13.07.2015 Views

Clinical Assessment of Child and Adolescent Personality and ...

Clinical Assessment of Child and Adolescent Personality and ...

Clinical Assessment of Child and Adolescent Personality and ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong><strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong><strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Behavior


<strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong><strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong><strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> BehaviorThird EditionPaul J. FrickUniversity <strong>of</strong> New Orleans, New Orleans, LA, USAChristopher T. BarryUniversity <strong>of</strong> Southern Mississippi, Hattiesburg, MS, USAR<strong>and</strong>y W. KamphausGeorgia State University, Atlanta, GA, USA


Paul J. FrickUniversity <strong>of</strong> New OrleansNew Orleans, LAUSApfrick@uno.eduChristopher T. BarryUniversity <strong>of</strong> Southern MississippiHattiesburg, MSUSAchristopher.barry@usm.eduR<strong>and</strong>y W. KamphausGeorgia State UniversityAtlanta, GAUSArkamphaus@gsu.eduISBN 978-0-387-89642-7 e-ISBN 978-1-4419-0641-0DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-0641-0Springer New York Dordrecht Heidelberg LondonLibrary <strong>of</strong> Congress Control Number: 2009926176© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 20101st edition: © Allyn & Bacon, 19962nd edition: © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2005All rights reserved. This work may not be translated or copied in whole or in part withoutthe written permission <strong>of</strong> the publisher (Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, 233 SpringStreet, New York, NY 10013, USA), except for brief excerpts in connection with reviews orscholarly analysis. Use in connection with any form <strong>of</strong> information storage <strong>and</strong> retrieval,electronic adaptation, computer s<strong>of</strong>tware, or by similar or dissimilar methodology nowknown or hereafter developed is forbidden.The use in this publication <strong>of</strong> trade names, trademarks, service marks, <strong>and</strong> similar terms,even if they are not identified as such, is not to be taken as an expression <strong>of</strong> opinion as towhether or not they are subject to proprietary rights.While the advice <strong>and</strong> information in this book are believed to be true <strong>and</strong> accurate at the date<strong>of</strong> going to press, neither the authors nor the editors nor the publisher can accept any legalresponsibility for any errors or omissions that may be made. The publisher makes no warranty,express or implied, with respect to the material contained herein.Printed on acid-free paperSpringer is part <strong>of</strong> Springer Science+Business Media (www.springer.com)


To my inspiration, Vicki, Josh, Jordan, <strong>and</strong>Jacob (PJF)To my “home team,” Tammy <strong>and</strong> Andersen (CTB)To the memory <strong>of</strong> my parents,Richard <strong>and</strong> Nancy Kamphaus (RWK)


C o n t e n t sPrefacexiPart IBasic IssuesChapter 1Historical Trends 3Chapter Questions 3Definitions <strong>of</strong> Terms in <strong>Personality</strong><strong>Assessment</strong> 4Early History <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong><strong>Assessment</strong> 6Projective Techniques 7The Diagnostic <strong>and</strong> Statistical Manual<strong>of</strong> Mental DisordersDiagnostic Systems 16IDEA <strong>and</strong> Special Education 17Future Trends 19Chapter Summary 19Chapter 2Measurement Issues 21Chapter Questions 21Defining <strong>Personality</strong> Tests 22Scores, Norms, <strong>and</strong> Distributions 25Reliability 34Construct Validity 37Utility 44Conclusions 45Chapter Summary 45Chapter 3Classification <strong>and</strong>DevelopmentalPsychopathology 47Chapter Questions 47Science <strong>and</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> 47Classification 48Developmental Psychopathology 58Conclusions 64Chapter Summary 65Chapter 4St<strong>and</strong>ards <strong>and</strong> Fairness 67Chapter Questions 67APA Ethical Principles <strong>of</strong> Psychologists 68Test St<strong>and</strong>ards 69Conclusions 79Chapter Summary 79Chapter 5Planning the Evaluation<strong>and</strong> Rapport Building 81Chapter Questions 81Non-specifics in <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> 81Clarifying the Referral Question 82Designing the Evaluation 85To Test or Not to Test 88Rapport Building 89Conclusions 96Chapter Summary 97vii


viiicontentsPart II<strong>Assessment</strong> MethodsChapter 6Self-Report Inventories 101Chapter Questions 101Omnibus <strong>Personality</strong> Inventories 101Single Construct <strong>Personality</strong>Inventories 138Conclusions 138Chapter Summary 139Chapter 7Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher RatingScales 141Chapter Questions 141Evaluating <strong>Child</strong>ren via ParentRatings 141Evaluating <strong>Child</strong>ren via TeacherRatings 143Overview <strong>of</strong> Omnibus Parent <strong>and</strong> TeacherRating Scales 144Behavior <strong>Assessment</strong> System for <strong>Child</strong>ren,2nd Edition (BASC-2) 145Achenbach System <strong>of</strong> Empirically Based<strong>Assessment</strong> (Achenbach & Resorla,2000, 2001) 156<strong>Child</strong> Symptom Inventory-4(CSI-4) 166Conners, 3rd Edition (Conners-3) 170<strong>Personality</strong> Inventory for <strong>Child</strong>ren-2(PIC-2); Student Behavior Survey(SBS) 174Sample Impairment-Oriented Scales 183Conclusions 185Chapter Summary 187Chapter 8Behavioral Observations 189Chapter Questions 189Basics <strong>of</strong> Observational Systems 192Examples <strong>of</strong> Observational Systems 200Conclusions 207Chapter Summary 208Chapter 9Peer-Referenced<strong>Assessment</strong> 211Chapter Questions 211Ethics <strong>of</strong> Peer-ReferencedStrategies 212Types <strong>of</strong> Peer-Referenced Techniques 214Other Peer-Referenced Techniques 223Conclusions 223Chapter Summary 223Chapter 10Projective Techniques 225Chapter Questions 225The Controversy Surrounding ProjectiveTechniques 225Inkblot Techniques 231Thematic (Storytelling) Techniques 236Sentence Completion Techniques 243Drawing Techniques 245Conclusions 250Chapter Summary 250Chapter 11Structured DiagnosticInterviews 253Chapter Questions 253History 253Overview 254Evaluation <strong>of</strong> Diagnostic Interviews 259Recommendations for Use <strong>of</strong> StructuredInterviews 261Focus on the NIMH DiagnosticInterview Schedule for <strong>Child</strong>ren(DISC-iv) 265Conclusions 269Chapter Summary 269


contentsixChapter 12Assessing FamilyContext 271Chapter Questions 271Introduction 271Assessing Family Functioning:General Issues 276General Considerations in AssessingFamily Functioning 277Parenting Styles <strong>and</strong> Practices 280Parenting Stress 288Marital Conflict 291Parental Adjustment 293Conclusions 296Chapter Summary 296Chapter 13History Taking 299Chapter Questions 299Content 307Formats 307Conclusions 313Chapter Summary 314Chapter 14Adaptive BehaviorScales 315Chapter Questions 315History <strong>of</strong> the Construct 315Characteristics <strong>of</strong> Adaptive BehaviorScales 318Omnibus Adaptive Behavior Scales 322General Recommendations 327Measuring Social Skills 332Conclusions 334Chapter Summary 334Part IIIAdvanced TopicsChapter 15Integrating <strong>and</strong> Interpreting<strong>Assessment</strong> Information 339Chapter Questions 339Introduction 339Integrating Information acrossInformants 342A Multistep Strategy for IntegratingInformation 347Step1: Document all <strong>Clinical</strong>ly SignificantFindings Regarding the <strong>Child</strong>’sAdjustment 349Step2: Look for Convergent FindingsAcross Sources <strong>and</strong> Methods 349Step3: Try to Explain Discrepancies 353Step4: Develop a Pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>and</strong> Hierarchy<strong>of</strong> Strengths <strong>and</strong> Weaknesses 353Step5: Determine Critical Informationto Place in the Report 354Conclusions 355Chapter Summary 355Chapter 16Report Writing 357Chapter Questions 357Reporting Problems <strong>and</strong> Challenges 357Report Writing as Part <strong>of</strong> Evidence-Based<strong>Assessment</strong> 359Pitfalls <strong>of</strong> Report Writing 360Suggested Practices 365Adapting Reports to Audience<strong>and</strong> Setting 368The Sections <strong>of</strong> the PsychologicalReport 369Communicating Results Orally 373Conclusions 375Chapter Summary 376


xcontentsChapter 17<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> AttentionDeficit Hyperactivity<strong>and</strong> DisruptiveBehavior Disorders 377Chapter Questions 377Introduction 377Attention-Deficit HyperactivityDisorder 378Conduct Problems 398Conclusions 410Chapter Summary 412Chapter 18<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression<strong>and</strong> Anxiety 413Chapter Questions 413Internalizing Disorders 413<strong>Child</strong>hood Depression 414An <strong>Assessment</strong> Strategy for Depression 422Anxiety Disorders <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong>hood 425An <strong>Assessment</strong> Strategyfor Anxiety 431Conclusions 434Chapter Summary 434Chapter 19<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Autism SpectrumDisorders 437Chapter Questions 437Definitional Issues 437Specialized Measures <strong>of</strong> Autism 439An <strong>Assessment</strong> Strategy for Autism 445A Sample Case <strong>of</strong> Autism in a <strong>Child</strong>with Neurological Impairment 447Conclusions 452Chapter Summary 452References 453Author Index 491Subject Index 503


P r e f a c ePsychologists <strong>of</strong>fer an increasing variety<strong>of</strong> serv ices to the public. Among theseservices, psychological assessment <strong>of</strong> personality<strong>and</strong> behavior continues to be acentral activity. One main rea son is thatother mental health pr<strong>of</strong>essionals <strong>of</strong>tendo not possess a high level <strong>of</strong> competencein this area. When one views psychologistswho serve children <strong>and</strong> adolescents,psychological, assessment seems to take onan even greater role. It follows, then, thatcomprehensive <strong>and</strong> enlightened graduatelevelinstruction in assess ment should be ahigh priority for educators <strong>of</strong> psychologistswho are destined to work with youth.This book is an outgrowth <strong>of</strong> our effortsto improve our own instruction <strong>of</strong> child<strong>and</strong> adoles cent assessment skills. We foundthat existing textbooks were not servingus well. Most <strong>of</strong> them were encyclopedic,edited volumes that were (1) uneven inthe quality across chapters <strong>and</strong>/or (2) notgeared either in format or level <strong>of</strong> presentationfor beginning graduate instruction.The few single- or co-authored volumesavailable tended to lack the breadth <strong>of</strong> coveragewe deemed necessary. Some focusedlargely on the oretical issues related to psychologicaltesting, with minimal discussion<strong>of</strong> practical applications <strong>and</strong> use <strong>of</strong> specifictests. Others focused solely on summaries<strong>of</strong> individual tests, without reviewingthe theoretical or empirical contextwithin which to use the tests appropriately.Hence, this volume reflects our desire toprovide a more helpful tool for instruction- one that provides a scien tific contextwithin which to underst<strong>and</strong> psychologicaltesting with children <strong>and</strong> adolescents <strong>and</strong>that translates this scientific context intopracti cal guidelines for using individualtests in clinical practice.Among our specific objectives for thisvolume are the following:• To focus on measures specificallydesigned to assess the emotional, behavioral,<strong>and</strong> social functioning <strong>of</strong> children<strong>and</strong> adolescents• To provide current research findingsthat enable students to draw heavilyon science as the basis for their clinicalpractice• To help in the translation <strong>of</strong> researchinto practice by providing specific <strong>and</strong>practical guidelines for clinical practice• To include a broad coverage <strong>of</strong> assessmentmethods from a variety <strong>of</strong>theo retical, practical, <strong>and</strong> empirical traditions• To systematically compare tests <strong>and</strong>assess ment methods using researchfindings, reviews, <strong>and</strong> our own synthesis<strong>of</strong> positions• To provide a readable volume that woulden hance the interest <strong>and</strong> retention <strong>of</strong>students through the use <strong>of</strong> numerousxi


xiicase examples, tables, research notes, <strong>and</strong>other “boxes” containing practical adviceIn writing a readable text that goes beyonda cursory survey <strong>of</strong> available instruments,we were faced with the difficult task <strong>of</strong>determining what specific instruments toinclude in the book. As we struggled withthis decision, we eventually chose severalselection criteria. Our main crite rion forinclusion was a test’s ability to serve as anexemplar <strong>of</strong> some specific type <strong>of</strong> assessmentin strument or theoretical approachto assessment. In many cases we lookedfor “prototypes” that we thought wouldhighlight some key points to the readerthat could then be used in evaluatingother assessment instruments not coveredspecif ically in the text. Other criteriaincluded a test’s popularity or our estimate<strong>of</strong> a new test’s poten tial impact on the field.Granted, this decision- making was highlysubjective, but we strove to be analytic <strong>and</strong>to limit to the extent possible our personalfeelings <strong>and</strong> biases. Fortunately, severalexternal anonymous reviewers <strong>of</strong> earlierdrafts <strong>of</strong> the text helped us to be moreobjective.A final point that should be clearly outlinedis our basic orientation to psychologicalassess ment. We feel that the goal<strong>of</strong> psychological as sessment is the measurement<strong>of</strong> psychological constructs <strong>and</strong>,for clinical practice, measurement <strong>of</strong> psychologicalconstructs that have importantclinical implications, such as documentingthe need for treatment or the type <strong>of</strong> interventionthat is most appropriate. For anindividual child the constructs that need tobe assessed will vary from case to case <strong>and</strong>depend on the referral question. But whatis important from this con ceptualization isthat our view <strong>of</strong> psychological assessmentis not test-driven but construct-driven.Without exception, assessment techniqueswill measure some constructs well<strong>and</strong> other psychological dimensions lesswell. Another important implication fromprefacethis view <strong>of</strong> testing is that it is critical thatassessors become familiar with <strong>and</strong> maintainfamiliarity with research on the constructsthey are trying to assess. In short,the most critical component in choosing amethod <strong>of</strong> assessment <strong>and</strong> in interpretingassessment data is underst<strong>and</strong>ing what oneis trying to measure.In this volume, we have focused on themeas urement <strong>of</strong> psychological constructsthat em phasize a child’s emotional, behavioral,<strong>and</strong> social functioning. There is stilla great debate over definitional issues inthis arena <strong>of</strong> psychological functioning interms <strong>of</strong> what should be called “personality,”“temperament,” “behav ior,” or “emotions,”with distinctions usually de terminedby the level <strong>of</strong> analysis (e.g., overt behaviorvs. unconscious motivational processes),assumed etiology (e.g., biologically determinedvs. learned), or proven stability(e.g., transient problems vs. a stable pattern<strong>of</strong> functioning) (see Frick, 2004; Martin,1988). Unfortunately, people <strong>of</strong>ten usethe same terms with very different connotations.In writing this text, we tried toavoid this debate by maintaining a broadfocus on “psychological constructs,” which<strong>of</strong>ten vary on the most ap propriate level <strong>of</strong>analysis, assumed etiol ogy, or stability. Thisdefinitional variability adds a level <strong>of</strong> complexityto the assessment process becauseassessors must always consider what theyare attempting to measure in a partic ularcase <strong>and</strong> what research suggests aboutthe best way <strong>of</strong> conceptualizing this constructbefore they can select the best way<strong>of</strong> measuring it. It would be much easier ifone could develop ex pertise with a singlefavorite instrument or a sin gle assessmentmodality that could be used in all evaluationsto measure all constructs. Becausethis is not the case, psychologists mustdevelop broad-based assessment expertise.Hence, our overriding objective in writingthis volume is to provide the breadth<strong>of</strong> coverage that we feel is needed by thepsychologist in training.


prefacexiiiWe have organized the text into threesec tions consistent with our approach toteaching. Part I provides students with anintroduction to the psychological knowledgebase necessary for modern assessmentpractice including historical perspectives;meas urement science; research in developmentalpsychopathology; ethical, legal, <strong>and</strong>cultural issues; <strong>and</strong> the basics <strong>of</strong> beginningthe assessment process (e.g., planning theevaluation, rapport building). Part II givesstudents a broad review <strong>of</strong> the specificassess ment methods used by psychologists,accompa nied by specific advice regardingthe use <strong>and</strong> the strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses <strong>of</strong>each method. In Part III we help studentsperform some <strong>of</strong> the most sophisticated <strong>of</strong>assessment practices: inte grating <strong>and</strong> communicatingassessment results <strong>and</strong> infusingassessment practice with knowledge <strong>of</strong>child development <strong>and</strong> psychopathology.We think that, on completion <strong>of</strong> this volume,<strong>and</strong> a similar one covering aspects <strong>of</strong>cognitive assess ment (Kamphaus, 2001 <strong>and</strong>in press), that the student psy chologist hasthe background necessary for supervisedpracticum experiences in the assessment <strong>of</strong>chil dren <strong>and</strong> adolescents.Paul J. FrickChristopher T. BarryR<strong>and</strong>y W. Kamphaus


P a r t IBasic Issues


C h a p t e r 1Historical TrendsChapter Questionsl Who were the major innovators in thefield <strong>of</strong> personality assessment?l How were these innovations extended tothe assessment <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents?l What is meant by the terms personality<strong>and</strong> behavior?l What is meant by the terms objective<strong>and</strong> projective personality assessment?l Who conducted the seminal research<strong>and</strong> coined the terms internalizing <strong>and</strong>externa lizing behavior problems?<strong>Personality</strong> assessment is a process thatmost individuals engage in throughouttheir lives (Martin, 1988). Mothers labeltheir children as happy, cranky, or similarlyshortly after birth, <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>ten in utero(e.g., active). The musings <strong>of</strong> Alfred Binetabout the personality <strong>of</strong> his two daughtersare typical <strong>of</strong> observations made by parents.He described Madeleine as silent, cool, <strong>and</strong>concentrated, while Alice was gay, thoughtless,giddy, <strong>and</strong> turbulent (Wolf, 1966).<strong>Adolescent</strong>s are keenly aware <strong>of</strong> personalityevaluation as they carefully considerfeedback from their peers to perform theirown self-assessments. <strong>Personality</strong> assessmentis also prized by the business community,in which human resources personnelconsult with managers <strong>and</strong> others to gaugethe effects <strong>of</strong> their personality on coworkers<strong>and</strong> productivity.Early personality assessment emphasizedthe assessment <strong>of</strong> enduring traits that werethought to underlie behavior or, in modernterminology, latent traits. Kleinmuntz (1967)described personality as a unique organization<strong>of</strong> factors (i.e., traits) that characterizesan individual <strong>and</strong> determines his or her pattern<strong>of</strong> interaction with the environment.Thus, personality structure is commonlythought to be a result <strong>of</strong> multiple individualtraits interacting with one another, <strong>and</strong> withthe person’s environment.3P.J. Frick et al., <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Behavior,DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-0641-0_1, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010


4CHAPTER 1 HISTORICAL TRENDSDefinitions <strong>of</strong> Termsin <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>TraitsA trait is <strong>of</strong>ten conceptualized as a relativelystable disposition to engage in particular actsor ways <strong>of</strong> thinking (Kamphaus, 2001 <strong>and</strong> inpress). A child, for example, may be describedby her parents as either shy or extroverted.The shy (introverted in psychological terms)child may tend to cope with stressful situationsby withdrawing from social contact,whereas the extrovert readily approachessocial situations. For parents <strong>and</strong> psychologistsalike, these traits are <strong>of</strong>ten thought tohave value for predicting human behavior,because <strong>of</strong> the presumption <strong>of</strong> trait stabilityacross time <strong>and</strong>, in many cases, environments.In fact, because <strong>of</strong> trait stability, parents maytake special precautions to ensure that theshy child adapts well to the social aspects<strong>of</strong> attending a new school by asking one <strong>of</strong>their child’s friends who attends the sameschool to accompany the child on the firstday. Similarly, a stable tendency to be shy orintroverted should manifest itself in numeroussocial situations such as interactions inthe neighborhood, at church, <strong>and</strong> in balletclass. <strong>Personality</strong> traits, then, are characterizedby longitudinal <strong>and</strong> situational stability,not unlike other enduring characteristics <strong>of</strong>a person such as intelligence, height, <strong>and</strong>activity level.The Big Five <strong>Personality</strong>Traits (Factors)In 1961, Tupes <strong>and</strong> Christal discoveredfive factors <strong>of</strong> personality that appeared inthe reanalysis <strong>of</strong> numerous data sets fromscales <strong>of</strong> bipolar personality descriptors.These central personality traits have subsequentlybecome the focus <strong>of</strong> an extensiveresearch effort, including the development<strong>of</strong> tests designed to assess the constructs.One <strong>of</strong> the well known scales used to identifythe “big five” in adults is the NEO<strong>Personality</strong> Inventory (NEO-PI; Costa &McCrae, 1985).Although commonly referred to as “factors”because <strong>of</strong> their origins in factor analysis,they are prototypical examples <strong>of</strong> traitswith the requisite characteristic <strong>of</strong> presumedstability. The big five factors are typicallyidentified by bipolar comparisons that aresummarized in Table 1.1. These factors are<strong>of</strong>ten assessed using forced-choice item formatsin which adjectives are used as personalitydescriptors. This item format is in directcontrast to the more commonplace true/falseitem format that is typical <strong>of</strong> many psychologicaltests.Table 1.1 Early Descriptions <strong>of</strong> the BigFive <strong>Personality</strong> Dimensions (Goldberg,1992)Factor I – Surgency (or introversion–extroversion)Unenergetic vs. energeticSilent vs. talkativeTimid vs. boldFactor II – Agreeableness (or pleasantness)Cold vs. warmUnkind vs. kindUncooperative vs. cooperativeFactor III – Conscientiousness (or dependability)Disorganized vs. organizedIrresponsible vs. responsibleNegligent vs. conscientiousFactor IV – Emotional stability (vs. neuroticism)Tense vs. relaxedNervous vs. at easeFactor V – Culture, intellect, openness,or sophisticationUnintelligent vs. intelligentUnanalytical vs. analyticalUnreflective vs. reflective


CHAPTER 1 HISTORICAL TRENDS5Commercially available instrumentssuch as the NEO-PI have provided newopportunities to study <strong>and</strong> refine theseconstructs. Given the amount <strong>of</strong> research<strong>and</strong> development in this area, the big fivepersonality factors could eventually have asubstantial impact on the field <strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong>adolescent personality assessment. Withsome noteworthy exceptions (e.g., Lynamet al., 2005) however, big five research haslargely been focused on adult populations.TemperamentA concept related to personality is temperament,which also emphasizes the measurement<strong>of</strong> specific traits that are hypothesizedas underlying behavior across settings. Inthis regard, Goldsmith <strong>and</strong> Rieser-Danner(1990) observed, “most researchers considertemperament to be the behavioralmanifestation <strong>of</strong> biologically influencedprocesses determining both the infant’scharacteristic response to the environment<strong>and</strong> his or her style <strong>of</strong> initiating behavior”(p. 250). Therefore, some researchers distinguishtemperament from personalitybased on the presumed biological basis<strong>of</strong> temperament, whereas personality isthought to be formed by a dynamic interplay<strong>of</strong> biological <strong>and</strong> social factors overdevelopment (Frick, 2004; Martin, 1988).Predictably, much <strong>of</strong> the research on temperamentis conducted with infants <strong>and</strong>young children. In this conceptualization,personality may be viewed as beingsuperimposed on a person’s temperamentalfoundation. This distinction betweentemperament <strong>and</strong> personality, however, isnot universally agreed upon.BehaviorIn contrast to temperament <strong>and</strong> personalitytrait assessment, the assessment <strong>of</strong> behaviorfocuses on the measurement <strong>of</strong> observablebehaviors, although recently the definitionhas been broadened to include cognitionsas a type <strong>of</strong> behavior. For most purposes,Martin (1988) provides a useful definition<strong>of</strong> behavior.When applied psychologists speak <strong>of</strong> behavior,they are usually referring to that range <strong>of</strong>human responses that are observable withthe naked eye during a relatively brief period<strong>of</strong> time in a particular environment.This conception <strong>of</strong> behavior rules out biochemical<strong>and</strong> neurological events, for example,because they are not observable bythe unaided eye. Behavior is differentiatedfrom traits or dispositions because the lattermay only be seen if behavior is aggregatedover relatively long periods <strong>of</strong> time <strong>and</strong> ina number <strong>of</strong> environmental contexts. Classicalexamples <strong>of</strong> observed behaviors <strong>of</strong> interestto child psychologists include tantrumbehavior among young children, aggressiveinteractions with peers, attempts at conversationinitiation, <strong>and</strong> so forth (p. 13).There are, therefore, several distinguishingfeatures <strong>of</strong> behavioral assessment methodsthat differentiate them from trait assessmentmeasures. First, behavioral assessmentmethods have a different theoretical foundation<strong>and</strong> associated set <strong>of</strong> premises. Behavioralassessment methods draw heavily onthe theory <strong>and</strong> research tradition <strong>of</strong> operantconditioning as exemplified by the work <strong>of</strong>B. F. Skinner (Skinner, 1963). This researchtradition also emanates primarily from laboratoryresearch, as opposed to clinical practice;thus, it is <strong>of</strong>ten considered to be moreempirically based.Second, behavioral assessment methodsare distinguished from medical models<strong>of</strong> assessment more than are trait-basedmethods. The medical model assumesthat symptoms are caused by underlyingconditions, <strong>and</strong> it is the medical conditionthat must be measured, diagnosed,<strong>and</strong> treated to remove the symptoms (seeChap. 3 for a more extended discussion<strong>of</strong> the medical model). In direct contrast,behavioral assessment emphasizes themeasurement <strong>and</strong> treatment <strong>of</strong> the symptoms


6CHAPTER 1 HISTORICAL TRENDSor behavior itself, <strong>and</strong> makes no assumptionsregarding underlying cause.Third, behavioral assessment places apremium on the assessment <strong>of</strong> discretebehaviors. For example, behavioral assessmentmay emphasize the measurement <strong>of</strong>finger tapping while completing seatworkin school, as opposed to aggregating severalbehaviors to form a test or scale with severalitems that measure “motor activity” inthe classroom. This situation is changing,however, with the work <strong>of</strong> Thomas Achenbach,Keith Conners, Cecil Reynolds, <strong>and</strong>others (see Chap. 7), all <strong>of</strong> whom begancombining behaviors into dimensions <strong>of</strong>behavior that may or may not differ fromtrait-based assessment methods.As we suggested in the preface, we thinkthat it is premature to reify any <strong>of</strong> theseapproaches as the ultimate method forassessing children’s psychological adjustment.We merely seek progress in our methods<strong>and</strong> theories. This cautious approachseems warranted as the distinctions betweenthe methods have become blurred as thescience <strong>of</strong> assessment emerges. Furthermore,we think that each approach maybe more or less helpful for answering particularassessment questions. Clearly, some<strong>of</strong> the questions directed at psychologistsare trait-based, whereas others require themeasurement <strong>of</strong> distinct behaviors. Forexample, a parent who asks, “Will my childever become more outgoing like his sister?”is asking for trait assessment, but the parentwho queries, “How can I get him to stopwetting the bed?” may require behavioralassessment expertise.Early History <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong><strong>Assessment</strong>Formal personality measures emerged asa logical outgrowth <strong>of</strong> other efforts to measureindividual differences, most notably theexperimental methods <strong>of</strong> Wundt, Galton, <strong>and</strong>others (Ch<strong>and</strong>ler, 1990). Of the early assessmentluminaries, Sir Francis Galton is one<strong>of</strong> the most notable. Although well knownfor his intelligence measurement contributions,he also studied the measurement <strong>of</strong>“character.” In order to introduce the utility<strong>of</strong> personality measurement, Galton (1884)recounted the personality test invented byBenjamin Franklin as a crude form <strong>of</strong> personalitymeasure. The scale was described inthe tale <strong>of</strong> “The H<strong>and</strong>some <strong>and</strong> DeformedLeg,” in which Franklin recounts how hisfriend tested people so as to avoid those who,“being discontented themselves, sour thepleasures <strong>of</strong> society, <strong>of</strong>fend many people,<strong>and</strong> make themselves everywhere disagreeable”(p. 9). This friend sought to diagnosesuch pessimistic individuals by showingthem an attractive leg <strong>and</strong> a malformedone. If the stranger showed more interestin the ugly leg, the friend became suspicious<strong>and</strong> subsequently avoided this person. Franklinastutely identified this “test” as a grotesquebut, nevertheless, an effective personalityassessment device. Galton concluded: “Theother chief point that I wish to impress is,that a practice <strong>of</strong> deliberately <strong>and</strong> methodicallytesting the character <strong>of</strong> others <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>ourselves is not wholly fanciful, but de-servesconsideration <strong>and</strong> experiment” (p. 10).Intelligence tests, acknowledged as thefirst fruits <strong>of</strong> the psychometric movement,reached prominence early in the twentiethcentury with the introduction <strong>of</strong> the originalBinet <strong>and</strong> Simon scale <strong>and</strong> numerous variants(Kamphaus, 2001 <strong>and</strong> in press). A lesserknown fact is that Alfred Binet developedsome intelligence test items that resembledstimuli used 30 years later in apperceptivetechniques for assessing personality(DuBois, 1970). Test development activityalso received a boost from the World WarI effort, when ability testing became widespread(Kamphaus, 2001 <strong>and</strong> in press).Thus, it is no coincidence that the first formal<strong>and</strong> widely used measures <strong>of</strong> personalitywere developed about this same time.


CHAPTER 1 HISTORICAL TRENDS7Robert S. WoodworthThe Woodworth Personal Data Sheet waspublished in 1918 as a result <strong>of</strong> the surge<strong>of</strong> interest in testing potential soldiers.Woodworth developed a list <strong>of</strong> 116 questionsabout daydreaming, worry, <strong>and</strong> otherproblems. Some sample items from theWoodworth Personal Data Sheet (Woodworth,1918) include:Do people find fault with you much?Are you happy most <strong>of</strong> the time?Do you suffer from headaches <strong>and</strong> dizziness?Do you sometimes wish that you had neverbeen born?Is your speech free from stutter or stammer?Have you failed to get a square deal in life?The examinee responded to each questionwith “yes” or “no” (French & Hale, 1990).According to French <strong>and</strong> Hale (1990),the Woodworth Personal Data Sheetserved as the foundation for the development<strong>of</strong> the Thurstone <strong>Personality</strong> Scale<strong>and</strong> the Allport Ascendance-SubmissionTest, among others. DuBois (1970)described the Personal Data Sheet as “thelineal ancestor <strong>of</strong> all subsequent personalityinventories, schedules <strong>and</strong> questionnaires”(p. 94).The Personal Data Sheet was an importantpractical innovation because, prior tothis time, all military recruits suspected <strong>of</strong>having mental health disorders, stress disordersin particular, had to be identifiedby being interviewed by trained interviewers.The Personal Data Sheet allowed forthe screening <strong>of</strong> large numbers <strong>of</strong> recruitswithout the time <strong>and</strong> expense <strong>of</strong> using hugecadres <strong>of</strong> interviewers (Kleinmuntz, 1967).Thus, it was not basic researchon personality or employee selection thatled to the eventual popularity <strong>of</strong> personalitytesting. Instead, it was the need for diagnosiscreated by World War I which providedconsiderable evidence <strong>of</strong> the need for personalitytests. The successful World War I<strong>and</strong> then later World War II applications <strong>of</strong>psychological testing proved that psychologycould make practical contributions tosociety by identifying, accurately <strong>and</strong> timeefficiently,those in need <strong>of</strong> mental healthservices.After World War II the mental healthneeds <strong>of</strong> citizens, veterans in particular,were the focus <strong>of</strong> greater attention. In thepostwar years, the U.S. Veteran’s Administrationbegan to hire psychologists inlarge numbers to diagnose <strong>and</strong> treat veteranssuffering from significant emotionaldisturbance. Psychologists brought theirpsychometric expertise to bear again bycontributing new methods to the diagnosticprocess. The increased need for postwarmental health services, therefore, createdthe fertile ground in which personalitytesting flourished. As Kleinmuntz (1967)noted, “The most popular personality tests<strong>of</strong> the past 30 years grew out <strong>of</strong> the needto diagnose or detect individuals whosebehavior patterns were psychopathological”(p. 10). The use <strong>of</strong> personality tests afterthe first <strong>and</strong> second world wars exp<strong>and</strong>edbeyond diagnosis into many areas includingcounseling, personnel selection, <strong>and</strong>personality research (Kleinmuntz, 1967).Projective TechniquesThe central assumption underlying projectivetesting is that the use <strong>of</strong> less welldefinedstimuli that are prone to a variety<strong>of</strong> interpretations will encourage clientsto reveal information that they otherwisewould not share in response to direct questioning(Ch<strong>and</strong>ler, 1990). Given that teststimuli (e.g., ink blots) or questions werenot clearly linked to known personalitytraits, projective testing depended heavilyon the explicit or implicit personality theorythat was favored by the test developer.Theory was necessary to determine theunderlying nature or cause <strong>of</strong> the projectedthoughts, emotions, or behaviors.


8CHAPTER 1 HISTORICAL TRENDSThe most popular theory <strong>of</strong> the post-World War I era was psychodynamic personalitytheory as espoused by SigmundFreud (1936) <strong>and</strong> others. Psychodynamicpersonality theory provided a useful theoreticalframework for the development <strong>of</strong>projective assessment measures due to theconcepts <strong>of</strong> repression, projection, <strong>and</strong>other constructs that are entirely consistentwith the use <strong>of</strong> atypical test stimulifor identifying personality traits (seeChap. 10 for a more extended discussion<strong>of</strong> the basic assumptions <strong>of</strong> the projectivetechnique).Association TechniquesThe use <strong>of</strong> association techniques, such asword association methods, for assessmentpurposes can be traced as far back as thework <strong>of</strong> Aristotle (DuBois, 1970). In relativelyrecent history, Sir Francis Galtonbegan studying association techniques asearly as 1879. Galton’s contribution to thestudy <strong>of</strong> association was his introduction <strong>of</strong>scientific rigor to the enterprise. He usedexperimental methods to study associationmethods, including quantitative scaling <strong>of</strong>the results (DuBois, 1970).Subsequently, Kraepelin, Wundt, Cattell,Kent, <strong>and</strong> Rosan<strong>of</strong>f studied the associations<strong>of</strong> patients <strong>and</strong> research participantsto word lists, recording such variablesas response time <strong>and</strong> type <strong>of</strong> association.The latter names, Kent <strong>and</strong> Rosan<strong>of</strong>f, maybe least familiar to many readers becausethe other names are linked with the illustrioushistory <strong>of</strong> intellectual assessment.Kent <strong>and</strong> Rosan<strong>of</strong>f made their contributionsolely to the study <strong>of</strong> associations bydeveloping a list <strong>of</strong> 100 stimulus words <strong>and</strong>systematically recording the associations<strong>of</strong> 1,000 normal subjects (DuBois, 1970).This effort represents an important initialattempt at developing norms to whichresearchers <strong>and</strong> clinicians could comparethe responses <strong>of</strong> clinical subjects.The renowned psychoanalyst CarlJung made extensive use <strong>of</strong> associationtechniques for the study <strong>of</strong> personality. Inan address at Clark University in 1909, hedescribed his research efforts in detail <strong>and</strong>provided some insight into the types <strong>of</strong>interpretations commonly made <strong>of</strong> thesemeasures. Jung described his associationword list as a formulary. His list consisted<strong>of</strong> 54 words including head, to dance, ink,new, foolish, <strong>and</strong> white. According to Jung(1910), normality could be distinguishedfrom psychopathology with this formularyusing variables such as reaction time<strong>and</strong> response content. In his speech, heprovided a transcript <strong>of</strong> the responses <strong>of</strong>a normal individual <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong> a “hysteric.”A sampling <strong>of</strong> their associations to theformulary follows:Stimulus Normal HystericalTo sin Much This is totallyunfamiliar tome, I do notrecognize itTo pay Bills MoneyBread Good To eatWindow Room BigRich Nice Good,convenientFriendly <strong>Child</strong>ren A manAs noted previously for other measures,reaction time to the stimulus words wasalso interpreted by Jung. He gave a glimpse<strong>of</strong> one such interpretation in the followingquote:The test person waives any reaction; for themoment he totally fails to obey the originalinstructions, <strong>and</strong> shows himself incapable <strong>of</strong>adapting himself to the experimenter. If thisphenomenon occurs frequently in an experiment,it signifies a higher degree <strong>of</strong> disturbancein adjustment (p. 27).


CHAPTER 1 HISTORICAL TRENDS9These early word association methods setthe stage for the development <strong>of</strong> otherassociation (projective) techniques, suchas the Thematic Apperception Technique<strong>and</strong> Rorschach’s test, both <strong>of</strong> which usedpictures in lieu <strong>of</strong> word lists to elicit associations.Thematic Apperception TestThe Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) <strong>of</strong>Henry A. Murray constitutes a prototypicalexample <strong>of</strong> a projective device. Charles E.Thompson summarized the central tenet<strong>of</strong> the projective approach in the followingquote taken from his 1949 adaptation <strong>of</strong>Murray’s TAT. 1If the pictures are presented as a test <strong>of</strong> imagination,the subject’s interest, together withhis need for approval, can be so involved inthe task that he forgets his sensitive self <strong>and</strong>the necessity <strong>of</strong> defending it against the probing<strong>of</strong> the examiner, <strong>and</strong>, before he knows it,he has said things about an invented characterthat apply to himself, things which he wouldhave been reluctant to confess in response to adirect question (p. 5).The TAT is unique among projective measuresin that it has traditionally beeninterpreted qualitatively, even though quantitativescoring methods are available(Kleinmuntz, 1967). Murray’s originalapproach to TAT scoring was entirelyqualitative <strong>and</strong> psychoanalytically based.He proposed the following categories foranalyzing the characteristics <strong>of</strong> the storiesgiven by the subject (Murray, 1943).1Thompson’s modification is identical to the originalTAT with the exception that African Americanfigures are used as characters on the stimulus cards.Thompson found that African Americans did notrespond optimally to the original TAT pictures. Infact, one <strong>of</strong> his patients asked if he could imagine thatthe people in the pictures were “colored,” <strong>and</strong> if hecould make up some stories about “colored people.”1. The Hero. This is the person with whomthe subject seems to identify. The heromay share characteristics such as age,gender, occupation, or other featureswith the subject that aid identification.The hero’s traits should be evaluatedto determine the self-perceptions <strong>of</strong>the subject including superiority, intelligence,leadership, belongingness, solitariness,<strong>and</strong> quarrelsomeness.2. Needs <strong>of</strong> the Hero. Needs may includethose for order, achievement, <strong>and</strong> nurturance.3. Environmental Forces. Factors that affectthe hero <strong>and</strong> these are also referred to aspress. An example would involve scoringaggression if the hero’s property or possessionswere destroyed in a story.4. Outcomes. The success <strong>of</strong> the hero <strong>and</strong>the hero’s competencies are assessed byevaluating the outcomes <strong>of</strong> stories.5. Themas. Themas assess the interplay <strong>of</strong>needs <strong>and</strong> presses, <strong>and</strong> they reveal theprimary concerns <strong>of</strong> the hero.6. Interests, Sentiments, <strong>and</strong> Relationships.For this aspect <strong>of</strong> scoring the examinerrecords the hero’s preferences for topics.Murray’s qualitative scoring system forthe TAT is a classic example <strong>of</strong> systemsthat dominated the early interpretation <strong>of</strong>projective devices. Numerous quantitativescoring systems followed Murray’s originalwork as exemplified by scoring systems eventuallydeveloped for Rorschach’s test.The RorschachHermann Rorschach (1884–1922) was amajor figure in Swiss psychiatric researchwho began his work as a physician in 1910.He married a Russian colleague who becamehis comrade <strong>and</strong> collaborator (Morgenthaler,1954). He served as a physician in a hospitalin Herisau until his death from complications<strong>of</strong> appendicitis in 1922. His death was


10CHAPTER 1 HISTORICAL TRENDSdescribed as a critical blow to Swiss psychiatry.In a 1954 eulogy to Dr. Rorschach,published in the English translation <strong>of</strong> hisoriginal work, Morgenthaler attempted todescribe Rorschach for future generations.Flexibility <strong>of</strong> character, rapid adaptability, fineacumen, <strong>and</strong> a sense for the practical werecombined in Hermann Rorschach with a talentfor introspection <strong>and</strong> synthesis. It was thiscombination which made him outst<strong>and</strong>ing. Inaddition to this rare nature, which temperedpersonal emotional experience with practicalknowledge, he possessed sound traits <strong>of</strong>character most valuable in a psychiatrist. Mostimportant <strong>of</strong> these were an unerring tendencyto search for the truth, a strict critical facultywhich he did not hesitate to apply to himself,<strong>and</strong> a warmth <strong>of</strong> feeling <strong>and</strong> kindness. (p. 9)Rorschach’s approach to personality assessmentwas novel in many respects. The teststimuli used were inkblots placed on paperthat was then folded in half. Rorschachwas not, however, interested in the content<strong>of</strong> the subject’s response to the inkblots.Rather, he was interested in the form <strong>of</strong>the response (or its function). Some functions<strong>of</strong> interest included the number <strong>of</strong>responses, perception <strong>of</strong> color or movement,<strong>and</strong> perception <strong>of</strong> the whole vs. theparts. These <strong>and</strong> other characteristics <strong>of</strong>Rorschach responses continue as part <strong>of</strong>modern scoring systems (see Chap. 10).Rorschach first <strong>of</strong>fered his methodas an experiment. His original sample isdescribed in Table 1.2. He expressed adesire for larger sample sizes but notedthat the number <strong>of</strong> experiments was limitedbecause the stimulus figures were damagedby passing through hundreds <strong>of</strong> h<strong>and</strong>s.Rorschach’s legacy, his original inkblots,<strong>and</strong> many <strong>of</strong> the associated scoring criteriaremain influential as the test continues toenjoy popularity. Several scoring systemshave been <strong>of</strong>fered for the Rorschach, withthe Exner Comprehensive System (Exner& Weiner, 1982) contributing most to thecontinuing usage <strong>of</strong> the instrument.Table 1.2 Rorschach’s Original ResearchSampleSampleNormal, educated 55Normal, uneducated 62Psychopathic personality 20Alcoholic cases 8Morons, imbeciles 12Schizophrenics 188Manic-depressives 14Epileptics 20Paretics 8Senile dements 10Arteriosclerotic dements 5Korsak<strong>of</strong>f <strong>and</strong> similar states 3Source: Adapted from Rorschach (1951).Sentence CompletionTechniquesSentence completion techniques are venerablepersonality assessment methods <strong>of</strong>the association tradition that can tracetheir roots to Payne (1928). The sentencecompletion method, however, obtained asubstantial boost in popularity because <strong>of</strong>its use by the U.S. Office <strong>of</strong> Strategic Services(OSS), the forerunner <strong>of</strong> the CentralIntelligence Agency (CIA). Henry Murraywas the coordinator <strong>of</strong> a sophisticatedOSS assessment effort. About 60 militaryassessment stations, staffed by Americanpsychologists, were situated in the USA<strong>and</strong> abroad to screen recruits for sensitive<strong>and</strong> dangerous assignments. Some <strong>of</strong> themethods used in this ambitious programare described in the following quote fromDuBois (1970):In one <strong>of</strong> the stations near Washington, recruitsin fatigue uniforms assumed a falseidentity <strong>and</strong> developed a cover story, which thestaff members during the three-day stay endeavoredto break. The procedures describedin a comprehensive report (OSS, <strong>Assessment</strong>Staff, 1948) were varied: casual conversations,N


CHAPTER 1 HISTORICAL TRENDS11searching interviews, the sentence completiontest, questionnaires about health <strong>and</strong> workingconditions <strong>and</strong> personal history, conventionalaptitude tests such as map memory <strong>and</strong> mechanicalcomprehension, <strong>and</strong> a number <strong>of</strong> situationaltests (p. 111).After World War II, the sentence completiontechnique continued to enjoy somefavor among psychologists. The wellknownRotter Incomplete Sentences Blankwas published in 1950 (Rotter & Rafferty,1950). Sentence completion methods havea lengthy history <strong>of</strong> use with children <strong>and</strong>adolescents as well. They enjoyed worldwideuse in countries such as Finl<strong>and</strong>, Germany,Denmark, India, Japan, <strong>and</strong> Taiwan(Haak, 1990).Projective Techniques for<strong>Child</strong>renThe use <strong>of</strong> projective techniques withchildren dates back to the early part <strong>of</strong>the twentieth century when FlorenceGoodenough began to study children’shuman figure drawings (DuBois, 1970).Goodenough noted, as did others, thatchildren’s drawings were affected bytheir emotionality. The typical paradigmfor drawing techniques has been tohave the child draw a picture <strong>of</strong> a person.Traditionally, the content <strong>of</strong> the drawingshas been interpreted as a measure <strong>of</strong>child adjustment <strong>and</strong> personality. Someaspects <strong>of</strong> content that were extensivelystudied for adults included (Swensen,1968):Size <strong>of</strong> the person depictedPlacement on the page (bottom, top, corner,etc.)Stance (vertical, horizontal, balanced, etc.)Line quality (heavy, light, etc.)ShadingErasuresOmissions (missing body parts)Distortion (poor proportion <strong>of</strong> body parts)Various interpretations have been associatedwith these <strong>and</strong> other content variablesover the years. Heavy lines, for example,have been associated with assertive <strong>and</strong>aggressive individuals <strong>and</strong> light lines havebeen viewed as being indicative <strong>of</strong> passiveindividuals (Koppitz, 1968). Swensen(1968) found such interpretations to behighly unreliable. The most reliable <strong>and</strong>valid interpretive approach involved makinggeneral judgments about the mentalhealth status <strong>of</strong> the individual based on theoverall quality <strong>of</strong> the drawing, rather thanspecific content interpretations (Cummings,1986).The TAT, among other projectivemethods, has also been adapted by manyfor use with children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. One<strong>of</strong> the most well-known TAT adaptationsis the <strong>Child</strong>ren’s Apperception Test (CAT)(Bellak & Bellak, 1949b), designed forages 3–10. The CAT consists <strong>of</strong> ten pictureswith animals as stimuli in contrast tothe TAT’s depictions primarily <strong>of</strong> people.The Rorschach has also been widely usedwith children, <strong>and</strong> several compendiums<strong>of</strong> child responses have been published toaid interpretation (e.g., Ames, Metraux,Rodell, & Walker, 1974).The proper interpretation <strong>of</strong> children’sprojective responses remains a topic <strong>of</strong> debate.Indeed, the degree to which children obeythe projective hypothesis has been questioned.Ch<strong>and</strong>ler (1990) elucidates the nature <strong>of</strong> theprojective hypothesis as follows:Projection, in common usage, means to castforward. In this sense, projection implies adirect extension <strong>of</strong> psychological characteristicsonto the outer world. But projectionalso has a specific meaning within psychoanalytictheory. Freud (1936) used the termto refer to the process that occurs when theego, faced with unacceptable wishes or ideas,thrusts them out onto the external world asa means <strong>of</strong> defense. In projection the individualattributes his or her own thoughts<strong>and</strong> actions to someone else. Thus, if one’sown faults or feelings are unacceptable to


12CHAPTER 1 HISTORICAL TRENDSthe ego, they may be seen as belonging tosomeone else; in the process, the materialmay become distorted or remain partiallyrepressed. From such a perspective, projectivematerial would not be seen as directrepresentation <strong>of</strong> aspects <strong>of</strong> the personality,certainly not with the sort <strong>of</strong> one-to-onecorrespondence that the first meaning <strong>of</strong>projection implies (p. 57).For adults as well as children, the process<strong>of</strong> projection still rests primarily on a theoreticalrather than empirical foundation. Inthe absence <strong>of</strong> data to support the projectivehypothesis, psychologists have focusedon the use <strong>of</strong> psychometric methods toassess the reliability <strong>of</strong> obtained scores<strong>and</strong> the validity <strong>of</strong> score inferences. Thisshift to the accumulation <strong>of</strong> psychometricevidence for measures is reflected bestin the work <strong>of</strong> Exner. In the 1960s, JohnExner began a research program designedto take the best <strong>of</strong> the Rorschach scoringsystems <strong>and</strong> incorporate their features intoa comprehensive system (Exner & Weiner,1982). Further, a st<strong>and</strong>ard method for scoringresponses on the test has led to scoresthat have proven to be reliable <strong>and</strong>, as aresult, has set the stage for direct tests <strong>of</strong>the validity <strong>of</strong> various interpretations thatcan be made from them. The application<strong>of</strong> psychometric st<strong>and</strong>ards to projectivemeasures is a clear departure from a longhistory <strong>of</strong> qualitative analysis <strong>and</strong> interpretation.The efforts <strong>of</strong> Exner <strong>and</strong> othershave set a new course for projective measuresin that they are increasingly held tothe same st<strong>and</strong>ard as tests <strong>of</strong> intelligence,adaptive behavior, <strong>and</strong> “objective” personalityassessment methods.Objective TestsAlthough we acknowledge that the distinctionbetween projective <strong>and</strong> objectivetesting is an oversimplification, it is neverthelessuseful for pedagogical purposes.Objective methods can be differentiatedfrom projective tests in several ways. First,objective methods are <strong>of</strong>ten consideredto be atheoretical <strong>and</strong>/or empirical. Asopposed to requiring the examiner to usetheory to interpret results, the results <strong>of</strong>tenderive their meaning from empirical procedures,such as matching a person’s resultsto those <strong>of</strong> a clinical sample. Second, objectivemethods are not likely to be based onpsychodynamic theory. Hence, the results<strong>of</strong> objective measures are <strong>of</strong>ten consideredto be less useful for providing insight intothe dynamics <strong>of</strong> an individual’s interactionswith the world. Third, objective methodstake greater advantage <strong>of</strong> measurementscience for the development <strong>of</strong> tests. Issues<strong>of</strong> item selection, reliability, <strong>and</strong> validityare <strong>of</strong>ten emphasized in the test manuals.Minnesota Multiphasic<strong>Personality</strong> Inventory (MMPI)Until the advent <strong>of</strong> the MMPI, projectivetechniques reigned supreme. In a 1961survey <strong>of</strong> tests used by psychologists in theUSA, the MMPI was the only nonprojectivemeasure mentioned among the topten most used tests. Of the top ten tests,five were intelligence tests <strong>and</strong> four wereprojective measures (Sundberg, 1961). Aconfluence <strong>of</strong> circumstances, includingthe expansion <strong>of</strong> clinical psychology practiceduring <strong>and</strong> after World War II, <strong>and</strong>the emergence <strong>of</strong> an extensive researchbase led to almost immediate acceptance<strong>of</strong> this self-report personality inventory(Kleinmuntz, 1967). Further, the MMPIwas one <strong>of</strong> the first tests to gain popularitywith others outside <strong>of</strong> the mental healthpr<strong>of</strong>essions (see Box 1.1). However, thispopularity led to significant friction <strong>and</strong>disagreements over the relative merits<strong>of</strong> the MMPI, <strong>and</strong> its objective methods,compared with the popular projectivetechniques. This tension is reflected in thecomments <strong>of</strong> Paul Meehl that are summarizedin Box 1.2.The MMPI (Hathaway & McKinley,1942) differed from its predecessors (such


CHAPTER 1 HISTORICAL TRENDS13Box 1.1Sample Items from 1960s MMPISpo<strong>of</strong>s<strong>Personality</strong> testing eventually became popularenough to warrant derision by members<strong>of</strong> Congress, well-known humorists such asArt Buchwald, <strong>and</strong> others. Some <strong>of</strong> thesealternate MMPI items were published in a1965 issue <strong>of</strong> American Psychologist (p. 990)to poke fun at this method <strong>of</strong> personalityassessment.When I was younger I used to tease vegetables.I think beavers work too hard.I use shoe polish to excess.When I was a child I was an imaginary playmate.Box 1.2Meehl on Science <strong>and</strong> TechnicsPaul Meehl is considered one <strong>of</strong> the founders<strong>of</strong> modern personality assessment <strong>and</strong> diagnosticpractice. His 1973 collection <strong>of</strong> selectedpapers published by the University <strong>of</strong> MinnesotaPress provides a unique glimpse <strong>of</strong> thegenius <strong>of</strong> an astute clinician. In the followingquote, Dr. Meehl discusses the tension betweenscience <strong>and</strong> practice in psychology <strong>and</strong> takes astance against theoretical dogmatism:Doubtless every applied science (orwould-be science) presents aspects <strong>of</strong> thisproblem to those working at the interfacebetween science <strong>and</strong> technics, as isapparent when one listens to practicingattorneys talking about law pr<strong>of</strong>essors,practitioners <strong>of</strong> medicine complainingabout medical school teaching, real engineersin industry poking ambivalent funat academic physicists, <strong>and</strong> the like. So Ido not suggest that the existential predicament<strong>of</strong> the clinical psychologist is uniquein this respect, which it certainly is not.But I strongly suspect that there arefew if any fields <strong>of</strong> applied semiscientificknowledge in which the practitionerwith scientific interests <strong>and</strong> trainingis presented daily with this problem inone guise or another, or in which its poignancy,urgency, <strong>and</strong> cognitive tensionsare so acute. I am aware that there aresome clinical psychologists who do notexperience this conflict, but I have met,read, or listened to very few such duringthe thirty years since I first began workingwith patients as a clinical psychologytrainee. Further, these rare exceptionshave seemed to me in every case to beeither lacking in perceptiveness <strong>and</strong> imaginationor, more <strong>of</strong>ten, deficient in scientifictraining <strong>and</strong> critical habits <strong>of</strong> mind.When I encounter a hard-nosedbehaviorist clinician who knows (for sure)that Freud’s theory <strong>of</strong> dreams is 100 percenthogwash <strong>and</strong> is not worth five hours<strong>of</strong> his serious attention; or, toward theother end <strong>of</strong> the continuum, when I conversewith a devoted Rorschacher whoknows (for sure) that the magic inkblotsare highly valid no matter what the publishedresearch data indicate-I find both<strong>of</strong> these attitudes hard to underst<strong>and</strong> orsympathize with (p. viii).as the Personal Data Sheet) in at least onefundamental way. It was one <strong>of</strong> the firsttests to use an empirical approach to objectivepersonality test development. Mosttests <strong>of</strong> the day used a priori or rational–theoretical approaches (Martin, 1988).Rational approaches, as the name implies,depend heavily on the test author’s theory<strong>of</strong> personality for many aspects <strong>of</strong> testconstruction, including item development<strong>and</strong> scoring methods. On the other h<strong>and</strong>,empirical approaches make greater use <strong>of</strong>empirical data to make such decisions (seeChap. 2 for a more detailed discussion <strong>of</strong>this distinction).The MMPI used an item selection methodcalled empirical criterion keying (Anastasi &


14CHAPTER 1 HISTORICAL TRENDSUrbina, 1998). Simply stated, this methodinvolved selecting items that meet an empiricalcriterion. In the case <strong>of</strong> the MMPI, itemswere selected if they were able to routinelydifferentiate clinical groups from samples <strong>of</strong>“normal” subjects, <strong>and</strong> distinguish clinicalgroups from one another. For example, itemsfor the Psychasthenia scale (a scale designedto assess anxiety-related problems such asobsessions <strong>and</strong> fears) were selected basedon a clinical group <strong>of</strong> 20 cases, the results <strong>of</strong>which were compared with “normals” <strong>and</strong>other clinical groups to identify items thatbest differentiated the target clinical groupfrom the others.The original version <strong>of</strong> the MMPI consisted<strong>of</strong> 550 statements printed on separatecards. The cards were separated by thepatient into three categories: true, false,<strong>and</strong> cannot say. The first MMPI clinicalscales were linked to the major diagnosticnosology <strong>of</strong> the day (Kleinmuntz, 1967),which is another factor that contributedto its popularity. The ten clinical scales <strong>of</strong>the original version included are providedin Table 1.3. The MMPI has undergoneTable 1.3 The Original Scales from theMMPI<strong>Clinical</strong> scalesHypochondriasisDepressionHysteriaPsychopathic deviateMasculinity–FemininityParanoiaPsychastheniaSchizophreniaHypomaniaSocial IntroversionValidity scalesQuestion scaleLie scaleF scaleCorrection scaleSource: Kleinmuntz, 1967.many changes since its inception, with themost recent edition entitled the MMPI-2. Infact, some <strong>of</strong> the scale names (e.g., Psychasthenia)had fallen into disuse at about the time<strong>of</strong> original publication (Kleinmuntz, 1967). Achronology <strong>of</strong> MMPI developments is listednext <strong>and</strong> a thorough discussion <strong>of</strong> this importantmeasure is provided in Chapter 6.MMPI VersionMMPI 1942MMPI-2 1989MMPI-A (<strong>Adolescent</strong>) 1992Publication DateThe “<strong>Child</strong>ren’s MMPI”Not surprisingly, the MMPI pr<strong>of</strong>oundlyinfluenced child assessment practiceincluding the development <strong>of</strong> the <strong>Personality</strong>Inventory for <strong>Child</strong>ren (PIC) inthe 1950s. The PIC was based on a pool<strong>of</strong> 600 items; hence, it was comparable inlength to the MMPI. A central differencebetween the MMPI <strong>and</strong> the PIC was theinformant. The PIC was not a self-reportmeasure. Instead, a parent rated the child’sbehavior. Lachar (1990) gave the followingrationale for this decision:Selection <strong>of</strong> the parents as the source <strong>of</strong> PICtest responses helps overcome two <strong>of</strong> themajor obstacles posed by requesting the referredchild or adolescent to respond to numerousself-report descriptions in order toobtain a multiple-scale objective evaluation.The majority <strong>of</strong> children seen by mentalhealth pr<strong>of</strong>essionals in a variety <strong>of</strong> settingsappear for such an evaluation because <strong>of</strong>their noncompliant behaviors <strong>and</strong>/or documentedproblems in academic achievement,most notably in the development <strong>of</strong> readingskills. Therefore, it seems unlikely thata technique requiring such children to read<strong>and</strong> respond to a large set <strong>of</strong> self-descriptionswill find broad acceptance in routine clinicalpractice (p. 299).


