_list.asp?uni=dss11&DepAffil=Ps<br />

ychiatry<br />

Daniel Schechter’s focuses on the effects of mothers’violencerelated<br />

posttraumatic stress disorder(PTSD) on their functioning<br />

as caregivers. In further examining the impact on the children’s<br />

development of stress reactivity, psychiatric symptoms,<br />

and mental representations of self and other prospectively,<br />

his team hopes to understand how psychosocial interventions<br />

might more effectively target and interrupt intergenerational<br />

cycles of violence and related psychopathology.<br />

Philippe Cotter<br />

University of Geneva<br />

http://www.eclectica.ch/<br />

Philippe Cotter’s research initially focused on collective violence<br />

within the parameters of traditional historical analysis.<br />

Then, intellectual curiosity incited him to look at other forms of<br />

extreme violence. He found a number of similarities between<br />

the psychology of the serial killer and that of political extremists,<br />

the Nazi leaders in particular. His field of study then extended<br />

to include terrorist violence in the wake of 9/11, making<br />

use of all relevant scientific disciplines. His subsequent publications<br />

are the result of the unexpected convergence between<br />

the functioning of individual and collective forms of extreme<br />

violence. His latest book is La vengeance des humiliés (written<br />

with Gilbert Holleufer).<br />

19


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

20<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

Round Table Panellists<br />

Hans-Peter Schmoll-Flockerzie<br />

AHBasel Krisentintervention für Jugendliche, Basel<br />

http://psychologie.ahbasel.ch/index_html?content=Team<br />

Hans-Peter Schmoll-Flockerzie is Psychologists specialized in<br />

Clinical Psychology, Criminology, Behavioral Therapy and Supervision.<br />

He treats violent adolescents with the method of Anti-Aggressivitäts-Training<br />

AAT © and Coolness Training CT. He<br />

gives courses of AAT/CT to tutors and teachers in the Institute<br />

of Confrontational Pedagogy IK-S (www.ik-s.ch).<br />

Stephen Thomas<br />

Security Officer for UEFA and Assistant Chief Constable British<br />

Transport Police<br />

Security Officer for UEFA and Assistant Chief Constable British Transport<br />

Police<br />

Stephen has performed his UEFA role in many European countries<br />

for club competitions and the European Championships<br />

of 2004 and 2008. He was a Syndicate Director for the UEFA<br />

Safety and Security Education <strong>Program</strong>me.<br />

He provides advice to those hosting international football tournaments,<br />

club competition final games and on measures improve<br />

safety and security. His policing role includes being the<br />

head of Football Policing in the UK, where he works closely<br />

with football authorities, clubs and police organizations to reduce<br />

violence in football, as well as leading English Police Delegations<br />

to other European countries for national team and club<br />

games.<br />

Bruno Gravier<br />

Head of the Service de Médecine et Psychiatrie Pénitentiaires<br />

Associate Professor/Psychiatry Department/ Centre Hospitaler<br />

Universitaire Vaudois, University of Lausanne<br />

http://www.chuv.ch/psy/smpp.htm<br />

Bruno Gravier has devote himself to the developpment of medical<br />

and psychiatric care in correctional facilities. He has created<br />

in the CHUV of Lausanne a specific service for treating<br />

mentally ill offenders inside the prisons of the Canton de Vaud.<br />

His main fields of interest and research are the assessment of<br />

dangerousness, the treatment of sexual offenders and the articulation<br />

between care and law in a forensic field. He his president<br />

of the swiss association of doctors working in prison and<br />

vice president of the Swiss Forensic Psychiatry Society.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

Speakers, Discussants and Round Table Panelists<br />

Gregoire Rubovszky<br />

Head of Emergency Psychiatry Service, HCUGE, Geneva<br />

Dr Gregoire Rubovszky is leading the emergency psychiatry unit<br />

in Geneva. He was consultant in the Interdisciplinary Consultation<br />

of Medicine and Prevention of Violence. He is in charge of<br />

the PTSD consultation team inside the consultation liaison and<br />

he is also contributing for the Consultation for Victims of Tortures<br />

and War.<br />

Christopher Mikton<br />

Technical Officer, Prevention of Violence Unit, WHO,<br />

Switzerland<br />

http://www.who.int/violence_injury_prevention/about/whos_who/mikton/<br />

en/index.html<br />

Dr Mikton’s work at WHO focuses on promoting the integration<br />

of violence prevention activities in Official Development<br />

Assistance agency programmes; the prevention of child maltreatment;<br />

maintaining the Violence Prevention Alliance secretariat;<br />

and evaluating the effectiveness of violence prevention<br />

interventions. Before joining WHO’s Violence Prevention Team,<br />

he worked as Clinical Scientist for the UK Ministry of Justice<br />

and Department of Health’s Dangerous and Severe Personality<br />

Disorder <strong>Program</strong>me, primarily conducting research on the effectiveness<br />

of the programme.<br />

Jakob Lund<br />

Director of the SMART programs in Scandinavia.<br />

http://www.artofliving.org/<br />

Jacob Lund teaches breathing techniques (SMART) as a deep<br />

therapy for releasing anger and negative emotions. Jacob is<br />

instructor in marshal art and conducts courses inside and outside<br />

prison for persons who have problems with drugs, crime<br />

and violent behaviour; and for prison staff, psychologist, social<br />

workers and other persons involved in this area. The first<br />

SMART course was taught in Denmark, in the beginning of the<br />

year 2000. The affectivity of the courses is now being recognized<br />

by the Danish Department of Justice and the municipality<br />

(local authority).<br />

21


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

22<br />

Marie-Claude Hofner<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

Medical doctor - Violence Medical Unit - University Center of<br />

Legal Medicine Western Switzerland – University Department<br />

of Community Medicine. Lausanne – Switzerland.<br />

http://www.artofliving.org/<br />

Marie-Claude Hofner is a medical doctor specialised in public<br />

health. Since 1998 she is directing an interdisciplinary prevention<br />

program of interpersonal violence, which lead to the<br />

creation of the Violence Medical Unit (VMU) of the University<br />

Hospital Centre of Lausanne. The VMU is an original example<br />

of medico-legal and community oriented service for adults victims<br />

of violence. She is also currently deputy to represent Switzerland<br />

within the European network “Violence and Injury Prevention”<br />

of the Whorl Health Organisation.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong>s for Speakers<br />

Part 1: Speaker <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

23


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-1<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong>s for Speakers<br />

De v e l o p m e n t a l t r a j e c t o r i e s o f h u m a n p h y s i -<br />

c a l v i o l e n c e a n D t h e i r D e t e r m i n a n t s : fr o m<br />

g e n e s t o s o c i a l p o l i c y (o r v i c e v e r s a)<br />

Tremblay, Richard E.<br />

Research Unit on Children’s Psychosocial Maladjustment University<br />

of Montreal, Canada<br />

The aim of this presentation is to summarize recent results on:<br />

1. developmental trajectories of physical aggression from birth to adulthood;<br />

2. long term outcomes of early chronic physical aggression;<br />

3. genetic and environmental (physical and social) risk factors of chronic<br />

physical aggression;<br />

4. genetic and environmental mechanisms leading to chronic physical aggression;<br />

5. effectiveness of preventive interventions;<br />

6. implications of the 5 previous topics for the prevention of chronic physical<br />

aggression. The presentation will conclude with examples of new experiments<br />

for advancing knowledge on preventable causal mechanisms.<br />

25


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-2<br />

26<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

th e n e u r o b i o l o g y o f impulsivity a n D a g g r e ss<br />

i o n in aDhD<br />

Lesch, Klaus-Peter<br />

Department of Psychiatry, Psychsomatics, and Psychotherapy,<br />

University of Würzburg, Germany<br />

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; MIM #143465) is defined<br />

as a clinically heterogeneous neurodevelopmental syndrome comprising the<br />

triad of inattention, hyperactivity and increased impulsivity. It is the most<br />

common behavioral disorder in children with persistence into adulthood<br />

which profoundly compromises functioning in multiple areas throughout<br />

the life span and can significantly contribute to a variety of health, social,<br />

and economic problems. Affected individuals are at increased risk for poor<br />

educational and occupational achievement despite normal cognitive and<br />

intellectual abilities, low income, underemployment, impaired social skills<br />

and relationships, family dysfunction, legal difficulties, and delinquency. On<br />

the other hand, high IQ and a highly supportive, wellstructured family environment<br />

are protective factors against ADHD-related behavioral limitations.<br />

While an age-dependent fading may render symptoms not prominent<br />

enough to justify diagnosis of ADHD in adulthood, they are frequently associated<br />

with clinically significant impairment of cognitive and executive<br />

functions as well as stress coping and emotion regulation. As a result, adult<br />

ADHD is characterized by high co-morbidity with depression, anxiety disorders,<br />

alcohol/drug dependence, and antisocial personality disorders.<br />

Twin, adoption, and molecular genetic studies revealed that ADHD is a highly<br />

heritable disorder (h2: 70–80%) with a multifactorial pattern of inheritance,<br />

likely due to several genes of small or moderate effect size. Genom-wide linkage<br />

and microarray-based association and copy number variation analyses<br />

identified several susceptibility loci and risk genes. Frequency and ancestry<br />

of the susceptibility variants are consistent with the concept that traits associated<br />

with the ADHD phenotype have been subject to positive selection<br />

and that ADHD is the extreme of a normal variation exacerbated by adverse<br />

environmental circumstances. With focus on relevance to pathophysiological<br />

and pharmacotherapeutic mechanisms, the candidate gene approach<br />

has also been utilized in case-control or family-based studies. Investigations<br />

concentrated on genes which modulate synaptic transmission and association<br />

with ADHD was detected for genes encoding key modulators of<br />

the dopaminergic and serotonergic signalling pathways. Moreover, gene<br />

targeting approaches, e.g. generation of knockout mice, provide informative<br />

insight into pathophysiological mechanisms of locomotor hyperactivity<br />

and effects of psychostimulant drugs, such as methylphenidate or cocaine.<br />

Finally, complex interactions are to be expected between environmental<br />

factors and multiple genes each with a small to moderate influence on different<br />

traits. Perinatal complications, low socio-economic status, disruptive<br />

family environment and other psychosocial adversity have been identified


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-2<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong>s for Speakers<br />

th e n e u r o b i o l o g y o f impulsivity a n D a g g r e ss<br />

i o n in aDhD (co n t i n u e D )<br />

Lesch, Klaus-Peter<br />

Department of Psychiatry, Psychsomatics, and Psychotherapy,<br />

University of Würzburg, Germany<br />

as predisposing environmental risk factors. While prenatal and parental risk<br />

factors may be critical mediators of influences on the risk, the association<br />

between these variables and ADHD is generally indirect.<br />

Converging evidence from animal and human studies including structural<br />

and functional neuroimaging implicates dysregulation of prefrontal–striatal–<br />

thalamic– cerebellar excitatory and inhibitory circuits with broadening to a<br />

multi-pathway framework in the pathophysiology of ADHD. It is widely accepted<br />

that ADHD is the common final behavioral consequence of an array<br />

of dysfunctions in each of several independent systems, such as cognitive,<br />

motivational, and executive pathways, as well as circuitries of stress adaptation<br />

and emotion regulation. Executive functions, which consist of a set<br />

of higher order thought processes required for adaptive and future-oriented<br />

behavior (e.g. deliberate suppression of a response to achieve a later, internally<br />

represented goal) and are controlled by frontal-subcortical circuits,<br />

include behavioral inhibition, working memory, attention setshifting, interference<br />

control, planning, and sustained attention. Impairment of executive<br />

functions with failure of inhibitory control; dysregulation of brain systems<br />

mediating reward and response cost; and deficits in arousal, activation,<br />

and effortful control, are central to the pattern of neuropsychological deficits.<br />

Deficits in arousal and effort lead to state-dependent cognitive deficits<br />

and underscore the view of an impairment in regulating cognitive functions<br />

rather than core deficits in any single function. Inattention but not hyperactivity/impulsivity<br />

is associated with deficits in executive functioning and<br />

poor academic achievement, whereas hyperactivity/impulsivity appears to<br />

be more closely related to dysfunctions of reward mechanisms.<br />

Functional neuroimaging studies have assessed the degree of brain activation<br />

associated with neuropsychological tasks of attention and disinhibition.<br />

The findings are consistent with the structural studies indicating delays in<br />

brain maturation processes and locating abnormalities of brain activation<br />

in patients with ADHD in fronto–subcortical–cerebellar circuits. Since the<br />

spectrum of ADHD features is not explained by a single neuropsychological<br />

deficit, disorder-associated impairments are heterogeneous and this complexity<br />

corresponds with causal heterogeneity. Despite recognition of ADHD<br />

as a neurodevelopmental condition, only few causal explanations have considered<br />

the two-way interactions between pre-existing abnormal functioning<br />

and biological, cognitive, emotional, motor and social developmental processes,<br />

and their contribution to the expression of the clinical phenotype.<br />

27


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-3<br />

28<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

ag g r e s s i o n, s e r o t o n i n , a n D g e n e X e n v i r o n -<br />

m e n t i n t e r a c t i o n s<br />

Suomi, Stephen J.<br />

Laboratory of Comparative Ethology, the National Institute of<br />

Child Health & Human Development (NICHD), National Institutes<br />

of Health (NIH), Bethesda, Maryland<br />

Recent research has disclosed marked individual differences in patterns<br />

of biobehavioral development exhibited by rhesus monkeys across the life<br />

span. For example, approximately 5-10% of rhesus monkeys growing up in<br />

the wild consistently exhibit impulsive and/or inappropriately aggressive responses<br />

to mildly stressful situations throughout development; those same<br />

individuals also show chronic deficits in their central serotonin metabolism.<br />

These characteristic patterns of biobehavioral response emerge early in life<br />

and remain remarkably stable from infancy to adulthood. Laboratory studies<br />

have demonstrated that although these characteristics are highly heritable,<br />

they are also subject to major modification by specific early experiences,<br />

particularly those involving early social attachment relationships.<br />

For example, a specific polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene<br />

is associated with deficits in serotonin metabolism, extreme aggression,<br />

and excessive alcohol consumption among monkeys who have experienced<br />

insecure early attachment relationships but not in monkeys who have developed<br />

secure attachment relationships with their mothers during infancy<br />

(“maternal buffering”). Moreover, because the attachment style of a monkey<br />

mother is typically “copied” by her daughters when they grow up and<br />

become mothers themselves, similar buffering is likely to occur for the next<br />

generation of infants carrying that specific polymorphism.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-4<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong>s for Speakers<br />

th e ev o l u t i o n an D aD a p t i v e fu n c t i o n s o f<br />

an g e r , ag g r e s s i o n a n D vi o l e n c e<br />

Blanchard, Caroline<br />

Pacific Biosciences Research Center, and Dept. of Genetics and<br />

Molecular Biology, John A. Burns School of Medicine, University<br />

of Hawaii<br />

Evolutionary analyses of behavior begin with information indicating that the<br />

behavior in question is present, to varying degrees, in individuals within a<br />

species; that it can be analyzed in terms of antecedent conditions and probable<br />

outcomes in situations similar to those in which the species evolved;<br />

and that there is evidence of a genetic influence on that behavior. Additional<br />

factors, such as relatively consistent sex or age-based modulation of<br />

that behavior may contribute to the specificity and rigor of analysis.<br />

For aggression and violence, long regarded as aberrant behaviors, all of<br />

these conditions are met. Under a wide range of known conditions, aggression<br />

and some other forms of violence may be highly adaptive responses,<br />

for both the aggressive individual and its relatives. This talk will examine<br />

the circumstances eliciting or facilitating different aspects of aggression and<br />

violence, across mammalian species, with attention to factors such as resource<br />

competition, bodily threats to the individual, weapon systems, social<br />

structures, and age and sex differences. While the focus will be on nonhuman<br />

mammals, application of information from such species may be applied<br />

to aggression and violence in people. The human experience of anger<br />

is strongly associated with some forms of aggression, as well as aggressive<br />

motivations that may not be expressed in actions. Anger may therefore<br />

provide insights into the situations that elicit aggression and violence in<br />

humans, and the goals with which it is associated, as well as social, demographic,<br />

and individual factors that contribute to the inhibition of overt aggression<br />

and violence.<br />

29


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-5<br />

30<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

a r o l e f o r a r g i n i n e v a s o p r e s s i n a n D<br />

o X y t o c i n in s h a p i n g h u m a n s o c i a l b e h a v i o r<br />

Ebstein, Richard<br />

Psychology Department Hebrew University and Herzog Hospital<br />

Jerusalem Israel<br />

Increasing evidence suggests that two nonapeptides, arginine vasopressin<br />

and oxytocin, shape human social behavior in both nonclinical and clinical<br />

subjects. We will discuss evidence that in autism spectrum disorders genetic<br />

polymorphisms in the arginine vasopressin receptor 1a (AVPR1a), the oxytocin<br />

receptor (OXTR), CD38 and neurophysin I & II contribute to deficits<br />

in socialization skills in this group of patients. Additionally, we will present<br />

molecular genetic evidence that in nonclinical subjects both the AVPR1a and<br />

OXTR receptors contribute to prosocial or altruistic behavior inventoried by<br />

two experimental paradigms, the Dictator Game and Social Values Orientation.<br />

We will further discuss the strategy of employing experimental economic<br />

games combined with molecular genetics towards understanding the<br />

social brain. Finally, we will present evidence that intranasal administration<br />

of vasopressin increases social stress using the Trier Social Stress Test.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-6<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong>s for Speakers<br />

ne u r o g e n e t i c a n D n e u r o h o r m o n a l m e c h a -<br />

n i s m s o f p r o s o c i a l a n D a g g r e s s i v e b e h a v i o r<br />

Meyer -Lindenberg, Andreas<br />

Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim, Germany<br />

Well-being and survival in primates, including humans, depends critically<br />

on social interactions, and disturbed social behavior is a key component of<br />

diseases such as autism, schizophrenia, and anxiety disorders. However,<br />

little is known about specific neurobiological factors shaping the human social<br />

brain. Since many aspects of social function are highly heritable (4), we<br />

have adopted a genetic approach to identify molecular and systems-level<br />

mechanisms of social cognition in humans. Studies in Williams Syndrome, a<br />

genetic condition with pronounced hypersociability, identified abnormal prefrontal<br />

regulation of amygdala as a neural substrate of social fear regulation<br />

under genetic control. Studies of candidate gene polymorphisms (5-HTTLPR,<br />

MAOA vNTR) impacting on personality, aggressive behavior, and emotional<br />

regulation modulate on similar circuitry. In animals, oxytocin and vasopressin<br />

are key mediators of complex emotional and social behaviors, reduce<br />

anxiety and impact on fear conditioning and extinction. Recently, oxytocin<br />

administration in humans was shown to increase trust, suggesting involvement<br />

of the amygdala, a central component of the neurocircuitry of fear and<br />

social cognition that has been linked to trust and highly expresses oxytocin<br />

receptors in many mammals. We report on functional neuroimaging studies<br />

in healthy human subjects. In males receiving oxytocin or placebo, oxytocin<br />

potently reduced activation of the amygdala and reduced coupling of<br />

the amygdala to brainstem regions implicated in autonomic and behavioral<br />

manifestations of fear. We also report on imaging genetic studies characterizing<br />

the effects of genetic variation in the vasopressin receptor (AVPR1A)<br />

and the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR), implicated in risk for autism, on<br />

brain structure and function related to emotional regulation and social behavior.<br />

Taken together, the results suggest neural mechanisms for social<br />

cognition in the human brain that begin to define mechanisms for both pro-<br />

and antisocial behavior as well as molecular approaches to modulate and<br />

treat these behaviors.<br />

31


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-7<br />

32<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

ty p e s o f a g g r e s s i o n a n D t y p e s o f b r a i n<br />

m e c h a n i s m s . th e r o l e o f g l u c o c o r t i c o i D s<br />

Haller, Joseph<br />

Behavioural Neurobiology Department Institute of Experimental<br />

Medicine Budapest, Hungary<br />

Aggression is a behavioral mechanism of competition, which consists of acts<br />

that are aimed at or threat with, inflicting physical harm in social conflict.<br />

Aggressiveness is expressed by all species that are endowed with appropriate<br />

motor skills and neural systems. As such, this behavior cannot be considered<br />

abnormal per se. Yet, aggressiveness constitutes a major problem<br />

in human societies. Those manifestations of aggression that raise the largest<br />

concerns either loose their competitive role (they are internally driven<br />

rather than goal oriented) or disregard social rules, for which reasons they<br />

cannot be considered normal any more. Not surprisingly, such abnormal<br />

manifestations of aggressiveness usually occur in the context of various<br />

psychological disorders.<br />

We argue here that the neural control of normal and abnormal aggression<br />

is different, and changes in glucocorticoid secretion patterns are among the<br />

factors that underlie the behavioral and neural shifts that lead to abnormal<br />

aggression. This proposal will be substantiated by laboratory studies<br />

involving models of abnormal aggression. We will show that factors that<br />

lead to psychopathology-associated abnormal aggression in humans result<br />

in the development of abnormal aggression patterns in laboratory rodents.<br />

Among the eliciting factors, the ones that affect glucocorticoid secretion<br />

will be in focus. We will also show that the neural control of such abnormal<br />

aggression patterns is different from the one that underlies male rivalry aggression,<br />

which can be considered normal in laboratory rodents. Important<br />

differences were found at many levels of the neuronal circuitry that controls<br />

aggression, e.g. in the prefrontal cortex, septum, extended amygdala,<br />

hypothalamus, and brainstem. We will also show that normal and abnormal<br />

aggression responds differently to pharmacological agents, suggesting that<br />

agents that decrease rivalry aggression will not necessarily decrease abnormal<br />

manifestations of aggressiveness. We suggest that different types of<br />

human aggression need different pharmacological approaches.<br />

Our findings demonstrate that aggression is not a unitary phenomenon,<br />

neither in terms of behavioral manifestations nor in terms of neural control.<br />

A differentiated view on these phenomena offers the chance of a deeper<br />

understanding and may enable the development more efficient treatment<br />

strategies.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-8<br />

th e vi o l e n c e o f in e q u a l i t y<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong>s for Speakers<br />

Wilkinson, Richard<br />

University of Nottingham Medical School, UK; International Centre<br />

for Health and Society, University College London<br />

Rates of violence and homicide in societies are part of a wider pattern of social<br />

dysfunction. If you look at rich countries and compare life expectancy,<br />

mental health, homicide rates, conflict between school children, teenage<br />

birth rates, drug abuse, obesity rates, levels of trust or the strength of community<br />

life, you find that countries which tend to do well on one of these<br />

measures tend to do well on all of them, and the ones which do badly, do<br />

badly on all of them. What accounts for the difference?<br />

The key seems to be the amount of inequality in each society. The picture is<br />

consistent whether we compare rich countries or the 50 states of the USA.<br />

It seems likely that the extent of income inequality in each society serves<br />

as a determinant and expression of the scale and importance of social class<br />

stratification. The greater the inequality, the greater the status competition<br />

and the more prevalent are all the problems associated with relative<br />

deprivation. However, although the amount of inequality has its greatest<br />

effect on rates of problems among the poor, its affects extend to almost all<br />

income groups reducing levels of well-being among the vast majority of the<br />

population.<br />

Inequality has always been regarded as socially corrosive. Now comparisons<br />

between rich market democracies show that even small differences in<br />

inequality affect the quality of social relations in society at large. Research<br />

on the social determinants of health provides some pointers to why are we<br />

so sensitive to inequality: it increases status insecurity, raises the “social<br />

evaluative threat” and heightens issues of respect and disrespect. Particularly<br />

important are the effects of low social status, poor friendship networks<br />

and early childhood experience. These affect forms of psychosocial insecurity,<br />

anxiety and people’s sense of whether they are valued and appreciated.<br />

Through these pathways inequality not only affects social relation within a<br />

society, but also leads to differences in international relations.<br />

33


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-9<br />

34<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

mi c r o-s i t u a t i o n a l a n t e c e D e n t s o f v i o l e n t<br />

a t r o c i t y<br />

Klusemann, Stefan<br />

Sociology MA, University of Pennsylvania, USA<br />

My paper presents an analysis of video-recordings showing micro-situational<br />

events that preceded the 1995 Srebrenica massacre in which more<br />

than 7,000 Bosnian-Muslim men were killed by troops of the Bosnian Serb<br />

Army. The paper focuses on the sequential unfolding of micro-interactions<br />

and emotional dynamics that preceded the atrocity. It is argued that micro-interactions<br />

constitute situational turning points towards, or away from<br />

atrocities. Even if there are pre-planned plots or macro-structural background<br />

conditions which lead particular persons to be motivated to commit<br />

violence, a micro-situational, emotional momentum is needed for atrocities<br />

to occur. The paper breaks new ground methodologically by analyzing video-material<br />

in violent situations with Paul Ekman’s research tools and the<br />

micro-sociological theories of Randall Collins, Jack Katz, Theodore Kemper,<br />

and Thomas Scheff.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-10<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong>s for Speakers<br />

ba l a n c i n g r i s k s a n D r e w a r D s: t h e l o g i c<br />

o f v i o l e n c e<br />

Broom, Mark<br />

Department of Mathematics University of Sussex, UK<br />

The use of violence is widespread throughout the natural world, prominent<br />

examples being predatory violence between species, seasonal violent competition<br />

for mating rights and territories within species and competition for<br />

food both within and between species. These interactions are generally between<br />

unrelated individuals with no social connection. There are, however,<br />

also examples of violent behaviour which occurs within groups of individuals<br />

who otherwise cooperate to live, have significant social bonds and may<br />

also be related, and that is the primary focus of this talk. Examples are in<br />

the establishment and maintenance of dominance hierarchies, or in infanticide,<br />

where (usually) incoming males attempt to kill existing infants in a<br />

group. Such violence can at first sight seem paradoxical, but in fact is often<br />

perfectly logical from the perspective of the individual perpetrating the<br />

violence, as distinct from the group as a whole. We discuss such situations<br />

from the point of view of evolutionary game theory, and also touch on wider<br />

questions of intraspecific violence in general.<br />

35


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-11<br />

36<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

se X u a l s e l e c t i o n a s a n e X p l a n a t i o n o f<br />

h u m a n s e X D i f f e r e n c e s in a g g r e s s i o n<br />

Archer, John<br />

The Aggression Research Group University of Central Lancashire,<br />

UK<br />

Darwin proposed that human sex differences in aggression arose from sexual<br />

selection, principally male competition. In psychology, there has been a<br />

long tradition of environmentally-based explanations, currently represented<br />

by social role theory. Both approaches are outlined, including recent formulations<br />

that address variability and flexibility within a broadly biological<br />

framework. Meta-analytic summaries show that the magnitude of the sex<br />

difference increases in magnitude with the degree of risk involved, consistent<br />

with a sexual selection view emphasizing more risky male than female<br />

competition. Sex differences in physical aggression are found early in life,<br />

and peak in young adulthood, again consistent with an evolutionary origin.<br />

Likely mediators of the sex difference are greater female fear of physical<br />

danger, greater male impulsiveness, and greater female empathy, all of<br />

which fit both a sexual selection and a social role interpretation. Greater<br />

male than female variability in physical aggression is consistent with an<br />

alternative life history perspective derived from sexual selection; variability<br />

according to the internalization of social roles is consistent with a social role<br />

view. Both ecologically-produced and role-related variability are consistent<br />

with both explanations. Overall, there is considerable evidence consistent<br />

with a sexual selection origin for human sex differences in aggression, and<br />

some evidence that social roles influence immediate causation. A range<br />

of other sex differences places these differences in aggression within the<br />

context of an adaptive complex, consistent with humans being a sexuallyselected<br />

species. A very different pattern of sex differences is found for<br />

between-sex aggression in western samples, in particular between partners:<br />

women and men are equally likely to physically aggress. There is considerable<br />

cross-national variability, which is highly correlated with gender<br />

empowerment. An evolutionary origin for partner violence is proposed, in<br />

terms of the conflicts of interest and inequality of coercive power between<br />

the sexes, with societal gender roles accounting for cross-national variability.<br />

