08.09.2015 Views

The Meme Machine

TheMemeMachine1999

TheMemeMachine1999

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS
  • No tags were found...

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

THE BIG BRAIN 73<br />

proportion to the scale of the endeavour. Big brains are so expensive that if you<br />

could catch your prey with a slightly smaller one you would have an advantage.<br />

Many pack animals hunt extremely effectively with brains that are small by<br />

human standards. Indeed, as we have seen, it rather looks as though Homo<br />

erectus began eating more meat to feed the growing brain rather than vice versa.<br />

Something else must have been driving brain size.<br />

Early hominids obtained much of their food by foraging. So perhaps a big<br />

brain was needed for extracting difficult foods or for the spatial ability and<br />

cognitive maps needed to find food in patchy and unpredictable environments.<br />

However, animals with very small brains manage to store and find food in vast<br />

numbers of separate locations, and many animals, such as squirrels and sewer<br />

rats, make cognitive maps of large areas. Species with such good spatial skills<br />

do show differences in brain structure but not in overall size. Also, predictions<br />

concerning brain size and foraging range have not generally supported this kind<br />

of theory (Barton and Dunbar 1997; Harvey and Krebs 1990).<br />

Other theories emphasise the social environment. <strong>The</strong> Cambridge<br />

psychologist Nicholas Humphrey (1986) suggested that early hominids took an<br />

important step beyond their ancestors by beginning to look into their own minds<br />

as a way of predicting what others would do. For example, if you want to know<br />

whether that huge male gorilla is likely to attack you if you try to mate with this<br />

attractive female, you should try to imagine what you would do in the same<br />

situation. This introspection is the origin of what Humphrey calls ‘Homo<br />

psychologicus’, of humans capable of understanding that others have minds, and<br />

ultimately of self-awareness.<br />

Consciousness itself is something we value highly and tend to think of as<br />

uniquely human and special, but whether it provides any selective advantage is a<br />

fiercely debated issue (e.g. Blakemore and Greenfield 1987; Chalmers 1996;<br />

Dennett 1991). Some argue that consciousness could not have evolved unless it<br />

had a function, while others maintain that consciousness is not the sort of thing<br />

that could have a function. For example, if consciousness is an epiphenomenon<br />

of attention or language or intelligence, then the selective advantage would be<br />

for those capabilities, not for consciousness itself. More radically, some believe<br />

that consciousness is an illusion, or that the whole idea of consciousness will<br />

ultimately be dropped, just as the idea of the ‘life force’ was dropped when we<br />

began to understand the mechanisms of life. Clearly, consciousness cannot help<br />

us explain the big brain; you cannot solve one mystery by invoking another.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!