01.05.2017 Views

3658925934

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

aspects of invention and, at some points, its “essentially deductive character.” 35 Inventions derive<br />

largely from conscious volition of the inventor, and occasionally may even follow, he suggested, from<br />

a syllogistic sort of reasoning: (1) the inventor recognizes a particular goal as desirable; (2) he seeks<br />

to achieve the goal with existing means, but if these are for some reason insufficient, he (3) affirms<br />

the necessity for such action as can generate a new means by which the goal can be attained. To<br />

generate sufficient means, he seeks to create an appropriate invention.<br />

But while this conscious, goal-seeking model offered a partial explanation of invention, Tarde was<br />

quick to assert the necessity of additional factors, for example, the creative inspiration of various<br />

emotions and moods which an inventor would experience at different times. And in contrast to the<br />

more rational bent of certain late nineteenth-century sociologists like Weber or Durkheim (here, once<br />

again, his indebtedness to the tradition of Spontaneity was apparent), he felt that the unconscious part<br />

of the mind, or the “sub-self,” could break through into consciousness as a source of creative<br />

inspiration.<br />

Bordering on the physiological, Tarde further pointed out an apparent overlap between the ages of<br />

greatest intellectual as well as sexual creativity: the years from about twenty-five to thirty-five. Many<br />

of his examples supporting this generalization were drawn from the natural sciences, and, indeed,<br />

subsequent more systematic studies have shown these years to be the period of greatest creativity for<br />

many of the natural sciences—although this is not the case for all fields. 36<br />

While stressing the “external” factors producing invention, Tarde nevertheless was more careful<br />

than many social scientists of his time in calling attention to the “internal” factors of intellectual<br />

development. 37 He formulated two basic principles in this area. The first, the principle of<br />

accumulation, emphasized the essentially cumulative character of all knowledge: new discoveries do<br />

not simply replace older ones (although they may in terms of technological use, they do not from the<br />

standpoint of intellectual development); there is continual addition to the legacy of la connaissance<br />

humaine. Knowledge may grow at different rates in varying periods and societies, but the overall<br />

tendency is toward progressive accumulation of knowledge in virtually all areas. Related to the<br />

principle of accumulation is that of irreversibility. The growth of some types of knowledge may take<br />

place in essentially random order; there is no logical necessity, for example, to explore one part of an<br />

uncharted territory before other parts. But in other cases, certain inventions presuppose certain<br />

others: a cart, for example, can be created only after wheels are available. Knowledge, Tarde<br />

stressed, advances in small increments, and even the most creative discoveries are heavily dependent<br />

on antecedent ideas. The most outstanding inventions are those which recombine already acquired<br />

bits of knowledge into highly original syntheses. But although the general lines of intellectual<br />

development are governed by the dual principles of accumulation and irreversibility, the specific<br />

directions as well as the rate of advance are heavily dependent on psychological and social factors.<br />

Social stratification, in Tarde’s eyes, was functional to invention because it produced greater<br />

leisure time for the upper classes, thereby allowing greater communication and social interaction<br />

within the elite; and with this increasing density of stimulating contacts and increasing exchange of<br />

ideas, correspondingly greater numbers of creative discoveries would emerge. A certain minimal<br />

rigidity of class lines also insulates the elite from the inhibiting ideas of the populace; if leaders are<br />

forced to submit to opinions of the majority at every turn—and here Tarde cited the dysfunctional<br />

consequences of democracy as detailed by de Tocqueville—they will not be free to invent. Social<br />

stratification can thus favor discovery in the same way that university structures guarantee a certain<br />

minimum of academic freedom for those working inside them.<br />

In addition to class differences, national differences in inventiveness are also evident, Tarde

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!