The evidence therefore indicates a different operation of evolutionary<br />

and social forces according to sex of the opponent. Overall, sexual selection<br />

provides the most comprehensive explanation for same-sex aggression<br />

and a mix of evolutionary-based conflicts of interest and social roles for<br />

between-sex aggression.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-12<br />

criminal ps y c h o p a t h y : as s e s s m e n t,<br />

vi o l e n c e , a n D ne u r o s c i e n c e<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong>s for Speakers<br />

Kiehl, Kent<br />

Mobile Imaging Core and Clinical Cognitive Neuroscience,<br />

Mind Research Network, Albuquerque, New Mexico<br />

Psychopathy is a personality disorder that includes interpersonal and affective<br />

traits such as glibness, lack of empathy, guilt or remorse, shallow affect,<br />

and irresponsibility, and behavioral characteristics such as impulsivity,<br />

poor behavioral control, and promiscuity. Individuals with psychopathy constitute<br />

approximately 20% of incarcerated populations. Longitudinal studies<br />

have shown that psychopaths commit a disproportionate amount of violent<br />

crime. Recently we have deployed a one-of-a-kind mobile MRI system to<br />

study psychopathy in incarcerated populations. Brain imaging results from<br />

our first 500 offenders will be presented.<br />

37


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-13<br />

38<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

ge t t i n g t h e p h e n o t y p e s r i g h t : th e<br />

e s s e n t i a l i n g r e D i e n t t o u n D e r s t a n D i n g<br />

a e t i o l o g i c a l m e c h a n i s m s a n D D e v e l o p i n g<br />

e f f e c t i v e t r e a t m e n t s<br />

Hodgins, Sheilagh<br />

Institute of Psychiatry King’s College, University of London<br />

UK<br />

The population of individuals who as adults engage in persistent physical<br />

violence towards others is not homogeneous. The available evidence suggests<br />

that it includes at least three sub-groups. While all of the sub-groups<br />

display an early-onset pattern of antisocial behaviour that remains stable<br />

across the life-span, the sub-groups differ in behaviour, mental disorders,<br />

personality traits, emotion processing, cognitive abilities, attentional processes,<br />

and stress reactivity. These differences emerge at an early age and<br />

the sub-groups appear to remain distinct from childhood through adulthood.<br />

Yet, knowledge of the development of these sub-groups of individuals<br />

who become persistent violent offenders is very limited. Consequently,<br />

inadequate characterization of participants in investigations of genes that<br />

confer vulnerability for persistent violent behaviour, of brain structure and<br />

function, and in studies of the links between specific genes and brain structure<br />

and function limits the meaningfulness of the results. Accurate characterization<br />

of sub-groups of persistently violent individuals, at each developmental<br />

stage, is necessary in order to elucidate the complex mechanisms<br />

through the course of development that lead to an adult who persistently<br />

engages in violent behaviour towards others. Without understanding these<br />

developmental mechanisms, the establishment of effective prevention and<br />

treatment programmes is not likely.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-14<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong>s for Speakers<br />

mu l t i m o D a l c h a r a c t e r i z a t i o n o f g e n e t i c<br />

r i s k m e c h a n i s m s f o r impulsive a g g r e s s i o n<br />

Buckholtz, Joshua W.<br />

Neuroscience Vanderbilt Brain Institute Department of Psychology<br />

Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN USA<br />

It has long been noted that antisocial traits and behaviors tend to run in<br />

families. More recently, family, adoption and twin studies have confirmed the<br />

heritability of antisocial aggression, demonstrating that genetic influences<br />

are in great part responsible for its intergenerational transmission. However,<br />

even for the most promising candidate gene for antisociality – MAOA<br />

– statistical genetic associations to disorders of aggression are often weak<br />

and inconsistent. By contrast, a robust and replicated gene-by-environment<br />

interaction has been demonstrated between childhood maltreatment and<br />

allelic variation in the MAOA promoter¸ with markedly higher rates of antisocial<br />

behavior found in male carriers of a low expressing allele (MAOA-L)<br />

who have been abused as children. I will present neuroimaging evidence<br />

that the MAOA-L allele is associated with profound alterations in the structure<br />

and function of, and connectivity between, key neural nodes for affect<br />

processing, emotion regulation and social evaluation. This “socio-affective<br />

scaffold” – comprised of amygdala, rostral cingulate, and medial prefrontal<br />

cortex – appears to be uniquely vulnerable to the effect of elevated serotonin<br />

levels during development, as other putative genetic risk factors for<br />

violence are also linked to an ontogenic excess of serotonin. I will outline a<br />

model whereby genetic predisposition to aggression – by altering structure<br />

and function within the socio-affective scaffold – amplifies the impact of<br />

early adverse life experience, creating stable sociocognitive biases which,<br />

in turn, lead to impulsive aggressive behavior. Finally, I will detail potential<br />

epigenetic mechanisms through which early adverse life experience might<br />

interact with genetic variation in MAOA to bring about the development of<br />

adult impulsive violence.<br />

39


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-15<br />

40<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

ag g r e s s i o n, s e r o t o n i n , a n D g e n e X e n v i -<br />

r o n m e n t i n t e r a c t i o n s<br />

Suomi, Stephen J.<br />

Laboratory of Comparative Ethology, the National Institute<br />

of Child Health & Human Development (NICHD), National<br />

Institutes of Health (NIH), Bethesda, Maryland<br />

Recent research has disclosed marked individual differences in patterns<br />

of biobehavioral development exhibited by rhesus monkeys across the life<br />

span. For example, approximately 5-10% of rhesus monkeys growing up in<br />

the wild consistently exhibit impulsive and/or inappropriately aggressive responses<br />

to mildly stressful situations throughout development; those same<br />

individuals also show chronic deficits in their central serotonin metabolism.<br />

These characteristic patterns of biobehavioral response emerge early in life<br />

and remain remarkably stable from infancy to adulthood. Laboratory studies<br />

have demonstrated that although these characteristics are highly heritable,<br />

they are also subject to major modification by specific early experiences,<br />

particularly those involving early social attachment relationships.<br />

For example, a specific polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene<br />

is associated with deficits in serotonin metabolism, extreme aggression,<br />

and excessive alcohol consumption among monkeys who have experienced<br />

insecure early attachment relationships but not in monkeys who have developed<br />

secure attachment relationships with their mothers during infancy<br />

(“maternal buffering”). Moreover, because the attachment style of a monkey<br />

mother is typically “copied” by her daughters when they grow up and<br />

become mothers themselves, similar buffering is likely to occur for the next<br />

generation of infants carrying that specific polymorphism.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-16<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong>s for Speakers<br />

ne u r o n a l p l a s t i c i t y, h o m e o s t a s i s a n D<br />

v i o l e n c e<br />

Ansermet, François1 ; Magistretti, Pierre2 1Service of psychiatry of children and adolescents Geneva<br />

University Hospital<br />

2Laboratory of Neuroenergetics and Cellular Dynamics,<br />

Brain Mind Institute, <strong>EPFL</strong><br />

In this presentation, we will address the issue of violence through a theoretical<br />

model in which experience, traces left by experience through the mechanisms<br />

of neuronal plasticity and the associated somatic states are dynamically<br />

connected. Another related issue will be the notion of homeostasis and<br />

in particular the homeostatic function of the trace left by experience. Thus,<br />

as we previously discussed in our writings (e.g. Biology of Freedom, Other<br />

Press, NY, 2007) we propose a model through which the subject constitutes<br />

from experience an internal reality both conscious and unconscious. This<br />

internal reality associates somatic states (S) with representations (R). The<br />

prototypical model for the homeostatic role of the trace is the experience<br />

of satisfaction during the perinatal stages. Here the newborn is exposed<br />

not only to a new environment after leaving the homeostatic intra-uterine<br />

condition but also to a considerable set of stimulations originating from his/<br />

her body, through the interoceptive system which generates a considerable<br />

perturbation of the homeostasis. The newborn can only re-establish<br />

the homeostatic status through the intervention of the Other. This process<br />

generates a set of traces which inscribes this homeostatic experience in the<br />

neuronal network.<br />

It appears justified to consider that most of these interoceptive stimulations<br />

are associated with unpleasure which cannot be related to meaningful<br />

representations. In other words, the newborn is passive in front of these internally-derived<br />

unpleasant sensations which leave traces of somatic states<br />

(S) that cannot be related with meaningful representations (R). Thus, one<br />

can view this condition of the newborn as a state of chronic stress. With<br />

the intervention of the Other, these internal states which are enigmatic to<br />

the subject as they are not associated with a representation, progressively<br />

become meaningful. Thus the somatic states (S) become buffered by the<br />

representations (R) through a process generated by the action of the Other.<br />

If this action of the Other does not take place, for example as consequence<br />

of maternal deprivation, the homeostatic function of the trace does not occur.<br />

This sustains an absence of association between somatic states (S) and<br />

representations (R) leading to a state of chronic stress which requires an<br />

action to create a representation (R). We posit that violence, by generating<br />

a response from the external environment following the violent action of the<br />

subject, has a paradoxical homeostatic function, as under these conditions<br />

the somatic states become associated with a “meaningful” representation.<br />

Thus, a chronic state of stress due to the absence of association between<br />

somatic states and meaningful representations due to the absence of response<br />

or interactions with the Other at the early stages of life, generates a<br />

need for a discharge which can take place as a violent act.<br />

41


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-17<br />

42<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

ge n e t i c a n D e a r l y e n v i r o n m e n t a l f a c t o r s<br />

s h a p i n g a D u l t m a l e a g g r e s s i o n<br />

Neumann, Inga D.<br />

Department of Behavioural and Molecular Neuroendocrinology<br />

University of Regensburg, Germany<br />

Aggression constitutes a central problem in our societies and is also linked<br />

to several psychopathologies, including stress-related affective disorders.<br />

Moreover, adverse early life experiences like emotional neglect and abuse,<br />

are established risk factors not only for the development of anxiety- and depression-related<br />

disorders, but also of abnormal or excessive forms of adult<br />

aggression. However, comparable animal studies on aggression are lacking.<br />

Here, we present animal models to study the influence of the genetic background<br />

for either high (HAB) or low (LAB) anxiety-related behaviour, as well<br />

the influence of early life stress on adult intermale aggression.<br />

First, we analyzed the aggression behaviour of HAB and LAB male Wistar<br />

rats during a 10-min resident-intruder (RI) test. LAB males displayed more<br />

aggressive behaviour than HAB or non-selected NAB males, which was reflected<br />

by more attacks and a shorter attack latency, and accompanied by<br />

a more pronounced plasma ACTH response, differential neuronal activation<br />

within relevant brain regions (PVN, septum), and a decrease in lateral septal<br />

vasopressin release as measured by intracerebral microdialysis (1,2).<br />

Moreover, LAB males show abnormal aggression towards a narcotized male,<br />

and a female intruder, respectively.<br />

Moreover, we analyzed the effects of maternal separation (MS) on juvenile<br />

play-fight behaviour as well as on adult intermale aggression. Juvenile MS<br />

rats showed a significant increase in offensive play-fighting (more nape<br />

attacks and vigorous fur pulling, less supine postures), and an increase in<br />

adult aggression during the RI test (more lateral threat, keep down and offensive<br />

upright). In order to understand possible underlying neurobiological<br />

mechanisms, we quantified vasopressin mRNA expression, as the brain<br />

vasopressin system is an important modulator of complex social behaviors<br />

including aggression. Interestingly, an increased vasopressin mRNA expression<br />

was found in the PVN of juvenile and adult MS rats (3).<br />

In order to reveal genetic X environmental interaction, HAB and LAB rats<br />

were exposed to MS. Quantification of adult intermale aggression revealed<br />

differential effects of MS depending on the genetic background thus confirming<br />

recent results in the context of anxiety- and stress-related adult<br />

parameters in early life-stressed HAB and LAB rats (4,5).<br />

Thus, the use and establishment of relevant animal models will enable us to<br />

reveal the genetic and early environmental factors which shape adult male<br />

aggression and underlying neuronal mechanisms.<br />

Supported by BMBF and DFG (IDN)


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-18<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong>s for Speakers<br />

ag g r e s s i o n a n D vi o l e n c e in a n i m a l s; r o l e<br />

o f p e r s o n a l i t y f a c t o r s<br />

Koolhaas, Jaap M.<br />

Behavioral Physiology, University of Groningen,<br />

The Netherlands<br />

While most animal studies of aggression concern aggressive behavior as a functional<br />

and adaptive form of social behavior, studies in humans often focus on<br />

impulsive aggression and violence as maladaptive forms of aggressive behavior.<br />

Violence can be defined as a form of aggression that is both out of control and out<br />

of context and lacks its social communicative function. Little is known about the<br />

factors involved in the change from adaptive aggression into violence. A number of<br />

animal experiments will be discussed aimed at the interaction between individual<br />

predisposition and previous social experience in the development of violence.<br />

Our research on feral (wild-derived rodents) shows that offensive aggression is<br />

more generally related to coping with environmental challenges. Highly aggressive<br />

males adopt a proactive coping style, whereas low levels of aggression reflect<br />

a more passive or reactive coping style. These divergent coping styles have now<br />

been observed in a wide variety of species and determine adaptive capacity and<br />

vulnerability to stress related pathologies, including disorders characterized by<br />

outbursts of intense aggression and violence. Recent experiments show that the<br />

proactive coping style is characterised by a high propensity to develop violence.<br />

The underlying factor in the transition of adaptive aggression into violence seems<br />

to be the gradual reduction of inhibitory control of aggression in the course of repeated<br />

winning experience. For example, an extensive sequential analysis of the<br />

pre-attack behavior of two interacting males shows that the aggressive behavior<br />

of the proactive animal becomes more and more independent of the opponent<br />

after repeated winning. Experienced, highly aggressive males no longer respond<br />

to social signals from the opponent and develop escalated, indiscriminating and<br />

injurious forms of offensive aggression.<br />

Our animal models allow the identification of underlying physiological and neurochemical<br />

systems involved in the transition from aggression to violence. The<br />

presentation will focus on brain serotonin (5-HT) as the major orchestrator of<br />

aggression in many species including humans. However, the current view of the<br />

role of 5-HT in aggression is too simple. For example, the paradox between the<br />

serotonin deficiency hypothesis of aggression and the inhibitory effects of 5-HT on<br />

aggression clearly needs a solution. Recent studies on the 5-HT system before,<br />

during and after normal adaptive and pathological forms of aggression emphasize<br />

the importance of a clear distinction between aggression and violence. Offensive<br />

aggression, aimed at territorial control and social dominance is positively related<br />

to 5-HT neuronal activity. The violent form of aggression is associated with a tonic,<br />

trait-like reduction in 5-HT activity. Experiments in male rats and mice show that<br />

this reduction is due to an excessive serotonergic auto-inhibition predominantly<br />

mediated via an enhanced sensitivity of the somatodendritic 5-HT1A auto-receptors.<br />

Future research will address the question how this dynamic change in the<br />

tonic inhibitory control of the 5-HT neuron in the course of the development of<br />

violent trait characteristics may affect the activity of the neuron during the violent<br />

act itself.<br />

43


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-19<br />

44<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

st r e s s a s a g e n e r a t o r o f s o c i a l i m b a l -<br />

a n c e<br />

Sandi, Carmen; Cordero, M. Isabel; Marquez, Cristina;<br />

Timmer, Marjan<br />

Laboratory of Behavioral Genetics Brain Mind Institute<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong>, Switzerland<br />

Stress is frequently regarded as an inevitable drawback of many hierarchical<br />

arrangements of social groups. Most notably, subordinate individuals<br />

have frequently been shown to display clear signs of chronic stress (including<br />

weight loss, increased levels of glucocorticoid hormones, deficiencies in<br />

the immune system, and even increased death rate). Given that high stress<br />

and increasing stress hormone levels are linked to both physical and mental<br />

health problems, stress has been proposed as a key mediator of the wealth<br />

of health problems resulting from social inequity.<br />

Despite substantial evidence indicating that, on its turn, stress can have<br />

a major impact on aspects of social dominance –such as aggressiveness,<br />

success in competition for resources and social anxiety– the contribution<br />

of stress to creating social imbalance is still largely disregarded in psychosocio-biological<br />

contexts. This talk will present evidence from animal work<br />

showing that stress affects social hierarchies in different aspects, and potentiates<br />

aggressions among affected individuals, with all these examples illustrating<br />

how, eventually, stress has a profound impact not only in the directly<br />

stressed individuals but also in their interacting partners and offspring. The<br />

first part of the talk will deal with a model whereby being stressed at the<br />

time of a first social encounter (already established hierarchies are largely<br />

immune to disruption by stress) can have major consequences on the longterm<br />

establishment of the resulting social hierarchy, with stress acting as a<br />

two-stage mechanism: (i) by influencing the rank achieved after the social<br />

encounter; and (ii) by having a long-term impact on the subsequent status<br />

of the individual in future encounters with either the same (by promoting a<br />

long-term memory for the specific hierarchy) or with other (winner-loser effects)<br />

individual/s. The second part of the talk will discuss how stress during<br />

early development can affect individuals’ aggressiveness later on life and,<br />

on its turn, life quality and health in their social environments.<br />

Identifying stress among the critical mechanisms contributing to create social<br />

imbalance widens the potential interventions to reduce psychosocial<br />

and societal problems due to uneven status.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-20<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong>s for Speakers<br />

gr o u p s , by s t a n D e r s a n D t h e in f o r m a l<br />

re g u l a t i o n o f vi o l e n c e<br />

Levine, Mark<br />

Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, UKA<br />

Ever since Nisbett and Wilson’s (1977) essay, “Telling More Than We Can<br />

Know”, social psychologists have been sensitised to the fact that people<br />

have very little introspective access to the underlying causes of their own<br />

behaviour. In other words, what our participants tell us in questionnaires<br />

and interviews and focus groups should not be treated as a veridical route<br />

to psychological reality. This raises some important questions about methods<br />

and the conclusions we draw from them. For example, in the study of<br />

helping behaviour, there is often a gap between what people say and what<br />

they do in real-life helping situations. Thus, reactive and non-reactive measures<br />

need to be combined in order to develop a more complete picture of<br />

the likelihood of helping.<br />

This strategy of combining verbal reports and behavioural studies works<br />

well, as long as it is possible to combine both levels of analysis. However,<br />

there are some domains in which it is difficult, for ethical and practical reasons,<br />

to carry out robust behavioural research. The study of violence, and in<br />

particular, bystander intervention in violence, is one such domain. There are<br />

many ethical challenges to be overcome in attempting to expose research<br />

participants to violent events, and many logistical difficulties in (re)creating<br />

violent events that can be used and manipulated in robust empirical studies.<br />

In this paper I will suggest that, one consequence of this has been a rather<br />

partial view of the role of groups in social psychological explanations of<br />

violence. For the most part, in traditional social psychology, groups tend to<br />

be seen as psychologically dangerous. On the one hand, the presence of<br />

others can lead to ‘peer-group pressure’, ‘mob violence’ or mass hysteria<br />

– all threats to individual rationality which are likely to produce anti-social<br />

behaviour. On the other hand, the presence of others can lead to ‘diffusion<br />

of responsibility’, ‘pluralistic ignorance’ and ‘bystander apathy’ – threats to<br />

individual rationality that are likely to prevent people from helping those<br />

under threat of violent attack. Thus, groups are damned for causing ‘excitation’<br />

and damned for causing ‘inhibition’ – with both paths deriving from the<br />

same set of circumstances.<br />

I will argue that this idea of the danger of the group misunderstands the<br />

role of group processes in violence. I will demonstrate, using data from<br />

experiments exploring bystander behaviour in emergencies, that groups<br />

can facilitate pro-social behaviour. I will then go on to describe a detailed<br />

behavioural analysis of a corpus of 42 clips of CCTV footage<br />

45


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-20<br />

46<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

gr o u p s , by s t a n D e r s a n D t h e in f o r m a l<br />

re g u l a t i o n o f vi o l e n c e (co n t .)<br />

Levine, Mark<br />

Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, UKA<br />

of violent incidents. I will show that an analysis of the behaviours of perpetrators,<br />

targets and bystanders to real life violence reveals that, as group<br />

size increases, more pro-social than anti-social acts occur. Moreover, I will<br />

show that there is an implicit pattern to the sequences in the CCTV clips<br />

such that violent and non-violent episodes are distinguished by what happens<br />

on the ‘third bystander act’. I will argue that the violence in these<br />

episodes cannot be understood with reference to the perpetrator, or the<br />

victim, or even the relationship between perpetrator and victim. Instead<br />

I will suggest that violence (and the informal control of potentially violent<br />

episodes) needs to be understood as a group level ‘choreography’. It is what<br />

the bystanders do – or do not do – that determines the trajectory of the<br />

violence.<br />

I will conclude by arguing that these findings would not be available from<br />

methods that rely on participant accounts or other forms of reactive data<br />

collection. To that end I will speculate on how conducting experiments using<br />

violence in Virtual Environments can create the empirical conditions<br />

required to test some of the important outstanding questions about the role<br />

of group processes in understanding violence.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-21<br />

vi r t u a l re a l i t y in th e st u D y of<br />

eX t r e m e si t u a t i o n s<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong>s for Speakers<br />

Slater, Mel<br />

Virtual Environments Department of Computer Science<br />

University College London, UK<br />

When people experience situations and events in an immersive virtual reality,<br />

they typically respond as if these were really happening, even though<br />

they know for sure that nothing real is taking place. This power of virtual<br />

reality to induce response-as-if-real makes it a powerful tool in the investigation<br />

of how people would respond in dangerous and extreme situations<br />

– but avoiding the practical and ethical problems that would be involved in<br />

real exposure. In this talk we will introduce the main characteristics of a virtual<br />

reality system, and describe a number of experimental case studies on<br />

this theme. We will conclude with an discussion of the use of virtual reality<br />

in the study of violence.<br />

47


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-22<br />

48<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

wi n n e r s , l o s e r s a n D b y s t a n D e r s : t h e<br />

s t r u c t u r e o f D o m i n a n c e h i e r a r c h i e s<br />

Dugatkin, Lee Alan<br />

Department of Biology, University of Louisville, USA<br />

Dominance hierarchies are a fundamental part of the social fabric of animals<br />

that live in groups, and behavioral ecologists have sought to understand the<br />

dynamics and stability of such dominance hierarchies. I present a family of<br />

models that my colleagues and I have developed to examine how winner effects,<br />

loser effects and bystander effects impact the structure and stability<br />

of dominance hierarchies. We have found that when winner effects alone<br />

are in play, a strict linear hierarchy emerges in which all individuals hold an<br />

unambiguous rank and fights are common. When examining loser effects in<br />

the absence of winner effects, a clear top-ranked individual always emerges,<br />

but the rank of others in the group is unclear. In addition, when loser<br />

effects are in play, interactions are primarily of the form “attack-retreat,”<br />

wherein one animal opts to fight, but the other does not.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-23<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong>s for Speakers<br />

re-e X p o s u r e t o co m b a t in vi r t u a l re a li<br />

t y a s a tr e a t m e n t f o r ptsD<br />

Rizzo, Albert<br />

Institute for Creative Technologies University of Southern<br />

California, USA<br />

According to the DSM-IV [1], PTSD is caused by experiencing or witnessing<br />

an event involving actual or threatened death or serious injury, or a threat<br />

to the physical integrity of self or others. Such events may include military<br />

combat, violent personal assault, being kidnapped or taken hostage, rape,<br />

terrorist attack, torture, incarceration as a prisoner of war, natural or manmade<br />

disasters, automobile accidents, or being diagnosed with a life-threatening<br />

illness. The disorder also appears to be more severe and long lasting<br />

when the event is caused by human means and design (bombings, shootings,<br />

combat, etc.). Such incidents would be distressing to almost anyone,<br />

and are usually experienced with intense fear, horror, and helplessness. The<br />

essential feature of PTSD is the development of characteristic symptoms<br />

that may include intrusive thoughts, nightmares or flashbacks, avoidance of<br />

reminders of the traumatic event, emotional numbing, hyper-alertness, anger,<br />

isolation, anxiety, depression, substance abuse, survivor guilt, suicidal<br />

feelings and thoughts, negative self-image, memory impairment, problems<br />

with intimate relationships, emotional distance from family and others and<br />

denial of social problems. Symptoms of PTSD are often intensified when the<br />

person recalls or is exposed to stimulus cues that resemble or symbolize<br />

the original trauma in a non-therapeutic setting. Such uncontrolled cue exposure<br />

may lead the person to react with a survival mentality and mode of<br />

response that could put himself/herself and others at considerable risk.<br />

The violence of war is one of the most challenging environments that a<br />

human can experience. The cognitive, emotional and physical demands of<br />

a combat environment place enormous stress on even the best-prepared<br />

military personnel. Veterans exposed to combat are at increased risk of<br />

PTSD, with estimates of lifetime prevalence for PTSD at 26.9% and 30.9%<br />

for female and male veterans of the Vietnam War respectively, compared to<br />

a lifetime prevalence of 10.4% for women and 5% for men in the general<br />

population [2-4].<br />

The Iraq/Afghanistan combat environments, with its ubiquitous battlefronts,<br />

ambiguous enemy identification and repeated extended deployments, was<br />

anticipated to produce a significant number of returning American Service<br />

Members (SMs) with PTSD and other mental disorders. Recent studies are<br />

now confirming this expectation in that significant numbers of veterans of<br />

the current combat operations in Iraq and Afghanistan are in fact at risk for<br />

developing PTSD [5-8]. Hoge and colleagues [5] administered an anonymous<br />

survey to U.S. SMs before and after deployment to Iraq and Afghanistan.<br />

Results of the PTSD Checklist (PCL) showed that following deployment<br />

to Iraq, a significantly higher percentage of SMs screened positive for PTSD<br />