CHAPTER 1 HISTORICAL TRENDS15The scales <strong>of</strong> the PIC were derived usingfactor-analytic methods. Thus, the PIC,like the MMPI, was developed with aheavy emphasis on empirical methods (seeChap. 6). In the 1960s, empirical methods<strong>of</strong> test development were also applied tothe development <strong>of</strong> other types <strong>of</strong> childassessment devices.Rating ScalesParent <strong>and</strong> teacher ratings <strong>of</strong> children’sbehavior <strong>and</strong> emotions trace their rootsto the assessment <strong>of</strong> adult psychopathologyin hospital settings. Conceptualizedas one type <strong>of</strong> observational method, ratingscales were developed in the 1950sfor use by nurses <strong>and</strong> other caretakerswho worked closely with patients forextended periods <strong>of</strong> time. One <strong>of</strong> the firstsuch measures was the Wittenborn PsychiatricRating Scales (1955). Accordingto Lorr (1965), the scales were designedfor recording currently observable behavior<strong>and</strong> symptoms in hospitalized mentalpatients. The Wittenborn could be completedby a social worker, psychologist,psychiatrist, nurse, attendant, or otherindividual familiar with the patient’s dayto-daybehavior. The original scale consisted<strong>of</strong> 52 symptoms that were combinedto yield 9 scores for acute anxiety, conversionhysteria, manic state, depressedstate, schizophrenic excitement, paranoidcondition, paranoid schizophrenic,hebephrenic schizophrenic, <strong>and</strong> phobiccompulsive. An item assessing withdrawalincluded the following options:No evidence <strong>of</strong> social withdrawalDoes not appear to seek out the company <strong>of</strong>other peopleDefinitely avoids peopleThe Wittenborn was used for diagnosticpurposes as well as for the design <strong>and</strong> evaluation<strong>of</strong> treatment (Kleinmuntz, 1967).Reviewers <strong>of</strong> the day found many reasonsto recommend the Wittenborn, includinga thorough research base (Eysenck, 1965)<strong>and</strong> easy administration <strong>and</strong> scoring (Lorr,1965). There was considerable concern,however, about overlapping scales. Thehebephrenic schizophrenic <strong>and</strong> schizophrenicexcitement scales correlated at .88<strong>and</strong> the paranoid condition <strong>and</strong> paranoidschizophrenic scales correlated at .79. Onthe basis <strong>of</strong> these data, Eysenck (1965) <strong>and</strong>Lorr (1965) recommended that these scalesbe combined to reflect this overlap.Other rating scales <strong>of</strong> adult psychopathologyfor use in inpatient settings included theHospital Adjustment Scale (McReynolds,Ballachey, & Ferguson, 1952) <strong>and</strong> the InpatientMultidimensional Psychiatric Scale(Lorr, 1965), a rating <strong>of</strong> symptomatologycompleted by the clinician after a diagnosticinterview. Such measures probably fell intodecline for many reasons, one <strong>of</strong> the mostprominent being the deinstitutionalizationmovement <strong>of</strong> the 1970s. These instrumentsdid, however, clearly demonstrate the utility<strong>of</strong> ratings <strong>of</strong> behavior as practical <strong>and</strong> usefulassessment tools. These scales set the stagefor the development <strong>of</strong> parent <strong>and</strong> teacherrating scales <strong>of</strong> child behavior.Internalizing <strong>and</strong> ExternalizingDimensionsResearch into the diagnosis <strong>of</strong> child psychopathologyled to increased attention tothe use <strong>of</strong> rating scales for child diagnosis.In a 1978 article in Psychological Bulletin,Thomas Achenbach <strong>and</strong> Craig Edelbrockintroduced many clinicians to the termsinternalizing <strong>and</strong> externalizing psychologicaldisorders <strong>of</strong> childhood. These dimensions,or types <strong>of</strong> child psychopathology,were based on an extensive empirical analysis(typically using factor analysis) <strong>of</strong> parent<strong>and</strong> teacher behavior problem ratingscales. <strong>Child</strong>ren experiencing adjustmentdifficulties <strong>of</strong> the internalizing variety havealso been described as over-controlled,with problems <strong>of</strong> inhibition, anxiety, <strong>and</strong>,perhaps, shyness (Edelbrock, 1979). On


16CHAPTER 1 HISTORICAL TRENDSthe other h<strong>and</strong>, children with externalizingproblems have been described asundercontrolled with difficulties such asaggression, conduct problems, <strong>and</strong> actingoutbehavior (Edelbrock, 1979).These two dimensions <strong>of</strong> child psychopathologytrace their roots to the work<strong>of</strong> Peterson (1961), who labeled the syndromesas conduct problem (externalizing)<strong>and</strong> personality problem (internalizing). Theveracity <strong>of</strong> the broad internalizing <strong>and</strong>externalizing categorizations <strong>of</strong> child psychopathologyis supported by many factor-analyticinvestigations <strong>of</strong> both parent<strong>and</strong> teacher rating scales alike (Edelbrock,1979). The utility <strong>of</strong> these behavioral distinctionswas also demonstrated in an earlystudy <strong>of</strong> 163 consecutive referrals to a childpsychiatry outpatient department (Cohen,Gotlieb, Kershner, & Wehrspann, 1985).<strong>Child</strong>ren were classified as externalizers<strong>and</strong> internalizers based on the Achenbach<strong>Child</strong> Behavior Checklist (CBCL; a parentreport form) <strong>and</strong> the Teacher Report Form(TRF) (see Chap. 7). The resulting analysesuncovered distinct differences betweenthe two groups, particularly on the TeacherForm. Internalizers were found to be moreintelligent, better readers, less egocentric,<strong>and</strong> they used more adaptive means <strong>of</strong> copingwith stressful situations. Internalizerswere also generally rated as being less disruptivethan externalizers.Numerous independent researchstudies, many <strong>of</strong> which have been conductedinternationally (Ivanova et al.,2007), have demonstrated strong factoranalyticsupport for these two types <strong>of</strong>child behavioral adjustment. This preponderance<strong>of</strong> evidence, supported byother types <strong>of</strong> validity evidence, hasresulted in these factors serving as thefoundation for the development <strong>of</strong> manyteacher <strong>and</strong> parent rating scales, mostnotably the Achenbach <strong>Child</strong> BehaviorChecklist (Achenbach, 1991b) <strong>and</strong> theBehavior <strong>Assessment</strong> System for <strong>Child</strong>ren(BASC-2; Reynolds & Kamphaus,2004). Furthermore, the terms internalizing<strong>and</strong> externalizing are now a part <strong>of</strong>psychologists’ everyday parlance whendiscussing child behavior problems.The Diagnostic <strong>and</strong>Statistical Manual<strong>of</strong> Mental DisordersDiagnostic SystemsDiagnostic systems have had a pr<strong>of</strong>oundimpact on child assessment by definingsymptoms <strong>and</strong> other diagnostic indicesthat have subsequently been incorporatedinto various assessment methods. The mostobvious link exists between the various editions<strong>of</strong> the Diagnostic <strong>and</strong> Statistical Manual<strong>of</strong> Mental Disorders (currently DSM-IV-TR;American Psychiatric Association, 2000)<strong>and</strong> structured interview methods designedto assess symptomatology associated withvarious DSM diagnostic categories (seeChap. 11). Given this interdependence, athorough knowledge <strong>of</strong> the nature <strong>of</strong> theDSM <strong>and</strong> its variants is prerequisite to thestudy <strong>of</strong> child assessment.As mental disorders became recognizedas conditions worthy <strong>of</strong> medical treatment,the need for diagnostic systems becamemore pressing. Consistent diagnosis wasnecessary for communication among clinicians<strong>and</strong> for the conduct <strong>of</strong> epidemiologicalresearch <strong>and</strong> other scientific investigations.The American Medico-PsychologicalAssociation (now the American PsychiatricAssociation) began efforts to st<strong>and</strong>ardizediagnostic procedures in 1917. The firstdiagnostic manual, a classification <strong>of</strong> mentaldisease, was produced by the AmericanPsychiatric Association in conjunction withthe U.S. Census Bureau (Widiger, Frances,Pincus, Davis, & First, 1991).The first edition <strong>of</strong> the DSM (Diagnostic<strong>and</strong> Statistical Manual) appeared in 1952.Part <strong>of</strong> the impetus for the creation <strong>and</strong>


CHAPTER 1 HISTORICAL TRENDS17frequent updating <strong>of</strong> the DSM has beenprovided by the International StatisticalClassification <strong>of</strong> Diseases, Injuries, <strong>and</strong> Causes<strong>of</strong> Death (ICD). The ICD, currently ICD-11,is published by the World Health Organization.The DSM has been revised both tocoordinate with the ICD <strong>and</strong> to add criteriafor conditions that are <strong>of</strong> concern to USclinicians, <strong>and</strong> delete conditions that arenot apparent in the USA (Widiger et al.,1991). The DSM has also been revisedbecause <strong>of</strong> a desire to make the diagnosticcategories more evidence-based. Priorto the development <strong>of</strong> the DSM-III, thesystem was based primarily on the expertjudgment <strong>of</strong> a relatively small number <strong>of</strong>clinicians. The DSM-II, for example, wasfinalized after review by 120 psychiatristsin February <strong>of</strong> 1967 (Widiger et al., 1991).The DSM-IV (APA, 1994) was based ona more comprehensive research base thanany <strong>of</strong> its predecessors. According to Widigeret al. (1991), three research methodshave formed the empirical cornerstone forthe development <strong>of</strong> DSM-IV.1. Literature reviews: Comprehensivereviews <strong>of</strong> the research were completedto advise the various committeescharged with proposing diagnostic criteriafor conditions. These reviews wereseen as a way to mitigate against biaseson the part <strong>of</strong> some committees (Widigeret al., 1991).2. Data reanalyses: Existing data sets weremade available to the DSM-IV committeessupported by funding from theJohn D. <strong>and</strong> Catherine T. MacArthurFoundation. According to Widigeret al. (1991), these data set reanalysesallowed the committees to evaluatethe validity <strong>of</strong> current diagnostic algorithms<strong>and</strong> pilot-test new proposals formaking diagnoses.3. Field trials: These studies were particularlyuseful for testing the reliability <strong>and</strong>validity <strong>of</strong> diagnostic categories (Widigeret al., 1991).Table 1.4 Chronology <strong>of</strong> Diagnostic SystemsDeveloped Under the Auspices <strong>of</strong> theAmerican Psychiatric Association1917 Classification <strong>of</strong> Mental Disease1933 St<strong>and</strong>ard Classified Nomenclature<strong>of</strong> Disease1952 Diagnostic <strong>and</strong> Statistical Manual<strong>of</strong> Mental Disorders I (DSM-I)1968 Diagnostic <strong>and</strong> Statistical Manual<strong>of</strong> Mental Disorders II (DSM-II)1980 Diagnostic <strong>and</strong> Statistical Manual<strong>of</strong> Mental Disorders III (DSM-III)1987 Diagnostic <strong>and</strong> Statistical Manual<strong>of</strong> Mental Disorders III-Revised(DSM-III-R)1994 Diagnostic <strong>and</strong> Statistical Manual<strong>of</strong> Mental Disorders IV (DSM-IV)2000 Diagnostic <strong>and</strong> Statistical Manual<strong>of</strong> Mental Disorders IV-Text Revision(DSM-IV-TR)The DSM-IV-TR, because <strong>of</strong> its greaterreliance on empirical methods, has had aneven more substantial impact on the personalityassessment process (see Chap. 3).The chronology <strong>of</strong> the DSM is provided inTable 1.4.IDEA <strong>and</strong> SpecialEducationThe 1974 Education <strong>of</strong> H<strong>and</strong>icapped <strong>Child</strong>ren’sAct, better known as Public Law94–142 (IDEA), <strong>and</strong> its reauthorization,the Individuals with Disabilities EducationImprovement Act (IDEIA), m<strong>and</strong>atedspecial education <strong>and</strong> related services forchildren classified as having an emotionaldisturbance. As a result, some method hadto be developed to define child problems<strong>and</strong> determine children’s eligibility for specialeducation services. Under IDEA, <strong>and</strong>the subsequent IDEIA with few substantive


18CHAPTER 1 HISTORICAL TRENDSchanges, the classification <strong>of</strong> severe emotionaldisturbance was defined as follows:The term means a condition exhibiting oneor more <strong>of</strong> the following characteristics overa long period <strong>of</strong> time <strong>and</strong> to a marked degreewhich adversely affects school performance:(a) an inability to learn which cannot be explainedby intellectual, sensory, or healthfactors; (b) an inability to build or maintainsatisfactory relationships with peers <strong>and</strong>teachers; (c) inappropriate types <strong>of</strong> behavioror feelings under normal circumstances; (d)a general pervasive mood <strong>of</strong> unhappinessor depression; or (e) a tendency to developphysical symptoms or fears associated withpersonal or school problems (Federal Register,1999).With its passage in the 1970s, this laweffectively m<strong>and</strong>ated US public schoolsto identify <strong>and</strong> serve children withbehavioral or emotional problems, many<strong>of</strong> which had previously been educated ina variety <strong>of</strong> settings, including residentialtreatment programs or state mental hospitals.Consequently, these laws exp<strong>and</strong>edschool-based diagnostic practices toinclude evaluation for the presence <strong>of</strong>“emotional disturbance,” just as had beenmore commonly done for developmental<strong>and</strong> learning disorders. These federalm<strong>and</strong>ates also enhanced the popularity<strong>of</strong> rating scales (particularly teacher ratings)as assessment methods <strong>of</strong> choice inmany school systems.The IDEA nosology <strong>of</strong> emotional disturbance(the word severe has now beenremoved) has long been the target <strong>of</strong> criticismsthat it is invalid, restrictive, or otherwiseflawed (Forness & Kritzer, 1992).Bower (1982), the recognized developer<strong>of</strong> the conceptual basis <strong>of</strong> the IDEA diagnosticcategories, raised similar questionsabout the system. He noted that:Section ii [which excludes the socially maladjustedfrom the IDEA act] is, one wouldguess, a codicil to reassure traditional psychopathologists<strong>and</strong> budget personnel thatschizophrenia <strong>and</strong> autism are indeed seriousemotional disturbances on the one h<strong>and</strong>, <strong>and</strong>that just plain bad boys <strong>and</strong> girls, predelinquents,<strong>and</strong> sociopaths will not skyrocket thecosts on the other h<strong>and</strong>. It is clear what thesemodifications <strong>and</strong> additions were intendedto do. It is perhaps not clear what such publicpolicy <strong>and</strong> fiscal modifications do to theconceptual integrity <strong>of</strong> the definition <strong>and</strong> thenature <strong>and</strong> design <strong>of</strong> its goals (p. 56).Despite such controversy, the IDEA classificationsystem remains as the “gold st<strong>and</strong>ard”nosology when determining childeligibility for <strong>of</strong>t-costly special education<strong>and</strong> related services.Constructs (Dimensions)vs. CategoriesThe measurement problems associatedwith categorical diagnostic systems suchas the DSM are well known (Kamphaus& Campbell, 2006), <strong>and</strong> the advantages<strong>of</strong> using dimensional methods are recognizedas well. Achenbach <strong>and</strong> McConaughy(1996), for example, noted thatthe yes/no nature <strong>of</strong> categorical methodsdoes not necessarily account for childrenwhose problems vary in degree orseverity. As a result, the nexus betweennormality <strong>and</strong> psychopathology cannotbe well understood with categoricalmethods, since most high prevalenceproblem behaviors <strong>of</strong> childhood, suchas inattention <strong>and</strong> hyperactivity, arenot classifiable when below diagnosticthreshold levels. Substantial evidence isemerging to suggest that child behaviorproblems such as inattention, hyperactivity,depression, <strong>and</strong> conduct problems,in fact, fall along continua in thepopulation (Hudziak et al., 1998; Scahillet al., 1999).As a result, dimensional classificationmethods have demonstrated their usefulness


CHAPTER 1 HISTORICAL TRENDS19in the study <strong>of</strong> psychopathology. Forexample, dimensional approaches havedemonstrated more predictive validitythan categorical approaches (Fergusson& Horwood, 1995), as well as statisticalreliability (Cantwell, 1996). Such methodsalso minimize the need for clinicaljudgment <strong>and</strong> inference (Haynes &O’Brien, 1988), provide greater sensitivityto the presence <strong>of</strong> comorbid conditions(Caron & Rutter, 1991), <strong>and</strong> havethe ability to depict multiple symptompatterns in a given individual simultaneously(Cantwell, 1996). Furthermore,the use <strong>of</strong> dimensional, person-orientedapproaches to identify subtypes or clusters<strong>of</strong> individuals may lead to moreefficient, streamlined subtype-specificintervention <strong>and</strong> prevention services(Achenbach, 1995; Bergman & Magnusson,1997). The overlap <strong>and</strong> tensionsbetween categorical <strong>and</strong> dimensionalclassification methods will be elucidatedthroughout this text. It is important todo so because a merger <strong>of</strong> these methodologiesis likely in the DSM-V <strong>and</strong> otherfuture diagnostic systems (Rounsavilleet al., 2002).Future Trendsassessment. The developments outlinedabove encompass observable behavior,structured <strong>and</strong> unstructured use <strong>of</strong> tests<strong>and</strong> interviews, <strong>and</strong> assessment <strong>of</strong> broad<strong>and</strong>narrow-b<strong>and</strong> aspects <strong>of</strong> personality, allwithin the context <strong>of</strong> a person’s situation/environment. Thus, in spite <strong>of</strong> variouscriticisms <strong>and</strong> some apparent decrease inthe use <strong>of</strong> personality assessment instruments(they were referring to projective devicesprimarily), all indications point to vigorousactivity in the area that promises tocontinue.” (p. 23; italics added).In the past, the technology <strong>of</strong> personalityassessment has been viewed as laggingbehind other areas <strong>of</strong> assessment, suchas intelligence <strong>and</strong> achievement testing(Martin, 1998). This conclusion is nolonger true. New measurement sciencerigor is being applied to the development<strong>of</strong> behavioral rating scales, interviewmethods, <strong>and</strong> diagnostic systems.Two trends <strong>of</strong> the past few decades arecontinuing; relatively less emphasis ontraining in projective methods (Belter &Piotrowski, 1999) <strong>and</strong> increased use <strong>of</strong>rating scales (Archer & Newsom, 2000).In fact, a veritable explosion in the creation<strong>and</strong> publication <strong>of</strong> behavior ratingscales alone necessitated creation <strong>of</strong> this,our third edition <strong>of</strong> this text.The pace <strong>of</strong> change in personality assessmentis ever hastening. There is increasinginterest in the development <strong>of</strong> newchild assessment methods, providingclinicians with a wide array <strong>of</strong> assessmentoptions. In 1990, Tuma <strong>and</strong> Elbert(1990) identified test development <strong>and</strong>research trends that remain true to thepresent day.It is apparent that personality assessmentis undergoing rapid development in allareas: projective, objective, <strong>and</strong> behavioralassessment; clinical interviewing <strong>and</strong>informal assessment; <strong>and</strong> environmentalChapter Summary1. <strong>Personality</strong> is typically considered to becomposed <strong>of</strong> traits, a more enduring set<strong>of</strong> characteristics <strong>of</strong> the individual.2. Formal personality measures emerged as alogical outgrowth <strong>of</strong> other efforts to measureindividual differences, most notablythe experimental methods <strong>of</strong> Wundt,Galton, <strong>and</strong> others.3. The Woodworth Personal Data Sheetwas published in 1918 as a result <strong>of</strong> the


20CHAPTER 1 HISTORICAL TRENDSsurge <strong>of</strong> interest in testing potentialsoldiers.4. The needs for diagnosis created by WorldWar I <strong>and</strong> World War II provided consider-ableimpetus for the development <strong>of</strong>personality tests.5. A major assumption underlying projectivetesting is that the use <strong>of</strong> stimuli thatare prone to a variety <strong>of</strong> interpretationswill encourage clients to reveal informationthat they otherwise would notshare in response to direct questioning.6. The Rorschach test stimuli were originallyinkblots placed on paper that wasthen folded in half.7. The use <strong>of</strong> projective techniques withchildren dates back to the early part <strong>of</strong>this century, when Florence Goodenoughbegan to study children’s humanfigure drawings.8. The MMPI was one <strong>of</strong> the first tests touse an empirical approach for personalitytest development <strong>and</strong> used an itemselection method called empirical criterionkeying.9. The use <strong>of</strong> informant rating scales forthe assessment <strong>of</strong> child psychopathologytraces its roots to the assessment <strong>of</strong> adultpsychopathology in hospital settings.10. In a 1978 article in Psychological Bulletin,Achenbach <strong>and</strong> Edelbrock introducedthe terms internalizing <strong>and</strong> externalizingwhen referring to psychological disorders<strong>of</strong> childhood.11. The first edition <strong>of</strong> the DSM (Diagnostic<strong>and</strong> Statistical Manual) appeared in1952.12. The most recent edition <strong>of</strong> the manual,the DSM-IV-TR, is based on a morecomprehensive research base than any<strong>of</strong> its predecessors.13. The 1974 Education <strong>of</strong> all H<strong>and</strong>icapped<strong>Child</strong>ren’s Act, better knownas Public Law 94–142, <strong>and</strong> its reauthorization,the Individuals withDisabilities Education ImprovementAct (IDEIA), have m<strong>and</strong>ated specialeducation <strong>and</strong> related services forchildren classified as emotionally disturbed.


C h a p t e r 2Measurement IssuesChapter Questionsl What type <strong>of</strong> information is yieldedfrom a T-score?l How does skewness affect scaling decisions?l How has factor analysis been used todevelop personality tests <strong>and</strong> diagnosticschedules?Users <strong>of</strong> instruments assessing personality<strong>and</strong> other aspects <strong>of</strong> behavioral, emotional,<strong>and</strong> social functioning should havea thorough underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> measurementprinciples. The discussion that follows,however, hardly qualifies as thoroughbecause measurement instruction is notthe purpose <strong>of</strong> this book. This chaptermerely points out some <strong>of</strong> the most importantmeasurement concepts for conductingassessments <strong>of</strong> youth.We assume that the user <strong>of</strong> this texthas had, at a minimum, undergraduatecourses in statistics, tests <strong>and</strong> measurements,as well as at least one graduatelevelmeasurement course. If a user <strong>of</strong>this text is not acquainted with some<strong>of</strong> the principles discussed here, then astatistics <strong>and</strong>/or measurement textbookshould be consulted. There are a number<strong>of</strong> excellent measurement textbooksavailable, including Anastasi <strong>and</strong> Urbina(1998) as well as Allen <strong>and</strong> Yen (1979).The reader is also referred to the Code<strong>of</strong> Fair Testing Practices in Education(Joint Committee on Testing Practices,2004) for a discussion <strong>of</strong> the appropriateprocedures for test development, testselection, scoring, interpretation, <strong>and</strong>communication <strong>of</strong> results.This chapter begins by defining thenature <strong>of</strong> the tests that assess psychologicalconstructs. Then, a review <strong>of</strong> basic principles21P.J. Frick et al., <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Behavior,DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-0641-0_2, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010


22 CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUES<strong>of</strong> statistics <strong>and</strong> measurement is presented,including topics ranging from measures <strong>of</strong>central tendency to factor analysis. The lastpart <strong>of</strong> the chapter introduces measurementissues that are specific to the use <strong>and</strong> interpretation<strong>of</strong> personality tests <strong>and</strong> similarinstruments.Defining <strong>Personality</strong> TestsThere is a plethora <strong>of</strong> methods, includingtests, designed to assess similar-soundingpsychological constructs, including personalityscales, behavior rating scales, <strong>and</strong>diagnostic schedules. The available personalitymeasures differ to such an extentthat they can be subtyped in order to clarifytheir psychometric properties. A definitionfor a psychological test, taken from an early,well-known personality assessment text,may be a good starting point. Kleinmuntz(1967) defines a psychological (includingpersonality) test by observing, “A psychologicaltest is a st<strong>and</strong>ardized instrument orsystematic procedure designed to obtain anobjective measure <strong>of</strong> a sample <strong>of</strong> behavior”(pp. 27–28). This rather broad definitionprovides a useful starting point for conceptualizingthe great variety <strong>of</strong> measuresavailable.The central characteristic <strong>of</strong> this definitionis the notion <strong>of</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardization <strong>of</strong>behavioral sampling. St<strong>and</strong>ardization has atleast two meanings: st<strong>and</strong>ardization in thesense <strong>of</strong> collecting a sample for the purpose<strong>of</strong> norm referencing <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardization asadministration <strong>of</strong> the measure accordingto a consistent set <strong>of</strong> rules. Most <strong>of</strong> themeasures discussed in this volume fit thefirst notion <strong>of</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardization in that theyare norm-referenced. That is, these measuresuse norm groups for gauging a child’sperformance in comparison to some referencegroup. Furthermore, the principle<strong>of</strong> administration structure or consistencyapplies to all <strong>of</strong> the measures in this text.For example, respondents should completethe measure in an environment free <strong>of</strong> distractions<strong>and</strong> should clearly comprehendthe response-format (e.g., true/false, frequencyratings, etc.) <strong>and</strong> time frame (e.g.,the last 6 months) referenced by the test.St<strong>and</strong>ardized procedure emanates fromexperimental psychology, where laboratorycontrol is central to obtaining reliable<strong>and</strong> valid results (Kamphaus, 2001). Similarly,in the case <strong>of</strong> personality assessment,st<strong>and</strong>ardized administration procedureis necessary to produce reliable <strong>and</strong> validmeasurements <strong>of</strong> behavior.All psychological tests take a sample <strong>of</strong>behavior from which the findings are subsequentlygeneralized (Anastasi & Urbina,1998). This ability to generalize findings isthe central strength <strong>of</strong> psychological tests<strong>and</strong> is probably the reason for their widespreaduse. Without these tests, psychologicalmeasurement would be impracticalbecause <strong>of</strong> the time <strong>and</strong> expense required.Of course, a sample can always be in error, afact that should always be considered wheninterpreting results (Dahlstrom, 1993).Types <strong>of</strong> TestsHow does one identify an instrument thatassesses personality or behavioral, social, <strong>and</strong>emotional functioning? <strong>Personality</strong> tests havetraditionally attempted to assess personalitytraits such as introversion, agreeableness, <strong>and</strong>anxiety. As noted in Chap. 1, traits are usuallyconsidered to be relatively stable characteristics<strong>of</strong> the individual (Martin, 1988). Forchildren <strong>and</strong> adolescents, such characteristicsmay be similarly conceptualized underthe term personality traits or, typically foryounger children, temperament. Research hasclearly indicated that individual differencesin a variety <strong>of</strong> personality domains in youthare measurable (see Shiner & Caspi, 2003),<strong>and</strong> relatively stable (e.g., Durbin, Hayden,Klein, & Olino, 2007; Hampson, Andrews,Barckley, & Peterson, 2007).


CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUES23Rating scales, one <strong>of</strong> the most popularchild assessment methods, may fall into yetanother category <strong>of</strong> test called diagnosticschedules (Kamphaus et al., 1995). Kamphauset al. define a diagnostic schedule as aspecialized psychometric method that providesa structured procedure for collecting<strong>and</strong> categorizing behavioral data that correspondto diagnostic categories or systems.A diagnostic schedule, then, is not designedto assess a trait, but rather to diagnose a syndrome.How does one identify a diagnosticschedule? One clue is the source <strong>of</strong> the itempool. The <strong>Child</strong>ren’s Depression Inventory(CDI; Kovacs, 1992) is a popular measure<strong>of</strong> childhood depression that used the DSMas its item source. It was designed to assessthe symptoms <strong>of</strong> depression in order toassist with making the diagnosis <strong>of</strong> depression.It was not designed to assess a stablepersonality trait or temperament but ratherto allow the examiner to make the diagnosis<strong>of</strong> depression with confidence. In fact, a cutscore that indicates the possible presence <strong>of</strong>clinical depression is <strong>of</strong>ten used for interpretingscores (Kovacs).However, adding further complexityto underst<strong>and</strong>ing how rating scalesfit within the array <strong>of</strong> tools availablefor clinical assessments is the fact thatmany widely used rating scales cannotbe considered diagnostic schedules. Forexample, although the Behavior <strong>Assessment</strong>System for <strong>Child</strong>ren-2 (BASC-2;Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004) assessesclinically relevant domains for youth(e.g., hyperactivity, aggression, depression,anxiety), elevated scores on thosedomains do not necessarily mean that theindividual being assessed meets the criteriafor a corresponding diagnosis. Suchrating systems routinely have items thatdo not directly map onto the diagnosticcriteria. Rather, the content <strong>of</strong> thesescales may be indicative <strong>of</strong> aspects <strong>of</strong>the young person’s functioning that maylend themselves to recommendationsfor intervention, as well as help signala diagnosis or impairment in a particulardomain. Furthermore, some rating scalesmay blend the elements <strong>of</strong> personalitytests <strong>and</strong> diagnostic schedules, makingsound clinical judgment essentialin drawing the most sound conclusionsfrom the data collected. The primarypurpose <strong>of</strong> the assessment (e.g., diagnosticclarification vs. identifying areas <strong>of</strong>behavioral, social, or emotional concern)should guide the selection <strong>of</strong> diagnosticschedules <strong>and</strong>/or behavior rating scales.Further, we would argue that if given achoice, clinicians should initially seektools that provide a broad screening <strong>of</strong> avariety <strong>of</strong> possible problems rather thannarrowing in too quickly on a specificdiagnosis.Mash <strong>and</strong> Hunsley (2005) have articulatedthe problems with considering afocus on specific diagnoses as synonymouswith psychological assessment:“Although formal diagnostic systems…provideone alternative for framing the range<strong>of</strong> disorders <strong>and</strong> problems to be considered,there is no need to limit the range <strong>of</strong>problems to those detailed in a diagnosticsystem. Refraining from excessive relianceon formal diagnostic systems is warrantedgiven the well-documented shortcomings inthe nature <strong>and</strong> development <strong>of</strong> such systems(e.g., Beutler & Malik, 2002; Mash & Dozois,2003; Scotti, Morris, McNeil, & Hawkins,1996) <strong>and</strong> the lack <strong>of</strong> evidence that suchdiagnostic systems provide the best wayto match a treatment to a child (Bickman,2002)” (p. 368).Despite these concerns, diagnostic schedulesor checklists may still play a criticalrole in helping to address a referral question<strong>and</strong> make treatment recommendationsthat are diagnostically-relevant (e.g.,classroom accommodations for a childwho meets criteria for ADHD). Diagnosticschedules have evolved from behavioralassessment methods, as has the DSM,which now emphasizes the tally <strong>of</strong> behaviors


24 CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUES(symptoms) in order to make a diagnosticdecision. <strong>Personality</strong> tests <strong>and</strong> many ratingscales, on the other h<strong>and</strong>, are rooted in thepsychometric tradition in which such testsare designed to assess traits across a continuum.While such instruments may not leaddirectly to a diagnostic decision, as notedabove, they can play other important rolesby identifying traits that have implicationsfor the course or prognosis <strong>of</strong> a disorder, oreven for treatment.While diagnostic schedules are practicalfor making diagnostic decisions, suchmeasures have limitations for studyingthe nature <strong>of</strong> individual differences or forcontributing to other important aspects<strong>of</strong> the assessment process. These limitationsare inherent in diagnostic schedulesbecause they <strong>of</strong>ten lack a clear theoreticalbasis or evidence <strong>of</strong> a priori defined trait(s)that can be supported with construct validityevidence. Therefore, the emergence <strong>of</strong>diagnostic schedules as the instruments <strong>of</strong>choice for much <strong>of</strong> assessment practice isevidence <strong>of</strong> the pr<strong>of</strong>ound impact <strong>of</strong> behavioral-baseddiagnostic systems on psychometrictest development, particularly overthe last decade or two, as well as the (realor perceived) need to provide diagnoses asa result <strong>of</strong> all assessments due to managedhealth care.Appropriate conclusions that couldbe drawn based on diagnostic schedulesinclude statements like the following:l Tonya suffers from major depression,single episode, severe.l Tony exhibits nearly enough symptomsto be diagnosed as having conduct disorder.l Traci has attention problems that areworse than those <strong>of</strong> 99% <strong>of</strong> the childrenher age.Alternatively, conclusions that could be<strong>of</strong>fered based on psychometric tests <strong>of</strong> personalityor behavioral, emotional, or socialfunctioning could include:l Allison shows evidence <strong>of</strong> poor adaptabilityto new situations <strong>and</strong> changes inroutine, which puts her at risk for schooladjustment problems.l Patrick’s high score on the sensationseeking scale warrants consideration aspart <strong>of</strong> his vocational counseling <strong>and</strong>educational planning.l Maria’s somatization tendencies revealthe need for counseling in order toreduce her frequency <strong>of</strong> emergencyclinic visits.l Andersen’s apparent signs <strong>of</strong> depressionindicate a need for further evaluation <strong>and</strong>intervention.A central difference between these interpretivestatements is that those made basedon diagnostic schedules are dependent ondiagnostic nosologies. A variation <strong>of</strong> thispremise is the third statement exemplifyingdiagnostic-based conclusions, whichmay result from a norm-referenced behaviorrating scale that has a scale devoted toinattention. Such norm-based informationcan typically be gleaned from personalitytests or other rating scales as well.The interpretive statements made basedon psychometric tests, however, can be<strong>of</strong>fered independently <strong>of</strong> diagnosis. Theseconclusions are based on the measurement<strong>of</strong> traits or tendencies that may or may notrepresent diagnostic symptoms or signs,<strong>and</strong> yet, these conclusions contribute substantiallyto the assessment process.Widely used rating scales such as thoseto be discussed later in this volume haveseveral scales with the same name as adiagnostic category such as depression oranxiety. At the same time, such measuresare scaled similarly to traditional personalitytests with st<strong>and</strong>ard scores based onnorms.Although research is emerging onthis issue (e.g., Ferdin<strong>and</strong>, 2008; Kerr,Lunkenheimer, & Olson, 2007), generallyspeaking, we do not know the extentto which these scales demonstrate the


CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUES25stability associated with traits or the diagnosticaccuracy <strong>of</strong> the DSM system. Theirpopularity for clinical practice, however,continues to increase due to their costeffectiveness <strong>and</strong> time efficiency. Furthermore,rating scales allow for the rapid <strong>and</strong>accurate identification <strong>of</strong> domains <strong>of</strong> deviantbehavior that may require diagnosis ortreatment (Hart & Lahey, 1999).In this volume, the term personality testwill occasionally be used generically toapply to personality trait measures, diagnosticschedules, syndrome scales, <strong>and</strong>related measures, always assuming that thereader is aware <strong>of</strong> the distinctions betweensubtypes <strong>of</strong> measures.Scores, Norms,<strong>and</strong> DistributionsTypes <strong>of</strong> ScoresIn this section, some <strong>of</strong> the basic properties<strong>of</strong> score types are reviewed, with particularemphasis on the T-score st<strong>and</strong>ard scoremetric <strong>and</strong> its variants. The properties <strong>of</strong>these scores will be highlighted in order toencourage psychometrically appropriatescore interpretation.Raw ScoresThe first score that the clinician encountersafter summing item scores is usuallycalled a raw score. Raw scores, on mosttests, are simply the sum <strong>of</strong> the item scores.The term raw is probably fitting for thesescores in that they give little informationabout a child’s performance as compared tohis or her peers. Raw scores are not particularlyhelpful for norm-referenced interpretation.Raw scores merely identify thenumber <strong>of</strong> behaviors or symptoms present,not how deviant this amount <strong>of</strong> symptomatologyis from the norm nor how impairingit is for the individual.Norm-Referenced Scores<strong>Personality</strong> test interpretation <strong>of</strong>ten focuseson norm-referenced interpretation, the comparison<strong>of</strong> children’s scores to some st<strong>and</strong>ardor norm. For the purposes <strong>of</strong> assessingpsychological constructs, scores are usuallycompared to those <strong>of</strong> children the sameage. Norm-referenced achievement tests,by contrast, may compare children’s scoresto those <strong>of</strong> others in the same grade, <strong>and</strong>college admission counselors may comparean incoming student’s GPA to that <strong>of</strong> freshmenwho entered the year before.Norm referencing is <strong>of</strong> importancein personality <strong>and</strong> behavioral assessmentbecause it allows the clinician to gauge deviance,which is <strong>of</strong>ten central to the referralquestion. Parents who refer a child for apsychological evaluation <strong>of</strong>ten have normreferencingin mind. They ask questionssuch as “Is her activity level normal for herage?” or “Everyone says he is just a boy,but fire setting isn’t normal, is it?” Normreferencingallows the clinician to answersuch questions objectively. The remainingscores discussed in this section are normreferencedscores that allow the clinician tomake these important comparisons.St<strong>and</strong>ard ScoresThe st<strong>and</strong>ard score is a type <strong>of</strong> derived scorethat has traditionally been the most popularfor psychometric test interpretation. St<strong>and</strong>ardscores convert raw scores to a distributionwith a set mean <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation<strong>and</strong> with equal units along the scale (Anastasi& Urbina, 1998). The typical st<strong>and</strong>ardscore scale used for personality tests <strong>and</strong>behavior rating scales is the T-score, whichhas a mean <strong>of</strong> 50 <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation <strong>of</strong>10. Another popular st<strong>and</strong>ard score that iscoming into more frequent use for personalitytest interpretation has the mean setat 100 <strong>and</strong> the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation at 15,similar to the IQ metric (see Table 2.1).Because they have equal units along the


26 CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUESTable 2.1 St<strong>and</strong>ard Score, T-Score, Scaled Score, <strong>and</strong> Percentile Rank Conversion TableSt<strong>and</strong>ardScoreM = 100SD = 15T-ScoreM = 50SD = 10ScaledScoreM = 10SD = 3PercentileRankSt<strong>and</strong>ardScoreM = 100SD = 15T-ScoreM = 50SD = 10ScaledScoreM = 10SD = 3PercentileRank160 90 99.99 128 69 97159 89 99.99 127 68 97158 89 99.99 126 67 96157 88 99.99 125 67 15 95156 87 99.99 124 66 95155 87 99.99 123 65 94154 86 99.99 122 65 92153 85 99.98 121 64 92152 85 99.97 120 63 14 91151 84 99.96 119 63 89150 83 99.95 118 62 88149 83 99.94 117 61 87148 82 99.93 116 61 86147 81 99.91 115 60 13 84146 81 19 99.89 114 59 83145 80 99.87 113 59 81144 79 99.84 112 58 79143 79 99.80 111 57 77142 78 99.75 110 57 12 75141 77 99.70 109 56 73140 77 18 99.64 108 55 71139 76 99.57 108 55 69138 75 99 107 55 67137 75 99 106 54 65136 74 99 105 53 11 65135 73 17 99 104 53 62134 73 99 103 52 57133 72 99 102 51 55132 71 98 101 51 52131 71 98 100 50 10 50130 70 16 98 99 49 48129 69 97 98 49 45(Continues)


CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUES27Table 2.1 (Continued)St<strong>and</strong>ardScoreM = 100SD = 15T-ScoreM = 50SD = 10ScaledScoreM = 10SD = 3PercentileRankSt<strong>and</strong>ardScoreM = 100SD = 15T-ScoreM = 50SD = 10ScaledScoreM = 10SD = 3PercentileRank97 48 43 68 29 296 47 40 67 28 195 47 9 38 66 27 194 46 35 65 27 3 193 45 33 64 26 193 45 31 63 25 192 45 29 63 25 191 44 27 62 25 190 43 8 25 61 24 .4989 43 23 60 23 2 .3688 42 21 59 23 .3087 41 19 58 22 .2586 41 17 57 21 .2085 40 7 16 56 21 .1684 39 14 55 20 1 .1383 39 13 54 19 .1182 38 12 53 19 .0981 37 11 52 18 .0780 37 6 9 51 17 .0679 36 8 50 17 .0578 35 8 49 16 .0478 35 7 48 15 .0377 35 6 48 15 .0276 34 5 47 15 .0275 33 5 5 46 14 .0174 33 4 45 13 .0173 32 3 44 13 .0172 31 3 43 12 .0171 31 3 42 11 .0170 30 4 2 41 11 .0169 29 2 40 10 .01


28 CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUES0.13 % 2.14 % 13.59% 34.13% 34.13% 13.59% 2.14 % 0.13 %-4SD -3SD -2SD -1SD Mean +1SD +2SD +3SD +4SDTest Scorez scoreI-4I I I I I I I I-3 -2 -1 0 +1 +2 +3 +4T scoreI I I I I I I I I10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90PercentileRankI I I I I I I I I I I I I1 5 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 95 99Figure 2.1A normal distribution <strong>of</strong> scoresscale, st<strong>and</strong>ard scores are useful for statisticalanalyses <strong>and</strong> for making comparisonsacross tests. The equal units (or intervals)that are characteristic <strong>of</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard scoresare shown for various st<strong>and</strong>ard scores <strong>and</strong>percentile ranks in Table 2.1. This tablemay also be useful for converting a scorefrom one scale to another. In the T-scoremetric, the distance between 20 <strong>and</strong> 30 isthe same as that between 45 <strong>and</strong> 55.St<strong>and</strong>ard scores are particularly usefulfor test interpretation because they allowfor comparisons among various subscales,scales, <strong>and</strong> composites yielded by the sametest, allowing the clinician to comparetraits. In other words, st<strong>and</strong>ard scores allowthe clinician to answer questions such as“Is she more anxious than depressed?” thusfacilitating pr<strong>of</strong>ile analysis. Most modernpersonality tests use T-scores.In a normal distribution (a frequentlyuntenable assumption in personality <strong>and</strong>behavior assessment, as is shown in a latersection), a normalized st<strong>and</strong>ard score dividesup the same proportions <strong>of</strong> the normal curve.However, because many scales on syndromemeasures in particular are heavily skewed(the most frequent scenario is that mostindividuals are not experiencing psychopathology<strong>and</strong> a few are, resulting in positiveskewness), some test developers opt for theuse <strong>of</strong> linear T-scores. Linear T scores maintainthe skewed shape <strong>of</strong> the raw score distribution,which means that the same T-scoreon different scales may divide up differentportions <strong>of</strong> the norming sample. Specifically,50% <strong>of</strong> the norming sample may scorebelow a linear T-score <strong>of</strong> 50 on the Anxietyscale, whereas 55% <strong>of</strong> the norming samplemay score below a linear T-score <strong>of</strong> 50 on the


CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUES29Aggression scale. Essentially, then, the use<strong>of</strong> linear T-scores makes the relationship <strong>of</strong>percentile ranks to T-scores unique for eachscale.Percentile RanksA percentile rank gives an individual’srelative position within the norm group.Percentile ranks are very useful for communicatingwith parents, administrators,educators, <strong>and</strong> others who do not have anextensive background in scaling methods(Kamphaus, 2001). It is relatively easy forparents to underst<strong>and</strong> that a child’s percentilerank <strong>of</strong> 50 is higher than approximately50% <strong>of</strong> the norm group <strong>and</strong> lower thanapproximately 50% <strong>of</strong> the norm group.This type <strong>of</strong> interpretation works well solong as the parent underst<strong>and</strong>s the differencebetween the percentile rank <strong>and</strong> thepercent <strong>of</strong> items passed.Figure 2.1 shows that percentileranks have one major disadvantage incomparison to st<strong>and</strong>ard scores. Percentileranks have unequal units alongtheir scale. The distribution in Fig. 2.1(<strong>and</strong> in Table 2.1) shows that the differencebetween the 1st <strong>and</strong> 5th percentile ranksis larger than the difference between the40th <strong>and</strong> 50th percentile ranks. In otherwords, percentile ranks in the middle <strong>of</strong>the distribution tend to overemphasize differencesbetween st<strong>and</strong>ard scores, whereaspercentile ranks at the tails <strong>of</strong> the distributiontend to underemphasize differences inperformance (Kamphaus, 2001).Here is an example <strong>of</strong> how confusingthis property <strong>of</strong> having unequal units canbe. A clinician would typically describe aT-score <strong>of</strong> 55 as average. When placed onthe percentile rank distribution, however,a T-score <strong>of</strong> 55 corresponds to a percentilerank <strong>of</strong> 69 in a normal distribution <strong>of</strong>scores (see Table 2.1). The percentile rank<strong>of</strong> 69 sounds as though it is higher thanaverage. Examples such as this clearly showthe caveats needed when dealing with anordinal (unequal scale units) scale <strong>of</strong> measurementsuch as the percentile rank scale.It is important to remember that the ordinalproperties <strong>of</strong> the scale are due to thefact that the percentile rank merely placesa score in the distribution. In most distributions,the majority <strong>of</strong> the scores arein the middle <strong>of</strong> the distribution, causingsmall differences between st<strong>and</strong>ard scoresin the middle to produce large differencesin percentile ranks.Uniform T-scoresA uniform T-score (UT) is a special type<strong>of</strong> T score that was used for development<strong>of</strong> the MMPI-2 norms (Tellegen & Ben-Porath, 1992). This derived score is aT-score like all other normalized st<strong>and</strong>ardscores with the exception that it maintainssome (but not all) <strong>of</strong> the skewness <strong>of</strong> theoriginal raw score distributions. The UTis like a normalized T-score in that therelationship between percentile ranks <strong>and</strong>T-scores is constant across scales, <strong>and</strong> itresembles a linear T-score metric in thatsome <strong>of</strong> the skewness in the raw score distributionis retained. The problem <strong>of</strong> a lack <strong>of</strong>percentile rank comparability across scalesis described by Tellegen <strong>and</strong> Ben-Porath(1992) in reference to the MMPI-2:“For example, the raw score distribution <strong>of</strong>Scale 8, Schizophrenia (Sc), is more positively(i.e., right-) skewed than that <strong>of</strong> Scale9, Hypomania (Ma). This means that a linearT-score <strong>of</strong>, say, 80 represents different relativest<strong>and</strong>ings on these two scales in the normativesample. For women in the MMPI-2 normativesample, the percentile values <strong>of</strong> a linear T-score<strong>of</strong> 80 are 98.6 for Scale 8 <strong>and</strong> 99.8 for Scale 9;for men, the corresponding values are similar,98.6 <strong>and</strong> 99.7” (p. 145).In order for the UT scale score to have theproperties <strong>of</strong> percentile rank comparabilityacross scales <strong>and</strong> reflection <strong>of</strong> raw score distributionskewness, the UT-score is based


30 CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUESon the average skewness value across all <strong>of</strong>the clinical scales (Tellegen & Ben-Porath,1992). This approach meets the objectivesoutlined by the developers, but it relies onthe assumption that the skewness <strong>of</strong> theMMPI-2 clinical scales is similar. Thereis, however, evidence that some MMPI-2clinical scales (e.g., Hypochondriasis <strong>and</strong>Schizophrenia) are far more skewed thanothers (see Tellegen & Ben-Porath). Itappears that the UT is a compromise metricthat meets test development objectiveswhile, at the same time, not addressingcompletely the issue <strong>of</strong> different skewnessacross scales. More research <strong>and</strong> clinicalexperience with the UT metric is necessaryto determine whether or not this methodshould be adopted by other test developers.Norm DevelopmentSamplingNorm development is one area in which thetechnology <strong>of</strong> personality <strong>and</strong> behavioralassessment has generally lagged behindthat <strong>of</strong> intelligence or achievement testing(Martin, 1988). Intelligence tests, forexample, have routinely collected stratifiednational samples <strong>of</strong> children to use asa normative base. Stratification is used tocollect these samples in order to match, tothe extent possible, the characteristics <strong>of</strong> thepopulation at large. Common stratificationvariables include age, gender, race, geographicregion, community size, <strong>and</strong> parentalsocio-economic status (SES; Kamphaus,2001). These variables are used presumablybecause they are related to score differences.Of these widely used stratification variables,SES is known to produce the most substantialscore differences on intelligence measures(Kamphaus). The precedent, then,is set for the norming <strong>of</strong> personality <strong>and</strong>behavioral assessment tools.This precedent, however, has not beenfollowed in several important respects. Untilrecently, many relatively popular personalityscales have not done a good job <strong>of</strong> stratifyingtheir samples. Some norming samples do notcontrol for geographic region, <strong>and</strong> others failto control for SES. The result is a normativest<strong>and</strong>ard <strong>of</strong> unknown utility. Whilepoor norming is less likely to be toleratedin intelligence <strong>and</strong> academic achievementassessment, it is less frequently criticizedor even noted in discussions <strong>of</strong> personalityassessment. We will, however, note thecharacteristics <strong>of</strong> norming samples in subsequentsections <strong>of</strong> this text. This is importantbecause users <strong>of</strong> personality <strong>and</strong> behaviortests should know the characteristics <strong>of</strong> atest’s norming sample in order to make thebest decisions <strong>and</strong> gauge the amount <strong>of</strong> confidenceto place in the obtained scores.Intelligence, achievement, <strong>and</strong> adaptivebehavior tests typically feature interpretationbased on a national norm sample. Incontrast, a national normative st<strong>and</strong>ardhas <strong>of</strong>ten not been <strong>of</strong>fered for personalitytests. A substantial number <strong>of</strong> personalitytests <strong>of</strong>fer only local norms, a subset <strong>of</strong> thenational normative sample. Local normsanswer different questions than do nationalnorms. Hence, their potential utility hasto be evaluated prior to test selection <strong>and</strong>interpretation.Local NormsLocal norms, or norms based on a specificpopulation in a specific setting or location,may sometimes be more useful thannational norms, particularly in terms <strong>of</strong>their relevance for the clinician’s work, <strong>and</strong>in some cases, recency relative to nationalnorms (Elliott & Bretzing, 1980; Petersen,Kolen, & Hoover, 1989). In order for localnorms to be meaningful, however, therange <strong>of</strong> their usefulness must be definedclearly.Regardless <strong>of</strong> the use <strong>of</strong> local or nationalnorms, typical norm-referenced questions<strong>of</strong> interest to psychologists are diagnosticones. Common questions might include:


CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUES31l Does Lindsey have attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder?l Is Jose clinically depressed?l Is Stephanie more anxious than otherchildren her age?One <strong>of</strong> the goals <strong>of</strong> diagnostic practiceis consistency, which is fostered by thepublication <strong>of</strong> diagnostic criteria. Consistentmethods <strong>of</strong> diagnosis allow cliniciansto communicate clearly with one another.If, for example, Dr. Ob Session in Seattlesays that a patient is suffering from conductdisorder, then Dr. Sid Ego in Atlantawill know what to expect from this adolescentwhen he enters his <strong>of</strong>fice for followuptreatment.National norms similarly promote consistency.If a clinician concludes that a childhas clinically significant attention problemsbased on a deviant score on an inattentionscale, then others may reasonably concludethat this child has attention difficultiesthat are unusual for her age. Popular tests,however, may <strong>of</strong>fer different local normsthat can hamper consistent communication.Similarly, local norms also will be lessgeneralizable to the general population <strong>of</strong>children who do or do not meet criteria fora particular diagnosis. The clinician mustthen balance these disadvantages <strong>of</strong> localnorms with the potential for local normsto be more relevant to the population withwhich he/she works.Gender-Based Norms<strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> behavior measures areunusual in that gender-referenced (local)norms are sometimes <strong>of</strong>fered by test developers.This practice is unusual in comparisonto other domains <strong>of</strong> assessment where,although significant gender differences exist,national combined gender norms are typicallythe only ones provided. Specifically,intelligence, academic achievement, <strong>and</strong>adaptive behavior scales produce mean scoredifferences between gender groups, but localnorms by gender are rarely <strong>of</strong>fered. Whythen are gender local norms commonly<strong>of</strong>fered for personality tests? Tradition couldbe the most parsimonious explanation.When comparing a child to his orher gender group, the effects <strong>of</strong> genderdifferences in behavior are removed.Another way <strong>of</strong> expressing this is to say that,when gender norms are utilized, roughlythe same proportion <strong>of</strong> boys as girls isidentified as having problems. Because, forexample, boys tend to have more symptoms<strong>of</strong> hyperactivity than girls (DSM-IV, APA,1994), the use <strong>of</strong> gender local norms woulderase this difference in epidemiology.Gender norm-referencing would identifyapproximately the same percentage <strong>of</strong> girls<strong>and</strong> boys as hyperactive, such that a boywould require more severe symptomatologyto be identified as elevated on hyperactivityrelative to other boys. Depression isanother example <strong>of</strong> how gender normsmay affect diagnostic rates. Much evidencesuggests that girls express more depressivesymptomatology than boys in adolescence(Weiss & Weisz, 1988). The use <strong>of</strong> gendernorms for a depression scale would resultin the same number <strong>of</strong> adolescent boysas girls exceeding a particular cut score,whereas general national norms wouldretain the known greater prevalence amongadolescent girls.Are gender local norms a problem? Notso long as clinicians are clear about thequestions they are asking. A gender normquestion would be “Is Traci hyperactivewhen compared to other girls her age?”whereas a national norm question wouldbe “Is Traci hyperactive in comparison toother children her age?” General nationalnorms are preferred when a diagnosticquestion is asked. An example <strong>of</strong> a diagnosticquestion is, “Does Frank have enoughsymptoms <strong>of</strong> depression to warrant a diagnosis?”The DSM-IV diagnostic criteria donot have differing thresholds for boys, so agender norm would be inappropriate.


32 CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUESAge-Based NormsBecause there are substantial differencesacross age groups, intelligence <strong>and</strong> academicachievement tests routinely <strong>of</strong>fernorms separately by age groups, typicallyusing age ranges <strong>of</strong> 1 year or less. By contrast,age ranges as large as 5–7 years arefrequently used for personality tests. Thistradition <strong>of</strong> articulating norms for largerage groups may be attributable to personalitytraits <strong>of</strong>ten having smaller normativesamples than intelligence <strong>and</strong> achievementtests <strong>and</strong> a lack <strong>of</strong> age group differencesin personality <strong>and</strong> behavior characteristics(Martin, 1988). Some data suggestthat the latter explanation may be moreappropriate. That is, differences betweenadjacent age groups are <strong>of</strong>ten insignificantfor behavior rating scales, whereas moremeaningful differences only occur overlonger developmental periods (e.g., Reynolds& Kamphaus, 2004).<strong>Clinical</strong> NormsA more unique norm group is a sample <strong>of</strong>children who have been previously diagnosedas having a mental health problem.This clinical norm-referenced comparisoncan answer questions such as:l How aggressive is Sheila in comparisonto other children who are receivingpsychological services?l Are Tonya’s psychotic symptoms unusualin comparison to other children who arereferred for psychological evaluation?There is not a clear precedent for the development<strong>of</strong> clinical norms. The relevantdemographic stratification variables havenot been identified, making it difficult tojudge the quality <strong>of</strong> clinical norms. Shouldclinical norms, for example, attempt tomimic the epidemiology <strong>of</strong> childhood disordersincluding 10% depression cases, 5%ADHD cases, <strong>and</strong> so on? Should normsattempt to match the epidemiology <strong>of</strong> specificdisorders within child clinic-referredpopulations (i.e., include mostly externalizingdisorders)? Should norms be <strong>of</strong>feredseparately by diagnostic category to <strong>of</strong>fera more exact comparison? Or shouldattempts be made to address each <strong>of</strong> theseissues?Until such st<strong>and</strong>ards emerge, cliniciansshould seek clinical norms that are at leastwell-described. A clear description <strong>of</strong> thesample will allow the clinician to determineif the clinical norm group has the potentialto answer questions <strong>of</strong> interest. For example,the clinician who works in an inpatientsetting may have more interest in aclinical sample <strong>of</strong> inpatients, whereas othersmay prefer that clinical norms be based ona referral population. If the clinical normgroup for a test is not well-described, theclinician cannot meaningfully interpretthe norm-referenced comparisons.The normal curve refers to the graphicdepiction <strong>of</strong> a distribution <strong>of</strong> test scoresthat is symmetrical (normal), resemblinga bell. In a normal distribution, there area few people with very low scores (thesepeople are represented by the tail <strong>of</strong> thecurve on the left in Fig. 2.1), a few withvery high scores (the tail on the right), <strong>and</strong>many individuals with scores near the average(the highest point in the curve).When a distribution is normal or bellshaped,as is the case in Fig. 2.1, the st<strong>and</strong>arddeviation always divides up the sameproportion. Specifically, ±1 st<strong>and</strong>ard deviationalways includes approximately 68% <strong>of</strong>the cases in a normal distribution, <strong>and</strong> ±2st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations always include approximately95% <strong>of</strong> the cases. The normal curveis also sometimes referred to as the normalprobability, or Gaussian curve.Normal distributions, however, cannotbe assumed for personality tests orbehavior ratings. While intelligence <strong>and</strong>academic tests <strong>of</strong>ten produce near-normaldistributions, personality tests <strong>of</strong>tenproduce skewed distributions. Examples


CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUES33<strong>of</strong> skewed distributions are shown in Figs.2.2 <strong>and</strong> 2.3. The distribution depicted inFig. 2.2 is negatively skewed; a positiveskew is shown in Fig. 2.3. A mnemonicfor remembering the distinction betweenpositive <strong>and</strong> negative skewness is to notethat the valence <strong>of</strong> the skewness appliesto the tail, when positive is on the right<strong>and</strong> negative is on the left.It is underst<strong>and</strong>able that diagnosticschedules <strong>and</strong> syndrome scales such asbehavior rating scales produce skewed distributions.After all, only a small proportion<strong>of</strong> the population is experiencing aparticular disorder at some point in time,<strong>and</strong> the majority <strong>of</strong> individuals are free <strong>of</strong>such symptomatology (positive skew). Onthe other h<strong>and</strong>, it is quite likely that thedistributions for many adaptive skills orFigure 2.2A hypothetical example <strong>of</strong> a negativelyskewed distribution <strong>of</strong> scoresFigure 2.3A hypothetical example <strong>of</strong> a positivelyskewed distribution <strong>of</strong> scoresbehaviors would be negatively skewed, inthat the majority <strong>of</strong> the population wouldpossess high levels <strong>of</strong> such skills, particularlywith age (c.f., Sparrow, Cichetti, &Balla, 2005).The <strong>of</strong>ten skewed distributions obtainedfor personality measures, particularly diagnosticschedules, produce more controversyregarding scaling methods. If, forexample, a distribution is heavily skewed,should normalized st<strong>and</strong>ard scores (whichforce normality on the shape <strong>of</strong> the st<strong>and</strong>ardscore distribution regardless <strong>of</strong> theshape <strong>of</strong> the raw score distribution) orlinear transformations (which maintainthe shape <strong>of</strong> the raw score distribution) beused? Petersen et al. (1989) maintain that“usually there is no good theoretical reasonfor normalizing scores” (p. 226), <strong>and</strong>we concur with this opinion.What differences does the scaling methodmake (i.e., normalized versus lineartransformations)? The primary differenceis in the relationship between the st<strong>and</strong>ardscores (T-scores) <strong>and</strong> percentile ranksyielded by a test. The positively skeweddistribution shown in Fig. 2.3 is a goodexample <strong>of</strong> how this relationship can beaffected. If this distribution was normalized(i.e., forced normal by converting rawscores to normal deviates <strong>and</strong> then the normaldeviates to T-scores), then a T-score<strong>of</strong> 70 will always be at the 98th percentile.If linear transformations were used for thescale distribution shown in Fig. 2.3, thenthe corresponding percentile rank wouldmost certainly be something other than 98.Clearly the type <strong>of</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard score used forscaling a test affects diagnostic <strong>and</strong>, perhaps,treatment decisions. If normalizedst<strong>and</strong>ard scores were used for a positivelyskewed scale (e.g., one measuring conductproblems), then potentially more childrenwould be identified as having significantproblems. On the other h<strong>and</strong>, normalizedst<strong>and</strong>ard scores make the clinician’s jobeasier by fostering interpretation acrossscales. Herein lies the debate: Is the inter-


34 CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUESpretive convenience <strong>of</strong> normalized st<strong>and</strong>ardscores worth the trade-<strong>of</strong>f in lack <strong>of</strong>precision?Clinicians will find that many testsuse normalized st<strong>and</strong>ard scores (usuallyexpressed in a T-score metric) even whenclear evidence <strong>of</strong> significant skewnessexists. We suggest that readers note thescaling method used by tests discussed inthis volume as they consider the strengths<strong>and</strong> weaknesses <strong>of</strong> each measure.ReliabilityThe reliability <strong>of</strong> a test refers to the degreeto which test scores are free from measurementerror <strong>and</strong> includes the presumedstability, consistency, <strong>and</strong> repeatability <strong>of</strong>scores for a given individual (AERA, APA,NCME, 1999).The reliability <strong>of</strong> a personality test isexpressed by the computation <strong>of</strong> a reliabilitycoefficient, which is a special type<strong>of</strong> correlation coefficient. One essentialdifference between a reliability coefficient<strong>and</strong> a correlation coefficient is that reliabilitycoefficients are typically not negative,while negative correlation coefficientsare eminently possible. Reliability coefficientsrange, then, from 0 to +1. Reliabilitycoefficients represent the amount<strong>of</strong> reliable variance associated with a test.In other words, a reliability coefficient isnot squared, as is the case with correlationcoefficients, to calculate the amount <strong>of</strong> reliablevariance (Anastasi & Urbina, 1998).For example, the reliable variance <strong>of</strong> a testwith a reliability coefficient <strong>of</strong> .90 is 90%,an unusually easy computation!The error variance associated with atest is also easy to calculate. It is doneby subtracting the reliability coefficientfrom 1 (perfect reliability). Taking theprevious example, the error variance fora test with a reliability coefficient <strong>of</strong> .90is 10% (1 - .90).The sources <strong>of</strong> measurement error,while potentially crucial for interpretation,are <strong>of</strong>ten not specified, leaving the psychologistto engage in speculation. Errormay result from changes in the patient’sattitude toward assessment or cooperation,malingering, rater biases, patients’ healthstatus, subjective scoring algorithms, oritem content that is incomprehensible tothe examinee, among other factors.For this reason, it is important to considerstatistics that document both the reliable<strong>and</strong> error variance <strong>of</strong> a scale or test.In addition, multiple reliability coefficients<strong>and</strong> error estimates based on classical <strong>and</strong>modern test theory methods are necessaryto guide clinical <strong>and</strong> research practice.Logically, then, it follows that no singleestimate <strong>of</strong> reliability or error discussed inthis section is adequate to support routineuse <strong>of</strong> a test <strong>of</strong> assessment procedure.Test–Retest MethodA popular method for computing the stability<strong>of</strong> personality test scores is the testretestmethod. In this method the sametest, for example the MMPI-A, is administeredto the same group <strong>of</strong> individualsunder the same or similar conditionsover a brief period <strong>of</strong> time (typically 2–4weeks). The correlation between the first<strong>and</strong> second administrations <strong>of</strong> the test isthen computed, yielding a test-retest reliabilitycoefficient that is optimally veryclose to 1.0. Of course, the importance <strong>of</strong>such reliability depends on the constructbeing assessed. If clinicians seek to assesschanges in specific, discrete behaviors asa result <strong>of</strong> an intervention, for example,then test-retest reliability becomes less<strong>of</strong> a concern. On the other h<strong>and</strong>, if aclinician seeks to evaluate what are presumablyrelatively stable indicators <strong>of</strong>behavioral functioning or personality,the test-retest reliability <strong>of</strong> the measurebecomes paramount.


CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUES35Internal ConsistencyCoefficientsAnother type <strong>of</strong> reliability coefficient typicallyreported in test manuals is an internal consistencycoefficient. This estimate differsfrom test-retest or stability coefficients inthat it does not directly assess the stability<strong>of</strong> the measure <strong>of</strong> personality over time.Internal consistency coefficients assesswhat the name implies-the average correlationamong the items in a test or scale.In other words, this index <strong>of</strong> reliabilityassesses the homogeneity <strong>of</strong> the test itempool. Internal consistency coefficients areinexpensively produced, since they onlyrequire one administration <strong>of</strong> the test.Typical formula used for the computation<strong>of</strong> internal consistency coefficients includesplit-half coefficients, Kuder Richardson20, <strong>and</strong> Coefficient (or Cronbach’s) Alpha.On occasion, there are differencesbetween internal consistency <strong>and</strong> test-retestcoefficients that can affect test interpretation.A test may, for example, have a relativelypoor internal consistency coefficient<strong>and</strong> yet a strong test-retest coefficient (Kamphaus,2001). Because internal consistencycoefficients are imperfect estimates <strong>of</strong> stabilitycoefficients, both types <strong>of</strong> coefficientsshould be recorded in the manual for a test(AERA, APA, NCME, 1999). It is then upto the pr<strong>of</strong>essional making use <strong>of</strong> the test todetermine if the reliability is suitable for thepurpose for which the tool is to be used.Variables that Affect ReliabilityClinicians who use personality or behaviortests should recognize factors that canaffect reliability. Some factors that the clinicianshould keep in mind when estimatingthe reliability <strong>of</strong> a test for a particularchild include the following:1. Reliability can differ for different scorelevels. A test that is very reliable foremotionally disturbed students is notnecessarily as reliable for nondisabledstudents without research evidence tosupport its use (AERA, APA, NCME,1999).2. Reliability can suffer when there isa long interval between assessments(Nitko, 1983).3. Reliability can be affected by rater orchild characteristics such as age, readinglevel, <strong>and</strong> fatigue. Reliability <strong>of</strong> personalitymeasurement, for example, maydrop if the child does not underst<strong>and</strong>the test items.4. Analogously, error may be introduced ifa poor translation <strong>of</strong> a test is used.Reliable Specific VarianceSubtest specificity is the amount <strong>of</strong> reliablespecific variance that can be attributed toa single subtest or scale. Kaufman (1979)popularized the use <strong>of</strong> subtest specificityin clinical assessment as a way <strong>of</strong> gaugingthe amount <strong>of</strong> confidence a clinicianshould have in conclusions that are basedon a single subtest. In effect, knowledge <strong>of</strong>subtest specificity makes clinicians morecautious about drawing conclusions basedon a single scale.A reliability coefficient represents theamount <strong>of</strong> reliable variance associated with ascale. An example would be an anxiety scaletaken from a larger battery <strong>of</strong> 13 tests, all <strong>of</strong>which are part <strong>of</strong> a major personality test battery.The anxiety scale has a test-retest reliabilitycoefficient <strong>of</strong> .82. On the surface, thistest appears reliable. If this scale producesthe child’s highest score, the examiner maywish to say that the child has a problem withanxiety. The examiner can then make thisstatement with confidence because the test isrelatively reliable, right? Not necessarily. AsKaufman (1979) points out, the conclusionbeing drawn by the clinician is about someskill, trait, or ability (in this case, anxiety) that


36 CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUESis specific or measured only by this one scale.The reliability coefficient, on the other h<strong>and</strong>,reflects not just reliable specific variance butalso reliable shared variances. Subtest specificityis typically computed in the followingway (Kamphaus, 2001):1. Compute the multiple correlation (R)between the scale in question <strong>and</strong> allother scales in the battery, <strong>and</strong> square it(R 2 ). This computation yields the amount<strong>of</strong> reliable shared variance between thescale in question, in this case anxiety, <strong>and</strong>the other scales in the battery.2. Subtract the squared multiple correlationcoefficient from the reliabilitycoefficient, or r tt. If R 2 = .30, .82 - .30 =.52. This formula yields the reliable specificvariance.3. Compare the amount <strong>of</strong> reliable specificvariance (.52) to the amount <strong>of</strong> errorvariance (1 – .82 = .18). If the reliablespecific variance exceeds the error varianceby .20 or more, then the scale isconsidered to have adequate specificityfor interpretive purposes. By convention,if the reliable specific varianceexceeds the error variance by .19 orless, then the test lacks specificity, <strong>and</strong>it should be cautiously interpreted. Ifthe reliable specific variance does notexceed the error variance, then interpretation<strong>of</strong> the scale is ill-advised.St<strong>and</strong>ard Error <strong>of</strong> MeasurementThe st<strong>and</strong>ard error <strong>of</strong> measurement (SEM)gives an indication <strong>of</strong> the amount <strong>of</strong> errorassociated with test scores. In more technicalterms, the SEM is the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation<strong>of</strong> the error distribution <strong>of</strong> scores. Thereliability coefficient <strong>of</strong> a test is one way <strong>of</strong>expressing the amount <strong>of</strong> error associatedwith a test score in order to allow the userto gauge the level <strong>of</strong> confidence that shouldbe placed in the obtained scores. An examinermay report a personality test score for achild as being 63 with a test-retest reliabilitycoefficient <strong>of</strong> .95. This practice, however,is unorthodox <strong>and</strong> clumsy. The typicalpractice is to report a test score along withthe test’s st<strong>and</strong>ard error <strong>of</strong> measurement,as is frequently done for opinion polls conductedby the popular media (e.g., the errorrate or margin <strong>of</strong> error <strong>of</strong> this poll is…).The st<strong>and</strong>ard error <strong>of</strong> measurement is simplyanother way <strong>of</strong> reflecting the amount <strong>of</strong>error associated with a test score.In classical test theory, if a child wereadministered a personality test 100 timesunder identical conditions, he or shewould not obtain the same score on all 100administrations. Rather, the child wouldobtain a distribution <strong>of</strong> scores that approximatesa normal curve. This error distributionwould have a mean. The mean <strong>of</strong>this theoretical distribution <strong>of</strong> scores is thechild’s true score. A true score is a theoreticalconstruct that can only be estimated. This errordistribution, like other distributions, notonly has a mean, but it can also be dividedinto st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations. In an error distribution,however, instead <strong>of</strong> being calleda st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation, it is called the SEM.As one would predict, then, in this errordistribution <strong>of</strong> scores ±1 SEM dividesup the same portion <strong>of</strong> the normal curve(68%) as does a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation, <strong>and</strong> ±2SEMs divide up the same proportion <strong>of</strong>the error distribution (95%) as ±2 st<strong>and</strong>arddeviations do for a normal distribution <strong>of</strong>obtained scores.Confidence B<strong>and</strong>sA confidence b<strong>and</strong> is a probability statementabout the likelihood that a particularrange <strong>of</strong> scores includes a child’s truescore. As is done with opinion polls, cliniciansuse the SEM to show the amount<strong>of</strong> error, or unreliability, associated withobtained scores. Obtained scores arethen b<strong>and</strong>ed with error. “B<strong>and</strong>ing” isfrequently accomplished by subtracting1 SEM from, <strong>and</strong> adding 1 SEM to, the


CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUES37obtained score. If, for example, the childobtained a T-score <strong>of</strong> 73 on the Reynolds<strong>Child</strong> Depression Scale (RCDS; Reynolds,1989), one could apply the theory <strong>of</strong>st<strong>and</strong>ard error <strong>of</strong> measurement to b<strong>and</strong>this score with error. For the total RCDSsample, the st<strong>and</strong>ard error <strong>of</strong> measurementrounds to 4 T-score points. Giventhat ±1 SEM includes approximately68% <strong>of</strong> the error distribution <strong>of</strong> scores,the clinician could then say that there is a68% likelihood that the child’s true scorelies somewhere in the range <strong>of</strong> 69–77. Anexaminer who wanted to use a more conservative±2 SEMs could say that thereis a 95% probability that the child’s truescore lies somewhere between 65 <strong>and</strong> 81.Confidence b<strong>and</strong>s can be obtained for avariety <strong>of</strong> levels if one knows the SEM<strong>of</strong> the scale. Some manuals include confidenceb<strong>and</strong>s at the 68%, 85%, 90%,95%, <strong>and</strong> 99% levels.Construct ValidityValidity is defined as “the degree to whichaccumulated evidence <strong>and</strong> theory supportspecific interpretations <strong>of</strong> test scoresentailed by proposed uses <strong>of</strong> a test” (AERA,APA, NCME, 1999, p. 184). There are anumber <strong>of</strong> different ways <strong>of</strong> evaluating thevalidity <strong>of</strong> a test. Some <strong>of</strong> the more commontypes <strong>of</strong> validity evidence will be discussedin this section. Validity is the most importantpsychometric characteristic <strong>of</strong> a test.A test can be extremely well normed <strong>and</strong>extremely reliable <strong>and</strong> yet have no validityfor the assessment <strong>of</strong> personality. Onecould, for example, develop a very goodtest <strong>of</strong> fine motor skill, but if one triedto make interpretations about someone’spersonality from this test, such interpretationswould not be valid. That is, validity isessentially an issue pertaining to the uses <strong>of</strong>a test <strong>and</strong> the interpretations that one seeksto make from test results.Virtually every aspect <strong>of</strong> a test eithercontributes to or detracts from its abilityto measure the construct <strong>of</strong> personality orbehavior, or, in other words, its constructvalidity. Construct validity is the degree towhich a test measures some hypotheticalconstruct. As such, the construct validity<strong>of</strong> a personality test cannot be establishedbased on a single research investigation orthe study <strong>of</strong> only one type <strong>of</strong> validity (e.g.,factor analysis). Construct validity is basedon the long-term accumulation <strong>of</strong> researchevidence about a particular instrument,using a variety <strong>of</strong> procedures for the assessment<strong>of</strong> validity.Based on the information provided inthe previous paragraphs, it is clear that astatement that a test is valid or invalid isinappropriate. Instead, certain interpretationscan have more or less evidence tosupport their validity <strong>and</strong> the accumulation<strong>of</strong> evidence in support <strong>of</strong> these interpretationsis always ongoing (AERA, APA,NCME, 1999).Content ValidityOne <strong>of</strong> the reasons that many people woulddisagree with using a test <strong>of</strong> vocabularyknowledge as a measure <strong>of</strong> personality isthat it does not appear to possess valid content.Content validity refers to the appropriatesampling <strong>of</strong> a particular contentdomain. Content validity has been mostclosely associated with the development <strong>of</strong>tests <strong>of</strong> academic achievement (Anastasi &Urbina, 1998). Typically, procedures forthe establishment <strong>of</strong> content validity arejudgmental (Petersen, Kolen, & Hoover,1989).<strong>Personality</strong> test developers have <strong>of</strong>tenrelied on empirical test developmentmethods, in which items are assigned toscales based on statistical properties only(such as factor loadings, to be discussedlater), <strong>and</strong> many manuals do not providea clear indication <strong>of</strong> the source <strong>of</strong> items. In


38 CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUESsome cases, the item source is clear, such aswith the <strong>Child</strong>ren’s Depression Inventory(CDI; Kovacs, 1991), where items werebased on accepted diagnostic nosologiessuch as the DSM. Even in such cases, however,personality test developers usually donot go to the lengths <strong>of</strong> other test developersto document adequate sampling <strong>of</strong>the content (or psychopathology) domain.Few personality tests or behavioral ratingscales, for example, use panels <strong>of</strong> experts todevelop item content.Problems with regard to content validitymay be identified as cases <strong>of</strong> constructunderrepresentation or construct irrelevance.A depression scale may suffer constructunderrepresentation, for example,if it lacks both cognitive (e.g., excessiveself-deprecation) <strong>and</strong> vegetative symptoms<strong>of</strong> depression (e.g., problems sleeping).In this scenario, it may be said that thereare not enough items on the scale that areknown to be “indicators,” or symptoms <strong>of</strong>depression, resulting in questionable contentvalidity.The reader will note in later chaptersthat construct irrelevant items are a moreserious problem in behavior assessment.This problem is likely to occur when onlyempirical methods are used to constructscales <strong>and</strong> select items for scales (i.e., factoranalysis). Examples <strong>of</strong> construct irrelevanceare listed in Chap. 17 as they relateto the assessment <strong>of</strong> ADHD. In comparisonto some others, ADHD is a well-studiedcondition with a widely agreed-uponset <strong>of</strong> symptoms (e.g., motor hyperactivity<strong>and</strong> inattention). What if, however, anitem such as “My child is adopted” wasplaced on an inattention scale <strong>of</strong> a parentrating scale? As is noted in Chap. 17,such an item would likely be identified as asource <strong>of</strong> construct irrelevant variance forthis scale, a source that would lead to a lessvalid assessment <strong>of</strong> attention problems forthe child undergoing evaluation.In our view, construct irrelevance <strong>and</strong>construct underrepresentation are likelyto become problems at the item selectionstage <strong>of</strong> test development. We, therefore,caution test users to carefully review theprocess <strong>of</strong> item selection <strong>and</strong> scale constructionfor each test that they utilize. Bydoing so, we think that clinicians will bebetter able to judge the implications <strong>of</strong> testcontent for interpretation.Criterion-Related ValidityCriterion-related validity assesses thedegree to which tests relate to other tests ina theoretically appropriate manner. Thereare two kinds <strong>of</strong> criterion-related validity:concurrent <strong>and</strong> predictive.Concurrent ValidityThis type <strong>of</strong> validity stipulates that a testshould show substantial correlations withother measures to which it is theoreticallyrelated. One <strong>of</strong> the important criteria forthe evaluation <strong>of</strong> personality or behaviormeasures since their inception has beenthat they show a substantial correlationwith other indicators <strong>of</strong> psychopathology,such as well-validated tests or clinicians’ratings or diagnoses. The typical concurrentvalidity investigation involves administeringa new behavior rating scale <strong>and</strong> an existingwell-validated measure <strong>of</strong> psychopathologyto a group <strong>of</strong> children. If a correlation <strong>of</strong> .20is obtained, then the concurrent validity <strong>of</strong>the new test would be in question. A .75 correlation,on the other h<strong>and</strong>, would be supportive<strong>of</strong> the validity <strong>of</strong> the new test.Predictive ValidityPredictive validity refers to the ability <strong>of</strong> a testto predict (as shown by its correlation) somelater criterion. This type <strong>of</strong> research investigationis conducted very similarly to a concurrentvalidity study, with one important exception.The critical difference is that in a predictivevalidity study the new personality test is first


CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUES39administered to a group <strong>of</strong> children, <strong>and</strong> thensometime in the future- perhaps two months,three months, or even six years-a criterionmeasure (such as clinicians’ ratings <strong>of</strong> adjustment)is administered to the same group <strong>of</strong>children (see Verhulst et al., 1994).Correlations with Other TestsOne can use correlations with other teststo evaluate the validity <strong>of</strong> a behavior orpersonality test. In a sense, this method isa special type <strong>of</strong> concurrent validity study.The difference is that the correlation is notbetween a personality measure <strong>and</strong> somecriterion variable, such as clinicians’ ratings<strong>of</strong> adjustment, but between a personalitytest <strong>and</strong> a measure <strong>of</strong> the same construct,another personality measure. For example,if a new test <strong>of</strong> anxiety is published, itshould show a substantial relationship withprevious measures, but not an extremelyhigh relationship (Anastasi & Urbina,1998). If a new personality test correlates.99 with a previous personality test, then itis not needed, as it is simply another form<strong>of</strong> an existing test <strong>and</strong> does not contributeto increasing our underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> the construct<strong>of</strong> personality. If a new anxiety scalecorrelates only .15 with existing well-validatedanxiety scales, it is also likely not tobe a good measure <strong>of</strong> personality. New personalitytests should show a moderate tostrong relationship with existing tests, yetcontribute something new to our underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>of</strong> the construct <strong>of</strong> interest.Convergent/Discriminant ValidityConvergent validity is established when ascale correlates with constructs with which itis hypothesized to have a strong relationship.Discriminant validity is supported when apersonality measure has a poor correlationwith a construct with which it is hypothesizedto be unrelated. These types <strong>of</strong> validitymay be important to consider if there are noexisting, well-normed, or relatively recentmeasures <strong>of</strong> a construct. That is, one may notbe able to judge the criterion-related validity<strong>of</strong> a measure because no other suitable measures<strong>of</strong> that particular construct exist. Ofcourse, convergent <strong>and</strong> discriminant validityare important indicators <strong>of</strong> validity for existing/establishedmeasures as well.If one were assessing the convergent<strong>and</strong> discriminant validity <strong>of</strong> a measure <strong>of</strong>anxiety, one would expect high correlationswith other measures <strong>of</strong> anxiety <strong>and</strong>moderate correlations with other measures<strong>of</strong> depression, given the well documentedassociation between anxiety <strong>and</strong> depression(Klein et al., 2005). However, one wouldexpect only minimal correlations betweenanxiety <strong>and</strong> measures <strong>of</strong> learning problems,thus providing support for its divergentvalidity.Factor AnalysisFactor analysis is a popular technique forvalidating modern tests <strong>of</strong> personality thattraces its roots to the work <strong>of</strong> the eminentstatistician Karl Pearson (1901). Factoranalysis has become increasingly popularas a technique for test validation. A wealth<strong>of</strong> factor-analytic studies dates to the 1960swhen computers became available. Factoranalysis is difficult to explain in only a fewparagraphs. Those readers who are interestedin learning factor analysis need a separatecourse on this technique <strong>and</strong> a greatdeal <strong>of</strong> independent reading <strong>and</strong> experience.A thorough discussion <strong>of</strong> factor-analytictechniques can be found in Gorsuch(1988). An introductory-level discussioncan be found in Anastasi & Urbina (1998)<strong>and</strong> Kamphaus (2001).Factor analysis is a data reduction techniquethat attempts to explain variancein the most efficient way. Most scales oritems included in a test correlate with oneanother. It is theorized that this correlationis the result <strong>of</strong> one or more common factors.The purpose <strong>of</strong> factor analysis is toreduce the correlations between all scales


40 CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUES(or items) in a test to a smaller set <strong>of</strong> commonfactors. This smaller set <strong>of</strong> commonfactors will presumably be more interpretablethan all <strong>of</strong> the scales in a personality testbattery considered as individual entities.Factor analysis begins with the computation<strong>of</strong> an intercorrelation matrix showingthe correlations among all <strong>of</strong> the itemsor scales in a test battery. Most studies<strong>of</strong> behavior or personality tests use itemintercorrelations as input. These intercorrelationsthen serve as the input to a factoranalyticprogram that is part <strong>of</strong> a popularstatistical analysis package.The output from a factor analysis thatis frequently reported first in test validationresearch is a factor matrix showingthe factor loading <strong>of</strong> each subtest on eachfactor. A factor loading is, in most cases <strong>of</strong>exploratory factor analysis, the correlationbetween a scale <strong>and</strong> a larger factor. 1Factor loadings range from -1 to +1 just ascorrelation coefficients do. Selected factorloadings for the MMPI-A factor analysis<strong>of</strong> the st<strong>and</strong>ardization sample (Butcheret al., 1992) are shown in Table 2.2. A highpositive correlation between a scale <strong>and</strong> afactor means the same thing as a high positivecorrelation between two scales in thatthey tend to covary to a great extent. Onecan see from Table 2.2 that the Hysteriascale is highly correlated with Factor 1, forexample, <strong>and</strong> that Mania is not highly correlatedwith Factor 1, but it is highly correlatedwith Factor 2.Once the factor matrix, as shown inTable 2.2, is obtained, the researcher mustlabel the obtained factors. This labelingis not based on statistical procedures, buton the theoretical knowledge <strong>and</strong> perspective<strong>of</strong> the individual researcher. For the1When orthogonal (independent or uncorrelated)rotation techniques are used (<strong>and</strong> these techniquesare very frequently used in test validation research),the factor loading represents the correlation betweenthe subtest <strong>and</strong> a factor. This is not the case whenoblique or correlated methods <strong>of</strong> factor analysis areused (Anastasi & Urbina, 1998).MMPI-A, there is general agreement as tothe names <strong>of</strong> the factors. The first factor istypically referred to as general maladjustment<strong>and</strong> the second as overcontrol. Thethird <strong>and</strong> fourth factors are named afterthe scales with the highest loadings oneach: social introversion <strong>and</strong> masculinityfemininity(Butcher et al., 1992).Test developers <strong>of</strong>ten eliminate scales oritems based on factor analyses. They alsocommonly design their composite scoresbased on factor-analytic results. This processwas not followed in the development<strong>of</strong> the MMPI-A, as this test was developedlong before the ready availability <strong>of</strong>factor-analytic procedures. Although theMMPI-A appears to be a four-factor test, itproduces 10 clinical scale T-scores, <strong>and</strong> nocomposite scores corresponding to the fourobtained factors are <strong>of</strong>fered. More recentlydeveloped tests, such as the CBCL, madeheavy use <strong>of</strong> factor analytic methods in thedevelopment <strong>of</strong> scale <strong>and</strong> composite scores(see Chap. 7).Generally, consumers <strong>of</strong> factor-analyticresearch seek comparability between thefactors <strong>and</strong> composite scores <strong>of</strong>fered forinterpretation. If there is, for example,a one-to-one relationship between thenumber <strong>of</strong> factors found <strong>and</strong> the number<strong>of</strong> composite scores produced, then thevalidity <strong>of</strong> the composite scores is likelyenhanced.Confirmatory Factor AnalysisThe procedures discussed thus far aregenerally referred to as exploratory factoranalyticprocedures. A newer factor-analytictechnique is called confirmatory factoranalysis (Kamphaus, 2001). These tw<strong>of</strong>actor-analytic procedures differ in somevery important ways. In exploratory factoranalysis, the number <strong>of</strong> factors to be yieldedis typically dictated by the characteristics<strong>of</strong> the intercorrelation matrix. That is, thenumber <strong>of</strong> factors selected is based on theamount <strong>of</strong> variance that each factor explains


CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUES41Table 2.2 Selected MMPI-A Factor Loadings1General MaladjustmentFactors2Social Overcontrol3Introversion4Masculinity FemininityHs .77 .09 .31 .05D .69 −.23 .51 −.08Hy .88 −.15 −.22 −.15Pd .71 .28 .21 .19Mf .08 .01 .07 −.84Pa .70 .19 .23 .25Pt .52 .39 .67 .06Sc .61 .38 .53 .36Ma .31 .78 −.04 .33Si .19 .10 .91 .02Note: These are varimax rotated factor loadings.Source: Adapted from Butcher et al., 1992.in the correlation matrix. If a factor, forexample, explains 70% <strong>of</strong> the variance inthe correlation matrix, then it is typicallyincluded as a viable factor in further aspects<strong>of</strong> the factor analysis. If, on the other h<strong>and</strong>,the factor only accounts for 2% <strong>of</strong> thevariance in a factor matrix, then it may notbe included as a viable factor.In confirmatory factor analysis, the number<strong>of</strong> factors is not dictated by data, but ratherby the theory underlying the test under investigation.In confirmatory factor analysis, thenumber <strong>of</strong> factors is selected a priori, as wellas the scales that load on each factor (Keith,1990). The primary test in confirmatory factoranalyses is the correspondence (i.e., fit)between the factor structure dictated a priori<strong>and</strong> the obtained data. If there is a great deal<strong>of</strong> correspondence between the hypothesizedstructure <strong>and</strong> the obtained factor structure,then the validity <strong>of</strong> the personality testis supported (hence the term confirmatory)<strong>and</strong> the theory is confirmed. If, for example,a researcher hypothesized the existence <strong>of</strong>four factors in a particular personality test,the confirmatory factor analysis will test howwell the data from a specific sample conformto this hyopothesized test structure.Thorough confirmatory factor-analyticstudies use a variety <strong>of</strong> statistics to assessthe fit <strong>of</strong> the hypothesized factor structureto the data. These statistics may include achi-square statistic, goodness-<strong>of</strong>-fit index,adjusted goodness-<strong>of</strong>-fit index, or rootmean square residual (RMR). Several statisticsare desirable for checking the fit<strong>of</strong> a confirmatory factor analysis becauseall <strong>of</strong> these statistics have strengths <strong>and</strong>weaknesses. The chi-square statistic, forexample, is highly influenced by samplesize (Glutting & Kaplan, 1990).Cluster AnalysisSimilarly to factor analysis, cluster analysisattempts to reduce the complexity <strong>of</strong> a dataset. In factor analysis, it is typical to try toreduce a large number <strong>of</strong> variables (e.g.,items) to a smaller set. In cluster analysis,researchers are most <strong>of</strong>ten interested ingrouping individuals (as opposed to variables)


42 CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUESinto groups <strong>of</strong> people who share commoncharacteristics. Ward’s (1963) hierarchicalagglomerative method is one example <strong>of</strong> apopular cluster-analytic technique.Several steps are common to cluster-analytictechniques, including the following:1. Collect a sample <strong>of</strong> individuals whohave been administered one test yieldingmultiple scores or a battery <strong>of</strong> tests.2. For each variable (e.g., depressionscores), compute the distance betweeneach pair <strong>of</strong> children.3. These distances between each individualon each variable are then used toproduce a proximity matrix. This matrixserves as the input for the cluster analysisin the same way that correlation orcovariance matrices are used as input infactor analysis.4. Apply a cluster-analytic method that sortsindividuals based on the distances betweenindividuals that were plotted in the proximitymatrix. In simple terms, clusteringmethods in this step match individualswith the smallest distance between individualson a particular variable.5. This sorting process continues untilgroups <strong>of</strong> individuals are formed thatare homogeneous (i.e., have pr<strong>of</strong>iles <strong>of</strong>scores <strong>of</strong> similar level <strong>and</strong> shape).6. Just as in factor analysis, the researcherhas to decide next on the number<strong>of</strong> clusters that is the most clinicallymeaningful. Statistical indexes areprovided as an aid to the researcher inthis step.Cluster-analytic techniques are useful inpsychopathology research for identifyingsubtypes <strong>of</strong> disorders or for designingdiagnostic systems (Borgen & Barnett,1987). Cluster-analytic techniques havefrequently been applied to identify subgroupsbased on their performance ona particular personality measure (e.g.,LaCombe et al., 1991).In a series <strong>of</strong> investigations, Kamphaus<strong>and</strong> colleagues have used cluster analysis<strong>of</strong> large data sets to identify children withsubsyndromal behavior problems (Huberty,Kamphaus, & DiStefano, 1997; Kamphaus,Huberty, DiStefano, & Petoskey,1997; Kamphaus et al., 1999). These studies<strong>of</strong> elementary school children suggestthat there are numerous children with pr<strong>of</strong>ilessuggestive <strong>of</strong> functional impairmentin school or at home who, nevertheless,are either not diagnosed or do not meetaccepted diagnostic criteria. Thus, thesecluster analyses helped to classify childrenwithout mental health diagnoses but whomay require prevention or treatment.Sensitivity <strong>and</strong> SpecificityIdentification <strong>of</strong> a diagnosis is one <strong>of</strong> theprimary reasons for conducting an evaluation.A test that is to be used for such apurpose should possess evidence <strong>of</strong> sensitivity,or the ability to identify true positives(i.e., the percentage <strong>of</strong> children whoactually have the disorder). A prototypicalstudy might involve administering an electronicmeasure <strong>of</strong> inattention to a group <strong>of</strong>children with ADHD <strong>and</strong> a group withoutany psychiatric diagnoses (“normals”). Inthis type <strong>of</strong> investigation, electronic measures<strong>of</strong> inattention <strong>of</strong>ten demonstrategood sensitivity by correctly identifyingthe vast majority <strong>of</strong> cases <strong>of</strong> ADHD, afinding that then triggers investigation <strong>of</strong>specificity. Specificity refers to the relativepercentage <strong>of</strong> true negatives, or the correctidentification <strong>of</strong> individuals who donot have the disorder as not having thedisorder. This same measure <strong>of</strong> inattentionmay also identify only 50% <strong>of</strong> the nondiagnosedsample as “normal.” Therefore, itmay have demonstrated good sensitivitybut inadequate specificity (i.e., a high rate<strong>of</strong> false positives). Electronic measures <strong>of</strong>inattention <strong>of</strong>ten produce results <strong>of</strong> thisnature. In an exhaustive review <strong>of</strong> the


CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUES43literature on such measures, Riccio <strong>and</strong>Reynolds (2003) have found that, whilesensitivity is typically good, evidence <strong>of</strong>specificity is <strong>of</strong>ten poor.In a later chapter, we will observe thatthe st<strong>and</strong>ards for this type <strong>of</strong> sensitivity<strong>and</strong> specificity have been raised considerablyby the most recent St<strong>and</strong>ardsfor Educational <strong>and</strong> Psychological Testing(AERA, APA, NCME, 1999). Now testsmust demonstrate the ability to differentiateamong diagnostic categories-not justbetween a diagnostic group <strong>and</strong> normality.Unfortunately, few test manuals provideevidence <strong>of</strong> this nature <strong>and</strong> many journalarticles test only the relatively easy distinctionbetween some condition <strong>and</strong> normality.The clinician, however, routinely hasthe more difficult task <strong>of</strong> differentiatingamong diagnostic categories. Again, electronicmeasures <strong>of</strong> inattention have notshown good evidence <strong>of</strong> diagnostic groupdifferentiation. In fact, Riccio <strong>and</strong> Reynolds(2003) concluded that when childrenwith a number <strong>of</strong> problems are included,the proportion <strong>of</strong> children correctly classifieddrops significantly.More recent work has focused on thedevelopment <strong>of</strong> an evidence-base that willimprove problem specificity, or the abilityto distinguish particular problems fromeach other (Mash & Hunsley, 2005). Cliniciansroutinely are faced with this taskwhich is also <strong>of</strong>ten considered “differentialdiagnosis.” The call for a larger moresound evidence base also raises awareness<strong>of</strong> positive predictive power (i.e., the ability<strong>of</strong> an item to correctly identify a child witha particular problem) <strong>and</strong> negative predictivepower (i.e., the ability <strong>of</strong> an item tocorrectly identify a child without a problem;Pelham, Fabiano, & Massetti, 2005).More research on these issues can onlyserve to assist in clinical decision making,but increased evidence will still not replaceclinical judgment in integrating informationfrom a variety <strong>of</strong> sources that havevarying degrees <strong>of</strong> validity.Threats to ValidityReadabilityAn obvious, but easily overlooked, threatto validity is the lack <strong>of</strong> ability <strong>of</strong> theparent, teacher, or child to underst<strong>and</strong>the personality test items. While concernis <strong>of</strong>ten expressed about the ability <strong>of</strong>children to read test items, parents mayalso have difficulty due to limited educationalattainment or cultural or linguisticdifferences. Harrington <strong>and</strong> Follett (1984)found that most tests available at thetime they conducted their study failed toaddress the issue in their test manuals.They provide several suggestions to thepractitioner for screening informants inorder to guard against readability servingas a threat to validity.For parents, Harrington <strong>and</strong> Follettrecommend having examiners read the testinstructions for the informant <strong>and</strong> paraphrase.<strong>Child</strong>ren can be asked to read someitems from the beginning, middle, <strong>and</strong> end<strong>of</strong> the instrument aloud so the examinercan gauge the child’s reading skill.Related to this point is the problem <strong>of</strong>translational equivalence or the degree to whicha translation <strong>of</strong> a test is equivalent to its originallanguage form (AERA, APA, NCME,1999). Evidence <strong>of</strong> translational equivalenceshould be <strong>of</strong>fered to reassure the test user thata threat to validity is not present.Response SetsA response set is a tendency to answer questionsin a biased fashion, thus maskingthe true feelings <strong>of</strong> the informant. Theseresponse sets are <strong>of</strong>ten mentioned, <strong>and</strong>addressed in construction <strong>and</strong> interpretation,in some personality tests .The social desirability response set is thetendency <strong>of</strong> the informant to respond toitems in a socially acceptable way (Anastasi& Urbina, 1998). Some personalitytests include items <strong>and</strong> scales to assess thepotential effects <strong>of</strong> such a response set.


44 CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUES“I like everyone that I meet” might bean item on such scales. The acquiescenceresponse set is the tendency to answer “true”or “yes” to a majority <strong>of</strong> the items (Kaplan& Saccuzzo, 1993). A third response setis called deviation, <strong>and</strong> it comes into playwhen an informant tends to give unusualor uncommon responses to items (Anastasi& Urbina, 1998).Guarding Against Validity Threats<strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> behavior tests <strong>of</strong>teninclude other validity scales or indexesin order to allow the examiner to detectvalidity threats. Some tests include fakebad scales, which assess the tendency toexaggerate problems. Computer scoring<strong>of</strong> personality tests has allowed for theinclusion <strong>of</strong> consistency indexes. One suchindex allows the examiner to determine ifthe informant is answering questions in apredictable pattern. A consistency indexmight be formed by identifying pairs<strong>of</strong> test items that correlate highly. If aninformant responds inconsistently to suchhighly correlated items, then his or herveracity may be suspect.Examiners <strong>of</strong>ten also conduct informalvalidity checks. One quick check is todetermine whether or not the informantresponded to enough items to make the testresult valid. Another elementary validitycheck involves scanning the form for patternedresponding. A form that routinelyalternates between true <strong>and</strong> false responsesmay reflect a patterning <strong>of</strong> responses.One way to limit the influence <strong>of</strong>response sets is to ensure that informantsare clear about the clinician’s expectations.Some clients may also need to take thepersonality test under more controlled circumstances.If an examiner has reason tobelieve, for example, that a child is oppositional,then the self-report personalitymeasure may best be completed in thepresence <strong>of</strong> the examiner.UtilityAs described earlier, clinical utility is the“next frontier” in evidence-based assessment.By the time an assessment instrumentis well-known <strong>and</strong> widely used inclinical settings, it usually has demonstratedadequate reliability <strong>and</strong> construct validity.However, as Mash <strong>and</strong> Hunsley (2005)describe, the question <strong>of</strong> utility or whetherthe instrument provides “psychologistswith the kinds <strong>of</strong> information that can beused in ways that will make a meaningfuldifference in relation to diagnostic accuracy,case formulation considerations, <strong>and</strong>treatment outcomes” (p. 365) remains. Thisconcept can also be applied to the inclusion<strong>of</strong> a particular informant in the assessmentprocess.In short, a rating scale, for example,may be a valid indicator <strong>of</strong> depression,but its utility indicates how valuable thatparticular rating scale is for an assessment<strong>of</strong> depression relative to other measures<strong>and</strong> relative to the cost (monetary <strong>and</strong>time) involved in administering it. Validity,including incremental validity (i.e., theimproved assessment decision as a result<strong>of</strong> adding a measure), is a necessary conditionfor utility, <strong>and</strong> establishing such validityevidence for an assessment tool, <strong>and</strong>especially an entire assessment battery, isarduous. Nevertheless, various forms <strong>of</strong>validity evidence are likewise not sufficientfor demonstrating utility. Ultimately, inaddition to cost effectiveness, the clinicianmust take into account the assessment’srole in translating to effective intervention<strong>and</strong> subsequent positive change for a child(Mash & Hunsley, 2005).Calls to examine the clinical utility <strong>of</strong>assessment are not entirely recent (e.g.,Hayes, Nelson, & Jarrett, 1987). However,give the current state <strong>of</strong> affairs, our discussion<strong>of</strong> utility is necessarily brief. As themove toward evidence-based assessmentbecomes strengthened by a larger collection


CHAPTER 2 MEASUREMENT ISSUES45<strong>of</strong> empirical research <strong>and</strong> improved communicationabout assessment strategies,future volumes will hopefully be well poisedto take on a detailed review <strong>of</strong> evidence onclinical utility.ConclusionsKnowledge <strong>of</strong> psychometric principlesis crucial for the proper interpretation<strong>of</strong> personality tests. As psychometricsbecome more complex, clinicians have tobecome increasingly sophisticated regardingpsychometric theory. Because personalityassessment technology has generallylagged behind other forms <strong>of</strong> child assessment,knowledge <strong>of</strong> psychometric theorymust be considered more <strong>of</strong>ten by the clinicianwhen interpreting scores.Some personality tests, for example, donot include basic psychometric propertiessuch as st<strong>and</strong>ard errors <strong>of</strong> measurement inthe manual. Such oversights discourage theuser from considering the error associatedwith scores, which is a basic considerationfor scale interpretation. Omissions like thisone are rare in academic <strong>and</strong> intelligenceassessment. The application <strong>of</strong> the SEMis merely one example <strong>of</strong> the psychometricpitfalls to be overcome by the user <strong>of</strong>personality tests. This chapter ends, however,on an optimistic note. Newer tests<strong>and</strong> recent revisions are providing moreevidence <strong>of</strong> validity <strong>and</strong> test limitations intheir manuals.Chapter Summary1. A T-score is a st<strong>and</strong>ard score that has amean <strong>of</strong> 50 <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation <strong>of</strong> 10.2. A percentile rank gives an individual’srelative position within the norm group.3. In order to select a representativesample <strong>of</strong> the national populationfor any country, test developers typicallyuse what are called stratificationvariables.4. Local norms are those based on somemore circumscribed subset <strong>of</strong> a largerpopulation.5. The reliability <strong>of</strong> a test refers to thedegree to which its scores are repeatedover several measurements.6. The st<strong>and</strong>ard error <strong>of</strong> measurement(SEM) is the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation <strong>of</strong> theerror distribution <strong>of</strong> scores.7. A confidence b<strong>and</strong> is a probability statementabout the likelihood that a particularrange <strong>of</strong> scores includes a child’strue score.8. The reliability <strong>of</strong> a test may differ forvarious score levels.9. Construct validity is the degree to whichtests measure what they purport tomeasure.10. Factor analysis is a data reduction techniquethat attempts to explain the variancein a personality or behavior testparsimoniously.11. In cluster analysis researchers are most<strong>of</strong>ten interested in grouping individuals(as opposed to variables) into clustersthat share common behavior ortraits.12. <strong>Personality</strong> tests <strong>and</strong> behavioral ratingscales <strong>of</strong>ten include other validityscales or indexes in order to allow theexaminer to detect validity threats.13. Sensitivity refers to the ability <strong>of</strong> a testto identify true positives <strong>and</strong> specificityto the ability <strong>of</strong> a test to identify truenegatives.14. <strong>Clinical</strong> utility concerns how well aparticular tool provides necessaryinformation <strong>and</strong> does so in a unique<strong>and</strong> cost-effective manner relativeto other tools (or informants).


C h a p t e r 3Classification <strong>and</strong> DevelopmentalPsychopathologyChapter QuestionsScience <strong>and</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>l Why is underst<strong>and</strong>ing the basic researchon children’s <strong>and</strong> adolescents’ emotional<strong>and</strong> behavioral functioning important toclinical assessment?l How are classification <strong>and</strong> assessmentrelated?l What are some <strong>of</strong> the models used forthe classification <strong>of</strong> the emotional <strong>and</strong>behavioral functioning <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong>adolescents?l What are some <strong>of</strong> the advantages <strong>and</strong>dangers <strong>of</strong> classification?l What are some <strong>of</strong> the most importantimplications <strong>of</strong> the basic research in thefield <strong>of</strong> developmental psychopathologyfor the clinical assessment <strong>of</strong> children<strong>and</strong> adolescents?A basic assumption underlying the writing<strong>of</strong> this text is that, to be competent in theclinical assessment <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents,much more knowledge is requiredthan being able to simply administer tests,this being the easier part. Many other crucialareas <strong>of</strong> expertise are necessary for appropriatelyselecting the tests to be administered<strong>and</strong> for interpreting them after administration.One such area <strong>of</strong> expertise was the focus<strong>of</strong> the previous chapter: an underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>of</strong> the science <strong>of</strong> measuring psychologicalconstructs. However, a more basic level <strong>of</strong>knowledge is needed for using measurementtheory appropriately. That is, one musthave a thorough underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> the nature<strong>of</strong> the phenomenon being measured beforedetermining the best method for measuring47P.J. Frick et al., <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Behavior,DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-0641-0_3, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010


48 CHAPTER 3 CLASSIFICATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGYit. In particular, the constructs <strong>of</strong> interest discussedin this chapter are the emotional <strong>and</strong>behavioral functioning <strong>of</strong> youth. The first hintat the importance <strong>of</strong> this basic underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>of</strong> psychological constructs was made in theprevious chapter on psychometrics. It shouldhave become clear that good psychometricproperties are not absolutes. They dependon the nature <strong>and</strong> characteristics <strong>of</strong> the specificpsychological construct being assessed.For example, childhood depression is frequentlycharacterized by multiple episodes <strong>of</strong>depression interspersed with periods <strong>of</strong> normalmood (Kovacs, 2001). Therefore, highstability estimates over lengthy time periodsshould not be expected. In fact, if such stabilityoccurs, then one is measuring somethingthat is not an episodic depression.In addition to appropriately utilizing psychometrictheory, underst<strong>and</strong>ing the nature<strong>of</strong> the phenomenon to be assessed is crucialto almost every aspect <strong>of</strong> a clinical assessment,from designing the assessment battery<strong>and</strong> selecting the tests, to interpreting theinformation, <strong>and</strong> communicating it to thechild <strong>and</strong> parent. For these reasons, science<strong>and</strong> clinical practice are inextricably linked.There are many areas <strong>of</strong> basic research thatenhance an assessor’s ability to conduct psychologicalevaluations, but we have selectedtwo that we feel are the most critical to theclinical assessment <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.First, a thorough underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong>the theories that guide the different models<strong>of</strong> classification are necessary because theframework used to define <strong>and</strong> classify psychologicalfunctioning determines how onedesigns <strong>and</strong> interprets an assessment battery.Secondly, the clinical assessment <strong>of</strong> childrenmust be conducted in the broad context <strong>of</strong>developmental psychopathology.Developmental psychopathology refersto an integration <strong>of</strong> two scientific disciplines:child development <strong>and</strong> childpsychopathology. The integration restson the basic assumption that the mostappropriate way to view the emotional<strong>and</strong> behavioral functioning <strong>of</strong> children,both normal <strong>and</strong> problematic, is within acomprehensive framework that includesthe influence <strong>of</strong> developmental processes(Rutter & Garmezy, 1983). A noteddevelopmental psychopathologist, JudyGarber, summarized her views on thisfield as being “concerned with both thenormal processes <strong>of</strong> change <strong>and</strong> adaptation,<strong>and</strong> the abnormal reactions to stressor adversity, as well as the relationshipbetween the two” (Garber, 1984, p. 30).Thus, developmental psychopathology isa framework for underst<strong>and</strong>ing children’semotions <strong>and</strong> behaviors that has manyimplications for the assessment process.It is beyond the scope <strong>of</strong> this book toprovide an intensive <strong>and</strong> exhaustive discussion<strong>of</strong> classification theories, or the manyimportant findings in the field <strong>of</strong> developmentalpsychopathology. Instead, thischapter illustrates the criticality <strong>of</strong> thesetwo knowledge areas to the assessment process,<strong>and</strong> also provides a basic frameworkfor applying this knowledge to the assessment<strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. The discussionthat follows highlights some <strong>of</strong> theissues in both areas that we feel have themost relevance to the assessment process.ClassificationClassification refers to the process <strong>of</strong> placingpsychological phenomena into distinctcategories according to some specifiedset <strong>of</strong> rules. There are two levels <strong>of</strong> classification.One level <strong>of</strong> classification isthe method <strong>of</strong> determining when a psychologicalfunctioning is abnormal, deviant,<strong>and</strong>/or in need <strong>of</strong> treatment, whilethe second level <strong>of</strong> classification is themethod <strong>of</strong> distinguishing among the differentdimensions or types <strong>of</strong> psychologicalfunctioning. Thus, clinical assessmentis considered partly, as a process <strong>of</strong> classification.It involves (1) determining whethersome areas <strong>of</strong> psychological functioning


CHAPTER 3 CLASSIFICATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY49in a child <strong>and</strong> adolescent are pathological<strong>and</strong> need treatment <strong>and</strong> (2) determiningthe types <strong>of</strong> pathology that may be present.Alternatively, according to Achenbach(1982) “assessment <strong>and</strong> classificationare two facets <strong>of</strong> what should be a singleprocess: assessment aims to identify thedistinguishing features <strong>of</strong> individual cases;taxonomy (classification) is the grouping <strong>of</strong>cases according to their distinguishing features”(p. 1). Therefore, underst<strong>and</strong>ing theissues involved in classification is essentialto clinical assessment.The first issue involves acknowledgingthat any classification system <strong>of</strong> psychologicalfunctioning will be imperfect. Psychologicalphenomena do not fall into specificcategories <strong>of</strong> normal <strong>and</strong> abnormal, or intoclear, non-overlapping dysfunction types.This seems to be especially true with children;there is <strong>of</strong>ten no clear demarcation<strong>of</strong> when a dimension <strong>of</strong> behavior should beconsidered normal <strong>and</strong> when it should beconsidered pathological. Further, there is<strong>of</strong>ten a high degree <strong>of</strong> overlap among thevarious forms <strong>of</strong> psychopathology in children.Finally, any classification system isonly as good as the research used to createit. As the research advances, so should theclassification system.Therefore, any system <strong>of</strong> classification isbound to be imperfect. Due to this imperfection,many experts have argued againstthe need for any formal classification system.Instead, they argue that psychological functioningshould be assessed <strong>and</strong> describedidiosyncratically for each individual person.That is, each person is a unique individualwhose psychological functioning shouldsimply be described in ways that maintainthis uniqueness without comparing it withthat <strong>of</strong> other individuals or fitting it intoartificial categories. This argument has anintuitive appeal given the complexity <strong>of</strong>human nature. However, there are severalcompelling arguments for the need for goodclassification systems, in spite <strong>of</strong> the factthat even the best system will be imperfect.The Need for ClassificationSystemsCommunicationThe main purpose <strong>of</strong> classification systemsis to enhance communication among pr<strong>of</strong>essionals(Blashfield, 1984; Quay, 1986).A classification system defines the rules bywhich psychological constructs are defined.In the absence <strong>of</strong> such a system, psychologicalconstructs are defined by idiosyncraticrules developed by each pr<strong>of</strong>essional, <strong>and</strong>one cannot underst<strong>and</strong> the terminology usedby a pr<strong>of</strong>essional unless the rules employedin defining the terms is understood. Forexample, the term depression is a psychologicalconstruct that has several meaningsin the psychological literature on children.It can refer to Major Depressive Episodes,as defined by the Diagnostic <strong>and</strong> StatisticalManual <strong>of</strong> Mental Disorders, FourthEdition-Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR; APA,2000). In contrast, it can also refer to elevationson a rating scale <strong>of</strong> depression (Curry& Craighead, 1993), or to a set <strong>of</strong> responseson a projective technique (Exner & Weiner,1982). The concept <strong>of</strong> masked depression isused to describe the belief that many childhoodproblems (e.g., hyperactivity, enuresis,learning disabilities) are the result <strong>of</strong>an underlying depressive state (Cytryn &McKnew, 1974). Not surprisingly, each <strong>of</strong>these definitions identifies a different group<strong>of</strong> children.Thus, simply saying that a child exhibitsdepression does not communicate much toanother pr<strong>of</strong>essional unless there is furtherexplanation on how this classification wasmade. On the contrary, if one states that thechild meets the criteria for Major Depressionaccording to the DSM-IV-TR system,then a classification is said to be madeusing a system with clearly defined rules.Also, another pr<strong>of</strong>essional will then havea clear idea <strong>of</strong> how depression is defined,even if he or she does not agree with theDSM-IV-TR system. However, this communicationrequires precision in the use


50 CHAPTER 3 CLASSIFICATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY<strong>of</strong> terms. In this example, using the termMajor Depression would be misleading, <strong>and</strong>actually impair the communication, if theterm was used without ensuring that theDSM-IV-TR criteria were met (e.g., basedon responses from a projective technique).Documentation <strong>of</strong> Need for ServicesClassification systems allow for the documentation<strong>of</strong> the need for services. Thisencompasses documenting the need forspecial educational services for a child,determining the need for mental healthservices within a given catchment area,attempting to determine appropriate staffingpatterns within an institution, or documentingthe need for services to third-partypayers (e.g., insurance companies). Theseuses <strong>of</strong> the classification systems have beenthe most controversial because the imperfectionsinherent in the existing systemscan lead to very deleterious outcomes formany people. For example, a child can bedenied services by a school or payment forthe services by an insurance company canbe denied if the child’s problems do not fitinto the classification system being used.Unfortunately, the task <strong>of</strong> documentingneed is inextricably linked to classificationbecause it requires some method <strong>of</strong> differentiatingbetween those in need <strong>of</strong> services<strong>and</strong> those that are not in need <strong>of</strong> services.The solution is not to eliminate the classificationsystems, but instead (1) to developbetter systems <strong>of</strong> classification that moredirectly predict the need for services <strong>and</strong>(2) to educate other pr<strong>of</strong>essionals on thelimitations <strong>of</strong> the classification systems, sothat they can be used more appropriatelyfor documenting the need for services.Dangers <strong>of</strong> ClassificationDue to the reasons already stated, it is generallyaccepted that explicit classificationsystems are needed. However, users <strong>of</strong> theclassification systems must be aware <strong>of</strong> thedangers <strong>and</strong> limitations <strong>of</strong> such systems.Because clinical assessment is a process <strong>of</strong>classification, the clinical assessor must beespecially cognizant <strong>of</strong> these issues. Many<strong>of</strong> these dangers can be limited if classificationsystems are used appropriately. Therefore,in the discussion that follows, we havetried to not only outline the dangers <strong>of</strong>classification but also to present practicesthat minimize or eliminate potentiallyharmful effects.Because psychological phenomena, <strong>and</strong>the persons they represent, do not fallneatly into categories, one loses informationby attempting to fit people into arbitrarycategories. People within the samecategory (e.g., Major Depression) share certaincharacteristics (e.g., depressed mood,loss <strong>of</strong> interest in activities, disturbances insleep, impaired concentration), but therealso exist many differences among personswithin a category (e.g., the number <strong>of</strong>depressive episodes, whether the depressionstarted after the death <strong>of</strong> a relative). Theshared characteristics should provide someimportant information about the personsin the category (e.g., prognosis, response totreatment), else the classification becomesuseless. However, given the loss <strong>of</strong> informationinherent in any classification grouping,classification should not be considered theonly information necessary for an adequatecase conceptualization. Instead, any classification,whether it is a diagnosis or an elevationon a behavior rating scale, should beone part <strong>of</strong> a larger description <strong>of</strong> the case.This approach allows one to take advantage<strong>of</strong> the positive aspects <strong>of</strong> formal classification(e.g., enhanced communication); yet,it acknowledges the limits <strong>of</strong> such systems<strong>and</strong> integrates classification into a broaderunderst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> the case. In this book,case studies that illustrate this approach areprovided. In each case, diagnoses or othermethods <strong>of</strong> classification are integrated intoa more complete clinical description <strong>of</strong> thechild being assessed.


CHAPTER 3 CLASSIFICATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY51A second danger <strong>of</strong> classification systemsis that they foster the illusion <strong>of</strong> aclear break between normal <strong>and</strong> psychopathologicalfunctioning. For example, ifthe classification is based on an elevationon a rating scale (over a T-score <strong>of</strong> 70), itgives the illusion <strong>of</strong> a dramatic differencebetween children with T-scores <strong>of</strong> 69 (notclassified) <strong>and</strong> children with T-scores <strong>of</strong>70 (classified). Stating that this is an illusiondoes not imply that all psychologicaltraits are on a continuum with normality,because some are clearly not. For example,in Jerome Kagan’s work with behaviorallyinhibited children, there seem to be anumber <strong>of</strong> qualitative differences betweenchildren with behaviorally inhibited temperament<strong>and</strong> those without this temperament(Kagan & Snidman, 1991). However,if one was using a measure <strong>of</strong> behavioralinhibition with some cut-<strong>of</strong>f for classifyinginhibition (e.g., a T-score <strong>of</strong> 65), theremay be some children close to this threshold(e.g., T-scores <strong>of</strong> 60–64) who were notclassified due to imperfections in the measurementtechnique (Ghiselli, Campbell,& Zedeck, 1981). Therefore, whether theillusion <strong>of</strong> a clear break is due to a normallydistributed trait or due to measurementerror, it is still an illusion.A third danger <strong>of</strong> classification is thedanger <strong>of</strong> stigmatization associated withthe psychological labels, <strong>of</strong>ten the endresult <strong>of</strong> classification. How strong theeffect <strong>of</strong> labeling is on psychological functioningis not clear from research. There isno evidence to suggest that the act <strong>of</strong> labelingcreates significant pathology througha self-fulfilling prophesy. However, it isalso clear that labels can affect how others,either lay persons (e.g., Snyder, Tanke,& Berscheid, 1977) or clinicians (Rockett,Murrie, & Boccaccini, 2007), interact withchildren who have been diagnosed withcertain mental health problems. Giventhis potential danger, classificatory terms(labels) should be used cautiously <strong>and</strong> onlywhen there is a clear purpose for doing so(e.g., when it influences treatment considerations).Also, when such terms are used,great efforts should be made to clearlydefine the meaning <strong>of</strong> the term to avoidmisinterpretations. And, finally, termsshould be worded to emphasize the classification<strong>of</strong> a psychological construct <strong>and</strong>not classification <strong>of</strong> the person. For example,it is better to use the phrase “a childwith conduct disorder” rather than stating“a conduct disordered child”.Evaluating ClassificationSystemsThus far, we have argued that classificationsystems are necessary despite the potentialdangers <strong>and</strong> misuses. However, this is onlythe case for good classification systems. Ifa classification system tells little about aperson, then nothing is gained in terms <strong>of</strong>communication, <strong>and</strong> all the dangers (e.g.,loss <strong>of</strong> information, stigmatization) aremaintained. Therefore, it is essential tocritically evaluate any system <strong>of</strong> classification<strong>and</strong>, even within a system, to evaluatethe individual categories.As discussed in the previous chapter, whileillustrating the association between classification<strong>and</strong> assessment, one finds that evaluation<strong>of</strong> classification systems is similar to theevaluation <strong>of</strong> assessment <strong>and</strong> procedures, ingeneral. Specifically, the primary considerationsfor evaluating a system are its reliability<strong>and</strong> the validity <strong>of</strong> interpretations derivedfrom it (Quay, 1986). In terms <strong>of</strong> reliability, auser <strong>of</strong> a system must be able to make classificationsconsistently, such as over short timeperiods (test-retest reliability) or betweentwo independent users who make the classification(interrater reliability). In order forclassification systems to be reliable, the rules<strong>of</strong> classification must be simple <strong>and</strong> explicit.However, reliability is important primarilybecause it limits the validity <strong>of</strong> a classificationsystem. Therefore, the validity <strong>of</strong> a systemis <strong>of</strong> paramount importance. Classification


52 CHAPTER 3 CLASSIFICATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGYmust allow for some valid interpretations tobe made. That is, classification must meansomething. It should tell something aboutthe causes <strong>of</strong> the child’s emotional or behavioralproblems or the likely course <strong>of</strong> theproblems. Most importantly, it should tellwhether the child needs treatment, <strong>and</strong> if so,what type <strong>of</strong> treatment.Models <strong>of</strong> ClassificationSo far, our discussion <strong>of</strong> classification systemshas been on issues that transcend anysingle type <strong>of</strong> classification system. Theuses <strong>of</strong> a system, the dangers inherent inclassification, <strong>and</strong> the methods <strong>of</strong> evaluatingsystems are all pertinent, irrespective<strong>of</strong> the model on which a system is based.However, there are several different theoreticalmodels on which a classificationsystem can be based. A model is a specificframework for viewing classification, suchas whether abnormal behavior is viewed asa statistical deviance (“Is this level <strong>of</strong> functioningrare in the general population?”) orin terms <strong>of</strong> its functional impairment (“Doesit affect a person’s adaptive functioning?”).The theoretical model <strong>of</strong> a system willdetermine the rules <strong>of</strong> classification. Aswill become evident, the different types<strong>of</strong> assessment techniques discussed in thisbook were designed to provide informationabout the different models <strong>of</strong> classification.The following sections will reviewtwo general models <strong>of</strong> classification thathave strongly influenced the classification<strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents, <strong>and</strong> have had amajor influence on the types <strong>of</strong> assessmentprocedures that have been developed.Medical ModelsThe first major model <strong>of</strong> classification, themedical model, was largely derived fromclinical experience with disturbed children<strong>and</strong> adolescents (Achenbach, 1982; Quay,1986). In this type <strong>of</strong> classification, a diagnosticentity is assumed to exist, <strong>and</strong> thesystem defines the characteristics that areindicative <strong>of</strong> this diagnosis. The approachis called a medical model approach because itassumes there is a disease entity, or a coredeficit, which is the disorder. It then definesthe symptoms that are indicative <strong>of</strong> the presence<strong>of</strong> the disorder.There are two primary characteristics <strong>of</strong>the medical model approach to classification.First, because <strong>of</strong> the emphasis on a core deficit,medical model systems differ dramaticallydepending on the theory or theories usedto define the deficits considered to underliethe psychological disorders. That is, medicalmodel systems are strongly influenced bythe theory <strong>of</strong> abnormal behavior espoused bythe system, such as psychodynamic theoriesor biological theories. Second, because <strong>of</strong> theemphasis on a pathological core (e.g., the diseaseentity), medical model systems typicallymake sharp distinctions between disordered<strong>and</strong> non-disordered individuals. There istypically an underlying assumption that thereare qualitative differences between individualswith <strong>and</strong> without a disorder.Multivariate ApproachesThe second major approach to classificationthat has been extremely influential inthe clinical assessment <strong>of</strong> children has beenlabeled the multivariate statistical (Quay,1986) or the psychometric approach(Achenbach, 1982). In this approach, multivariatestatistical techniques are used toisolate interrelated patterns <strong>of</strong> behavior.Therefore, unlike the clinically derivedsyndromes that are defined by theory <strong>and</strong>clinical observations, behavioral syndromesare defined by the statistical relationshipbetween behaviors or their patterns <strong>of</strong>covariation. In this approach, behaviorsform a syndrome if they are highly correlatedwith each other, <strong>and</strong> there is no necessaryassumption <strong>of</strong> a pathological coreto underlie the symptoms, as is the case inmedical models <strong>of</strong> classification.


CHAPTER 3 CLASSIFICATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY53Table 3.1 An Example <strong>of</strong> a Multivariate Classification SystemExternalizingInternalizingInattention HI/ODD CD Social Anxiety Depression SAD/FearsDisorganizedInterruptsothersFights Timid/shy Sad Upset overseparationDistractible Stubborn Spreads rumors Not self-confident Low energy Worriedabout parentForgetful Loud Bullies Nervous in groups Anhedonia Afraid to leavehouseSloppy/messy Noisy StealsDaydreams Talks a lot LiesNote: Dimensions are based on a series <strong>of</strong> exploratory <strong>and</strong> confirmatory factor analyses <strong>of</strong> caretaker ratings <strong>of</strong>1,358 children <strong>and</strong> adolescents <strong>of</strong> ages 4–17 (Lahey et al., 2004). Behaviors listed are just examples <strong>and</strong> not thecomplete list <strong>of</strong> items used in the factor analyses. HI hyperactivity-impulsivity, ODD oppositional defiant disorder,CD conduct disorder, SAD separation anxiety disorder.In addition to being based on statisticalcovariation, the psychometric approachis also different from medical models <strong>of</strong>classification because it emphasizes quantitativedistinctions rather than qualitativedistinctions. Once behavioral syndromesare isolated through statistical analyses, achild’s level <strong>of</strong> functioning along the variousdimensions <strong>of</strong> behavior is determined.Behavioral syndromes are conceptualizedalong a continuum, from normal to deviant.Interpretations are typically made bycomparing an individual case to a representativenormative sample, <strong>and</strong> choosingsome level <strong>of</strong> functioning as being so rarein the average population that it should beconsidered deviant. Classifications are thusbased on how a child falls into a certaindimension <strong>of</strong> functioning (e.g., anxiety/withdrawal) relative to some comparisongroup (e.g., compared to other children <strong>of</strong>the same age group).In Table 3.1, we provide an example <strong>of</strong>a multivariate approach to the classification<strong>of</strong> childhood emotional <strong>and</strong> behavioralfunctioning reported by Lahey et al.(2004). This system was based on a series <strong>of</strong>exploratory <strong>and</strong> confirmatory factor analyses<strong>of</strong> caretaker ratings <strong>of</strong> 1,358 children <strong>and</strong>adolescents <strong>of</strong> ages 4–17. It shows sixdimensions <strong>of</strong> behavior that can be subsumedunder two broad dimensions <strong>of</strong>Externalizing <strong>and</strong> Internalizing problems.Classification in the Future:An Integration <strong>of</strong> Medical <strong>and</strong>Multivariate ApproachesThese two basic approaches to classificationare important for clinical assessmentbecause the design <strong>of</strong> assessmentinstruments is <strong>of</strong>ten consistent withone <strong>of</strong> these basic approaches. Moreimportantly, the interpretation <strong>of</strong>assessment instruments is basically aprocess <strong>of</strong> classification. Therefore,it is <strong>of</strong>ten guided by these models orsome variation <strong>of</strong> them. The clinicalassessor should be aware <strong>of</strong> the issuesinvolved in the classification generally,<strong>and</strong> the advantages <strong>and</strong> disadvantages<strong>of</strong> these two models <strong>of</strong> classificationspecifically, to aid in the interpretation<strong>of</strong> assessment measures.Research has indicated that both themedical <strong>and</strong> the multivariate models have


54 CHAPTER 3 CLASSIFICATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGYflaws that make their exclusive use problematic(see Quay, 1986). For example, thedependence <strong>of</strong> medical model approacheson theory has led to many “disorders” beingcreated with little support from research.Also, the medical model approach, with itsemphasis on qualitative distinction, masksa continuum with normality that seemsmost appropriate for underst<strong>and</strong>ing manydimensions <strong>of</strong> functioning. In contrast,the multivariate approach with its dependenceon statistical analyses in the absence<strong>of</strong> clear theory has resulted in syndromesthat are hard to generalize across samples<strong>and</strong> with different sets <strong>of</strong> symptoms. Also,while some psychological phenomena inchildren <strong>and</strong> adolescents are best conceptualizedon a continuum with normality,there are others that may fit with morequalitative distinctions (e.g., Kagan &Snidman, 1991; Lahey et al., 1990) <strong>and</strong>are not captured well by the multivariateapproach.As a result, future classificationsshould look towards an integration <strong>of</strong> theapproaches. For example, clinical diagnosescan be improved by conducting multivariateanalyses to see if the covariation<strong>of</strong> symptoms for the diagnosis is supported(e.g., Frick, et al., 1993; Lahey et al.,2004). However, there are other ways inwhich the correspondence between statisticallyderived syndromes <strong>and</strong> the clinicallyderived diagnoses can be explicitly tested(e.g., Eiraldi, Power, Karustis, & Goldstein,2000), thereby improving the validity<strong>of</strong> both the approaches <strong>and</strong> leading to classificationsystems that accommodate thediverse nature <strong>of</strong> psychological constructs.In the following section, we provide anoverview <strong>of</strong> one <strong>of</strong> the most commonlyused classification systems, the Diagnostic<strong>and</strong> Statistical Manual <strong>of</strong> Mental Disorders,which is published by the American PsychiatricAssociation. Although initially thissystem <strong>of</strong> classification was based largelyon a medical model system <strong>of</strong> classification,more recent revisions have attemptedto capture the best characteristics <strong>of</strong> thetwo major classification approaches.Diagnostic <strong>and</strong> StatisticalManual <strong>of</strong> Mental Disorders,Fourth Edition (DSM-IV; APA,1994 & DSM-IVTR; APA, 2000)The DSM approach to defining psychiatricdisorders has undergone dramaticchanges in its many revisions since its firstpublication in 1952. The biggest changecame with the publication <strong>of</strong> its thirdrevision in 1980. In the first two editions,the definition <strong>of</strong> disorder was clearlybased on a medical model approach toclassification. The definition assumedan underlying pathological core, <strong>and</strong> theconceptualization <strong>of</strong> the core was largelybased on psychodynamic theory. In thethird edition, there was an explicit switchfrom a medical model view <strong>of</strong> disorders<strong>and</strong> a dependence on the psychodynamictheory. In the third <strong>and</strong> subsequent editions,a functional approach <strong>of</strong> viewingdisorders was used in which mentaldisorders were defined as “a clinicallysignificant behavioral or psychologicalsyndrome or pattern that occurs in anindividual <strong>and</strong> is typically associated withpresent distress (e.g., a painful symptom)or disability (i.e., impairment in one ormore important areas <strong>of</strong> functioning), orwith a significantly increased risk <strong>of</strong> suffering,death, pain, disability, or importantloss <strong>of</strong> freedom.” (p. xxxi, AmericanPsychiatric Association, 2000).Another major change in the thirdedition, also maintained in subsequentrevisions, is an increase in the level <strong>of</strong>specificity with which disorders aredefined. In the first two editions <strong>of</strong> themanual, disorders were <strong>of</strong>ten poorlydefined, leading to problems <strong>of</strong> obtaininghigh levels <strong>of</strong> reliability in the diagnosticclassifications (Spitzer & Cantwell,1980). In contrast, later revisions include


CHAPTER 3 CLASSIFICATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY55more detailed diagnostic definitions withexplicit symptom lists, which have led toan increase in the reliability <strong>of</strong> the system(e.g., Spitzer, Davies, & Barkley, 1990).To illustrate this change, the DSM-II(APA, 1968) definition <strong>of</strong> HyperkineticReaction <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong>hood is contrasted withthe analogous DSM-III-R definition <strong>of</strong>Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorderin Box 3.1.As a result <strong>of</strong> these changes, the mostrecent revision <strong>of</strong> the manual (DSM-IV;APA, 1994) has characteristics <strong>of</strong> boththe medical <strong>and</strong> multivariate models <strong>of</strong>classification. For example, its functionalapproach to defining disorders, which doesBox 3.1A Comparison <strong>of</strong> DSM-II <strong>and</strong> DSM-III-R Diagnostic CriteriaDSM-II: Hyperkinetic Reaction<strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong>hood (Adolescence)This disorder is characterized by overactivity,restlessness, distractibility,<strong>and</strong> short attention span, especially inyoung children; the behavior usuallydiminishes in adolescence. If this behavior iscaused by brain damage, it should be diagnosedunder the appropriate non-psychoticorganic brain syndrome.DSM-III-R: Attention-DeficitHyperactivity DisorderNote: Consider a criterion met only if thebehavior is considerably more frequent thanthat <strong>of</strong> most people <strong>of</strong> the same mental age.A. A disturbance <strong>of</strong> at least 6 months duringwhich at least eight <strong>of</strong> the following arepresent:1. Often fidgets with h<strong>and</strong>s or feet orsquirms in seat2. Has difficulty remaining seated whenrequired to do so3. Is easily distracted by extraneous stimuli4. Has difficulty awaiting turn in games orgroup situations5. Often blurts out answers to questionsbefore they have been completed6. Has difficulty following throughinstructions from others (not due tooppositional behavior or failure <strong>of</strong> comprehension),e.g., fails to finish chores7. Has difficulty sustaining attention intasks or play activities8. Often shifts from one uncompletedactivity to another9. Has difficulty playing quietly10. Often talks excessively11. Often interrupts or intrudes on others,e.g., butts into other children’s games12. Often does not seem to listen to whatis being said to him or her.13. Often loses things necessary for tasksor activities at school or at home (e.g.,toys, pencils, books, assignments)14. Often engages in physically dangerousactivities without considering possibleconsequences (not for the purpose <strong>of</strong>thrill seeking), e.g., runs into streetwithout lookingB. Onset before the age <strong>of</strong> 7.C. Does not meet criteria for a PervasiveDevelopmental Disorder.Sources: Diagnosis <strong>and</strong> Statistical Manual <strong>of</strong> Mental Disorders, Second Edition, American PsychiatricAssociation, 1967 <strong>and</strong> Diagnosis <strong>and</strong> Statistical Manual <strong>of</strong> Mental Disorders, Third Edition, Revised,American Psychiatric Association, 1987. Reproduced with permission <strong>of</strong> the publisher.


56 CHAPTER 3 CLASSIFICATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGYnot assume a pathological core, is consistentwith the multivariate approach toclassification. Also, many <strong>of</strong> the disordersare based, at least in part, on the patterns<strong>of</strong> symptom covariation, which is the hallmark<strong>of</strong> multivariate models (e.g., Frick,et al., 1994). In contrast to the multivariateapproach, DSM-IV definitions classifydisorders into discrete categories, whichis more consistent with the medical modelapproach, although many <strong>of</strong> the cut-<strong>of</strong>fswere empirically determined rather thanbeing based purely on theoretical considerations(Lahey, et al., 1994).One <strong>of</strong> the major changes in DSM-IVfrom its predecessors is the emphasis onusers having access to the basic researchunderlying the various diagnosticcategories. For example, in the manual,each disorder is initially introduced bysummarizing the current research on itsbasic characteristics, associated features likeage, gender, <strong>and</strong> cultural trends, prevalence,course, <strong>and</strong> familial pattern (APA, 1994).These introductory descriptions wereenhanced in a later version <strong>of</strong> the manual:the Diagnostic <strong>and</strong> Statistical Manual<strong>of</strong> Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition,Text Revision (DSMIV-TR; AmericanPsychiatric Association, 2000).In this revision, no changes were madeto the basic structure <strong>and</strong> diagnostic criteriafor the individual disorders, nor were anydisorders added or deleted. Instead, workgroups were formed to identify any errorsor omissions in the introductory materialfor each disorder, <strong>and</strong> to provide additionalmaterial to enhance the description <strong>of</strong> thedisorders <strong>and</strong> their basic characteristics,if these could be justified by a review <strong>of</strong>the relevant research. In addition, thepublication <strong>of</strong> the DSM-IV <strong>and</strong> DSM-IV-TRwere accompanied by the publication <strong>of</strong> anumber <strong>of</strong> edited volumes called “DSM-IVSource Books” (e.g., Widiger et al., 1994)that provide more extended reviews <strong>of</strong> theresearch that led to the diagnostic criteriaincluded in these manuals. As such, thesemost recent revisions <strong>of</strong> the DSM havetried to enhance the ability <strong>of</strong> users <strong>of</strong>this system to gain access to the researchfindings related to the disorders includedin the manual.DSM-IV maintained the multiaxial system<strong>of</strong> classification that was initiated inDSM-III. As discussed previously in thischapter, a major disadvantage <strong>of</strong> any classificationsystem is its inability to captureall relevant dimensions <strong>of</strong> a person’s functioningwithin a single given category ordiagnosis. For a broader view <strong>of</strong> classification,DSM-IV specifies several dimensions<strong>of</strong> functioning (axes) that are relevantto underst<strong>and</strong>ing a person’s functioning.Specifically, Axis I (<strong>Clinical</strong> Disorders)<strong>and</strong> II (<strong>Personality</strong> Disorders/MentalRetardation) are fairly typical <strong>of</strong> otherclassification systems <strong>and</strong> comprise themajor categories <strong>of</strong> mental disorders. Box3.2 provides a summary <strong>of</strong> the Axis I <strong>and</strong>Axis II diagnoses that are most relevantfor children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. However,DSM-IV includes three other dimensionson which a child can be classified. AxisIII allows the system user to indicate anyphysical disorder that is potentially relevantfor the underst<strong>and</strong>ing or managing<strong>of</strong> a case. Axis IV allows for a reporting <strong>of</strong>the psychosocial <strong>and</strong> environmental stressorsthat may affect the diagnosis, treatment,<strong>and</strong> prognosis <strong>of</strong> mental disordersin Axes I <strong>and</strong> II. Axis V provides a scale toindicate the highest level <strong>of</strong> adaptive functioning(psychological, social, <strong>and</strong> occupational/educational)that is currently beingexhibited or the highest level <strong>of</strong> adaptivefunctioning that has been exhibited withinthe past year. Clearly, this multiaxialapproach <strong>of</strong> DSM-IV is not sufficient totake the place <strong>of</strong> an adequate case formulation.However, it highlights the need toplace diagnoses in the context <strong>of</strong> manyother important aspects <strong>of</strong> a person’s psychologicalfunctioning.


CHAPTER 3 CLASSIFICATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY57Box 3.2A Summary <strong>of</strong> DSM-IV Axes I <strong>and</strong> II Diagnoses Relevant to <strong>Child</strong>ren <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong>sIntellectualLearningMotor SkillsPervasiveDevelopmentalBehavioralMental RetardationMathematics DisorderDisorder <strong>of</strong> WrittenExpressionReading DisorderLanguage <strong>and</strong> SpeechExpressive Language DisorderMixed Receptive-ExpressiveLanguage DisorderPhonological DisorderStutteringSelective MutismDevelopmental CoordinationDisorderAutistic DisorderRhett’s Disorder<strong>Child</strong>hood DisintegrativeDisorderAsperger’s DisorderAttention-DeficitHyperactivity DisorderOppositional Defiant DisorderConduct DisorderEmotionalIdentityPhysical(Eating)Physical(Motor)Physical(Elimination)Bipolar Disorders (I &II)*Cyclothymia*Adjustment Disorder withDepressed Mood*Gender Identity Disorder<strong>of</strong><strong>Child</strong>hoodReactive AttachmentDisorder <strong>of</strong> Infancy orEarly <strong>Child</strong>hoodAnorexia Nervosa*Bulimia Nervosa*PicaRumination DisorderTourette’s DisorderChronic Motor or VocalTic DisorderTransient Tic DisorderStereotypic MovementDisorderEncopresisEnuresisEmotional(Anxiety)Emotional(Mood)Separation Anxiety DisorderGeneralized Anxiety Disorder *Panic Disorder *Agoraphobia *Social Phobia*Obsessive CompulsiveDisorder *Post-Traumatic StressDisorder *Adjustment Disorder withAnxious Mood*Major Depression *Dysthymia*Physical(Somatic)PsychosisSubstance–RelatedDisordersSomatization Disorder*Conversion Disorder*Pain Disorder *Hypochrondriasis*Body DysmorphicDisorder *Adjustment Disorder withPhysical Complaints*Schizophrenia*Alcohol (Amphetamine,Cannabis, etc.)Dependence**Denotes disorders that have the same criteria for children <strong>and</strong> adults.Note: The selection <strong>of</strong> disorders most relevant to children <strong>and</strong> adolescents <strong>and</strong> the grouping <strong>of</strong> disorders weremade by the authors <strong>and</strong> not by DSM-1V.


