21.01.2017 Views

YSM Issue 90.1

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

cognitive science<br />

FOCUS<br />

ABOUT THE AUTHOR<br />

ANNA WUJCIAK<br />

ANNA WUJCIAK is a senior Biomedical Engineering Major in Saybrook<br />

College. She works in Jay Humphrey's lab studying cardiovascular<br />

biomechanics and is a member of the Women's Club Water Polo Team.<br />

THE AUTHOR WOULD LIKE TO THANK Laurie Santos, Angie Johnston,<br />

and Paul Holden for their time and passion for their research.<br />

IMAGE COURTESY OF THE DOG BREED INFO CENTER<br />

►Dingoes look very similar to dogs. Yet,<br />

there are stark differences, both physical and<br />

psychological, between the two species of<br />

canines.<br />

FURTHER READING<br />

Smith, B. & Litchfield, C. (2010). Dingoes (Canis dingo) can use human social<br />

cues to locate hidden food. Animal Cognition, 13, 367-376.<br />

and error behavior, while other species are<br />

more inclined to engage in this behavior<br />

because they do not automatically accept<br />

the information others have given them.<br />

Canine independence<br />

At the Canine Cognition Center at Yale,<br />

professor Laurie Santos, PhD candidate<br />

Angie Johnston, and research assistant<br />

Paul Holden explored whether dogs displayed<br />

overimitation. Dingoes were studied<br />

similarly at the Dingo Discovery Center<br />

(DDC) in Australia. Dogs were then<br />

compared to dingoes, a closely related but<br />

non-domesticated species, in an effort to<br />

explore the role of domestication.<br />

In four different trials, animals were<br />

tasked with opening a box to retrieve the<br />

treat inside. These boxes, which Holden<br />

designed at the Yale Center for Engineering<br />

Innovation and Design, were equipped<br />

with a lever on the side and a treat underneath<br />

a flip lid. Although the flip lid was<br />

necessary for getting the treat, the lever<br />

was completely ineffective. At the beginning<br />

of each test, a human completed both<br />

the relevant and irrelevant steps in the<br />

process of opening the box three times<br />

as a display for the animal. The box was<br />

considered solved if the animal was able to<br />

retrieve the treat.<br />

Results showed the dogs and dingoes<br />

filtered out the irrelevant lever action,<br />

even though it was demonstrated to them.<br />

With each of the four tries the test subjects<br />

had with the box, the rate of using the irrelevant<br />

lever decreased and the rate of<br />

solving the box increased. This indicated<br />

that both species of canines were learning<br />

which actions were relevant and which actions<br />

were not based on their own learning<br />

experiences.<br />

Although neither canine species displayed<br />

overimitation, there were differences<br />

in the two species. “Dingoes were<br />

using the lever less than dogs,” Holden<br />

said. “At first glance, dogs and dingoes<br />

look quite similar, but they’re actually very<br />

different.” Dingoes are clever problem<br />

solvers because they are more attentive<br />

and independent, Johnston added. Dogs<br />

have become domesticated over time due<br />

to their companionship with humans, and<br />

therefore they tend to look toward their<br />

human more often when a problem arises.<br />

As dogs have come to rely on humans<br />

more, they may have also become less<br />

adept at independent problem solving,<br />

which might explain why they used the lever<br />

more frequently than dingoes.<br />

A human behavior<br />

The brain mechanism behind overimitation<br />

is not yet well understood. Although<br />

it is outside the scope of Santos’s psychology<br />

lab, other researchers are exploring the<br />

complex brain processes that control decision-making<br />

and overimitation.<br />

Unfortunately, outside of dogs, dingoes,<br />

and chimpanzees, not many species have<br />

been tested for overimitation behavior.<br />

Dogs were selected as a species to study<br />

specifically at Yale because they are highly<br />

social. Chimpanzees have previously been<br />

studied because of their high intelligence<br />

and social tendencies. Scientists assume<br />

that since the closest relative to the human,<br />

the chimpanzee, does not display<br />

this behavior, it is human specific. The absence<br />

of overimitation in chimpanzee behavior<br />

also indicates that humans evolved<br />

this trait sometime within the last seven<br />

million years since our species diverged.<br />

Corvid species, which include birds and<br />

crows, may be valuable to explore next,<br />

as they are also very intelligent creatures.<br />

“Most animals exhibit innate behaviors<br />

that they’ve developed over many years<br />

of evolution,” Johnston said. For example,<br />

think of squirrels who bury nuts every<br />

winter.<br />

If not from overimitation, how do other<br />

species pass on information? Johnston<br />

explained that animals learn through observation,<br />

as opposed to the intentional<br />

instruction method of passing information<br />

like humans. Some information, such<br />

as locomotion, is instinctual. “It’s not that<br />

other animals can’t imitate, it’s just that<br />

they only do it when they need to, whereas<br />

humans tend to do it much more often<br />

than necessary. Animals are better at<br />

prioritizing the information they really<br />

need,” Johnston said. While humans have<br />

complex technology and intricate cultural<br />

practices, other species have more basic<br />

tasks that they can typically address on<br />

their own. Our species is the only one<br />

with a culture that forces us to rely on others.<br />

There are two general hypotheses about<br />

the driver of human overimitation. The<br />

first is that humans can’t help but overimitate.<br />

The second is that humans assume<br />

that the actions they observe are the culturally<br />

appropriate way to behave. “My<br />

thoughts are that it’s really a combination<br />

of both of these hypotheses that cause humans<br />

to overimitate,” Santos said.<br />

Overimitation, for better or worse,<br />

seems to be a distinctly human behavior.<br />

It assists our species to pass on complex<br />

cultural information and maintain a high<br />

degree of advanced technology. It certainly<br />

appears that overimitation is one of the<br />

reasons that humans differ so greatly from<br />

all other species.<br />

www.yalescientific.org<br />

December 2016<br />

Yale Scientific Magazine<br />

19

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!