(between 18 and 19.9%) than Soldiers deployed to Afghanistan (11.5%)<br />

49


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-23<br />

50<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

re-e X p o s u r e t o co m b a t in vi r t u a l re a li<br />

t y a s a tr e a t m e n t f o r ptsD (co n t .)<br />

Rizzo, Albert<br />

Institute for Creative Technologies University of Southern<br />

California, USA<br />

or before deployment (9.4%). Furthermore, the prevalence of PTSD among<br />

SMs deployed to Iraq increased in a linear manner with the number of firefights<br />

experienced, such that PTSD rates were 4.5% for those involved in no<br />

firefights and increased to 19.3% for those involved in more than five firefights.<br />

Hoge et al., [6] in a more recent study analyzed the Post Deployment<br />

Health Assessment (PDHA) responses of over 300,000 SMs after deployment<br />

to Iraq and Afghanistan. Although the PDHA uses a different screening tool<br />

for PTSD, (the Primary Care PTSD screen; PC-PTSD), results were similar to<br />

previous research indicating that the PTSD rates for SMs deployed to Iraq<br />

were higher than the PTSD rates for SMs deployed to Afghanistan. Also,<br />

SMs who screened positive for any mental health concern were more likely<br />

to leave military service within a year after deployment, and their mental<br />

health concerns are likely to persist. Seal and colleagues [7], in a recent<br />

study of 103,788 OIF/OEF veterans seen at VA health facilities, reported<br />

that 31% received mental health/or psychological diagnoses and that 16%<br />

of veterans who sought treatment at VA facilities between September 2001<br />

and September 2005 met criteria for PTSD. Furthermore, these estimates<br />

were made before the violence escalated in the last 2 years, and the number<br />

of persons affected is likely to have grown in step with the intensity of<br />

the conflict. For example, VA statistics report that there were 19,356 OIF/<br />

OEF veterans receiving compensation for PTSD as of March 2006, while five<br />

months after in August 2006, 34,380 discharged soldiers from Iraq and Afghanistan<br />

were diagnosed by the VA with PTSD, of the 63,767 diagnosed<br />

with a mental disorder with current estimates suggesting approximately<br />

300,000 OIF/OEF Veterans at risk for developing PTSD [8]. These findings<br />

highlight the need and potential value in addressing this problem early, intensively<br />

and with the best non-stigmatizing treatment tools available for<br />

combat veterans still in service as well as upon discharge.<br />

Among the many approaches that have been used to treat PTSD, exposure<br />

therapy appears to have the best-documented therapeutic efficacy. Such<br />

treatment typically involves the graded and repeated imaginal reliving of<br />

the traumatic event within the therapeutic setting and is believed to provide<br />

a low-threat context where the patient can begin to therapeutically process<br />

trauma-relevant emotions as well as de-condition the learning cycle of the<br />

disorder via a habituation/extinction process. While the efficacy of imaginal<br />

exposure has been established in multiple studies with diverse trauma populations,<br />

many patients are unwilling or unable to effectively visualize the<br />

traumatic event. To address this problem, researchers have recently turned<br />

to the use of Virtual Reality (VR) to deliver exposure therapy by immersing<br />

patients in simulations of trauma-relevant environments that allow


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

S-23<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong>s for Speakers<br />

re-e X p o s u r e t o co m b a t in vi r t u a l re a li<br />

t y a s a tr e a t m e n t f o r ptsD (co n t .)<br />

Rizzo, Albert<br />

Institute for Creative Technologies University of Southern<br />

California, USA<br />

for precise control of stimulus conditions.<br />

Since 2004, the University of Southern California Institute for Creative<br />

Technologies has spearheaded an extensive scientific program for the development<br />

and evaluation of a novel Virtual Reality system for exposure<br />

therapy for PTSD. The initial prototype version of this system (Virtual Iraq/<br />

Afghanistan) has undergone successful clinical tests via an open clinical trial<br />

at the Naval Medical Center-San Diego and Camp Pendleton and a randomized<br />

clinical trial is underway at Ft. Lewis, WA. Direct clinical application and<br />

other comparative clinical trials are ongoing at approximately 20+ sites.<br />

This talk will present an overview of PTSD exposure therapy, a description<br />

of VR and the rationale for how this technology has been applied as a tool to<br />

deliver exposure therapy along with a brief review of current research. We<br />

then provide a description of the Virtual Iraq/Afghanistan exposure therapy<br />

system and treatment protocol and present initial results from an open<br />

clinical trial with active duty military personnel. The talk will conclude with<br />

a summary of future directions where VR technology can be applied to more<br />

comprehensively address a range of issues relevant to the emotional consequences<br />

of the violence of war. Background and current results from this<br />

project can be found in Rizzo et al., [9-11].<br />

51


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

52<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong>


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

<strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

Part 2: Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

53


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-1<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

es t i m a t i o n o f a s t r a t e g y o f n e g o t i a t i o n -<br />

s u b m i s s i o n b y u r b a n a r e a o f a D o l e c e n t s<br />

w i t h a h i g h p r e s e n c e o f criminal<br />

o r g a n i z a t i o n .<br />

Baralla, Francesca 1 ; Tramontano, Carlo 1 ; Giannini, Anna<br />

Maria 1 ; Sgalla, Roberto 2 ; Marchetti, Marco 3<br />

1 Sapienza, University of Rome; 2 Italian Ministry of Interior, Department<br />

of Public Security; 3 University of Molise<br />

The reconciliation hypothesis predicts that individuals try to “undo” the social<br />

damage inflicted by aggression (de Waal, 2000). Inside the most wide<br />

research on legality of young context in south Italy regions with a strong<br />

presence of criminal organization (Giannini 2008), it has been wanted<br />

to control how a negotiation-submission behaviour has been lived from<br />

a group of 1988 high school adolescents (51% male, 49% female, average<br />

age 16.43). A scenario has been shown in which presents a situation<br />

where a group of young people approaches in a threatening way a parked<br />

car; in the car there is a boy and a girl, and the group of young people<br />

warns the boys that it is dangerous to stop in that place without an adequate<br />

protection; the boy in the car, in answer to this, takes the wallets<br />

and hands them over to the group.<br />

Only 8% of the sample reserved acceptable this situation, considering that<br />

the law was not violated and affirms besides that it could present with a<br />

certain frequency in the place where they live. Such group could be characterized<br />

as more sensible to the logical deviation, even in virtue of the<br />

social pressure (Sutherland, 1934). Besides, between these subjects, 41%<br />

however affirms that considering probable that the victims of the aggressive<br />

interaction would return home to find friends and go back to take<br />

one’s revenge. In areas where the presence of social pressure to accept a<br />

deviate behaviour could be very strong, the negotiation-submission could<br />

be seen, superficially, as an implicit approval to a logical deviant. For a<br />

better comprehension as such a complex phenomenon, it could be useful<br />

to make a careful study of the dynamics at the base of the process of affiliation,<br />

as well as innate bases of our answers to aggression.<br />

55


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

56<br />

P-2<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

in D i v i D u a l D i f f e r e n c e s in a g g r e s s i o n<br />

m o D u l a t e e a r l y f r o n t a l r e s p o n s e t o<br />

a g g r e s s i v e f a c i a l D i s p l a y s .<br />

Bediou Benoit, 1 3 ; Eimer, Martin 2 ; Calder, Andrew J. 1<br />

1 Medical Research Council - Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit,<br />

15 Chaucer road, CB2 7EF Cambridge, UK.; 2 School of Psychology,<br />

Birkbeck College, University of London, Malet Street, London<br />

WC1E 7HX, UK; 3 Swiss Center for Affective Sciences CISA – University<br />

of Geneva, Rue des Battoirs, 7, CH 1205 Geneva, SWIT-<br />

ZERLAND<br />

Converging evidence from human and animal research suggest that aggressive<br />

behaviour arises from a dysfunction in the neural circuitry of<br />

emotion-regulation, especially in the connection between the prefrontal<br />

cortex and the amygdala. Both clinically aggressive individuals and healthy<br />

volunteers with elevated risk for aggression show increased amygdala and<br />

decreased prefrontal responses to facial signals of aggression. Individual<br />

differences in reward-drive have been associated with increased attention<br />

toward facial signals of aggression, heightened experience of anger and<br />

vulnerability to display aggressive behaviour.<br />

Here, we investigated the temporal dynamics of the interaction between<br />

an aggression-related personality trait (‘reward-drive’—the drive to gain<br />

reward, Carver &White, 1994) and the neural response to angry faces.<br />

We recorded high-density (64-electrodes) Event Related brain Potentials<br />

(ERPs) in subjects scoring high (N=12) and low (N=12) on the rewarddrive<br />

scale while they viewed angry, sad and neutral faces.<br />

Replicating previous findings, ERP results showed an enhanced frontal and<br />

frontocentral positivity in response to angry and sad faces compared with<br />

neutral faces from 130 to 200ms post stimulus. Crucially, reward-drive selectively<br />

modulated the midline frontocentral response to aggressive facial<br />

displays within 200-300ms after stimulus onset—compared to low rewarddrive<br />

subjects, individuals scoring high on the reward-drive scale showed<br />

a reduced midline frontocentral positivity for angry faces relative to neutral<br />

and sad expressions.<br />

Results are consistent with recent fMRI studies showing reduced ventromedial<br />

prefrontal response to angry faces relative to neutral and sad expressions<br />

in subjects with heightened risk for aggression (see Calder et al<br />

2008). Moreover, the temporal properties of this modulation are consistent<br />

with the idea of a reduced functional coupling from the vmPFC to the<br />

amygdala (but not vice versa) in subjects scoring high on reward-drive<br />

(see Passamonti et al 2008).


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-3<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

Do st r e s s a n D ag g r e s s i o n in f l u e n c e o u r<br />

pe r c e p t i o n o f ot h e r s ?<br />

Bertsch, Katja 1 ; Khan, Robina 1 ; Kruk, Menno 2 ; Naumann,<br />

Ewald 1<br />

1 Department of Psychology, University of Trier, Germany; 2 Department<br />

of Medical Pharmacology, Leiden University, Netherlands<br />

For humans emotional facial expressions are the most immediate and<br />

salient cues to the state of mind of other individuals. The results of animal<br />

and clinical studies suggest a strong relationship between stress, the<br />

processing of conflict-relevant signals, and aggressive behavior. In a recent<br />

EEG experiment, we were able to confirm this and could show that<br />

experimentally provoked aggression has an impact on the processing of<br />

emotional facial expressions even in healthy individuals. In a second EEG<br />

study, we investigated the influence of stress and aggression on the processing<br />

of emotional facial expressions. Therefore, we administered either<br />

20 mg of hydrocortisone (stress group) or a placebo (placebo group) to<br />

healthy participants and provoked half of them with the Taylor Aggression<br />

Paradigm. Thereafter, all participants performed an emotional Stroop task<br />

with emotional facial expressions (happy, neutral, angry, fearful). The preliminary<br />

results indicate an influence of stress (hydrocortisone), aggressive<br />

behavior, as well as the interaction of stress and aggression on the<br />

processing of the emotional faces. These results are in line with the findings<br />

from animal and clinical studies and show a strong relationship between<br />

stress, aggression, and the processing of conflict-relevant signals.<br />

This is a further indication for a vicious circle between stress, aggression,<br />

and information processing which might explain the elicitation and escalation<br />

of aggressive behavior.<br />

57


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

58<br />

P-4<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

as s o c i a t i o n b e t w e e n e X p e r i e n c e o f<br />

a g g r e s s i o n a n D a n X i e t y in m a l e m i c e :<br />

ef f e c t s o f D i a z e p a m a n D b u s p i r o n e<br />

Bondar, Natalia 1 ; Kudryavtseva, Natalia 1<br />

1 Neurogenetics of Social Behavior Sector, Institute of Cytology<br />

and Genetics SD RAS, Novosibirsk, Russia<br />

The sensory contact technique increases aggressiveness in male mice and<br />

allows an aggressive type of behavior to be formed as a result of repeated<br />

experience of social victories in daily agonistic interactions. In the low aggressive<br />

and high emotion mice of CBA/Lac strain, repeated positive fighting<br />

experience leads to increased plus maze anxiety in the winners after<br />

10 days of aggression experience and much more after 20 days. Aggressive<br />

motivation in the winners was significantly increased as revealed by<br />

parameters of partition test measuring behavioral reactivity to other conspecifics.<br />

Thus, anxiety as a consequence of repeated experience of aggression<br />

associates with the increase of aggressive motivation in CBA/Lac<br />

mice. Repeated experience of aggression is accompanied by the increase<br />

of anxiety and decrease of aggressive motivation in 20 days-winners of<br />

high aggressive C57BL/6J strain. It was concluded, that 1. Repeated experience<br />

of aggression provokes the development of anxiety in male mice.<br />

2. The level of anxiety as well as it’s behavioral realization depends on the<br />

duration of aggressive experience and genetic strain.<br />

Anxiolytics buspirone (1 mg/kg, i.p) and diazepam (0.5 mg/kg, i.p) induced<br />

anxiogenic effect and reduced aggression in the winners with 3<br />

days experience of aggression. No significant effects of buspirone on aggressive<br />

and anxious behaviors were found in mice with 20 days experience<br />

of aggression. In this group of winners diazepam produced anxiolytic<br />

and antiaggressive effects. Opioid receptor antagonist naltrexone (1 mg/<br />

kg, i.p.) also had different effects in the winners with long and short experience<br />

of aggression. It was hypothesized that previous aggression experience<br />

modified animal sensitivity to drug treatment.<br />

This work was supported by grant No. 07-04-00014 from the Russian<br />

Foundation for Basic Research and grant MK-2109.2007.4 from Russian<br />

Federation President in support of young scientists.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-5<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

tr a n s D u c e r o f r e g u l a t e D creb a c t i v i t y<br />

1 (torc1) : a r o l e in r e w a r D-r e l a t e D<br />

l e a r n i n g a n D m e m o r y a n D b e h a v i o r a l<br />

r e s p o n s e s t o e m o t i o n a l s t i m u l i ?<br />

Breuillaud, Lionel1 ; Pierre J., Magistretti,1 Olivier, Halfon2 ;<br />

1 2<br />

Jean-René, Cardinaux<br />

1 Center for Psychiatric Neuroscience; 2 Service of Child and Ado-<br />

lescent Psychiatry<br />

The transcription factor cAMP-responsive element binding protein (CREB)<br />

has been shown to be essential for the generation of long lasting forms of<br />

synaptic plasticity that are associated with learning and memory and the<br />

gating of emotional responses in the CNS. TORC1 is a newly discovered<br />

CREB coactivator that strongly enhances its transcriptional activity. We<br />

have shown that TORC1 functions as a calcium- and cAMP-sensitive coincidence<br />

detector in neurons and that it is involved in the maintenance of<br />

late-phase long-term-potentiation (L-LTP) in the rat hippocampus (Kovács<br />

et al., 2007. PNAS 104, 4700-4705). Given the importance of synaptic<br />

integration of dopamine- and glutamate-mediated signals in rewardrelated<br />

learning and memory mechanisms, we decided to study the role<br />

of TORC1 in this context. To this end we have produced a mouse with a<br />

deficient Torc1 gene using a gene trap approach. We have obtained viable<br />

heterozygous and TORC1-null mice that we are currently phenotyping. We<br />

are focusing on a putative alteration in the acquisition of cocaine addiction<br />

(cocaine-conditioned place preference and cocaine self-administration paradigms)<br />

and a general characterization of their behavior (memory, depression<br />

and anxiety). The study of this mouse line could make a contribution<br />

to the understanding of the molecular basis of neuropsychiatric conditions.<br />

59


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

60<br />

P-6<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

se r o t o n e r g i c a u t o r e g u l a t o r y m e c h a n i s m s<br />

in v i o l e n t m i c e<br />

Caramaschi, Doretta 1 3 Natarajan, Deepa 1 3 ; de Weerd,<br />

Henk 2 ; van der Want, Han 2 ; Mulder, Kees C. 1 ; de Boer, Sietse<br />

F. 1 ; Koolhaas, Jaap M. 1<br />

1 Biologisch Centrum, Dept. Behavioral Physiology, University of<br />

Groningen, Haren, The Netherlands; 2 University Medical Center<br />

Groningen, Molecular Imaging and Electron Microscopy, Dept.<br />

Cell Biology, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands;<br />

3 These authors contributed equally<br />

Human violence has been traditionally linked to a lowered functionality<br />

of the central serotonin (5-HT) system. Despite the vast accumulation<br />

of data linking reduced brain 5-HT activity to pathological aggressive<br />

behavior, information about the possible mechanisms underlying such a<br />

decreased 5-HT functioning is virtually absent. A major obstacle in this<br />

respect is the lack of relevant animal models of violence, and in particular<br />

the omission of objective criteria for assessing violence in preclinical studies.<br />

We recently established, using a rigorous ethological methodology,<br />

that feral mice originally genetically selected for Short Attack Latency (SAL<br />

line) show distinct characteristics of pathological aggression comparable to<br />

human violence.<br />

Employing this mouse model, we have investigated extensively the serotonergic<br />

system, focusing in particular on the autoregulatory control<br />

mechanisms.<br />

First, in line with the 5-HT deficiency hypothesis of aggression, we found<br />

that the pugnacious SAL mice have lower frontolimbic 5-HT levels than<br />

their docile Long Attack Latency (LAL) counterparts. However, this difference<br />

only appeared after the animals had acquired experience with repeated<br />

social conflicts and was not visible beforehand when animals were<br />

naïve with resident-intruder testing. This difference in 5-HT level seems<br />

not to be due to the activity of either the 5-HT-synthesis limiting enzyme<br />

tryptophan-hydroxylase or the main 5-HT-degrading enzyme monoamineoxidase.<br />

Rather, SAL mice showed less functional 5-HTT activity (serotonin<br />

transporter), regardless of the exposure to social experience. Moreover,<br />

SAL mice showed enhanced 5-HT1A autoreceptor and heteroreceptor functionality,<br />

which seems to correlate to the social experience and the escalation<br />

of aggression levels.<br />

These data suggest that the combination of a constitutionally low 5-HT<br />

reuptake functionality and (experience-driven) high 5-HT autoinhibitory<br />

activity leads to disrupted aggression regulation and accompanying lower<br />

levels of 5-HT.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-7<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

lo n g -t e r m e f f e c t s o f p e r i-p u b e r t y s t r e s s<br />

o n a g g r e s s i v e b e h a v i o u r in f e m a l e r a t s<br />

Cordero, Maria Isabel 1 ; Sandi, Carmen 1<br />

1 Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzer-<br />

land<br />

Experiencing adverse events during adolescence enhances the risk of<br />

developing different psychopathologies and violent behaviour in humans<br />

and animals. Previously, our group has reported that repeated exposure<br />

to stress during the peripuberty period increases pathological aggression<br />

in male rats. However, very little is known about the effects of stress on<br />

aggression in females. Although women are usually less violent than men,<br />

available evidence suggests soaring levels of aggression and serious violence<br />

among young women, and some studies have found a positive correlation<br />

with stress events experienced during development. For example,<br />

findings from the Rochester Youth Development Study suggest that growing<br />

up in disorganized, violent neighbourhoods is a risk factor for gang<br />

involvement among young women (Thornberry, 1997). In this work, we<br />

studied the long term effects of peripubertal stress on aggressive behaviour<br />

in female rats. For this purpose, female rats were stressed during the<br />

peri-pubertal period and their aggressive behaviour assessed at 3-months<br />

of age using the Resident-Intruder paradigm twice, once during diestrus<br />

cycle and 1 week after, during estrus. Similar to previous findings in male<br />

rats, peripuberty stressed females showed not only an increase in aggressive<br />

behaviour compared to control rats but an ‘abnormal’ aggressive<br />

behaviour, such as continual attacking of intruders displaying submissive<br />

behaviour. Furthermore, the differences found during diestrus were maintained<br />

during the estrus cycle, though female rats usually do not show<br />

aggressive behaviour during this phase of the menstrual cycle. Our results<br />

suggest that adolescence is a highly vulnerable period to stress induced<br />

aggression, in females as well as in males, and strongly suggests that<br />

social-aggression prevention campaigns should be addressed to both boys<br />

and girls.<br />

Thornberry, TP (1997) Membership in youth gangs and involvement in serious<br />

and violent offending. In R. Loeber and D.P. Farrington (Eds.), Serious<br />

and Violent Juvenile Offenders: Risk Factors and Successful Interventions<br />

(pp.147-166). Thousand Oaks, CA:Sage Publications.<br />

61


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

62<br />

P-8<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

impulsivity, aggression anD<br />

acaDemic performance in<br />

chilDren<br />

Cosi, Sandra 1 ; Vigil-Colet, Andreu 1 ; Canals, Josepa 1<br />

1 Universitat Rovira i Virgili, Tarragona, Spain.<br />

Different studies have shown that impulsivity is related to aggression<br />

and poor scholastic achievement in adolescents (Colom et al., 2007; Vigil-<br />

Colet &Morales-Vives, 2006). The present study intends to test<br />

this pattern of relationships in children. With this purpose we<br />

developed two questionnaires, a children’s version of Barrat’s<br />

impulsivity scale (Cosi et al., 2008) and a proactive-reactive<br />

aggression questionnaire reported by teachers (Cosi et al., in press)<br />

which were administered to 267 children aged between 9 and 12 years.<br />

Furthermore teachers rated the academic performance of children.<br />

Proactive and reactive scales showed a negative relationship with<br />

academic performance, a positive relationship with the Motor<br />

impulsiveness and non-planning scales of BIS-c and a negative<br />

relationship with the cognitive impulsivity scale, being these<br />

relationships greater for boys than for girls. When impulsivity scores<br />

where partialled out, only proactive aggression showed a significant<br />

relationship with academic performance, although its magnitude was<br />

lower.<br />

The results presented are similar to the ones founded in adolescent<br />

samples, indicating that impulsivity is related to aggression and,<br />

that the relationship between aggression and academic failure is<br />

mainly due to impulsivity. Furthermore it seems that these<br />

relationships are higher for boys than for girls, perhaps because<br />

their higher levels of aggression.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-9<br />

by s t a n D e r re s p o n s e s t o vi r t u a l<br />

vi o l e n c e<br />

Swapp, David 1 ; Rovira, Aitor 1 ; Slater, Mel 1<br />

1 University College London, London, UK<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

A significant impediment to the study of violent behaviour is the inability<br />

to recreate such situations in an experimental setting. Utilising a virtual<br />

environment, inhabited by virtual characters (avatars), enables the simulation<br />

of violent situations and therefore the ability to study the behaviour<br />

of experiment participants. This is especially the case since it has been<br />

shown that people do tend to respond realistically to situations and events<br />

that occur in an immersive virtual environment (IVE). In this abstract we<br />

report an ongoing study of people’s responses to simulated violence in an<br />

IVE, in particular concentrating on aspects of the bystander phenomenon<br />

moderated by in-group and out-group identification.<br />

The experiment uses an IVE system commonly known as a CAVE. This is<br />

a room in which three walls and the floor are screens upon which stereo<br />

imagery is projected, and which dynamically adjusts to the participant’s<br />

perspective as they move about the room. The advantage of such a setup<br />

is that it enables the participant to move naturally within the simulated<br />

environment, and thus results in natural behaviours such as proxemics in<br />

interactions with virtual characters.<br />

The scenario is a bar in which participants witness a violent confrontation<br />

involving two football fans of rival teams. Our purpose at this stage of the<br />

study is simply to explore the degree of realism of the response of participants<br />

to this violent behaviour, as measured by their tendency to try<br />

to intervene, and their physiological responses. To create the simulation,<br />

we used 3D motion capture equipment to record the performance of two<br />

actors; this capture was then used to animate the avatars in the scenario.<br />

The purpose of early trials is to better understand how to construct a virtual<br />

scenario that might lead to more or less realistic responses.<br />

63


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-10<br />

64<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

th e vicious c i r c l e t o w a r D s v i o l e n c e :<br />

fo c u s o n t h e n e g a t i v e f e e D b a c k<br />

m e c h a n i s m s o f b r a i n s e r o t o n i n<br />

n e u r o t r a n s m i s s i o n.<br />

de Boer, Sietse F. 1 ; Caramaschi, Doretta 1 ; Natarajan, Deepa<br />

1 ; Koolhaas, Jaap M. 1<br />

1 Department of Behavioral Physiology, University of<br />

Groningen,The Netherlands.<br />

Individuals differ widely in their propensity for violence during social conflict.<br />

While some individuals escalate aggression, others control their aggression<br />

or refrain from fighting. Our research on feral (wild-derived)<br />

rodents shows that offensive aggression is strongly related to coping with<br />

other challenges. Highly aggressive individuals adopt a proactive coping<br />

style, whereas low levels of aggression indicate a more passive or reactive<br />

style of coping. These divergent stress coping styles have now been<br />

identified in a range of species and can be considered important personality<br />

trait-like characteristics determining the individual adaptive capacity<br />

and hence vulnerability to stress-related pathologies, including mood<br />

and personality disorders characterized by outbursts of intense aggression<br />

and violence. Recent experiments show that violent characteristics<br />

can be engendered in proactively- but not reactively- coping individuals<br />

by permitting them repeatedly to dominate conspecifics during daily resident-intruder<br />

contests. Clearly, these high-aggressive phenotypes develop<br />

gradually, over the course of repeated victories, escalated (short-latency,<br />

high-frequency and –intensity attacks), persistent (lack of attack inhibition<br />

by defeat/submission signals from the victim), indiscriminating (attacking<br />

female and anesthetized male intruders) and injurious (enhanced vulnerable-body<br />

region attacks and wounding) forms of offensive aggression.<br />

This re-developed methodological approach highlights the rewarding and<br />

positive reinforcing nature of winning experiences that transform adaptive<br />

aggression into a violent pathological form resembling human aggression<br />

of clinical concern. This animal model allows us to identify the neurochemical<br />

changes that underlie such a transformation. We focus on brain serotonin<br />

(5-HT), as this well-conserved neurotransmitter system is the major<br />

molecular orchestrator of aggression in many species including humans.<br />

Based on the results of an extensive series of experiments, we provide<br />

evidence that excessive serotonergic auto-inhibition leading to reduced<br />

5-HT neurotransmission in several brain structures including frontal cortex<br />

paves the way to violent aggressive outbursts.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-11<br />

si z e m a t t e r s: in c r e a s e D g r e y m a t t e r<br />

in c h i l D r e n w i t h c o n D u c t p r o b l e m s a n D<br />

c a l l o u s-u n e m o t i o n a l t r a i t s<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