58 CHAPTER 3 CLASSIFICATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGYDevelopmentalPsychopathologyAs mentioned earlier, the overridingprinciple <strong>of</strong> developmental psychopathologyis that children’s emotional <strong>and</strong>behavioral functioning must be understoodwithin a developmental context(Rutter & Garmezy, 1983). Therefore,it follows that the assessment <strong>of</strong> children’semotional <strong>and</strong> behavioral functioning mustalso be conducted within a developmentalframework. Such themes as underst<strong>and</strong>ingbehavior in a developmental context<strong>and</strong> conducting assessment within a developmentalframework are broad principlesthat have several important specific implicationsfor the assessment process.Developmental NormsFirst, a developmental approach recognizesthat a child’s emotional <strong>and</strong> behavioralfunctioning must be understood withinthe context <strong>of</strong> developmental norms. Tobe specific, there are numerous behaviors<strong>of</strong> children that are common at one age,but relatively uncommon at others. Forexample, bedwetting is quite common priorto age 5, <strong>and</strong> even at age 5, it is present in 15 -20% <strong>of</strong> children (Doleys, 1977; Walker,Milling, & Bonner, 1988). Similarly, childhoodfears tend to be quite common, <strong>and</strong>the types <strong>of</strong> fears that are most commonshow a regular progression with childdevelopment (Campbell, 1986). For example,separation anxiety is not uncommonin infants toward the end <strong>of</strong> the first year<strong>of</strong> life (Bowlby, 1969), whereas fears <strong>of</strong>the dark <strong>and</strong> imaginary creatures are quitecommon in preschool <strong>and</strong> school-age childrenbut decrease in prevalence with age(Bauer, 1976).These are just a few <strong>of</strong> the many development-relatedchanges in the prevalence<strong>of</strong> specific child behaviors. Knowledge <strong>of</strong>these developmental changes in behavioris crucial to clinical assessment becausethe same behavior may be developmentallyappropriate at one age but indicative<strong>of</strong> pathology at another. Therefore, assessment<strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents mustallow development-based interpretations.The critical nature <strong>of</strong> these interpretationsimplies that selection <strong>of</strong> assessmenttechniques must be based, at least in part,on the availability <strong>of</strong> age-specific norms.Further, given the rapid developmentalchanges experienced by children <strong>and</strong> adolescents,comparisons must be made withinfairly limited age groups. Whereas foradults using a comparison group that spansthe ages from 25 to 35 may be justifiable,a comparison group for children that spansthe ages 5–15 would be meaningless, giventhe many changes in development thatare subsumed within this period. Becausethe normative information provided by anassessment instrument <strong>and</strong> the appropriateuse <strong>of</strong> norm-referenced information bythe assessor are critical components to theclinical assessment <strong>of</strong> children, these issuesare discussed in great detail throughoutthis book.Developmental ProcessesUnfortunately, many assessors believe thatsimply comparing the assessment informationwith age norms is all that is neededto take a developmental approach to child<strong>and</strong> adolescent assessment. This is a muchtoo limited view <strong>of</strong> development, <strong>and</strong> howcan it be applied to underst<strong>and</strong>ing bothnormal <strong>and</strong> pathological outcomes in children.A developmental approach is a “process-oriented”approach. Put simply, thismeans that any developmental outcome,be it a normal personality dimension or aproblematic behavioral pattern, is the endresult <strong>of</strong> an interaction <strong>of</strong> numerous interrelatedmaturation processes (e.g., socioemotional,cognitive, linguistic, biological).


CHAPTER 3 CLASSIFICATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY59This process-oriented approach has severalimportant implications for how assessorsconceptualize what they are trying to assess<strong>and</strong> how they go about doing it.First, given the interrelated nature <strong>of</strong>the maturation processes, this approachrecognizes that a focus on any single type <strong>of</strong>developmental process will provide only alimited, <strong>and</strong> sometimes misleading, underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>of</strong> a child’s psychosocial functioning.For example, underst<strong>and</strong>ing the familyenvironment <strong>of</strong> a child with behavioralproblems will only provide a limited perspectiveon how these behavioral problemsdeveloped without considering the child’stemperament, which may make the childmore difficult to raise. Such transactionalrelations among developmental processesnecessitate that an assessor design batteriesthat assess the many different types <strong>of</strong>processes <strong>and</strong> take into consideration thepotential transactional relations amongthese processes when interpreting the testingresults.Second, the process-oriented developmentalapproach recognizes that the samedevelopmental process (e.g., a permissiverearing environment) may result in differentoutcomes (e.g., some children whoare creative, others who are dependent,<strong>and</strong> others who are antisocial), leadingto a concept called “multifinality”. Thecomplementary concept is “equifinality”<strong>and</strong> refers to the possibility that thesame outcome (e.g., antisocial behavior)can result from very different developmentalprocesses among individuals (e.g.,very strong emotional reactivity leadingto strong, angry <strong>and</strong> aggressive reactionsor weak emotional reactivity leading toproblems in the experience <strong>of</strong> empathy<strong>and</strong> guilt). The implications for the assessmentprocess are that the assessors need toexpect that the same personality pattern orpsychopathological condition may resultfrom very different processes across individuals,<strong>and</strong> assessment batteries need to bedesigned to assess these “causal pathways.”This, in fact, may be one <strong>of</strong> the most criticalconcepts in a developmental psychopathologicalapproach to assessment becauseit places the assessment process in theimportant role <strong>of</strong> uncovering the uniquecausal pathway that leads to a child’s currentfunctioning so that interventions canbe better tailored to the unique needs <strong>of</strong>the child (see, e.g., Frick, 2006).Third, how the various processes unfoldover development leads to specific “tasks”that may make certain behaviors more likelyto occur at certain points in development.These unique dem<strong>and</strong>s, or “developmentaltasks,” lead to many <strong>of</strong> the age-relatedchanges in children’s emotions <strong>and</strong> behaviorsdiscussed previously. Comparing achild’s behavioral or emotional functioningto the developmental norms can helpdetermine whether the child’s functioningis deviant compared to other children whoare experiencing similar developmentaldem<strong>and</strong>s. However, simply comparing achild’s behavior to developmental norms<strong>and</strong> determining whether or not the child’sfunctioning is deviant compared to otherchildren <strong>of</strong> the same age does not allowthe assessor to determine whether (1) thechild’s problems should be considered anexaggeration <strong>of</strong> the normal maturationalprocesses operating at that developmentstage or (2) the child’s problems should beconsidered as a qualitative deviation fromnormal development (i.e., not consistentwith the specific dem<strong>and</strong>s <strong>of</strong> that developmentalstage), with the latter <strong>of</strong>ten beingindicative <strong>of</strong> a more severe pathologicalprocess.These two different interpretations<strong>of</strong> deviations from developmental normscan be illustrated in the assessment <strong>of</strong>conduct problems in children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.Research has documented anincrease in the acting-out behavior forboth boys <strong>and</strong> girls that coincides withthe onset <strong>of</strong> adolescence (e.g., Offord,et al., 1989). The first implication <strong>of</strong> thisfinding is that the assessment <strong>of</strong> adolescent


60 CHAPTER 3 CLASSIFICATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGYconduct problems should be based on acomparison with the adolescent norms,so that age-specific deviations can bedetermined. However, to interpret theseage-specific deviations, it is also helpfulto realize that an increase in acting-outbehavior in adolescence is consistentwith the identity formation processoutlined in Erik Erikson’s psychosocialtheory <strong>of</strong> personality development(Erikson, 1968). Specifically, Eriksoncharacterizes adolescence as a time whenyouths are struggling with the development<strong>of</strong> an individual identity, one thatis separate from their parents. Rebellion<strong>and</strong> questioning <strong>of</strong> authority aremanifestations <strong>of</strong> adolescents’ rejection<strong>of</strong> parental <strong>and</strong> societal values, as theystruggle to develop their unique identity.Underst<strong>and</strong>ing this process allows for anadditional interpretation when one documentsdeviations from age norms. If ayouth exhibits developmentally deviantlevels <strong>of</strong> conduct problems for the firsttime in adolescence, the conduct problemsmay be best conceptualized as anexaggeration <strong>of</strong> a normal developmentalprocess (e.g., identity development).In contrast, a preadolescent child whoexhibits deviant levels <strong>of</strong> conduct problemsis showing a behavior that seemsmore qualitatively different from whatis expected from normal developmentalprocesses. It may, therefore, be an indication<strong>of</strong> more severe pathology. This isconsistent with research, indicating thatconduct problems that have onset in adolescenceare more likely to be transient,whereas conduct problems with a prepubertalonset tend to be more severe <strong>and</strong>chronic (M<strong>of</strong>fitt, 2003).Stability <strong>and</strong> ContinuityIssues regarding the stability <strong>of</strong> childhoodbehavioral <strong>and</strong> emotional functioning areimportant from a developmental perspectiveto psychopathology. These complex issueshave important implications for the assessmentprocess. The basic issue <strong>of</strong> stability isnot unique to the assessment <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong>adolescents, but has been a long-st<strong>and</strong>ingcontroversy in psychological assessmentthroughout the life span. For example, manyhave questioned the concept <strong>of</strong> personality,because it implies a consistency <strong>of</strong> behaviorover place <strong>and</strong> time that is <strong>of</strong>ten not apparentin human behavior (Mischel, 1968). Thisissue is more relevant to children than adultsbecause childhood behavior seems to beless stable over time <strong>and</strong> situation, makingthe concept <strong>of</strong> personality in children evenmore controversial. Our view <strong>of</strong> the debate,which is similar to the view <strong>of</strong> many othertheorists (e.g., Buss, 1995; Martin, 1998), isthat the concept <strong>of</strong> personality can be usefulif conceptualized appropriately, but dangerousif viewed wrongly.For example, many measures <strong>of</strong> children’sbehavior or other aspects <strong>of</strong> personalityshow much less stability in childrenthan do analogous constructs in adults.Specifically, Roberts <strong>and</strong> DelVeccio (2000)reported a meta-analysis <strong>of</strong> 152 longitudinalstudies assessing the average stability<strong>of</strong> personality traits in different age groupsover a 6–7-year period. They reported thatthe average stability coefficient for children<strong>and</strong> adolescents was 0.31, comparedto 0.54 for young adults, 0.64 for middleagedadults, <strong>and</strong> 0.75 for adults over theage <strong>of</strong> 50. These findings are not surprisinggiven the rapid developmental changesthat occur in childhood. However, thefindings have important implications forthe interpretation <strong>of</strong> personality measuresin children. Specifically, interpretations <strong>of</strong>dispositional characteristics must be madecautiously in children so that there is noimplication <strong>of</strong> strong stability over time,unless data are available to support suchan interpretation. Given that the data arelacking in most cases, the term personalitymay be misleading for many domains <strong>of</strong>child behavior.


CHAPTER 3 CLASSIFICATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY61Although we feel that this caution iswarranted, there are also several ways inwhich this general statement must be qualified.First, there is clearly some continuityin children’s behavior, <strong>and</strong> the degree <strong>of</strong>stability (or instability) seems to be dependenton the domain <strong>of</strong> behavior beingassessed. For example, research generallyindicates that externalizing behaviors (e.g.,hyperactivity, aggression, antisocial behavior)tend to be more stable over time thaninternalizing behaviors (e.g., fears, depression)(e.g., Frick & Loney, 1999; Ollendick& King, 1994). Therefore, interpretations<strong>of</strong> the stability <strong>of</strong> behavior must be dependenton the dimension <strong>of</strong> behavior that is<strong>of</strong> interest.In addition, aggregates <strong>of</strong> behaviors(behavioral domains) tend to be more stablethan discrete behaviors. For example,Silverman <strong>and</strong> Nelles (1989) reported onthe 1-year stability <strong>of</strong> mothers’ reports <strong>of</strong>fears in their children between the ages <strong>of</strong> 8<strong>and</strong> 11. Over the 1-year study period, therewas only a 10% overlap between Time 1<strong>and</strong> Time 2 in the ten specific objects orsituations that mothers reported as elicitingthe most fear in their children. However,the correlation between the absolutenumber <strong>of</strong> fears was quite high. Althoughthe specific types <strong>of</strong> fears were not stableover the study period, the level <strong>of</strong> fears wasstable. Some have argued that aggregationallows one to pick up generalized responsetendencies that are not captured by discretebehaviors (Martin, 1988). However,this increase in stability through the aggregation<strong>of</strong> behaviors can also be conceptualizedfrom basic measurement theory. It hasconsistently been shown that increasingthe number <strong>of</strong> items on a measure <strong>of</strong> a traitalso increases its reliability (Ghiselli et al.,1981). Hence, the increased stability maybe a function <strong>of</strong> a more reliable method <strong>of</strong>measurement.Finally, stability can be affected bywhether one is viewing the developmentaloutcome or the processes that may haveled to this outcome. For example, the type<strong>of</strong> adjustment problem may be episodic, asin the case <strong>of</strong> depression, but the factorsthat led to this problem, such as problemsin emotional regulation, may continue evenafter the depression has remitted <strong>and</strong> couldplace the child at risk for other adjustmentproblems in the future. As such, depressionmay not appear stable, but the problems inemotional regulation are stable (Keenan,2000). Again, this illustrates the relevance <strong>of</strong>the process-oriented approach to assessmentthat is consistent with the developmentalpsychopathology framework. This frameworkillustrates the importance <strong>of</strong> not simplyassessing the developmental outcomes (e.g.,psychopathological conditions), but alsoassessing the various interacting processesthat lead to these outcomes.Situational StabilityExplicit in the developmental psychopathologicalperspective is a transactional view <strong>of</strong>behavior. That is, a child’s behavior influenceshis or her context <strong>and</strong> is also shapedby the context. As a result, one expects ahigh degree <strong>of</strong> situational variability inchildren’s behavior based on the differingdem<strong>and</strong>s present across situations.Providing some <strong>of</strong> the best data onthis issue, Achenbach, McConaughy, <strong>and</strong>Howell (1987) conducted a meta-analysis<strong>of</strong> 119 studies that reported correlationsbetween the reports <strong>of</strong> different informantson children’s <strong>and</strong> adolescents’ (ages1 1/2 to 19 years) emotional <strong>and</strong> behavioralfunctioning. The correlations between differenttypes <strong>of</strong> informants (e.g., parentteacher)were fairly low, averaging about0.28. This low correlation is not a goodindicator <strong>of</strong> cross-situational specificity byitself, because reduced correlations couldalso be due to the individual bias <strong>of</strong> differentinformants rather than to actual differencesin a child’s behavior across settings.However, the mean correlations between


62 CHAPTER 3 CLASSIFICATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGYinformants who typically observe the childin similar situations (between two parentsor two teachers) were generally muchhigher, averaging about 0.60. The relativelylow correlations (0.28 vs. 0.60) acrossthe informant types compared to thosewithin the informant types serve as a morerelevant indicator <strong>of</strong> the high variability inchildren’s behavior across settings.These findings by Achenbach et al.(1987) were replicated in a later meta-analysisby Renk <strong>and</strong> Phares (2004). Also, themodest correlation among ratings fromdifferent informants may not be limited toonly children or adolescents. For example,the average correlation between the adult’sratings <strong>of</strong> their own personality <strong>and</strong> theratings that their spouse make <strong>of</strong> themonly show an average correlation <strong>of</strong> 0.39(McCrae, Stone, Fagan, & Costa, 1998).Thus, in general, the correlation betweendifferent raters <strong>of</strong> a person’s personality<strong>and</strong> behavior across different informantsappears to be quite modest across the agerange. Achenbach et al. (1987) highlightseveral important implications <strong>of</strong> thesefindings to the assessment process. Theseare summarized in Box 3.3.In addition, there are several issues oncross-situational specificity that are analogousto those discussed on the stability<strong>of</strong> childhood behavior. First, like stability,the low correlations across situationsmay depend on the type <strong>of</strong> behavior beingassessed. Specifically, externalizing problemstend to show higher correlations acrossinformants than internalizing problems(Achenbach et al., 1987; Renk & Phares,2004). Second, the situational specificity<strong>of</strong> behavior may be a function <strong>of</strong> whetheror not aggregated domains <strong>of</strong> behaviorare studied, or whether discrete behaviorsare studied. For example, Biederman,Keenan, <strong>and</strong> Faroane (1990) compared thereports <strong>of</strong> both the parent <strong>and</strong> teacher onthe symptoms <strong>of</strong> attention deficit disorder(ADD). Individual symptoms showed anaverage correlation across home <strong>and</strong> schoolsettings <strong>of</strong> about 0.20. In contrast, on adiagnostic level there was a much higheragreement. There was a 90% probability<strong>of</strong> a teacher reporting enough symptoms toreach a diagnosis <strong>of</strong> ADD if the child wasdiagnosed by parents’ report. Similar tothe findings on stability, this suggests that,although individual behaviors (symptoms)may show a high level <strong>of</strong> specificity acrosssituations, the broader construct (diagnosis)<strong>of</strong> aggregated behaviors seems to showgreater consistency across situations.ComorbiditiesComorbidity is a medical term that refers tothe presence <strong>of</strong> two or more diseases thatoccur simultaneously in an individual. Thisterm has also been applied in the psychologicalliterature to denote the presence <strong>of</strong> twoor more disorders, or two or more problematicareas <strong>of</strong> adjustment co-occurring withinthe same individual. There can be severalreasons for comorbidity. For example,comorbidity can involve the co-occurrence<strong>of</strong> two independent disorders, two disordershaving a common underlying etiology,or two disorders having a causal relationbetween themselves (Kendall & Clarkin,1992). Unfortunately, research in most areas<strong>of</strong> psychology has not allowed for a cleardelineation <strong>of</strong> the various causes <strong>of</strong> comorbidityamong psychological problems.Despite this inadequate underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>of</strong> the causes <strong>of</strong> comorbidity, this conceptis important for the clinical assessment <strong>of</strong>children, for it is clear that comorbidityis the rule, rather than the exception, inchildren with psychological difficulties(Bird, Gould, & Staghezza, 1993). Specifically,children’s problems are rarelycircumscribed to a single problem area;instead, children tend to have problemsin multiple areas <strong>of</strong> adjustment. Forexample, in children with severe conductproblems, 50%–90% have a co-occurringADD, 62% have a co-occurring anxietydisorder, 25% have a learning disability,


CHAPTER 3 CLASSIFICATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY63Box 3.3Research Note: Meta-Analysis <strong>of</strong> Cross-Informant Correlations for <strong>Child</strong>/<strong>Adolescent</strong>Behavioral <strong>and</strong> Emotional ProblemsAs noted in the text, Achenbach et al. (1987)conducted a meta-analysis <strong>of</strong> 119 studiesthat reported correlations between differentinformants on children’s <strong>and</strong> adolescents’emotional <strong>and</strong> behavioral functioning. Themeta-analysis included studies that correlatedratings <strong>of</strong> parents, teachers, mental healthworkers, observers, peers, <strong>and</strong> self-reportratings. The overall findings suggested thatcorrelations between different types <strong>of</strong> informants(e.g., parents <strong>and</strong> teachers) are fairlylow, averaging about 0.28, indicating a highdegree <strong>of</strong> variability in the report <strong>of</strong> a child’semotional <strong>and</strong> behavioral functioning acrossdifferent types <strong>of</strong> informants. The correlationswere higher when calculated betweensimilar informants (e.g., between two parentsor between two teachers), averaging about0.60. This suggests that the low correlationsbetween different informants may be atleast partially due to differences in children’sbehavior in different settings, rather thanto idiosyncratic methods <strong>of</strong> rating behavioracross informants.The authors <strong>of</strong> this meta-analysis discussseveral important implications <strong>of</strong> their findingsto the clinical assessment <strong>of</strong> children.1. “The high correlations between pairs <strong>of</strong>informants who see children in similar settingssuggest that data from a single parent,teacher, observer, etc. would providea reasonable sample <strong>of</strong> what would be providedby other informants <strong>of</strong> the same typewho see the child under generally similarconditions” (p. 227).2. “In contrast, the low correlations betweendifferent types <strong>of</strong> informants suggest thateach type <strong>of</strong> informant provides substantiallyunique information that is notprovided by other informants. The highdegree <strong>of</strong> situational specificity poses aspecific challenge to clinical assessmentsintended to categorize disorders accordingto fixed rules” (p. 227), such as theDiagnostic <strong>and</strong> Statistical Manual <strong>of</strong> MentalDisorders-Fourth Edition, Revised (APA,1994). Such systems specify symptomsthat must be judged present or absent,<strong>and</strong> the low correlations across settingssuggest that in most cases the presence orabsence will depend on the setting.3. As a result <strong>of</strong> the high degree <strong>of</strong> specificity,clinical assessments <strong>of</strong> childrenshould obtain information from differentinformants who see the child in differentsettings. “In such assessments, disagreementbetween informants’ reports areas instructive as agreement because theycan highlight variations in judgments <strong>of</strong>a child’s functioning across situations” (p.228). In Chap. 15, we discuss the issuesinvolved in deciding how to interpretthese variations in reports <strong>of</strong> a child’s oradolescent’s emotional <strong>and</strong> behavioralfunctioning.Sources: Achenbach, T. M., McConaughy, S. H., & Howell, C. T. (1987). <strong>Child</strong>/adolescent behavioral <strong>and</strong>emotional problems: Implications <strong>of</strong> cross-informant correlations for situational specificity. Psychological Bulletin,101, 213–232.<strong>and</strong> 50% have substantial problems inpeer relationships (see Frick & O’Brien,1994; Strauss et al., 1988). Similar rates<strong>of</strong> comorbidity are found in many othertypes <strong>of</strong> child psychopathology.Research attempting to underst<strong>and</strong>comorbidity in childhood psychopathologyhas had a major impact on our underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>of</strong> the causes <strong>of</strong> several childhooddisorders (e.g., Hinshaw, 1987). However,comorbidity also has a more immediateimpact on the clinical assessment <strong>of</strong> children<strong>and</strong> adolescents. In a special issue<strong>of</strong> the Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong>


64 CHAPTER 3 CLASSIFICATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGYPsychology (Kendall & Clarkin, 1992), severalarticles highlighted the unique treatmentneeds <strong>of</strong> children with various types <strong>of</strong>comorbid psychopathological conditions.The high degree <strong>of</strong> comorbidity <strong>and</strong> itsimportance to the design <strong>of</strong> effectivetreatment programs makes the assessment<strong>of</strong> comorbid conditions crucial inthe clinical assessment <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong>adolescents.Because <strong>of</strong> the importance <strong>of</strong> comorbidity,most clinical assessments <strong>of</strong> children<strong>and</strong> adolescents should be comprehensive.Specifically, assessments must cover multipleareas <strong>of</strong> functioning so that, not onlyare the primary referral problems assessedadequately, but potential comorbid problemsin adjustment are also assessed. Ingeneral, the clinical assessment must bedesigned with a thorough underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>of</strong> the high degree <strong>of</strong> comorbidity presentin child psychopathology <strong>and</strong> the mostcommon patterns <strong>of</strong> comorbidity that arespecific to the referral question.Practical Implications for <strong>Assessment</strong>Although we have tried to summarizesome <strong>of</strong> the major findings in the field <strong>of</strong>developmental psychopathology that haveparticular relevance to clinical assessment,sometimes it is difficult to translate researchinto guidelines for practice. The followingis a summary <strong>of</strong> some <strong>of</strong> the major implications<strong>of</strong> the findings discussed in this sectionapplied to the clinical assessment <strong>of</strong>children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. These implicationsare exp<strong>and</strong>ed <strong>and</strong> applied to specificsituations throughout this book.1. A competent assessor needs to beknowledgeable in several areas <strong>of</strong>basic psychological research to competentlyassess children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.In addition to competence inmeasurement theory, knowledge <strong>of</strong>developmental processes <strong>and</strong> basiccharacteristics <strong>of</strong> childhood psychopathologyis also essential.2. <strong>Child</strong>ren’s behaviors <strong>and</strong> emotions mustbe understood within a developmentalcontext. Therefore, an important characteristic<strong>of</strong> assessment instrumentsfor children is their ability to providedevelopmentally sensitive normativecomparisons. On a more general level,appropriate interpretations <strong>of</strong> testscores, even if they are based on agespecificnorms, should be guided by aknowledge <strong>of</strong> developmental processes,<strong>and</strong> their effect on a child’s behavioral<strong>and</strong> emotional functioning.3. <strong>Child</strong>ren’s behavior is heavily dependenton the contexts in which the childis participating. Therefore, assessments<strong>of</strong> children must be based onmultiple sources <strong>of</strong> information thatassess a child’s functioning in multiplecontexts. In addition, an assessment <strong>of</strong>the relevant aspects <strong>of</strong> the many importantcontexts in which a child functions(e.g., at school, at home, with peers) iscrucial in underst<strong>and</strong>ing the variationsin a child’s behavior across settings.4. Most assessments <strong>of</strong> children mustbe comprehensive. This is necessarybecause <strong>of</strong> the need to adequately assessthe many important situational contextsthat influence a child’s adjustment.<strong>Child</strong>ren <strong>of</strong>ten exhibit problems inmultiple areas <strong>of</strong> functioning that spanemotional, behavioral, learning, <strong>and</strong>social domains. Effective treatmentsmust be based on the unique strengths<strong>and</strong> weaknesses <strong>of</strong> the child across thesemultiple psychological arenas.ConclusionsThe main theme <strong>of</strong> this chapter <strong>and</strong>, in fact,<strong>of</strong> this entire text, is that appropriate assessmentpractices are based on the knowledge<strong>of</strong> the basic characteristics <strong>of</strong> the phenomenabeing assessed. As a result, the competentassessor is knowledgeable not only intest administration but is also well versed


CHAPTER 3 CLASSIFICATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY65in psychometric theory, child development,<strong>and</strong> childhood psychopathology.<strong>Clinical</strong> assessment can be conceptualizedas a process <strong>of</strong> classification. Therefore,underst<strong>and</strong>ing the issues involved inclassifying psychological functioning isimportant. Formal classification systemsare needed to promote communicationbetween pr<strong>of</strong>essionals, to utilize researchfor underst<strong>and</strong>ing individual cases, <strong>and</strong> todocument the need for services. However,classification systems can also be quitedangerous if they are poor systems or ifthey are not used appropriately.Two models <strong>of</strong> classification have had agreat influence on our underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong>assessment <strong>of</strong> children’s emotional <strong>and</strong>behavioral functioning: clinically-derivedmedical models <strong>and</strong> statistically-derivedmultivariate models. Underst<strong>and</strong>ing thebasic assumptions <strong>of</strong> these approachesto classification, <strong>and</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>ing theadvantages <strong>and</strong> disadvantages <strong>of</strong> each areimportant for interpreting the assessmentinformation.Being knowledgeable about basicresearch within the field <strong>of</strong> developmentalpsychology is also crucial in conducting<strong>and</strong> interpreting psychological assessments<strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. This researchillustrates the importance <strong>of</strong> conducting<strong>and</strong> interpreting assessments within adevelopmental context, the importance <strong>of</strong>underst<strong>and</strong>ing the stability <strong>and</strong> situationalspecificity <strong>of</strong> children’s psychological functioning,<strong>and</strong> the importance <strong>of</strong> comorbidityin childhood psychopathology. Theseresearch findings have many practicalapplications to the assessment process;these applications are discussed throughoutthis text.Chapter Summary1. To be competent in the clinicalassessment <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents,one must be knowledgeable about thecurrent research base on emotional<strong>and</strong> behavioral disorders.2. Classification refers to a set <strong>of</strong> rulesthat delineates some levels or types <strong>of</strong>psychological functioning as pathological<strong>and</strong> places these significant areas <strong>of</strong>pathology into distinct categories oralong certain dimensions.3. Appropriately developed <strong>and</strong> competentlyused classification systems can aid incommunication among pr<strong>of</strong>essionals, inapplying research to clinical practice, <strong>and</strong>in documenting the need for services.4. Poor classification systems or inappropriatelyused classification systemscan foster an illusion <strong>of</strong> few differencesamong individuals within a givencategory, can foster an illusion <strong>of</strong> a clearbreak between normality <strong>and</strong> pathology,<strong>and</strong> can lead to stigmatization.5. Medical model approaches toclassification assume an underlyingdisease entity, <strong>and</strong> tend to classifypeople into distinct categories.6. Multivariate approaches baseclassification on patterns <strong>of</strong> behavioralcovariation <strong>and</strong> tend to classifybehavior along continuous dimensions,from normality to pathology.7. The Diagnostic <strong>and</strong> Statistical Manual<strong>of</strong> Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition TextRevision (APA, 2000) is one <strong>of</strong> the mostcommonly used classification systemswith characteristics <strong>of</strong> both medicalmodel <strong>and</strong> multivariate approaches.8. Developmental psychopathology providesa framework for underst<strong>and</strong>ing theadjustment <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.9. Based on this framework, assessmentsmust be conducted with a knowledge <strong>of</strong>age- specific patterns <strong>of</strong> behavior, witha knowledge <strong>of</strong> normal developmentalprocesses, <strong>and</strong> with consideration <strong>of</strong>issues regarding the stability <strong>of</strong> behaviorover time <strong>and</strong> across situations.10. Because research has shown that childrenwith problems in one area <strong>of</strong>


66 CHAPTER 3 CLASSIFICATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGYadjustment typically have problemsin multiple areas, most assessments<strong>of</strong> children need to cover multipledomains <strong>of</strong> functioning.11. Because research has shown that children’sbehaviors are heavily dependenton the contexts in which they occur,clinical assessments must assess achild across multiple contexts <strong>and</strong>assess the characteristics <strong>of</strong> the mostimportant contexts in which a childfunctions.


C h a p t e r 4St<strong>and</strong>ards <strong>and</strong> FairnessChapter Questionsl Are pr<strong>of</strong>essional guidelines availablefor the assessment <strong>of</strong> individuals fromdiverse cultural, linguistic, ethnic, economic<strong>and</strong> other backgrounds?l Why is it problematic to use the phrase,“valid test?”The majority <strong>of</strong> problems that occurin applied clinical assessment are duenot to inherent flaws in the tests, butto the inappropriate use <strong>of</strong> tests, <strong>and</strong>misinterpretation <strong>of</strong> their results, byclinicians (Anastasi, 1992). Test misuse isprimarily due to subst<strong>and</strong>ard practice byclinicians, just as most auto accidents arecaused by driver error <strong>and</strong> not by the carper se. Even a widely used <strong>and</strong> accepted testcan become a tool for disserving a client.And these cases <strong>of</strong> misuse are common,<strong>and</strong> include misuses ranging from incorrectscoring to interpretations <strong>of</strong> scores thathave not been shown to be valid by severalindependent research studies (Eydeet al., 1993). Consequently, psychologicalassessment practice has long been governedby peer-developed guidelines <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardsthat have proliferated <strong>and</strong> become moreexplicit <strong>and</strong> sophisticated as the fieldmatures (AERA, APA, NCME, 1999). Thischapter is devoted to providing an executivesummary <strong>of</strong> some <strong>of</strong> the major publicationsin this area, especially those developed byrelevant learned societies. It also providesguidance for practice based on some <strong>of</strong> themost widely cited ethical principles, testst<strong>and</strong>ards, regulations, <strong>and</strong> recent treatisesthat give suggestions for assessing diverseclientele. (A self examination for enhancingretention <strong>of</strong> these issues is given in Box 4.1.)67P.J. Frick et al., <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Behavior,DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-0641-0_4, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010


68 CHAPTER 4 STANDARDS AND FAIRNESSBox 4.1Ethics <strong>and</strong> St<strong>and</strong>ards Self-ExaminationChecklistPeriodic completion <strong>of</strong> this checklist mayserve as a quick reference for the clinician tocue adherence to optimal practice methods.Principle/Guideline Questions1. Do I have adequate training to use thetests/methods that I plan to use?2. How might the individual’s backgroundcultural,linguistic, social, economic, orotherwise-affect the planning <strong>of</strong> my evaluationor the interpretation <strong>of</strong> my results?3. Are the tests that I am using validated forthe specific purposes that I have in mind?4. Are there particularly unreliable scalesthat I should refrain from interpreting?5. Have I received informed consent <strong>and</strong>assent prior to initiating the evaluation?6. Will I provide feedback to the client or tothe others concerned, such as the child’sparents, teachers, or pediatrician?7. Did I adequately protect patient privacy?8. Do I have written permission to shareconfidential information with concernedparties?9. Whom do I need to assist with in thisexamination-a translator, patient, socialworker, community member, etc.?10. Have I consulted a pr<strong>of</strong>essional colleagueregarding questionable issues asneeded?Use <strong>and</strong> MisuseThere are many elements <strong>of</strong> competent testusage. According to one empirical study<strong>of</strong> test usage, there are exactly 86 <strong>of</strong> them(Eyde et al., 1993). These competenciesrange from “accepting responsibility forcompetent use <strong>of</strong> the test to “not makingphotocopies <strong>of</strong> copyrighted materials” to“restricting test administration to qualifiedpersonnel.” In their unique casebook, Eyde<strong>and</strong> her colleagues assembled 78 case studies<strong>of</strong> test “misuse,” accompanied by instructionalquestions <strong>and</strong> documentation <strong>of</strong> eachviolation <strong>of</strong> the 86 elements. This workrepresents a unique effort to document therelationship between clinician behavior <strong>and</strong>assessment practices that makes it a recommendedreading for students <strong>of</strong> assessment.Furthermore, the vignettes provide ampleevidence that guidelines <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ards arenecessary for promoting optimal assessmentpractices.APA Ethical Principles<strong>of</strong> PsychologistsVirtually every pr<strong>of</strong>essional organizationadopts some ethical responsibility for itsmembers. An initial step is the development<strong>and</strong> dissemination <strong>of</strong> ethical principlesfor the members <strong>of</strong> the organization.Many organizations also adjudicate ethicalcomplaints against members made by thepublic or others.Psychology has a long history <strong>of</strong> involvementin test development <strong>and</strong> assessmentpractice, resulting in the frequent use <strong>of</strong>the term psychometrics. Hence, the ethicalst<strong>and</strong>ards published by the American PsychologicalAssociation (APA) are amongthe most well known sets <strong>of</strong> ethical principlespromulgating st<strong>and</strong>ards for assessmentpractice. This section presents some <strong>of</strong> therelevant APA principles <strong>and</strong> provides sampleapplications <strong>of</strong> their use. These st<strong>and</strong>ardsprovide helpful guidance regardingthe restriction <strong>of</strong> test use from a pr<strong>of</strong>essionalcontext only, requirements for evidenceor scientifically-based test interpretation,restriction <strong>of</strong> test use to qualified persons,adherence to relevant testing guidelines<strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ards <strong>of</strong> practice, proper <strong>and</strong> fullexplanation <strong>of</strong> test results, <strong>and</strong> maintainingthe security <strong>of</strong> test items <strong>and</strong> other contentthat may make the test useless if released tothe general public.


CHAPTER 4 STANDARDS AND FAIRNESS69It can be challenging to maintain thesecurity <strong>of</strong> test content given the access torecords by many sources, <strong>and</strong> numerousrequests from parents, lawyers, or patientsthemselves to see the actual record formsused for the evaluation. Some internet sitesprovide sample items that are analogous toitems found in popular tests, such as theMMPI-2. However, for the most part, testitem content can be shielded from wouldbetest takers. When faced with questionsabout test security, it has commonly beenconsidered a good practice to a) explainthe problems associated with release ifitems have the ability to practice psychologicalassessment with others, or b) agreeto release test record forms only to otherqualified pr<strong>of</strong>essionals, to interpret themappropriately for the person making therequest. These two responses are an oversimplification<strong>of</strong> the various request types<strong>and</strong> potential responses, suggesting thatconsultation with colleagues will be consideredwise under these circumstances.Test St<strong>and</strong>ardseducational testing, employment testing,<strong>and</strong> program evaluation <strong>and</strong> public policy.These st<strong>and</strong>ards are well-articulated<strong>and</strong> thorough, which portends that theyare influential in court proceedings<strong>and</strong> forensic work, formation <strong>of</strong> publicpolicy, <strong>and</strong>, hopefully, in the assessmenttraining <strong>of</strong> psychologists <strong>and</strong> other users<strong>of</strong> tests (Kamphaus, 1998).One <strong>of</strong> the vital points presented by thetest st<strong>and</strong>ards is that it is incorrect to usethe phrase “the validity <strong>of</strong> the test,” becauseit cannot be concluded that a particular testis valid for all children under all assessmentsituations (AERA, APA, NCME, 1999). Thevalidity <strong>of</strong> a test should be gauged properlyin relation to every assessment situation inwhich it may be put to use. Clinicians whoassess children’s personalities must, therefore,learn how to use more than one personalitytest well in order to validly assesschildren, families, institutions, or systems.A small sampling <strong>of</strong> some <strong>of</strong> the importantaspects <strong>of</strong> psychological test use <strong>and</strong> validationare given in the following excerptsfrom the test st<strong>and</strong>ards.The St<strong>and</strong>ards for Educational <strong>and</strong> PsychologicalTesting (AERA, APA, NCME,1999) discuss in great detail the manyissues introduced by the ethical principles.In fact, this volume should bepart <strong>of</strong> every psychologist ’ s library, <strong>and</strong> itwould serve as a useful adjunct to this orany other textbook dealing with clinicalassessment.This latest version <strong>of</strong> the test st<strong>and</strong>ardsis ambitious, <strong>and</strong> includes chapterson validity, reliability, test development,scales <strong>and</strong> norms, test administration,scoring <strong>and</strong> reporting, supporting documentationfor tests, fairness, rights<strong>of</strong> test takers, testing individuals fromdiverse linguistic backgrounds, testingindividuals with disabilities, responsibilities<strong>of</strong> test users, psychological testing,Evidence-Based InterpretationGiven the scope <strong>and</strong> complexity <strong>of</strong> thest<strong>and</strong>ards, only a few <strong>of</strong> them can be summarizedhere. Of course, there is alwayssome loss <strong>of</strong> content when one summarizesan original source. Therefore, again, thereader is advised to read the original st<strong>and</strong>ards.Also, the Test St<strong>and</strong>ards should bea required study for all doctoral programsin psychology, as is currently the case forrelevant ethical st<strong>and</strong>ards.While it is understood that it is simplisticto say that a test is valid or not valid, itis, therefore, also incorrect to consider aparticular test interpretation to be validfor all children being evaluated. First, testdevelopers must present a rationale foreach intended interpretation for their test


70 CHAPTER 4 STANDARDS AND FAIRNESS(AERA, APA, NCME, 1999). This rationalemust include documentation <strong>of</strong> validityevidence <strong>and</strong> theory relevant to the interpretationin a “comprehensive summary”(St<strong>and</strong>ard 1.1; AERA, APA, NCME, 1999).In addition, although test developers arerequired to produce such evidence, individualpsychologists have to evaluate the quality<strong>of</strong> the evidence, as it relates to specificcircumstances.With regard to validity, the followingst<strong>and</strong>ards are also <strong>of</strong>fered (paraphrasedfrom AERA, APA, NCME, 1999).1. Uses <strong>and</strong> interpretations <strong>of</strong> test scores,intended populations <strong>of</strong> application,<strong>and</strong> the construct(s) assessed must beclearly stated by the test developer.(St<strong>and</strong>ard 1.2)2. Users should be cautioned not to interprettests in a manner inconsistent withthe available evidence. (St<strong>and</strong>ard 1.3)3. Psychologists must justify <strong>and</strong> collectevidence for an interpretation <strong>of</strong> a testthat is not justified. (St<strong>and</strong>ard 1.4)4. Validity <strong>of</strong> study samples should bedescribed in detail. (St<strong>and</strong>ard 1.5)5. Procedures for selecting the test contentshould be specified. (St<strong>and</strong>ard1.6)6. If experts are used in the test design,their roles <strong>and</strong> credentials should bedescribed in detail. (St<strong>and</strong>ard 1.7)7. A rationale should be <strong>of</strong>fered for interpretation<strong>of</strong> items or item subsets whensuch interpretations are advised by thedeveloper. (St<strong>and</strong>ard 1.10)8. The relationships among scores withina test should be supported with evidence.(St<strong>and</strong>ard 1.11)9. A rationale <strong>and</strong> scientific evidenceshould be provided for all interpretations<strong>of</strong> score differences <strong>and</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>iles.(St<strong>and</strong>ard 1.12)10. The conditions under which validityevidence was gathered should bedescribed. (St<strong>and</strong>ard 1.13)11. The relationship between a test <strong>and</strong>scores on other measures should be theoreticallyconsistent. (St<strong>and</strong>ard 1.14)12. When statistical adjustments, such asthose for restriction <strong>of</strong> range, are made,both adjusted <strong>and</strong> unadjusted valuesshould be given. (St<strong>and</strong>ard 1.18)13. If a test is used to recommend alternativetreatments, evidence <strong>of</strong> differentialtreatment outcomes should be providedwhen feasible. (St<strong>and</strong>ard 1.19)14. When unintended consequences<strong>of</strong> test use occur, testinvalidity should be ruled out as acause for such consequences. (St<strong>and</strong>ard1.24)With regard to psychologists, the followingst<strong>and</strong>ards are given.1. Psychologists should limit their assessmentpractice to the use <strong>of</strong> tests that arequalified by training. (St<strong>and</strong>ard 12.1)2. Psychologists should refrain from makingbiased interpretations that serve asspecial interests. (St<strong>and</strong>ard 12.2)3. Tests should be selected only if theyare suitable for the characteristics <strong>of</strong>the patient. (St<strong>and</strong>ard 12.3)4. Evidence must be provided if it is suggestedthat an interpretation can bemade based on combinations <strong>of</strong> testscores. (St<strong>and</strong>ard 12.4)5. Tests used in combination to make adiagnosis must show adequate validity(sensitivity <strong>and</strong> specificity), <strong>and</strong> thepsychologist must meet the user qualificationsrequired to interpret the testsinvolved. (St<strong>and</strong>ard 12.5)6. Psychologists should choose tests fordifferential diagnosis only if evidenceshows that the test can differentiatebetween clinical samples <strong>of</strong> interest,not just between a clinical sample<strong>and</strong> the general population. (St<strong>and</strong>ard12.6)


CHAPTER 4 STANDARDS AND FAIRNESS717. When a test is used to help make adiagnosis, the diagnostic category mustbe carefully defined. (St<strong>and</strong>ard 12.7)8. Psychologists must ensure that psychometristswho work under theirsupervision are adequately trained.(St<strong>and</strong>ard 12.8)9. Psychologists should describe the testingprocedures in a language underst<strong>and</strong>ableby the patient. (St<strong>and</strong>ard 12.10)10. Confidentiality <strong>of</strong> the results should bemaintained consistent with legal <strong>and</strong>ethical requirements. (St<strong>and</strong>ard 12.11)11. Psychologists should use a setting <strong>and</strong>equipment necessary to obtain validresults. If this setting is not available, thetest should also be administered underoptimal conditions when possible forcomparison purposes. (St<strong>and</strong>ard 12.12)12. Psychologists should be familiar withthe reliability <strong>and</strong> validity <strong>of</strong> evidence<strong>of</strong> each test they use, <strong>and</strong> they shouldprovide a logical <strong>and</strong> coherent analysis<strong>of</strong> the results that support their inferences.(St<strong>and</strong>ard 12.13)13. Qualitative information, such as backgroundinformation <strong>and</strong> observations,should be considered when making testinterpretations. (St<strong>and</strong>ard 12.14)14. The quality <strong>of</strong> actuarial or computerbasedinterpretations <strong>and</strong> the norms onwhich they are based should be evaluatedfor their quality. (St<strong>and</strong>ard 12.15)15. Psychologists should not imply that arelationship exists between test results<strong>and</strong> prognoses or treatment outcomesunless evidence is available for patientswho are similar to the patient beingevaluated. (St<strong>and</strong>ard 12.16)16. Interpretations that suggest how apatient will perform on other measuresor outcomes should be supported byevidence <strong>of</strong> criterion-related validity.(St<strong>and</strong>ard 12.17)17. Psychologists should base their interpretationson several sources <strong>of</strong> data,test results, or other evidence, <strong>and</strong>they should be cognizant <strong>of</strong> the theory,empirical evidence, <strong>and</strong> limitations <strong>of</strong>each test used. (St<strong>and</strong>ard 12.18)18. Construct irrelevant factors (e.g., motivation,response sets, health factors,suboptimal testing conditions) shouldbe considered as an alternative explanationfor a set <strong>of</strong> test results. (St<strong>and</strong>ard12.19)19. Normally, psychologists should discussthe results with the patient in a languagethat he or she can underst<strong>and</strong>.(St<strong>and</strong>ard 12.20)One reason for delineating these twosets <strong>of</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ards is to give the reader anappreciation <strong>of</strong> their scope, which is trulyimpressive. Awareness <strong>of</strong> the scope <strong>of</strong> thest<strong>and</strong>ards should alert psychologists that acompliance with these st<strong>and</strong>ards requiresdedicated effort <strong>and</strong> self monitoring.A few themes in these st<strong>and</strong>ards deserveelaboration. First, interpretation must beevidence-based <strong>and</strong>, when it is not, the factshould be made known to all consumers <strong>of</strong>the results. We suggest that psychologistsask themselves a few questions about a testinterpretation in order to encourage compliancewith the st<strong>and</strong>ards. If we conclude,for example, that a child has depression, ourself-monitoring questions could include:1. On what assessment results do we basethis interpretation?2. Do the structured interviews <strong>and</strong>self-report measures have adequateevidence <strong>of</strong> reliability, sensitivity, <strong>and</strong>specificity?3. Is this scientific evidence based onadolescent samples with similar demographics(e.g., sex, age, ethnicity, geographicregion, language, etc.)?4. Is this evidence based on studies thatuse clinical control samples in additionto comparisons with the generalpopulation?


72 CHAPTER 4 STANDARDS AND FAIRNESS5. Are the scales <strong>and</strong> interviews free <strong>of</strong>construct-irrelevant variance (e.g.,“threatens others,” which is not a coresymptom <strong>of</strong> depression but is an indicator<strong>of</strong> aggression)?6. Is there an evidence <strong>of</strong> constructunder-representation (e.g., scale doesnot include any “vegetative” symptoms<strong>of</strong> depression)?7. Are the procedures for developingdiagnostic interview <strong>and</strong> scale contentdescribed <strong>and</strong> are reasonable?8. Were the interview results, test findings,patient background/history, <strong>and</strong>other evidence integrated into a coherentrationale for the diagnosis?9. Could there be an alternate explanationfor the conclusions drawn by theconstruct-irrelevant variance (e.g.,child was coached on the content <strong>of</strong>some test items)?10. What is the definition <strong>of</strong> depression(e.g., DSM-IV diagnostic criteria)?11. Is a classification or diagnostic decisionmade only to receive insurance reimbursementor other remuneration,financial or otherwise?12. Have these results been shared with theclient in a manner that he or she can fullyunderst<strong>and</strong>?13. Has confidentiality <strong>of</strong> the clients’results been maintained?Of course, a different set <strong>of</strong> questions maybe posed for non-diagnostic interpretationssuch as prognosis, treatment, or programevaluation.The issue <strong>of</strong> confidentiality is worthadditional comment. Rights to privacycontinue to be threatened due to changesin health insurance practices <strong>and</strong> increasingaccess to electronically stored information,among other factors (Alderman &Kennedy, 1995).St<strong>and</strong>ards for Privacy <strong>of</strong> IndividuallyIdentifiable Health Information have beenissued by the U.S. Department <strong>of</strong> Health<strong>and</strong> Human Services (Federal Register:December 28, 2000, volume 65, number250). These st<strong>and</strong>ards are remarkable inat least three ways: (1) They specificallyinclude records <strong>of</strong> psychologists who provide“qualified psychologist services”; (2)Psychotherapy notes that are “separatedfrom the rest <strong>of</strong> the individual’s medicalrecord” are generally excluded fromrelease, including release to the patient; (3)A patient may be denied access to his orher medical record if“a licensed health care pr<strong>of</strong>essional has determined,in the exercise <strong>of</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>essionaljudgment, that the access requested is reasonablylikely to endanger the life or physicalsafety <strong>of</strong> the individual or another person.”(p. 82823)These medical record release st<strong>and</strong>ardsare, therefore, reflective <strong>of</strong> some <strong>of</strong> thevicissitudes <strong>of</strong> mental health care. Theexclusion <strong>of</strong> psychotherapy notes from themedical record shows a high regard for thesensitivity <strong>of</strong> such information. For thepurposes <strong>of</strong> this text, however, it shouldbe noted that psychotherapy notes are notde-fined to include the “results <strong>of</strong> clinicaltests,” “diagnosis,” <strong>and</strong> so on.With regard to the other aspects <strong>of</strong>the medical record that may include psychologicaltest <strong>and</strong> assessment results <strong>and</strong>interpretations, a psychologist may alsodeny access if the “protected health information”makes reference to another individualwho may suffer harm if informationis released. Finally, release may be deniedto the patient ’ s personal representative, ifthe provider thinks that harm may occur.Of course, a patient may ask for a review<strong>of</strong> any denial request <strong>and</strong> certain exclusions(e.g., criminal activities) from thisdenial provision are stipulated. However,these regulations appear to give psychologistssome discretion regarding the release<strong>of</strong> patient records.


CHAPTER 4 STANDARDS AND FAIRNESS73BIAS <strong>and</strong> Cultural CompetenceTest BiasThe perception that bias is inherent inpsychological tests has spurred many challenges<strong>and</strong> accusations (Kamphaus, 2001).A review <strong>of</strong> some <strong>of</strong> the major technicalissues follows.Mean Score DifferencesMost definitions <strong>of</strong> test bias do not usuallyconsider the issue <strong>of</strong> mean score differencesas a meaningful test <strong>of</strong> bias (Reynolds& Kaiser, 1990). Instead, importantquestions related to the validity <strong>of</strong> a personalitytest’s inferences across groupsforms a test <strong>of</strong> bias. In this approach tobias, an evidence <strong>of</strong> the construct validity fora personality test score inference differingacross groups exist. Numerous studieshave addressed these technical issues. Forthe purpose <strong>of</strong> this chapter, the definition<strong>of</strong> test bias <strong>of</strong>fered by Reynolds <strong>and</strong> Kaiseris most appropriate.“Test bias refers in a global sense to systematicerror in the estimation <strong>of</strong> some true value fora group <strong>of</strong> individuals. The key word hereis systematic; all measures contain error, butthis error is assumed to be r<strong>and</strong>om unlessshown to be otherwise.” (p. 624)Given this definition, one would expectscore differences to indicate genuine differencesin behavior or personality, if thetest works the same way (i.e., measures thesame constructs in a valid way) for variousgroups. An interesting finding with regardto ratings <strong>of</strong> child behavior is that fewmean differences exist between cultural<strong>and</strong> linguistic groups even for tests developedprimarily for use in the United States.Crijnen, Achenbach, <strong>and</strong> Verhulst (1997),for example, found remarkably small differencesbetween groups for parent ratingsusing the <strong>Child</strong> Behavior Checklist. Theystudied the results for 13,697 children <strong>and</strong>adolescents from 12 cultures includingChina, Israel, Sweden, German, Jamaica,<strong>and</strong> the United States. Their results have astriking similarity across culture, includingsimilarities in cross-sectional changes associatedwith age. They also noted that sexdifferences were invariant across cultures:With no significant exceptions, boys obtainedhigher externalizing scores but lowerinternalizing scores than girls. This genderdifference in the kinds <strong>of</strong> problems that parentsreport might thus be a “cultural universal”…(p. 1276).Several aspects <strong>of</strong> this study were corroboratedfor four cultural groups, for bothparent <strong>and</strong> teacher ratings in a study byKamphaus et al. (2000). This investigationevaluated the differences between U.S.Anglo, U.S. African American, U.S. Hispanic,<strong>and</strong> Colombian (Medellin) samplesfor both parent <strong>and</strong> teacher ratings fromthe BASC. Kamphaus et al. also found thatdifferences were small between groups forboth parent <strong>and</strong> teacher ratings, <strong>and</strong> sexdifferences were consistent regardless <strong>of</strong>cultural/linguistic groups.Taken together, these studies, <strong>and</strong> manyothers, revealed that differences amongracial, ethnic, cultural, <strong>and</strong> linguisticgroups for behavior problem measures arerelatively small, a trend that st<strong>and</strong>s in contrastto other realms <strong>of</strong> testing (i.e., intelligence<strong>and</strong> academic achievement) withlarge differences (Kamphaus, 2001). And,while sex differences are relatively smallfor cognitive measures (i.e., intelligence),they are greater for child behavior problemmeasures. While interesting, however,these studies <strong>of</strong> mean score differences donot serve as clear indices <strong>of</strong> bias.Content Validity BiasContent validity was one <strong>of</strong> the first areas<strong>of</strong> investigation <strong>of</strong> test bias. This searchfor bias is underst<strong>and</strong>able given that a


74 CHAPTER 4 STANDARDS AND FAIRNESSfrequent bias concern is usually directedat item content that seems inappropriateor, perhaps, even <strong>of</strong>fensive to a group <strong>of</strong>individuals. Again, a very helpful definition<strong>of</strong> content validity bias can be taken fromReynolds <strong>and</strong> Kaiser (1990):“An item or subscale <strong>of</strong> a test is considered tobe biased in content when it is demonstratedto be relatively more difficult for members<strong>of</strong> one group than for members <strong>of</strong> another ina situation where the general ability level <strong>of</strong>the groups being compared is held constant<strong>and</strong> no reasonable theoretical rationale existsto explain group differences on the item (orsubscale) in question.” (p. 625)Numerous procedures have been proposedfor assessing bias in individual items, butthe logic behind item bias detection techniquesis fairly simple (Kamphaus, 2001).The fundamental aspect <strong>of</strong> most statisticalmethods that assess bias across cultural orgender groups is to match the groups onan overall score level, which is the first stepin the procedure. If, for example, one waslooking for gender bias in a pool <strong>of</strong> personalitytest items, one would first match boys<strong>and</strong> girls on their overall test score, be itst<strong>and</strong>ard or raw score. So, if one wanted toevaluate biased items in the MMPI-A, forexample, one would first statistically groupthe cases, with perhaps all the boys <strong>and</strong>girls with f-scores above 90 on a particularscale as one group, those between 80 <strong>and</strong>89 as another group, those between 70 <strong>and</strong>79 as a third group, <strong>and</strong> so on (it should benoted, however, that this is not the exactprocedure used by most item bias techniquesbut an oversimplification <strong>of</strong> suchprocedures). Subsequently, some statisticaltest <strong>of</strong> significance is applied to checkif, within these various score groups, thereare still significant differences in responseto the items <strong>of</strong> one group or another.This discussion relates to another itembias detection technique: judgmental biasreviews. The procedure used by somepublishers is to have groups <strong>of</strong> individualsreview the items carefully. This procedureensures that members <strong>of</strong> a number <strong>of</strong> culturalgroups review the items to determinenot only the potential bias, but also theitems that may be inappropriate for variouscultural groups. There is, however, muchdisagreement between judgmental reviews<strong>of</strong> items <strong>and</strong> statistical analyses <strong>of</strong> bias. Itappears that statistical analyses <strong>of</strong> bias aremore reliable (Reynolds <strong>and</strong> Kaiser, 1990).In an investigation <strong>of</strong> judgmental biasreviews for intelligence test items, S<strong>and</strong>oval<strong>and</strong> Mille (1979) compared the ratings <strong>of</strong>45 WISC-R items by 38 African American,22 Mexican American, <strong>and</strong> 40 undergraduatestudents. This study found that minority<strong>and</strong> non-minority judges did not differin their ability to identify the culturallybiased items. The conclusions <strong>of</strong> S<strong>and</strong>oval<strong>and</strong> Mille were that: (1) Judges are not ableto detect items that are more difficult for aminority child than for a Caucasian child,<strong>and</strong> (2) the item selection for minority childrenby judges <strong>of</strong> ethnic background did notshow any difference.Item bias, however, may be subtle <strong>and</strong>difficult to detect. Canino <strong>and</strong> Bravo (1999)cite an example <strong>of</strong> a problem with contentequivalence on the Diagnostic InterviewSchedule for <strong>Child</strong>ren (DISC; Shafferet al., 2000). They found that, initially, itwas difficult to translate items regardingseasonal depression into Spanish. Later itwas found that even successful translation<strong>of</strong> the symptoms was <strong>of</strong> no value becauseseasonal depression never occurred for thechildren in sunny Puerto Rico.Construct Validity BiasA workable definition <strong>of</strong> construct validitybias by Reynolds <strong>and</strong> Kaiser (1990) reads as:“Bias exists in regard to construct validitywhen a test is shown to measure differenthypothetical traits (psychological constructs)for one group or another, or to measure thesame trait but with differing degrees <strong>of</strong> accuracy”(p. 632).


