28.10.2016 Views

gender differential paper IJCRB

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

ijcrb.webs.com<br />

INTERDISCIPLINARY JOURNAL OF CONTEMPORARY RESEARCH IN BUSINESS<br />

JUNE 2011<br />

VOL 3, NO 2<br />

“felt job stress” (Motowidlo, Packard, &Manning, 1986; Parker & DeCotiis, 1983). Felt job<br />

stress refers to logic of time pressure, anxiety, and concern that is related with job tasks. The<br />

mere occurrence of stressors does not automatically lead to such felt stress. Individuals may<br />

suffer stressors or scatter their effects either cognitively or behaviorally, and thus felt job stress<br />

can be familiar both from stressors and from physiological symptoms. The attention theory of<br />

stress suggests that there should be a positive association between felt job stress and job<br />

performance. Extensive experimental research supports Easterbrook’s (1959) argument that<br />

although stress depletes an individual’s resources, it paradoxically has the effect of concentrating<br />

remaining resources on the task at hand (Huguet, Galvaing, Monteil, & Dumas, 1999).<br />

Yet workplace-based evidence for a relationship between felt job stress and job performance is<br />

weak and conflicting (Beehr, 1995; Jex, 1998).In this section result of insufficient notice to the<br />

Sources of stress. LePine and his colleagues (2005) showed during meta-analysis that stressors<br />

that are hindrance-oriented (e.g., organizational politics, red tape, role ambiguity) are negatively<br />

related to job performance, but challenge-oriented stressors (e.g., high workload, time pressure,<br />

job scope). Encourage workers and can be positively associated to job performance even as they<br />

suggest other strains such as exhaustion and tiredness. There are likely to be restrictions to the<br />

range, even in reaction to challenges, within which improved stress results in the fruitful<br />

redirection of attention. Researchers have long recommended such limits: Yerkes and Dodson<br />

(1908) hypothesized an inverted U-shaped association between stresses and performance. Lepine<br />

et al. (2005) argued that countervailing negative sound effects of strains can accompany positive<br />

effects of felt stress consequential from challenges to achieve, even when hindrance stressors are<br />

not present. As persons devote growing awareness to symptoms of stress such as exhaustion,<br />

they may become less able to give the focused awareness to the task at hand that, attention theory<br />

suggests, will drive performance. The reason of the consideration approach implies not presently<br />

limits to the stress-performance association, but possible moderators of that association.<br />

Attention theory distinguishes between jobs or task attributes that are to be responded to and<br />

attributes that are to be unnoticed. Individuals who feel stress organize their attention<br />

<strong>differential</strong>ly, so that performance on attributes of the extent requiring an answer may increase<br />

even as responses to ignorable scope diminish sharply (Matthews & Margetts, 1991). The<br />

attention approach is complete significant when an assigned task has a high priority for an<br />

individual and when the task is familiar to the individual (Easterbrook, 1959; Matthews &<br />

Margetts, 1991)Organizational settening commitments and job experience are possible to<br />

moderate the stress performance relationship. Commitments (which leads individuals to struggle<br />

toward organizational goals) and experiences (which breeds task knowledge) each should guide<br />

individuals to focus more significantly on job performance under stress. We therefore turn next<br />

to discussions of these possible moderators and associated hypotheses concerning their effects on<br />

performance.<br />

Commitment as a Moderator of the Stress-Performance Relationship<br />

Commitment has been studied broadly and has been theoretically linked to work outcomes such<br />

as job performance and absenteeism. At least six meta-analyses address the relationship between<br />

Commitment and performance (Cohen, 1991; Jaramillo, Mulki, & Marshall, 2005; Mathieu &<br />

Zajac, 1990; Randall, 1990; Riketta, 2002; Wright & Bonett, 2002).<br />

Regardless of extensive research and strong theoretical reasons to expect that individuals with<br />

higher levels of organizational commitment will achieve better, verification of this relationship<br />

is, surprisingly, mixed (Mowday, Porter, & Steers, 1982; Wright & Bonett, 2002). Refinement<br />

COPY RIGHT © 2011 Institute of Interdisciplinary Business Research 383

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!