De Brito, Stéphane1 ; Mechelli, Andrea2 ; Wilke, Marko3 ;<br />

Laurens, Kristin1 ; Jones, Alice4 ; Barker, Gareth5 ; Hodgins,<br />

Sheilagh1 4 6<br />

; Viding, Essi<br />

1 Department of Forensic Mental Health Science, Institute of Psychiatry,<br />

King’s College, London, UK; 2 Department of Psychological<br />

Medicine and Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College,<br />

London, UK; 3 Department of Pediatric Neurology and Developmental<br />

Medicine, University of Tübingen, Germany; 4 Department<br />

of Clinical, Health and Educational Psychology, University College,<br />

London, UK; 5 Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Institute<br />

of Psychiatry, King’s College, London, UK; 6 Institute of Cognitive<br />

Neuroscience, University College London<br />

Brain imaging studies of adults with psychopathy have identified structural<br />

and functional abnormalities in limbic and prefrontal regions that<br />

are involved in emotion recognition, decision-making, morality and empathy.<br />

Among children with conduct problems, a small subgroup presents<br />

callous-unemotional traits (CP/CU+) thought to be antecedents of psychopathy.<br />

No structural brain imaging study has examined this subgroup<br />

of children. The present study used voxel-based morphometry (VBM) to<br />

compare whole brain gray matter volumes and concentrations of boys with<br />

elevated levels of CP/CU+ traits and typically developing (TD) boys and<br />

explored four a priori regions of interest. sMRI scans were collected from<br />

23 boys with elevated levels of CP/CU+ traits (mean age = 11 years 8<br />

months) and 25 TD boys (mean age = 11 years 6 months) selected from a<br />

community sample of children. Data were analysed using optimised VBM.<br />

Study-specific probability maps were created and four a priori regions<br />

of interest identified (Orbitofrontal [OFC], anterior cingulate [ACC], and<br />

anterior insular cortices, and amygdala). Both gray matter volume and<br />

concentration were examined controlling for cognitive ability and hyperactivity-inattention<br />

symptoms. Boys with CP/CU+ traits, as compared to TD<br />

boys, presented increased gray matter concentration in the medial OFC<br />

and ACC, as well as increased gray matter volume and concentration in<br />

the temporal lobes bilaterally. These findings may indicate a delay in cortical<br />

maturation in several brain areas implicated in decision-making, morality,<br />

and empathy in boys with CP/CU+ traits.<br />

65


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-12<br />

66<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

un D e r s t a n D i n g vi o l e n c e in we s t e r n<br />

su b u r b s: an in t e r c u l t u r a l clinical<br />

po i n t o f vi e w<br />

Derivois, Daniel 1 ; Guillier, Nathalie 1<br />

1 University of Lyon, France<br />

Understanding the current violence in western suburbs requires the articulation<br />

of several approaches in a global context. Our approach is in the<br />

field of intercultural clinical psychology. It incorporates historic and geopolitical<br />

dimensions understanding of this complex phenomenon.<br />

This communication is based on research conducted on the course of victim<br />

/ offender adolescents through various circles of life (family, schools,<br />

institutions of protection, prison, suburbs, etc.) in France. Some results<br />

of this research show the influence of the colonial divide and political violence<br />

in the development of victimization and delinquency behaviours in<br />

adolescence.<br />

Consequently, accompanying these adolescents requires from the professional<br />

taking into account not only of family, social and cultural factors but<br />

also the global dimension.<br />

Keywords : Adolescents in suburbs - Accompaniment - Interculturalities -<br />

Postcolonialism - Identity<br />

This communication is based on research conducted on the course of victim<br />

/ offender adolescents through various circles of life (family, schools,<br />

institutions of protection, prison, suburbs, etc.) in France. Some results<br />

of this research show the influence of the colonial divide and political violence<br />

in the development of victimization and delinquency behaviours in<br />

adolescence.<br />

Consequently, accompanying these adolescents requires from the professional<br />

taking into account not only of family, social and cultural factors but<br />

also the global dimension.<br />

Keywords : Adolescents in suburbs - Accompaniment - Interculturalities -<br />

Postcolonialism - Identity --


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-13<br />

Decision-ma k i n g in ec o n o m i c ga m e s<br />

is re l a t e D t o so c i a l be h a v i o r in<br />

aD o l e s c e n c e<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

d’Acremont, Mathieu 1 ; Bossaerts, Peter 1 ; Van der Linden,<br />

Martial 2<br />

1 Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland;<br />

2 University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland<br />

Economic games have been developed to study social interactions in an<br />

experimental setting. In these games, individuals have the possibility to<br />

exchange goods and their decisions are taken as indicator of their social<br />

preferences like altruism or equity. Past researches with economic games<br />

have mainly highlighted gender and age differences. However, it is unknown<br />

if decisions in economic games are related to social behavior in<br />

a natural setting. This absence of ecological validity has exposed game<br />

theory to serious criticism. The aim of this study was to test whether decisions<br />

in economic games were related to social behavior in adolescence,<br />

an age period when antisocial behavior is more frequent. Adolescents (44<br />

boys, 49 girls, 11-15 years old) completed economic games with their<br />

peers. Adolescents’ behavior in classroom was assessed by their teachers.<br />

Results showed that teenagers with a higher score of antisocial behavior<br />

shared less of their points when the opponent had no way to react (dictator<br />

game), but not when the opponent could refuse the offer (ultimatum<br />

game) or could send back part of the offer (trust game). Teenagers with a<br />

high score of social anxiety accepted lower offers (ultimatum game). Hyperactivity<br />

was not related to decision-making in these games. It can be<br />

concluded that decision-making in economic games is specifically related<br />

to social behavior in adolescence and that economic games have an ecological<br />

validity.<br />

67


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-14<br />

68<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

co m m u n a l n e s t i n g , a n e a r l y s o c i a l<br />

e n r i c h m e n t , s h a p e s s o c i a l b e h a v i o r a n D<br />

c o p i n g r e s p o n s e t o s o c i a l s t r e s s in a D u l t<br />

m o u s e<br />

D’Andrea, Ivana 1 ; Cirulli, Francesca 1 ; Branchi, Igor 1 ; Alleva,<br />

Enrico 1<br />

1 Section of Behavioural Neurosciences, Department of Cell Biology<br />

and Neuroscience, Istituto Superiore di Sanità, Rome, Italy.<br />

Early experiences produce persistent changes in behavior and brain function.<br />

Being reared in a Communal Nest (CN), consisting of a single nest<br />

where three mothers keep their pups together and share care-giving behavior<br />

from birth to weaning, provides an highly stimulating social environment<br />

to the developing pup. CN characterizes the natural ecological<br />

niche of the mouse species and represents a form of early social enrichment.<br />

In the CN, both mother-offspring and peer-to-peer interactions are<br />

markedly increased. At adulthood, CN mice show higher propensity to<br />

interact socially with conspecifics and more elaborate social competencies<br />

compared to mice reared in standard laboratory conditions (SN). In<br />

particular, CN mice play the role of either the dominant or the subordinate<br />

starting from the first agonistic encounter, while SN mice need five social<br />

encounters to fully show their social role. Furthermore, CN mice display<br />

high levels of aggressive behavior only when appropriate in an ecoethological<br />

perspective, i.e. when they have to set up or defend their own<br />

territory. With regard to emotional behavior and hypothalamic-pituitaryadrenal<br />

(HPA) axis activation, CN mice are less vulnerable to anhedonia<br />

following psychosocial stress and display a reduced activation of the HPA<br />

axis after acute or prolonged exposure to social challenge. The present<br />

findings show that being reared in a CN plays a crucial role in structuring<br />

adult social competencies in the mouse. Overall, the social environment to<br />

which an organism is exposed during critical developmental periods, exerts<br />

a major effects in shaping social behavior at adulthood. Supported by<br />

EU, project INTELLIMAZE contract n 037965.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-15<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

a v o X e l-b a s e D m o r p h o m e t r y (vbm) s t u D y<br />

o f b r a i n s t r u c t u r e in m a l e a D o l e s c e n t s<br />

w i t h co n D u c t Di s o r D e r<br />

Fairchild, Graeme 1 ; Passamonti, Luca 2 ; Hurford, Georgina<br />

1 ; von dem Hagen, Elisabeth 2 ; van Goozen, Stephanie 3 ;<br />

Fletcher, Paul 1 ; Goodyer, Ian 1 ; Calder, Andrew 2<br />

1 Department of Psychiatry, Cambridge University; 2 Medical Research<br />

Council Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge;<br />

3 School of Psychology, Cardiff University<br />

Recent studies from our group have demonstrated neurocognitive and<br />

emotional impairments in male adolescents with early-onset and adolescence-onset<br />

forms of Conduct Disorder (CD), a psychiatric diagnosis associated<br />

with increased aggressive and antisocial behaviour. To investigate<br />

the mechanisms underlying such impairments, we acquired structural<br />

magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data from these individuals and used<br />

a data-driven approach, voxel-based morphometry (VBM), to quantify<br />

differences in brain structure between groups. We report preliminary results<br />

from 20 control participants and 30 participants with CD (20 had<br />

early-onset CD and 10 had adolescence-onset CD). Collapsing across CD<br />

subgroups, CD participants overall showed reductions in rostral anterior<br />

cingulate cortex (rACC; p>.001 uncorrected) and intraparietal sulcus<br />

(p>.001 uncorrected) grey matter volume relative to controls. Using<br />

a less stringent threshold (p>.01 uncorrected), volume reductions were<br />

observed across the anterior cingulate cortex, amygdala and anterior insular<br />

cortex in participants with CD. Compared with controls, participants<br />

with early-onset CD showed volume reductions in dorsal anterior cingulate<br />

cortex (dACC; p>.001 uncorrected) and intraparietal sulcus (p>.001<br />

uncorrected). These data suggest that the most pronounced structural<br />

abnormalities in CD occur in brain regions involved in emotion regulation.<br />

Findings of reduced dACC volume are consistent with a previous functional<br />

imaging study showing abnormal patterns of dACC activation during<br />

emotion processing in early-onset CD, but diverge somewhat from prior<br />

structural imaging studies primarily using subjects with comorbid CD and<br />

ADHD. This study differs from the others in the literature because most<br />

(73%) of the participants with CD did not have a comorbid ADHD diagnosis.<br />

69


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-16<br />

70<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

on t h e n e u r a l p r o c e s s i n g o f v i o l e n t a n D<br />

n o n -v i o l e n t c o m p u t e r g a m e s c e n a r i o s in<br />

g a m e r s a n D n o n -g a m e r s<br />

Fehr, Thorsten 1 ; Regenbogen, Christina 2 ; Herrmann, Manfred<br />

1<br />

1 Center for Cognitive Sciences, Dept. Of Neuropsychology/Behavioral<br />

Neurobiology, Center for Advanced Imaging Bremen/Magdeburg,<br />

Bremen University, Germany; 2 Center for Cognitive Sciences,<br />

Dept. Of Neuropsychology/Behavioral Neurobiology, Bremen<br />

University, Dept. of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, RWTH Aachen<br />

University, Germany<br />

There is a large public and scientific discussion about the effects of violent<br />

video games on behaviour-related aspects such as arousal, emotion, and<br />

cognition. The ability to differentiate between reality and fiction has been<br />

discussed to be reduced in individuals who intensively use media particularly<br />

displaying violent content. In the present fMRI study violent computer<br />

game scenarios and an analogous parallel form of video sequences<br />

displaying real characters were presented to a group of 22 young males<br />

with (gamers) and without (non-gamers) history of long-term violent<br />

video game experience. A comparison between violent and non-violent<br />

scenarios suggested large differences in activation patterns between gamers<br />

and non-gamers as well as between the processing of the virtual and<br />

the real scenarios. A lack of sub-cortical network activation, as identified<br />

in both group- and single-individual analyses, indicated a rather ‘cold’ and<br />

neocortical represented and less arousal driven form of emotional processing<br />

of both virtual and real violent scenarios in both groups. This might<br />

indicate a general desensitisation-to-violence-effect in both gamers and<br />

non-gamers, potentially caused by predominantly miss-attributed and perverted<br />

forms of any kind of violence in all public media always present and<br />

chronically available for everyone. Additionally, large individual differences<br />

point to the necessity to particularly consider individual learning histories<br />

and developmental factors. Taken together, the present data do not support<br />

the view that gamers are impaired in differentially processing virtual<br />

and real contextual information, even after a long learning history of computer<br />

game play.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-17<br />

an i n t e g r a t i n g s y s t e m o f m o r a l<br />

D i s e n g a g e m e n t a n D a g g r e g a t i o n<br />

w i t h D e v i a n t p e e r g o v e r n i n g v i o l e n t<br />

a g g r e s s i o n<br />

Fida, Roberta 1 ; Paciello, Marinella 1 ; Tramontano, Carlo 1 ;<br />

Caprara, Gian Vittorio 1<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

1 Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy<br />

Earlier aggression predicts different manifestations of later violence (Farrington,<br />

1994 ; Loeber &Loeber, 1998; Stattin &Magnusson, 1989). Nevertheless<br />

not all aggressive behavior becomes violent over years, whereas<br />

violent conduct often derives from earlier aggression.<br />

We focused on the interconnection between contextual and individual factors<br />

as a mediator of the relation between aggression in pre-adolescence<br />

and violent outcomes in youth. We included aggregation with deviant peer<br />

as a relational risky context variable and moral disengagement as crucial<br />

individual dimension related to the recourse to social cognitive mechanisms<br />

that legitimate behaviors that hurt other people. We considered<br />

Bandura’s triadic reciprocal determinism as a pivotal model of causation in<br />

which violent aggression in youth can be seen as the result of interactive<br />

relation between moral disengagement and aggregation with deviant peer<br />

that operate as interrelated determinants that influence each other bidirectionally.<br />

In particular we hypothesized that aggression at 13/14 years<br />

influence violent aggression at 21/22 years directly and indirectly through<br />

the interrelation between the development of both moral disengagement<br />

and aggregation with deviant peer from 15 to 20 years. We use Growth<br />

Latent Curve analysis among a sample of 324 Italian adolescents (50%<br />

boys) part of an ongoing longitudinal two cohorts design assessed five<br />

times.<br />

We assessed earlier aggression using Physical and Verbal Aggression Scale<br />

(Caprara &Pastorelli, 1993) rated by peers; Moral Disengagement using<br />

Bandura’s scale (Bandura et al., 1996); Aggregation with deviant peer using<br />

Capaldi and Patterson’s scale (1989); Violent aggression using 3 items<br />

selected from Physical and Verbal Aggression Scale (Caprara, Pastorelli,<br />

1993) and 3 items selected from Violence Scale (Caprara, Mazzotti, &Prezza,<br />

1990).<br />

Results attested the reciprocal reinforcement between intra-individual and<br />

inter-personal processes during adolescence that can lead aggression in<br />

pre-adolescence into violent outcomes in youth. We discuss implication in<br />

preventing violent aggression in youth.<br />

71


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-18<br />

72<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

ca m u s \’ \”st r a n g e r \”: wh y cr i m e s<br />

pr o s e e D<br />

Galyona, Eugene 1<br />

1 Karazin Kharkov National University, Ukraine<br />

As any literature which has a crime as central event, in “Stranger” it is<br />

made attempt to understand: what is the crime and why does it exist.<br />

In “Stranger” Camus criticize European system of justice. It should be<br />

noticed that the judicial system was not always been total resource of the<br />

violence prevention. As the majority ethnographers said there was nothing<br />

in archaic societies that reminds modern judicial system. Rene Girard<br />

results the following gradation of institutions of internal conflicts settlement:<br />

revenge, religious ritual, court. In his opinion efficiency of modern<br />

judicial system has led to disappearance (or more truly to dissolution in<br />

other institutions) of the sacrifice. However up to the XX-th century these<br />

three institutions were represented by three various practices of the conflict<br />

resolution: revenge (duel), religious ritual (Christian ethics) and court<br />

(the state law). In the XX-th century the judicial system becomes the only<br />

powerful institution of the violence prevention but just to its efficiency Camus<br />

had questions.<br />

From the ethological point of view (Konrad Lorenz) intraspecific aggression<br />

is the reason of an individualization and formation of the subject (through<br />

the pacification and greeting rituals). Thus thanks to violence there is<br />

the certain order generated by system of distinctions. When this order is<br />

broken, there is a situation which Girard names «sacrificial crisis» that is<br />

always accompanied by violence. Girard and Lorentz underline value of<br />

asymmetries. It not the violence apologia, it shows that the system which<br />

have to resists to it does not function; that the murder censure, without<br />

studying of deep sources of the violence, and any equality declared by democracy<br />

and humanistic ideas ignore by-effect of themselves that loosens<br />

basis on which they are generated.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-19<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

fr e q u e n c y a n D ty p e s o f Do m e s t i c ab u s e<br />

a m o n g el D e r l y r e f e r r i n g t o pa r k s o f<br />

te h r a n .<br />

Ghorbi, Banafsheh 1 ; Manoochehri, Houman 1 ; Hosseini,<br />

Meymanat 1 ; Nasiri Oskouei, Navideh 1 ; Karbakhsh, Mojgan 2<br />

1 Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Science and Health Srevices,<br />

Tehran, Iran; 2 Tehran University of Medical Science and<br />

Health Srevices, Tehran, Iran<br />

Aging has always been concerned throughout history. The second half of<br />

the 20th century has been referred as population graying due to increased<br />

number of elderly people. Of related familial and social outcomes is the<br />

upsurge of antisocial behaviors and violence. Domestic elder mistreatment<br />

or abuse as a major health problem is a type of familial violence<br />

with a significantly increasing incidence in the last 2 decades. Since there<br />

is no specific organization to be responsible for this problem in developing<br />

countries like Iran, no information or evidence is available and limited<br />

studies are only limited to developed countries. This descriptive study was<br />

conducted to determine the degree and types of domestic abuse in the<br />

elderly referring to parks of Tehran.<br />

400 elders 60 years and over including men and women from all parts<br />

of Tehran took part in the study. A questionnaire in 3 parts including demographics,<br />

types of abuse and open questions was used for data collection.<br />

The tool was validated by face and content methods and made<br />

reliable by internal consistency as well as test-retest methods. The questionnaire<br />

was completed by the researcher through interviewing the subjects.<br />

Findings showed that %28 of the elderly was between 70 and 74,<br />

%38.2 had secondary education, %65.1 was married and %32.9 lived<br />

with their spouses. %87.8 of them experienced at least 1 type of abuse,<br />

%12.2 no abuse and %24.9 all 4 types of abuse. %84.8 experienced<br />

emotional abuse; %68.3, neglect; %40.1, financial exploitation; and<br />

%35.2, physical abuse.<br />

It can be concluded that domestic abuse is a widespread social problem<br />

which requires urgent, continuous and fundamental interventions.<br />

Clarification of the issue from different aspects, identification of predisposing<br />

and preventive factors and identification of at-risk elders are of high<br />

priorities in this regard.<br />

Keyword: Elderly, Domestic abuse, Parks.<br />

73


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-20<br />

74<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

impairments in nonverbal<br />

emotion recognition as a<br />

risk factor for aggressive<br />

behavior<br />

Gomez, Jean-Marc 1 ; Van der Linden, Martial 2<br />

1 Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva,<br />

Switzerland; 2 Department of Psychology, University of<br />

Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland<br />

Background: As researchers have postulated and shown (Marsh &Blair,<br />

2008), the perception of nonverbal affective cues may contribute in important<br />

ways to aggressive behavior in children. Indeed, misinterpretation of<br />

affective cues inevitably affects social interaction. This study examined the<br />

reliability of impairments in nonverbal emotion recognition in children with<br />

aggressive problems. Linguistic competence was also measured in order to<br />

determine its influence on facial emotional recognition. Method: 58 children<br />

(age range=7.5-13.8 years, M=11.4 years, SD=1.5), who attended<br />

special education in 13 classes in Geneva, participated in the study. The<br />

teachers assessed the aggressive behavior of their pupils with a questionnaire<br />

(French version of SDQ, d�Acremont &Van der Linden, 2008). Moreover,<br />

the children completed two standardized tests of nonverbal perception<br />

abilities (the first including emotional pictures, the second videos and<br />

auditory stimuli), as well as three control tests assessing the perception of<br />

faces and levels of syntax and vocabulary. Results: As predicted, children<br />

showing more pronounced prosocial behavior were better at recognizing<br />

emotions, especially fear and disgust. Children showing higher relational<br />

competence did particularly well at recognizing fear stimuli. Furthermore,<br />

children showing more prosocial behaviors were not only better, but also<br />

faster at recognizing facial and auditory emotional stimuli than were children<br />

with fewer empathic behaviors. We also observed a link between<br />

emotion perception and linguistic abilities. Conclusion: These results support<br />

the idea that deficits in emotion perception, which seem to be linked<br />

to linguistic competence, contribute to problematic behavior in children.<br />

More specifically, rapid information processing underlying the recognition<br />

of nonverbal emotional cues seems to be �underdeveloped� in children<br />

showing problematic social behavior. In conclusion, it is suggested that<br />

intervention programs specifically designed to boost the perception of affective<br />

cues may help resolve behavioral problems in aggression-prone<br />

children.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-21<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

the structural phenotypes of<br />

persistent violence: a nipa<br />

structural mri stuDy<br />

Guidi, Raffaella; Magistretti, Pierre J.; Halfon, Olivier; Cardinaux,<br />

Jean-René<br />

1 Institute of Psychiatry, Kings College London, UK<br />

Detailed neurobiological models of reactive violence in ASPD and instrumental<br />

violence in psychopathy have emerged in the last decade. However,<br />

potential structural brain differences between psychopathic men and<br />

antisocial men who do not meet psychopathy criteria remain to be established.<br />

The Neurobiological Investigations of Persistent Aggression (NIPA)<br />

study is attempting to distinguish between these groups across multiple<br />

modalities. The current study (a work in progress) examined the differences<br />

in grey matter volume between 3 groups of men matched for age<br />

and IQ: 1) violent offenders with ASPD but without psychopathy (PCL-<br />

R>25) (n=20); 2) violent offenders with ASPD and psychopathy (PCL-R<br />


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-22<br />

76<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

im p o r t a n c e o f cbp a n D torc1 in creb<br />

t r a n s c r i p t i o n a l a c t i v a t i o n<br />

Guidi, Raffaella1 2 ; Magistretti, Pierre J. 1 3 ; Halfon, Olivier2 ;<br />

1 2<br />

Cardinaux, Jean-René<br />

1 Centre for Psychiatric Neuroscience, CHUV, Lausane; 2 Child and<br />

Adolescent Psychiatry (SUPEA), Lausanne; 3 Brain Mind Institute,<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong>, Lausanne<br />

cAMP-response-element-binding protein (CREB) is a member of a family<br />

of proteins that function as transcription factors. It is expressed in all cells<br />

in the brain and its activation is known to play a crucial role in learning<br />

and memory, as well as in several neuropsychiatric disorders. However,<br />

depending on the brain region and the stimuli leading to its transcriptional<br />

activation, neuronal functions may be affected in different manners. In<br />

the context of addiction, CREB was shown to be critical. When CREB is<br />

phosphorylated, it recruits a coactivator called CBP, and activates the expression<br />

of a set of genes that modifies the properties of striatal medium<br />

spiny neurons in the reward pathway. Moreover, we have recently shown<br />

that another coactivator called Transducer Of Regulated CREB activity 1<br />

(TORC1) functions as a calcium- and cAMP-sensitive coincidence detector<br />

in neurons, and is involved in hippocampal long-term synaptic plasticity<br />

(Kovacs et al., 2007.PNAS 104, 4700-4705). Given the importance of calcium<br />

and cAMP signaling pathways for reward-related long-term memory<br />

formation, we hypothesized that TORC1 could play a role in this process.<br />

To determine the respective involvement of CBP and TORC1 in the regulation<br />

of CREB-mediated transcription, we are developing different strategies<br />

to selectively interfere with these CREB coactivators in mouse primary<br />

neurons. Ultimately, we would like to better define the role of CREB, CBP<br />

and TORC1 in reward-related learning and memory and drug addiction.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-23<br />

un D e r s t a n D i n g vi o l e n c e in we s t e r n<br />

su b u r b s : an in t e r c u l t u r a l clinical<br />

po i n t o f vi e w<br />

Derivois, Daniel 1 ; Guillier, Nathalie 1<br />

1 University of Lyon, France<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

Understanding the current violence in western suburbs requires the articulation<br />

of several approaches in a global context. Our approach is in the<br />

field of intercultural clinical psychology. It incorporates historic and geopolitical<br />

dimensions understanding of this complex phenomenon.<br />

This communication is based on research conducted on the course of victim<br />

/ offender adolescents through various circles of life (family, schools,<br />

institutions of protection, prison, suburbs, etc.) in France. Some results<br />

of this research show the influence of the colonial divide and political violence<br />

in the development of victimization and delinquency behaviours in<br />

adolescence.<br />

Consequently, accompanying these adolescents requires from the professional<br />

taking into account not only of family, social and cultural factors but<br />

also the global dimension.<br />

Keywords : Adolescents in suburbs - Accompaniment - Interculturalities -<br />

Postcolonialism - Identity --<br />

77


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-24<br />

78<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

ne u r a l co r r e l a t e s o f aD a p t i v e Decision<br />

ma k i n g u n D e r un c e r t a i n t y in vi o l e n t<br />

cr m i n a l of f e n D e r s w i t h ps y c h o p a t h y<br />

a m D bo r D e r l i n e pe r s o n a l i t y Di s o r D e r<br />

Prehn, Kristin 1 ; Schlagenhauf, Florian 2 ; Schulze, Lars 1 ;<br />

Berger, Christoph 1 ; Vohs, Knut 1 ; Wagner, Sabine 3 ; Hauenstein,<br />

Karlheinz 3 ; Domes, Gregor 4 ; Herpertz, Sabine C. 1<br />

1 Dept. of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Rostock University, Rostock,<br />

Germany; 2 Dept. of Psychiatry, Charité University Medicine,<br />

Berlin, Germany; 3 Department of Radiology, Rostock University,<br />

Rostock, Germany; 4 Dept. of Psychology, University of Zurich,<br />

Zurich, Switzerland<br />

In everyday life individuals are required to make decisions that bear a certain<br />

degree of uncertainty regarding potential gains and losses. Individuals<br />

differ with respect to their ability to adjust their choices and behavior in<br />

the face of higher risk.<br />

To investigate the neural systems mediating these differences, we presented<br />

healthy controls (n=13) and two groups of criminal offenders [11<br />

patients with psychopathy as defined by the Hare Psychopathy Checklist<br />

and 12 patients with DSM-IV borderline personality disorder (BPD)] with<br />

the Behavioral Investment Allocation Strategy task (Kuhnen &Knutson,<br />

2005) and monitored brain activity using fMRI. During this task, participants<br />

were required to choose between two stocks and a bond. At the<br />

beginning of an experimental block one of the two stocks was randomly<br />

assigned to be the “good” (+10 €) or the “bad” stock ( 10 €) without the<br />

participants´ knowledge. The bond always paid 1 Euro.<br />

When uncertainty which stock to choose was high (e.g., at the beginning<br />

of a block) controls showed adaptive risk-avoidant behavior (i.e.,<br />

were more likely to choose the bond). Chosing the bond in trials with high<br />

uncertainty was preceded by increased activity in a functional network<br />

including insula, inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), and pre-supplementary motor<br />

area. Uncertainty was represented in rostral anterior cingulate gyrus<br />

(rACC). Criminal offenders with BPD did not choose more bonds when<br />

uncertainty was high. This risk-seeking behavior was accompanied by less<br />

activity in IFG before choosing the bond in contrast to controls. Psychopaths<br />

only differed from controls by decreased activity in rACC reflecting a<br />

deficit to emotionally represent uncertainty.<br />

These results support the theory that criminal offenders with psychopathy<br />

are characterized by a lack of emotion, whereas criminal offenders with<br />

BPD show a more pronounced deficit in behavioral control to adjust their<br />

choices in the face of higher risk.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-25<br />

ch a r a c t e r i s t i c s o f victims o f v i o l e n c e<br />

visiting a m e D i c o l e g a l c o n s u l t a t i o n :<br />

a n o t h e r p a t h t o u n D e r s t a n D v i o l e n c e ?<br />

Hofner, M.-Cl. 1 ; Romain, N. 1 ; Mangin, P. 1<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