CHAPTER 4 STANDARDS AND FAIRNESS75The most popular method used for thestudy <strong>of</strong> construct validity bias is factoranalysis. Numerous researchers have usedsimilar procedures. The central characteristic<strong>of</strong> these procedures is to conductfactor analyses separately for various cultural<strong>and</strong> gender groups, <strong>and</strong> determineif a similar factor structure is yielded foreach group. The most popular procedurefor assessing agreement between factorstructures across groups is a coefficient <strong>of</strong>congruence, which is interpreted similar toa correlation coefficient.Lachar <strong>and</strong> Gruber (1994) provide anexample <strong>of</strong> this method for the <strong>Personality</strong>Inventory for Youth (see Chap. 6). Theyconducted factor analyses separately by gender<strong>and</strong> ethnicity, <strong>and</strong> then compared the factorsyielded separately for the groups. Theirfindings were similar to those for abilitytests (Kamphaus, 2001) in that correlationsbetween the obtained factors were uniformlyhigh, in the low 0.90 at their worst.Predictive Validity BiasThe final type <strong>of</strong> bias that has received agreat deal <strong>of</strong> attention is predictive validitybias. A working definition <strong>of</strong> predictivevalidity bias is“A test is considered biased with respect topredictive validity if the inference drawnfrom the test score is not made with thesmallest feasible r<strong>and</strong>om error or if there isconstant error in an inference for predictionas a function <strong>of</strong> membership in a particulargroup”. (p. 638) (Reynolds & Kaiser, 1990)The issue <strong>of</strong> predictive or criterion-relatedvalidity is that these coefficients should notdiffer significantly across cultural or gendergroups. One <strong>of</strong> the typical procedures in thisresearch literature is to compare the predictivevalidity coefficients across groups. Astudy might compare the ability <strong>of</strong> a depressionmeasure to predict future adjustmentfor various groups, for example.If different predictive validity coefficientswere obtained for two or more groups, theresults would be called slope bias. In order tounderst<strong>and</strong> the concept <strong>of</strong> slope bias, it ishelpful to recall how correlation coefficientsare learned in introductory statistics courses.Such procedures are typically taught by havingthe students collect data on two variables<strong>and</strong> plot the scores <strong>of</strong> a group <strong>of</strong> individualson these two variables. This plot results in ascatter plot. Then students compute a correlationcoefficient <strong>and</strong> draw a line <strong>of</strong> bestfit through the scatter plot. This line <strong>of</strong> bestfit is a visual representation <strong>of</strong> the slope. Acorrelation coefficient (predictive validitycoefficient) <strong>of</strong> 0.90 would produce a slopethat is very different from that obtainedwith a correlation coefficient <strong>of</strong> 0.30. Consequently,this form <strong>of</strong> bias in prediction is<strong>of</strong>ten referred to as slope bias.Summary Comments on BiasWhile psychometric evidence <strong>of</strong> test biascan be found, little compelling evidence<strong>of</strong> bias is found for various groups residingin the United States (Figueroa, 1990). As aresult, the focus has now changed to implicatetest misuse as the major contributingfactor to improper assessment <strong>of</strong> individual<strong>and</strong> groups <strong>of</strong> children. This misuse, however,includes more than individuals. Government,school district, or other entitiesmay, for example, create unwise policiesthat inadvertently produce biased <strong>and</strong> untowardoutcomes for children, such as imposingstrict cut-<strong>of</strong>f scores that affect assessment<strong>and</strong> conceptualization <strong>of</strong> the case.FairnessThe term fairness refers to “… the principlethat every test taker should be assessed inan equitable way” (AERA, APA, NCME,1999, p. 175). Some issues related to achievingthis objective are discussed in this section.The renewed focus on test use comes


76 CHAPTER 4 STANDARDS AND FAIRNESSat a time when psychologists are seekingto improve test use for various linguistic<strong>and</strong> cultural groups. Cultural plurality hasposed a challenge to assessment <strong>and</strong> diagnosticpractice since the early days <strong>of</strong> themental tests. The testing movement wasforced early on to change tests <strong>and</strong> testingpractice in the United States because <strong>of</strong>the tremendous influx <strong>of</strong> new immigrants.Between 1901 <strong>and</strong> 1910, over nine millionimmigrants entered the United States-moreimmigrants than the combined populations<strong>of</strong> New York, Maryl<strong>and</strong>, <strong>and</strong> New Hampshirein 1900 (French & Hale, 1990). Onecomponent <strong>of</strong> the initial appearance <strong>of</strong> theWechsler scales as an alternative to the Stanford-Binetmonopoly <strong>of</strong> the time was the factthat Wechsler included a performance scale thatcould be used with some success with non-English speakers (Kamphaus, 2001).Little has changed since the days <strong>of</strong> massmigration to the United States. In manyways, psychologists have used the same strategiesfor dealing with clients from diversecultures. A popular approach involves adaptingexisting assessment instruments. TheThompson adaptation <strong>of</strong> the TAT for adults<strong>of</strong> African American heritage during the1930s is one <strong>of</strong> the early examples <strong>of</strong> suchattempts. Psychologists with multi-culturalexpertise should be able to adequately assessthe needs <strong>of</strong> a child from a culture that maydiffer from their own, even if test instrumentsthat are not specifically designed forthe child ’ s culture are the only ones available.A study by Malgady <strong>and</strong> Costantino(1998) highlighted the need for developingnew cultural competencies. They evaluatedthe effects <strong>of</strong> language <strong>and</strong> ethnicity<strong>of</strong> the clinician on diagnostic decision making,using Spanish-dominant adult patients<strong>of</strong> Puerto Rican <strong>and</strong> Dominican descent.These patients were then seen by boardcertifiedpsychiatrists <strong>and</strong> clinical psychologistsfor diagnostic interviews. Patients werematched on DSM-IV diagnosis <strong>and</strong> dividedinto four interview language <strong>and</strong> clinicianassessment groups: English only/non-Hispanic clinician; Spanish only/Hispanicclinician; English only/Hispanic clinician;Bilingual/Hispanic clinician. Several <strong>of</strong> theMalgady <strong>and</strong> Costantino’s results are noteworthy.First, they found no differences inthe diagnoses or symptoms between psychiatrists<strong>and</strong> psychologists. Symptom severitywas highest among patients interviewed inSpanish by a bilingual interviewer. Symptomseverity was rated lowest when a patient wasinterviewed in English by an Anglo clinician.While concern has been expressed that a clinicianwho does not share ethnicity <strong>and</strong> languagewith a patient will pathologize (Cohen& Kasen, 1999), these results suggest that itis also possible that such a mismatch betweenpatient <strong>and</strong> clinician could lead to failure inidentifying psychopathology, which couldexacerbate the symptoms <strong>of</strong> those deniedaccess to treatment. Either way, researchdoes suggest a need for broader training <strong>of</strong>clinicians in multi-cultural competencies.Emic Versus Etic PerspectivesAn emic perspective refers to behavior thatis considered specific to a culture, whereasan etic perspective presupposes that much <strong>of</strong>the behavior <strong>and</strong> laws <strong>of</strong> psychology areapplicable cross-culturally. Anastasi (1992)proposes that both perspectives are validby theorizing that learned behavior maybe culture-specific (emit) but that the “laws<strong>of</strong> learning” apply cross-culturally (etic).She hypothesizes further that hierarchicalmodels <strong>of</strong> personality may be most usefulin studying their behavior, as is the casefor studies on intelligence when supportingevidence can be found for a “g” factor<strong>and</strong> for specific traits (e.g., spatial ability)at lower levels <strong>of</strong> the hierarchy. There is,for example, evidence that several temperamenttraits can be identified cross-culturally(Martin, 1988).Inappropriate, ill-informed, or insensitiveinterpretations may also be made <strong>of</strong>


CHAPTER 4 STANDARDS AND FAIRNESS77the “clinical” data. An examiner may concludethat a 13-year-old girl <strong>of</strong> Asian heritageis socially introvert, shy, <strong>and</strong> perhapsin need <strong>of</strong> assertive training because <strong>of</strong> herbehavior during an interview with a maleclinician. She may have been demure <strong>and</strong>made no eye contact. The examiner maydraw such a conclusion despite the factthat she appeared friendly <strong>and</strong> outgoingwhen she was observed on the school playground<strong>and</strong> seemed to interact openly withher family members. This client may not,in fact, be pathologically shy; rather, shemay be adhering to a prohibition againstmaking eye contact with a male because<strong>of</strong> cultural values that suggest that this isa sexually seductive behavior (or an indication<strong>of</strong> a lack <strong>of</strong> respect) that is deemedinappropriate for her (Hasegawa, 1989). Inthis case, the clinician was simply ignoringrelevant data, <strong>and</strong> the clinician ’ s lack <strong>of</strong>familiarity with the child’s culture resultedin an erroneous interpretation.The clinician, however, must alsoremember the importance <strong>of</strong> individualizinginterpretation. Within a culturalgroup, variability can be substantial (Zuckerman,1990). It may be assumed by somethat Vietnamese <strong>and</strong> Chinese children havesimilar values due to early Chinese domination<strong>and</strong> the inculcation <strong>of</strong> Vietnameseculture with Confucian ethics. There havealso been other influences on this culturethat may affect a child ’ s behavior, includingEuropean Roman Catholicism, brought bythe French conquest <strong>of</strong> 1958, the influence<strong>of</strong> U.S. culture from the Vietnam War,<strong>and</strong> Buddhist influences from neighboringCambodia (Huang, 1989). Classifyingchildren by race, culture, or languagebackground is an appealing approach forresearchers <strong>and</strong> clinicians alike that isfraught with errors, primarily due to thetendency to overgeneralize a particulargroup <strong>of</strong> people (Zuckerman, 1990).Inclan <strong>and</strong> Herron (1985) cite the “culture<strong>of</strong> poverty” as another subculture thatmay affect a variety <strong>of</strong> groups. This “culture”is formed by a clash between those who haveachieved material wealth <strong>and</strong> prosperity <strong>and</strong>those who struggle to achieve economic paritywith little hope <strong>of</strong> doing so. <strong>Child</strong>renreared in a culture <strong>of</strong> poverty possess identifiablecharacteristics: an orientation to presenttime, inability to delay gratification, impulsivity,sense <strong>of</strong> predetermined fate, resentment<strong>of</strong> authority, alienation <strong>and</strong> distrust <strong>of</strong> others,<strong>and</strong> lack <strong>of</strong> emphasis on rigor, discipline,<strong>and</strong> perseverance (Inclan & Herron, 1989).They note that some impoverished parents<strong>of</strong> adolescents may be assessed by a therapistas being too rigid <strong>and</strong> controlling theiryoungsters at a time when parents shouldbe giving their children more freedom. It ispossible, however, that poor parents may beall too familiar with the culture <strong>of</strong> poverty<strong>and</strong> may be seeking control, not for its ownsake, but rather to ensure that their child oradolescent does not fall prey to the negativeconsequences <strong>of</strong> the behavior associated withthat culture (Inclan & Herron, 1989).These examples demonstrate the needfor clinicians to develop an enlarged knowledgebase in order to deal effectively withtheir referral population. Just as cliniciansneed to have knowledge <strong>of</strong> behavioral principles,psychometrics, child development,child psychopathology, <strong>and</strong> physiologicalpsychology to conduct an evaluation competently,it is increasingly clear that theymust know the history, culture, <strong>and</strong> language<strong>of</strong> their community extremely wellin order to not use assessment proceduresinappropriately, <strong>and</strong> to avoid making naïve<strong>and</strong> inappropriate interpretations.Guidelines for Assessing<strong>Child</strong>ren from DiverseBackgroundsResources for assessing children fromdiverse groups are now more readily available(e.g., Dana, 1999; Geisinger, 1992).Two developments that can assist practitionersare (1) the availability <strong>of</strong> guidelines


78 CHAPTER 4 STANDARDS AND FAIRNESSfrom blue ribbon panels <strong>and</strong> committees<strong>and</strong> (2) the increasing availability <strong>of</strong> formalmeasures <strong>of</strong> acculturation.Numerous sets <strong>of</strong> guidelines providespecific advice for the psychologist who isunsure <strong>of</strong> what procedures to use in questionablesituations. The Guidelines for Providers <strong>of</strong>Psychological Services to Ethnic, Linguistic, <strong>and</strong>Culturally Diverse Populations give specific<strong>and</strong> helpful advice to the clinician seeking tocarry out a competent evaluation <strong>of</strong> a childfor whom cultural/social/linguistic issuesloom large (see www.apa.org).A good example <strong>of</strong> the guidance to begained from such publications deals withthe frequently occurring situation <strong>of</strong> a languagedifference between the examiner <strong>and</strong>the child or other family members. Guideline6a suggests that a cascade <strong>of</strong> threeoptions applies to the examiner faced withsuch a case: (1) Refer the child to a clinicianwho can communicate in the client’spreferred language; (2) if this is not possible,use a translator who also possessespr<strong>of</strong>essional training; <strong>and</strong>, lastly, (3) oneis advised to use a parapr<strong>of</strong>essional fromthe community to translate. Moreover, thenext guideline, 6b, highlights the potentialthreat to validity <strong>of</strong> using a translator whohas a dual relationship with the client (e.g.,a gr<strong>and</strong>parent).Assessing AcculturationThe previously discussed guidelines forconsidering cultural <strong>and</strong> linguistic issueshint at the need to more carefully assessan individual’s level <strong>of</strong> adoption <strong>of</strong> theso-called “dominant culture,” which, <strong>of</strong>course, could change from one neighborhoodto the other. The guidelines indicatethe need to collect information, such as thenumber <strong>of</strong> generations <strong>of</strong> residence withinthe dominant culture, number <strong>of</strong> years<strong>of</strong> residence, dominant language fluency,community resources, <strong>and</strong> so on. This datacollection is an informal means <strong>of</strong> assessinglevel <strong>of</strong> acculturation. There are, however,more formal (some are quantifiable) methodsfor assessing acculturation. In fact, ithas been argued that the ready availability<strong>of</strong> such measures warrants their routine usein assessment practice (Geisinger, 1992).Marin (1992) defines the constructsrelevant to assessing ethnic identity <strong>and</strong>acculturation. He cites three components<strong>of</strong> ethnic identity: (1) “birth <strong>and</strong> gestationalhistory, (2) culture-specific behaviors <strong>and</strong>practices (e.g., language), <strong>and</strong> (3) culturespecificattitudes that include adherenceto a culture’s values <strong>and</strong> norms as well asin-group <strong>and</strong> out-group attitudes” (p. 236).The process <strong>of</strong> acculturation is defined as“… changes in individuals that are producedby contact with one or more culturalgroups” (p. 237). Several instruments arenow available for assessing the ethnic identification<strong>of</strong> individuals <strong>and</strong> the degree towhich acculturation has taken place.Dana (1993) provides a detailed compendium<strong>of</strong> measures <strong>of</strong> acculturation <strong>and</strong>identification with a particular culture.Some <strong>of</strong> these scales are listed below.African American MeasuresDevelopmental Inventory <strong>of</strong> Black Consciousness(DIB-C; Milliones, 1980)Racial Identity Attitude Scale (RIAS;Helms, 1986)African Self-Consciousness Scale (ASC;Baldwin & Bell, 1985)Asian American MeasuresEthnic Identity Questionnaire (EIQ;Masuda, Matsumoto, & Meredith, 1970)Hispanic American MeasuresAcculturation Rating Scale for MexicanAmericans (ARSMA; Cuellar, Harris, &Jasso, 1980)<strong>Child</strong>ren’s Acculturation Scale (Franco,1983)


CHAPTER 4 STANDARDS AND FAIRNESS79Cuban Behavioral Identity Questionnaire(CBIQ; Garcia & Lega, 1979)Hispanic Acculturation Scale (HAC;Marin et al., 1987)<strong>Child</strong>ren ’ s Hispanic Background Scale(CHBS; Martinez, Norman, & Delaney,1984)Cultural Life Style Inventory (Mendoza,1989)Multi-dimensional Scale <strong>of</strong> CulturalDifferences (MSCD; Olmedo, Martinez,& Martinez, 1978)Multi-cultural Experience Inventory(MEI; Ramirez, 1984)Behavioral Acculturation Scale (BAS;Szapocznik, Scopetta, & Aranalde, 1978)Bicultural Involvement Questionnaire(BIQ; Szapocznik & Kurtines, 1980)ConclusionsThe child psychologist <strong>of</strong> today has tobecome steeped in various ethical, legal,pr<strong>of</strong>essional, language <strong>and</strong> cultural issues, <strong>and</strong>st<strong>and</strong>ards <strong>of</strong> practice that face the pr<strong>of</strong>ession.It is necessary for the practitioner to seekthis knowledge through experiences duringgraduate school <strong>and</strong> beyond. Continuouspr<strong>of</strong>essional development is especiallyimportant in order to achieve fairness in theassessment process. Knowledge <strong>of</strong> cultural,linguistic, technology change, <strong>and</strong> othereffects on assessment remains in its infancy,thus portending considerable change in thefuture.<strong>and</strong> appropriate use <strong>of</strong> assessments <strong>and</strong>interventions, limiting use <strong>of</strong> psychologicaltests to qualified pr<strong>of</strong>essionals, <strong>and</strong>maintenance <strong>of</strong> test security.2. The St<strong>and</strong>ards for Educational <strong>and</strong> PsychologicalTesting (AERA, APA, NCME, 1999)provide a rich resource for maintainingst<strong>and</strong>ards <strong>of</strong> practice.3. While psychometric evidence <strong>of</strong> content-,construct-, or criterion-relatedvalidity test bias can be found, littlecompelling evidence <strong>of</strong> bias is found forvarious groups residing in the UnitedStates. As a result, the focus has nowchanged to implicate test misuse as themajor contributing factor in improperassessment <strong>of</strong> children.4. The renewed focus on test use comes ata time when psychologists are seekingto improve test use for various cultural<strong>and</strong> linguistic groups.5. The emic perspective refers to behaviorthat is thought to be specific to a culture,whereas the etic perceptive presupposesthe behavior theory <strong>and</strong> laws<strong>of</strong> psychology that are applicable crossculturally.6. The Guidelines for Providers <strong>of</strong> PsychologicalServices to Ethnic, Linguistic, <strong>and</strong>Culturally Diverse Populations give specific<strong>and</strong> helpful advice to the clinicianseeking to carry out a competentevaluation <strong>of</strong> a child for whom cultural/social linguistic issues provide threatsto test validity.7. There are now formal <strong>and</strong> quantifiablemethods for assessing acculturation.Chapter SummarySome <strong>of</strong> the APA ethics principles <strong>of</strong> relevanceto assessment include1. Evaluation, diagnosis, or interventionin a pr<strong>of</strong>essional context, competence


C h a p t e r 5Planning the Evaluation<strong>and</strong> Rapport BuildingChapter Questionsl Why is it important to carefully plan anevaluation?l What information is necessary for planninga focused clinical assessment?l What are some <strong>of</strong> the important considerationsin determining whether or not achild should be tested <strong>and</strong> who should dothe testing?l What is a scientific approach to testing?l What is rapport <strong>and</strong> why is it more difficultto develop in the clinical assessment<strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents than inmany other clinical endeavors?l How can informed consent be considereda rapport-building strategy?l What are some <strong>of</strong> the important strategiesthat can aid in developing rapportwith children <strong>and</strong> adolescents?Non-specifics in <strong>Clinical</strong><strong>Assessment</strong>A recurrent theme in this text is thatan assessor needs to have knowledge <strong>of</strong>several areas <strong>of</strong> basic research to appropriatelyselect <strong>and</strong> interpret psychologicaltests for children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. Inthis chapter, we consider another area<strong>of</strong> competence crucial to clinical assessmentthat goes beyond knowing howto administer specific tests. It is ratherdifficult to discuss this competence inobjective terms because it relates to difficulttopics for research <strong>and</strong>, as a result,there is only limited objective data toguide this practice. Instead, much inthis chapter is guided by clinical experience,not just our own experience,but the experience <strong>of</strong> other practicing81P.J. Frick et al., <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Behavior,DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-0641-0_5, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010


82 chapter 5 Planning the Evaluation <strong>and</strong> Rapport Buildingpsychologists who have written in thisarea.This chapter deals with setting anappropriate context in which testingtakes place. This is not simply the physicalcontext <strong>of</strong> testing, but the activities<strong>of</strong> the assessor that allow the clinicalassessment to achieve its goals. Many<strong>of</strong> the issues discussed involve clinicalskills that are difficult to teach, but <strong>of</strong>tenrequire refinement based on practicalexperience in testing children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.However, an analogy can bemade with the literature on psychotherapy.Many useful guides for practicingclinicians have been published that dealwith the non-specifics <strong>of</strong> psychotherapy.The term non-specifics has been usedto refer to several contextual factors,within which the psychotherapy techniquestake place, such as the relationshipbetween therapist <strong>and</strong> client or theprocess by which a therapist engages aclient in a therapeutic setting (Karver,H<strong>and</strong>lesman, Fields, & Bickman, 2006).In this chapter, we attempt to deal withthe non-specifics in the clinical assessment<strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.One critical component <strong>of</strong> setting anappropriate context for an evaluation iscareful planning. In the following section,we discuss a basic framework fordesigning clinical assessments for children<strong>and</strong> adolescents. Within this basicframework, however, evaluations mustbe tailored to the needs <strong>of</strong> the individualcase. The critical developmental issues,the most relevant areas <strong>of</strong> adjustment tobe assessed, <strong>and</strong> the most important elements<strong>of</strong> a child’s or adolescent’s environmentwill all vary from case to case.As a result, it is inappropriate to developspecific guidelines for designing evaluations.Instead, in this section we attemptto provide a framework for designingassessments that can be tailored to mostassessment situations.Clarifying the ReferralQuestionA crucial part <strong>of</strong> planning any evaluationis having enough information,prior to beginning the testing, to makeat least some initial decisions on thestructure <strong>and</strong> content <strong>of</strong> the assessmentprocess. This is not to say that the assessmentprocess should be so structuredfrom the outset that changes after testingis underway are not possible. However,obtaining crucial information before thefirst testing session enhances the likelihoodthat one will provide a focused <strong>and</strong> appropriateassessment. Almost every testingagency, whether clinic, school, hospital, orprivate practitioner, has some establishedintake process that provides preliminaryinformation on the child or adolescentbeing tested. There is no single best way tostructure the intake procedure. However,there are some pieces <strong>of</strong> information thatshould be obtained routinely in any intakeprocess, in addition to any basic information(e.g., name, address, phone number,insurance coverage) that is required by theagency.Purpose <strong>of</strong> TestingThe most important piece <strong>of</strong> intake informationfor planning an evaluation is theintended purpose <strong>of</strong> the evaluation. Amajor flaw in many clinical assessments isa lack <strong>of</strong> focus. From the outset, an evaluationshould have clearly specified goals<strong>and</strong> objectives. As discussed previously, itis erroneous to think in terms <strong>of</strong> an assessmenttechnique or battery being valid orinvalid. Results <strong>of</strong> the evaluation can bevalid for specific interpretations. Therefore,what interpretations one anticipates makingat the end <strong>of</strong> the evaluation shouldguide the selection <strong>of</strong> tests for the assess-


chapter 5 Planning the Evaluation <strong>and</strong> Rapport Building83ment battery. For example, if an assessmentis primarily intended to determine schoolplacement, then the focus <strong>of</strong> the evaluationwill not be to determine whether ornot a psychiatric diagnosis is warranted,but to determine whether or not the childmeets the eligibility requirements <strong>of</strong> theschool system. The assessor may feelthat more information is needed to makeappropriate recommendations to meet achild’s psychological <strong>and</strong> educational needsthan is required by these criteria. However,enough information to determine the eligibilityshould be part <strong>of</strong> the assessment,if this is the primary referral question.In our experience, it is not uncommonfor an otherwise sound <strong>and</strong> competentlyconducted evaluation to be useless forthe specific purposes for which a childwas referred.There are many other examples showinghow the intended use <strong>of</strong> the assessmentinformation determines the measures tobe used. This may be as broad as definingwhat areas need to be covered for acertain purpose (e.g., some residentialtreatment centers require a personalityassessment prior to acceptance) to as specificas requiring certain tests (e.g., someschool systems require specific tests to begiven for special education placement).The assessor should not give a test that,in his or her pr<strong>of</strong>essional judgment, isinappropriate for a particular use or isinappropriate for a particular client.However, if, at the time <strong>of</strong> referral, theintended use <strong>of</strong> the assessment is clarified<strong>and</strong> there is some question as to howappropriate certain requirements are fora given case, the assessor can attempt toaddress these issues before beginning theevaluation.Often the person or agency referring achild or adolescent for testing is not surehow the test results will be used. Instead,the child is referred because the agency isunsure <strong>of</strong> the nature <strong>of</strong> a child ’ s problem (oreven whether there is a problem), <strong>and</strong> thereferrer is unsure <strong>of</strong> what can be done tohelp the child. There are many variationson this theme, but, in essence, the goal <strong>of</strong>the assessment is to diagnose the source <strong>of</strong>a child ’ s difficulty <strong>and</strong> to make treatmentrecommendations based on this diagnosis.In Chap. 3, we discussed many importantissues in making diagnostic decisions.However, Martin (1988) provides a succinct<strong>and</strong> practical analysis <strong>of</strong> the specificgoals involved in diagnosis. These areto (1) predict future behavior, (2) differentiatebetween abnormal <strong>and</strong> normalbehavior, (3) make differential diagnoses,<strong>and</strong> (4) delineate individual differencesin competencies <strong>and</strong> disabilities. Martinalso provides some interesting recommendationsfor planning the evaluationto maximize the reliability <strong>of</strong> the diagnosticprocess. These are summarized inBox 5.1.Description <strong>of</strong> ReferralProblemsIn addition to underst<strong>and</strong>ing the purpose<strong>of</strong> the testing referral, it is also importantto obtain an initial description <strong>of</strong> the difficultiesthat a child is experiencing thatled to the referral. One <strong>of</strong> the reasons thatclinical assessments are so fascinating isthat, if done right, the assessment is a type<strong>of</strong> scientific inquiry. Based on the intakeinformation, the assessor should havesome initial hypotheses for underst<strong>and</strong>inga given case that will be tested during theevaluation. These hypotheses will guidethe initial planning <strong>of</strong> the evaluation <strong>and</strong>initial test selection. As in any good scientificendeavor, we must be clear <strong>of</strong> thedata that would support <strong>and</strong> those thatwould not support the various hypotheses.In contrast to many other scientific enterprises,however, the hypotheses can, <strong>and</strong>should, change during the investigation.


84 chapter 5 Planning the Evaluation <strong>and</strong> Rapport BuildingBox 5.1Planning the Evaluation to Enhance ReliabilityIn our chapter on psychometric theory, we discussedreliability as a key concept in underst<strong>and</strong>ingthe psychological measurement. Reliabilityis <strong>of</strong>ten considered as a property <strong>of</strong> individualtests. However, Martin (1988) discusses severalissues in planning an assessment battery thatcan maximize the reliability <strong>of</strong> the informationthat is obtained. Key to Martin’s approach is hisconceptualization <strong>of</strong> four primary sources <strong>of</strong>error variance that can affect the reliability <strong>of</strong>measurement <strong>of</strong> children’s social <strong>and</strong> emotionalfunctioning:(1) Temporal variance – changes in behaviorover time(2) Source or rater variance – differences ininformation due to characteristics <strong>of</strong> theinformant(3) Setting variance – differences due to differentdem<strong>and</strong> characteristics across settings(4) Instrument variance – unreliability inherentin individual instrumentsMartin uses the basic concept in measurementtheory to describe how these sources <strong>of</strong> errorvariance can be controlled in an assessment.Specifically, the primary method <strong>of</strong> controllingerror variance <strong>and</strong> increasing reliabilityis through aggregation. As the length <strong>of</strong>a test increases, the reliability <strong>of</strong> the scoresincreases. Thus, to control the temporal variance,repeated measurements on several occasionsshould be obtained. Similarly, to reducesource <strong>and</strong> setting variance, informationshould be obtained from multiple sources <strong>and</strong>across multiple settings. The implication <strong>of</strong>these psychometric considerations is the needfor a comprehensive evaluation.The final source <strong>of</strong> error variance in Martin’ s scheme is the instrument variance. Likethe other sources <strong>of</strong> variance, aggregatinginformation across instruments is a crucialmethod for increasing reliability. However,this is only the case if additional tests providereliable information. If one adds unreliabletests to a battery, then aggregationactually decreases the reliability <strong>of</strong> the battery.Clinicians, who have a favorite test that theyuse in the batteries, will <strong>of</strong>ten justify theiruse <strong>of</strong> the test, even if it has been provenunreliable, by the statement “I only use it asone part <strong>of</strong> a more comprehensive battery.”This is clearly better than using the test inisolation. However, adding a piece <strong>of</strong> unreliableinformation will only reduce the reliability<strong>of</strong> the aggregated information. In aseparate publication, Martin (1982) gives theexample <strong>of</strong> three umpires calling a baseballgame. If one <strong>of</strong> the umpires is blind, his callswill only serve to reduce the reliability <strong>of</strong>the calls made by the entire umpiring team.The moral <strong>of</strong> the story: Aggregation onlyincreases the reliability <strong>of</strong> the informationobtained if the individual tests are selectedto enhance reliability.Source: Martin (1988). <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Behavior Problems Infancy through Adolescence. New York:Guilford Press.As data accumulate on a case <strong>and</strong> it becomesclear that initial impressions <strong>of</strong> a case werewrong, the assessor must revise the assessmentaccordingly. To employ this scientificapproach to clinical assessment, enoughpreliminary information on a child’s functioningmust be obtained prior to startingthe evaluation, so that initial hypothesescan be formed. A case example that utilizesthis approach is provided in Box 5.2.


chapter 5 Planning the Evaluation <strong>and</strong> Rapport Building85Box 5.2A Scientific Approach to <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>: A Case ExampleJoshua is a 10-year-old boy who was referredto the outpatient psychiatry department <strong>of</strong> alarge inner-city pediatric hospital for testing.The intake worker determined that Joshuawas being referred by his parents becausehe was in danger <strong>of</strong> failing the fifth grade.According to the intake information, Joshuawas having great difficulty paying attentionin class <strong>and</strong> completing assignments.He was also described as being excessivelyfidgety <strong>and</strong> restless. The intake informationindicated that these school problems werenew this school year. He had been an A\Bstudent in the four previous school grades,which made his current poor performanceespecially puzzling.Based on this information, several initialhypotheses were formulated. It could be thatsimilar problems were experienced in the pastgrades but they had just increased in severityin the fifth grade; in which case, dispositionalcauses were possible such as an attention deficitdisorder <strong>and</strong>/or a learning disability. Alternatively,if this recent onset was supported inthe evaluation (through interviewing parentsabout past school performance, obtainingschool records, interviewing past teachers), itmay be that Joshua had experienced or was experiencingsome type <strong>of</strong> newly occurring stressor(e.g., parental divorce, sexual abuse) that wasresulting in the deterioration in behavior. Theevaluation was designed to test these initialhypotheses.Interestingly, during the assessment <strong>of</strong>potential stressors, Joshua’s mother reportedthat he had been involved in an automobileaccident during the summer prior to enteringthe fifth grade. He had sustained a closed headinjury <strong>and</strong> had lost consciousness for severalminutes. He was released from the hospitalwith no noticeable effects <strong>of</strong> the injury. Afterobtaining this information, another hypothesisbecame possible. Joshua might have sustainedneurological damage from the accident thatwas affecting his behavior. As a result, hewas referred for a neurological exam, whichuncovered neurological damage that seemedto be the most likely cause <strong>of</strong> his behavioraldifficulties. Although the initial hypotheseswere not correct, this illustrates how a scientificapproach to hypothesis testing can beuseful in structuring the assessment process.Designing the EvaluationWe concluded Chap. 3 by providing severalguidelines for clinical assessments<strong>of</strong> children that followed from researchin developmental psychopathology. Inthis section, we take these research-basedguidelines <strong>and</strong> use them to develop practicalconsiderations in designing clinicalassessments <strong>of</strong> children. Once again, theserecommendations are designed to providea generic framework that can be tailored tothe needs <strong>of</strong> the individual case.Developmental ConsiderationsFrom the discussion <strong>of</strong> developmental psychopathologyprovided in Chap. 3, it is clearthat assessments <strong>of</strong> children’s emotional <strong>and</strong>behavioral adjustment need to be sensitiveto a number <strong>of</strong> important developmentalissues. First, a basic tenet <strong>of</strong> developmentalpsychopathology is the importance<strong>of</strong> taking a “process-oriented” approachto conceptualizing children ’ s adjustment.As a result, it is important that clinicalassessments not only involve a st<strong>and</strong>ardized<strong>and</strong> comprehensive assessment <strong>of</strong> a child’s


86 chapter 5 Planning the Evaluation <strong>and</strong> Rapport Buildingbehavioral <strong>and</strong> emotional adjustment, butit is also important that they include anassessment <strong>of</strong> the developmental processes(e.g., temperamental tendencies, familycontext) that may be related to the child’scurrent pattern <strong>of</strong> adjustment. This morecomprehensive assessment requires thatassessors maintain a current knowledge <strong>of</strong>the research related to the type <strong>of</strong> problemsthey encounter in their evaluations, sothat they have an adequate underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>of</strong> the processes that may be involved inthe development <strong>of</strong> these problems.This research provides the assessorwith some initial hypotheses regardingthe problems that led to a child’s referralfor testing. Measures are selected so thatthese hypotheses can then be tested in theevaluation. For example, there is researchto suggest that there are distinct subgroups<strong>of</strong> children with conduct problems, some<strong>of</strong> whom react very strongly to peer provocation<strong>and</strong> emotional stimuli, <strong>and</strong> some <strong>of</strong>whom show a lack <strong>of</strong> reactivity to emotionalcues, leading them to ignore the potentialconsequences <strong>of</strong> their behavior on others(Frick, 2006). Solely assessing the child’slevel <strong>and</strong> severity <strong>of</strong> conduct problems <strong>and</strong>making a diagnosis <strong>of</strong> “Conduct Disorder,”without assessing the child’s affective<strong>and</strong> interpersonal style, would not allowone to distinguish between these differentsubgroups that may require differentapproaches to treatment (Frick & McMahon,2008).Secondly, it is important for the psychologistto consider the developmentalstage <strong>of</strong> the child to be assessed in designingan assessment battery. For example, itis important, when selecting tests for a battery,to determine whether the tests providegood norm-referenced scores for thedevelopmental stage <strong>of</strong> the child or adolescentbeing assessed. Because this is soimportant, a significant focus <strong>of</strong> the laterchapters (which provide reviews <strong>of</strong> specifictesting instruments) is on the description<strong>of</strong> the instruments’ norm-referencedscores. It is evident from these reviewsthat the adequacy <strong>of</strong> these scores can varyacross developmental stages (e.g., havinga very limited normative sample for olderadolescents). In addition to specific tests,some testing modalities may be more orless appropriate depending on the developmentallevel <strong>of</strong> the child. For example,in the chapter on structured interviews, wediscuss research suggesting that the childself-report format on these interviews maybe unreliable before age 9.Determining the RelevantPsychological DomainsA fairly ubiquitous finding in researchon childhood psychopathology is thehigh degree <strong>of</strong> overlap or comorbidity inproblem behaviors (Jensen, 2003). Thatis, children with problems in one area <strong>of</strong>emotional or behavioral functioning are athigh risk <strong>of</strong> having problems in other areas<strong>of</strong> emotional or behavioral functioning, aswell as problems in social <strong>and</strong> cognitivearenas. In addition, a key assumption to adevelopmental approach to underst<strong>and</strong>ingchildren’s adjustment is that all outcomesare influenced by multiple interactingprocesses. As a result, most evaluations<strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents must be fairlycomprehensive to ensure that all areas thatcould be relevant to treatment planningare assessed. In planning an evaluation, oneshould consider the most likely comorbiditiesassociated with the referral problem<strong>and</strong> the most likely factors that can leadto such problems, <strong>and</strong> design the evaluationto provide an adequate assessment <strong>of</strong>these areas. From the referral information,one may also gather some clues as to howintensive the assessment <strong>of</strong> these potentiallyimportant domains should be.For example, consider a referral <strong>of</strong> a7-year-old boy who is having significantproblems <strong>of</strong> being disorganized, being veryimpulsive, <strong>and</strong> having difficulty staying in


chapter 5 Planning the Evaluation <strong>and</strong> Rapport Building87his seat. An initial hypothesis may be thatthe child has attention-deficit hyperactivitydisorder (ADHD), <strong>and</strong> an evaluation isdesigned to test this hypothesis <strong>and</strong> to testthe many different processes (e.g., poorexecutive functioning) that could lead to thisdisorder (see Chap. 17). In addition, fromresearch on ADHD one knows that approximately30% <strong>of</strong> children with the disorderhave a co-occurring learning disability (Frick& Kimonis, 2008). Therefore, one needsto determine how this comorbidity can beassessed. However, in the initial intake, thechild’s mother states that her son has no realproblems academically, other than losing hisassignments frequently, <strong>and</strong>, in fact, he hasonly made two B’s on his report cards sinceentering school. Based on this piece <strong>of</strong> information,one may decide not to conduct anintensive evaluation <strong>of</strong> a potential learningdisability unless, during the course <strong>of</strong> theevaluation, some evidence <strong>of</strong> learning problemsis discovered.Screening <strong>of</strong> ImportantContextsResearch has indicated that children’sbehavior is strongly influenced by factorsin their psychosocial environment.Therefore, an important considerationin planning an evaluation is determiningthe aspects <strong>of</strong> a child’s environment beingassessed (e.g., teaching styles <strong>of</strong> specificteachers, affective tone <strong>of</strong> family interactions)<strong>and</strong> the assessment methodology(e.g., naturalistic observations, behaviorrating scales). However, the relevance <strong>of</strong>context will vary from child to child. Theintake information should provide enoughinformation so that an evaluation can beplanned, in which (1) informants fromeach <strong>of</strong> a child ’ s relevant contexts provideinformation on the child ’ s functioning<strong>and</strong> (2) the contexts that seem to have themost impact on a child ’ s functioning can beassessed in greater detail.One <strong>of</strong> the most influential contexts forthe majority <strong>of</strong> children is the family. Achapter in this text (Chap. 12) is devoted tothe assessment <strong>of</strong> a child’s family environment.However, what constitutes a familyfor a child is becoming increasingly diverse,<strong>and</strong> the intake can yield some preliminaryinformation on the family structure (e.g.,marital status <strong>of</strong> parents, degree <strong>of</strong> contactwith non-resident parents, other adult caretakersin the home) that provides the assessorwith some clues as to the best method<strong>of</strong> structuring an evaluation <strong>of</strong> the familycontext.Practical Considerations inDesigning an EvaluationIn an important clinical endeavor like psychologicaltesting that can have importantconsequences for a child, one does not liketo consider mundane factors such as time<strong>and</strong> expense in designing the evaluation.Clearly, these factors should not outweighwhat is in the best interest <strong>of</strong> the childbeing tested. However, sometimes thesefactors are unavoidable <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>ten expediencyis in the best interest <strong>of</strong> the child.For example, an adolescent who has animpending court date for a juvenile <strong>of</strong>fensemay need to have an evaluation completedbefore this date to help in determining themost appropriate placement <strong>and</strong> the mostappropriate services. One should take carenot to be so influenced by expediency thattreatment decisions are misguided by poorassessment results. But one must considerwhat can be meaningfully obtained withinthe available time frame, <strong>and</strong> possibly makeas part <strong>of</strong> the outcome <strong>of</strong> the evaluation arecommendation for the additional testingthat might be beneficial as time allows.How much to weigh cost <strong>and</strong> timeconstraints will vary from case to case.However, we feel that one should ask thefollowing two questions in designing anyevaluation:


88 chapter 5 Planning the Evaluation <strong>and</strong> Rapport Building1. What is the essential information neededto answer the referral question(s)?2. What is the most economical means<strong>of</strong> obtaining this essential informationwithout compromising the usefulness <strong>of</strong>information?To Test or Not to TestWhen a child is referred for testing, an importantquestion that should be asked is whether ornot an evaluation is in the child’s best interest.We feel that simply because an evaluation isrequested by someone is not sufficient reasonto conduct the evaluation. A pr<strong>of</strong>essionalmust make the decision as to whether or notan evaluation is likely to benefit the child oradolescent. Often this question is ignored forfinancial reasons. If you don’t do the evaluation,you don’t get paid. However, we feelthat a clinical assessor has the ethical obligationto estimate the potential benefit <strong>of</strong> theevaluation to the child <strong>and</strong> then convey thisdetermination to the referring agency.There can be several reasons why anevaluation would not be in a child ’ s bestinterest. For example, a child ’ s parentmay seek multiple evaluations becausethe parent does not agree with the findings<strong>of</strong> previous evaluations. We feel thatsecond opinions are not inappropriate inmany cases. However, if this is not consideredcarefully, a child may be subjectedto numerous intrusive evaluations thatare not necessary <strong>and</strong> the evaluator mayinadvertently reinforce a parent’s denial<strong>of</strong> a child ’ s special needs. Alternatively, theperson referring a child or adolescent mayhave unrealistic expectations from whatan evaluation can accomplish, or the reasonfor the evaluation may be insufficientto justify performing the evaluation. Anexample that illustrates both <strong>of</strong> these issuesis a child who is referred by a parent todetermine his future sexual orientation.Even if one determines that a child oradolescent may benefit from an evaluation,one must also question whether or not theassessor is the appropriate person to conductthe evaluation. The appropriateness<strong>of</strong> an assessor may simply be a matter <strong>of</strong>one ’ s competence, either because <strong>of</strong> uniquecharacteristics <strong>of</strong> the child (e.g., age, culture)or because <strong>of</strong> the specific nature <strong>of</strong>the referral question. Assessors must holdclosely to the principle noted in the St<strong>and</strong>ardsfor Educational <strong>and</strong> PsychologicalTesting published jointly by the AmericanEducational Research Association,American Psychological Association, <strong>and</strong>National Council on Measurement Educationthat “test users should not attemptto interpret the scores <strong>of</strong> test takers whosespecial needs or characteristics are outsidethe range <strong>of</strong> the user’s qualifications”(St<strong>and</strong>ard 11.3, p. 114).In addition to competence, a clinicalassessor must also question whether or notpersonal reasons might prevent him orher from conducting an objective evaluation.For example, an examiner may have apersonal relationship with a child or familythat might interfere with the ability toobjectively administer <strong>and</strong> interpret tests.Alternatively, the assessor may have personalissues related to the referral problemthat might prevent him or her from beingable to competently perform the evaluation.For example, a psychologist whohimself is dealing with memories <strong>of</strong> a pastsexual abuse may not be able to conductan evaluation <strong>of</strong> another sexual abuse victimbecause he is unable to transcend hisown issues related to the abuse. Thereare no specific guidelines for determiningwhen personal issues would interferewith an evaluation. Our point is to suggestthat assessors should routinely questionwhether or not they are appropriateto conduct an evaluation, <strong>and</strong> they shouldconsult with colleagues if there is any questionregarding their ability to competentlyconduct the evaluation.


chapter 5 Planning the Evaluation <strong>and</strong> Rapport Building89Rapport BuildingThere is no aspect <strong>of</strong> the assessment processthat is as difficult to define <strong>and</strong> toteach as the concept <strong>of</strong> rapport. However,rapport is a critical component <strong>of</strong> testingchildren <strong>and</strong> adolescents (Fuchs & Fuchs,1986), although it is rarely discussed inchild assessment texts (e.g., Achenbach &McConaughy, 1987; Ollendick & Hersen,1993) or in administration manuals for testsdesigned for children (see Fuchs, 1987).The Longman Dictionary <strong>of</strong> Psychology <strong>and</strong>Psychiatry (1984) defined rapport as “awarm, relaxed relationship that promotesmutual acceptance, e.g., between therapist<strong>and</strong> patient, or between teacher <strong>and</strong> student.Rapport implies that the confidenceinspired by the former produces trust <strong>and</strong>willing cooperation in the latter” (p. 619).To paraphrase <strong>and</strong> apply this definitionto the testing situation, rapport refers tothe interactions between the assessor <strong>and</strong>the person being assessed (client) thatpromote confidence <strong>and</strong> cooperation inthe assessment process. Rapport buildingis not something that is done at the outset<strong>of</strong> testing <strong>and</strong> then forgotten. Instead,it is a process that evolves throughout theentire assessment endeavor (Barker, 1990;Sattler, 1988).The importance <strong>of</strong> rapport is not specificto psychological assessment; it is acritical concept in most clinical endeavors.There are several recommendations thatcan be drawn from other clinical situationsthat apply equally well to child testing.For example, Phares (1984) described thebasic elements <strong>of</strong> establishing rapport inpsychotherapeutic relationships as “havingan attitude <strong>of</strong> acceptance, underst<strong>and</strong>ing,<strong>and</strong> respect for the integrity <strong>of</strong> the client”(p. 195). Phares goes on to point that thisattitude is not synonymous with establishinga state <strong>of</strong> mutual liking but is morerelated to a clinician ’ s ability to convey tothe client a sincere desire to underst<strong>and</strong> hisor her problems, <strong>and</strong> to help him or herto cope with them. This general attitude<strong>of</strong> the assessor is the basic component <strong>of</strong>establishing rapport with the client. As aresult, our specific recommendations aredesigned to foster this attitude in testingsituations.While the importance <strong>of</strong> rapport is notconfined to the psychological assessment<strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents, there are severalunique aspects to the assessment <strong>of</strong>youth that make rapport building a complicatedprocess in this context. First, theclinical assessment <strong>of</strong> children typicallyinvolves many people (e.g., child, parent,<strong>and</strong> teacher) who have varying levels <strong>of</strong>underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> the assessment process,<strong>and</strong> who possess varying levels <strong>of</strong> motivationfor the assessment. Therefore, theassessor must be skilled in enlisting <strong>and</strong>fostering the cooperation <strong>of</strong> many differentparticipants. The issue <strong>of</strong> motivationis especially salient in the evaluation <strong>of</strong>youth because children <strong>and</strong> adolescentsare <strong>of</strong>ten not self-referred. <strong>Child</strong>ren are<strong>of</strong>ten referred for evaluations becausetheir behavior causes problems for significantothers in their environment (Frick &Kimonis, 2008). Therefore, enlisting theircooperation <strong>and</strong> trust is a critical, but <strong>of</strong>tendifficult, process. Later in this chapter weprovide examples <strong>of</strong> how testing can bepresented to children <strong>and</strong> adolescents inways that foster the establishment <strong>of</strong> aworking relationship.A second factor that complicates thedevelopment <strong>of</strong> rapport in testing situationsis the presence <strong>of</strong> severe timelimitations. In many, if not most, testingsituations the assessor has limited timefor rapport building with all participants.Often testing is confined to one or severaldiscrete testing periods <strong>and</strong> testingstarts early in the first session. This isquite different from the many other clinicalcontexts, such as the psychotherapeuticcontext, in which there is likely to bemore flexibility in the time allowed for


90 chapter 5 Planning the Evaluation <strong>and</strong> Rapport Buildingdeveloping rapport prior to the initiation<strong>of</strong> some clinical intervention.Based on this discussion, it is evident thatestablishing rapport in the typical assessmentsituation for children <strong>and</strong> adolescentsinvolves enlisting the cooperation <strong>of</strong>multiple participants to divulge personal <strong>and</strong>sometimes distressing information, despitea potential lack <strong>of</strong> motivation <strong>and</strong> despitethe fact that the testing must be completedwithin a limited time frame. It is obvious fromthis description that rapport building is notalways an easy task in clinical assessments <strong>of</strong>youth. Therefore, it is important to outlinethe important considerations in the development<strong>of</strong> rapport in clinical assessments <strong>of</strong>children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.Informed ConsentWe view informed consent in two ways inthis book. The first, which is the more traditionalway, is to view it as a legal <strong>and</strong> ethicalright <strong>of</strong> the recipients <strong>of</strong> any psychologicalservice. The assessor has the responsibility<strong>of</strong> ensuring that informed consent is providedfor the assessment. However, we alsoview informed consent in a second way:as a basic element <strong>of</strong> rapport building. Asdiscussed previously, a fundamental elementin developing rapport is expressinga respect for the individual participatingin the evaluation. There is no more basicway <strong>of</strong> conveying respect than by placinggreat importance on the informed consentprocess.In Chap. 4, we discussed the legalrequirements <strong>of</strong> obtaining informed consentfrom a child ’ s legal guardian. However,the assessor can communicate asincere respect for the child ’ s guardianby spending a great deal <strong>of</strong> time reviewingall the testing procedures in very clear<strong>and</strong> specific terms, by discussing the limits<strong>of</strong> confidentiality in sensitive terms, byclearly reviewing the intended uses <strong>of</strong> thetest results, <strong>and</strong> by allowing <strong>and</strong> encouragingthe parents to ask questions about theseissues. In essence, the assessor should conveyto the parent that the consent proceduresare not just a legal formality, but areintended as the first step in establishing acollaborative effort between the parent <strong>and</strong>assessor. Also, there is no greater damageto the development <strong>of</strong> rapport than a parents’perception that some procedures wereused without his or her full knowledge <strong>and</strong>consent.The need to transcend legal requirementsis even more important with thechild. With the view that minors may notbe competent to make decisions regardingtheir need for certain medical or psychologicalprocedures, like psychologicaltesting, the right to informed consent generallyrests with a child’s parent or legalguardian. Unfortunately, many assessorstake this to mean that a child does not havethe right to have procedures explained tohim or her in underst<strong>and</strong>able language.Although in some situations we agree thata child may not have the right to refuseparticipation in an evaluation, we feel thatin all situations, irrespective <strong>of</strong> a child ’ s age,the assessor should explain to the child allthe procedures that he or she will undergoas part <strong>of</strong> the testing. Clearly, the degree<strong>of</strong> depth <strong>and</strong> sophistication <strong>of</strong> this explanationshould be made in recognition <strong>of</strong> thepossible fears about the evaluation that achild or adolescent might experience <strong>and</strong>with recognition <strong>of</strong> his or her varying levels<strong>of</strong> motivation. Boxes 5.3–5.5 provideexamples <strong>of</strong> how testing procedures canbe explained to children <strong>and</strong> adolescents<strong>of</strong> various ages in ways that enhance theestablishment <strong>of</strong> rapport.Discussing testing with the child oradolescent is critical for conveying respecttowards the child, <strong>and</strong> helps enlist the childas a collaborative participant in the process.It reduces the feeling <strong>of</strong> the child that thetesting is being done to him or her ratherthan for or with him or her. Also, manychildren arrive for testing with substantial


chapter 5 Planning the Evaluation <strong>and</strong> Rapport Building91Box 5.3Explaining Testing to a 5-Year-Old BoyWe have argued that all children should havetesting procedures explained to them in termsthat are underst<strong>and</strong>able given their developmentallevel. This is a crucial aspect <strong>of</strong> developingrapport with a child. However, manybeginning clinical assessors have difficultydescribing testing in terms comprehensible toyoung children <strong>and</strong> fail to recognize some <strong>of</strong>the fears <strong>and</strong> motivations that children bringto the evaluation. The following is an example<strong>of</strong> an explanation <strong>of</strong> procedures that is givento a 5-year-old boy referred to a private psychologistfor testing.“Hello, Johnny. My name is Dr. Test. I’mnot the type <strong>of</strong> doctor you come to whenyou’re sick, like with a stomachache or headache,but I’m the type <strong>of</strong> doctor who likes toget to know kids better, like how they feelabout some things <strong>and</strong> how they act sometimes.So what I’m going to do today is findout a lot more about you. I’m going to askyou to draw some pictures for me <strong>and</strong> tellme about them. I also have some pictures<strong>and</strong> I want you to make up stories aboutthem. And then, I have a bunch <strong>of</strong> questionsabout how you feel about certain things thatI’m going to help you answer. We will haveto work pretty hard together but I think itwill be fun, too. We’re going to take a lot <strong>of</strong>breaks <strong>and</strong> please let me know if you needto stop <strong>and</strong> go to the bathroom. Now, yourmom <strong>and</strong> dad have already been telling mea lot about you <strong>and</strong> I’m also going to betalking to your teacher at school. After I dothis, I’m going to take what you tell me, <strong>and</strong>what your parents <strong>and</strong> teacher tell me <strong>and</strong>try to get a good picture <strong>of</strong> what you ’ re like,how you feel about things, all the thingsyou ’ re doing well, <strong>and</strong> anything you mightneed help in. And then I will talk to yourparents <strong>and</strong> to you about what I find <strong>and</strong>let you know if there is anything that I cansuggest that might help you.”This explanation is designed to be anexample <strong>of</strong> the types <strong>of</strong> terms <strong>and</strong> phrasingthat can be used in explaining psychologicalprocedures to very young children. Ascan be seen from the content <strong>of</strong> the explanation,we feel that in this age group, one<strong>of</strong> the most important sources <strong>of</strong> anxiety isthe fear <strong>of</strong> the unknown. Therefore, we tryto let the child know that the procedureswill be pretty innocuous (e.g., answeringquestions, drawing). Obviously the actualcontent <strong>of</strong> the description will depend onthe procedures that are planned. But wefeel strongly that all procedures to be usedshould be explained to the child, albeit in alanguage that is underst<strong>and</strong>able.Also, to illustrate the level <strong>of</strong> explanation,the discourse was presented in a narrativeform. In actual practice it is helpful to involvethe child in the discussion by asking simplequestions (e.g., Do you like to draw?) <strong>and</strong>encouraging him or her to ask you questionsif there is anything he or she does not underst<strong>and</strong>.This helps the child feel more respected<strong>and</strong> valued in the assessment process. Finally,we <strong>of</strong>ten find it helpful in this age group topresent this information in the presence <strong>of</strong> thechild ’ s parent(s). When children see that theirparents are comfortable with the procedures,they <strong>of</strong>ten develop a greater sense <strong>of</strong> comfortthemselves.misconceptions about what the testing willentail (e.g., thinking that the psychologistis going to operate on their brain or thatthey will be punished for being bad). Simplyspending time to clearly review whythe child is being tested, what the childshould expect during testing, <strong>and</strong> whatwill happen with the test results helps toeliminate possible misconceptions <strong>and</strong>reduce unnecessary anxiety.


92 chapter 5 Planning the Evaluation <strong>and</strong> Rapport BuildingBox 5.4Explaining Testing to a 10-Year-Old GirlOlder, pre-adolescent children <strong>of</strong>ten have a betterunderst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> the basic nature <strong>of</strong> the testingsituation than do younger children. However,the procedures should still be explained in veryclear <strong>and</strong> simple terms to ensure that there are nomisconceptions. In this age group, we find thatthe explanation must be sensitive to the potentialthreat to a child’s self-concept that the testing maypresent. One <strong>of</strong> the major emotional tasks duringthe pre-adolescent period is the development <strong>of</strong> asense <strong>of</strong> mastery <strong>and</strong> a sense <strong>of</strong> competence. Testingcan be a threat to a child in these areas forseveral reasons. First, just the term “testing” conveysthe possibility <strong>of</strong> failure. Secondly, the childmay have been implicitly or explicitly told thatthe reason for the testing is to see “what’s wrongwith you.” The explanation <strong>of</strong> testing in this agegroup should be sensitive to these issues. Here, weprovide a sample explanation to a 10-year-old girlreferred for a comprehensive evaluation.“Jessica, I want to explain exactly whatwe are going to be doing together today,<strong>and</strong> give you a chance to ask me any questionsyou may have. Your parents wereconcerned about some <strong>of</strong> the problemsyou have been having at school <strong>and</strong> theywanted to know if there was anything morethey could be doing to help you. In orderfor me to answer this question, I have t<strong>of</strong>ind out a lot more about you-what you liketo do, how you feel about different things,what things you’re good at, what thingsyou might not be so good at. To do this,we are going to do a lot <strong>of</strong> different thingstogether. First, I am going to ask you to dosome reading <strong>and</strong> math problems with me.Then I will ask you to fill out some questionnairesthat will tell me how you feelabout different things, how you get alongwith kids in your class, <strong>and</strong> how you seeyour family. Finally, I am going to showyou some pictures <strong>and</strong> ask you to tell mesome stories about them. Before we starteach <strong>of</strong> these activities, I will tell you whatwe’re going to do <strong>and</strong> how to do eachthing. I promise to give you a chance to askme any questions you have about each task.I have already talked to your mother abouthow things go at home <strong>and</strong> I am going toask your teacher to fill out a questionnaireabout how she sees you at school. After Iget all the information, I should underst<strong>and</strong>you a little better <strong>and</strong> I will then talkabout what I found with you <strong>and</strong> your parents.Jessica, it is very important that youunderst<strong>and</strong> that I’m not looking for thingsthat are wrong with you. My guess is thatyou are like most kids. You have things thatyou ’ re good at <strong>and</strong> some things that you ’ renot so good at, <strong>and</strong> that there are thingsyou like <strong>and</strong> other things you don’t like. Iam just trying to get a good picture <strong>of</strong> allthese different parts <strong>of</strong> you.”Box 5.5Explaining Testing to an <strong>Adolescent</strong>There are several crucial issues that one mustkeep in mind when explaining testing to anadolescent. First, adolescents spend a greatdeal <strong>of</strong> energy trying to convince people thatthey are no longer children. Therefore, onemust be very careful not to come across ascondescending to them. Secondly, because <strong>of</strong>the importance <strong>of</strong> peers in adolescence, adolescentsare very concerned with fitting in.Coming in for psychological testing may beviewed as a threat to this by making them feeldifferent from other adolescents. Therefore,the explanation should attempt to normalize thetesting as much as possible. Thirdly, privacy is(Continues)


chapter 5 Planning the Evaluation <strong>and</strong> Rapport Building93Box 5.5 (Continued)a major issue for adolescents. In testing, adolescentsmay be asked many personal questions.They must be warned <strong>of</strong> these questions<strong>and</strong> informed as to how the information fromthe testing will be conveyed to other people.This is very threatening to most adolescents,<strong>and</strong> the explanation should be sensitive tothis issue. Fourth, a majority <strong>of</strong> adolescentsreferred for testing do not see the need forsuch testing <strong>and</strong> don ’ t want to be there. Amajor flaw we <strong>of</strong>ten see in presenting testingto adolescents is that the assessor tries to cajolethe adolescent into being happy to be there<strong>and</strong> into appreciating the potential benefits <strong>of</strong>testing. Clearly, the potential benefits <strong>of</strong> testingshould be discussed with the adolescent inan attempt to enhance motivation. However,this <strong>of</strong>ten has a minimal effect on motivation,<strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>ten one must simply acknowledge tothe adolescent that you underst<strong>and</strong> that he orshe is not happy about being there but, if youwork together, you will get through it quickly<strong>and</strong> relatively painlessly. The following is asample explanation <strong>of</strong> psychological testingprovided to a 16-year-old male.“Jeff, I want to explain what we will bedoing today <strong>and</strong>, please, feel free to ask meany questions about what I say. You probablyknow that your parents are concerned aboutyour behavior. They have seen some changesin you recently <strong>and</strong> they want to know if theycan do something more to help you. I underst<strong>and</strong>that you are not wild about being here,but if we work together, maybe we can see ifthere is anything that I can recommend tohelp you or at least put your parents’ mindsat ease. But if we’re going to get anything out<strong>of</strong> this we have to work together. I work witha lot <strong>of</strong> people <strong>of</strong> your age who don’t want tobe here at first, but end up getting a lot out<strong>of</strong> the experience. I will start by just askingyou about some <strong>of</strong> the things that have beengoing on with you lately to get your view onthings. I have already talked to your parentsabout their views <strong>of</strong> what’s going on. I alsohave some questionnaires for you to completeabout your feelings, your behaviors,<strong>and</strong> your attitudes. Some <strong>of</strong> these questionsare pretty personal, but they are importantfor me to get a better underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> you.After the testing, I will summarize the resultsin a report <strong>and</strong> go over it with you <strong>and</strong> yourparents. At that time we can discuss anythingthat I think may help you.”Building Rapport with the <strong>Child</strong>As mentioned previously, the child is <strong>of</strong>tennot the one seeking an evaluation but isusually referred by some significant adultwho feels that the child or adolescent needsthe testing. Therefore, the motivation <strong>of</strong>the child for the evaluation is <strong>of</strong>ten low.Another reason for low motivation is thatthe child <strong>of</strong>ten realizes, or has been explicitlytold, that the evaluation is promptedby problems either at home or school. Asa result, the child is legitimately concernedabout the outcome <strong>of</strong> the evaluation (i.e.,getting into more trouble). In addition, thetesting situation is <strong>of</strong>ten unique in mostchildren’s experiences. <strong>Child</strong>ren have had afew similar experiences, <strong>and</strong> therefore they<strong>of</strong>ten have little idea <strong>of</strong> what to expect in thetesting situation. Finally, the many developmentalstages that characterize childhood<strong>and</strong> adolescence imply that assessorsmust be familiar with development to beable to tailor their rapport-building strategiesto the unique needs <strong>of</strong> children at variousstages.We have already mentioned that rapportbuilding is a process that evolvesthroughout testing. It starts at the veryfirst contact between the assessor <strong>and</strong> the


94 chapter 5 Planning the Evaluation <strong>and</strong> Rapport Buildingchild. When an assessor greets a child, theassessor should (1) use a warm, friendly,<strong>and</strong> interesting tone, (2) be sure to greetthe child by name (don’t simply greet thechild’s parents), <strong>and</strong> (3) introduce him- orherself using his or her title (e.g., Dr., Ms.,Mr.). This last recommendation is a subject<strong>of</strong> considerable debate by practicingpsychologists (Barker, 1990). However,we feel that using a title is important inthe time-limited, task-oriented assessmentsituation because it sets the stage that youare a pr<strong>of</strong>essional (albeit a caring, friendly,<strong>and</strong> respectful one) who will be workingwith the child, <strong>and</strong> not a friend who willplay with the child.After informed consent, many authorsrecommend a period <strong>of</strong> time for discussinginnocuous <strong>and</strong> pleasant topics, suchas the children’s hobbies, pets, friends, orother interests (Barker, 1990). For youngerchildren, some authors even recommenda period <strong>of</strong> play to allow the children tobecome more accustomed to the examiner.In our experiences, such rapport-buildingstrategies should be used cautiously <strong>and</strong>sparingly. For many children, the assessormay be perceived as simply delayingthe inevitable by using these strategies.This could have the paradoxical effect <strong>of</strong>increasing their anticipatory anxiety. Inour experience, one <strong>of</strong> the best rapportbuildingstrategies is to begin the assessmenttasks quickly, so that the child beginsto realize that the procedures will not be asbad as they imagined.Periods <strong>of</strong> play before the evaluation areespecially problematic if structured testingis to follow. Young children <strong>of</strong>ten havedifficulty switching from unstructured tostructured tasks (Perry, 1990). Therefore, itis usually best when testing preadolescentchildren to start with the more structuredparts <strong>of</strong> the evaluation (e.g., rating scales,structured interviews) rather than startingwith less structured tasks (e.g., projectivedrawing tests). This is not only because<strong>of</strong> the greater difficulty in switching fromunstructured to structured tasks, but alsobecause the structured tasks have clearerdem<strong>and</strong> characteristics. That is, it is usuallyquite clear to children what is expected <strong>of</strong>them on these tasks <strong>and</strong> this, in turn, helpsthe children become more comfortable ina situation that is different from anythingthey have experienced in thepast.Box 5.6 provides a summary <strong>of</strong> someadditional rapport-building strategies foruse with children that were proposed byBarker (1990) in his book on interviewingchildrenBuilding Rapport with theParentThere are also some unique considerationsin building a working relationship with achild’s parents. Of course, the importance<strong>of</strong> rapport with parents will depend on thedegree <strong>of</strong> their involvement in the testing.However, in most situations their involvementwill be substantial. Although manyevaluations are conducted at the request <strong>of</strong>a parent, there are also many situations inwhich a child is referred by others (e.g.,school, court), <strong>and</strong>, in these situations,building rapport with the child’s parent iscritical. Under these circumstances, theassessor must allow the parent to expresshis/her views on the need for evaluationprior to the testing process. The assessorneed not necessarily agree with theseviews, but the assessor should convey tothe parents a sincere interest in underst<strong>and</strong>ingtheir views in order to build aworking relationship with them.Even for parents who have initiated thereferral for testing, the assessor should beaware <strong>of</strong> the potential threat to a parent ’ sself-esteem that many testing situationspresent. For many parents, acknowledgingthat their child might have sometype <strong>of</strong> disability is quite traumatic <strong>and</strong>can evoke a sense <strong>of</strong> failure. Also, parents<strong>of</strong>ten struggle with guilt blame for


chapter 5 Planning the Evaluation <strong>and</strong> Rapport Building95Box 5.6Rapport-Building StrategiesBarker (1990), in his book on conducting clinicalinterviews with children <strong>and</strong> adolescents,discussed several helpful strategies for establishingrapport. These can be summarized asfollows:1. A critical basis for rapport building isan assessor’s communication style. Theassessor who is able to adopt a warm,friendly, respectful, <strong>and</strong> interested communicationstyle is more likely to developa good working alliance with a child.2. The assessor’s physical appearance can alsoenhance rapport. Overly formal dress canmake a child feel ill at ease.3. Assessors should attempt to conform his orher posture, movements, speed <strong>of</strong> speech,voice tone <strong>and</strong> volume, etc. to the style <strong>of</strong>the person being tested. This should bedone sensitively <strong>and</strong> unobtrusively.4. Assessors should tailor their vocabulariesto match the vocabularies <strong>of</strong> theperson being tested. Few things impedethe establishment <strong>of</strong> rapport as much asrepeatedly using words <strong>and</strong> expressionsthat are unfamiliar to those with whomyou are speaking.5. Respect the views <strong>of</strong> those you are testing.This does not necessarily mean agreeingwith or approving <strong>of</strong> the views expressed.6. Occasionally the assessor should adopt aone-down position. To reduce the intimidationthat children sometimes feel withexperts, the assessor can sometimes aska child, from a position <strong>of</strong> ignorance,about something with which a child hasexpertise, such as video games, televisionshows, or soccer.7. Taking time during the testing to talk <strong>of</strong>experiences <strong>and</strong> interests that the assessor<strong>and</strong> child have in common can alsoincrease the trust between the assessor <strong>and</strong>the child.Barker (1990) also emphasizes that thedevelopment <strong>of</strong> rapport is continuousthroughout the testing process. “Rapportcan always be developed further; the reverseis also possible. Although it is certainly truethat once it is well established, rapport canwithst<strong>and</strong> a lot <strong>of</strong> stress, it nevertheless canbe damaged or even destroyed at any timeif continuing attention is not paid to maintainingit” (p. 35).Source: Barker (1990). <strong>Clinical</strong> Interviews with <strong>Child</strong>ren <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong>s. New York: Norton.their child ’ s problems <strong>and</strong> may be concernedthat testing will confirm theirpotential role in their child’s difficulties.An assessor should be sensitive to thesedynamics <strong>and</strong> allow the parents to expresstheir concerns at some point during thetesting. Additionally, the parents should besupported in their role <strong>of</strong> getting help fortheir child. For example, an assessor mighttell the parents how lucky their child is tohave parents who care enough to obtainhelp for him or her, <strong>and</strong> not just let thingsget worse. This helps to reframe the testingsituation as one that could increase theparents’ self-esteem, rather than one that isa threat to their self-concept.Several reasons were given for startingwith structured tasks in testing childrenin an effort to enhance rapport. In ourexperience, the opposite is true in rapportbuilding with parents. Even prior toobtaining specific background informationfrom a parent, it is important to let theparent discuss his or her concerns aboutthe child in an unstructured format. Theunstructured clinical interview is discussedin more detail in a later chapter. However,having such an interview at the start <strong>of</strong> the


96 chapter 5 Planning the Evaluation <strong>and</strong> Rapport Buildingevaluation conveys to the parent (1) a genuineconcern with his or her perceptions<strong>of</strong> their child ’ s adjustment <strong>and</strong> (2) that theevaluation will be personalized for theindividual child. If parents are immediatelyasked to fill the rating scales or administera structured interview as part <strong>of</strong> a st<strong>and</strong>ardevaluation, they <strong>of</strong>ten develop the impressionthat the assessor is more interested inadministering tests than in actually underst<strong>and</strong>ingtheir child ’ s needs. As one wouldexpect, such an impression is very damagingto the development <strong>of</strong> rapport.personally request the teachers’ participationin the evaluation, acknowledging <strong>and</strong>thanking the teacher for his or her efforts,rather than simply sending the material tothe teacher via the child, parent, or mail.Such a call is a pr<strong>of</strong>essional courtesy thatgreatly enhances the collaborative effort. Itsets the tone for the teacher being involvedin the evaluation as a valued pr<strong>of</strong>essionalwho has much to <strong>of</strong>fer in the assessment<strong>of</strong> the child.ConclusionsBuilding Rapport with TeachersIt is becoming increasingly clear that evaluations<strong>of</strong> children must involve informationfrom teachers (Loeber, Green, & Lahey,1990). The degree <strong>of</strong> teachers’ involvementvaries considerably depending on the focus<strong>of</strong> the evaluation. However, many assessorswho are not used to working in schoolsettings find themselves ill-equipped tocollaborate with teachers to conduct psychologicalevaluations (Conoley & Conoley,1991).In the introduction to the concept <strong>of</strong>rapport, we defined the basic ingredient torapport building as exhibiting an attitude<strong>of</strong> respect towards the client or informant.Although many psychologists work hard inrespecting <strong>and</strong> developing rapport with parents<strong>and</strong> children, <strong>of</strong>ten this respect is lostwhen dealing with other pr<strong>of</strong>essionals, suchas teachers. A key to demonstrate this attitudeis by respecting the importance <strong>of</strong> teachers’time. Scheduling phone calls during teacher’splanning times, eliminating all but the mostessential work for the teacher, <strong>and</strong> alwayspersonally thanking the teacher for his or herefforts in the evaluation are very simple, yetimportant, rapport-building strategies.If a teacher is sent assessment material forcompletion (e.g., rating scales), it is importantfor the assessor to call the teacher <strong>and</strong>In this chapter, some non-specifics <strong>of</strong> theclinical assessment <strong>of</strong> children were discussed.That is, a successful evaluationis not simply a matter <strong>of</strong> appropriatelyadministering <strong>and</strong> interpreting psychologicaltests. It is also dependent on anassessor’s ability to provide an appropriatecontext in which the testing takes place.The first major issue discussed was theimportance <strong>of</strong> good planning. A good evaluationis focused <strong>and</strong> goal-oriented. The purpose<strong>of</strong> the evaluation <strong>and</strong> the intended uses<strong>of</strong> the assessment results will have a majorimpact on how the assessment is structured.Enough information should be availableprior to actual testing so that the assessor hassome initial hypotheses to be tested in theevaluation.The second part <strong>of</strong> the chapter isfocused on rapport-building strategies withall participants in the evaluation. Developinga collaborative, respectful, <strong>and</strong> trustingworking relationship is crucial to a successfulevaluation. Being able to developrapport is a skill that <strong>of</strong>ten takes years<strong>of</strong> practical experience to develop fully.However, in this chapter we have tried tohighlight some <strong>of</strong> the important issues inrapport building with children <strong>and</strong> adolescents<strong>of</strong> various ages. We have also triedto make some practical recommendationsthat address these issues.


chapter 5 Planning the Evaluation <strong>and</strong> Rapport Building97Chapter Summary1. The first step in planning an evaluationis to clarify the reason for referral,both in terms <strong>of</strong> the purpose <strong>of</strong> testing<strong>and</strong> the types <strong>of</strong> behavior that led to thereferral.2. Two important decisions that a clinicalassessor should make prior to starting anyevaluation is whether or not a formalevaluation is warranted <strong>and</strong> whether heor she is the most appropriate person toconduct the evaluation.3. In addition to competently administeringtests, clinical assessors must createan appropriate environment withinwhich the evaluation can take place.4. Building rapport with a child refers todeveloping a collaborative <strong>and</strong> supportiverelationship with the child for thepurpose <strong>of</strong> conducting the evaluation.5. Building rapport with other importantpeople who will be involved in the evaluation(e.g., parents, teachers) is alsocritical to the assessment process.6. A thorough <strong>and</strong> sensitive informed consentprocedure can play a major rolein showing respect to the child client,<strong>and</strong> his or her parents <strong>and</strong> thereby cangreatly aid in the establishment <strong>of</strong> rapport.7. An explanation <strong>of</strong> the testing procedureswith a child must be sensitive to alarge number <strong>of</strong> motivational <strong>and</strong> developmentalissues.


