23.09.2015 Views

DEVELOPMENT

The pdf-version - Eesti Koostöö Kogu

The pdf-version - Eesti Koostöö Kogu

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

The fact that Estonia’s position on the map of world values<br />

has generally remained the same does not mean that<br />

the population’s values have not changed at all during<br />

the last 20 years. There are a number of studies that<br />

have examined the value change in Estonia in the period<br />

following re-independence (see, for instance, Lauristin et<br />

al. 1997; Tart 2011) which show that a significant shift<br />

towards hedonistic as well as post-modern or Western<br />

values took place during the transition period (1991–<br />

2003; Kalmus & Vihalemm 2008; Vihalemm & Kalmus<br />

2009). The latter is partially confirmed by the data of<br />

the World Values Survey, according to which, since the<br />

mid-1990s, the importance of so-called post-modern<br />

and self-expressive values, such as a general trust of<br />

people, a sense of well-being and tolerance has slowly<br />

increased. At the same time, regardless of the growth of<br />

these indicators, compared to the people in Scandinavia,<br />

for instance, Estonia’s residents continue to be considerably<br />

less trusting, less tolerant and less happy, and<br />

tend to place greater emphasis on fighting rising prices<br />

and maintaining order in the state than on the general<br />

freedom of speech and political participation.<br />

If we think of Estonia’s economic development<br />

during the last decade (which regardless of the complicated<br />

situation in the world economy, has been<br />

characterised by the growth of both the GDP and<br />

exports), should we have expected a greater emphasis<br />

on self-expressive values in Estonia than current data<br />

indicates? According to Inglehart’s (1997) socialisation<br />

hypothesis, which is described above, values change<br />

steadily as the society’s wealth increases, through generational<br />

turnover, whereas the basis for the value shift<br />

is the economic situation in the society during a person’s<br />

formative years, i.e. during childhood and youth.<br />

Therefore, a state’s socio-economic development is not<br />

accompanied by a sudden change in values. People’s values<br />

are relatively conservative, and they may not exactly<br />

follow the changes in the economy and social organisation<br />

of life. Based on this approach, we really have<br />

no reason to expect greater changes in values during<br />

the last decade, because economic development and the<br />

growth of wealth did not start in Estonia until the mid-<br />

2000s. Consequently, as socio-economic development<br />

continues, we should expect to see a more significant<br />

emergence of self-expressive values in “Generation Z”<br />

which now consist of 10-years-olds, and those somewhat<br />

older and younger. It won’t be possible to check<br />

this hypothesis until the new data collection waves of<br />

the World Values Survey are completed, which will<br />

hopefully include Estonia, in a dozen or so years.<br />

The latest data from the autumn of 2011 indicates<br />

that every subsequent generation, with one important<br />

exception, actually supports self-expressive values more<br />

than the previous generation. The exception is the generation<br />

of “free Estonia’s” children, who were born in<br />

the early 1990s, and whose values are moving back to a<br />

greater emphasis on survival, or to less political activism,<br />

less trust and less tolerance than the two previous generations<br />

(see, for instance, Figure 1.5.9). Future studies<br />

must provide the answers to the questions of whether<br />

this somewhat reactionary value shift will be permanent,<br />

and what the reasons are for this (i.e. the complicated<br />

economic and socio-political situations in Estonia in the<br />

early 1990s, etc.).<br />

The focus and volume of this chapter does not<br />

allow for a more detailed analysis of value changes<br />

separately by Estonians and non-Estonians, but earlier<br />

research has found considerable differences in their<br />

value preferences (see, for instance, Magun & Rudnev<br />

2010; Tart 2011). Data from the last wave of the World<br />

Values Survey (2011) shows that the values of the Estonian-<br />

and Russian-speaking populations differ significantly<br />

only in regard to one dimension – that of survival<br />

versus self-expression. Compared to the Estonians, the<br />

values related to survival predominate slightly more<br />

among Russian-speaking people, which, as a whole, may<br />

tilt Estonia slightly to the left on the cultural map of<br />

the world 5 . However, the difference in value preferences<br />

between the two language groups is not excessively<br />

large, and becomes even smaller when the Estonian and<br />

non-Estonian cohorts born in the 1980s and 1990s are<br />

compared. Therefore, the World Values Survey results<br />

confirm that the values of Estonians and non-Estonians<br />

are becoming more similar through generational turnover<br />

(Sõmer 2011).<br />

1.5.8<br />

Conclusions<br />

In conclusion, it should be emphasised that Inglehart and<br />

his colleagues’ approach to values and the cultural map<br />

of the world (Inglehart & Welzel 2005, 2010) do not lay<br />

claim to the absolute truth, and it is only one of many<br />

theories of cultural values and cultural dimensions that<br />

have been suggested by various researchers through the<br />

years. On the one hand, the basis for the popularity of<br />

Inglehart and his colleagues’ approach is its simplicity<br />

and, at the same time, its great capacity for generalisation<br />

– only two clear and easily interpreted value dimensions<br />

make it possible to explain over two-thirds of the total<br />

crossnational variance. The second advantage of the study<br />

is that the surveys have been carried out over a period of<br />

30 years, which makes it possible to speak about value<br />

trends and their changes over time. In addition, as mentioned<br />

above, the reliability of the World Values Survey<br />

research is increased further by the fact that at least the<br />

survival versus self-expression value dimension has been<br />

shown to be strongly related to other well-known cultural<br />

dimensions such as individualism–collectivism (Hofstede<br />

1980) and autonomy-conservation(Schwartz 1994), which<br />

all express people’s wishes and aspirations for greater<br />

autonomy and self-determination. Therefore, regardless<br />

5 In the case of the five qualities that form the basis for the survival vs. self-expression value dimension, the greatest difference between the<br />

two language groups appears in their attitude toward homosexuality. If 42% of the Estonian-speaking respondents think that homosexuality is<br />

never justified, among the Russian-speaking respondents the corresponding indicator is 59%. The differences between the two language groups<br />

also continue to exist in the younger cohorts: for example, of the young people born between 1980 and 1989, 24% of the Estonian-speaking<br />

respondents think that homosexuality is never justified, while 41% of the Russian-speaking respondents take this position.<br />

Estonian Human Development Report 2012/2013<br />

57

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!