1 Violence Medical Unit (VMU), University Centre for Legal Medicine,<br />

University Hospital Centre, Lausanne, Switzerland<br />

To improve care and services to victims of interpersonal violence, a Violence<br />

Medical Unit(VMU) was set up at the Lausanne University Hospital,<br />

Switzerland in 2006. The VMU consultation offers medico-legal and community<br />

care. Patients are received by forensic nurses for support, forensic<br />

examination, needs assessment and community orientation.<br />

A descriptive study of medical reports was conducted in 2008 with the<br />

aim to explore characteristics of this specific population. During the study<br />

period, 03.01.2006 to 31.12. 2007, the VMU registered 911 patients. Regarding<br />

eligibility criteria (complete medical file and actual interpersonal<br />

victimisation), final sample was 863, including: 55% of men and 45% of<br />

women, with a mean age of 30,5 years. 89% of men reported community<br />

violence for 37% of women, 63% of women reported domestic violence<br />

for 11% of men. For 55% the present violent event was not the first one<br />

in their adult lifetime and 25% reported violence during childhood. If 56%<br />

didn’t have Swiss nationality, more than 90% were speaking French fluently.<br />

85% of the aggressors were men for any type of violence and both<br />

sex of victims.<br />

Distribution of type of violence and socio demographic characteristics in<br />

our population does not differ from literature results: the population is<br />

young, men are more concerned by community violence and women by<br />

domestic one .However, a quarter of our population reported violence in<br />

childhood, more than a half already experienced being victim of violence<br />

and aggressors independently of type of violence or sex of the victims are<br />

men. Discussion of these results from a neuro scientific as well as a systemic<br />

point of view would be of great interest to enlarge understanding of<br />

the violent interpersonal interactions.<br />

79


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-26<br />

80<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

genDer inequality , hiv/aiDs<br />

&violence : triple jeoparDy of<br />

women<br />

Joshi, Shambhu 1 ; Bhandari, rajan 2 ; Kumar, Ashok 3<br />

1 District Public Health Office, Malakhet, Nepal; 2 Community<br />

Health and Environmental Society Nepal,KTM, Nepal; 3 Univrsity<br />

of Bombay,Mumbai,India<br />

Background: HIV infection in Nepal has a female face because of it growing<br />

fastest in this subpopulation. How Gender, violence &HIV/AIDS make<br />

women jeopardized?<br />

Summary/Objectives: A study was conducted with PLWHA women during<br />

2005-2007. To examine the complexity of violence, HIV/AIDS and to learn<br />

more about the specific problems faced by women living with HIV - how<br />

the concept of gender, violence &HIV/AIDS make their life vulnerable.<br />

Case Studies and Informal Interviews with HIV infected women. Data was<br />

analysed with EPI info program.<br />

Results: Case studies and interviews with women from the study illustrate<br />

that low status in family, sexual violence, economic and social problems<br />

such as poverty, lack of education are some of the primary reasons to get<br />

infection. Cultural orientation inhibits them to talk about sex to their partners,<br />

which results in infectious status. In the middle-aged women, after<br />

sterilization they do not practice regular use of condoms, because they<br />

think it is primarily for family planning. Among the newly-married women<br />

they know their status only at time of pregnancy, which results in psychological<br />

trauma and other related aspects. Most of them are widows and<br />

they know their sero status at a later stage of their partner’s HIV infected<br />

life. After the death of their partner, some of them are being expelled from<br />

their home and undergo various violations of human rights.<br />

Lessons learned: This study revealed the need to develop appropriate program<br />

would be emphasing the target communities. Due to illiteracy, poverty,<br />

gender inequality women and girls are facing with spousal battering<br />

, sexual abuse of female children, dowry related violence, rape including<br />

marital rape, traditional practices harmful to female, no spousal violence,<br />

sexual harassment and intimidation at work and in school, trafficking of<br />

women, forced prostitution, rape in war, female infanticide, constant belittling<br />

includes controlling behaviors such as isolation from family &friends,<br />

monitoring her movements, restrict her access to resources.<br />

Govenment,NGOs,INGOs also have crucial role to work hand to hand in<br />

this issues by empowering female, law and policy, equal education and<br />

economic opportunities to female.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-27<br />

Depicting t h e r o l e o f t h e kap1<br />

e p i g e n e t i c r e g u l a t o r in b e h a v i o u r a l<br />

v u l n e r a b i l i t y t o s t r e s s<br />

Marquis, Julien 1 ; Jakobsson, Johan 2 ; Bisaz, Reto 1 ; Sandi,<br />

Carmen 1 ; Trono, Didier 1<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

1 Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland;<br />

2 Faculty of medicine, Lund university, 22184 Lund, Sweden<br />

Growing evidence points to the role of epigenetics in translating environmental<br />

stimuli into long-lasting changes of gene expression in the brain.<br />

Contrasting with the abundance of data pointing to the key role of epigenetics<br />

in animal behaviour, relatively little is known about the molecular<br />

mediators of this process. As part of a broad effort to define the roles of<br />

the KRAB/KAP1 gene regulation pathways in vivo, we carried out a reverse<br />

genetic approach by generating a mouse model in which the KAP1 gene<br />

was specifically inactivated in neurons of the adult forebrain. Behavioral<br />

studies reveal heightened level of anxiety-like and exploratory activity in<br />

these mice, as well as stress-induced alterations in spatial learning and<br />

memory (Jacobsson et al., Neuron, 2008, 60, 818-31). At the molecular<br />

level, transcriptome analyses detect the dysregulation of a small number<br />

of genes correlated with a decrease of H3K9-me3 and an increase of<br />

H4Ac, consistent with KAP1-dependent chromatin changes.<br />

Our initial molecular studies were performed on whole hippocampus, that<br />

is, on a mixture of KAP1-negative and KAP1-positive cells. We are now<br />

using laser-dissection to recover selectively KAP1-deleted cells and also<br />

analyze separately different hippocampal sub-area. We anticipate that,<br />

associated with transcriptome and chromatin analyses, the method should<br />

generate robust data to help deciphering the cascade of molecular event<br />

linking KAP1 to the observed “stress phenotype”.<br />

The Cre mediated hippocampal-KAP1 deletion occurs at approximately<br />

15-30 days of age, that is, when murine macroscopic brain structures are<br />

fully developed. Nevertheless, we cannot rule out that KAP1 deletion does<br />

not primarily impact on immature, developing neurological networks. To<br />

address this point, we have generated a similar mouse model but expressing<br />

a tamoxifen-inducible form of Cre. We will now study whether inducing<br />

KAP1 deletion at different ages has an impact on the “stress behaviour”.<br />

Along the same line, we will study the reversibility of that phenotype by<br />

controlling the re-expression of KAP1 through doxycycline treatment of<br />

animals carrying an rtTA-regulated expression system.<br />

Work performed in our laboratory indicates that KAP1 is an important mediator<br />

of stem cell proliferation/differentiation. Interestingly, several observations<br />

support the hypothesis that depression could be correlated with<br />

an altered adult neurogenesis in the hippocampus. Therefore, through<br />

combined labelling with BrdU and several neuronal differentiation marker,<br />

we are testing the possibility that adult neuronal maturation could be affected<br />

in hippocampal KAP1-deleted animals. The results of this analysis<br />

may provide insight into the biology of depression.<br />

81


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-28<br />

82<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

ef f e c t s o f a c u t e a n D c h r o n i c hpa a X i s<br />

a c t i v i t y o n a g g r e s s i o n - D i f f e r i n g e f f e c t s<br />

f o r m a l e s a n D f e m a l e s<br />

Khan, Robina 1 ; Bertsch, Katja 1 ; Kruk, Menno 2 ; Naumann,<br />

Ewald 1<br />

1 University of Trier, Trier, Germany; 2 Leiden University, Leiden,<br />

the Netherlands<br />

Aggression and the understanding of its causes and biological mechanisms<br />

in men and women is today as important as ever. Animal studies suggest<br />

that aggressive behavior is influenced by both basal and reactive Hypothalamus-Pituitary-Adrenal<br />

(HPA) axis activity. In humans, results from<br />

clinical samples are inconsistent, experimental studies are rare, and both<br />

are mostly conducted with male samples. We therefore pharmacologically<br />

enhanced cortisol levels in 28 healthy subjects by administering 20mg of<br />

hydrocortisone (stress group), 28 others were given a placebo (placebo<br />

group). We then induced aggressive behavior with the Taylor Aggression<br />

Paradigm in half of the stress and the placebo group, respectively. Sex<br />

was balanced across groups. We furthermore analyzed the relationship of<br />

basal HPA axis activity, which was measured prior to the experiment, and<br />

aggression. Preliminary analysis suggests, that while the administration of<br />

hydrocortisone enhanced aggressive behavior in women compared to the<br />

placebo group, it reduced aggressive behavior in men. Furthermore, basal<br />

HPA axis activity seems to be negatively correlated with aggressive behavior,<br />

but only in women. The results suggest, that the acute and chronic<br />

state of the HPA axis activity are differentially related to aggressive behavior<br />

in men and women.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-29<br />

th e p s y c h o p a t h o l o g y o f r e p e a t e D<br />

a g g r e s s i o n: an e X p e r i m e n t a l s t u D y<br />

Kudryavtseva, Natalia 1<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

1 Institute of Cytology and Genetics SD RAS, Novosibirsk, Russia<br />

The sensory contact model allows the aggressive type of behavior to be<br />

formed as a result of repeated experience of victories in daily agonistic<br />

interactions in male mice. Some behavioral domains confirm the development<br />

of learned aggression in the winners similar to those in humans. The<br />

features are: repeated experience of aggression reinforced by victories;<br />

elements of learned behavior after period of confrontations; intent, measured<br />

by increase of the aggressive motivation prior agonistic confrontation;<br />

decreased emotionality. Positive fighting experience in daily social<br />

confrontations changes many characteristics of individual and social behaviors,<br />

these having been estimated in varied situations. Neurochemical<br />

data confirm the activation of brain dopaminergic systems and functional<br />

inhibition of serotonergic system in the winners under influence of repeated<br />

aggression, that create the low threshold for aggressive reaction in<br />

even weakly provoking environment. Changes in opioidergic systems form<br />

aggression abuse in male mice. Long experience of aggression is accompanied<br />

by development of abnormal and violent aggression accompanying<br />

by hyperkinetic and stereotypic reaction, hostile behavior, hostility, maniclike<br />

states, disturbances in social recognition, pronounced anxiety, addictive<br />

state etc [review, Kudryavtseva, 2006].<br />

Kudryavtseva N.N. Psychopathology of repeated aggression: a neurobiological<br />

aspect. In “Perspectives on the Psychology of Aggression” Ed. J.P.<br />

Morgan, NOVA Science Publishers, Inc. 2006. Chapter 2, pp.35-64<br />

83


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-30<br />

84<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

ne u r a l c o r r e l a t e s o f a b n o r m a l<br />

a g g r e s s i o n f o l l o w i n g p e r i p u b e r t a l s t r e s s<br />

in r a t s<br />

Marquez, Cristina 1 ; Vaucher, Angélique 1 ; Sonnay, Aliénor 1 ;<br />

Sigmund, Coralie 1 ; Marquis, Julien 2 ; Groner, Anna Claire 2 ;<br />

Sandi, Carmen 1<br />

1 Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland;<br />

2 Laboratory of Virology and Genetics, School of Life Science,<br />

Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Lausanne,<br />

Switzerland<br />

Early life stress in humans enhances the risk for psychopathologies, including<br />

excessive aggression and violence. Adolescence is an important<br />

maturation phase during which critical neurodevelopmental events occur<br />

in brain regions associated with motivation, emotion and cognition. It is<br />

also a relevant period for developing social competences required for adult<br />

life. In rodents, stress can induce long-lasting changes in emotional and<br />

neuroendocrine responsiveness to stress that can be associated with several<br />

psychopathologies. Here we examine behavioral and molecular consequences<br />

of exposure to stress during the peripubertal period. Male Wistar<br />

rats were subchronically exposed to stress (predator odour and open elevated<br />

spaces) during peripuberty (7 days of stress across the P28-P42<br />

period). The long term effects of peripubertal stress were examined when<br />

animals were 3 months-old. Peripuberty-stressed animals displayed increased<br />

anxiety and aggressive behaviors. In the resident-intruder test,<br />

peripuberty-stressed rats attacked more the vulnerable parts of their opponent<br />

and showed a lack of inhibitory control of their behavior (continued<br />

attacking despite clear signals of submission). In order to elucidate the<br />

mechanisms involved in this model of abnormal aggression, we studied<br />

(i) basal brain energy metabolism using 14C-2deoxyglucose autoradiography,<br />

(ii) the pattern of activation of different brain areas after an aggressive<br />

encounter (resident-intruder test) using c-fos immunohistochemistry,<br />

(iii) the levels of expression of the serotonin transporter using qPCR.<br />

Peripuberty-stressed animals showed an increased basal metabolism in<br />

amygdala and bed nucleus of stria terminalis, both areas related to anxiety,<br />

and an increase in the basal expression of the serotonin transporter<br />

in the prefrontal cortex. The aggressive animals showed increase in c-fos<br />

expression in medial amygdala and a lower activation of the medial orbitofrontal<br />

cortex, after the resident-intruder test. Our findings highlight the<br />

relevance of this peripubertal stress model to investigate the neurobiological<br />

correlates of abnormal aggression and confirm the serotonergic system<br />

and the interactions between amygdala and prefrontal cortex as key elements<br />

in the understanding of violence.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-31<br />

Depicting t h e r o l e o f t h e kap1<br />

e p i g e n e t i c r e g u l a t o r in b e h a v i o u r a l<br />

v u l n e r a b i l i t y t o s t r e s s<br />

Marquis, Julien 1 ; Jakobsson, Johan 2 ; Bisaz, Reto 1 ; Sandi,<br />

Carmen 1 ; Trono, Didier 1<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

1 Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland;<br />

2 Faculty of medicine, Lund university, 22184 Lund, Sweden<br />

Growing evidence points to the role of epigenetics in translating environmental<br />

stimuli into long-lasting changes of gene expression in the brain.<br />

Contrasting with the abundance of data pointing to the key role of epigenetics<br />

in animal behaviour, relatively little is known about the molecular<br />

mediators of this process. As part of a broad effort to define the roles of<br />

the KRAB/KAP1 gene regulation pathways in vivo, we carried out a reverse<br />

genetic approach by generating a mouse model in which the KAP1 gene<br />

was specifically inactivated in neurons of the adult forebrain. Behavioral<br />

studies reveal heightened level of anxiety-like and exploratory activity in<br />

these mice, as well as stress-induced alterations in spatial learning and<br />

memory (Jacobsson et al., Neuron, 2008, 60, 818-31). At the molecular<br />

level, transcriptome analyses detect the dysregulation of a small number<br />

of genes correlated with a decrease of H3K9-me3 and an increase of<br />

H4Ac, consistent with KAP1-dependent chromatin changes.<br />

Our initial molecular studies were performed on whole hippocampus, that<br />

is, on a mixture of KAP1-negative and KAP1-positive cells. We are now<br />

using laser-dissection to recover selectively KAP1-deleted cells and also<br />

analyze separately different hippocampal sub-area. We anticipate that,<br />

associated with transcriptome and chromatin analyses, the method should<br />

generate robust data to help deciphering the cascade of molecular event<br />

linking KAP1 to the observed “stress phenotype”.<br />

The Cre mediated hippocampal-KAP1 deletion occurs at approximately<br />

15-30 days of age, that is, when murine macroscopic brain structures are<br />

fully developed. Nevertheless, we cannot rule out that KAP1 deletion does<br />

not primarily impact on immature, developing neurological networks. To<br />

address this point, we have generated a similar mouse model but expressing<br />

a tamoxifen-inducible form of Cre. We will now study whether inducing<br />

KAP1 deletion at different ages has an impact on the “stress behaviour”.<br />

Along the same line, we will study the reversibility of that phenotype by<br />

controlling the re-expression of KAP1 through doxycycline treatment of<br />

animals carrying an rtTA-regulated expression system.<br />

Work performed in our laboratory indicates that KAP1 is an important mediator<br />

of stem cell proliferation/differentiation. Interestingly, several observations<br />

support the hypothesis that depression could be correlated with<br />

an altered adult neurogenesis in the hippocampus. Therefore, through<br />

combined labelling with BrdU and several neuronal differentiation marker,<br />

we are testing the possibility that adult neuronal maturation could be affected<br />

in hippocampal KAP1-deleted animals. The results of this analysis<br />

may provide insight into the biology of depression.<br />

85


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-32<br />

86<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

wh y m e n r a p e: so c i o -c u l t u r a l f a c t o r s<br />

p r e D i s p o s i n g m e n t o a c t s o f r a p e in<br />

ke n y a<br />

Muchoki, Samuel 1 ; Wandibba, Simiyu 2<br />

1 Department of Culture and Heritage, National Museums of Kenya;<br />

2 Institute of Anthropology, Gender and African Studies,<br />

University of Nairobi<br />

Rape is one type of gender-based violence that is on increase in Kenya<br />

today. This paper is derived from a study designed to investigate the<br />

individual motivations and the socio-cultural factors that predispose men<br />

to acts of rape in Kenya between the month of January and March 2008.<br />

The study sample was drawn from Naivasha, Kamiti and Nyeri main Prisons<br />

in Kenya. Data was collected using the survey method, case histories<br />

and key informant interviews. The findings suggest that a number of social<br />

cultural factors may predispose a man to rape. The factors identified<br />

include view of rape as sexual act rather than an act of violence, social<br />

attitude that the woman ‘invited’ the rape, early childhood environment,<br />

cultural practices, peer influence and a lack of parental advice on sex. As<br />

an introduction, the paper highlights the theories put forward to explain<br />

the existence of rape in our society. However, looking at rape from the<br />

perspective of the rapists, we argue that rape is a socially acquired behavior<br />

where some rapists accustom themselves with the normative cultural<br />

expectations on women and within this framework they endeavor to create<br />

an image that shifts the blame from themselves to their victims. We<br />

conclude thus that rape emanates from the social environment that an<br />

individual grows in or finds himself in later in life


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-33<br />

an as s e s s m e n t o f t h e pr e v a l e n c e o f<br />

se X u a l vi o l e n c e : a ca s e st u D y o f<br />

na i v a s h a District<br />

Mwangi, Maryann 1<br />

1 St. Paul’s University, Limuru, Kenya<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

Naivasha District has reported more rape cases and sexual assaults than<br />

any other town in Kenya. Sexual violence is both a global and local problem<br />

which is happening all around us. More women and children than<br />

men, are affected, and it becomes difficult to detect and prosecute the<br />

perpetuators as many incidents of rape and sexual assaults are committed<br />

by friends and close family members. Despite documentation and the fact<br />

that attention is being paid to the issue of sexual violence against women<br />

at the International and local level, the problem is getting worse. The<br />

purpose of this study is to find out why this District has been reported to<br />

have such high incidences of sexual violence. A survey will be conducted,<br />

which will involve dividing the District into 4 divisions. Questionnaires,<br />

interview schedules, key informants and focus group discussions, will be<br />

used as tools for data collection. A pilot study will be conducted in one<br />

division to validate the tools for research. Data collected will be analysed<br />

using SPSS <strong>Program</strong>me. This will assist the researcher in isolating the key<br />

factors that make Naivasha to have high prevalence of rape with a view<br />

of designing effective intervention mechanisms. It is hoped that the findings<br />

of this study, will be useful to a larger extent to the local community,<br />

Church and Government in their effort of eradicating gender violence and<br />

child defilement which has contributed greatly to the spread of HIV and<br />

AIDS.<br />

87


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-34<br />

88<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

a st r u c t u r a l mo D e l o f ca u s a l in f l u e n c e<br />

b e t w e e n ag g r e s s i o n a n D ps y c h o l o g i c a l<br />

t r a i t s: su r v e y o f fe m a l e co r r e c t i o n a l<br />

facility in ja p a n<br />

Naomi, Matsuura 1 2 ; Toshiaki, Hashimoto 3 ; Motomi, Toichi 4<br />

1 School of Education, Tokyo University and Graduate School of<br />

Social Welfare, Japan; 2 Research Center for Special Needs Education,<br />

Nara University of Education , Japan ; 3 Department of<br />

Pediatrics, Tokushima Red Cross Hinomine Rehabilitation Center<br />

for the Disabilities, Japan; 4 School of Health Science, Kyoto University<br />

Faculty of Medicine, Japan<br />

(<strong>Abstract</strong>)<br />

The purpose of this study was to survey and clarify the characteristics of<br />

self-esteem, aggressiveness, AD/HD symptoms, and ACE in inmates of a<br />

female juvenile correctional facility and age- and gender-matched controls,<br />

analyze relationships among factors, and establish a causal model.<br />

The subjects were 41 juvenile (all females) admitted to “A” Female Juvenile<br />

Correctional facility, aged 15-18. Average students at an average high<br />

school were used as control group. The inmate group showed markedly<br />

lower levels of self-esteem but no difference in aggressiveness compared<br />

with the control group. Results of AD/HD Youth Self Report suggested<br />

marked AD/HD symptoms in the inmate group even in the primary school<br />

period. Multidimensional factors correlated with their aggression. There<br />

was a possibility that low self-esteem, a high AD/HD-YSR score, and high<br />

ACE score increase aggressiveness.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-35<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

an et h o l o g i c a l pe r s p e c t i v e o n vi o l e n c e<br />

Natarajan, Deepa 1 ; Caramaschi, Doretta 1 ; deVries, Han 2 ;<br />

deBoer, Sietse 1 ; Koolhaas, Jaap 1<br />

1 Dept of Behavior Physiology, University of Groningen, The Netherlands;<br />

2 Dept of Behavioral Biology, University of Utrecht, The<br />

Netherlands<br />

Bottlenecks to violence research have been manifold incl. lack of animal<br />

models, limited methodologies (frequency, duration of aggression) and<br />

objective definitions that cannot be implemented experimentally. These<br />

factors apart, a prime concern has been in delineating violence from functional<br />

aggression in animal models. The latter is identified as functional<br />

since it has endpoints of ecological advantage incl. acquiring resources<br />

and ranking in a given environment and hence a ubiquitous phenomenon<br />

in the wild. We achieved this distinction using both, mouse strains that<br />

were genetically selected for high/ low aggression as well as novel measures<br />

of quantifying inter-male agonistic combats namely Threat/ (Attack<br />

+ Chase), Offense/ Withdrawal ratios and context dependency. Sophisticated<br />

methodologies incl. the first-order Markov chain analysis was also<br />

carried out using frequency-based agonistic behavior transition matrices<br />

of both the residents as well as their interaction with opponents. We thus,<br />

identified violence as an un-inhibited (offense-oriented) form of aggression<br />

regardless of sex, state (free-moving/ anaesthetized) and the opponent<br />

subordination signals which is at once both socially dysfunctional as<br />

well as context-independent maladaptive, leading to reduced individual/<br />

population fitness. We thus report violence as the agonistic trait in the<br />

short attack latency (SAL) mice while the agonistic trait is functional aggression<br />

in Turku Aggressive (TA) and North Carolina (NC900) mouse<br />

strains.<br />

Natarajan D and Caramaschi D contributed equally to this abstract.<br />

89


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-36<br />

90<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

th e e f f e c t o f e m o t i o n s r e s u l t e D f o r m<br />

w a t c h i n g g a m e s o n s o m e ph y s i o l o g i c a l<br />

c h a n g e s o f e l D e r p e o p l e<br />

omara, osama 1<br />

1 South valley University, kena, Egypt<br />

World health organization in 1972 emphasized that care for the old people<br />

is a moral obligation. This is obviin the forms of care as food, health, care<br />

and leisure which forms a big part in their lives. Being old is bio-state<br />

viewed by the society as retirement which affects them psychologically,<br />

physically and socially thought they have great abilities in the form of<br />

experience that con not be neglected. The research aimed at identifying<br />

the effect of the emotions resulting from watching games on some physiological<br />

changes of the old people. It hypothesized that there are significant<br />

mean differences between the mean scores of the pre-post-testing in<br />

some physiological changes favoring the pre-testing. Twofold procedures<br />

were followed, firsts is the design and second is the experimental design<br />

with its pre and post testing was implemented.<br />

The community of the research is the old people living in Old Dormitory of<br />

the ministry of social affairs 2008 (46) and the group selected were (10).<br />

Data collection tools were: Growth percentage (age- tall-weight) and tests<br />

relating to the physiological changes (blood pressure, heart beat, white<br />

and red cells, chest outer and inner breathing). Results of the research<br />

concluded that the it verified the first hypothesis and achived identifying<br />

the emotions result from watching football matches and its effect on some<br />

physiological changes of old people. Finally it recommended that leisure<br />

programs should be designed to affect positively on the physiological<br />

changes to be searched. Community and sport media should contribute in<br />

decreasing the importance of winning or loosing a match.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-37<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

ca n e v o l u t i o n a r y i n f o r m e D p r i n c i p l e s o f<br />

p a r e n t a l i n v e s t m e n t p r e D i c t p a t t e r n s o f<br />

c h i l D homiciDe in no r w a y ?<br />

Ottesen, Vibeke 1<br />

1 Centre for Research and Education in Forensic Psychiatry, Oslo,<br />

Norway<br />

Background: Following principles of modern evolutionary theory one can<br />

deduce under what conditions parental investment in offspring can be expected<br />

to increase or decrease respectively in a given species. The use of<br />

such evolutionary informed principles of parental investment has proven<br />

successful in predicting patterns of child homicide in countries such as<br />

USA, Canada, England and Wales as well as a range of traditional cultures.<br />

The increasing list of cultures where principles of parental investment have<br />

predicted patterns of child homicide testifies to the universal validity of the<br />

principles and of their inherent understanding of the psychology of child<br />

homicide. An ongoing study of child homicide in Norway caters for the opportunity<br />

to test the validity of an evolutionary informed approach to child<br />

homicide in yet another culture.<br />

Methods: The present study will use a comprehensive national sample of<br />

child homicide cases that have been prosecuted and where the defendant<br />

was found guilty of murder, voluntary or involuntary manslaughter in Norway<br />

from 1980 to present day. The method will be document analysis of<br />

court rulings and forensic psychiatric evaluations of the defendants. Theses<br />

two sources of information were chosen for the elaborate information<br />

they hold. The court rulings include narratives of the incidents in question<br />

as presented by both prosecution and the defendant. They also include<br />

background information on the defendant´s previous convictions and his<br />

or her relationship with the victim. The forensic psychiatric evaluations<br />

include information about the defendant´s mental health history and also<br />

information on his or her family background and childhood. Both the data<br />

collection and analyses will be quantitative.<br />

Results: Preliminary results are expected to be available for presentation<br />

and discussion at the conference.<br />

91


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-38<br />

92<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

a st a t i s t i c a l an a l y s i s o f cr i m e Da t a<br />

Panaretos,John, 1 2 ; Tsiamtsiouri, Alexandra 1<br />

1 Athens University of Economics and Business, Athens, Greece;<br />

2 University of California, Berkeley, USA<br />

We study and analyze Greek crime data. The data are reported crimes<br />

for the periods 1987-1997 (monthly data) and 1982-1997 (annual data).<br />

Crimes examined include Commonly Dangerous Crimes, Robbery, Theft,<br />

Murder, Rape, Arson, Personal Vengeance, Drug Violations, Smuggling of<br />

Antiquities and Beggary.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-39<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

pe r s o n a l i t y t r a i t s a n D e n D o c r i n e<br />

r e s p o n s e a s p o s s i b l e a s y m m e t r y f a c t o r s<br />

o f agonistic o u t c o m e in k a r a t e a t h l e t e s<br />

Harold, Dadomo 1 ; Alessandro, Bartolomucci 1 ; Pierfrancesco,<br />

Ferrari 1 ; Paola, Palanza 1 ; Alessandro, Vignali 2 ; Riccardo,<br />

Volpi 2 ; Stefano, Parmigiani 1<br />

1 Department of Evolutionary and Functional Biology, University of<br />

Parma, Parma,Italy.; 2 Department of Internal Medicine and Biomedical<br />

Sciences, University of Parma, Parma,Italy.<br />

Kumite (real fight) and Kata (highly ritualized fight) were studied in male<br />