P a r t I I<strong>Assessment</strong> Methods


C h a p t e r 6Self-Report InventoriesChapter Questionsl What are the strengths <strong>and</strong> limitations<strong>of</strong> self-report measures for child <strong>and</strong>adolescent assessments?l What are some <strong>of</strong> the key differencesbetween omnibus measures <strong>and</strong> singledomain measures?l How does the MMPI-A differ from itsadult counterpart?l Which <strong>of</strong> the self-report measures possessesgood evidence <strong>of</strong> content validity?l What are validity scales <strong>and</strong> how canthey be used to interpret self-reportmeasures?Omnibus <strong>Personality</strong>InventoriesThe use <strong>of</strong> self-report inventories withchildren is a relatively new phenomenon.It was heret<strong>of</strong>ore commonly believed thatchildren could not accurately report ontheir own feelings, perceptions, <strong>and</strong> behaviors.As a result, parent <strong>and</strong> teacher reportshave routinely been preferred over the use<strong>of</strong> self-report inventories in child personalityassessment. One <strong>of</strong> the first popular childassessment instruments, for example, the<strong>Personality</strong> Inventory for <strong>Child</strong>ren (PIC;Wirt et al., 1984), resembled a “junior”MMPI. It included a large item set similar to101P.J. Frick et al., <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Behavior,DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-0641-0_6, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010


102 CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIESthe MMPI, <strong>and</strong> the name conveys similaritiesto omnibus personality inventories. Yet, thePIC was, <strong>and</strong> is (PIC-2; Lachar & Gruber,2001) a parent rating scale.As a result, many <strong>of</strong> the omnibus scalesdescribed in this chapter are relatively new.This is a result <strong>of</strong> the growing consensus thatat least older children <strong>and</strong> adolescents can provideuseful information about their feelings<strong>and</strong> behavior. However, as with any assessmenttool, there are limitations to the reliability,validity, <strong>and</strong> usefulness <strong>of</strong> any self-reportmeasure, <strong>and</strong> these limitations should betaken into account when designing an assessmentbattery <strong>and</strong> interpreting its results.Unfortunately, all <strong>of</strong> the complexity<strong>of</strong> using <strong>and</strong> interpreting omnibus selfreportinventories cannot be conveyed inone chapter. Most <strong>of</strong> the inventories discussedherein have entire volumes devotedto their interpretation. The following discussionserves primarily as an introductoryguide for studying the larger literature thatis available for an instrument. The eventualuser <strong>of</strong> any <strong>of</strong> the omnibus inventoriesdiscussed will have to spend considerabletime with the test manuals <strong>and</strong> additionalreadings <strong>and</strong> seek supervised interpretationpractice. What has become more clear inrecent research is that despite their relativelack <strong>of</strong> validity for the assessment <strong>of</strong> veryyoung children, self-report inventories canprovide invaluable information as to theyouth’s perception <strong>of</strong> his/her functioning<strong>and</strong> the factors that ameliorate or exacerbatehis/her problems.Whether or not a self-report inventoryhas a place in a psychological assessment willdepend on many factors, including the client’sdevelopmental level, presenting problem, <strong>and</strong>the purpose <strong>of</strong> the assessment. This chapterin no way represents an exhaustive review <strong>of</strong>the self-report inventories available. Instead,we have reviewed what appear to be the mostwidely used <strong>and</strong>/or well-researched measures.In addition, although the appropriate uses <strong>of</strong>self-report measures are highlighted, thereare limitations to any assessment technique.For self-report rating scales in particular,one must consider the client’s comprehension<strong>of</strong> items <strong>and</strong> the potential for responsesets including socially desirable responsetendencies. The instruments reviewedbelow vary in the degree to which they haveappeared to take these factors into account.Therefore, the burden rests with the clinicianto select tools that will answer the referralquestion in a legitimate, comprehensive, <strong>and</strong>cost-effective manner (Table 6.1).Behavior <strong>Assessment</strong> Systemfor <strong>Child</strong>ren Self-Report <strong>of</strong><strong>Personality</strong> (BASC-2-SRP;Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004)The Behavior <strong>Assessment</strong> System for <strong>Child</strong>ren,2nd edition-Self-Report <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> (BASC-2-SRP; Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004) isbased closely on its predecessor, albeit withsome unique features. For example, theBASC-2 includes a college report form forstudents aged 18–25. Our discussion, however,will focus on the child version (SRP-C) for ages 8–11 <strong>and</strong> the adolescent version(SRP-A), which is normed for ages 12–21.Interested readers should consult theBASC-2 manual (Reynolds & Kamphaus)for a discussion <strong>of</strong> the development <strong>of</strong> theunique college student report version. Aswith the previous version <strong>of</strong> this instrument,the BASC-2-SRP is the component<strong>of</strong> the BASC-2 that attempts to gaugethe child’s perceptions <strong>and</strong> feelings aboutschool, parents, peers, <strong>and</strong> his or her ownbehavioral problems. The estimated minimumreading level <strong>of</strong> the SRP is the thirdgrade. The SRP may also be read to childrenin order to ensure comprehension <strong>of</strong>the items. According to its authors, it takesapproximately 20–30 min to administer.Scale ContentThe SRP includes 16 scales: 12 clinical <strong>and</strong>4 adaptive. The scales were developedusing a combination <strong>of</strong> rational, theoretical,


CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIES103Table 6.1 Overview <strong>of</strong> Self-Report InventoriesInventory Ages ContentBehavior<strong>Assessment</strong>System for<strong>Child</strong>ren,2nd edition(BASC-2;Reynolds &Kamphaus,2004)AchenbachYouth Self-Report (YSR;Achenbach& Rescorla,2001)8–11;12–21;18–25139–185 items; Emotionalsymptoms index, inattention/hyperactivity, internalizingproblems, school maladjustment,personal adjustment;3 validity scales; 12 clinicalscales, 4 adaptive scales, 4optional content scales11–18 112 items; Internalizing,Externalizing, Competence;8 scalesMinnesotaMultiphasic<strong>Personality</strong>Inventory-<strong>Adolescent</strong>(MMPI-A;Butcher et al.,1992)14–18 478 items; 10 clinical scales;6 validity scales; 15 contentscales; 6 supplementaryscales; 28 Harris-Lingoesscales; 3 Si scalesReadingLevelTime toAdministerNormSample Reliability Validity MI3rd grade 20–30 min E Internal consistency(good);4–8 week test–retest (good)Good relationswith similartools; supportfor factorsE5th grade 15–20 min G Internalconsistency(good); 7 monthtest–retest (moderate);interrater(low to moderate)5th–6thgrade90 min G Internal consistency;test–retest; contentscales betterreliability thanclinical scalesGood validity(clinical vs. nonclinicalgroups);limited info. onrelations withother toolsItem overlapdetracts fromcontent validity;scores do notmatch 4 factorsolution foundin researchEP(Continues)


104 CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIESTable 6.1 (Continued)Inventory Ages ContentReadingLevelTime toAdministerNormSample Reliability Validity MIConners3rd Edition,Self-Report(Conners-3;Conners,2008a)<strong>Personality</strong>Inventory forYouth (PIY;Lachar &Gruber, 1994)8–18 59 items; 39-item shortform; 3 validity scales, 6empirical scales, 1 rationalscale, 5 DSM-IV-TR symptomscales9–18 270 items, 80 item screener;9 clinical scales (several subscales);4 validity scales3rd grade 20 min E Internal consistency<strong>and</strong> test–retest (good)3rd–4thgrade30–60 min G Internalconsistency;test–retest;good reliabilitybut lower forsubscalesGood evidence<strong>of</strong> criterionrelated<strong>and</strong>discriminantvalidityGood validity(clinical vs. nonclinicalgroups);some supportfor criterionrelatedvalidityE aGNote: Norm sample evaluated as E = excellent, G = good; MI = degree <strong>of</strong> enhancement <strong>of</strong> muti-informant assessment; E = Excellent, G = Good, P = Poora We view the Conners-3 as providing an excellent means <strong>of</strong> assessment <strong>of</strong> externalizing problems; however, if an extensive assessment <strong>of</strong> internalizing or learningdifficulties is desired, the Conners Comprehensive Behavior Rating Scales (CBRS; Conners, 2008b) is recommended.


CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIES105<strong>and</strong> empirical approaches to test development.Based on relatively low reliabilities<strong>of</strong> some SRP scales on the original BASC<strong>and</strong> preliminary analyses comparing SRPversions with different response formats,the authors developed the BASC-2-SRPto include both True-False items <strong>and</strong>Frequency-based items (i.e., Never, Sometimes,Often, Almost Always). Items fromthe original BASC were reviewed, particularlyfor reliability <strong>and</strong> developmentalappropriateness. Scales were definedbased largely on the original version, itemswere retained or new items developedbased on these definitions, <strong>and</strong> covariancestructure analysis was used to enhance thehomogeneity <strong>of</strong> scale content (Reynolds& Kamphaus, 2004). The SRP includesscales that are typical <strong>of</strong> self-report measures(e.g., anxiety, depression), as well assome that are relatively unique (e.g., selfreliance,locus <strong>of</strong> control; see scale definitionsin Table 6.2). The BASC-2 systemalso includes critical items that indicateclinically significant problems that warrantfurther follow-up assessment.Five composites were constructed for theSRP using factor analysis: Emotional SymptomsIndex; Inattention/Hyperactivity;Internalizing Problems; Personal Adjustment;<strong>and</strong> School Problems. The Inattention/Hyperactivitycomposite includesthe Attention Problems <strong>and</strong> Hyperactivityscales. The School Maladjustment compositeincludes the Attitude Toward School <strong>and</strong>Attitude Toward Teacher scales. The PersonalAdjustment composite assesses selfperceivedpersonal strengths <strong>and</strong> includesthe four adaptive scales (i.e., Relationswith Parents, Interpersonal Relations, Selfesteem,<strong>and</strong> Self-reliance). The Internalizingcomposite consists <strong>of</strong> the remainingsix scales (i.e., Atypicality, Locus <strong>of</strong> Control,Social Stress, Anxiety, Depression, <strong>and</strong>Sense <strong>of</strong> Inadequacy) The ESI representsthe scores for the six scales with the highestloadings on an unrotated first factor (i.e.,Social Stress, Anxiety, Depression, Sense<strong>of</strong> Inadequacy, Self-esteem reversed scored,<strong>and</strong> Self-reliance reverse scored). Accordingto the authors, this index is “the mostglobal indicator <strong>of</strong> serious emotional disturbance”(p. 81).An additional feature, new to the adolescentversion <strong>of</strong> the BASC-2-SRP, is theinclusion <strong>of</strong> four content scales: AngerControl, Ego Strength, Mania, <strong>and</strong> TestAnxiety. These scales were formed viaa combination <strong>of</strong> rational <strong>and</strong> empiricalmethods <strong>and</strong> include some items that arepart <strong>of</strong> other SRP scales <strong>and</strong> other itemsthat are not part <strong>of</strong> another scale. Thesescales are considered optional <strong>and</strong> are notpart <strong>of</strong> the h<strong>and</strong> scoring <strong>of</strong> the BASC-2(Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004). Obviously,research is needed regarding the usefulness<strong>of</strong> these scales in clinical assessments, butthey may very well represent importantconstructs heret<strong>of</strong>ore not assessed.Administration <strong>and</strong> ScoringThe SRP is available in three administrationformats: a scannable form, a carbonlessh<strong>and</strong>-scored form, <strong>and</strong> a computer-scorableform. The SRP has a reasonable administrationtime, given that there are 139 items onthe SRP-C <strong>and</strong> 176 items on the SRP-A.Although efforts were made to keep the itemstems brief <strong>and</strong> at a third grade reading level,some young people may still have difficultyusing the SRP. This <strong>and</strong> other self-reportinventories are not recommended for usewith children with significant cognitive deficitsor severe reading problems. Oral administrationor the use <strong>of</strong> the available audiotapefor the SRP is possible, but the child withcomprehension problems may still have agreat deal <strong>of</strong> difficulty with this procedure.Validity ScalesThe BASC-2-SRP includes three validityscales, as well as two indexes that alert theclinician to response patterns or inconsistencyin responses (i.e., the ResponsePattern Index <strong>and</strong> Consistency Index,respectively). The three validity scales are


106 CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIESTable 6.2 BASC-2-SRP Scale DefinitionsScaleAnxietyAttention ProblemsAttitude to SchoolAttitude to TeachersAtypicalityDepressionHyperactivityInterpersonal RelationsLocus <strong>of</strong> ControlRelations with ParentsSelf-EsteemSelf-RelianceSensation SeekingSense <strong>of</strong> InadequacySocial StressSomatizationDefinitionFeelings <strong>of</strong> nervousness, worry, <strong>and</strong> fear; the tendency to be overwhelmedby problemsTendency to be easily distracted <strong>and</strong> unable to concentrate more thanmomentarilyFeelings <strong>of</strong> alienation, hostility, <strong>and</strong> dissatisfaction regarding schoolFeelings <strong>of</strong> resentment <strong>and</strong> dislike <strong>of</strong> teachers; beliefs that teachers areunfair, uncaring, or overly dem<strong>and</strong>ingThe tendency toward bizarre thoughts or other thoughts <strong>and</strong> behaviorsconsidered “odd”Feelings <strong>of</strong> unhappiness, sadness, <strong>and</strong> dejection; a belief that nothing goesrightTendency to report being overly active, rushing through work or activities,<strong>and</strong> acting without thinkingThe perception <strong>of</strong> having good social relationships <strong>and</strong> friendships withpeersThe belief that rewards <strong>and</strong> punishments are controlled by externalevents or other peopleA positive regard for parents <strong>and</strong> a feeling <strong>of</strong> being accepted by themFeelings <strong>of</strong> self-esteem, self-respect, <strong>and</strong> self-acceptanceConfidence in one’s ability to solve problems; a belief in one’s personaldependability <strong>and</strong> decisivenessThe tendency to take risks <strong>and</strong> seek excitementPerceptions <strong>of</strong> being unsuccessful in school, unable to achieve one’s goals,<strong>and</strong> generally inadequateFeelings <strong>of</strong> stress <strong>and</strong> tension in personal relationships; a feeling <strong>of</strong> beingexcluded from social activitiesThe tendency to be overly sensitive to, experience, or complain aboutrelatively minor physical problems <strong>and</strong> discomfortsNote: From Reynolds & Kamphaus (2004).patterned after those found in the MinnesotaMultiphasic <strong>Personality</strong> Inventory(MMPI) tradition (see below). These scalesprovide a variety <strong>of</strong> checks on the validity <strong>of</strong>a child’s results. The F Index is designed toindicate if a respondent may have answeredin an overly negative (i.e., “fake bad”)manner. The L Index does the opposite inthat it evaluates a tendency to respond in anoverly positive manner. The V Index onthe SRP consists <strong>of</strong> nonsensical items(e.g., “I have never been to sleep”) that, ifendorsed, would likely indicate carelessnessor lack <strong>of</strong> cooperation. The ResponsePattern Index (available in the computerscoring program) assesses the degree towhich an item response is the same as theresponse to the previous item. For example,marking “True” for 15 items in a row wouldyield a higher number in this Index <strong>and</strong>would suggest that the respondent indicated“True” regardless <strong>of</strong> item content.The Consistency Index (also availablein the computer scoring program)is based on response patterns to itemsthat should be answered similarly. Theauthors also point out that the change toa mixture <strong>of</strong> True-False <strong>and</strong> Likert-type


CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIES107response formats on the new version <strong>of</strong>the SRP may also help safeguard againstresponse sets (Reynolds & Kamphaus,2004). It is still quite possible, however,to have the SRP invalidated by a response setor lack <strong>of</strong> cooperation. The validity indexesshould be examined at the outset <strong>of</strong> interpretation;further, the clinician should examinefor himself/herself response patterns thatdo not seem to fit the other evidence providedabout a case. In particular, a child oradolescent might present himself/herselfin an unrealistically favorable light, givenreferral concerns (i.e., that is, nearly allT-scores on the clinical scales are below 50;see Box 6.1).Box 6.1An Example <strong>of</strong> Fake Good Response SetSelf-report inventories, despite the best efforts<strong>of</strong> test developers, always remain susceptibleto response sets. In the following case example,the BASC-2-SRP was utilized.Bethany is a 14-year-old girl who wasadmitted to the inpatient psychiatric unit <strong>of</strong>a general hospital with a diagnosis <strong>of</strong> MajorDepression following threats <strong>of</strong> suicide. Bethany’sparents reportedly divorced two yearsago. She currently lives with her mother <strong>and</strong>sees her father about once a month. Bethanydoes not have any siblings. Her motherreported that Bethany tends to isolate herselfat home <strong>and</strong> seems sad <strong>and</strong> irritable most <strong>of</strong>the time. Bethany is currently in the eighthgrade, <strong>and</strong> her school attendance during thisyear has been poor. She has been suspendedfrom school on numerous occasions this year,including for getting into fights with peers,refusing to follow teachers’ directions, <strong>and</strong>damaging school property. Her grades arereportedly poor, although her mother indicatedthat she used to make mostly “As”<strong>and</strong> “Bs” until this year. In addition, it wasreported that Bethany has a history <strong>of</strong> problemsconcentrating at school.Bethany was talkative during the diagnosticinterview, yet it appeared that she was tryingto portray herself in a favorable light, as sheendorsed very few symptoms. When asked abouthobbies, for example, she said that she liked toread. When questioned further, however, shecould not name a book that she had read.According to her mother, Bethany’s familyhistory is significant for depression on bothher maternal <strong>and</strong> paternal side. Bethany’sfather reportedly had difficulty in school.Bethany’s results show evidence <strong>of</strong> a socialdesirability response set. On the L-scale <strong>of</strong> theSRP, Bethany obtained a raw score <strong>of</strong> 12 whichis in the Extreme Caution range. Furthermore,all but one <strong>of</strong> her clinical scale scores were lowerthan the normative T-score mean <strong>of</strong> 50, <strong>and</strong> all<strong>of</strong> her adaptive scale scores were above the normativemean <strong>of</strong> 50. In other words, the SRPresults suggest that Bethany is well-adjustedwhich is inconsistent with her reported currentfunctioning <strong>and</strong> background information.This example clearly indicates the needto consider all evidence gathered rather thanstrictly relying on the results <strong>of</strong> any one assessmentstrategy. Her SRP scores were:Scale<strong>Clinical</strong> ScalesT-ScoreAttitude to School 43Attitude to Teachers 39Attention Problems 44Hyperactivity 40Sensation Seeking 39Atypicality 40Locus <strong>of</strong> Control 38Somatization 37Social Stress 40Anxiety 32Depression 42Sensation Seeking 51Sense <strong>of</strong> Inadequacy 42Adaptive ScalesRelations with Parents 51Interpersonal Relations 56Self-Esteem 55Self-Reliance 59


108 CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIESNormingThe SRP was normed on a national sample<strong>of</strong> 1,500 children aged 8–11 <strong>and</strong> 1,900 adolescentsaged 15–18. Equal numbers <strong>of</strong> boys<strong>and</strong> girls were included within each agegroup (i.e., 8–11; 12–14; 15–18). Based onthe occurrence <strong>of</strong> age group differences onmany subscales, these age groups were usedin the development <strong>of</strong> T-scores (i.e., separateT-score distributions were used for 8–11-year olds from 12–14-year olds). Reynolds<strong>and</strong> Kamphaus (2004) report that “Withineach sex at each age grouping, the Generalnorm samples were matched to targeted USpopulation estimates taken from the March2001 Current Population Survey (CurrentPopulation Survey, 2001)” (p. 116–117). Thematched variables were socioeconomic status,race/ethnicity variable <strong>and</strong> geographicregion. The authors also considered thepresence <strong>of</strong> emotional <strong>and</strong> behavioral problemsin the General norm sample relative tothe proportion <strong>of</strong> youth with such problemsin the general population.The SRP <strong>of</strong>fers clinical norms for asample <strong>of</strong> 577 children <strong>and</strong> 950 adolescentsselected from an unspecified number<strong>of</strong> special education classrooms <strong>and</strong> mentalhealth settings throughout the UnitedStates. Data presented by the authorsshow good correspondence to the generalUnited States population for the Generalnorm group on the demographic variablesconsidered. In addition, the <strong>Clinical</strong> normgroup showed variability in race/ethnicity,geographic region, <strong>and</strong> age for eachcategory (i.e., Learned Disability, ADHD,other; Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004). Aswith the previous version <strong>of</strong> the SRP, separategender norms were also devised. Thepurpose <strong>of</strong> this procedure was to reduceany sex differences in T-scores on the variousscales <strong>and</strong> composites. Such norms maybe <strong>of</strong> interest if the clinician wants to considerthe severity <strong>of</strong> a child’s problems relativeto others <strong>of</strong> the same sex. However, inmost assessment situations, the question isregarding the degree <strong>of</strong> problems relativeto the overall population <strong>of</strong> same-aged children,which would suggest the use <strong>of</strong> normsthat include both boys <strong>and</strong> girls (Reynolds& Kamphaus, 2004).ReliabilityThe reliability <strong>of</strong> the SRP scales is good asindicated by a variety <strong>of</strong> methods. Medianinternal consistency coefficients are generallyin the .80s (see Table 6.3). Test-retestcoefficients taken between 4 <strong>and</strong> 8 weekslater are generally in the .70s. As shown inTable 6.3, the lowest internal consistencyTable 6.3 BASC-2-SRP Median InternalConsistency CoefficientsScaleCoefficientAnxiety 0.86Attention Problems 0.78Attitude to School 0.82Attitude to Teachers 0.79Atypicality 0.83Depression 0.86Hyperactivity 0.76Interpersonal Relations 0.79Locus <strong>of</strong> Control 0.78Relations with Parents 0.87Self-Esteem 0.82Self-Reliance 0.70Sensation Seeking 0.70Sense <strong>of</strong> Inadequacy 0.79Social Stress 0.83Somatization 0.67Median 0.80Composites School Problems 0.85Inattention/Hyperactivity 0.84Internalizing Problems 0.96Personal Adjustment 0.89Emotional Symptoms Index 0.94


CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIES109was for the Somatization scale, whichmay make sense in that this scale consists<strong>of</strong> physical symptoms which may not behighly interrelated.ValidityThe BASC-2 manual provides an extensivereport <strong>of</strong> exploratory <strong>and</strong> confirmatoryfactor analyses <strong>of</strong> the SRP items.Three factors were initially found, withAttention Problems <strong>and</strong> Hyperactivityloading on the Internalizing Problemsfactor; however, a four-factor solutionwith these two scales included as a separatefactor was judged superior. This factorstructure is shown in Table 6.4. Theschool maladjustment factor remainsa relatively unique contribution <strong>of</strong> theBASC self-report. The personal adjustmentfactor is also unique in that it providesa multidimensional assessment <strong>of</strong>Table 6.4 BASC-2-SRP Factors <strong>and</strong>Scale MembersSchool MaladjustmentAttitude to SchoolAttitude to TeachersSensation Seeking (adolescent only)Internalizing ProblemsAnxietyDepressionLocus <strong>of</strong> ControlSense <strong>of</strong> InadequacySomatizationSocial StressPersonal AdjustmentRelations with ParentsInterpersonal RelationsSelf-EsteemSelf-RelianceInattention/HyperactivityAttention ProblemsHyperactivityadaptation or potential strengths. Theimplications <strong>of</strong> this composite for longtermprognosis are unclear, however.The criterion-related validity <strong>of</strong> theSRP was evaluated by correlating it withthe Achenbach Youth Self-Report, theConners-Wells <strong>Adolescent</strong> Self-reportScale, <strong>Child</strong>ren’s Depression Inventory,Revised <strong>Child</strong>ren’s Manifest AnxietyScale. Notably, research on the correlationsbetween the adolescent version <strong>of</strong>the BASC-2 <strong>and</strong> the MMPI-A has not yetbeen conducted.In general, the SRP scales correlatedhighly (i.e., r = .65 <strong>and</strong> higher) with analogousscales from the Achenbach YouthSelf-report, whereas the correlations weregenerally moderate between the SRP-A <strong>and</strong>the Conners-Wells <strong>Adolescent</strong> Self-reportScale. Interestingly, the Depression scale <strong>of</strong>the SRP showed only modest correlationswith the <strong>Child</strong>ren’s Depression Inventorysubscales <strong>and</strong> total score (i.e., r = .09–.42) forthe child version <strong>of</strong> the SRP. However, theadolescent version <strong>of</strong> the SRP demonstratedmuch more congruence with thecorrelation coefficients ranging from .39to .69. The Anxiety scale <strong>of</strong> the SRP demonstratedgenerally moderate correlationswith the Revised <strong>Child</strong>ren’s Manifest AnxietyScale (i.e., r = .31 to .60 for the SRP-C;r = .33 to .49).The original BASC was the focus<strong>of</strong> a number <strong>of</strong> investigations <strong>of</strong> the correlates<strong>of</strong> its scales in varied populations <strong>and</strong>for varied purposes (e.g., basic researchquestions on child functioning; treatmentoutcome research; see Kamphaus & Frick,2005 for a review). However, to date, veryfew such investigations have used theBASC-2 system. The BASC-2 manualprovides initial results on the correlates<strong>of</strong> the SRP scales; however, much moreresearch is needed, particularly with clinicalpopulations to help provide further underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>of</strong> the information garnered fromthese scales.


110 CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIESInterpretationAlthough the composites <strong>of</strong> the SRP havesome factor-analytic evidence to supporttheir validity, there is little evidence as tothe validity <strong>and</strong> utility <strong>of</strong> the composites.The composites may very well provide parsimonious<strong>and</strong> clinically relevant information,but such studies simply have not beenconducted yet. In the BASC-2 manual, theauthors provide results indicating that theInternalizing Composite <strong>and</strong> EmotionalSymptoms Index show morderate tostrong correlations with virtually all scales<strong>and</strong> composites from the Achenbach YouthSelf-report. The Inattention-Hyperactivitycomposite showed correlations withthe ADHD-oriented scales on the Achenbach<strong>and</strong> on the Conners-Wells <strong>Adolescent</strong>Self-report Scale that were r = .54 <strong>and</strong>higher. However, given the relative lack <strong>of</strong>research to date <strong>and</strong> the difficulty in interpretingthese composites given their variedcontent (see Table 6.4), until furtherresearch studies are available, initial effortsat SRP interpretation should focus on thescale level. Examples <strong>of</strong> such interpretationsare provided in Box 6.2. The scales <strong>of</strong>the BASC-2, <strong>and</strong> by extension, the originalBASC, are better understood because thescale contents have some rational, theoretical,<strong>and</strong> research basis.Many items from the original BASC-SRP were retained or slightly altered forthe BASC-2-SRP (see Reynolds & Kamphaus,2004), which lends some confidencein scale interpretation for the clinicianwith experience using the previous version.Item-level factor-analytic results provideone empirical clue to the meaning <strong>of</strong> SRPscales. Some <strong>of</strong> the items with the mostsubstantial factor loadings on each scaleare identified in Table 6.5. These symptomscan be linked to background informationin order to interpret SRP results withgreater certainty. However, the elevations<strong>of</strong> SRP scales may also lead the clinicianto follow-up on issues that might not havebeen raised during the gathering <strong>of</strong> backgroundinformation.We propose the following steps in interpretingthe SRP (which can be appliedto other omnibus rating scales as well:(1) check validity scales; (2) check criticalitems if available; (3) determine whichscales are elevated; (4) examine the itemsthat appeared to lead to scale elevations.Some caution is needed in keeping afocus on interpretation at the scale level. Inparticular, scales may have intuitive appeal<strong>and</strong> some relevant content, but questionablereliability would hamper the validity<strong>of</strong> interpretations garnered based onunreliable scales. The reliability estimatesshown in Table 6.3 can help a cliniciangauge the level <strong>of</strong> confidence in scale-levelinterpretations for the BASC-2-SRP.Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesThe BASC-2-SRP is a potentially usefultool in child <strong>and</strong> adolescent assessment.The BASC-2-SRP has numerousstrengths that make it a viable option foruse with children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. Notablestrengths include:1. A broader age range than is typicallyavailable for omnibus inventoriesdesigned to obtain self-report fromchildren.2. Unique scales that are relevant to themilieus <strong>of</strong> children, such as attitudetoward teachers <strong>and</strong> school <strong>and</strong> parentchildrelations3. A normative sample that is well-described<strong>and</strong> seems appropriately reflective <strong>of</strong> theUS population as <strong>of</strong> 2001.4. Good reliability estimates (see Reynolds& Kamphaus, 2004)5. Ease <strong>of</strong> administration <strong>and</strong> scoring.6. Items account for heterogeneity<strong>of</strong> behaviors <strong>and</strong> symptoms withindomains (McMahon & Frick, 2005).


CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIES111Box 6.2A Sample Case Illustrating the Interpretation <strong>of</strong> the BASC-2-SRPFarrah is a 14-year-old high school freshmanwho was recently discharged from a psychiatricinpatient unit, where she was hospitalizedfor suicidal statements <strong>and</strong> severe cuttingbehaviors.Farrah presented for the evaluation withdepressed mood <strong>and</strong> flat affect. She criedseveral times during the evaluation. She complainedabout her difficulty getting alongwith her mother <strong>and</strong> the fact that she rarelyhears from her father. Farrah reportedly hadbehavioral problems at school since a veryyoung age <strong>and</strong> tends to interact with peers <strong>of</strong>whom her mother does not approve. She hashad a recent increase in physical complaints<strong>and</strong> has missed several days <strong>of</strong> school this yearbecause <strong>of</strong> those complaints.Farrah’s BASC-2-SRP results were consistentwith background information: T-scores<strong>and</strong> brief descriptions <strong>of</strong> her results for elevatedscales are belowDepression 80This finding is consistent with observationsduring the evaluation <strong>and</strong> backgroundinformation suggesting recent suicidality <strong>and</strong>increased anhedonia.Sense <strong>of</strong> Inadequacy 74Farrah expressed a lack <strong>of</strong> confidence abouther ability to do well in school <strong>and</strong> succeed inother tasks. Her grades this year have beenmostly “Ds” <strong>and</strong> “Fs,” <strong>and</strong> she has a history <strong>of</strong>getting into trouble at school. However, hermeasured intellectual functioning <strong>and</strong> academicachievement are in the High Averagerange, suggesting that her lack <strong>of</strong> confidenceregarding schoolwork may be unrealistic.Attention Problems 68Farrah’s reports <strong>of</strong> difficulty concentrating onthe BASC-2 are consistent with her reportedproblems concentrating in general <strong>and</strong> herrelative inattentiveness during test.Attitude to School 74Farrah appears to have low self-efficacyregarding school, <strong>and</strong> she reports that schoolis boring.Attitude to Teacher 71During interview, Farrah described herteachers as uncaring <strong>and</strong> as having unrealisticexpectations.Locus <strong>of</strong> Control 76During an interview, she reported that sheis unfairly blamed for things at home <strong>and</strong> atschool <strong>and</strong> that her mother’s expectations aretoo high – reports that are consistent with herreports on the BASC-2.Somatization 86Farrah reportedly complains <strong>of</strong> nausea <strong>and</strong>headaches frequently. These complaints appearto be independent <strong>of</strong> her anti-depressant medicationregimen <strong>and</strong> also seem to be a strategy forher to avoid going to school.Social Stress 75Farrah reported that she usually feelsuncomfortable around others, includingpeople her own age. She also reported feelinglonely most <strong>of</strong> the time for the last twoyears.Relations with Parents 28This adaptive scale score is low <strong>and</strong> fits withFarrah’s long history <strong>of</strong> parent-child enmity.Self-Esteem 30Farrah views herself as less attractive thanothers <strong>and</strong> does not see herself as having anyparticular strengths.


112 CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIESTable 6.5 SRP Key Symptoms as Indicated by Items with the Highest Factor Loadings perScaleScaleAnxietyAttention ProblemsAttitude to SchoolAttitude to TeachersAtypicalityDepressionHyperactivityInterpersonal RelationsLocus <strong>of</strong> ControlRelations with ParentsSelf-EsteemSelf-RelianceSensation SeekingSense <strong>of</strong> InadequacySocial StressSomatizationKey SymptomsNervousness, being bothered by little things, worry, fearTrouble paying attention, getting in trouble for not paying attentionNot caring about school, feelings <strong>of</strong> wanting to quit school, expressions <strong>of</strong>school hatred, expressions <strong>of</strong> boredom at schoolFeeling that teachers are unfair, feeling that teacher is not proud <strong>of</strong> him/her, report that teacher gets mad at him/her for no reasonHearing voices/things that others cannot, feeling like someone is watching,seeing things.Feeling that does nothing right <strong>and</strong> that life is getting worse, feeling thatnothing goes his/her way.Is told by others to be still, having trouble sitting still, feeling like has tomove aroundFeeling that nobody likes respondent (reverse scored), feeling that othershate to be with him/her (reverse scored), being made fun <strong>of</strong> by others(reverse scored)Complains <strong>of</strong> being blamed for things that he or she can’t help or didn’tdo, people get mad at respondent for no reasonParents listen to what respondent says, parents are proud <strong>of</strong> him/her,parents trust him/herWishes he/she were different (reverse scored), respondent likes the way heor she looksSays that he or she is dependable <strong>and</strong> is good at making decisionsLikes it when dared to do something, likes to ride in a car going fastFails even when trying hard, wants to do be better but is unable, has difficultykeeping mind on school workFeels left out, feels that others find things wrong with him/her, people findthings wrong with him/herComplains <strong>of</strong> stomach upsets, nausea, <strong>and</strong> dizziness7. Availability <strong>of</strong> a range <strong>of</strong> derived scores<strong>and</strong> norms for general, clinical, <strong>and</strong>gender-referenced samples.8. Inclusion <strong>of</strong> validity scales that areintuitive.9. Exp<strong>and</strong>ed, user-friendly manual.10. A clear link between teacher <strong>and</strong> parentrating scales (BASC-2-PRS <strong>and</strong>BASC-2-TRS) which enhances its usein multi-informant assessment.To date, notable weaknesses <strong>of</strong> the BASC-2-SRP are:1. Lack <strong>of</strong> research regarding this version<strong>of</strong> the BASC <strong>and</strong> the improvementsmade from the original BASC-SRP2. Limited criterion-related validity evidence,particularly for the adaptive scales3. Lack <strong>of</strong> case studies in the manual4. Lack <strong>of</strong> validity evidence for the validityscales5. An excessive number <strong>of</strong> Self-Esteem scaleitems that deal with self-perceptions <strong>of</strong>personal appearance which is also a concernraised about the previous version <strong>of</strong>the SRP (Hoza, 1994)


CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIES113Achenbach System <strong>of</strong>Empirically Based <strong>Assessment</strong>Youth Self-Report (YSR;Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001)The Youth Self-Report (SRP; Achenbach& Rescorla, 2001) is one component <strong>of</strong> thelarger set <strong>of</strong> assessment instruments <strong>of</strong>feredwithin the Achenbach System <strong>of</strong> EmpiricallyBased <strong>Assessment</strong> that includes a parentrating scale, a teacher rating scale, anobservation scale, <strong>and</strong> other measures. TheYSR is designed for ages 11 through 18,<strong>and</strong> obtains adolescents’ reports about theirown competencies <strong>and</strong> problems in a formatsimilar to that <strong>of</strong> the parent CBCL <strong>and</strong>teacher TRF, discussed in the next chapter.As with their predecessors, the most recentmeasures in the Achenbach system haveconsiderable content overlap, which can beboth an advantage <strong>and</strong> disadvantage <strong>of</strong> thissystem, depending on what the clinician isseeking from behavioral rating scales.An additional form covers the ages <strong>of</strong>18–30 entitled the Young Adult Self-Report(YASR; Achenbach, 1997). The YASRcontent is similar to that <strong>of</strong> the YSR withnoteworthy differences such as scales forsubstance use <strong>and</strong> some unique adaptivefunctioning scales (e.g., Friends, Education,Job, etc.). In light <strong>of</strong> the developmentalperiods <strong>of</strong> focus in this text, the YASR willnot be discussed in detail here, but an introductionmay be found at http://www.aseba.org/products/yasr.html.Scale ContentComposite scores reflecting externalizing<strong>and</strong> internalizing dimensions <strong>and</strong> a totalcomposite are <strong>of</strong>fered. The following clinicalscales contribute to these composites.Withdrawn/Depressed- preferring to bealone, shy, sulks, sad, lacking energy, etc.Somatic Complaints- nausea, headaches,dizziness, etc.Anxious/Depressed- crying, fears, nervous,suicidal ideation, etc.Rule-Breaking Behavior- lying, substanceabuse, truancy, stealing, etc.Aggressive Behavior- teasing others,arguing, fighting, destruction <strong>of</strong> property,etc.Included in the Total Composite but notthe Internalizing or Externalizing compositesare the following:Social Problems- jealous <strong>of</strong> others,teased by others, clumsy, etc.Thought Problems- strange behaviors,hoarding objects, sleeping less, hallucinatoryexperiences, etc.Attention Problems- failing to finishassignments, immature, impulsivity, daydreaming,etc.Scales referred to as Social Competenceare also included that assess participationin a variety <strong>of</strong> activities (e.g., sports) <strong>and</strong>social interactions (e.g., friendships).The clinical scales are empiricallyderivedvia factor analysis, <strong>and</strong> the competencescales are rationally derived.Critical items (e.g., harming self, settingfires, etc.) are also available on this version<strong>of</strong> the YSR. In addition, six DSM-Oriented scales are available for theYSR. These scales were formed based onpsychiatrists’ impressions <strong>of</strong> items (seeAchenbach, Dumenci, & Rescorla, 2001)that theoretically map on to the DSMrelateddomains being assessed (i.e.,Affective Problems, Anxiety Problems,Somatic Problems, Attention/HyperactivityProblems, Oppositional DefiantProblems, Conduct Problems). Researchhas supported the structure <strong>of</strong> the syndromescales <strong>of</strong> the YSR across over 20cultures (Ivanova et al., 2007).Administration <strong>and</strong> ScoringThe YSR is designed to be self-administered<strong>and</strong> requires approximately 15–20min (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). TheYSR uses a three choice response format:“Not True, Somewhat/Sometimes True,<strong>and</strong> Very True or Often True.” Some items


114 CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIESalso provide space for additional informationsuch as, for example, “Describe:” following“I store up too many things I don’tneed.”Templates are used for h<strong>and</strong>-scoring,<strong>and</strong> computer scoring is also available. Anintegrative computer program can be usedto compare results for several raters (e.g.,a parent, two teachers, <strong>and</strong> the YSR), <strong>and</strong>this option is discussed in great detail byAchenbach <strong>and</strong> Rescorla (2001). The level<strong>of</strong> comparability facilitates the study <strong>of</strong>inter-rater agreement in clinical or researchsettings <strong>and</strong> aids in clinical interpretation<strong>of</strong> converging evidence <strong>and</strong> discrepanciesin reports <strong>of</strong> the child’s functioning.the range <strong>of</strong> low scores on the clinical scales<strong>and</strong> high scores on the competence scales.For example, T-scores for the clinical scaleswere not allowed to be articulated below avalue <strong>of</strong> 50. The transformation to reduceskewness <strong>and</strong> truncated score range bothserve to make the T-score distribution forthe YSR different from original sample results.The intent <strong>of</strong> this approach is to aid in interpretation<strong>of</strong> strengths <strong>and</strong> difficulties acrossdomains. However, this lack <strong>of</strong> reflection<strong>of</strong> sample characteristics in the T-scoresmakes them <strong>of</strong> dubious value for researchpurposes in particular. For most researchquestions, raw scores would likely be moreappropriate than normalized T-scores.NormingThe design <strong>of</strong> the YSR norming sampleattempted to mimic the national population<strong>of</strong> school children for ages 11 through18 in terms <strong>of</strong> SES, geographic region,<strong>and</strong> ethnicity. The sample included 1,057youth, 52% <strong>of</strong> whom were boys. <strong>Child</strong>renwho had reports <strong>of</strong> mental health, substanceabuse, or special education serviceswere excluded, thus making this a “normal”sample rather than a “normative sample.Most participants (i.e., 53%) were froma middle SES background, whereas 16%were from lower SES homes. The samplewas 60% White, 20% African American,8% Latino, <strong>and</strong> 11% Mixed or Other. Fromthese statistics, both African Americans <strong>and</strong>individuals identifying as Latino(a) appearto be underrepresented (Current PopulationSurvey, 2001). Approximately 40% <strong>of</strong>participants were from the southern part<strong>of</strong> the United States with the Northeast,Midwest, West each being represented byapproximately 20% <strong>of</strong> the sample participants(see Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001).The derived T-scores for the YSR arenormalized, which results in changing theshape <strong>of</strong> the raw score distribution (i.e., reducingskewness). Furthermore, the T-scoredistributions are truncated, which limitsReliabilityInternal consistency estimates are reasonablefor the clinical scales falling between.71 <strong>and</strong> .89. The internal consistencies <strong>of</strong>the composites are all above .90. Internalconsistency estimates are somewhat lowerfor the competence scales (see Achenbach& Rescorla, 2001).Short-term (i.e., approximately oneweekinterval) test-retest coefficients aregenerally good, with only the Withdrawn/Depressed scale having a coefficient below.70. Seven-month test-retest coefficientswere adequate with coefficients generallyin the .50 range. Coefficients for the Withdrawn/Depressedscale <strong>and</strong> Somatic Complaintsscale were somewhat lower.ValidityThe YSR manual does not report evidence<strong>of</strong> criterion-related validity, particularly inregards to the correspondence between theYSR <strong>and</strong> other measures <strong>of</strong> emotional <strong>and</strong>behavioral functioning.Some differential validity data arepresented, with the scales <strong>of</strong> the YSRconsistently differentiating between clinicreferred<strong>and</strong> non-referred youth. Exceptionsto this differential validity were the


CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIES115DSM-Oriented Anxiety Problems <strong>and</strong>Somatic Problems scales. The ability <strong>of</strong> theYSR to differentiate among clinical groupsis not addressed. Achenbach <strong>and</strong> Rescorla(2001) indicate that differential validity isthe driving force behind content selectionfor the current YSR <strong>and</strong> its predecessors.The most recent YSR has six items thatdiffer from the items in the previous YSR.Generally speaking, the validity evidencereported in the manual concerning the YSRis minimal. However, the previous version<strong>of</strong> the YSR enjoys a great deal <strong>of</strong> validityevidence from independent researchers. Inaddition, in light <strong>of</strong> the close item correspondencebetween the two versions, onecan surmise that support for the validity <strong>of</strong>the earlier YSR can be taken as providingsome support for the current YSR, particularlythe problem scales.A study by Thurber <strong>and</strong> Hollingsworth(1992) compared YSR results withthe results <strong>of</strong> several other measures (e.g.,California <strong>Personality</strong> Inventory <strong>and</strong> BeckDepression Inventory) in a factor-analyticinvestigation. The sample for this studyincluded 102 adolescent inpatients. Supportfor the existence <strong>of</strong> the internalizing<strong>and</strong> externalizing dimensions was found,as these factors converged with measures<strong>of</strong> similar constructs to form recognizablefactors. Of interest was an additionalfinding that the Externalizing Scale maybe affected by a tendency to respond in asocially desirable way <strong>and</strong> deny problems.The Internalizing Scale also showed somesensitivity to response sets in that it wasaffected somewhat by minimizing symptoms(Thurber & Hollingsworth, 1992).Brown (1999) likewise found that “highrisk”adolescents tended to underreportbehavior problems when school records<strong>and</strong> police reports were used as externalcriteria. <strong>Adolescent</strong> reports tended to agreewith other reports for “more positivelyoriented items.” These findings shouldbe taken into account when interpretingself-report results <strong>and</strong> should be combinedwith corroborating evidence in drawingconclusions.In contrast, Sour<strong>and</strong>er, Helstelae, <strong>and</strong>Helenius (1999) found that Finnish adolescentsreported significantly more problemsthan their parents, <strong>and</strong> girls reported moredistress, especially internalizing problems,than boys. These authors concluded thatmany adolescents may not be receivingappropriate mental health services becausetheir problems go unrecognized by theirparents.A criterion-related validity study byH<strong>and</strong>werk, Friman, <strong>and</strong> Larzelere (2000)compared the YSR to the NIMH DiagnosticInterview Schedule for <strong>Child</strong>ren(DISC). They compared DISC <strong>and</strong> YSRresults to behavior in a treatment program<strong>and</strong>, generally speaking, found no differencesbetween diagnostic groups formedby using either instrument. Similar resultshave also been found for the YSR in comparisonto the DISC Version 2.1 (Morgan& Cauce, 1999).More validity studies exist for variouscultural groups on the previous version <strong>of</strong>the YSR. Reliability <strong>and</strong> factorial validity <strong>of</strong>the YSR have been found to be comparableto North American findings in the Netherl<strong>and</strong>s(de Groot, Koot, & Verhulst, 1996),Switzerl<strong>and</strong> (Steinhausen & Metzke, 1998),Japan (Kuramoto et al. 1999), <strong>and</strong> Spain(Abad, Forns, Amador, & Martorell, 2000).Research has led to the conclusion, giventhat sex differences appear very consistentlyon the YSR, that sex is a more importantconsideration in predicting psychopathologythan demographic factors such as age ornationality (Steinhausen & Metzke, 1998).However, unlike the teacher <strong>and</strong> parentreport measures <strong>of</strong> the Achenbach system,the YSR does not include gender-specificnorms.A predictive validity study <strong>of</strong> the previousYSR was conducted in Finl<strong>and</strong>, where121 adolescents were administered theYSR at age 14 or 15 <strong>and</strong> followed up toages 20 <strong>and</strong> 21 (Aronen, Teerikangas, &


116 CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIESKurkela, 1999). YSR problems were goodpredictors <strong>of</strong> adult symptomatology. Ofequal interest are the findings that internalizingsymptoms were better predictors<strong>and</strong> that self-report was more predictivethan parent report. Results such as thesespecifically support the practical utility <strong>of</strong>the YSR, <strong>and</strong> they suggest that self-reporttechnology should be used with all children<strong>and</strong> adolescents with adequate readingcomprehension.InterpretationThe Achenbach manual provides some casestudies, yet it does not provide interpretiveguidance. This omission is remediedto some extent by the existence <strong>of</strong> otherarticles on the YSR <strong>and</strong> by its amenabilityto general interpretive approaches such asthose described earlier in this chapter.A strength <strong>of</strong> the Achenbach approachto scale construction is the ease withwhich interpretations can be made acrossinformants because <strong>of</strong> the close item correspondenceon the different forms. Asmentioned above, Achenbach <strong>and</strong> Rescorla(2001) detail the approach by which statisticalcomparisons across informants may bemade. To the extent that informants agreeon the presence <strong>of</strong> a problem, the clinicianmay be more confident that a problem warrantingattention exists.However, it is possible to be too heavilyinfluenced by indexes <strong>of</strong> agreement inthe interpretive process. For example, onemight require agreement across raters tomake diagnostic or other decisions. Weprefer to consider each rater as a valuablesource <strong>of</strong> information that may bediagnostically or otherwise valuable forcase conceptualization in its own right,when combined with other information.To illustrate, if a clinician requires parentalagreement for a self-report finding, achild or adolescent may be denied neededservices (Sour<strong>and</strong>er et al., 1999). Youthmay be rich <strong>and</strong> valid sources <strong>of</strong> informationabout their emotional <strong>and</strong> behavioralfunctioning (Aronen et al., 1999; Barryet al., in press).Convergence across informants hasmerits for answering a referral question<strong>and</strong> making recommendations. However,we also hold to the philosophy that differentraters make unique contributions tothe underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> a child’s referral difficulties(see Chap. 15) <strong>and</strong> that disagreementamong informants is not necessarilyindicative <strong>of</strong> a measurement problem.Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesThe YSR has several strengths:1. Brief administration time2. A large research base on its closelyrelatedpredecessor3. Research conducted with individualsrepresenting many cultures <strong>and</strong> normsfrom a number <strong>of</strong> cultural groups4. A large base <strong>of</strong> experienced users5. Considerable item overlap with its parent<strong>and</strong> teacher report counterparts, whichaids in cross-informant interpretation6. A helpful Web site with information foradministration, purchasing, interpretation,<strong>and</strong> user discussion is available athttp://www.aseba.org/products/ysr.htmlWeaknesses <strong>of</strong> the YSR include:1. Little assessment <strong>of</strong> school-relatedproblems2. Limited assessment <strong>of</strong> adaptive competencies3. The absence <strong>of</strong> validity scales4. Limited construct validity evidence todate for the current YSR.5. Direct comparisons <strong>of</strong> norm sampledemographics to US population are notprovided; however, African Americans<strong>and</strong> Latino(a)s appear to be under-represented.


CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIES117Minnesota Multiphasic<strong>Personality</strong> Inventory-<strong>Adolescent</strong> (MMPI-A; Butcheret al., 1992)The Minnesota Multiphasic <strong>Personality</strong>Inventory-<strong>Adolescent</strong> (MMPI-A; Butcheret al.) has strong roots in the original work<strong>of</strong> Hathaway <strong>and</strong> McKinley (1940), <strong>and</strong>the authors <strong>of</strong> MMPI-A tried to maintaincontinuity with the original work (Butcheret al.). This objective was achieved, as all <strong>of</strong>the clinical <strong>and</strong> validity scales were retained.In addition, the MMPI-A includes contentscales (discussed below) which may be usefulfor addressing many clinical questions<strong>of</strong> interest. The MMPI-A is designed foryouth aged 14–18.Scale ContentKnowledge <strong>of</strong> the history, rationale, psychometricproperties, <strong>and</strong> item content <strong>of</strong>the MMPI-A clinical scales is importantfor proper interpretation. Consequently,each <strong>of</strong> the featured clinical scales is discussedin turn.Scale 1, Hs: Hypochondriasis. Items forthis scale were originally developed toidentify respondents with a history <strong>of</strong>symptomatology characteristic <strong>of</strong> hypochondriasis(Butcher et al., 1992). Items <strong>of</strong>this scale assess topics such as nausea, vomiting,upset stomach, sleep problems, chestpain, numbness, muscle twitching, bodilytenderness, dizziness, weakness, <strong>and</strong> lack <strong>of</strong>general feeling <strong>of</strong> wellness.Scale 2, D: Depression. Hathaway <strong>and</strong>McKinley (1942) described this measureas an index <strong>of</strong> general dissatisfaction withone’s life, including feelings <strong>of</strong> discouragement,hopelessness, <strong>and</strong> low morale. Itemcontent includes appetite changes, healthworries, anhedonia, work problems, tension,constipation, increased swearing,concentration problems, sleep problems,withdrawal, teasing animals, low self-confidence,low self-esteem, worry at bedtime,crying easily, decreased reading comprehension,weight change, <strong>and</strong> impairedmemory, among other things. The itemcontent <strong>of</strong> this scale is diverse, reachingfar beyond diagnostic criteria for depressionsuch as those included in the DSM<strong>and</strong> likely contributes to mediocre internalconsistency reliability coefficients.Scale 3, Hy: Hysteria. According toButcher et al. (1992), this scale consists <strong>of</strong>60 items that were originally selected toidentify individuals who respond to stresswith hysterical reactions that include sensoryor motor disorders without an organicbasis. Some <strong>of</strong> the item content includespoor appetite, fatigue, cold extremities,decreased work productivity, nausea <strong>and</strong>vomiting, urges to curse, poor concentration,disturbed sleep, health concerns,chest pain, unhappiness, difficulty persuadingothers, muscle twitching, irritability,worry about contracting diseases, dysfunctionalrelationships with family members,concern about others’ opinion, <strong>and</strong> dislike<strong>of</strong> school. Like D (scale 2), this item poolis very diverse, resulting in mediocre internalconsistency coefficients. It should alsobe noted that the first three scales <strong>of</strong> theMMPI-A share considerable item overlap.Scale 4, Pd: Psychopathic Deviate. Thisscale was originally constructed based onthe responses <strong>of</strong> individuals with histories<strong>of</strong> lying, stealing, sexual promiscuity, <strong>and</strong>alcohol abuse (Butcher et al., 1992). Furthermore,high scores on this scale areassociated with family, legal, <strong>and</strong> schooldifficulties (Butcher et al.). Item contentincludes loss <strong>of</strong> interest in daily activities,a desire to leave home, feeling misunderstood,feeling used, poor concentration,having unusual experiences, history <strong>of</strong>trouble because <strong>of</strong> sexual behavior, history<strong>of</strong> stealing, unhappiness, disapprovalby family members, winning arguments,inability to tolerate ridicule, regrettingactions, admissions <strong>of</strong> misbehavior, history<strong>of</strong> school disciplinary action, feeling likesomeone is out to get him/her, <strong>and</strong> weight


118 CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIESchanges. For the most part, this is the firstscale with item content that is distinctivefrom the first three.Scale 5, Mf Masculinity-Femininity. Thisscale was originally developed on a sample<strong>of</strong> adult men, described by Hathaway(1956) as “male sexual inverts” (Butcheret al., 1992). Presumably, males with highscores are more feminine, <strong>and</strong> women withclinically significant scores are thought tohave more masculine interests. Item contentincludes topics such as lack <strong>of</strong> interestin mechanics magazines, reluctance toincriminate oneself, a desire to be <strong>of</strong> theopposite gender, interest in love stories<strong>and</strong> poetry, sensitive feelings, lack <strong>of</strong> interestin forest ranger work, being a soldieror hunter, expressing the need to argue tomake a point, attending few parties, dislikefor wagering, interest in gardening <strong>and</strong>cooking, maintaining a diary, fear <strong>of</strong> snakes,worry, <strong>and</strong> talks about sex. This scale is, tosay the least, unique, <strong>and</strong>, by most currentunderst<strong>and</strong>ing, out <strong>of</strong> step with the times.Therefore, it is not particularly useful forclinical interpretation.Scale 6, Pa: Paranoia. This scale isdesigned to assess paranoid symptomatology.The scale includes item contentsuch as feelings <strong>of</strong> persecution, havingevil thoughts, feeling misunderstood,emotional lability, feeling possessed byevil spirits, unhappiness, sensation seeking,distrust <strong>of</strong> others, crying easily, feelingas though one is being followed orpoisoned by someone, ideas <strong>of</strong> reference,<strong>and</strong> history <strong>of</strong> legal trouble.Scale 7, Pt: Psychasthenia. This scaleassesses anxiety, particularly a tendencyto worry obsessively. The item content <strong>of</strong>Scale 7 includes health worries, loss <strong>of</strong> interestin activities, having shameful thoughts,emotional lability, poor concentration,fatigue, unhappiness, low self-esteem,feelings <strong>of</strong> regret, guilt, impaired readingcomprehension, impaired memory, worry,restlessness, excitability, fear <strong>of</strong> speaking infront <strong>of</strong> others, being easily embarrassed,impatience, counting unimportant things,rumination, <strong>and</strong> overreaction to failure.This scale has many items in common withscales 2, 3, <strong>and</strong> 8.Scale 8, Sc: Schizophrenia. This scale wasdesigned to identify patients with diagnoses<strong>of</strong> various forms <strong>of</strong> psychosis (Butcheret al., 1992). Scale 8 items sample content suchas lack <strong>of</strong> interest in daily activities, havingunwanted thoughts, desire to leave home,poor concentration, bizarre experiences,stealing, feelings <strong>of</strong> persecution, avoidance<strong>of</strong> others, day-dreaming, muscle twitching,urges to do something socially unacceptable,changes in speech pattern, decreasedreading comprehension, impaired memory,blackout spells, fear <strong>of</strong> losing control,impaired balance, restlessness, difficultyinitiating activity, excitability, numbness,de-creased taste sensitivity, sexual preoccupations,impaired relationships with parents<strong>and</strong> other family members, loneliness,lack <strong>of</strong> intimacy, impatience, <strong>and</strong> feelings<strong>of</strong> unreality. This scale shares many itemswith scales 2, 3, <strong>and</strong> 7. It is a long scale with77 items, making item overlap with otherscales a central characteristic. Its high correlationswith other scales are discussed ina later section. Some <strong>of</strong> the items that differentiatethis scale from others have to dowith impaired social relationships <strong>and</strong> poorreality contact.Scale 9, Ma: Hypomania. This scaleassesses a tendency toward excitability <strong>and</strong>includes items assessing tension, desire toleave home, crying spells, urges to do somethingsocially unacceptable, indecision,sensation seeking, racing thoughts, feelings<strong>of</strong> persecution, lack <strong>of</strong> fear <strong>of</strong> heights,blackout spells, occasional ability to makedecisions very easily, self-righteousness,restlessness, satisfaction with personalappearance, sweatiness, excitability, excessivethirst, <strong>and</strong> admiration for clevernesseven if it is criminal.Scale 10, Si: Social Introversion. Si scoreelevations are produced by content suchas failure to face crises or problems, poor


CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIES119concentration, poor sociability, unhappiness,fear <strong>of</strong> ridicule, lack <strong>of</strong> interest inparties, easily losing arguments, low sensationseeking, change in speech pattern,distrust <strong>of</strong> others, indecision, shyness, difficultywith small talk, brooding, concernsabout personal appearance, embarrassmentin front <strong>of</strong> groups, failure to initiate conversation,difficulty making friends, loneliness,envy <strong>of</strong> others’ successes, <strong>and</strong> lowself-esteem.Content scales are a relatively uniquefeature <strong>of</strong> the MMPI that were developeddifferently from the original clinical scales.Whereas empirical approaches, includingempirical criterion keying, were usedfor the development <strong>of</strong> the original scales,content scales depend more on a rational/theoretical approach to test development(Williams et al., 1992) in which scale contentconsiders both empirical factor loadings<strong>and</strong> homogeneity <strong>of</strong> content withinscales.The first step in the content scale developmentprocess was to select 22 contentcategories based on a review <strong>of</strong> the adultexperimental version (the predecessor tothe MMPI-2). In the second step, a total <strong>of</strong>three raters assigned items from the adultexperimental form to the 22 categories(Williams et al., 1992). A group consensuswas reached on the assignments, <strong>and</strong> someitems were discarded. A total <strong>of</strong> 21 contentscales remained after this step. In the nextstep, correlations <strong>and</strong> reliability indiceswere used to enhance the reliability <strong>and</strong>homogeneity <strong>of</strong> each scale. The fourthstage involved another “rational review”<strong>of</strong> the items in response to the aforementionedstatistical data. Some scales wererenamed <strong>and</strong> some dropped at this stage.In the fifth <strong>and</strong> final step, items that correlatedhigher with a scale <strong>of</strong> which theywere not a member were removed. Theresult was 15 MMPI-2 content scales forthe adult measure (Williams et al., 1992).These same procedures were applied tothe development <strong>of</strong> the MMPI-A contentscales, with the MMPI-2 content scalesserving as the foundation. Items wereadded <strong>and</strong> removed, <strong>and</strong> some new scaleswere developed (Williams et al., 1992).This step resulted in the retention <strong>of</strong> themajority <strong>of</strong> the MMPI-2 content scales foradolescents <strong>and</strong> the addition <strong>of</strong> three scales-Alienation, Low Aspirations, <strong>and</strong> SchoolProblems. Descriptions <strong>of</strong> the MMPI-Acontent scales are shown in Table 6.6.There are also six supplementary scaleson the MMPI-A (Butcher et al., 1992):Anxiety (A): distress, discomfort, conformity,being upset by social situations.Repression (R): tendency toward submissiveness<strong>and</strong> conventionality, avoidance<strong>of</strong> conflictMAC-R, MacAndrew AlcoholismScale-Revised: substance abuse problems,willingness to take risks, extraversionAlcohol/Drug Problem Acknowledgement(ACK): items that directly refer to drug<strong>and</strong> alcohol useAlcohol/Drug Problem Proneness(PRO): stimulus seeking, negative peergroup influence, rule-breaking, negativeattitudes toward achievementImmaturity (IMM): orientation to thepresent instead <strong>of</strong> future, lack <strong>of</strong> insight,hostility, self-centerednessAdministration <strong>and</strong> ScoringThe MMPI-A is unusually long (i.e., 478items) compared to other self-reportinventories designed for children <strong>and</strong> adolescents,which calls for special administrationguidelines. In total, the MMPI-Atakes approximately 90 min to administer,<strong>and</strong> some adolescents may have to takethe test in more than one session (Butcheret al., 1992). Furthermore, because manyadolescents require supervision duringthe administration <strong>of</strong> these scales, considerablymore examiner time may berequired. Substantial administration timesavings, however, can be gained by using


120 CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIESTable 6.6 MMPI-A Content ScalesScaleAnxiety (A-anx)Obsessiveness (A-obs)Depression (A-dep)Health Concerns (A-hea)Alienation (A-aln)Bizarre Mentation (A-biz)Anger (A-ang)Cynicism (A-cyn)Conduct Problems (A-con)Low self-esteem (A-lse)Low Aspirations (A-las)Social Discomfort (A-sod)Family Problems (A-fam)School Problems (A-sch)Negative Treatment Indicators (A-trt)DescriptionIncludes excessive worry, problems sleeping, problemsconcentrating, tensionUnreasonable worry, rumination, difficulty making decisions,reports that others are impatient with them, regretIncludes frequent crying, fatigue, self-deprecatingthoughts, hopelessnessPhysical complaints including nausea, dizziness, constipation,difficulty hearing, headachesFeeling disliked <strong>and</strong> misunderstood by others, feeling thatothers are out to get them, preferring to be aloneStrange thoughts <strong>and</strong> experiences, hallucinations, paranoiaStarting fights, cursing, destroying things, irritability,impatience with othersMistrust <strong>of</strong> others, feeling that others are unfair, feelingthatothers are jealousStealing, lying, disobeying rules, shoplifting, being disrespectfultoward othersFeeling unattractive, lacking self-confidence, feelings <strong>of</strong>uselessnessDislike <strong>of</strong> studying <strong>and</strong> reading, giving up quickly, difficultystarting tasksShyness, avoidance <strong>of</strong> others, dislike <strong>of</strong> crowds or socialgatheringsFamily discord, feeling that one cannot depend on familymembers, jealousy, limited family communicationPoor grades, negative attitudes toward teachers, suspensions,truancy, belief that school is a waste <strong>of</strong> time.Negative attitudes toward doctors <strong>and</strong> mental healthpr<strong>of</strong>essionals, feeling that faults <strong>and</strong> bad habits cannot beovercome, unwillingness to face problemsFrom Butcher et al. (1992).a computerized adaptive administrationformat (Forhey, H<strong>and</strong>el, & Ben-Porath,2000). In fact, time savings <strong>of</strong> 50–123items may be possible, with research suggestingno significant differences in averagescale scores or in the distributions <strong>of</strong>scale scores (Hays & McCallum, 2005).Checks on the adolescent’s readingcomprehension level are also required.Readability analyses <strong>of</strong> individual itemsshow readability at approximately the fifthto sixth grade level in most cases. However,when in doubt, an examiner may ask thechild to read some items aloud to get somesense <strong>of</strong> the child’s ability to comprehendthe item content. The validity checks provideanother useful alert to possible readabilityproblems.


CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIES121Validity ScalesThe MMPI series has a long tradition <strong>of</strong>the use <strong>of</strong> validity indexes which is reflectedin the adolescent version. Brief descriptions<strong>of</strong> the validity scales follow (Butcheret al., 1992):Cannot Say (?). This scale is comprised <strong>of</strong>the total number <strong>of</strong> items that the respondenteither failed to answer or endorsedas both true <strong>and</strong> false. If there are a largenumber <strong>of</strong> items fitting this description,the clinician should attempt to ascertainthe reason (e.g., carelessness, discomfort,difficulty with comprehension, defiance).LIE (L). This scale is intended to detectnaive attempts by adolescents to put themselvesin an overly favorable light.F, Fl, <strong>and</strong> F2 (Infrequency). The F scaleis the antithesis <strong>of</strong> the L scale in that itassesses the tendency <strong>of</strong> individuals toplace themselves in an unfavorable light,or “fake bad.” Items were selected for thisscale if they were endorsed in their deviantdirection by less than 20% <strong>of</strong> the normativesample.K (Defensiveness). According to Butcheret al. (1992), “This scale was designedoriginally to identify adults in psychiatricsettings who displayed significant degrees<strong>of</strong> psychopathology, but produced pr<strong>of</strong>ilesthat were within normal limits” (Meehl& Hathaway, 1946, p. 40). Butcher et al.(1992), however, suggest that an MMPI-Apr<strong>of</strong>ile should not be invalidated solely onthe basis <strong>of</strong> an elevated K score, particularlyif used with individuals who are notin a restrictive mental health or psychiatricsetting.VRIN (Variable Response Inconsistency).The VRN scale consists <strong>of</strong> pairs <strong>of</strong> itemsthat have either similar or opposing itemcontent. The score yielded by the VRINscale reflects the number <strong>of</strong> item pairsanswered inconsistently. A high score mayreveal a careless response style on the part<strong>of</strong> the client.TRIN (True Response Inconsistency). Thisscale is analogous to the VRIN scale inthat it is made up <strong>of</strong> pairs <strong>of</strong> items. It differsin that the TRIN scale consists solely<strong>of</strong> items with opposite content. An elevatedscore may reveal an acquiescence responseset, or the tendency for the test subject toindiscriminately answer True to the items.Conversely, a low TRIN score may revealnon-acquiescence.Some validity evidence exists to supportthe use <strong>of</strong> MMPI-A validity scales; astrength <strong>of</strong> these scales relative to similarscales from other measures. The L <strong>and</strong> Kscales have been shown to be reasonablygood at assessing symptom underreporting(Baer, Ballenger, & Kroll, 1998; Stein& Graham, 2005). Validity scale cut scoreshad to be lowered somewhat to detecta fake good response set when evaluatingadolescents in a correctional facility(Stein & Graham, 1999). The F, Fl, F2,<strong>and</strong> VRIN scales were best for assessingr<strong>and</strong>om responding for a sample <strong>of</strong> 354adolescents (Archer & Elkins, 1999).NormingThe MMPI-A was normed in eight statesin the continental United States on 1,620adolescents. One state, however (Washington),contributed only 14 cases to thenorming.The distribution <strong>of</strong> the sample byvariables such as gender, age, grade, <strong>and</strong>parental education <strong>and</strong> occupation aregiven in the manual. These variables werenot, however, used as stratification variablesin order to match US Census orother criteria as is common for clinical testinstruments. The Hispanic population, forexample, is clearly under-sampled, constitutingonly 2.2% <strong>of</strong> the female sample <strong>and</strong>2.0% <strong>of</strong> the male sample, which is smallerthan the sample <strong>of</strong> Native American children.However, a great deal <strong>of</strong> subsequentresearch on the MMPI-A has been conductedwith Hispanic individuals, increasingthe confidence one can have in usingthis instrument with Hispanic clients (see


122 CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIESButcher, Cabiya, Lucio, & Garrido, 2007).Similarly, the SES distribution may beskewed toward higher levels <strong>of</strong> SES thanthe national population. The authors notedthat, “This rough classification <strong>of</strong> occupationssuggests that mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers aredescribed by many children as having pr<strong>of</strong>essional<strong>and</strong> managerial occupations, whilerelatively low percentages are recorded forthe homemaker <strong>and</strong> unskilled” (Butcheret al., 1992, p. 13). The age distribution <strong>of</strong>the sample is also highly variable. At age18, only 42 male cases <strong>and</strong> 45 female caseswere collected.The small sample at age 18 may contributeto flawed estimates <strong>of</strong> psychopathology.A study by Shaevel <strong>and</strong> Archer(1996) revealed that 18-year olds scoredsubstantially differently on the MMPI-A<strong>and</strong> MMPI-2. More evidence <strong>of</strong> pathologywas obtained on the MMPI-2 with correspondinglylower validity scale values.Differences in T-scores between the twoinstruments were sometimes as high as 15points for the same scales.At the opposite end <strong>of</strong> the age range, onestudy <strong>of</strong> an inpatient population <strong>of</strong> 13-year oldsfound little difference in scores in comparisonto a matched group <strong>of</strong> 14-year olds (Janus, deGroot, & Toepfer, 1998). Another investigationfound little effect <strong>of</strong> demographic variableson MMPI-A T-scores (Schinka, Elkins,& Archer, 1998). However, Archer (2005) discussedthe tendency across several samples forsymptom endorsement on the MMPI-A to beinversely correlated with age.The norm sample also included 193individuals who had received mental healthservices, leading to a relatively large proportion<strong>of</strong> adolescents who do not appearelevated on the MMPI-A (Archer, 2005).However, removing these individuals <strong>and</strong>recalculating norms does not appear tochange the pattern <strong>of</strong> results a great deal(H<strong>and</strong>, Archer, H<strong>and</strong>el, & Forbey, 2007).The US clinical normative sample consisted<strong>of</strong> 420 boys <strong>and</strong> 293 girls. All <strong>of</strong> theclinical cases were taken from the Minneapolisarea (Butcher et al., 1992). Furtherdetails regarding the clinical sample can befound in Williams et al. (1992). The majority<strong>of</strong> cases (i.e., 71% <strong>of</strong> the boys <strong>and</strong> 56%<strong>of</strong> the girls) were undergoing treatment inalcohol/drug units (Williams et al., 1992),suggesting that the clinical sample couldbe reconceptualzied to more accuratelyreflect the preponderance <strong>of</strong> substanceabuse cases.ReliabilityThere are distinct scale differences inthe internal consistency estimates for theMMPI-A (see Table 6.7). Some <strong>of</strong> the clinicalscales (e.g., Hs, Pt, Si) have respectableestimates. In direct contrast, some <strong>of</strong> thescales have internal consistency estimatesthat raise questions about their content.The desirability <strong>of</strong> including a scale thatpossesses more error than reliable varianceis not clear. The Mf coefficients <strong>of</strong>.43 (boys) <strong>and</strong> .40 (girls) are the worst <strong>of</strong>those reported. The Pa, D, <strong>and</strong> Ma scalesare also less reliable than most <strong>of</strong> the scalesdescribed in this chapter.Internal consistency estimates for thevalidity scales range from unacceptably lowto impressively high, with most being moderate(.70s <strong>and</strong> .80s). According to Butcheret al. (1992), the lowest coefficients wereobtained for the L scale, where coefficientsranged from .53 in the female clinical sampleto .64 in the male normative sample. Incontrast, the F scale produced coefficientsranging from .81 (female clinical sample)to .90 (male normative sample).The internal consistency estimates forthe “content” scales <strong>of</strong> the MMPI-A aregenerally better than those for the originalclinical scales (see Table 6.7). The A-depscale coefficients are considerably betterthan those <strong>of</strong> the original D scale, rangingfrom a low <strong>of</strong> .80 for the normative sample<strong>of</strong> boys to a high <strong>of</strong> .89 for the clinical sample<strong>of</strong> girls (see Butcher et al., 1992).The lowest internal consistencies <strong>of</strong> thecontent scales are produced by the A-las


CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIES123Table 6.7 MMPI-A Median Internal Consistency Reliability Estimates<strong>Clinical</strong> Scale Boys (N = 805) Girls (N = 815) Content Scale Girls <strong>and</strong> BoysScale 1, Hs .78 .79 A-anx .80Scale 2, D .65 .66 A-ohs .74Scale 3, Hy .63 .55 A-dep .83Scale 4, Pd .63 .68 A-hea .82Scale 5, Mf .43 .40 A-aln .74Scale 6, Pa .57 .59 A-biz .75Scale 7, Pt .84 .86 A-ang .72Scale 8, Sc .88 .89 A-cyn .80Scale 9, Ma .61 .61 A-con .73Scale 10, Si .79 .80 A-lse .74A-las .61A-sod .78A-fam .82A-sch .70A-trt .76A .89R .53MAC-R .48ACK .66PRO .69IMM .82Note: From Butcher et al. (1992).scale, which has coefficients ranging from.55 to .66. These coefficients, however, arebetter than those <strong>of</strong> the MF clinical scale.Some <strong>of</strong> the supplementary scales arealso plagued by poor reliability estimates.The revised MacAndrew (MAC-R) scaleyields a median coefficient <strong>of</strong> .48, whichis, again, lower than most <strong>of</strong> the scalescited in this chapter. This lack <strong>of</strong> reliabilityalso makes the MAC-R scale difficultto validate because reliability is a necessarycondition for validity. The MMPI-A manualcautions that a cut-<strong>of</strong>f raw score <strong>of</strong> 28 onthe MAC-R may result in false positives;the existence <strong>of</strong> such poor reliability estimatesmakes one question the reliability <strong>of</strong>any cut score or, for that matter, the inclusion<strong>of</strong> the scale. Reliability coefficients inthe .40s are typically not seen as adequatefor clinical decision making.If one orders all <strong>of</strong> the MMPI-A clinical<strong>and</strong> content scales by their reliabilityestimates, some implications for interpretationbecome clear. Scales can be groupedby reliability coefficients with guidancefor interpretation as shown:This reliability-based interpretivehierarchy is, <strong>of</strong> course, overly simplisticbecause the validity <strong>of</strong> these scales is notequivalent for all purposes. The hierarchy,however, is useful in that there is a relationbetween reliability <strong>and</strong> validity. The fourscales with median coefficients below .60are less likely to be the beneficiaries <strong>of</strong> substantialvalidity evidence, as will be notedin the next section.


124 CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIESGo o d Re l i a b i l i t y (median coefficient .80)Scale 7, PtScale 8, ScA-anxA-depA-cynA-heaA-famAAd e q u at e Re l i a b i l i t y(median coefficient = .70 to .79)Scale 1, HsScale 10, SiA-obsA-alnA-bizA-angA-conA-lseA-sodA-schA-trtPo o r Re l i a b i l i t y (median coefficient = .60to .69)Scale 2, DScale 3, HyScale 4, PdScale 9, MaA-1asACK PROIn a d e q u at e Re l i a b i l i t y (median coefficient.59)Scale 5, MfScale 6, PaRMAC-RFor the MMPI-A, the preceding chartssuggest that the clinician could have moreconfidence in the information obtainedfrom the content scales than the clinicalscales. The higher internal consistency reliability<strong>of</strong> the content scales may very wellbe an artifact <strong>of</strong> the sometimes substantialitem overlap in the clinical scales. In light<strong>of</strong> this issue, for clinical scale elevations,caution must be taken to determine whatsorts <strong>of</strong> symptoms led to the elevations.As is typical for such scales, test-retestcoefficients differ from internal consistencyestimates. Test-retest coefficientsare somewhat more difficult to interpret,however, because it is unclear whether ornot some scales measure traits that theoreticallyshould be stable over at least shortperiods <strong>of</strong> time. Regardless, test-retest datacan be <strong>of</strong> value when gauging changes fromone evaluation to another.One scenario might involve an adolescentwho was hospitalized with paranoidideation that was reflected by a highT-score (78) on the Pa scale. It is conceivablethat this individual would obtain alower score <strong>of</strong> 61 on re-test prior to dischargetwo weeks after the initial assessment.One interpretation <strong>of</strong> these results isthat treatment has been effective. Anotherinterpretation is that Pa scale results arerelatively unstable (r = .65) <strong>and</strong> that theT-score <strong>of</strong> 78 was spuriously high or the61 was erroneously low. These test-retestdata do provide an alternative hypothesisfor this score difference that, in this case,may have implications for discharge planning.In such a scenario, when MMPI-Aresults may not be well-corroborated byother clinical findings, more careful outpatientfollow-up may be warranted to ensurethat paraniod ideation has abated significantlyenough so as to not adversely affectfunctioning in school or other settings.Overall, the reliability estimates for theMMPI-A are more variable than might beexpected. Such variability requires a morediscerning user who evaluates the reliability


CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIES125<strong>of</strong> results on a scale-by-scale basis, whichwould not be necessary with more uniformreliability coefficients. It is also noteworthythat the new content scales appear to bemore reliable on average than the originalclinical scales. The user may more confidentlyassume that the content scales possessadequate reliability.ValidityAn important fact to keep in mind wheninterpreting the factor structure <strong>of</strong> theMMPI-A is the extent <strong>of</strong> item overlap(Archer, Belevich, & Elkins, 1994). Theclinical scales were designed with manyoverlapping items that serve to strengthenthe correlation between the scales. Item31, for example, is included on six scales(2, 3, 4, 7, 8, 10). An analogous situationwould be to have some WISC-IV itemsincluded on several subtests or composites.It is difficult to imagine, but what if severalBlock Design items were allowed, because<strong>of</strong> their correlations with the Verbal Comprehensionsubtests, to be included in calculations<strong>of</strong> the Verbal ComprehensionIndex? Such a move would probably begreeted by skepticism, causing cliniciansto wonder about the distinction betweenthe Verbal Comprehension <strong>and</strong> PerceptualReasoning Indexes.Analogously, scales 7 (Pt) <strong>and</strong> 8 (Sc)correlate highly with one another at .85for females <strong>and</strong> .83 for males (Butcheret al., 1992). As might be expected, thesescales both “load” highly on the first factor.However, these scales have 17 items incommon, which parsimoniously explainsthe similar factor loadings <strong>of</strong> these scales.This validity evidence is potentially importantin that it warns against routinely interpretingthese scales as measures <strong>of</strong> distinctconstructs, traits, or symptom clusters.An early factor analysis <strong>of</strong> the MMPI-A revealed four factors: general anxiety,overcontrol or repression, the Si (thirdfactor) <strong>and</strong> Mf (fourth factor) scales(Butcher et al., 1992). This factor solutionreported in the manual is highly similarfor both males <strong>and</strong> females.The general anxiety factor accountsfor the vast majority <strong>of</strong> the variance in thecorrelation matrix. Factor 1 is marked byloadings for the Hs, D, Hy, Pd, Pa, Pt, <strong>and</strong>Sc scales. Based on these results, this factorlooks like a measure <strong>of</strong> general distress.The second (overcontrol) factor is identifiedby loadings for L <strong>and</strong> K for males <strong>and</strong>Ma for females. The Si factor is clear-cut forboth genders, whereas the Mf factor is clearcutfor males only.Another factor analysis <strong>of</strong> the same normativesample produced somewhat differentfindings, with 14 factors being yieldedfrom an exploratory factor analysis at theitem level <strong>and</strong> 8 factors when conductedat the scale level (Archer et al., 1994). Afactor analytic study <strong>of</strong> the MMPI-A contentscales, based on normative samples,suggested that the 15 content scales couldrepresent two latent variables-“generalmaladjustment” <strong>and</strong> “externalizing tendencies”(McCarthy & Archer, 1998). Asingle factor solution, on the other h<strong>and</strong>,may be more appropriate for girls.Other Validity StudiesThere is a wealth <strong>of</strong> external validity evidencefor the MMPI-A clinical <strong>and</strong> contentscales. The reader will note, however, thatthis evidence re-quires cross-validation;therefore, much <strong>of</strong> it is difficult to interpret.For example, the A-dep (Depressioncontent scale) was correlated with severalcriteria/variables for the normative <strong>and</strong>clinical samples. Correlations with thesecriteria ranged from a low (consideringthe absolute magnitude <strong>of</strong> the correlation)<strong>of</strong> −.18 with grades in school <strong>and</strong> outst<strong>and</strong>ingpersonal achievement to a high<strong>of</strong> .24 for increase in disagreements withparent(s). The correlations between a highA-dep score <strong>and</strong> suicidal ideation/gestures


126 CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIES<strong>and</strong> history <strong>of</strong> depression were .23 <strong>and</strong>.22 (Williams et al., 1992). By comparison,the A-anx (Anxiety) scale correlated.23 with a history <strong>of</strong> depression, <strong>and</strong> theA-Ise (Low Self-Esteem) also correlated.26 with depression history. The majority<strong>of</strong> the external validity coefficients seem tobe in the range <strong>of</strong> .20 to .30. The authorssuggest several reasons for these results<strong>and</strong> numerous methodological caveats,including the appropriateness <strong>of</strong> the criterionmeasures, sample sizes, composition<strong>of</strong> the clinical samples, <strong>and</strong> other factors(Williams et al., 1992). It has also beensuggested that the A-dep <strong>and</strong> A-anx contentscales simply do not discriminate wellbetween the anxiety <strong>and</strong> depression constructs(Arita & Baer, 1998).The A-cyn (Cynicism) <strong>and</strong> the A-trt(Negative Treatment Indicators) scales producedlittle external validity data to supporttheir use (Butcher et al., 1992; Williamset al., 1992). The A-sch (School Problems)fared better than most by producing 44 significantcorrelations with meaningful externalcriteria in the normative sample.Over the years, several studies havebeen conducted to assess the ability <strong>of</strong> theMMPI-A to discriminate between levels<strong>of</strong> symptomatology, diagnostic categories,<strong>and</strong> so forth. One such study has evaluatedthe ability <strong>of</strong> the MMPI-A to differentiatethree patterns <strong>of</strong> substance abuse (behavioralundercontrol, absence <strong>of</strong> behavioralundercontrol, <strong>and</strong> behavioral undercontrol<strong>and</strong> overcontrol) for a sample <strong>of</strong> 180“substance abusers” (Gallucci, 1997). Severalscales were needed to predict groupmembership at a 79% correct classificationrate including MAC-R, D, Pd, Ma,Hy, Alcohol/Drug Problem Proneness,<strong>and</strong> Alcohol/Drug Problem Acknowledgment.Still another investigation used theMMPI-A to differentiate adolescent criminal<strong>of</strong>fenders who had violent infractions(Hicks, Rogers, & Cashel, 2000). TheMMPI-A was predictive <strong>of</strong> total number<strong>of</strong> infractions, <strong>and</strong> Pd was associated morewith violent infractions. Archer <strong>and</strong> Slesinger(1999) investigated the relationshipbetween three MMPI-A suicidal ideationitems <strong>and</strong> score pr<strong>of</strong>iles. The three itemswere associated with higher clinical scaleT-scores in general. Finally, with regardto group differentiation validity, Cumella,Wall, <strong>and</strong> Kerr-Almeida (1999) used theMMPI-A to discriminate between cases<strong>of</strong> anorexia <strong>and</strong> bulimia. There are tw<strong>of</strong>indings <strong>of</strong> interest: (1) the MMPI-A didnot differentiate the groups as well as theolder MMPI, <strong>and</strong> (2) bulimia patients differedfrom anorexia patients across content,supplemental, clinical, <strong>and</strong> validityscales suggesting different symptomsunderlying these disorders.An additional two studies have madedirect comparisons between the MMPI-A<strong>and</strong> projective (i.e., Rorschach) <strong>and</strong> ratingscale (i.e., Achenbach CBCL <strong>and</strong> TRF)measures. Archer <strong>and</strong> Krishnamurthy(1997) compared the utility <strong>of</strong> the MMPI-A <strong>and</strong> Rorschach (Exner’s ComprehensiveSystem) for distinguishing between ConductDisorder <strong>and</strong> depression. In short, theMMPI-A did a better job <strong>of</strong> predicting diagnosticstatus with the Rorschach contributingonly two variables that accounted for asmall proportion <strong>of</strong> variance in a depressiondiagnosis, <strong>and</strong> none to a Conduct Disorderdiagnosis. The Rorschach results contributedno significant variance to the predictionbeyond that contributed by eitherthe D or A-dep scales. The MMPI-AA-con, A-cyn, <strong>and</strong> IMM scales were theonly predictors <strong>of</strong> Conduct Disorder diagnosis.Despite decades <strong>of</strong> research <strong>and</strong> useby clinicians, the MMPI-A as a valid<strong>and</strong> useful measure in adolescent assessmentis not completely understood.Archer (2005) has provided a summary<strong>of</strong> much <strong>of</strong> the work on the MMPI-A.In his summary, which is highlighted in Box6.3, Archer promotes an underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong>


CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIES127Box 6.3Underst<strong>and</strong>ing MMPI-A Results in a Developmental FrameworkArcher (2005) provided a review <strong>of</strong> MMPI/MMPI-A research that sought to demonstratehow the MMPI-A can contribute to knowledge<strong>of</strong> adolescent development. In doing so,Archer also highlights some issues to considerwhen interpreting the results <strong>of</strong> an MMPI/MMPI-A <strong>and</strong> making clinical decisions <strong>and</strong>recommendations. He emphasizes nine keypoints. The following is a brief summary <strong>of</strong>those points:1. “Generalizing adult findings to adolescentsis frequently inappropriate” (p. 258). Quitesimply, when adult norms are used withadolescents, even with available statisticalcorrections, adolescents tend to score in anelevated fashion2. “MMPI [MMPI-2] items are more effectivein discriminating normal from abnormalfunctioning for adults than [MMPI-Aitems are] for adolescents” (p. 260).3. “Maturational influences have pr<strong>of</strong>oundeffect on adolescents’ (<strong>and</strong> adults’) MMPIresponses” (p. 261). To illustrate this point,Archer presents data showing a decrease inadolescents’ MMPI-A raw scores throughoutthe teen years. In contrast, cross-sectionaldata reviewed by Archer show anincrease in scores on the MMPI-2 Hypochondriasisscale (Hs) with age.4. In general, “it is considerably more difficult todiscriminate normal from abnormal functioningamong adolescents than adults” (p. 263).Archer notes that many adolescents in clinicalsettings produce pr<strong>of</strong>iles with few, if any, elevations.Ironically, as Archer points out, thispattern may exist because many adolescentsin the non-clinical norm sample may haveendorsed a relatively high number <strong>of</strong> items.5. “The expression <strong>of</strong> psychopathology hasmany similarities across age groups (p.263). That is, despite notable developmentalinfluences on item endorsement, theunderlying implications <strong>of</strong> MMPI pr<strong>of</strong>ilesfor adolescents or adults do not appear todiffer greatly.6. “Acting out is the ubiquitous defensemechanism among adolescents” (p. 265).To support this notion, Archer reports thelower frequency <strong>of</strong> L <strong>and</strong> K scale elevationsfor adolescents than for adults as wellas the relative commonality <strong>of</strong> adolescentclinical pr<strong>of</strong>iles that involve the PsychopathicDeviate (Pd) scale.7. “<strong>Adolescent</strong>s in the juvenile justice <strong>and</strong>mental health systems are <strong>of</strong>ten similar”(p. 265) based on MMPI-A responses.8. “Given the fluid nature <strong>of</strong> symptomatologyduring adolescence, long-term predictionsbased on MMPI-A findings are ill-advised”(p. 267). Such a statement should be madeabout the results <strong>of</strong> any currently availableassessment tool for child or adolescent personality,emotional functioning, or behavioralfunctioning.9. A turbulent view <strong>of</strong> adolescence pr<strong>of</strong>feredby many early theories <strong>of</strong> adolescentdevelopment “receive substantialsupport from the MMPI/MMPI-A”(p. 267). This conclusion is drawn based onthe relatively high symptom endorsement byadolescents on the MMPI-A <strong>and</strong> the lack <strong>of</strong>evidence indicating that such results are predictive<strong>of</strong> long-term psychological difficulty.In other words, adolescents may, as a group,experience significant problems that warrantattention, but in many cases, these difficultiesare fortunately transient in nature.adolescent response patterns on the MMPI-A (<strong>and</strong> thus, evidence regarding potentialpsychopathology) within a developmentalframework.


128 CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIESInterpretationThe MMPI-A manual <strong>and</strong> subsequentliterature (e.g., Archer, Krishnamurthy,& Stredny, 2007) supplies several aids tointerpretation. Considerable psychometricinformation is available, including reliability<strong>and</strong> factor-analytic validity information.The potential import <strong>of</strong> reliability informationfor interpretation was outlined earlier.Similarly, factor-analytic data can help cliniciansunderst<strong>and</strong> MMPI-A results.If an adolescent obtains high score elevationson all <strong>of</strong> the clinical scales save Si <strong>and</strong>Mf, for example, then the client is producinga high factor 1 score. This client, consistentwith the factor-analytic research, isshowing a high level <strong>of</strong> general anxiety ordistress. This result may not be <strong>of</strong> particularimport for differential diagnosis, but itis a sensible <strong>and</strong> predictable finding in light<strong>of</strong> factor-analytic results.Furthermore, the MMPI-A, as with itsadult counterpart, includes Harris-Lingoesscales which are subscales that may helpthe clinician determine what types <strong>of</strong> itemsled to a clinical scale elevation. For example,The Depression (D) scale includes fiveHarris-Lingoes subscales (i.e., SubjectiveDepression, Psychomotor Retardation,Physical Malfunctioning, Mental Dullness<strong>and</strong> Brooding).The previous description <strong>of</strong> scale itemcontent can also be most useful for underst<strong>and</strong>ingscale elevations. The MMPI-Amanual (Butcher et al., 1992) also providesa list <strong>of</strong> items <strong>and</strong> their scale membership(Table E-1), which can be useful for scaleinterpretation.Some questions to ask oneself wheninterpreting the MMPI-A could include:llHow does an adolescent get a high scoreon this scale? What are the behaviors,perceptions, <strong>and</strong> feelings assessed by thisscale (i.e., the item content)?How reliable are the scales that I wish tointerpret as being <strong>of</strong> some clinical value inthis case?lllllIs there a content scale analogue that ismore reliable <strong>and</strong> potentially more valid(e.g., D versus A-dep) than the clinicalscale?What does the pattern <strong>of</strong> scale elevationssuggest in terms <strong>of</strong> factor-analyticresearch?How is this scale reflective <strong>of</strong> nontestbasedclinical symptomatology?Does external validity evidence exist(e.g., differential validity studies) to supportor refute my interpretation <strong>of</strong> thisscale?Has the validity study been independentlyreplicated?Interpretation <strong>of</strong> the MMPI is also supportedby practical <strong>and</strong> thorough bookson the topic (e.g., Archer, 1997) as well asnumerous work-shops <strong>and</strong> other continuingpr<strong>of</strong>essional development opportunities.The MMPI-A literature, while notnearly as extensive as that <strong>of</strong> the MMPI-2,is expansive. A specific example <strong>of</strong> this range<strong>of</strong> resources is a guide devoted singularlyto the use <strong>of</strong> the MMPI system in forensicwork (Pope, Butcher, & Seelen, 2000).A sample case (Box 6.4) on the followingpage illustrates briefly how MMPI-A scaleelevations can be used in conjunction withother assessment information to aid diagnosticdecisions <strong>and</strong> in determining the primarytarget(s) <strong>of</strong> intervention.Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesThe MMPI-A’s long history is simultaneouslyits greatest asset <strong>and</strong> liability.On the one h<strong>and</strong>, the volumes <strong>of</strong> MMPIresearch guide practice. In direct contrast,the original clinical scales are incongruentwith modern research on child <strong>and</strong> adolescentpsychopathology <strong>and</strong> test developmentmethods. The continually exp<strong>and</strong>ingresearch base <strong>and</strong> new scale developmentensures that the MMPI will enjoy continuedpopularity <strong>and</strong> utility. It is, after all, aunique self-report measure.


CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIES129Box 6.4A Sample Case Using the MMPI-AMichael is a 16-year-old high school juniorHe was referred for an evaluation because<strong>of</strong> recently increased anxiety symptoms thathave interfered with his social interactions<strong>and</strong> functioning at school. More specifically,Michael has experienced some increasedconcerns with cleanliness or organization athome, <strong>and</strong> he has has begun to avoid socialsituations for fear <strong>of</strong> experiencing noticeableanxiety in front <strong>of</strong> others. Michael also has ahistory <strong>of</strong> depression, according to his mother,which has included a trial <strong>of</strong> antidepressantmedication about three years ago. Medicationwas discontinued after a couple <strong>of</strong> monthsbecause his symptoms had diminished.Michael currently lives with his parents<strong>and</strong> his 11 year-old brother. He reportedlygets along well with his family, <strong>and</strong> he has historicallyperformed well academically until thecurrent school year when his grades have gonefrom “As” <strong>and</strong> “Bs” to “Bs,” “Cs,” <strong>and</strong> “Ds.”Michael was cooperative <strong>and</strong> attentivethroughout the test session. He appearedmotivated to do well. No speech, visual, auditory,or motor abnormalities were noted.Michael demonstrated tendencies towardperfectionism <strong>and</strong> anxiety during intelligencetesting. During the arithmetic subtesthe began to tremble <strong>and</strong> remarked, “I feelnervous.” He was visibly nervous (i.e., h<strong>and</strong>sshaking, voice cracking) when faced withrelatively difficult items. He also seemed perfectionisticin his response style as evidencedby his unwillingness to give up on difficultitems. Michael also displayed depressed affectduring a clinical interview, although he didsmile on occasion. Michael’s intellectual testfindings were all within the High Averagerange. Likewise, all <strong>of</strong> his achievement subtestscores were in the High Average range.These findings are consistent with his educationalhistory.On the MMPI-A, Michael indicatedmoderate levels <strong>of</strong> depressive symptoms <strong>and</strong>a tendency to isolate himself socially. He wasself-described as shy, which is consistent withhis mother’s report. Content scale elevationson the Social Discomfort <strong>and</strong> School Problemsscales are consistent with referral concerns.More specifically, he reported beinguncomfortable in social situations, has difficultyinteracting with others, <strong>and</strong> avoids socialevents. In regards to problems at school,Michael indicated that he does not careabout doing well at school <strong>and</strong> that school isboring. Structured interviews with Michaelindicate that these issues are relatively recent.However, his mother expressed concern thatMichael has had a tendency to lose interestin activities such as school <strong>and</strong> social outingsover the last few years.The Depression (D) clinical scale wassomewhat elevated. Michael indicated that hehas feelings <strong>of</strong> inadequacy <strong>and</strong> worthlessness.Also, on the MMPI-A, Michael acknowledgedhaving lost interest in activities <strong>and</strong>losing sleep due to worry. The reports <strong>of</strong>Michael <strong>and</strong> his mother on structured interviewsindicate that he meets diagnostic criteriafor Dysthymic Disorder. Since childhood,Michael reportedly has had periods <strong>of</strong> cryingeasily, difficulty making decisions, feelings <strong>of</strong>inadequacy, a lack <strong>of</strong> enjoyment from praiseor rewards, <strong>and</strong> feeling that he is not as goodas other people. He recently has experiencedincreasingly depressed mood, difficulty sleeping,<strong>and</strong> avoidance <strong>of</strong> activities.His social anxiety <strong>and</strong> negative attitudesabout school have apparently been recent developmentsthat do not warrant a diagnosis at thistime but should receive clinical attention.The findings <strong>of</strong> this evaluation support theneed for intervention at this time. Michaelcontinues to demonstrate depressive symptoms.In addition, he is beginning to exhibitsome anxiety in social situations which areinterfering with his desire to interact withothers <strong>and</strong> his enjoyment <strong>of</strong> school. A possible(Continues)


130 CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIESBox 6.4 (Continued)focus <strong>of</strong> psychotherapeutic interventions maybe on the self-deprecating nature <strong>of</strong> his cognitions<strong>and</strong> anxiety during social interactions.ScaleL 56F 54K 49Hs 48D 67Hy 58Pd 51Mf 33Pa 52Pt 55Sc 58Ma 48Si 65Content Scale ElevationsSocial Discomfort (A-sod) 72School Problems (A-sch) 71T-scoreThe MMPI-A is likely to continue toremain as one <strong>of</strong> the most widely used adolescentmeasures <strong>of</strong> personality in the world.Claiborn (1995) concluded:Strengths <strong>of</strong> the MMPI-A include:1. Its familiarity to a large group <strong>of</strong> devotedusers2. The existence <strong>of</strong> a number <strong>of</strong> valuablevalidity scales3. Interpretive flexibility because <strong>of</strong> thenumerous scales4. A thorough evaluation <strong>of</strong> the adolescent’sself-appraisal due to the variety <strong>of</strong> itemspresented5. The availability <strong>of</strong> numerous books, chapters,<strong>and</strong> empirical articles devoted toMMPI interpretation, many <strong>of</strong> which <strong>of</strong>ferhighly sensible interpretive guidancePotential weaknesses <strong>of</strong> the MMPI-Ainclude:“Its flaws are relatively minor, correctable,<strong>and</strong> enormously outweighed by the strengths<strong>of</strong> the inventory. Clearly, the MMPI-A wasdeveloped with a great deal <strong>of</strong> care, expertise,<strong>and</strong> sensitivity to the problems <strong>of</strong> adolescents<strong>and</strong> the needs <strong>of</strong> practitioners who work withthem” (p. 628).1. Retention <strong>of</strong> scales that have not beenwell-supported by validity evidence2. Retention <strong>of</strong> scales that lack internal consistency(Black, 1994)3. Failure to incorporate factor-analytic evidenceinto the test development process(e.g., consideration <strong>of</strong> composite scoresor clarifying how scales measure differingtraits or problems despite the fact that theyload on the same factor)4. Duplicating items on different scales whichproduces high intercorrelations, thusbringing into question the distinctiveness<strong>of</strong> measurement <strong>of</strong> individual constructs(Kline, 1995)5. Lack <strong>of</strong> a complete description <strong>of</strong> thenormative sample <strong>and</strong> little evidencethat the sample matches well a particularpopulation (e.g., US Census bureaustatistics)6. Length <strong>of</strong> the MMPI-A relative to otherself-report inventories, making the practicality<strong>of</strong> administering the MMPI-A anissue.In spite <strong>of</strong> the extraordinary amount <strong>of</strong> MMPIresearch available, much remains to be done.The majority <strong>of</strong> the available research isbased on adult samples. However, we referthe reader to the work <strong>of</strong> Archer <strong>and</strong> colleagues(e.g., Archer, 2005; Archer, H<strong>and</strong>el, &Lynch, 2001; Archer & Krishamurthy, 1994)who have greatly contributed to the body <strong>of</strong>knowledge regarding MMPI-A validity <strong>and</strong>interpretation.


CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIES131Conners, 3rd Edition, Self-Report (Conners-3 SR;Conners, 2008a)The Conners-3 SR (Conners, 2008a) is themost recent addition to a rating scale systemwith a long history <strong>of</strong> research <strong>and</strong> clinicaluse. However, despite this long history, aself-report instrument had largely beenconsidered experimental until the mostrecent revision <strong>of</strong> the Conners rating scalesystem. The Conners-3 self-report ratingscale consists <strong>of</strong> 59 items which are writtenat approximately a third grade reading level.A 39-item Short Form also exists.It takes approximately 20 min to completethe Long Form <strong>of</strong> the Conners-3 SR.The inclusion <strong>of</strong> a st<strong>and</strong>ard self-report formmakes the Conners system competitive withother well-known rating scale systems highlightedin this chapter. It should be notedthat the Conners-3 SR includes extensiveassessment <strong>of</strong> externalizing problems, particularlyADHD, with screening <strong>of</strong> anxiety<strong>and</strong> depression. Depending on the referralissue, this design may be either ideal or lessthan-idealfor the clinician.We focus on the Conners-3 rather thanits companion rating scale system, theConners Comprehensive Behavior RatingScales (CBRS; Conners, 2008b), becausethe Conners-3 is relatively unique in itsextensive evaluation <strong>of</strong> externalizing problems.The CBRS self-report form (for ages8–18) provides an assessment <strong>of</strong> severalareas <strong>of</strong> behavioral, emotional, <strong>and</strong> academicfunctioning <strong>and</strong> is desirable if the clinicianneeds information in more domainsthan externalizing problems.Scale ContentThe Conners-3 includes four contentscales: Hyperactivity/Impulsivity, Inattention,Aggression, Family Relations, <strong>and</strong>Learning Problems. There are five DSM-IV-TR Symptom scales (i.e., ADHD,Combined Type; ADHD Inattentive;ADHD Hyperactive-Impulsive, ODD,<strong>and</strong> CD). As noted above, the Conners-3SR includes screening items for depression<strong>and</strong> anxiety, as well as impairment itemsfor home, school, <strong>and</strong> social relationships.Like the BASC, the Conners-3 includescritical items that may signal the need forfurther follow-up. The critical items onthe SR are specific to severe conduct problems.Consistent with its predecessors,the Conners-3 includes a brief ADHDIndex. This scale is based on items thatbest differentiate ADHD from nonclinicalsamples. New to the Conners-3 are threevalidity scales: Positive Impression (or“fake good”), Negative Impression (“fakebad”), <strong>and</strong> the Inconsistency Index. Lastly, theConners-3 SR includes an open-endeditem assessing “strengths/skills.” Overall,the Conners-3 scales <strong>and</strong> the existence <strong>of</strong>the validity scales are in line with the currentstate-<strong>of</strong>-the-art in rating scale systems,as well as providing perhaps more detailedassessment <strong>of</strong> externalizing symptoms thanis typical for other rating scales.Administration <strong>and</strong> ScoringThe Conners-3 SR is designed for usewith youth ages 8–18. Responses are madeon a four-point scale, where 0 = not at alltrue (never, seldom), <strong>and</strong> 4 = very muchtrue (very much true, very frequent). Thetime frame for responses is the last monthprior to the assessment. Both h<strong>and</strong> scoring<strong>and</strong> computer scoring are availableas are secure Internet administration <strong>and</strong>scoring.The pr<strong>of</strong>ile form that is included withthe response form is used to convert rawscores to T-scores. The T-scores for theConners-3 are linear T-scores, meaningthat the scales maintain their distributionswhen converted to T-scores. Male<strong>and</strong> female norm-referenced scores are


132 CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIESobtained separately. In addition, each agehas separate norms. The T-scores shownon the available pr<strong>of</strong>ile forms are truncatedsuch that T-scores below 40 are notspecified. Detailed step-by-step scoringprocedures are available in the Conners-3manual (Conners, 2008a).NormingThe normative sample <strong>of</strong> 1,000 caseswas collected mostly in the UnitedStates, with “a limited amount <strong>of</strong> data”(Conners, 2008a; p. 139) collected inCanada. Recruitment was targeted toapproximate the ethnic/racial distribution<strong>of</strong> the population according to USCensus statistics. Data presented in theConners-3 manual indicate that accuraterepresentation across ethnic groups wasattained. Only 8% <strong>of</strong> cases came fromthe western part <strong>of</strong> the United States,indicating some underrepresentation <strong>of</strong>this region. The majority (i.e., 70%) <strong>of</strong>cases in the normative sample <strong>of</strong> the SRhad parents with at least some secondaryeducation.Equal numbers <strong>of</strong> girls <strong>and</strong> boys wereincluded in the normative sample at eachage. As noted above, norm-referencedT-scores are provided separately for boys<strong>and</strong> girls, which does limit interpretationto sex-only comparisons.ReliabilityInternal consistency coefficients for SR aregood for both the content scales <strong>and</strong> DSMsymptom scales. Specifically, coefficientswere all above .80 in all age groups acrossgenders, with the exception <strong>of</strong> the ConductDisorder scale for girls with coefficientsranging from .74 to .79 <strong>and</strong> the Aggressionscale for 8 <strong>and</strong> 9-year-old girls (i.e., .75).Two to four week test-retest reliabilitieswere also good, with coefficients all .71<strong>and</strong> higher (Conners, 2008a).ValidityFactor analysis was used extensively indevelopment <strong>of</strong> the Conners-3. Exploratory<strong>and</strong> confirmatory factor analysessupport a four-factor model (i.e., FamilyRelations, Learning Problems, Aggression,<strong>and</strong> Hyperactivity/Impulsivity; Conners,2008a). Thus, these content scales are consideredempirically-derived, whereas theInattention scale is considered theoretical/rational. The SR content <strong>and</strong> DSM scalesshow moderate to high intercorrelations<strong>and</strong> moderate correlations with analogousscales from the Conners-3 parent <strong>and</strong>teacher rating scales.Criterion-related validity was demonstratedthrough correlations with other ratingscales. Specifically, correlations betweenscales on the Conners-3 SR were moderatelyto highly correlated with analogousscales on the BASC-2-SRP <strong>and</strong> AchenbachYSR. The one exception was a non-significantnegative relation between the Relationswith Parents (higher scores indicatebetter relations) scale on the BASC-2-SRP<strong>and</strong> the Family Relations (higher scoresindicate worse relations) scale <strong>of</strong> the Conners-3SR for younger children (ages 8–11).This relation was significant <strong>and</strong> negativefor older youth (ages 12–18).Discriminant validity for the SR is alsosupported based on comparisons <strong>of</strong> clinical<strong>and</strong> general populations. In particular, theLearning Problems, Inattention, Hyperactivity/Impulsivity,<strong>and</strong> Aggression scales alldifferentiated not only the clinical samplefrom the general sample, but they also tendedto differentiate among individuals within theclinical sample. For example, scores on theAggression scale were higher for individualsdiagnosed with disruptive behavior disordersthan individuals with other diagnoses.This pattern was also the case for the FamilyRelations scale. The DSM scales also faredwell in these analyses.It should be noted that the above informationpertains to the Long Form <strong>of</strong> theConners-3 SR in particular. Reliability <strong>and</strong>


CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIES133validity evidence <strong>of</strong> the Short <strong>and</strong> Indexforms are similarly good (see Conners,2008a).InterpretationConners (2008a) provides a clear recommendedapproach to interpretation <strong>of</strong>the SR <strong>and</strong> other scales in the Conners-3family. This approach is well-alignedwith the approach recommended in thistext. First, the validity scales should beexamined as to the potential usefulness<strong>of</strong> the responses. The next interpretationshould occur at the scale level followedby a consideration <strong>of</strong> the overall “pr<strong>of</strong>ile”or pattern <strong>of</strong> elevations. Then, item-levelresponses should be examined, includingthe anxiety <strong>and</strong> depression screeningitems, the items on elevated content orDSM scales, critical items, <strong>and</strong> strength/skills items. Lastly, reports from the SRshould not st<strong>and</strong>-alone in an assessmentbut instead should be integrated withother information.Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesSome <strong>of</strong> the strengths <strong>of</strong> the Conners-3 SRare:1. Good initial validity evidence.2. User-friendly manual that assists withscoring <strong>and</strong> interpretation.3. Availability <strong>of</strong> empirically-derived,rational, <strong>and</strong> DSM-oriented scales.4. Thorough coverage <strong>of</strong> externalizingsymptomatology5. Availability <strong>of</strong> a Short Form for efficiency6. Availability <strong>of</strong> validity scales.Some characteristics that may be consideredweaknesses are:1. Limited assessment <strong>of</strong> internalizingproblems.2. Limited assessment <strong>of</strong> adaptive functioning.3. Limited independent validity research todate.<strong>Personality</strong> Inventory for Youth(PIY; Lachar & Gruber, 1994)The <strong>Personality</strong> Inventory for Youth (PIY;Lachar & Gruber, 1994) has its roots inthe well-known <strong>Personality</strong> Inventory for<strong>Child</strong>ren, which is designed as a parentrating scale (see Chap. 7). The PIY consists<strong>of</strong> 270 True-False items designed toassess emotional <strong>and</strong> behavioral adjustment,school adjustment, family characteristics<strong>and</strong> interactions, <strong>and</strong> academicability in children aged 9 through 18 years(Lachar & Gruber, 1994). The PIY <strong>of</strong>fersa substantial array <strong>of</strong> scales designed toassess these issues.Scale ContentThe PIY features four broad-b<strong>and</strong> factorscales; Externalizing/Internalizing, CognitiveImpairment, Social Withdrawal,<strong>and</strong> Social Skills Deficit. The Externalizing/Internalizingfactor includes severalscales, whereas the other three factors eachinclude the scale <strong>of</strong> the same name. Thescales <strong>and</strong> subscales <strong>of</strong> the PIY are:Cognitive ImpairmentPoor Achievement & Memory InadequateAbilitiesLearning ProblemsImpulsivity <strong>and</strong> DistractibilityBrashnessDistractibility & OveractivityImpulsivityDelinquencyAntisocial Behavior DyscontrolNoncomplianceFamily DysfunctionParent-<strong>Child</strong> ConflictParent MaladjustmentMarital Discord


134 CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIESReality DistortionFeelings <strong>of</strong> AlienationHallucinations & DelusionsSomatic ConcernPsychosomatic SyndromeMuscular Tension & Anxiety Preoccupationwith DiseasePsychological DiscomfortFear & Worry DepressionSleep DisturbanceSocial WithdrawalSocial Introversion IsolationSocial Skill DeficitsLimited Peer Status Conflict withPeersThe PIC item pool served as the basis fordeveloping items for the PIY (Lachar, 1999).In fact, only a few new items were added.The vast majority (i.e., about two-thirds)<strong>of</strong> PIY items are adapted from the PIC. Weraise the question regarding item contentbecause it is surprising to see a self-reportmeasure that does not produce familiarscales or composites (not subscales) forconstructs such as depression <strong>and</strong> anxiety.The authors explain (p. 34 <strong>of</strong> the TechnicalGuide) that they combined these scalesin order to measure “emotional distress.”The scale is labeled “Psychological Discomfort.”The lack <strong>of</strong> clear operationaldefinitions for Psychological Discomfort,Cognitive Impairment, Dyscontrol, <strong>and</strong>other scales <strong>and</strong> subscales hinders interpretation,<strong>and</strong> the clinician should pay closeattention to the item content within scales<strong>and</strong> subscales in making interpretations.Interpretation could be hinderedbecause an idiosyncratically defined scalecannot be interpreted with the assistance<strong>of</strong> the vast research associated with a wellresearchedconstruct such as depression.If a well-researched construct is defined<strong>and</strong> assessed (e.g., hyperactivity), then theuser can obtain information about diagnosis,prognosis, course <strong>of</strong> treatment, <strong>and</strong>so forth from the behavioral sciences atlarge-, rather than solely relying on theresearch base for a specific measure suchas the PIY. The content <strong>of</strong> the PIY is, inmany ways, broad <strong>and</strong> unique, but experiencewith administering <strong>and</strong> interpretingthe PIY would be necessary for successfullyarticulating the meaning <strong>of</strong> a client’sscale elevations <strong>and</strong> making appropriatedecisions.Validity ScalesFour validity scales are also featured. ThePIY Validity (VAL) scale is intended to assessthe presence <strong>of</strong> inattentive, oppositional,or provocative responses (e.g., responding“True” to “My teachers are trying to poisonme.”). The Inconsistency (INC) scale indicatesif a protocol was answered haphazardly.In other words, the INC scale gaugesthe respondent’s consistency on similarlyworded items. Dissimulation (FB) measuresthe tendency <strong>of</strong> the informant to fake bad ormalinger. Finally, the Defensiveness (DEF)scale provides an index that may reflect afake good or a social desirability responseset (Lachar & Gruber, 1994). A study <strong>of</strong>the validity <strong>of</strong> these scales found that theydetected inconsistent <strong>and</strong> overly positive ornegative responding as intended (Wrobelet al., 1999).Administration <strong>and</strong> ScoringThe PIY is a 270-item inventory in a True-False format. It takes approximately 30–60min to administer. The PIY manual (Lachar& Gruber, 1994) provides several helpfulguidelines for administration. The manualsuggests, for example, that the examinerexplain the directions to the examinee,even though the directions are includedin the Administration Booklet, in order toensure compliance <strong>and</strong> enhance rapport.The administration requires two components,the booklet, <strong>and</strong> WPS Autoscore TMAnswer Forms.A single “template” is included in theanswer form. The examiner adds rows


CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIES135<strong>and</strong> columns in order to obtain raw scores.These scores are then converted to genderbasedT-scores. The PIY does not <strong>of</strong>fercombined norms by gender as an interpretiveoption. Both PC <strong>and</strong> mail-in computerscoringservices are also available.Abbreviated FormThe PIY also includes an 80-item abbreviatedform which consists <strong>of</strong> the first 80items <strong>of</strong> the full PIY. In addition, a uniquefeature within the abbreviated PIY is a32-item classroom screening tool (CLASS)that assesses for difficulties in classroomadjustment (Lachar, 1999).NormingThe PIY normative sample consists <strong>of</strong>2,327 regular education students testedin 1991–1992 in five states. The samplewas stratified to meet US Census Bureaustatistics for ethnicity, parental educationallevel (SES), <strong>and</strong> community size.In addition, the marital status <strong>of</strong> theparent(s) was also considered. Therewas a slight under-sampling <strong>of</strong> AfricanAmerican children. An additional normingsample <strong>of</strong> 1,178 cases was collected toproduce clinical norms. These cases werecollected from 50 facilities serving a variety<strong>of</strong> inpatients <strong>and</strong> outpatients.ReliabilityInternal consistency coefficients are availablefor the PIY for both the normative<strong>and</strong> clinical samples (see Table 6.8 for dataon the clinical samples). The reliabilities <strong>of</strong>the scales for the clinical sample are generallygood, producing a median <strong>of</strong> .85. Themedian reliability for the subscales is lower,at .73. In fact, for the clinical sample, eight<strong>of</strong> the internal consistency coefficients fallbelow .70. The internal consistency coefficientfor the learning problems scale, forexample, was the lowest, at .44.Table 6.8 PlY Scales <strong>and</strong> Internal ConsistencyReliabilities for the <strong>Clinical</strong>SamplesScales <strong>and</strong> SubscalesInternalConsistencyCoefficientCognitive Impairment .74Poor Achievement <strong>and</strong>.65MemoryInadequate Abilities .67Learning Problems .44Impulsivity <strong>and</strong>.77DistractibilityBrashness .54Distractibility <strong>and</strong>.61OveractivityImpulsivity .54Delinquency .92Antisocial Behavior .83Dyscontrol .84Noncompliance .83Family Dysfunction .87Parent–<strong>Child</strong> Conflict .82Parent Maladjustment .74Marital Discord .70Reality Distortion .83Feelings <strong>of</strong> Alienation .77Hallucinations <strong>and</strong>.71DelusionsSomatic Concerns .85Pychosomatic Syndrome .73Muscular Tension <strong>and</strong>.74AnxietyPreoccupation with.60DiseasePsychological Discomfort .86Fear <strong>and</strong> Worry .78Depression .73Sleep Disturbance .70Social Withdrawal .80Social Introversion .78Isolation .59Social Skill Deficit .86Limited Peer Status .79Conflict with Peers .80Note: Adapted from Table 46 in Lachar <strong>and</strong> Gruber(1994).


136 CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIESThe 7–10 day test-retest coefficientsfor the clinical sample are also good atthe scale level, with a median <strong>of</strong> .82. Themedian coefficient at the subscale level wasagain lower, at .73. At the subscale level, atotal <strong>of</strong> seven subscales yielded test-retestcoefficients <strong>of</strong> .70 or less. The lowest coefficientwas .58 for the Impulsivity subscale,<strong>and</strong> the highest was .88 for the AntisocialBehavior <strong>and</strong> Dyscontrol subscales (Lachar& Gruber, 1994).The manual also reports useful estimatesfor the st<strong>and</strong>ard error <strong>of</strong> measurementfor each scale <strong>and</strong> subscale in T-scoreunits. The typical SEM at the scale levelis about 4 or 5 T-score points for the testretestestimates.ValidityThe major argument presented for contentvalidity <strong>of</strong> the PIY is that the itemswere derived from the PIC-R item pool.Lachar <strong>and</strong> Gruber argue that any validityevidence already gathered for the PIC-Rlends indirect support for the validity <strong>of</strong>the PIY. However, it is worth noting thatbecause items on the PIY are necessarilyself- referent <strong>and</strong> items on the PIC are necessarilynot, data regarding the PIC cannotbe extrapolated toward underst<strong>and</strong>ingresponse tendencies on the PIY. In regardsto adolescent self-reports <strong>of</strong> psychologicalconstructs, it is unclear why the PIY doesnot include any items directly relevant tohigh-incidence syndromes <strong>of</strong> childhoodsuch as depression <strong>and</strong> anxiety.Several criterion-related validity studiesare included in the manual. A study <strong>of</strong> 152adolescents produced very modest relationsbetween the PIY <strong>and</strong> the original MMPIclinical scales. Among the highest correlationswere Reality Distortion <strong>and</strong> MMPISchizophrenia (r = .66), <strong>and</strong> PsychologicalDiscomfort <strong>and</strong> Psychasthenia (r = .65).The majority <strong>of</strong> correlations were in the.20 to .50 range, <strong>and</strong> many <strong>of</strong> the correlationswere not statistically significant.Another study correlated the PIY withthe State-Trait Anxiety Inventory <strong>and</strong> theReynolds <strong>Adolescent</strong> Depression Scale for79 cases.These data allow for the evaluation <strong>of</strong>the criterion-related validity <strong>of</strong> the PIYPsychological Discomfort scale <strong>and</strong> itscomponent subscales. The correlationsranged from modest to strong. The State-Trait correlated .51 <strong>and</strong> the Reynolds .70with the PIY Psychological Discomfortscale, suggesting that this scale may measuremore depressive than anxiety symptomatology,although indicators <strong>of</strong> both constructsappear to be part <strong>of</strong> the Psychological Discomfortscale. It is difficult to summarizethe wealth <strong>of</strong> data included in these studiesin the limited space available here. The clinicianwho is seriously interested in the PIYwould be served well by reading the criterion-relatedvalidity studies included in themanual very carefully prior to interpretingthe scales <strong>and</strong> subscales.Several samples were also used to assessthe factor invariance <strong>of</strong> the PIY by gender<strong>and</strong> ethnicity (Lachar & Gruber, 1994).The results were generally supportive <strong>of</strong>the hypothesis <strong>of</strong> factor invariance acrossgroups, although cross-validation withindependent samples should be soughtbefore drawing definitive conclusions.An additional study supported the relevance<strong>of</strong> PIY items for detecting peerrelatedproblems. Wrobel, Lachar, <strong>and</strong>Wrobel (2005) found that PIY scales constructed<strong>of</strong> items indicative <strong>of</strong> peer problems<strong>and</strong> peer withdrawal were significantlyrelated to analogous peer-reported items.Another study in Bilingual adolescentssupported the correspondence between theEnglish version <strong>of</strong> the PIY <strong>and</strong> a Spanishversion <strong>of</strong> the instrument (Negy, Lachar, &Gruber, 2001).InterpretationA five-step interpretive system for the PIYis <strong>of</strong>fered by the authors (Lachar & Gruber,


CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIES1371994). The first step involves assessingthe validity <strong>of</strong> the obtained results usingthe four validity scales <strong>and</strong> a review <strong>of</strong> thecompleted form.The second interpretive step suggestsidentifying primary pr<strong>of</strong>ile elevations.A primary scale elevation is defined asa clinical scale with a T-score <strong>of</strong> 60 <strong>and</strong>subscale member <strong>of</strong> this same scale witha T-score <strong>of</strong> at least 65 for the majority<strong>of</strong> scales, although this cut score varies.These primary scale elevations may identifythe diagnostic issues most likely to requireadditional attention <strong>and</strong>/or study.The third interpretive step pertains tosecondary scale elevations. These significantpr<strong>of</strong>ile elevations are defined as scalesthat exceed the clinical cut score that arenot accompanied by corresponding subscalescores that exceed the clinical cut score. Inaddition, subscales that exceed the cut score(without a corresponding high scale score)are also interpreted in this step. These secondaryscale elevations may reveal mildproblems, issues linked to the primary area<strong>of</strong> concern (e.g., social problems secondaryto one’s externalizing behaviors), <strong>and</strong>/orfrequently occurring personality characteristics(Lachar & Gruber, 1994).Step four includes tallying <strong>and</strong> interpretingitems labeled as critical. This extensivelist is intended to identify items that suggestclinical issues that should be examinedin greater detail (Lachar & Gruber, 1994).The last interpretive step is probably themost crucial-the integration <strong>of</strong> PIY resultswith other findings. The complexity <strong>of</strong> thisinterpretive step cannot be overstated. Oursuggested approach for integrating informationis described in detail in Chap. 15.The PIY manuals provide detailedinterpretive guidance that is grounded inscientific evidence. Cut scores <strong>and</strong> manydecision rules are based on careful investigations<strong>of</strong> clinical samples. The chapter <strong>of</strong>case studies included in the administration<strong>and</strong> scoring manual should also be highlyvalued by users.Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesThe PIY is clearly the result <strong>of</strong> a thorough<strong>and</strong> thoughtful test development process asis duly noted in reviews <strong>of</strong> the instrument.Destefano (1998), for example, concluded:“Given the shortcomings <strong>of</strong> projective testing<strong>and</strong> parent report for this age group, thePIY is a welcome addition to clinical assessmentbatteries for children <strong>of</strong> ages 9 through18” (p. 757).Marchant <strong>and</strong> Ridenour (1998) also havehigh praise for the PlY: “Seldom does aself-report instrument enter the field withthe background <strong>of</strong> the PIY” (p. 758). Thesereviews also <strong>of</strong>fer some caveats that are <strong>of</strong>interest to potential users.We wish to add two caveats. First, wesuggest that PIY users study the itemcontent <strong>of</strong> the clinical scales, rather thanrelying on scale labels, in order to developan underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> the constructs beingmeasured <strong>and</strong> assess their correspondenceto similar constructs in the child psychopathologyliterature. Second, we cautionusers that virtually all <strong>of</strong> the extant PIYresearch to date was generated by members<strong>of</strong> the PIY authorship <strong>and</strong> researchteam. We hope that more independentinvestigations will be forthcoming tocross-validate, extend, <strong>and</strong> clarify the currentfindings.Some specific strengths <strong>of</strong> the PIY includethat:1. The PIY is supported by a thorough set<strong>of</strong> manuals that give considerable statisticaldata, guidelines for interpretation,<strong>and</strong> case studies.2. The PIY appears to assess a broad spectrum<strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong> adolescent behavior.3. The PIY is part <strong>of</strong> a comprehensiveassessment system that includes bothparent <strong>and</strong> teacher reports.4. The PIY interpretive system is thoughtful<strong>and</strong> helpful.


138 CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIES5. Computer scoring options are available forthe PIY, which enhances its practicality.Some potential weaknesses <strong>of</strong> the PIY mayinclude:1. Evidence <strong>of</strong> content validity is not compelling.Some <strong>of</strong> the item placementson scales do not seem consistent withrational/theoretical approaches to testdevelopment, which are gaining favorin personality assessment.2. The use <strong>of</strong> norm-referenced scoresat age 9 seems questionable given thesmall norming sample at this age (i.e., N= 70 for the regular education sample)(Lachar & Gruber, 1994).3. The subscales should likely be usedwith caution for clinical purposes, ornot used by the inexperienced PIY user,until further reliability studies are conducted(Destefano, 1998).4. The PIY has a limited research baseoutside <strong>of</strong> studies published by thedevelopers.Single Construct <strong>Personality</strong>InventoriesIn addition to an ever-growing body <strong>of</strong> omnibusself-report rating scales, a number <strong>of</strong>single construct or single domain scales exist.These scales are typically oriented towardolder children <strong>and</strong> adolescents <strong>and</strong> towardan additional assessment <strong>of</strong> internalizingproblems. Some examples <strong>of</strong> these scales arereviewed in later chapters when we discussthe assessment <strong>of</strong> specific constructs.Given the need to approach assessmentcomprehensively, particularly in regards toassessing for comorbidity, we recommendfirst administering interviews <strong>and</strong> ratingscales that evaluate a variety <strong>of</strong> domains. Asinformation indicates problems in a particulardomain or that more information ona construct would aid in decision-making<strong>and</strong> recommendations, well-validated singleconstruct inventories may be useful.ConclusionsThe current state <strong>of</strong> self-report personalityassessment continues to improve. Inparticular, a variety <strong>of</strong> assessment systemshave developed methods that allow forrelatively easy interpretation across self-,parent-, <strong>and</strong> teacher-reports. <strong>Personality</strong>assessment through self-report ratings continuesto lag behind assessment <strong>of</strong> intelligence<strong>and</strong> achievement testing in manyways. Subscale reliabilities are <strong>of</strong>ten stilltoo low to support diagnostic decisions.In some cases, they are too low to supporthypothesis generation. Only a few <strong>of</strong> theinstruments discussed herein have beenempirically checked for item bias, <strong>and</strong> fewhave used modern statistical methods suchas structural equation modeling <strong>and</strong> latenttrait methodology as is commonly done forintelligence <strong>and</strong> achievement tests.Improvements have been made innorming <strong>and</strong> the inclusion <strong>of</strong> reliability <strong>and</strong>validity data in test manuals. Some <strong>of</strong> thenoteworthy contributions have been theextended age range <strong>and</strong> expansion <strong>of</strong> adaptivecompetencies on the BASC-2-SRP, theidentification <strong>of</strong> correlates <strong>of</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>iles for thePIY, the history <strong>of</strong> numerous cross-culturalstudies with the YSR, the extensive assessment<strong>of</strong> various externalizing problems onthe Conners-3, <strong>and</strong> the burgeoning validityevidence for the MMPI-A content scales.Psychologists clearly have more <strong>and</strong> betteroptions than in years past, <strong>and</strong> youth selfreport<strong>of</strong> emotional, social, <strong>and</strong> behavioralfunctioning is valuable for obvious reasons.Still, much validity evidence remains to begathered, particularly with so many measuresbeing relatively new to the market.Relatively few validity studies are availablefor some <strong>of</strong> these scales. Much more evidencewill have to be gathered for all measuresin order to approximate the higher


CHAPTER 6 SELF-REPORT INVENTORIES139st<strong>and</strong>ards <strong>of</strong> practice required today <strong>and</strong> todevelop a truly evidence-based approach toassessment.Chapter Summary1. Until recently, parent <strong>and</strong> teacherreports have routinely been preferredover the use <strong>of</strong> self-report inventoriesin child personality assessment.2. Decisions to use self-report inventoriesshould be based, in part, on thechild’s developmental level, the constructsbeing assessed, <strong>and</strong> the purpose<strong>of</strong> the assessment. In general, olderchildren are more useful informants.Self-reports <strong>of</strong> covert conduct problems<strong>and</strong> internalizing symptoms maybe particularly informative.3. The SRP is one <strong>of</strong> many components<strong>of</strong> the BASC-2. The SRP attempts togauge children’s perceptions <strong>and</strong> feelingsabout school, parents, peers, <strong>and</strong>their own behavior problems.4. Five composites are available for the SRP:the Emotional Symptoms Index (ESI),Inattention/Hyperactivity, InternalizingProblems, School Maladjustment, <strong>and</strong>Personal Adjustment.5. The SRP includes three validityscales.6. The SRP was normed on a nationalsample <strong>of</strong> 1,500 children <strong>and</strong> 1,900adolescents.7. The reliability <strong>of</strong> the SRP scales is goodas indicated by a variety <strong>of</strong> methods.8. Until clinical experience <strong>and</strong> furtherresearch studies are available, initialefforts at SRP interpretation shouldfocus on the scale level rather thanthe composite level because these itempools have some rational, theoretical,<strong>and</strong> research basis.9. Relatively few independent studies havebeen conducted with the most recentlyupdated YSR. However, numerous studieshave demonstrated that the previousversion <strong>of</strong> the YSR produces similarscores across cultures <strong>and</strong> consistent sexdifferences across cultures.10. The YSR does not include validityscales. However, it includes an assessment<strong>of</strong> clinical <strong>and</strong> adaptive domainsthat are relevant for most purposes <strong>of</strong>child assessment.11. The MMPI-A has ten clinical scales,eight validity scales, <strong>and</strong> several empirically-basedcontent scales.12. The MMPI-A is unusually lengthy iscomparison to other self-report inventoriesdesigned for children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.This length entails specialadministration guidelines.13. There are distinct differences in theinternal consistency estimates for theMMPI-A, with many adequate scales<strong>and</strong> some that are below minimumst<strong>and</strong>ards.14. Factor analysis <strong>of</strong> the MMPI-Areveals four factors: the first twoare labeled general anxiety <strong>and</strong>overcontrol or repression, <strong>and</strong> thethird <strong>and</strong> fourth factors are composedsolely <strong>of</strong> the Si <strong>and</strong> Mf scales,respectively.15. The self-report form <strong>of</strong> the Conners-3is a recent addition to a long-st<strong>and</strong>ingrating scale system.16. The Conners-3 SR has good initialvalidity evidence. It <strong>of</strong>fers an extensiveassessment <strong>of</strong> ADHD <strong>and</strong> disruptivebehaviors but a limited assessment <strong>of</strong>internalizing problems.17. The PIY manual provides considerableinterpretive guidance.18. The PIY used the PIC item pool (seeChap. 7) as its source <strong>of</strong> items.19. A number <strong>of</strong> single domain self-reportrating scales, particularly for internalizingproblems, are in existence. Examples<strong>of</strong> these instruments are discussedin later chapters.