Karate athletes. In Kumite the dyads were matched for weight, technical<br />

ability and body mass index to achieve symmetrical agonistic confrontations.<br />

Individual variations in the plasma levels of hormones (testosterone<br />

(T) and cortisol (C)), before and after Kumite and Kata, were measured<br />

and analysed in relation with the agonistic outcome (i.e. winning or losing)<br />

and personality trait measures. T and C increased only during Kumite<br />

contest and pre and post-competition C levels were higher in losers than<br />

winners. personality traits revealed that losers showed higher levels of<br />

harm avoidance (risk assessment), lower levels of novelty seeking and<br />

higher levels of anxiety than winners. significant correlations existed between<br />

personality traits and pre-competition C in the kumite contest with<br />

the novelty seeking negatively correlate with C and the higher the level<br />

of risk assessment, of emotionality and insecurity indexes the higher the<br />

C level. Thus personality traits might be an important factor of asymmetry<br />

between athletes influencing both the probability of winning or losing an<br />

agonistic interaction and the different anticipatory endocrine response to<br />

the incipient fight. Thus the “biological” and “psychological” measures can<br />

be used to characterize the athlete in terms of attitude and performance<br />

and to enhance individual coping strategy in challenging situations.<br />

93


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-40<br />

94<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

ne u r a l b a s i s o f f a c i a l e X p r e s s i o n<br />

p r o c e s s i n g in m a l e a D o l e s c e n t s w i t h<br />

e a r l y-o n s e t co n D u c t Di s o r D e r<br />

Passamonti,Luca, 1 Fairchild, Graeme 2 ; Hurford, Georgina<br />

2 ; Goodyer, Ian 2 ; Calder, Andy 1<br />

1 Medical Research Council, Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit,<br />

Cambridge, UK; 2 Department of Psychiatry, Cambridge University,<br />

Cambridge, UK<br />

Facial expressions serve key communicatory functions that critically modulate<br />

emotional behaviour. Individuals with Conduct Disorder (CD), particularly<br />

the early-onset subtype, show persistent aggressive-antisocial<br />

behaviour, and, as recently demonstrated, a marked impairment in recognizing<br />

facial expressions. It has been also suggested that this latter deficit,<br />

alongside with other dysfunctions, plays an important role in the development<br />

of the disorder. Although the neural substrate of this impairment is<br />

still poorly understood, recent data implicated both the amygdala and the<br />

ventromedial/orbitofrontal cortex (vmPFC/OFC) as key regions. In particular,<br />

hypoactive amygdala response to fearful faces has been shown in CD<br />

with psychopathic traits relative to controls.<br />

Here, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to further<br />

explore differences in brain activations between CD (the majority with no<br />

comorbid ADHD) and age-matched controls when viewing angry, sad, and<br />

neutral facial expressions. Our task also included null events as a low-level<br />

baseline.<br />

The amygdala, OFC, and other brain areas (superior temporal sulcus, insula,<br />

ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, intraparietal sulcus) showed reduced<br />

activation in CD relative to controls for angry vs. neutral faces. However,<br />

comparisons relative to null events showed that this effect was driven by<br />

angry and neutral expressions evoking significant but statistically equivalent<br />

activation in CD (angry=neutral), but differential level of response in<br />

controls (angry


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-41<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

vi o l e n c e , t h e f i r s t l e a D i n g c a u s e, in j u r y<br />

s u r v e i l l a n c e s y s t e m, eg y p t<br />

saad, rania 1 ; Hafez, mohammed 2 ; Mamdouh, nasser 3<br />

1 Ministry of health , injury control unit, Egypt; 2 Ministry of health<br />

, injury control unit, Egypt; 3 Ministry of health , injury control<br />

unit, Egypt<br />

The Injury problem is considered one of the top priorities of the ministry<br />

of health since 1996. Violence is the first leading cause of morbidities and<br />

the third leading cause of mortalities counting more than 25% of the injury<br />

surveillance data annually.<br />

There are differences by age and show that young adults are by far the<br />

population that bears the greatest burden of injuries due to violence.<br />

Males are more affected than females.<br />

Inspite of the high magnitude of the problem of violence in Egypt, reporting<br />

is far from the ideal and there is underestimation of the problem.<br />

Hopefully the Use of ICD10 in classifying injuries data Since 2007 improved<br />

the reporting system but more national efforts needed for implementing<br />

a national strategic plan and engage victim counselling, parenting<br />

programmes and social developmental programmes.<br />

Currently, there are gaps in our national knowledge of causes and risk factors<br />

for violence and additional research is needed.<br />

95


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-42<br />

96<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

bl o c k e D r e w a r D t r a n s l a t e s i n t o e n h a n c e D<br />

f r u s t r a t i o n a n D r e s p o n s e f o r c e<br />

Rongjun, Yu 1 ; Dean, Mobbs 1 ; Andrew, Calder 1<br />

1 Medical Research Council, Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, 15<br />

Chaucer Road, Cambridge CB2 2EF, UK<br />

Mammalian studies show that frustration is experienced when goal-directed<br />

activity is thwarted or blocked (Amsel et al., 1952). It is well known<br />

that frustration is an antecedent to aggression (Dollard et al., 1939),<br />

yet the laws governing frustration are still not well understood. We<br />

tested a theory that the greater the motivation to reach the goal, the<br />

stronger the experienced frustration if the goal is blocked. In Experiment<br />

1, using a 4 trial reward schedule, we demonstrated that as the reward<br />

became proximal, participants responded faster, suggesting that their<br />

motivation increased as the reward proximity decreased. As participants<br />

approached the reward, their self-reported frustration increased after<br />

they failed to acquire the reward. We further demonstrate that responses<br />

direct following reward blocking showed that the applied force to the<br />

pressure-sensitive buttons increased with the enhanced self-reported<br />

frustration. In Experiment 2, using a multi trial reward schedule task, we<br />

further demonstrated that experienced frustration and applied forces<br />

following missed reward increase as a function of proximity after<br />

controlling for the efforts participants have devoted. We propose that<br />

frustration may serve an energizing function which translates the<br />

unfulfilled motivation into the vigor of subsequent behavior. Thus,<br />

blockage of proximal reward leads to enhanced frustration, which induces<br />

aggressive-like behavior.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-43<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

ag g r e s s i o n in psychiatric w a r D s :<br />

inDiviDual a n D e n v i r o n m e n t a l p r e D i c t o r s<br />

Salamin, Virginie 1 2 ; Schuwey-Hayoz, Aline 1<br />

1 Réseau fribourgeois de santé mentale, Centre de soins hospitaliers,<br />

Marsens, Switzerland; 2 Département de psychologie, Université<br />

de Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland<br />

Background. Violence and aggression are quite common phenomena in<br />

mental hospitals. Patient’s psychopathology, ward or environmental characteristics<br />

and staff variables may contribute to the development of aggressive<br />

interactions. The goal of this study is to identify the correlates of<br />

aggressive incidents in adult inpatient treatment wards. Method. We conducted<br />

a prospective study in seven wards in a mental hospital (admission<br />

wards, specialized wards for mood disorders, substance abuse, psychoses<br />

and crisis intervention). All aggressive incidents were recorded with the<br />

revised Staff Observation Aggression Scale (SOAS-R) over a 24-months<br />

period. Demographical data and diagnosis were routinely assessed. Results.<br />

The sample comprised 2671 treatment episodes of 1655 patients<br />

and a total of 95’365 treatment days. We recorded 9,5% of aggressive<br />

patients, mostly males, and a rather low incidence rate (0.52 aggressions<br />

per 100 treatment days). Younger age, a longer length of stay, male<br />

gender and personality disorders were associated with a higher risk for<br />

aggressive incidents. The prevalence of aggressive behaviour is highest<br />

among substance abuse (29%) and psychotic (23%) patients. Frequency<br />

and severity of aggressive behaviour are correlated to the monthly admission<br />

(respectively r=0.47 and r=0.37, p


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-44<br />

98<br />

cr e a t i n g a s t r e s s-f r e e m i n D a n D a<br />

v i o l e n c e -f r e e w o r l D<br />

Lund, Jakob 1 ; Schaller, Nathalie 2<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

1 The International Association for Human Values (IAHV), worldwide;<br />

2 The International Art of Living Foundation (IALF), worldwide<br />

Violence is directly connected to the level of stress in our system, and<br />

stress is directly connected to our state of mind. Learning to deal with<br />

our mind and negative emotions is the key to individual peace, which is<br />

the only way to break the cycle of violence in the society. Learning about<br />

our mind could be compared to learning how to play of an instrument in<br />

order to make music instead of noise. Inside every culprit there is a victim.<br />

Victim of ignorance, small-mindedness and lack of awareness. It is<br />

the stress, lack of broad vision about life, lack of understanding, and bad<br />

communication that leads to violence in society. When there are blocks,<br />

tensions, contractions in our system, the connection to our self is cut off.<br />

The expression of violence is the attempt to release and free oneself from<br />

these tensions, not knowing how else to release them.<br />

The breath is the link between the mind, the body and the emotions. By<br />

learning how to use and control our breath, we can learn to free ourselves<br />

from stress, tensions and traumas in a constructive and safe way, allowing<br />

the life energy to circulate freely again in the system, and therefore ending<br />

the circle of violence within ourselves.<br />

For each emotion there is a corresponding pattern of breath. It is obvious<br />

that when we are angry, our breath is affected. Therefore, by attending<br />

to our breath, we can transform our emotion and our state of mind. One<br />

of the most comprehensive breathing techniques derived from the ancient<br />

Vedic science and taught by the AOLF is Sudarshan Kriya (SK). SK is understood<br />

to use specific rhythms of breath to eliminate stress, support<br />

the various organs and systems within the body, transform overpowering<br />

emotions, and restore peace of mind.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-45<br />

ri s k a s s e s s m e n t in m e n t a l h e a l t h<br />

s e r v i c e s: a n a t i o n a l m o D e l<br />

Simpson, Alexander 1<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

1 Mason Clinic and University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand<br />

Risk assessment in mental health services has been controversial and often<br />

resisted by mainstream mental health services. In New Zealand, schema<br />

in use in mental health services were often actuarial in form, resulting<br />

in static risk categorisations that are not useful in building an understanding<br />

of risk with a client. If it is to be widely applied and acceptable, risk<br />

assessment needs to be both simple and integrated with recovery based<br />

intervention. This paper describes the development of a proforma that<br />

builds on core skills of multidisciplinary mental health professionals to<br />

develop a simple and robust risk formulation that is dynamic and easily<br />

communicated with the client and others involved in their care. It communicates<br />

a shared understanding of risk issues that describes the particular<br />

pathways to risk for that individual. The proforma and accompanied training<br />

programme is now taught as the national policy for risk assessment in<br />

mental health services in New Zealand and all mental health professionals<br />

are trained in its use. The benefits and risks of such a policy will be described.<br />

99


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-46<br />

100<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

ge n D e r a n D ag g r e s s i o n: an fmri-st u D y<br />

Strüber, Daniel1 2 ; Roth, Gerhard2 ; Herrmann, Manfred3 4 ;<br />

3 4<br />

Fehr, Thorsten<br />

1 Department of General Psychology, Helmut-Schmidt-University<br />

/ University of the Federal Armed Forces Hamburg, Germany;<br />

2 Brain Research Institute, University of Bremen, Germany; 3 Center<br />

for Cognitive Sciences, Department of Neuropsychology and<br />

Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Bremen, Germany; 4 Center<br />

for Advanced Imaging, Universities of Bremen and Magdeburg,<br />

Germany<br />

It is a well-known fact that men engage in more direct aggression than<br />

women. However, the neural correlates of this gender difference remain<br />

unclear. Here we employed a novel video stimulus inventory (BRAIN–BRemen<br />

Aggression INventory) to examine the affective responses and brain<br />

activations while 11 men and 11 women viewed aggressive and neutral<br />

scenes under natural viewing conditions. Per category, 30 video clips (5-6<br />

seconds duration) were presented in a pseudorandomized order. The behavioural<br />

results showed that the aggressive scenes induced more anger<br />

and less fear in men whereas women rated their emotions as more fearful<br />

and less angry. The fMRI data showed for both males and females activation<br />

patterns in postcentral and inferior parietal regions when watching the<br />

reactive aggressive situations in contrast to neutral ones. Females further<br />

showed occipital and occipital-temporal activations possibly reflecting a<br />

more intense and fear-based analysis of the situation, whereas males<br />

showed a more anterior distribution of activations in temporal and frontal<br />

brain regions. In addition, men only showed bilateral brainstem activations<br />

which might be linked to a more pronounced tendency of attack behaviour.<br />

Basic neural network components related to aggressive behaviour might<br />

be located predominantly in subcortical structures, whereas aggressionrelated<br />

action programs might reflect individual learning histories and,<br />

therefore, be located in group-specific and/or interindividually varying<br />

neocortical networks. These findings demonstrate gender-specific brain<br />

activations during the affective processing of aggressive situations that<br />

may relate to gender differences in direct aggression.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-47<br />

violence against women,<br />

a social construction<br />

resulting from a seXist anD<br />

patriarchal society<br />

Terzidis, Amanda, Muri, Sandra 1<br />

1 Viol-Secours Association<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

Understanding violence for Viol-Secours means to deal with sexual violence<br />

against women. Sexual violence appears in different forms, ranging<br />

from sexist verbal aggression to rape which is the ultimate crime. Our<br />

analysis underlines that violence start with trivialization of sexist advertising<br />

and pornography (promotion of women as sex object, who can be<br />

used and handled), eating disorder normalization, artificial and unattainable<br />

beauty criteria, promotion of plastic surgery, to finally culminate in<br />

rape. Rape is the extreme manner for a man to show his willingness to<br />

dominate and humiliate a woman. In fact, our experience highlights that<br />

a sexual aggression has nothing to do with sexual desire and/or sexual<br />

impulse. It is the will to dominate and humiliate which predominates for<br />

the aggressor. During a rape, the aggressor behaves as though he has the<br />

power to decide if “his victim” must live or die.<br />

We explain the origin of sexual violence against women neither as a fatality<br />

nor as a “male nature”, but as a social construction. This construction<br />

is the expression of a patriarchal society which assigns differentiated and<br />

unequal roles to people according to their gender. Discriminative gender<br />

differences appear in everyday life, as much in the public as in the private<br />

area. It starts with education still based on sexist stereotypes. In general,<br />

little girls are educated to be sweet, pleasant, altruistic, passive, while little<br />

boys are encouraged to be active, independent, conqueror and strong.<br />

It continues later in a social organisation which legitimates and perpetuates<br />

gender inequality.<br />

Consequently, in order to combat sexual violence, we are taking preventive<br />

action, notably in schools, and we continue to denounce sexism in<br />

everyday life, working for that, with both women and men.<br />

101


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-48<br />

102<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

ac u t e s t r e s s h a s a l o n g -l a s t i n g<br />

e f f e c t o n o f f e n s i v e b e h a v i o u r in a<br />

D o m i n a n c e h i e r a r c h y t e s t a n D l e a D s t o a<br />

D o w n r e g u l a t i o n o f m o n o a m i n e o X i D a s e a<br />

e X p r e s s i o n in l a t e r a l s e p t u m<br />

Timmer, Marjan 1 ; Sandi, Carmen 1<br />

1 Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzer-<br />

land<br />

Stress can have a major impact on social behaviour. We use a rat model to<br />

investigate the effect of a stressful experience on the establishment and<br />

maintenance of hierarchies. On day 1, one rat of a pair is exposed to foot<br />

shocks prior to a social encounter and a food competition test (FCT). On<br />

day 8, the memory for the hierarchy established on day 1 is tested in a<br />

water competition test (WCT). Previous experiments in our group showed<br />

that rats that are exposed to a stressful experience before the first encounter<br />

usually become the subordinate individual of a pair. Furthermore,<br />

the memory that is formed for the established hierarchy is enhanced by<br />

stress compared to pairs consisting of two non-stressed rats.<br />

In the present experiment rats were submitted to stress followed by a social<br />

encounter and a FCT on day 1. In a WCT on day 8 they were exposed<br />

to an unfamiliar partner that was either dominant or subordinate on day<br />

1 (without exposure to stress). Stressed rats became the dominant individual<br />

and showed a high ratio of offensive behaviour to a previously subordinate<br />

partner in the WCT, whereas this was not the case when paired<br />

with a previously dominant partner. In addition, we studied changes in<br />

the expression of monoamine oxidase A (MAO A) mRNA expression in the<br />

lateral septum under basal conditions, one week after exposure to stress<br />

and the social encounter. We found a downregulation in MAO A expression<br />

in stressed subordinate rats compared to naïve controls, and in the pairs<br />

consisting of a stressed and a non-stressed rat compared to pairs consisting<br />

of two non-stressed rats.<br />

Our results indicate that exposure to acute stress has a long-lasting effect<br />

on offensive behaviour, which might be explained by the downregulation in<br />

MAO A in lateral septum.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-49<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

th e e v a l u a t i o n o f criminal b e h a v i o u r a n D<br />

i t s relationship t o c e r t a i n inDiviDual<br />

c h a r a c t e r i s t i c s. a s t u D y o f a g r o u p o f<br />

a D o l e s c e n t s in so u t h e r n it a l y.<br />

Tramontano, Carlo 1 ; Baralla, Francesca 1 ; Giannini, Anna<br />

Maria 1 ; Sgalla, Roberto 2 ; Marchetti, Marco 3<br />

1 Sapienza University of Rome, Italy; 2 Italian Ministry of Interior,<br />

Department of Public Security, Italy; 3 University of Molise, Italy<br />

This study is part of a broader research project aimed at understanding<br />

attitudes toward deviant behaviour within specific social and interpersonal<br />

contexts. Scientific literature has demonstrated a relationship between<br />

early problematic behaviour and the forms of antisociality and deviance in<br />

adolescence and in adult life (Moffitt, 1993; Schaeffer, et al. 1993; Loeber,<br />

Hay, 1997; Nagin, Tremblay, 1999), as well as between assent to deviant<br />

behaviour and the frequenting of groups where violations of social norms<br />

prevail. This research (supported by European funds to the Italian Ministry<br />

of Interior) focused on four target southern regions which experience<br />

relatively high levels of organized crime and criminality. A paper-and-pencil<br />

assessment was conducted in the Spring of 2007 with 1988 high school<br />

adolescents (51% male, 49% female; average age 16.43, s.d. 1.53)<br />

in eight cities in four regions. Hypothetical scenarios describing criminal<br />

behaviours were presented. To assess how each criminal behaviour<br />

was viewed by the subjects, each was asked several questions including<br />

whether or not the situation was probable, if they believed that the behaviour<br />

was in violation of the law, and if they considered the act acceptable.<br />

Afterwards, there was an evaluation of a possible correlation to certain<br />

dispositional characteristics which are associated with and which regulate<br />

conduct and behaviour understood as moral (Bandura et al., 1997), taking<br />

into consideration the results relative to four different evaluations obtained<br />

through questionnaires intended to measure, respectively: civic disengagement,<br />

“permeability to illegality”, pro-social behaviour, anti-social<br />

behaviour. Six clusters (SLEIPNER <strong>Program</strong>; Bergman, El-Khouri, 1998),<br />

were identified, separately for males and females, which presented statistically<br />

significant differences in relation to the tendency to minimize the<br />

gravity of the crime, to the evaluation of probability and to the recognition<br />

of the violation of the law for the various types of crimes considered.<br />

Such differences were found also in the choice of behaviour in answer to<br />

the hypothetical crime situations.<br />

103


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-50<br />

104<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

op p o s i t e v a s o p r e s s i n r e l e a s e p a t t e r n s<br />

within t h e b e D n u c l e u s s t r i a terminalis<br />

a n D t h e l a t e r a l s e p t u m D u r i n g t h e D i s p l a y<br />

o f i n t e r m a l e a g g r e s s i o n<br />

Veenema, Alexa 1 ; Beiderbeck, Daniela 1 ; Neumann, Inga 1<br />

1 Department of Behavioral Neuroendocrinology, Institute of Zoology,<br />

University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany<br />

Vasopressin (AVP) has been implicated in intermale aggression, but little<br />

is known about AVP neurotransmission during the display of intermale<br />

aggression. We used intracerebral microdialysis to monitor the in vivo<br />

secretory activity of AVP neurons within the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis<br />

(BST) and the lateral septum of adult male Wistar rats exposed<br />

as residents to the 10-min resident-intruder test. A significant increase in<br />

AVP release within the BST was found in non-aggressive rats compared<br />

with aggressive rats and compared with basal AVP release. The behavioral<br />

specificity of AVP release patterns was confirmed as both non-aggressive<br />

and aggressive rats showed a similar increase in AVP release when exposed<br />

to 10-min of forced swimming. Administration of the AVP V1a receptor<br />

antagonist d(CH2)5Tyr(Me)AVP into the BST of non-aggressive rats<br />

did not increase the level of aggression. However, application of synthetic<br />

AVP into the BST of aggressive rats significantly reduced the level of aggression.<br />

In contrast to the BST, AVP release within the septum was significantly<br />

increased in aggressive rats compared with non-aggressive rats<br />

and compared with basal AVP release. Pharmacological manipulation of<br />

the septal AVP system by local application of either synthetic AVP to nonaggressive<br />

rats or the specific V1a receptor antagonist to aggressive rats<br />

did not change the level of aggression. Taken together, we are the first to<br />

show that the display of intermale aggression is associated with distinct<br />

AVP release patterns within the BST and septum. Our data further indicate<br />

that AVP release within the BST directly affects the display of aggression,<br />

whereas AVP release within the lateral septum may rather be the consequence<br />

of the display of aggression and may have delayed effects on<br />

aggression-related behaviors, like anxiety and social memory.<br />

This work was supported by the Bayerische Forschungsstiftung (AHV) and<br />

the Deutsche Forschungsstiftung (IDN).


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-51<br />

ag g r e s s i v e r e s p o n s e s t o t r o u b l e D<br />

s i t u a t i o n s in s a m p l e o f a D o l e s c e n t s:<br />

Vigil-Colet, A 1 ; Lorenzo-Seva, U 1 ; Morales-Vives, F 1<br />

1 Universitat Rovira i Virgili, Research Center for Behavioral As-<br />

sessment.<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

We developed a questionnaire for measuring aggressive behaviors taking<br />

into account different situations where these behaviors are executed. The<br />

questionnaire consisted of six situations (three related to unknown people<br />

and three related to relatives) and seven possible behaviours (supposedly<br />

related to physical and verbal aggression and to anger) which was<br />

administered to 350 adolescents . The corresponding person x situation<br />

x response data was analysed using a three-mode component analysis.<br />

Furthermore 127 participants completed also the Buss &Perry (1992) aggression<br />

questionnaire and the Dysfunctional Scale of Dickman’s Impulsivity<br />

Inventory (Dickman, 1991). The amount of variance explained by the<br />

persons x situations x responses interaction justified the three-mode component<br />

approach. Results showed the expected two latent dimensions for<br />

situations (unknown people and relatives) and three latent dimensions for<br />

responses (physical aggression, verbal aggression and anger). In addition<br />

we founded five latent dimensions for persons that reflected five different<br />

profiles which reflected the existence of interactions between the response<br />

components and the situations. When the scores on the five components<br />

where related to AQ and impulsivity, results indicated that traditional<br />

questionnaires such as them only can predict the scores of the component<br />

“physical aggression with unknowns”.<br />

105


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-52<br />

106<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

ge n e -e n v i r o n m e n t a l i n t e r p l a y o n t h e<br />

a s s o c i a t i o n b e t w e e n a g g r e s s i v e b e h a v i o r<br />

a n D a l c o h o l D e p e n D e n c e<br />

von der Pahlen, Bettina 1 ; Johansson, Ada 1 ; Sandnabba, N.<br />

Kenneth 1 ; Santtila, Pekka 1<br />

1 Center of Excellence in Behavior Genetics, Department of Psychology,<br />

Åbo Akademi University, Turku, Finland<br />

We investigated the interplay between genetic and environmental effects<br />

on the association between alcohol dependence and aggressive behaviour<br />

as well as sex differences in this interplay. There were 3141 male (mean<br />

age 26.2 [S.E. = 0.1]) and 6026 female (mean age 26.1[S.E. = 0.1]) participants<br />

in this extended twin study. Alcohol Use Disorder Identification<br />

Test (AUDIT), and two subscales of Aggression Questionnaire (AQ) were<br />

used to assess the phenotypes. The phenotypes were positively associated<br />

with men exhibiting higher levels on both phenotypes. In the model fitting,<br />

we explored first how the level of alcohol use moderated the genetic<br />

and environmental effects on aggressive behaviour separately for men<br />

and women. Second, we explored in similar fashion how the level of aggressive<br />

behaviour moderated the genetic and environmental effects on<br />

alcohol dependence. The implications of the results for understanding the<br />

connection between these two phenotypes will be discussed.


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

P-53<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

ag g r e s s i o n: bi o p s y c h o s o c i a l me c h a n i s m s<br />

Wahl, Klaus 1<br />

1 German Youth Institute (Deutsches Jugendinstitut) and Univer-<br />

sity of Munich, Germany<br />

We try to close the gap between biological, psychological and sociological<br />

approaches to aggression and violence. In order to do this we are looking<br />

at the mechanisms connecting systems for aggression at the genetic,<br />

neuronal, psychic, and social levels.<br />

After a broad study of new research findings in the different sciences we<br />

recently started an empirical research project combining brain studies<br />

(EEG, fMRI), psychological tests (attachment, affective reactions etc.) and<br />

biographic interviews with violent perpetrators and a control group ( n =<br />

24 in the pre-study). This interdisciplinary study is done in cooperation<br />

with researchers from German and Austrian universities (Bremen, Magdeburg,<br />

Heidelberg, Kassel,Innsbruck).<br />

107


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

108<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong>


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

Poster <strong>Abstract</strong>s<br />

109


Participants<br />

List


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

ADEGBITE, Timothy Folagbade<br />

E. I NIGERIA LIMITED<br />

OSOSAMI<br />

NG-23402 IBADAN<br />

timo7772003@yahoo.com<br />

ALSAHEB, Maha<br />

Woman&Child Healt/family violence section<br />

MOH Jordan<br />

Alshesani st. abdullah bin Omar st.42<br />

JO-11195 Amman<br />

maha_saheb@hotmail.com<br />

ANDARI, Elissar<br />

UMR 5229<br />

Centre de Neuroscience Cognitive<br />

67 BD Pinel<br />

FR-69675 Bron<br />

elissar_andary@hotmail.com<br />

ANDARI, Elissar<br />

UMR 5229<br />

Centre de Neuroscience Cognitive<br />

67 bd pinel bron<br />

FR-69675 Bron<br />

elissar_andary@hotmail.com<br />

ANDARI, Elissar<br />

UMR 5229<br />

Centre de Neuroscience Cognitive<br />

67 BOULEVARD PINEL<br />

FR-69675 Bron<br />

elissar_andary@hotmail.com<br />

ANDRES-PUEYO, antonio<br />

Psicologia de la Personalitat- Facultad de Psicologia<br />

Barcelona University<br />

Passeig Vall Hebron 171<br />

ES-08035 barcelona<br />

andrespueyo@ub.edu<br />

ANDREW, Calder<br />

MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit<br />

15, Chaucer Road<br />

GB-CB2 7EF Cambridge<br />

andy.calder@mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk<br />

ARCHER, John<br />

Psychology<br />

University of Central Lancashire<br />

Marsh Lane<br />

GB-PR1 2 HE Preston<br />

jarcher@uclan.ac.uk<br />

112<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

ARINAITWE, PATRICK<br />

REPRODUCTIVE HEALTH<br />

KADINET<br />

kampala,UGANDA<br />

UG-+256 KAMPALA<br />

arinaitwepatrick@yahoo.com<br />

ASSAL, Emmanuelle<br />

CTAS-Association Centre thérapeutique abus<br />

sexuels<br />

36 bd st Georges,<br />

CH-1205 Genève<br />

e.assal@hotmail.com<br />

AVONTROODT, Yolande<br />

Evens Foundation<br />

Van Breestraat 14<br />

BE-2018 Antwerpen<br />

ef@evensfoundation.be<br />

BABEL-GUERIN, Eliane<br />

Direction<br />

Croix-rouge genevoise<br />

9 route des Acacias<br />

CH-1211 Genève 4<br />

e.babel.guerin@croix-rouge-ge.ch<br />

BARALLA, Francesca<br />

Department of Psychology<br />

Faculty of Psychology, Sapienza University of<br />

Rome<br />

Via dei Marsi, 78<br />

IT-00185 Rome<br />

francesca.baralla@uniroma1.it<br />

BARBE, Remy<br />

DEA/Child Psychiatry<br />

University Hospital Geneva<br />

6 Rue Willy Donzé<br />

CH-1211 Genève<br />

remy.barbe@hcuge.ch<br />

BEDIOU, Benoit<br />

CISA Centre for Affective Sciences<br />

University of Geneva<br />

rue des battoirs, 7<br />

CH-1206 Geneva<br />

benoit.bediou@unige.ch<br />

BERNOULLI, Nicole<br />

Salève<br />

SPEA<br />

Petit-Bel-Air 2<br />

CH-1225 Chêne-Bourg<br />

nicole.bernoullifiaux@hcuge.ch


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

BERTSCH, Katja<br />

Department of Psychology<br />

University of Trier<br />

Universitätsring 15<br />

DE-54286 Trier<br />

bert1301@uni-trier.de<br />

BINNERT, Christophe<br />

Centre de Recherche Nestlé<br />

route du Jorat<br />

CH-1000 Lausanne<br />

christophe.binnert@rdls.nestle.com<br />

BISAZ, Reto<br />

Brain Mind institute, Science de la vie<br />

Ecole Polytechnique Federal de Lausanne<br />

(<strong>EPFL</strong>)<br />

Ecublens<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

reto.bisaz@epfl.ch<br />

BLANCHARD, Caroline<br />

Department of Genetics and Molecular Biology<br />

Pacific Biosciences Research Institute, University<br />

of Hawaii; and John A. Burns School of<br />

Medicine<br />

1993 East-West Road,<br />

US-96825 Honolulu<br />

blanchar@hawaii.edu<br />

BLANKE, Olaf<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

BMI<br />

Box 19<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

olaf.blanke@epfl.ch<br />

BOLEA, Sonia<br />

LN Doctoral School<br />

Biophore 2204, Unil-Sorge<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

sonia.bolea@unil.ch<br />

BONDAR, Natalia<br />

Neurogenetics of Social Behavior Sector<br />

Institute of Cytology and Genetics SD RAS<br />

Lavrentiev Ave, 10<br />

RU-630090 Novosibirsk<br />

nbondar@bionet.nsc.ru<br />

BONNARD, SABINE<br />

Dpt fédéral de l’économie<br />

Station de recherche agroscope Changins-<br />

Wädenswil, ACW<br />

rte de Duillier<br />

CH-1260 NYON<br />

sabine.bonnard@acw.admin.ch<br />

BREUILLAUD, lionel<br />

CNP CHUV SUPEA<br />

Site de cery, CNP 1er etage<br />

CH-1008 Prilly<br />

lionel.breuillaud@chuv.ch<br />

BROOM, Mark<br />

Department of Mathematics<br />

University of Sussex<br />

Mantell Building,<br />

GB-BN1 9RF Brighton<br />

m.broom@sussex.ac.uk<br />

Participants List<br />

BUTCHART, Alexander<br />

Violence and Injury Prevention and Disability<br />

World Health Organization<br />

Avenue Appia 20<br />

CH-1211 Geneva 27<br />

butcharta@who.int<br />

CADAS, Hugues<br />

Department of Cell Biology and Morphology<br />

Faculty of Biology and Medecine<br />

Rue du Bugnon 9<br />

CH-1005 Lausanne<br />

hugues.cadas@unil.ch<br />

CARAMASCHI, Doretta<br />

Dept. Behavioral Physiology<br />

University of Groningen<br />

Kerklaan 30<br />

NL-9750 AA Haren<br />

d.caramaschi@rug.nl<br />

CARDINAUX, Jean-René<br />

Department of Psychiatry-CHUV<br />

Center for Psychiatric Neuroscience<br />

Hospital of Cery<br />

CH-1008 Prilly-Lausanne<br />

jean-rene.cardinaux@chuv.ch<br />

CASTRO, Jorge<br />

SV/BMI/LGC<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

Station 15<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

jorge.castro@epfl.ch<br />

113


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

CHARDONNENS, Evelyne<br />

Institut de psychologie<br />

Unil<br />

1015 Dorigny Lausanne<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

evelyne.chardonnens@unil.ch<br />

CHICO, ELISEO<br />

Psicología<br />

Universidad Rovira i Virgili<br />

Carretera de Valls s/n<br />

ES-43007 Tarragona<br />

eliseo.chico@urv.cat<br />

CODORNIU-RAGA, Maria Jose<br />

Psychology<br />

Universitat Rovira i Virgili<br />

Carretera de Valls s/n<br />

ES-43007 Tarragona<br />

mariajose.codorniu@urv.cat<br />

COID, Jeremy<br />

Forensic Psychiatry Research Unit<br />

St Barts and the London Medical School,<br />

Queen Mary University of London<br />

William Harvey House<br />

61 bartholomew close<br />

GB-EC1A 7BE London<br />

j.w.coid@qmul.ac.uk<br />

CONBOY, Lisa<br />

Life Science<br />

Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne,<br />

SV-BMI-LGC, Bat. AAB 201,<br />

Station 15,<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

lisa.conboy@epfl.ch<br />

CORDERO, María Isabel<br />

BMI LGC<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

Ecublens<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

isabel.cordero@epfl.ch<br />

COSI MUñOZ, Sandra<br />

Psychology<br />

Universidad Rovira i Virgili<br />

Carretera de Valls s/n<br />

ES-43007 Tarragona<br />

alexandra.cosi@urv.cat<br />

114<br />

COTTINGHAM, Emily<br />

Psychology faculty<br />

University of Geneva<br />

40, bvd du Pont d’Arve<br />

CH-1211 Genève 4<br />

emily.cottingham@unige.ch<br />

CRISTOFORI, Irene<br />

UMR 5229<br />

Centre de Neuroscience Cognitive<br />

67 bd Pinel<br />

FR-69675 Bron<br />

irenecristofori@yahoo.it<br />

DAVID, Swapp<br />

Computer Science<br />

University College London<br />

Malet Place<br />

GB-WC1E 6BT London<br />

d.swapp@cs.ucl.ac.uk<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

DE BOER, Sietse<br />

Behavioral Physiology<br />

University of Groningen, Biological Center<br />

Kerklaan 30<br />

NL-9750 AA Haren<br />

s.f.de.boer@rug.nl<br />

DE BRITO, stephane<br />

Forensic Mental Health Science<br />

Institute of Psychiatry<br />

De Crespigny Park<br />

GB-SE5 8AF London<br />

stephane.debrito@iop.kcl.ac.uk<br />

DE KLOET, Ron<br />

Department of Medical Pharmacology<br />

Leiden University, LACDR<br />

Einsteinweg 55<br />

NL-2300RA Leiden<br />

e.kloet@lacdr.leidenuniv.nl<br />

DE SANTIS, Laura<br />

Psychiatry, CNP<br />

CHUV<br />

Cery<br />

CH-1008 Prilly/Lausanne<br />

laura.de-santis@chuv.ch<br />

DERIVOIS, DANIEL<br />

Institute of Psychology<br />

University of Lyon<br />

5, Ave Pierre Mendes France<br />

FR-69676 BRON<br />

daniel.derivois@univ-lyon2.fr


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

DOENLEN, Raphael<br />

Center of Phenogenomics<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

Station 19<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

raphael.doenlen@epfl.ch<br />

DOTTO, gian-paolo dotto<br />

University of Lausanne<br />

Department of Biochemistry<br />

Ch. des Boveresses 155<br />

CH-1066 Epalinges<br />

gian-paolo.dotto@unil.ch<br />

DUGATKIN, Lee<br />

Department of Biology<br />

University of Louisville<br />

Life Sciences Building<br />

US-40241 Louisville<br />

lee.dugatkin@louisville.edu<br />

D’ACREMONT, Mathieu<br />

Laboratory for Decision-Making under Uncertainty<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

Station 5<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

mathieu.dacremont@epfl.ch<br />

D’AMBROSIO, Arianna<br />

Vires, Organisme de traitement et de prévention<br />

des violences exercées dans le couple et<br />

la famille. Genève<br />

rue Gilbert, 17<br />

CH-1217 Meyrin<br />

arianna.dambrosio@libero.it<br />

D’ANDREA, Ivana<br />

Section of Behavioural Neurosciences, Department<br />

of Cell Biology and Neuroscience<br />

Istituto Superiore di Sanità<br />

Viale Regina Elena<br />

IT-00161 Rome<br />

ivana.dandrea@iss.it<br />

EBSTEIN, Richard<br />

Psychology Department<br />

Hebrew University<br />

Mt. Scopus Campus<br />

IL-91035 Jerusalem<br />

rpebstein@gmail.com<br />

EMILIE, Bovet<br />

FBM / CHUV<br />

IUHMSP<br />

Falaises 1<br />

CH-1005 Lausanne<br />

emilie.bovet@chuv.ch<br />

ERKOHEN, Miriam<br />

retired<br />

ch. de la Damataire 26<br />

CH-1009 Pully<br />

merkohen@hotmail.com<br />

EVENS, Corinne<br />

Fondation Evens<br />

7 rue Beautreillis<br />

FR-75004 Paris<br />

secretariat.ce@siiffa.com<br />

FAIRCHILD, Graeme<br />

Developmental Psychiatry Section<br />

Cambridge University<br />

Douglas House, 18b Trumpington Road<br />

GB-CB2 8AH Cambridge<br />

gff22@cam.ac.uk<br />

FANTIN, Martina<br />

SV-BMI<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

Station 15 1015 Lausanne<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

martina.fantin@epfl.ch<br />

FEHR, Thorsten<br />

Center for Cognitive Sciences<br />

University of Bremen<br />

Hochschulring 18<br />

DE-28359 Bremen<br />

fehr@uni-bremen.de<br />

FIDA, roberta<br />

psychology<br />

sapienza university of rome<br />

via dei marsi 78<br />

IT-00185 Roma<br />

roberta.fida@uniroma1.it<br />

FRANKEN, Paul<br />

Center for Integrative Genomics<br />

University of Lausanne<br />

Genopode building<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne-Dorigny<br />

paul.franken@unil.ch<br />

Participants List<br />

115


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

GAILLARD, Francois<br />

Psychology Institute<br />

UNIL<br />

Ch. du Trabandan 17<br />

CH-1006 Lausanne<br />

francois.gaillard@unil.ch<br />

GALYONA, Eugene<br />

Philosophy<br />

Karazin Kharkov National University<br />

4, Svoboda sq.<br />

UA-61000 Kharkov<br />

lux84@mail.ru<br />

GAO, Virginia<br />

BMI<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

18 Place du Tunnel<br />

CH-1005 Lausanne<br />

virginia.gao@gmail.com<br />

GARCIA, Clara<br />

Nutrition and Health<br />

Nestle Research Center<br />

Vers chez les Blanc<br />

CH-1000 Lausanne<br />

clara.garcia@rdls.nestle.com<br />

GEHRING, Alison<br />

Bute Medical School<br />

University of St Andrews<br />

Bute Building<br />

St Andrews<br />

GB-KY16 9TS Fife<br />

am2449@st-andrews.ac.uk<br />

GENOUX, David<br />

Laboratory of Behavioral Genetics<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

ecublens<br />

CH-1005 Lausanne<br />

david.genoux@epfl.ch<br />

GHORBI, banafsheh<br />

Nursing and Midwifery<br />

Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Science<br />

& Health Services<br />

29 Nobakht st. Apadana ave.<br />

IR-15547 Tehran<br />

b_ghorbi75@yahoo.com<br />

116<br />

GOMEZ, Jean-Marc<br />

Psychology<br />

University of Geneva<br />

7, rue des Battoirs<br />

CH-1205 Geneva<br />

jean-marc.gomez@unige.ch<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

GONZALEZ, Sonia<br />

Point du Jour Production<br />

23,rue de Cronstadt<br />

FR-75015 Paris<br />

sonia.gonzalez@pointdujour.fr<br />

GRAVIER, Bruno<br />

Dept de Psychiatrie/CHUV<br />

Service de Médecine et de Psychiatrie Pénitentiaires<br />

Site de Cery<br />

CH-1008 Prilly<br />

bruno.gravier@chuv.ch<br />

GREGORY, Sarah<br />

Dept of Forensic Mental Health Sciences<br />

Insitute of Psychiatry<br />

De Crespigny Park, Denmark Hill<br />

GB-SE5 8AF London<br />

sarah_gregory@iop.kcl.ac.uk<br />

GRENNINGLOH, Gabriele<br />

Brain Mind Institute<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong>-SV-BMI<br />

Station 19<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

gabriele.grenningloh@epfl.ch<br />

GUIDI, Raffaella<br />

Centre for Psychiatric Neuroscience<br />

CHUV / UNIL<br />

Hôpital de Cery, 1er étage<br />

CH-1006 Prilly<br />

raffaella.guidi@chuv.ch<br />

GUIGNARD, Gabriela<br />

Quality and Safety<br />

Nestec SA<br />

Vers-chez-les-Blanc<br />

CH-1000 LAUSANNE 26<br />

gabriela.guignard@rdls.nestle.com<br />

GUILLIER, Nathalie<br />

Institute of Psychology<br />

University of Lyon<br />

5, ave Pierre Mendès France<br />

FR-69676 BRON<br />

nathalie-g@hotmail.fr


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

HALFON, Olivier<br />

Service Universitaire de Psychiatrie de<br />

l’Enfance et de l’Adolescence<br />

CHUV<br />

Rue du Bugnon 23A<br />

CH-1011 Lausanne<br />

olivier.halfon@chuv.ch<br />

HASSAN, Ghada<br />

<strong>Program</strong>s & Research<br />

Dubai Foundation for Women & Children<br />

Al Awir Road<br />

AE-97727 DUBAI<br />

dr.ghada@dfwac.ae<br />

HAYMOZ PANTILLON, Sandrine<br />

Penal law and Criminology<br />

University of Zurich<br />

Ch. des secrétaires 3<br />

CH-1442 Montagny<br />

sandrine.haymoz@unil.ch<br />

HERPERTZ, Sabine C.<br />

Psychiatry and Psychotherapy<br />

Rostock University<br />

Gehlsheimer Str. 20<br />

DE-18147 Rostock<br />

sabine.herpertz@med.uni-rostock.de<br />

HERPERTZ, Sabine C.<br />

Clinic for Psychiatry and Psychotherapy<br />

University Rostock<br />

Gehlsheimer Str. 20<br />

DE-18147 Rostock<br />

sabine.herpertz@med.uni-rostock.de<br />

HERPERTZ, Sabine C.<br />

Clinic for Psychiatry and Psychotherapy<br />

University Rostock<br />

Gehlsheimer Straße 20<br />

DE-18147 Rostock<br />

sabine.herpertz@med.uni-rostock.de<br />

HIRLING, Harald<br />

SV - Decanat<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

SV 3809<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

harald.hirling@epfl.ch<br />

Participants List<br />

HODGINS, Sheilagh<br />

Department of Forensic Mental Health<br />

Institute of Psychiatry at King’s College London<br />

De Crespigny Park<br />

Denmark Hill<br />

GB-SE5 8AF London<br />

s.hodgins@iop.kcl.ac.uk<br />

HOFNER, Marie-Claude<br />

CURML (University Center for Legal Medicine)<br />

CHUV - FBM<br />

César Roux 19<br />

CH-1003 Lausanne<br />

marie-claude.hofner@chuv.ch<br />

HOFNER, Marie-Claude<br />

University Hospital Center Lausanne<br />

Violence Medical Unit<br />

Rue César Roux 19<br />

CH-1005 Lausanne<br />

marie-claude.hofner@chuv.ch<br />

HUBERT VAN BLYENBURGH, Ninian<br />

Département d’anthropologie et écologie<br />

Université de Genève<br />

12 rue Gustave Revilliod<br />

CH-1211 Genève 4<br />

ninian.hubert@unige.ch<br />

ISLAM, Dr. S. M. Shariful<br />

International Health<br />

Partners in Population and Development<br />

B-3, Road-1, Niketan, Gulshan-1<br />

BD-1212 Dhaka<br />

sharif.undp@gmail.com<br />

IYER, Keerthana<br />

SV<br />

BMI, LGC<br />

Batiment AAB 115, Station 15<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

keets85@gmail.com<br />

JAQUIER, Veronique<br />

Ecole des sciences criminelles UNIL<br />

Institut de criminologie et de droit pénal<br />

BCH<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

veronique.jaquier@unil.ch<br />

JAURY, Marie<br />

Point du Jour Production<br />

23, rue de Cronstadt<br />

FR-75015 Paris<br />

mjaury@hotmail.fr<br />

117


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

JÄGGI, Sarah<br />

IFB<br />

Marktgasse 29<br />

CH-3000 Bern<br />

sarah.jaeggi@ifkjb.ch<br />

JOHNSTON-WENGER, Nathalie<br />

DBCM<br />

Université de Lausanne<br />

Rue du Bugnon 9<br />

CH-1005 Lausanne<br />

nathalie.johnston-wenger@unil.ch<br />

JOSHI, Shambhu D.<br />

Health<br />

District Public health Office<br />

c/o Gokarna-9, KTM, Nepal<br />

NP-n/a Malakheti-3<br />

dr.sdjoshi@yahoo.com<br />

JOSHUA, Buckholtz<br />

Department of Psychology; Vanderbilt Brain<br />

Institute<br />

Vanderbilt University<br />

Vanderbilt University<br />

PMB 407817<br />

2301 Vanderbilt Place<br />

US-37240-7817 Nashville<br />

joshua.buckholtz@vanderbilt.edu<br />

JOZSEF, Haller<br />

Behavioral Neurobiology<br />

Institute of Experimental Medicine<br />

43 Szigony street<br />

1083 Budapest<br />

Hungary<br />

HU-1450 Budapest<br />

haller@koki.hu<br />

JULIEN, Marquis<br />

SV-GHI-LVG<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

Station 15<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

julien.marquis@epfl.ch<br />

KÖNIG, Chantal<br />

Kant. BEObachtungsstation Bolligen<br />

Hühnerbühlstrasse 206<br />

CH-3065 Bolligen<br />

chantal.koenig@jgk.be.ch<br />

118<br />

KELLER, Lucas<br />

IFB<br />

Im Surinam 156<br />

CH-4058 Basel<br />

lucas.keller@ifkjb.ch<br />

KHAN, Robina<br />

Department of Psychology<br />

University of Trier<br />

Universitätsring 15<br />

DE-54286 Trier<br />

khan1301@uni-trier.de<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

KIEHL, Kent<br />

Psychology and Neuroscience<br />

Univ of New Mexico & Mind Research Network<br />

1101 Yale Blvd NE<br />

Mind Research Network<br />

US-87131 Albuquerque<br />

kkiehl@unm.edu<br />

KLUSEMANN, Stefan<br />

Department of Sociology<br />

University of Pennsylvania<br />

3718 Locust Walk, McNeil Building, Suite<br />

113<br />

US-19104-6299 Philadelphia<br />

stefankl@ssc.upenn.edu<br />

KOOLHAAS, Jaap M<br />

Behavioral Physiology<br />

University Groningen<br />

Kerklaan 30<br />

9751 NN Haren<br />

The Netherlands<br />

NL-9750AA Haren<br />

j.m.koolhaas@rug.nl<br />

KREIS, Christian<br />

School of Criminal Justice, Institute of Criminology<br />

and Penal Law<br />

University of Lausanne<br />

Schneitstrasse 11<br />

CH-6315 Oberaegeri<br />

christian.kreis@unil.ch<br />

KUDRYAVTSEVA, Natalia<br />

Neurogenetics of Social Behavior Sector<br />

Institute of Cytology and Genetics SD RAS<br />

pr. ak. Lavrentjeva, 10<br />

RU-630090 Novosibirsk<br />

natnik@bionet.nsc.ru


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

KUHN, André<br />

Institut de Criminologie et de Droit Pénal<br />

Ecole des sciences criminelles<br />

Université de Lausanne<br />

Internef<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

andre.kuhn@unil.ch<br />

KUHN, Fabienne Kuhn<br />

Psychology<br />

Bern<br />

Missionsstrasse<br />

CH-4055 Basel<br />

fabiennekuhn1982@yahoo.com<br />

KUNZ, Tina<br />

Nutrition & Health<br />

Nestlé Research Center<br />

Vers-chez-les-Blanc<br />

CH-1000 Lausanne 26<br />

tina.kunz@rdls.nestle.com<br />

LARSEN, Marianne Hald<br />

Laboratory of Behavioral Genetics<br />

Ecole Polytechnique Federale de Lausanne<br />

AAB201 (Bâtiment AAB),<br />

Station 15<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

marianne.larsen@epfl.ch<br />

LAVANCHY, Tom<br />

LNCO<br />

BMI<br />

Ruelle des Châtaigniers 1<br />

CH-1026 Denges<br />

tom.lavanchy@epfl.ch<br />

LESCH, K. P.<br />

Department of Psychiatry<br />

University of Wuerzburg<br />

Fuechsleinstrasse 15<br />

DE-97080 Wuerzburg<br />

kplesch@mail.uni-wuerzburg.de<br />

LEVINE, Mark<br />

Department Of Psychology<br />

Lancaster University<br />

Fylde College<br />

GB-LA1 4YF Lancaster<br />

m.levine@lancaster.ac.uk<br />

LIGHTOWLERS, Carly<br />

CCSR<br />

Manchester University<br />

School of Social Sciences<br />

Kantorovich Building,<br />

Humanities Bridgeford Street<br />

University of Manchester<br />

GB-M13 9PL Manchester<br />

carly.lightowlers@postgrad.manchester.ac.uk<br />

LINDE, ANTONIA<br />

FACULTE DE DROIT ET SCIENCES CRIM-<br />

INELLES<br />

ECOLE DES SCIENCES CRIMINELLES<br />

rue Mornex 5B<br />

CH-1003 LAUSANNE<br />

tlinde75@hotmail.com<br />

LINGNAU, Eva<br />

Education and Behaviour Unit<br />

International Committee of the Red Cross<br />

19 Avenue de la Paix<br />

CH-1202 Geneva<br />

elingnau@icrc.org<br />

LOESCHE, Urs Caspar<br />

Law<br />

University Bern<br />

sulgenbachstr.29<br />

CH-3007 Bern<br />

caspar@students.unibe.ch<br />

LUCIA, Sonia<br />

Institut de criminologie et de droit pénal<br />

Ecole des Sciences Criminelles<br />

Batochime<br />

Dorigny<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

sonia.luciaesseiva@unil.ch<br />

Participants List<br />

LUDIVINE, challet<br />

département de zoologie et biologie animal<br />

Universite de genève<br />

Quai ernest ansermet 30<br />

CH-1200 geneva<br />

challet3@etu.unige.ch<br />

LUKSYS, Gediminas<br />

LGC / LCN<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

Rue J-J-Cart 1<br />

CH-1006 Lausanne<br />

gediminas.luksys@epfl.ch<br />

119


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

LUND, Jakob<br />

Director, International Breathe SMART programs<br />

The International Assoc. for Human Values<br />

Rue du Lac<br />

DK-1000 Copenhagen<br />

breathesmart@mail.dk<br />

LUTHI-CARTER, Ruth<br />

Brain Mind Institute<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

Station 15<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

ruth.luthi-carter@epfl.ch<br />

MACHADO, Françoise<br />

Neuropsychologie<br />

Ensemble Hospitalier de la Côte<br />

Hôpital de Gilly<br />

CH-1182 Gilly<br />

francoise.machado@ehc.vd.ch<br />

MAGISTRETTI, Pierre<br />

BMI/LNDC<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

SV 2511<br />

Station 19<br />

1015 Lausanne<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

pierre.magistretti@epfl.ch<br />

MARCHETTI, Marco<br />

Department of Health Sciences<br />

University of Molise<br />

Via De Santis, snc<br />

IT-86100 Campobasso<br />

marco.marchetti@unimol.it<br />

MARQUEZ, Cristina<br />

Laboratory of Behavioral Genetics<br />

BMI <strong>EPFL</strong><br />

AAB 2 01 (Bâtiment AAB)<br />

Station 19<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

cristina.marquez@epfl.ch<br />

MARQUIS, Julien<br />

SV-GHI-LVG<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

Station 15<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

julien.marquis@epfl.ch<br />

120<br />

MARTI FAVRE, Maude<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong>/SV/BMI<br />

Brain and Mind Institute<br />

station 15<br />

CH-1004 lausanne<br />

maude.marti@epfl.ch<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

MÜLLER, Sonja<br />

Institut de psychologie du travail et des organisations<br />

Université de Neuchâtel<br />

Rue de la Maladière 23<br />

CH-2000 Neuchâtel<br />

sonja.mueller@gmx.ch<br />

MÜLLER-ISBERNER, Rüdiger<br />

Haina Forensic Psychiatric Hospital<br />

Hoher-Lohr-Weg 10<br />

DE-D-35114 Haina<br />

rmi@psych-haina.de<br />

MCLEOD, Carol<br />

Scottish Violence Reduction Unit<br />

Strathclyde Police<br />

!st Floor, Pegasus House<br />

375 West George Street<br />

GB-G2 4LW Glasgow<br />

carol.mcleod@strathclyde.pnn.police.<br />

uk<br />

MESNIL, Yves<br />

Point du jour production<br />

23, rue de Cronstadt<br />

FR-75015 Paris<br />

y.mesnil@free.fr<br />

MEYER-LINDENBERG, Andreas<br />

Director<br />

Central Institute of Mental Health<br />

Square J 5<br />

DE-68159 Mannheim<br />

a.meyer-lindenberg@zi-mannheim.de<br />

MIKOSZ, Marta J.<br />

Department of Neurophysiology<br />

Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish<br />

Academy of Sciences<br />

3 Pasteur St.<br />

PL-02-093 Warsaw<br />

m.mikosz@nencki.gov.pl


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

MIKTON, Christopher<br />

Prevention of Violence Unit<br />

World Health Organization<br />

20 Avenue Appia<br />

CH-1211 Geneva<br />

miktonc@who.int<br />

MILTON, Mark<br />

Education 4 Peace Foundation<br />

Grand Rue 35<br />

CH-1180 Rolle<br />

michele@education4peace.org<br />

MIRABAUD, Madeleine<br />

Groupe de Protection de l’Enfant<br />

Hôpital des Enfants/HUG<br />

6 Rue Willy Donzé<br />

CH-1211 Genève 14<br />

madeleine.mirabaud@hcuge.ch<br />

MORGENTHALER, Florence<br />

Center for BioMedical Imaging (CIBM)<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

SB IPMC LIFMET<br />

CH F1, station 6<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

florence.morgenthaler@epfl.ch<br />

MOTTAZ, Hélène<br />

biology<br />

CIG / UNIL<br />

Quartier UNIL-Sorge<br />

Bâtiment Génopode<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

helene.mottaz@unil.ch<br />

MUCHOKI, Samuel<br />

Department of Culture and Heritage<br />

Nationa, Museums of Kenya<br />

Museum Hill<br />

KE-00100 Nairobi<br />

smmuchoki@yahoo.com<br />

MUELLER-ISBERNER, Ruediger<br />

Haina Forensic Psychiatric Hospital<br />

Hoher-Lohr-Weg 10<br />

DE-D-35114 Haina<br />

rmi@psych-haina.de<br />

MUGENZI, DESIRE<br />

REFORMED TECHNOLOGICAL/THEOLOGICAL<br />

COLLEGE<br />

UG-000256 KAMPALA<br />

figaidug2000@yahoo.com<br />

MURAEB, maher<br />

SOCIOLOGY<br />

GULMA UNIVER<br />

GULMA UNIVERCITY- BUIL/01 N/05<br />

DZ-00123 GULMA<br />

almuraeb@hotmail.fr<br />

MWANGI, Maryann<br />

St. Paul’s Institute of Life-long Learning<br />

St. Paul’s University<br />

Kabuku<br />

KE-254 Limuru<br />

maryann.spill@stpaulslimuru.ac.ke<br />

NADRA, Karim<br />

Department of medical genetics<br />

University of Lausanne<br />

rue du bugnon 27<br />

CH-1005 Lausanne<br />

karim.nadra@unil.ch<br />

NAOMI, MATSUURA<br />

Tokyo University of Social Welfare<br />

2-13-32, Marunouchi, Nakaku<br />

JP-460-0002 Nagoya<br />

matuuranaomi@yahoo.co.jp<br />

Participants List<br />

NASSIR, MUSOKE<br />

TRAINING AND CAPACITY BUILDING<br />

AIDS INFROMATION CENTRE- UGANDA<br />

MUSAJJA ALUMBWA ROAD- MENGO KISE-<br />

NYI<br />

UG-256 KAMPALA<br />

nassir@aicug.org<br />

NATARAJAN, Deepa<br />

Behavior Physiology<br />

University of Groningen<br />

Kerklaan 30<br />

NL-9751 NN Haren<br />

krishnapreethika@yahoo.com<br />

NEUMANN, Inga<br />

Behavioural and Molecular Neuroendocrinology<br />

University of Regensburg<br />

Universitystreet<br />

DE-93053 Regensburg<br />

inga.neumann@biologie.uni-regensburg.de<br />

NIKLAUS, Pia<br />

IFB<br />

Marktgasse 29<br />

CH-3000 Bern 7<br />

pia.niklaus@ifkjb.ch<br />

121


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

NIVEAU, Gérard<br />

Forensic psychiatry<br />

CURML<br />

Rue Michel-Servet 1<br />

CH-1211 Geneva<br />

gerard.niveau@hcuge.ch<br />

NOUCHINE, hadjikhani<br />

BMI/MCBI<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong>/HMS<br />

SV BMI AAB 011<br />

Station 15<br />

CH-1015 lausanne<br />

nouchine.hadjikhani@epfl.ch<br />

NURSE, Jo<br />

Regional Health Group<br />

Department of Health England<br />

Bridge House, 1 Walnut Tree Close, Guildford,<br />

Surrey<br />

GB-GU1 4GA Guildford<br />

jo.nurse@dh.gsi.gov.uk<br />

OMARA, osama<br />

kena college of physical education<br />

lecturer<br />

menavile village pharmacy- sfaga- red<br />

sea- Egypt<br />

EG-0020653 safaga<br />

dr_omara@hotmail.de<br />

OTTESEN, Vibeke<br />

Centre for Research and Education in Forensic<br />

Psychiatry<br />

Ullevål University Hospital<br />

Gaustad, Building 7<br />

NO-0407 Oslo<br />

vibeke.ottesen@kompetanse-senteret.<br />

no<br />

OUAKNINE, Nathalie<br />

Anthropole-Dorigny<br />

Institut de psychologie<br />

Quartier Dorigny<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

nathalie.ouaknine@unil.ch<br />

OUAKNINE, Nathalie<br />

Bâtiment Anthropole<br />

Institute of Psychology Lausanne<br />

Anthropole - Quartier UNIL-Dorigny-University<br />

of Lausanne<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

nathalie.ouaknine@unil.ch<br />

122<br />

OZOUX, Thomas<br />

Point du jour production<br />

23, rue de Cronstadt<br />

FR-75015 Paris<br />

tomtom.oz@free.fr<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

PANARETOS, John<br />

Statistics<br />

Athens University of Economics and Business<br />

76, Patision St.,<br />

Athens, Greece<br />

GR-104 34 Athens<br />

jpan@aueb.gr<br />

PARCHET, Grégoire<br />

BMI / LGC<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong> / SV<br />

Bâtiment AI<br />

station 15<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

gregoire.parchet@epfl.ch<br />

PARLANGE, Mary<br />

Independent<br />

Chemin des Paquis 12<br />

CH-1025 St-sulpice<br />

mary.parlange@gmail.com<br />

PARMIGIANI, STEFANO<br />

DEpartment of Evolutionary and Functional<br />

Biology<br />

UNIVERSITY OF PARMA (ITALY)<br />

University Campus<br />

Via Usberti 1/A<br />

IT-43100 Parma<br />

stefano.parmigiani@unipr.it<br />

PASQUIER, Miriella<br />

SV IBI LMBM<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

Station 15<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

miriella.pasquier@epfl.ch<br />

PASSAMONTI, Luca<br />

Medical Research Council<br />

Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit<br />

15 Chaucer Road<br />

GB-CB2 7EF Cambridge<br />

luca.passamonti@mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

PAUDEL KUMARI, Ambika<br />

advocacy and publication<br />

Conscious Media Forum<br />

COCAP BUILDING, Near Milan Bidhya<br />

mandir, Anamnagar<br />

NP-2332 Kathmandu<br />

pmayalu@gmail.com<br />

PEARCE, Jenny<br />

Peace Studies<br />

University of Bradford<br />

University of Bradford<br />

GB-BD7 1DP Bradford<br />

j.v.pearce@bradford.ac.uk<br />

PERRET, Nicolas<br />

svlgc<br />

Rue du Centre 1<br />

CH-1030 Bussigny<br />

nicolas.perret@epfl.ch<br />

PHILIPPE, Cotter<br />

Réseau Universitaire International de Genève<br />

University of Geneva<br />

Rue Varembé 9-11<br />

CH-1202 Genève<br />

pcotter@bluewin.ch<br />

PINGAULT, Jean-Baptiste<br />

Département des Sciences Sociales<br />

Paris V, René Descartes<br />

34 Av Reille<br />

FR-75020 Paris<br />

pingaultjb@yahoo.fr<br />

PONSONNET, Lionel<br />

CePO<br />

Chemin des Boveresses 155<br />

CH-1066 Lausanne<br />

lionel.ponsonnet@unil.ch<br />

PREHN, Kristin<br />

Dept. of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy<br />

Rostock University<br />

Gehlsheimer Strasse 20<br />

DE-18147 Rostock<br />

kristin.prehn@med.uni-rostock.de<br />

PYTHON, Agathe<br />

SB IPMC LIFMET/CIBM<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

Bâtiment CH<br />

Station 6<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

agathe.python@epfl.ch<br />

RADICATI, Vanessa<br />

Fondation Anouk<br />

11 Grand Rue<br />

CH-1204 Geneva<br />

vanessa@anouk.org<br />

RANIA, Saad<br />

Injury Control Unit<br />

Ministry of health<br />

Imbaba, eltayar st<br />

EG-22222 Cairo<br />

rania_33indep@hotmail.com<br />

RIZZO, Albert<br />

Institute for Creative Technologies<br />

University of Southern California<br />

13274 Fiji Way<br />

US-90292 Marina del Rey<br />

arizzo@usc.edu<br />

RONGJUN, Yu<br />

Cognition and Brain Science Unit<br />

University of Cambridge<br />

15 Chaucer Road<br />

GB-CB2 7EF Cambridge<br />

ry223@cam.ac.uk<br />

ROSA, Muriel<br />

Département de Biochimie<br />

Université de Lausanne<br />

Chemin des Boveresses 155<br />

CH-1066 Epalinges<br />

muriel.rosa@unil.ch<br />

ROSS, Fred<br />

SV-GHI-UPKIN<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

Station 15<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

fred.ross@epfl.ch<br />

ROSSETTI, Clara<br />

BMI- Lab. of Genetic Behavior<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

Station 15<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

clara.rossetti@epfl.ch<br />

Participants List<br />

ROTH, Beatrice<br />

Centre de Neurosciences psychiatriques<br />

European Dana Alliance for the Brain (EDAB)<br />

Site de Cery<br />

CH-1008 Prilly/Lausanne<br />

beatrice.roth@chuv.ch<br />

123


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

ROVIRA-PEREZ, Aitor<br />

Computer Science<br />

University College London<br />

Gower Street<br />

GB-WC1E 6BT London<br />

korama37@hotmail.com<br />

RUBOVSZKY, Gregoire<br />

DMCPR, SU, UAUP<br />

HUG<br />

Micheli-du-Crest 24<br />

CH-1211 Geneva<br />

gregoire.rubovszky@hcuge.ch<br />

SALAMIN, Virginie<br />

Service de psychologie<br />

Réseau fribourgeois de santé mentale<br />

Centre de soins hospitaliers<br />

CH-1633 Marsens<br />

salaminv@rfsm.ch<br />

SALEHI, basira<br />

brain and mind institute<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

<strong>EPFL</strong> SV BMI LGC<br />

AAB 2 01 (Bâtiment AAB)<br />

Station 19<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

basira.salehi@epfl.ch<br />

SANDI, Carmen<br />

SV / BMI / LGC<br />

Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne<br />

Station 15<br />

Ecublens<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

carmen.sandi@epfl.ch<br />

SARAGA, Michael<br />

PGE<br />

DP-CHUV<br />

Consultation de Chauderon, Echallens 9<br />

CH-1004 Lausanne<br />

michael.saraga@chuv.ch<br />

SCHALLER, Nathalie<br />

Director, International <strong>Program</strong>s & Partnerships<br />

The International Association for Human Values<br />

31 rue des Bains<br />

CH-1205 Geneva<br />

nathalie.schaller@iahv.org<br />

124<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

SCHECHTER, Daniel<br />

Service de Psychiatrie de l’enfant et de<br />

l’adolescent<br />

Hôpitaux Universitaires de Genève<br />

51 Bd de la Cluse,<br />

2ème étage<br />

CH-1205 Genève<br />

daniel.schechter@hcuge.ch<br />

SCHILLMÖLLER, Zita<br />

Gesundheitswissenschaften<br />

Hochschule für Angewandte Wissenschaften<br />

Hamburg<br />

Lohbrügger Kirchstrasse 65<br />

DE-21033 Hamburg<br />

zita.schillmoeller@ls.haw-hamburg.de<br />

SCHLEMPER, Manola<br />

Department of Justice<br />

Psychiatric-Psychological Service<br />

Feldstrasse 42<br />

CH-8090 Zürich<br />

manola.schlemper@ji.zh.ch<br />

SCHMIDBAUER, Walter M.<br />

Haina Forensic Psychiatric Hospital<br />

Landgraf-Philipp-Platz 3<br />

DE-35114 HAINA (Kloster)<br />

walter.schmidbauer@psych-haina.de<br />

SEVELINGE, Yannick<br />

Life sciences<br />

Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne<br />

Laboratory of Behavioural Genetics<br />

Brain Mind Institute<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

yannick.sevelinge@epfl.ch<br />

SIDJANSKI, Sacha<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong> (Ecole Polytechnique Federaled de Lausanne)<br />

School of Life Sciences<br />

SV-3814<br />

Station 19<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

sacha.sidjanski@epfl.ch<br />

SIEBER, Marc<br />

HES-SO<br />

HEF-TS<br />

Jean Prouvé 10<br />

CH-1752 Givisiez<br />

marc.sieber@hef-ts.ch


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

SIEGMUND, Coralie<br />

LGC<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

AI 3114<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

coralie.siegmund@epfl.ch<br />

SIMPSON, Sandy<br />

Mason Clinic<br />

University of Auckland<br />

Unitec Enrance 2<br />

Carrington Road<br />

NZ-1746 Auckland<br />

sandy.simpson@waitematadhb.govt.nz<br />

SLATER, Mel<br />

Faculty of Psychology / EVENT Lab<br />

University of Barcelona<br />

Campus de Mundet, Edifici Ponent,Passeig<br />

de la Vall d’Hebron, 171<br />

ES-08035 Barcelona<br />

melslater@gmail.com<br />

SPIELMANN, Thorsten<br />

Psychiatrisch Psychologischer Dienst ZH<br />

Feldstr. 42<br />

CH-8090 Zürich<br />

thorsten.spielmann@ji.zh.ch<br />

SPREUX-VAROQUAUX, Odile<br />

Pharmacology, Biology Department<br />

Medical University Versailles and Versailles<br />

Hospital<br />

177 rue de Versailles<br />

FR-78157 Le Chesnay<br />

ovaroquaux@ch-versailles.fr<br />

STRüBER, Daniel<br />

General Psychology<br />

Helmut-Schmidt-University, University of the<br />

Federal Armed Forces Hamburg<br />

Holstenhofweg 85<br />

DE-22043 Hamburg<br />

strueber@hsu-hh.de<br />

SULE, DANIEL<br />

LIAIS OFFICE ABUJA<br />

KOGI STATE GOVERNMENT<br />

T.Y DANJUMA STREET ASOKORO<br />

NG-234-09 ABUJA<br />

kogistatgovt@yahoo.com<br />

Participants List<br />

SUOMI, Stephen<br />

Laboratory of Comparative Ethology<br />

Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of<br />

Child Health & Human Development, National<br />

Institutes of Health<br />

6705 Rockledge Drive, Suite 8030<br />

US-20892-7971 Bethesda<br />

ss148k@nih.gov<br />

SWARTZ, Melody<br />

SV IBI LMBM<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

Station 15<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

melody.swartz@epfl.ch<br />

SYLVAIN, Lengacher<br />

BMI - LNDC<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

Station 15<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

sylvain.lengacher@epfl.ch<br />

TERCIER HOLST-RONESS, Florence<br />

Issues Affecting Women <strong>Program</strong>me<br />

Oak Foundation<br />

58, av. Louis Casai<br />

CH-1216 Cointrin<br />

florence.tercier@oakfnd.ch<br />

TERZIDIS, Amanda<br />

Viol-Secours Association<br />

3 place des Charmilles<br />

CH-1203 Geneva<br />

amanda.terzidis@viol-secours.ch<br />

THEWARAPPERUMA, udeni<br />

programme<br />

Women defining Peace<br />

0/4, Don Carolis Road, Colombo 05<br />

LK-0094 colombo<br />

taud1973@gmail.com<br />

THOMAS, Stephen<br />

British Transport Police<br />

25 Camden Road<br />

GB-NW1 9LN London<br />

stephen.thomas@btp.pnn.police.uk<br />

125


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

TIMMER, Marjan<br />

Brain Mind Institute - Laboratory of Behavioral<br />

Genetics<br />

Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne<br />

Faculty of Life Science<br />

AAB201<br />

Station 15<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

marjan.timmer@epfl.ch<br />

TRAMONTANO, Carlo<br />

Psychology<br />

Sapienza, University of Rome<br />

via dei Marsi 78<br />

IT-00185 Rome<br />

carlo.tramontano@uniroma1.it<br />

TREMBLAY, Richard<br />

Public health<br />

University College Dublin<br />

Belfield<br />

IE-Dublin 4 Dublin<br />

tremblar@grip.umontreal.ca<br />

TRONO, Didier<br />

Faculté des Sciences de la Vie<br />

Ecole polytechnique fédérale de Lausanne<br />

Station 19<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

didier.trono@epfl.ch<br />

VAROQUAUX, Odile<br />

Pharmacology, Biology Department<br />

Medical university Versailles and Versailles<br />

Hospital<br />

177 rue de Versailles<br />

FR-78157 Le Chesnay<br />

ovaroquaux@ch-versailles.fr<br />

VATNAR, Solveig<br />

psychiatry<br />

Ullevaal university hospital, Center for Research<br />

and Education in Forensic Psychiatry<br />

Gaustad, Building 7, 0407 OSLO, Norway<br />

NO-0407 OSLO<br />

solveig.vatnar@kompetanse-senteret.<br />

no<br />

VAUCHER, Angelique<br />

LGC<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong>-BMI<br />

AI 3114<br />

CH-1015 lausanne<br />

angelique.vaucher@epfl.ch<br />

126<br />

February 11-13 <strong>2009</strong><br />

VEENEMA, Alexa<br />

Department of Behavioral Neuroendocrinology<br />

University of Regensburg<br />

Universitaetsstrasse 31<br />

DE-93051 Regensburg<br />

alexa.veenema@biologie.uni-regensburg.de<br />

VERCRUYSSE, Filip<br />

SV<br />

<strong>EPFL</strong><br />

Ch. De Belle-Source 5<br />

CH-1007 Lausanne<br />

filipvercruysse@gmail.com<br />

VEUTHEY, Fernand<br />

Haute Ecole Fribourgeoise de travail social<br />

Jean-Prouvé 10<br />

CH-1762 Givisiez<br />

veutheyf@hef-ts.ch<br />

VIGIL-COLET, Andreu<br />

Psychology<br />

Universitat Rovira i Virgili<br />

Carretera de Valls s/n<br />

ES-43007 TARRAGONA<br />

andreu.vigil@urv.cat<br />

VIJENDRAVARMA, Roshan Kumar<br />

DEE<br />

UNIL<br />

UNIL,<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

roshan.vijendravarma@unil.ch<br />

VIJENDRAVARMA, Roshan Kumar<br />

Department of Ecology and Evolution<br />

University of Lausanne<br />

UNIL sorge<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

roshan.vijendravarma@unil.ch<br />

VLERICK, Ariane<br />

School of Life Sciences - <strong>EPFL</strong><br />

Dean’s Office<br />

SV 3814 / Station 19<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

ariane.vlerick@epfl.ch<br />

VON DER PAHLEN, Bettina<br />

Dept. of Psychology,<br />

Åbo Akademi University<br />

Biskopsgatan 3<br />

FI-20500 Turku<br />

bpahlen@abo.fi


<strong>EPFL</strong> <strong>Latsis</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2009</strong>: Understanding Violence<br />

WAGNER, Ullrich<br />

Laboratory for Neurology and Imaging of Cognition<br />

University of Geneva<br />

CMU, 1 rue Michel-Servet<br />

CH-1211 Geneva<br />

ullrich.wagner@unige.ch<br />

WAHL, Klaus<br />

Central Scientific Unit<br />

German Youth Institute (Deutsches Jugendinstitut)<br />

Nockherstr. 2<br />

DE-D-81541 München<br />

wahl@dji.de<br />

WALTER, Henrik<br />

Medical Psychology/Psychiatry<br />

University Clinic Bonn<br />

Sigmund-Freud-Str. 25<br />

DE-53105 Bonn<br />

henrik.walter@ukb.uni-bonn.de<br />

WANDERA, Stephen Ojiambo<br />

Population Studies<br />

Makerere University<br />

Makerere Hill Road<br />

UG-256 Kampala<br />

swandera@gmail.com<br />

WANG, Shr-Jie<br />

Research<br />

Rehabilitation and Research Centre for Torture<br />

Victims<br />

Borgergade 13<br />

DK-2014 Copenhagen<br />

sjw@rct.dk<br />

WESSLER, Adelheid<br />

Humanities and Social Sciences<br />

Volkswagen Foundation<br />

Kastanienallee 35<br />

DE-30519 Hannover<br />

wessler@volkswagenstiftung.de<br />

WILKINSON, Richard<br />

Epidemiology & Public Health<br />

University of Nottingham<br />

3 Old Lane Court,<br />

Colton,<br />

GB-LS24 8JQ Tadcaster,<br />

richard.wilkinson@nottingham.ac.uk<br />

WINCKLER, Lisa<br />

University Darmstadt<br />

Messelerstrasse 72<br />

DE-64291 Darmstadt<br />

winckler.l@googlemail.com<br />

YAHIA, Ghada<br />

<strong>Program</strong>s and Research<br />

Dubai Foundation for Women and Children<br />

Al Awir Street<br />

AE-0000 Dubai<br />

dr_ghada@yahoo.com<br />

Participants List<br />

YANG, Min<br />

Forensic Psychiatry Research Unit<br />

St Barts and the London Medical School,<br />

Queen Mary University of London<br />

William Harvey House, 61 Bartholomew<br />

Close<br />

GB-EC1A 7BE London<br />

m.yang@qmul.ac.uk<br />

ZAKANY, Jozsef<br />

Zoology and Animal Biology<br />

University of Geneva<br />

Sciences III, 30 Quai Ernest Ansermet<br />

CH-1211 Geneva<br />

jozsef.zakany@unige.ch<br />

ZARIC, Jelena<br />

CePO<br />

Ch des Boveresses 155<br />

CH-1066 Epalinges<br />

jelena.zaric@unil.ch<br />

ZUFFEREY, Christophe<br />

Ecole des Sciences Criminelles<br />

Institut de Criminologie et de Droit Pénal<br />

(ICDP)<br />

Batochime<br />

UNIL-Sorge<br />

CH-1015 Lausanne<br />

christophe.zufferey.2@unil.ch<br />

127

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!