C h a p t e r 7Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating ScalesChapter Questionsl How reliable are parent <strong>and</strong> teacherratings <strong>of</strong> child behavior problems?l What domains <strong>of</strong> behavior are assessedby parent <strong>and</strong> teacher rating scales?l How are parent <strong>and</strong> teacher ratingscales used in the typical psychologicalevaluation?l Why are teachers important sources <strong>of</strong>information about a child’s emotional<strong>and</strong> behavioral adjustment?l To what extent do parents <strong>and</strong> teachersagree in their ratings <strong>of</strong> children<strong>and</strong> adolescents?l What factors influence this agreement?l What factors should play a role in theuse <strong>and</strong> selection <strong>of</strong> parent <strong>and</strong> teacherrating scales?Evaluating <strong>Child</strong>ren viaParent RatingsIt has long been recognized that children are<strong>of</strong>ten less-than-accurate reporters <strong>of</strong> theirown behavior. Furthermore, children maynot have sufficient reading or oral expressionskills for self-report purposes (Lachar,1990). Problems with underreporting <strong>and</strong>response sets have always been well-recognizedby clinicians <strong>and</strong>, to some extent, havebeen documented by research (see previouschapter). These concerns about child selfreportshave undoubtedly contributed tothe popularity <strong>of</strong> parent rating scales. Furthermore,the parental perspective is <strong>of</strong>teninvaluable when conceptualizing a case; thatis, because children are <strong>of</strong>ten referred for anevaluation because <strong>of</strong> a parent’s concerns,information on the parent’s perspective <strong>of</strong> achild’s problems is critical.141P.J. Frick et al., <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Behavior,DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-0641-0_7, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010


142 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating ScalesParent ratings <strong>of</strong> child behavior possessadditional advantages, including brevity<strong>and</strong> cost efficiency (Hart & Lahey, 1999).The time-efficient nature <strong>of</strong> parent ratingsmakes it easy to collect additional informationabout child behavior. Given theimportance <strong>of</strong> parental influence on childbehavior, parental perceptions <strong>of</strong> behaviorshould routinely be collected in clinicalassessments.Today, the commonly used parent ratingscales routinely provide a broad coverage<strong>of</strong> problems. For example, whilethe unstructured interview may allowthe clinician to carefully evaluate a specificarea <strong>of</strong> functioning, other importantbehavioral problems or areas <strong>of</strong> concernmay be missed (Witt, Heffer, & Pfeiffer,1990). Parent or other caretaker ratingsalso foster objectivity <strong>and</strong> clarity inthe assessment process. Because <strong>of</strong> thebehavioral specificity <strong>of</strong> the typical itemcontent <strong>of</strong> these measures, parents arerequired to operationally define theirconcerns <strong>and</strong> provide specific <strong>and</strong> objectiveratings <strong>of</strong> hyperactivity, depression,nervousness, <strong>and</strong> the like (Witt et al.,1990).All rating scales, including parentratings, can be influenced by bias <strong>and</strong>rater response sets (Witt et al., 1990).Even biased reporting, however, can be<strong>of</strong> value. If, for example, parent ratingsprovide very different results when comparedto the ratings <strong>of</strong> others, the cliniciancan develop some important insightsinto the child’s family functioning. If achild’s father rates his son as having significantlymore behavioral problems thanthe mother, the clinician can explore thedynamics behind the ratings. A straightforwardexplanation may be that thefather is doing the majority <strong>of</strong> the childcare. This information could be importantto acquire if the presumption hadbeen that the child’s mother was providingmost <strong>of</strong> the caretaking.Factors Influencing ParentRatingsAs discussed in more detail in Chap. 15,research has indicated that parental, specificallymaternal, distress may influencethe ratings <strong>of</strong> child functioning in a negativeway. Although the issue <strong>of</strong> whether ornot maternal distress is directly influentialon the ratings <strong>of</strong> a child’s symptoms is farfrom settled, it st<strong>and</strong>s to reason that stressfulhome environments would be positivelycorrelated with parent reports <strong>of</strong> childsymptoms.The construct being evaluated <strong>and</strong>the child’s developmental level are twoadditional factors that may influence parents’reports. Teachers have traditionallybeen considered superior to parents asreporters <strong>of</strong> a child’s ADHD-like symptoms(Loeber, Green, & Lahey, 1990;Loeber et al., 1991; Tripp, Schaughency,& Clarke, 2006). However, parents arestill considered necessary <strong>and</strong> useful inproviding information about inattention<strong>and</strong> hyperactivity (Tripp et al., 2006) <strong>and</strong>in documenting the effects <strong>of</strong> treatmentfor ADHD (Biederman, Gao, Rogers, &Spencer, 2007). In addition, parents maybe in a unique position to underst<strong>and</strong> theantecedents <strong>of</strong> a child’s disruptive behaviors,as they can observe their child moreclosely than a teacher who works withseveral children simultaneously. Parentshave also been discussed as particularlyimportant observers <strong>and</strong> informants<strong>of</strong> child anxiety <strong>and</strong> depression (Klein,Dougherty, & Olino, 2005; Silverman &Ollendick, 2005).As reviewed by De Los Reyes <strong>and</strong> Kazdin(2005), research has reached mixedconclusions about the degree to whichthe child’s age influences agreement inratings across informants. Parents, inparticular, may be useful informants <strong>of</strong>a child’s functioning throughout childhood<strong>and</strong> adolescence, although there


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales143may be discrepancies between theirreports <strong>and</strong> the reports <strong>of</strong> others. Theinformation used in conjunction withparent reports may vary with age. Morespecifically, parents are obviously vitallyimportant sources <strong>of</strong> information foryoung children in such areas as conductproblems, whereas the childrenthemselves would not be reliable (<strong>and</strong>thus, not valid) informants. Teachers,however, could <strong>of</strong>fer useful perspectives<strong>of</strong> the young child’s social, academic, <strong>and</strong>behavioral functioning. For adolescents,parents may still provide valid <strong>and</strong> usefulinformation, but their knowledge<strong>of</strong> the child’s conduct <strong>and</strong> behaviorproblems may be more limited, as thebehaviors may sometimes occur outside<strong>of</strong> the parent’s awareness. The adolescent– provided that he or she is willingto provide such information – wouldbe the most knowledgeable informant<strong>of</strong> these behaviors, <strong>and</strong> the teacher’scontribution would also presumablydiminish.Finally, parent ratings are more likelyto attribute the child’s problems to dispositionalfactors in the child, whereas youthself-reports are more likely to indicate thefamily environment as a factor in need <strong>of</strong>intervention (see De Los Reyes & Kazdin,2005). Thus, informants (includingparents, teachers, <strong>and</strong> children) may basetheir ratings <strong>of</strong> a child’s functioning onthe attributions that they make regardingthe genesis <strong>and</strong> maintenance <strong>of</strong> the child’sproblems.That parent ratings may be influencedby factors that are not necessarily directlytied to the child’s actual functioning doesnot render parent ratings questionable.Instead, it calls to mind the many potentialvariables to consider in underst<strong>and</strong>ing thechild’s presenting problems – an underst<strong>and</strong>ingthat is critical for case conceptualization<strong>and</strong> subsequent recommendationsfor intervention.Evaluating <strong>Child</strong>ren viaTeacher RatingsAlthough teachers have traditionally beenconsidered an important source <strong>of</strong> informationabout children’s academic performance,they have not <strong>of</strong>ten been used inthe assessment <strong>of</strong> children’s behavioral<strong>and</strong> emotional functioning. However,knowing how a child behaves in the classroomis important for several reasons.First, school is a setting in which thechild spends several hours a day. Therefore,a child’s adjustment to the schoolsetting can have a dramatic impact onhis or her overall psychological functioning.Second, the multiple dem<strong>and</strong>s <strong>of</strong> theschool environment (e.g., to stay seated,to follow the dem<strong>and</strong>s <strong>of</strong> adults, to interactwith classmates) present many challengesto the child— challenges that maynot be present in other settings. Third,the dem<strong>and</strong>s <strong>of</strong> the school setting changeas a child progresses through school (e.g.,dem<strong>and</strong>s for organization, the importance<strong>of</strong> social acceptance). Therefore,underst<strong>and</strong>ing school-related problemsthat are unique to a given period can provideclues to specific problems in adaptationthat a child or adolescent mightexperience.On the basis <strong>of</strong> these considerations,there is increasing interest in assessing achild’s behavioral <strong>and</strong> emotional functioningin the school setting. Given the manyadvantages <strong>of</strong> behavior rating scales, suchas time-efficiency <strong>and</strong> objectivity, it is notsurprising that the primary assessmentinstruments for children’s school behaviorhave been teacher-completed behaviorrating scales. In addition to suggestionsfor appropriate use <strong>of</strong> rating scales in generaldiscussed previously, there are severalconsiderations for interpreting informationfrom teachers that warrant specialattention.


144 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating ScalesFactors Influencing TeacherRatingsAs described above for parents, the usefulness<strong>of</strong> teacher information may varydepending on what type <strong>of</strong> behavior isbeing assessed. Teachers are <strong>of</strong>ten consideredthe best source <strong>of</strong> information abouta child’s attention problems <strong>and</strong> overactivitybecause they have the opportunityto observe the child in a situation thatdem<strong>and</strong>s sustained attention <strong>and</strong> inactivity.In contrast, teachers’ ratings tend to be lessuseful in assessing many types <strong>of</strong> antisocialbehavior that are unlikely to occur in theschool environment (e.g., setting things onfire, being cruel to animals) or for internalizedtypes <strong>of</strong> problems that may not bereadily observable in the classroom setting(Loeber et al., 1991).The usefulness <strong>of</strong> teacher informationmay also vary according to the age <strong>of</strong> thechild. <strong>Child</strong>ren in early elementary schoolfrequently have one teacher who observes achild across several class periods, if not theentire school day. In contrast, high schoolteachers frequently have students for oneclass period during the day. Therefore, theusefulness <strong>of</strong> information may decrease asa child advances in school <strong>and</strong> contact withany single teacher decreases (Edelbrocket al., 1985).A final issue in interpreting teacherrating scales is underst<strong>and</strong>ing the frame<strong>of</strong> reference or st<strong>and</strong>ard used by teachers.As discussed previously (e.g., Piacentini,1993), a number <strong>of</strong> characteristics <strong>of</strong>a rater can influence his or her judgment<strong>of</strong> the intensity, quality, <strong>and</strong>/or frequency<strong>of</strong> a child’s behavior. In the case <strong>of</strong> teacherratings, a characteristic <strong>of</strong> teachers that caninfluence their ratings is their experiencewith many children <strong>of</strong> the same age. Experienceallows the teacher to make someinternal normative comparison <strong>of</strong> a child’sbehavior with the behavior <strong>of</strong> other childrenthe teacher has taught. This internalnorm is a double-edged sword. It <strong>of</strong>tengives the teacher a unique perspective <strong>of</strong>knowing both the individual child <strong>and</strong> thebehaviors that are age-appropriate. However,some teachers, such as teachers whowork in special education classrooms, mayhave a skewed base <strong>of</strong> comparison thatcould influence their ratings. That is, theirratings <strong>of</strong> a child’s behavior may be influencedby a comparison <strong>of</strong> the child withother disturbed children.Despite these cautions <strong>and</strong> limitations,we feel strongly that teacher ratings arean essential element <strong>of</strong> a comprehensiveclinical assessment <strong>of</strong> children’s behavioral<strong>and</strong> emotional functioning. Carlson <strong>and</strong>Lahey (1983), in an early review <strong>of</strong> teacherratings, reported that most <strong>of</strong> the teacherrating scales available at that time sufferedfrom significant psychometric problemsin development <strong>and</strong> inadequate norming.As a result, the available scales wereseverely limited in their usefulness forclinical evaluations. Fortunately, since that1983 review, there have been numerousadvances in the teacher rating scales <strong>and</strong>the emergence <strong>of</strong> new scales, with many<strong>of</strong> the inadequacies <strong>of</strong> earlier scales eliminatedor greatly reduced.Overview <strong>of</strong> Omnibus Parent<strong>and</strong> Teacher RatingScalesParent <strong>and</strong> teacher rating scales are notinterchangeable <strong>and</strong>, with the seeminglyexponential growth <strong>of</strong> such instruments,psychologists have to make many decisionsabout the utility <strong>of</strong> various measures.This chapter attempts to aid theclinician in decision making by providingan overview <strong>of</strong> the variety <strong>of</strong> scales available,with particular attention devotedto defining the strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses<strong>of</strong> each measure. Writing such a chapterrequires selectivity. Hence, if a scale is not


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales145mentioned in this chapter, it should notbe construed as a judgment <strong>of</strong> the quality<strong>of</strong> the scale. As with our chapter on selfreportrating scales, we have attempted toreview those instruments that are widelyused <strong>and</strong>/or part <strong>of</strong> a long-st<strong>and</strong>ing system<strong>of</strong> rating scales used for child <strong>and</strong>adolescent assessment. This broad overview<strong>of</strong> the various scales is not designedto replace information provided in thetechnical manuals that accompany theseinstruments, to be reviewed by any user<strong>of</strong> the scales. Optimally, however, theprinciples applied to evaluating parent<strong>and</strong> teacher rating scales in this chaptercan be used by psychologists to evaluateother scales as well.The parent <strong>and</strong> teacher rating scalesreviewed in this chapter are highlightedin Tables 7.1 <strong>and</strong> 7.2, respectively. Commonlyused omnibus measures that assessmany different domains, as opposed tosingle construct measures, are the focus.Although these scales are discussed in isolationto balance clarity <strong>and</strong> specificity, itshould be recalled that they are <strong>of</strong>ten part<strong>of</strong> larger multimethod assessment methodsthat are discussed in various chapters<strong>of</strong> this book. The integration <strong>of</strong> components<strong>and</strong> information from differentinformants <strong>and</strong> methods is discussed inthe context <strong>of</strong> interpretation in Chap. 15<strong>and</strong> in subsequent chapters that addressspecific syndromes.Behavior <strong>Assessment</strong> Systemfor <strong>Child</strong>ren, 2nd Edition(BASC-2)Parent Rating Scale (PRS)The BASC-2 Parent Rating Scale (BASC-2-PRS; Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004) ispart <strong>of</strong> the larger BASC system. The PRSwas published concurrently with the SRP(discussed in Chap. 6) <strong>and</strong> TRS (see below)as well as other components <strong>of</strong> the BASCassessment system (Reynolds & Kamphaus,2004). The PRS has three formscomposed <strong>of</strong> similar items <strong>and</strong> scales thatspan the preschool (2–5 years), child (6–11years), <strong>and</strong> adolescent (12–21 years) ageranges. The PRS takes a broad sampling <strong>of</strong>a child’s behavior in home <strong>and</strong> communitysettings.ContentAs with its predecessor, the BASC-2-PRSwas developed using both rational/theoretical<strong>and</strong> empirical means in combination toconstruct the individual scales. The benefit<strong>of</strong> this approach is that the resulting scaleshave relatively homogenous content. Theuniqueness <strong>of</strong> the scales was also enhancedby not including items on more than onescale. Table 7.3 provides item examples foreach scale. There are four composites: ExternalizingProblems, Internalizing Problems,Adaptive Skills, <strong>and</strong> a Behavioral SymptomsIndex that includes some internalizing <strong>and</strong>externalizing scales (i.e., Atypicality, AttentionProblems, Hyperactivity, Aggression,Depression, <strong>and</strong> Withdrawal).Two types <strong>of</strong> scales are included at eachage level: clinical <strong>and</strong> adaptive. <strong>Clinical</strong>scales <strong>of</strong> the PRS are designed to measurebehavior problems much like other measuresdiscussed below in that behavioralexcesses (e.g., hitting others) are the focus<strong>of</strong> assessment. The PRS also includes criticalitems that are thought to warrant followupor clinical attention in their own right.These items (e.g., “Has a hearing problem.”)are not necessarily indicative <strong>of</strong> themost severe pathology; instead, they maybe worthy <strong>of</strong> further questioning or recommendationsby the clinician. The adaptivescales measure behaviors (e.g., complimentsothers) or skills that are associated with


146 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating ScalesTable 7.1 Overview <strong>of</strong> Parent Rating ScalesInventory Ages ContentBehavior <strong>Assessment</strong>System for<strong>Child</strong>ren, 2ndedition (BASC-2;Reynolds &Kamphaus, 2004)Achenbach <strong>Child</strong>Behavior Checklist(CBCL; Achenbach& Rescorla,2000; Achenbach& Rescorla, 2001)2–5; 6–11;12–21134 –160 items;BehavioralSymptoms Index,Adaptive Skills,InternalizingProblems; 3 validityscales; 9 clinicalscales, 5 adaptivescales; 7 optionalcontent scales1½– 5; 6–18 100–113 items;Internalizing,Externalizing,Competence; 8problem scales;6 DSM-orientedscales<strong>Child</strong> SymptomInventory-4(Gadow &Sprafkin, 1998)ESI/CSI/ASI3–5; 5–12;12–1897 items correspondingto 17disordersReadingLevelTime toAdministerNormSample Reliability Validity MI4th grade 10–20 min E Internal consistency(good); 1–7week test–retest(good); Interrater(moderate to high)Moderate to highwith similar tools;Support forfactorsENot given 15–20 min G Internal consistency(good forschool age version);1 week test–retest(good); interrater(moderate forpreschool version;good for school ageversion)Not given 10–15 min F Internal consistency(moderate togood); Test-retest;content (moderateto good)Good validity(clinical vs.nonclinicalgroups); limitedinfo. on relationswith other tools;limited info. oncurrent versionContent derivedfrom DSMcriteria;Evidence <strong>of</strong>construct validity(good)EG(Continues)


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales147Table 7.1 (Continued)Inventory Ages ContentReadingLevelTime toAdministerNormSample Reliability Validity MIConners 3rd Edition,Parent RatingScale (Conners-3;Conners, 2008a)<strong>Personality</strong> Inventoryfor <strong>Child</strong>ren,2nd edition(PIC-2; Lachar &Gruber, 2001)6–18 110 items;43-item shortform; 3 validityscales; 6 empiricalscales; 1 rationalscale; 5 DSM-IV-TR scales5–19 275 items, 96item short form;9 clinical scales(several subscales);3 validityscales4th–5thgrade20 min E Internal consistency,2–4 weektest–retest, <strong>and</strong>interrater reliabilityall good4th grade 40 min E Internal consistency;test–retest;interrater; Goodreliability, somewhatlower forsubscalesGood evidence <strong>of</strong>criterion-related<strong>and</strong> differentialvalidityValidity (based onteacher <strong>and</strong> selfreports<strong>of</strong> samesystem); lack <strong>of</strong>research on relationswith othercriteriaE aGNote: Norm Sample evaluated as E = Excellent, G = Good, F = Fair; MI = Degree <strong>of</strong> enhancement <strong>of</strong> multi-informant assessment; E = Excellent, G = Good;ESI = Early Symptom Inventory; CSI = <strong>Child</strong> Symptom Inventory; ASI = <strong>Adolescent</strong> Symptom Inventory.aWe view the Conners-3 as providing an excellent means <strong>of</strong> assessment <strong>of</strong> externalizing problems; however, if an extensive assessment <strong>of</strong> internalizing or learningdifficulties is desired, the Conners Comprehensive Behavior Rating Scales (CBRS; Conners, 2008b) is recommended.


148 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating ScalesTable 7.2 Overview <strong>of</strong> Teacher Rating ScalesInventory Ages ContentBehavior <strong>Assessment</strong>System for<strong>Child</strong>ren, 2ndedition (BASC-2;Reynolds &Kamphaus, 2004)AchenbachTeacher RatingForm (TRF;Achenbach &Rescorla, 2000;Achenbach &Rescorla, 2001)<strong>Child</strong> SymptomInventory-4(Gadow &Sprafkin, 1998)ESI/CSI/ASIConners 3rdEdition, TeacherRating scale(Conners-3-T;Conners, 2008a)2–5; 6–11;12–21100–139 items; BehavioralSymptoms Index,Adaptive Skills, ExternalizingProblems,Internalizing Problems,School Problems;3 validity scales; 10clinical scales, 5 adaptivescales, 7 optionalcontent scales1½–5; 6–18 100–113 items; Internalizing,Externalizing,Adaptive Functioning;8 problem scales; 6DSM-oriented scales3–5; 5–12;12–1877 Items correspondingto 13 disorders8–18 115 Items; 39-itemshort form; 3 validityscales, 6 empiricalscales, 1 rational scale,5 DSM-IV-TRsymptom scalesReadingLevelTime toAdministerNormSample Reliability Validity MINot given 10–20 min E Internal consistency(good); 1–9week test–retest(good); interraterreliability (goodwith exception <strong>of</strong>Somatiz)Moderate to highrelations with similartools; Supportfor factorsENot given 15–20 min G Internal consistency(good); 7month test–retest(moderate);interrater (low tomode-rate)Good validity (clinicalvs. nonclinicalgroups); limitedinfo. on relationswith other toolsENot given 10 min F Test–retest;limited evidenceavailable4th–5thgrade20 min G Internal consistency<strong>and</strong> test–retest reliability(good). Interraterreliability (moderateto good)Content derivedfrom DSM criteria;Classification ratesmatch well withepidemiologyGood evidence <strong>of</strong>criterion-related<strong>and</strong> differentialvalidityEE a(Continues)


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales149Table 7.2 (continued)Inventory Ages ContentReadingLevelTime toAdministerNormSample Reliability Validity MIStudent BehaviorSurvey (SBS;Lachar, Wingenfeld,Kline, &Gruber, 2000)5–18 102 items; AcademicResources, HealthConcerns, EmotionalDistress, UnusualBehavior, Social, <strong>and</strong>Aggression compositesNot given 10–15 min E Good reliability(internal consistency;test–retest)Good convergentvalidity for mostscales; DBD scalesdo not overlap fullywith DSM criteria;Limited validityevidence for somescalesFNote: Norm Sample evaluated as E = Excellent, G = Good; MI = Degree <strong>of</strong> enhancement <strong>of</strong> multi-informant assessment; E = Excellent, G = Good, F = Fair; ESI =Early Symptom Inventory; CSI = <strong>Child</strong> Symptom Inventory; ASI = <strong>Adolescent</strong> Symptom Inventory; DBD = Disruptive Behavior Disorder.aWe view the Conners-3 as providing an excellent means <strong>of</strong> assessment <strong>of</strong> externalizing problems; however, if an extensive assessment <strong>of</strong> internalizing or learningdifficulties is desired, the Conners Comprehensive Behavior Rating Scales (CBRS; Conners, 2008b) is recommended.


150 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating ScalesTable 7.3 BASC-2-PRS Scale Definitions <strong>and</strong> Key Symptoms as Indicated by Items with theHighest Factor Loadings Per ScaleActivities <strong>of</strong> Daily LivingAdaptabilityAggressionAnxietyAttention ProblemsAtypicalityConduct ProblemsDepressionFunctional CommunicationHyperactivityLeadershipSocial SkillsSomatizationWithdrawalSkills associated with performing everyday tasks; “Acts in a safe manner”,“Sets realistic goals”, “Attends to issues <strong>of</strong> personal safety”Ability to adapt to changes in the environment; “Adjusts easily to newsurroundings”, “Adjusts well to changes in family plans”, “Recoversquickly after a setback”Tendency to act in hostile or threatening manner; “Is cruel to others”,“Loses temper too easily”, “Annoys others on purpose”Tendency to be nervous, fearful, or worried; “Worries about makingmistakes”, “Worries about what other children think”, “Is nervous”Tendency to be easily distracted or have difficulty concentrating;“Has a short attention span” “Listens carefully” (reverse scored); “Iseasily distracted”Tendency to behave in odd manner; “Acts strangely.” “Says thingsthat make no sense”, “Seems out <strong>of</strong> touch with reality”Tendency to engage in antisocial <strong>and</strong> rule-breaking behavior; “Breaksthe rules”; “Deceives others”; “Gets into trouble”Feelings <strong>of</strong> unhappiness, sadness, or stress; “Is negative about things”,“Says ‘I don’t have any friends’”, “Seems lonely”Ability to communicate ideas <strong>and</strong> express oneself clearly; “Communicatesclearly”, “Responds appropriately when asked a question”,“Accurately takes down messages”Tendency to be overly active <strong>and</strong> act without thinking; “Acts out <strong>of</strong>control”, “Interrupts others when they are speaking”, “Disrupts otherchildren’s activities”Possessing skills needed to accomplish goals, ability to work with others;“Gives good suggestions for solving problems”, “Is creative”, “Isa ‘self-starter’”Having the skills necessary to interact successfully with peers <strong>and</strong>adults; “Encourages others to do their best”, “Offers to help otherchildren”, Congratulates others when good things happen to them”Tendency to be sensitive to, <strong>and</strong> complain about, minor physical ailments;“Complains about health”; “Gets sick”; “Complains <strong>of</strong> beingsick when nothing is wrong”Tendency to avoid others; “Makes friends easily” (reverse scored),“Avoids other children”, “Quickly joins group activities” (reversescored)Note: Adapted from Tables 7.7 <strong>and</strong> 10.3 <strong>of</strong> the BASC-2 manual (Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004).The clinicalnorms may be especially important when assessing a child in a residential setting to be able to compare him orher to others with relatively severe difficulties. That is, it may be understood that a child is functioning poorlycompared to most other children his/her age (i.e., elevations on general norms), but it may be informative toconsider how the child functions (e.g., “How severe are his conduct problems?”) in comparison to other childrenwith emotional <strong>and</strong> behavioral difficulties for treatment planning purposes.


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales151good adaptation to home <strong>and</strong> community(see Table 7.3).Each <strong>of</strong> the parent forms <strong>of</strong> the BASC-2includes seven optional content scales:Anger Control, Bullying, Developmental,Social Disorders, Emotional Self-control,Emotional Self-control, ExecutiveFunctioning, Negative Emotionality, <strong>and</strong>Resiliency. As with the BASC-2-SRP(see Chap. 6), the content scales for theBASC-2-PRS were constructed via theoretical<strong>and</strong> empirical methods. Thesescales were developed based on currenttheoretical perspectives about importantdomains <strong>of</strong> youth functioning (Reynolds &Kamphaus, 2004). There exists very littleresearch on these scales, yet their labels <strong>and</strong>item content are intriguing <strong>and</strong> warrantfurther investigation <strong>of</strong> their reliability, validity,<strong>and</strong> clinical utility. Initial analyses indicatethat the PRS content scales possess adequate(i.e., 0.70 <strong>and</strong> higher) internal consistencies(Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004).Administration <strong>and</strong> ScoringThe PRS uses a four-choice response format(i.e., never, sometimes, <strong>of</strong>ten, almost always)with no space allowed for parent elaboration.According to the authors, the scale takesabout 10–20 min for parents to complete.A variety <strong>of</strong> derived scores <strong>and</strong> interpretivedevices are <strong>of</strong>fered. Linear T-scoresare available for all scales <strong>and</strong> composites,meaning that the original distributions forthese indices in the norming sample weremaintained. Other scores available includepercentile ranks, confidence b<strong>and</strong>s, <strong>and</strong>statistical methods for identifying high <strong>and</strong>low points in a pr<strong>of</strong>ile. Both h<strong>and</strong>-scoring<strong>and</strong> computer entry scoring are availablefor the PRS.NormingPRS provides three norm-referenced comparisonsdepending on the questions <strong>of</strong>interest to the clinician. Some examples <strong>of</strong>questions <strong>and</strong> their implications for normIs Daniel inattentive incomparison to children<strong>of</strong> the same age?Is Daniel inattentive incomparison to a large samplesample <strong>of</strong> children who arecurrently diagnosed <strong>and</strong>receiving treatment?Is Daniel inattentive incomparison to boys <strong>of</strong> thesame age?group selection include the following.Question Norm GroupGeneral nationalsample<strong>Clinical</strong> nationalsampleMale nationalThese various norm-referenced comparisonsare more than are typically <strong>of</strong>feredfor such scales. The general national normingsample is advised as the starting pointfor most purposes (Reynolds & Kamphaus,2004). Of course, as just noted, dependingon the question, the clinician may opt forgender-specific or clinical norms. Genderspecificnorms may be useful in trying toconvey a child’s current level <strong>of</strong> functioningto others, such as parents. In otherwords, one might present the child’s scoresrelative to the general population <strong>and</strong>then emphasize how the child comparesto other boys/girls on areas <strong>of</strong> concern inorder to provide a more complete picture.However, too much information may causeconfusion for some parents. Gender-basednorms may also help answer some specificresearch questions (i.e., correlates <strong>of</strong> inattention<strong>and</strong> hyperactivity among girls <strong>and</strong>among boys).The general norm sample for the PRSincluded 1,200 preschoolers, 1,800 children,<strong>and</strong> 1,800 adolescents. Cases werecollected at test sites in 40 states. Across agegroups, the PRS sample closely matchesUS Census statistics (Current PopulationSurvey, 2001) in terms <strong>of</strong> sex, race/ethnic-


152 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scalesity, <strong>and</strong> socioeconomic status (SES). Thenorming sample also represents a good fit tocensus data on geographic region (seeReynolds & Kamphaus, 2004 for moredetails). The clinical norming sample forthe PRS included responses from 1,975parents, with most cases being classified ashaving a learning disability or ADHD.ReliabilityThe median reliability coefficients providedin the manual suggest good evidence forthe reliability <strong>of</strong> the individual scales <strong>and</strong>composites. All scales <strong>and</strong> composites havemedian reliability estimates <strong>of</strong> 0.80 <strong>and</strong>above, with the exception <strong>of</strong> the Activities<strong>of</strong> Daily Living <strong>and</strong> Atypicality Scales. TheBASC-2 manual also provides informationon 1–7-week test-retest reliability <strong>and</strong> interraterreliability between parents. Test-retestreliability coefficients were 0.70 <strong>and</strong> higher,with the exception <strong>of</strong> Depression for thepreschool form which was .66. Interraterreliability was generally good, with coefficientsin the same range, with the exception<strong>of</strong> Aggression on the preschool <strong>and</strong> childforms (i.e., 0.59 <strong>and</strong> 0.58, respectively) <strong>and</strong>Anxiety on the preschool form (.56).ValidityThe PRS appears to have a broad contentcoverage. The PRS assesses a variety <strong>of</strong>externalizing behavior problems (McMahon& Frick, 2002) <strong>and</strong> has an exp<strong>and</strong>edassessment <strong>of</strong> adaptive skills. In addition,the PRS enjoys considerable factoranalytic support for a three factormodel consisting <strong>of</strong> externalizing problems,internalizing problems, <strong>and</strong> adaptiveskills. The strongest measures <strong>of</strong> theexternalizing factor are the Aggression,Conduct Problems <strong>and</strong> Hyperactivity.The Internalizing factor is marked byloadings by the Atypicality, Depression,Anxiety, Somatization, <strong>and</strong> Withdrawalscales. Adaptive skills scales that loadhighly on this factor include Activities <strong>of</strong>Daily Living, Functional Communication,Leadership, <strong>and</strong> Social Skills.Some <strong>of</strong> the secondary loadings for thescales may also have implications for interpretation.Specifically, the factor-analyticdata suggest that the following pr<strong>of</strong>iles arereasonable:l Poor Adaptive Skills with AttentionProblemsl Good Adaptive Skills with Anxietyl Internalizing Problems accompanied byPoor AdaptabilityCriterion-related validity analyses producedconsistent associations between the PRS <strong>and</strong>other parent rating scales. This pattern particularlyholds for the composites <strong>and</strong> for theexternalizing problem scales. Generally, theinternalizing problem scales (e.g., Anxiety,Depression, Somatization) show moderatecorrelations with analogous scales from othermeasures (see Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004).InterpretationThe same logical interpretive steps that wereoutlined for the BASC-SRP (discussed inChap. 6) also apply to the BASC-PRS. Briefly,the clinician should:1. Assess validity using validity indexes <strong>and</strong>informal means (e.g., inspect for a highnumber <strong>of</strong> items with no response).2. Inspect critical items <strong>and</strong> follow-up asappropriate.3. Interpret scores on scales <strong>and</strong> composites,with particular attention to elevations(T-scores <strong>of</strong> 65–70 <strong>and</strong> higher) onclinical scales <strong>and</strong> low scores (T-scores <strong>of</strong>35 <strong>and</strong> below) on adaptive scales.4. Attend to items that appeared to haveled to scale elevations (or low adaptivescores).5. Integrate score with information fromother informants.


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales1536. Integrate data with information fromother assessment tools (e.g., interview,behavioral observations, intelligencetesting).7. Set objectives for treatment/interventionAs was the case with the SRP, we again recommenda focus on interpretation at the scale level, as thereliabilities <strong>of</strong> the PRS scales are generally good,<strong>and</strong> elevations on scales are more specificallyinformative than would be the case for elevatedcomposite scores.The original PRS enjoyed a great deal <strong>of</strong>research support <strong>and</strong> research use. There isquite limited information available to dateon the BASC-2-PRS. Nevertheless, thecombined rational <strong>and</strong> empirical approachto scale development has intuitive appealfor use in clinical situations. Clinicians arestill urged to keep abreast <strong>of</strong> the researchliterature discussing the strengths <strong>and</strong> limitations<strong>of</strong> any assessment tool.Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesThe BASC-2-PRS has a number <strong>of</strong> apparentstrengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses as follows:The strengths <strong>of</strong> the PRS are:1. Good psychometric properties based onthe information reported in the BASC-2manual.2. A variety <strong>of</strong> scales that may be usefulfor differential diagnosis (e.g., AttentionProblems vs. Hyperactivity, <strong>and</strong> Anxietyvs. Depression).3. The availability <strong>of</strong> validity scales <strong>and</strong>critical items.4. An exp<strong>and</strong>ed group <strong>of</strong> norm-referencedadaptive scalesAmong the weaknesses <strong>of</strong> the PRS are:1. A response format that does not allowparents to provide additional detail abouttheir responses2. Cross-informant <strong>and</strong> cross-scale comparisonsnot as readily made as on othermeasures, as different forms (e.g., parentvs. self-report) include differentitem content <strong>and</strong> scales3. Limited research on the latest edition <strong>of</strong>the PRS.Teacher Rating Scale (TRS)The BASC-2 Teacher Rating Scale (BASC-2-TRS; Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004)allows the clinician to gather informationon a child’s observable behavior from thechild’s teacher <strong>and</strong> place that information inthe context <strong>of</strong> other information obtained inthe overall BASC system (e.g., self-reportscale, parent rating scales, classroom observationsystem). As with the PRS, there arethree forms <strong>of</strong> the BASC-2-TRS: preschool(ages 2–5), child (6–11), <strong>and</strong> adolescent(12–21). The three forms contain behavioraldescriptors that are rated by the teacher on afour-point scale <strong>of</strong> frequency, ranging from“Never” to “Almost Always.” The threeforms have 100 items for the preschool version<strong>and</strong> 139 for both the child <strong>and</strong> adolescentversions.ContentAs with the other BASC-2 rating scales, theitems <strong>of</strong> the BASC-2-TRS were chosen tomeasure multiple aspects <strong>of</strong> a child’s personality<strong>and</strong> behavior. The TRS includesboth positive (adaptive) <strong>and</strong> pathological(clinical) dimensions. For the most part,the BASC-2-TRS has maintained the contentareas <strong>of</strong> the original BASC. The onlyscale additions to the current version <strong>of</strong> theTRS were the Functional Communicationscale for all age groups <strong>and</strong> the Adaptabilityscale for the adolescent version.The BASC-2-TRS consists <strong>of</strong> five composites(i.e., Behavioral Symptoms Index,School Problems, Externalizing Problems,Internalizing Problems, Adaptive Skills)


154 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scalesacross all age ranges, with 11 scales inthe preschool version <strong>and</strong> 15 scales in thechild <strong>and</strong> adolescent versions. The scalesgrouped into the composites – except forthe Behavioral Symptoms Index whichincludes the Hyperactivity, Atypicality,Depression, Aggression, Attention Problems,<strong>and</strong> Withdrawal scales – are providedin Table 7.4. The TRS also has the sameoptional content scales as those providedfor the PRS (see above). Because these area new feature <strong>of</strong> the BASC-2, very limitedinformation is available on their psychometricproperties or clinical utility.The content coverage <strong>of</strong> the BASC-2-TRS scales has several unique features relativeto other teacher rating scales. First,it provides comprehensive coverage <strong>of</strong>several areas <strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior. Second,the current version <strong>of</strong> the TRS continuesthe strategy <strong>of</strong> including separate scales formotor hyperactivity <strong>and</strong> attention problems,which aids in the differentiation <strong>of</strong>subtypes <strong>of</strong> Attention-Deficit HyperactivityDisorder (Vaughn, Riccio, Hynd, &Hall, 1997). Third, there are separate scalesfor anxiety, depression, <strong>and</strong> withdrawal,which aid in the assessment <strong>of</strong> emotionaldifficulties. Fourth, the BASC-2-TRSTable 7.4 Composites <strong>and</strong> Scales <strong>of</strong> BASC-2-TRSCompositeExternalizing ProblemsInternalizing ProblemsSchool ProblemsAdaptive SkillsScalesAggression HyperactivityConductProblemsAnxiety DepressionSomatizationAttention ProblemsLearning ProblemsAdaptability FunctionalCommunicationLeadershipSocial SkillsStudy Skillsincludes items that screen for learningproblems that <strong>of</strong>ten accompany emotional<strong>and</strong> behavioral problems in children.Administration <strong>and</strong> ScoringThe BASC-TRS takes approximately10–20 min to complete. The cover <strong>of</strong> therecord provides instructions to the teacherfor completing the form <strong>and</strong> space forrecording background information aboutthe child <strong>and</strong> teacher (e.g., age, gender,type <strong>of</strong> class, length <strong>of</strong> time in class). Bothh<strong>and</strong> scoring <strong>and</strong> computer scoring areavailable. Norm tables in the BASC manualare provided so that any <strong>of</strong> four sets <strong>of</strong>norms can be used: general, male, female,<strong>and</strong> clinical (see above for discussion <strong>of</strong>the uses <strong>of</strong> these different types <strong>of</strong> norms).Both T-scores <strong>and</strong> percentile ranks arelisted for each set <strong>of</strong> norms, with linearT-scores again being utilized for the TRS.As with the other BASC-2 rating scales,the BASC-2-TRS scoring sheet highlightscritical items (e.g., “I want to kill myself’)that are clinically important or that warrantfurther follow-up.NormingThe norming group included 1,050 preschoolers,1,800 children (ages 6–11), <strong>and</strong>1,800 adolescents (ages 12–21) with equalsex distributions in all age groups. Respondentswere recruited from sites throughoutthe USA. As described previously, the samplingprocedures for obtaining the normativesample were designed to closely mirrorUS Census statistics in terms <strong>of</strong> race/ethnicity,SES, <strong>and</strong> geographic region, <strong>and</strong>this goal was accomplished (see Reynolds& Kamphaus, 2004). Details regarding the1,779-member clinical sample for the TRSare also provided in the BASC-2 Manual.ReliabilityThe manual for the BASC-2-TRS (Reynolds& Kamphaus, 2004) provides evidence


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales155on three types <strong>of</strong> reliability: internal consistency,test-retest reliability, <strong>and</strong> interraterreliability. With very few exceptions,the scales <strong>of</strong> the BASC-2-TRS proved tobe quite reliable in the normative sample.More specifically, internal consistency coefficientstended to average well above 0.80across all age groups, <strong>and</strong> all were 0.75 orhigher. Similarly, test-retest reliability overone to nine weeks was high, with the exception<strong>of</strong> the Anxiety scale, with coefficientsranging from 0.64 for the adolescent versionto 0.77 for the adolescent version. Still,these coefficients are adequate. Finally, theconsistency <strong>of</strong> ratings between two teacherswas tested in samples <strong>of</strong> preschool-agechildren (n = 74), school-age children (n =38), <strong>and</strong> adolescents (n = 58), with moderatereliability estimates emerging acrossage group samples (median coefficients <strong>of</strong>0.69, 0.60, <strong>and</strong> 0.52, respectively). Correlationcoefficients tend to be somewhathigher for externalizing than for internalizingproblems consistent with pastresearch (Achenbach, McConaughy, &Howell, 1987). It is also worth noting thatthe coefficients tended to be lower foradolescents, which may be associated withthe limited contact an individual teachermay have with students <strong>of</strong> that age group.Interrater agreement for Somatization(r = 0.25), Withdrawal (r = 0.24), <strong>and</strong> Atypicality(r = 0.31) was particularly low forteacher ratings <strong>of</strong> adolescents.ValidityThe TRS is closely, but not exactly, alignedwith the item content <strong>of</strong> the PRS. However,the TRS has additional scales (i.e., StudySkills, Learning Problems) that seem particularlyvalid for use with a teacher ratingscale. The BASC-2 manual provides factoranalytic support for the construct validity<strong>of</strong> the scales <strong>and</strong> composites <strong>of</strong> the TRS.In addition, initial research on the TRSshows generally high correlations withanalogous scales from other teacher ratingscales. However, the correspondenceto analogous scales is somewhat lower forinternalizing types <strong>of</strong> problems than forthe indices <strong>of</strong> externalizing problems (seeReynolds & Kamphaus, 2004). One notablefinding was the lack <strong>of</strong> a correlation (i.e.,r = .03) between the TRS Somatizationscale <strong>and</strong> the Somatic Complaints scale <strong>of</strong>the Achenbach Teacher Report Form. Asignificant limitation <strong>of</strong> the latest version<strong>of</strong> the TRS is the very limited research onits validity <strong>and</strong> utility outside <strong>of</strong> what wasconducted by the developers.InterpretationThe BASC-2-TRS includes validity scalesthat provide a useful <strong>and</strong> efficient firstpoint <strong>of</strong> interpretation. More specifically,it contains a “fake bad” index (F), whichhelps to assess the possibility that a teacherrated a child in an overly negative pattern.Therefore, interpretation <strong>of</strong> this scale, inparticular, should be the first step in theinterpretative process, keeping in mind that ahigh score on the F index may actually indicatesignificantly problematic functioning.Therefore, this validity index shouldbe interpreted in the context <strong>of</strong> otherassessment data. The Consistency Index<strong>and</strong> the Response Pattern Index availablefor the TRS (as are available for the PRS<strong>and</strong> SRP) provide another initial point <strong>of</strong>interpretation. Critical items should bereviewed promptly, because these itemstend to be clinically important indicatorsthat deserve careful follow-up assessment.The reliability estimates at the scalelevel <strong>of</strong> the TRS are good; therefore, weagain recommend focusing interpretationmainly at the scale, rather than composite,level since more specific information isavailable through the TRS scales. Interpretationsat the item level must be made quitecautiously because <strong>of</strong> the low reliability <strong>of</strong>individual items. It is <strong>of</strong>ten informative tosee which items led to a child’s or adolescent’selevation on a given clinical scales.For example, it may be informative for achild with an elevation on the Adaptabil-


156 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scalesity scale to determine if this elevation waslargely due to problems specifically withinthe interpersonal domains or due to moregeneral problems in adapting to changes inroutine. Finally, interpretation at the scalelevel for the parent form is a viable earlystep in interpretation (see above); therefore,interpretation at the scale level <strong>of</strong> theTRS facilitates integration <strong>of</strong> informationacross parent <strong>and</strong> teacher ratings. In addition,considering individual items withinelevated scales on both rating forms mayhelp determine the source <strong>of</strong> consistencies<strong>and</strong> inconsistencies across parent <strong>and</strong>teacher ratings, further informing caseconceptualization <strong>and</strong> recommendations.Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesLike its predecessor <strong>and</strong> its companion parentrating scale, the BASC-2-TRS has anumber <strong>of</strong> strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses. Notablestrengths include:1. It is part <strong>of</strong> a multimethod, multi-informantsystem that aids in a comprehensiveclinical evaluation with item content thatcovers important problematic <strong>and</strong> adaptivedomains <strong>of</strong> classroom behavioral <strong>and</strong>emotional functioning.2. The assessment <strong>of</strong> adaptive functioningis enhanced on this version <strong>of</strong> the TRS.3. The preschool age range <strong>of</strong> the BASC-2-TRS is exp<strong>and</strong>ed from the age rangeavailable from the original TRS.4. The BASC-2-TRS has a large nationwidenormative sample on which normreferencedscores are based, allowing forone to confidently make many normreferencedinterpretations <strong>of</strong> scores.Weaknesses <strong>of</strong> the BASC-2-TRS include:1. The limited research base for the currentedition.2. The relatively lower correlationsbetween internalizing scales on the TRF<strong>and</strong> analogous scales from other teacherrating scales.3. The different item content across informants,especially with the SRP, makesintegration <strong>of</strong> BASC-2 informationsomewhat more challenging.A sample case using the BASC-2-PRS <strong>and</strong>BASC-2 TRS is provided in Box 7.1.Achenbach System <strong>of</strong> EmpiricallyBased <strong>Assessment</strong>(Achenbach & Rescorla,2000, 2001)Parent Report: <strong>Child</strong> BehaviorChecklist (CBCL)The <strong>Child</strong> Behavior Checklist (CBCL;Achenbach, 2001) <strong>and</strong> its predecessors havea long history <strong>of</strong> prominence in child assessment.The CBCL scale is the product <strong>of</strong> anextensive multiple-decade research effort,<strong>and</strong> it has a distinguished history <strong>of</strong> researchusage. The current version <strong>of</strong> the CBCL ismuch like its predecessors with some itemchanges, response format changes, <strong>and</strong> theintroduction <strong>of</strong> DSM-Oriented scales (seebelow; see Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001).The CBCL is part <strong>of</strong> an extensive system<strong>of</strong> scales including teacher rating (TRF),self-report (YSR), <strong>and</strong> classroom observationmeasures. The newest version <strong>of</strong> the CBCL(Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001) has two separateforms: one for ages 1½–5 <strong>and</strong> the otherfor children <strong>of</strong> ages 6–18.The development <strong>of</strong> the CBCL <strong>and</strong> itsrevisions reflects the author’s belief thatparent reports are an important part <strong>of</strong> anymulti-informant system <strong>of</strong> child evaluation.In Achenbach’s (1991) own words:Parents (<strong>and</strong> parent surrogates) are typicallyamong the most important sources <strong>of</strong> data aboutchildren’s competencies <strong>and</strong> problems. Theyare usually the most knowledgeable about their


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales157Box 7.1Sample Case Using the BASC-2 PRS <strong>and</strong> TRSJohnny was referred for a psychological evaluationby his mother because <strong>of</strong> his academicdifficulties <strong>and</strong> distractibility. He is a 12-yearoldsixth-grader. Johnny has reportedly hadtrouble completing schoolwork since the firstgrade. Johnny was retained in the second gradebecause <strong>of</strong> poor work completion <strong>and</strong> academicprogress. He was reportedly placed inspecial education in the third grade in a learningproblem class. He was reportedly placedin a regular class again in the fourth grade. Apsychological evaluation conducted at the end<strong>of</strong> the fourth grade resulted in the conclusionthat he was a “slow learner,” according to hismother. This year, he is again having difficultyconcentrating, <strong>and</strong> he rarely completes hisassignments. Academic progress is still unacceptableto his mother <strong>and</strong> teachers.Johnny’s developmental milestones wereslightly delayed. He still reportedly has problemsdrawing <strong>and</strong> using scissors. Last year, hewas diagnosed with juvenile diabetes, accordingto his mother. His family psychiatric historyis significant for depression (mother), <strong>and</strong>Johnny’s father reportedly had difficulty academicallywhen he was in school.Johnny was exceedingly cooperative duringthe evaluation. He addressed the examinerpolitely <strong>and</strong> would occasionally answer questionsby saying “yes sir.” He had considerable difficultycomprehending instructions on an intelligencetest. He was reluctant to admit to not knowingan answer, <strong>and</strong> he worked extremely slowly. Thetest session had to be conducted over two daysbecause <strong>of</strong> his slow response style.He responded impulsively to items on occasion.He also wiggled in his seat <strong>and</strong> frequentlylooked around the room <strong>and</strong> asked questions<strong>of</strong> the examiner. His full scale IQ score in thisevaluation was 85. His achievement test scoreswere similarly in the Low Average range.His BASC-2-PRS (completed by hismother) <strong>and</strong> BASC-2-TRS (completed by hiscurrent teacher) results are highly consistentwith background information.HyperactivityT = 63 (parent report)T = 60 (teacher report)The reports <strong>of</strong> relatively mild levels <strong>of</strong> hyperactivityare consistent with Johnny’s historywhich indicates no history <strong>of</strong> disruptive orimpulsive behaviors.Attention ProblemsT = 76 (parent)T = 77 (teacher)The parent <strong>and</strong> teacher reports <strong>of</strong> significantattention problems (e.g., has trouble concentrating,is easily distracted, daydreams) isconsistent with reports <strong>of</strong> Johnny’s difficultiesdating back to the first grade.SomatizationT = 67 (parent)T = 63 (teacher)Mild to moderate concerns in the area <strong>of</strong> Somatizationappear to be related to Johnny’s history<strong>of</strong> diabetes <strong>and</strong> its attendant difficulties.Learning ProblemsT = 72 (teacher)Johnny’s teacher reported significant learningproblems for Johnny, indicating concerns inall academic areas. It was recommended thathe be further evaluated through a Responseto Intervention (RTI) procedure at his school,<strong>and</strong> services available to him should be plannedaccordingly.AdaptabilityT = 39 (parent)T = 32 (teacher)Functional CommunicationT = 30 (parent)T = 32 (teacher)Johnny’s mother <strong>and</strong> teacher reported concernswith his ability to adjust to changes inplans <strong>and</strong> to adequately communicate his needfor help. These difficulties may interfere withhis academic performance.Johnny’s results were strikingly similarfor both teachers <strong>and</strong> parents. The diagnosis<strong>of</strong> ADHD, Predominantly InattentiveType was made based on Johnny’s history asreported by his mother <strong>and</strong> teacher duringinterviews <strong>and</strong> based on these scores on thePRS <strong>and</strong> TRS.


158 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scaleschild’s behavior across time <strong>and</strong> situations. Furthermore,parent involvement is required in theevaluation <strong>of</strong> most children, <strong>and</strong> parents’ views<strong>of</strong> their children’s behavior are <strong>of</strong>ten crucial indetermining what will be done about the behavior.Parents’ reports should therefore be obtainedin the assessment <strong>of</strong> children’s competencies <strong>and</strong>problems whenever possible (p. 3).Table 7.5 Sample Content <strong>of</strong> CBCL(6–18-Year-Old Version) Syndrome ScalesAnxious/Depressed: Cries a lot, is fearful,feels too guilty, talks <strong>of</strong> suicideWithdrawn/Depressed: Would rather bealone, shy/timid, sadSomatic Complaints: Feels dizzy, constipated,has headaches, nauseaSocial Problems: Dependent, lonely, getsteased, prefers to be with younger kidsThought Problems: Cannot get mind <strong>of</strong>f <strong>of</strong>certain thoughts, sees things, stores things,strange behaviorAttention Problems: Cannot concentrate,daydreams, impulsive, cannott sit stillRule-breaking Behavior: Drinks alcohol,lacks guilt, breaks rules, sets fires, prefers to bewith older kidsAggressive Behavior: Argues a lot, destroysothers’ things, gets in fights, mood changes,attacks peopleFrom Achenbach & Rescorla (2001).ContentThe CBCL includes 100 items for thepreschool version <strong>and</strong> 113 items for theschool age version. Responses are made ona three-point scale (i.e., Not True; Sometimes/SomewhatTrue; Very True/OftenTrue).The CBCL syndrome scales are primarilyempirically derived, with substantialuse <strong>of</strong> factor-analytic methods. TheCBCL scales were also derived separatelyby gender <strong>and</strong> age group (see Achenbach &Rescorla, 2000, 2001). Throughout the testdevelopment process, the CBCL developersalso emphasized the derivation <strong>of</strong> scalesthat were common across raters (e.g., parents<strong>and</strong> teachers). The CBCL parent <strong>and</strong>teacher scales have closely matched items<strong>and</strong> scales that make it easier for cliniciansto make cross-informant comparisons.Sample item content from the CBCL scalesis shown in Table 7.5.The item content for the preschool(ages 1½–5) version <strong>of</strong> the CBCL is notablydifferent from the version for 6–18-yearolds, with somewhat different syndromescales. The syndrome scales for the 1½–5-year-old version are Emotionally Reactive,Anxious/Depressed, Somatic Complaints,Withdrawn, Sleep Problems, AttentionProblems, <strong>and</strong> Aggressive Behavior(Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000).On both versions, there is a Total Problemsscore (the most global score availableon the CBCL) as well as composites forInternalizing Problems <strong>and</strong> ExternalizingProblems (see Achenbach & Rescorla,2000, 2001). The CBCL also includes competencescales (except for the preschoolversion) that are designed to discriminatesignificantly between children referred formental health services <strong>and</strong> non-referredchildren (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001).DSM-Oriented scales were formedbased on experts’ ratings (see Achenbach,Dumenci, & Rescorla, 2001) <strong>of</strong> how wellthe items fit DSM criteria for relevant disordersor groups <strong>of</strong> disorders (e.g., MajorDepression <strong>and</strong> Dysthymia for the AffectiveProblems scale). For the school-ageversion <strong>of</strong> the CBCL, the DSM-Orientedscales are Affective Problems, Anxiety Problems,Somatic Problems, Attention/HyperactivityProblems, Oppositional DefiantProblems, Conduct Problems. The fiveDSM-oriented scales on the preschool version<strong>of</strong> the CBCL are Affective Problems,Anxiety Problems, Pervasive DevelopmentalProblems, Attention Deficit/HyperactivityProblems, <strong>and</strong> Oppositional DefiantProblems. The DSM-Oriented scales are anew feature to the Achenbach system <strong>and</strong>


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales159were designed to more closely align scoresthat were available from these instrumentsto current diagnostic nomenclature.Administration <strong>and</strong> ScoringThe CBCL is easily administered in 15–20min. The CBCL is somewhat unique inthat adaptive behavior is assessed with acombined fill-in-the-blank <strong>and</strong> Likert scaleresponse format. In addition, some <strong>of</strong> theproblem behavior items require the parentto elaborate on or describe the problemendorsed. This format is advantageous inthat it allows the parent to respond in anopen-ended format. Clinicians can gainaccess to qualitative information <strong>of</strong> valueusing this format. Open-endedness, however,also has a disadvantage: It may extendadministration time <strong>and</strong> requires moredecision making on the part <strong>of</strong> the parent.H<strong>and</strong> scoring <strong>and</strong> computer scoringare available for the CBCL. The CBCL<strong>of</strong>fers normalized T-scores as the featuredinterpretive scores. Percentile ranks arealso provided. T-scores are available for allscales <strong>and</strong> three composites: Externalizing,Internalizing, <strong>and</strong> Total. T-scores are nowalso <strong>of</strong>fered for the Competence scales.The advantages <strong>and</strong> disadvantages <strong>of</strong>using normalized versus linear T’s aredebatable (see Kline, 1995). On the oneh<strong>and</strong>, the advantage <strong>of</strong> comparable percentileranks across scales was recognizedby the MMPI-A author team (see Chap.6). Normalized scores, however, clearlychange the shape <strong>of</strong> the many skewed rawscore distributions forcing the T-score distributionto take a shape that it does notactually take in the general population(see Chap. 2). In addition, the reporting <strong>of</strong>T-scores on the CBCL is truncated for theSyndrome <strong>and</strong> DSM-Oriented scales suchthat low scores are reported simply as T ≤50. For the Competence scales, the distributionis truncated above a T-score <strong>of</strong> 65<strong>and</strong> below a T-score <strong>of</strong> 35.NormingThe norming <strong>of</strong> the school age CBCL isbased on a national sample <strong>of</strong> 1,753 childrenaged 6 through 18 years. This samplewas collected in 40 states <strong>and</strong> the District<strong>of</strong> Columbia (Achenbach & Rescorla,2001). Relevant stratification variablessuch as age, gender, ethnicity, region, <strong>and</strong>SES were recorded in an attempt to closelymatch US Census statistics on these variables.The respondents were mothers in72% <strong>of</strong> the cases <strong>and</strong> fathers in 23% <strong>of</strong> thecases (5% <strong>of</strong> the cases used “others”). Sixtypercent <strong>of</strong> the respondents were classifiedas White, with Hispanics appearing to besomewhat underrepresented (9%). Fiftyonepercent <strong>of</strong> cases were from a middleSES background, 33% were from an upperSES background, <strong>and</strong> 16% were from alower SES background. Forty percent <strong>of</strong>respondents were from the southern part<strong>of</strong> the USA (see Achenbach & Rescorla,2001). From this sample, separate normswere developed for ages 6–11 <strong>and</strong> 12–18,with each <strong>of</strong> these groups further delineatedby gender.The norming sample for the preschoolCBCL version for ages 1½–5 was alsorecruited in an attempt to match US Censusstatistics on the same variables. Thissample consisted <strong>of</strong> 700 respondents (76%mothers, 22% fathers, 2% “others”). Fiftysixpercent <strong>of</strong> respondents were White,21% African American, 13% Latino, <strong>and</strong>10% Mixed or Others. In the preschoolnorming sample, 33% <strong>of</strong> respondentswere from an upper SES background, 49%from middle SES, <strong>and</strong> 17% from a lowerSES background. Again, 40% <strong>of</strong> theserespondents were from the southern USA(Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000).<strong>Child</strong>ren were excluded from the sampleif they had “received mental health orspecial education classes during the previous12 months” (Achenbach & Rescorla,2001, p. 76). Separate clinical norms arenot <strong>of</strong>fered for the CBCL.


160 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating ScalesReliabilityThe CBCL has good evidence <strong>of</strong> reliabilitywith internal consistency coefficients rangingfrom 0.78 to 0.97 on the Syndrome scales,0.72 to 0.91 on the DSM-oriented scales, <strong>and</strong>somewhat lower internal consistency on theCompetence scales (i.e., 0.63 to 0.79; Achenbach& Rescorla, 2001). On the preschoolversion <strong>of</strong> the CBCL, the internal consistencycoefficients for the Syndrome scales<strong>and</strong> composites ranged from 0.66 to 0.95. Forthe DSM-Oriented scales, internal consistenciesranged from 0.63 to 0.86 (Achenbach &Rescorla, 2000). The data from a test-reteststudy for a sample <strong>of</strong> 73 (mean interval = 8days) children yielded coefficients rangingfrom 0.80 for the Anxiety DSM-Orientedscale to 0.93 for the DSM-Oriented ConductProblems scale on the 6–18-year-oldversion (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). Forthe preschool CBCL (n = 68), 8-day testretestreliabilities were good, ranging from0.74 for the Attention-Deficit/HyperactivityProblems DSM-Oriented scale to 0.92for the Sleep Problems scale (Achenbach &Rescorla, 2000).A two-year stability study <strong>of</strong> 67 childrenyielded coefficients ranging from 0.45 forSomatic Problems to 0.81 for AggressiveBehavior for the school age CBCL.Twelve-month test-retest coefficients forthe preschool version (n = 80) ranged from0.52 for two <strong>of</strong> the DSM-Oriented scales to0.62 for the Anxious/Depressed SyndromeScale. These coefficients are indicative <strong>of</strong>strong reliability in light <strong>of</strong> the lengthyinterval <strong>and</strong> the expected natural instabilityin some <strong>of</strong> these areas over time. Lastly,mother–father interrater agreement on theCBCL was generally good on the schoolage version with all coefficients except theActivities scale being 0.63 or higher. Theinterrater agreement on the preschoolversion was lower, with coefficients rangingfrom 0.48 to 0.67 (see Achenbach &Rescorla, 2000).ValidityMuch <strong>of</strong> the validity evidence reported bythe authors <strong>of</strong> the CBCL focuses on theability <strong>of</strong> the scale to differentiate clinicalfrom nonclincal samples. Results <strong>of</strong> theseanalyses indicated good differential validityacross scales for both boys <strong>and</strong> girls. Asnoted in Chap. 2, however, evidence <strong>of</strong> differentialvalidity must now also show differentiationbetween clinical samples. Todate, such evidence is lacking for the latestversions <strong>of</strong> the CBCL.The factor structure <strong>of</strong> the CBCLcontinues to raise some conceptual issuesregarding the content validity <strong>of</strong> the scales.For example, it is unusual for depression<strong>and</strong> anxiety items to be included on the samescale. In addition, the Attention Problemsscale includes items that appear more indicative<strong>of</strong> hyperactivity <strong>and</strong> impulsivity thaninattention (see Table 7.5). High scores onthese scales still require a great deal <strong>of</strong> clinicaljudgment as to what characteristics ledto the high scores <strong>and</strong> should be the focus<strong>of</strong> further attention.Validity studies as well as basic <strong>and</strong>applied research investigations using theprevious versions <strong>of</strong> the CBCL are legion.Although the research base <strong>of</strong> the currentCBCL is not as well-established, some evidenceon its validity is promising. For example,the preschool version <strong>of</strong> the CBCL hasbeen found to be useful in screening forAutism Spectrum Disorders based on theWithdrawal <strong>and</strong> Pervasive DevelopmentalProblems scales (Sikora et al., 2008), <strong>and</strong>the CBCL has been touted for its abilityto screen for a variety <strong>of</strong> problem areas <strong>and</strong>its strong convergent <strong>and</strong> divergent validity(Scholte, Van Berckelaer-Onnes, & Van derPloeg, 2008). However, the correspondence<strong>of</strong> the DSM-Oriented Anxiety scale on theCBCL to DSM criteria for anxiety disordershas been called into question (Ferdin<strong>and</strong>,2008). Clearly, more research is needed onthe latest rating scales in the Achenbachsystem, but just as clearly, the CBCL has


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales161enjoyed <strong>and</strong> continues to enjoy a great deal<strong>of</strong> empirical support.InterpretationInterpretation <strong>of</strong> the CBCL is bolstered bymany articles by Achenbach <strong>and</strong> colleaguesdevoted to its clinical use dating to McConaughy<strong>and</strong> Achenbach’s (1989) informativework on this subject. The CBCL useris fortunate to have many interpretiveresources available.More specifically, McConaughy <strong>and</strong>Achenbach (1989) provide an assessmentmethodology for the identification <strong>of</strong>severe emotional disturbance in the schools.Their multi-axial empirically based assessmentmodel proposes five axes for suchassessment situations: (1) parent reports(Achenbach, CBCL), (2) teacher reports(Achenbach Teacher Report Form), (3)cognitive assessment, (4) physical assessment,<strong>and</strong> (5) direct assessment <strong>of</strong> the child(i.e., Achenbach Direct Observation Form<strong>and</strong> Youth Self-Report). McConaughy <strong>and</strong>Achenbach assist the psychologist workingin schools further by linking each CBCLscale to the accepted criteria for severeemotional disturbance. High scores on theAnxious/Depressed scale may, for example,indicate the presence <strong>of</strong> a general pervasivemood <strong>of</strong> unhappiness, which, in turn, mayqualify a child as severely emotionally disturbed<strong>and</strong> document eligibility for specialeducation <strong>and</strong> related services. Of course,these examples fit best with previous versions<strong>of</strong> the CBCL <strong>and</strong> are only as useful astheir degree <strong>of</strong> correspondence with eligibilitycategories used by school systems.For interpreting the CBCL specifically,because there are no established validityscales <strong>and</strong> because there are some scalesthat include heterogeneous content, werecommend more attention to item-levelinterpretation than we have for other measures.That is, the clinician should pay closeattention to scale elevations <strong>and</strong> draw mostconclusions at the scale level; however, itwould behoove practitioners to determineany concerning aspects <strong>of</strong> the parent’s itemresponse style that would render the protocolinvalid. Additionally, interpretationshould not stop at the scale level. Rather,one should inspect the items that led to thescale elevations to determine the best wayto describe the child’s difficulties. Fortunately,the scoring methods available forthe CBCL make linking item responses toscale elevations a straightforward process.Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesThe CBCL has many strengths that continueto make it a popular choice for clinicians.Noteworthy strengths include:1. An ever-growing research base on thecurrent CBCL, as well as a wealth <strong>of</strong>validity research on its predecessors.2. Its popularity among pr<strong>of</strong>essionalswhich facilitates communication aboutits results3. Several writings that provide interpretiveguidance above <strong>and</strong> beyond thatprovided by the manual4. Improved approach to assessing competence<strong>and</strong> available <strong>of</strong> new DSM-Oriented scales that are aligned to DSMcriteria.5. Some response flexibility in that parentsare asked to elaborate on their answersto some items.Weaknesses <strong>of</strong> the CBCL include:1. Lack <strong>of</strong> validity scales which are nowcommon among behavioral ratingscales.2. Lack <strong>of</strong> close correspondence betweenthe empirically derived scales <strong>and</strong> commondiagnostic criteria (e.g., DSM;Hart & Lahey, 1998)3. Heterogeneous content within somescales.


162 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating ScalesThe CBCL continues to be a preferredchoice <strong>of</strong> many child clinicians because <strong>of</strong>its history <strong>of</strong> successful use <strong>and</strong> popularitywith researchers. The continuing development<strong>of</strong> the CBCL database bodes wellfor its future. The most recent versions <strong>of</strong>the CBCL would benefit from researchaimed at assessing the construct validity <strong>of</strong>its scales, particularly the DSM-Orientedscales which were not part <strong>of</strong> the previousCBCL. Such research efforts are necessaryto define further the degree <strong>of</strong> confidencethat a clinician can place on specific scalesfor making differential diagnostic decisions.Teacher Report: Teacher ReportForm (TRF)The Achenbach Teacher Report Form(TRF; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001) isdesigned to be completed by teachers <strong>of</strong>children between the ages <strong>of</strong> 6 <strong>and</strong> 18 forthe school-age version <strong>and</strong> between 1½ <strong>and</strong>5 for the preschool version which is labeledthe “Caregiver-Teacher Report Form”(Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000). The itemcontent <strong>of</strong> the TRF is very closely matchesthe CBCL item content.ContentThe school age version <strong>of</strong> the TRF includesseveral background questions (e.g., “Howlong have you known this pupil?” “Howwell do you know him/her?”), a teacher’srating <strong>of</strong> a child’s academic performance,<strong>and</strong> a four-item screening <strong>of</strong> a child’s adaptivebehavior with scoring on a 1–7 scale(e.g., “How hard is he/she working?” “Howappropriately is he/she behaving?” “Howmuch is he/she learning?” “How happy ishe/she?”). The preschool TRF includes thesame background questions but does notinclude the other items. These backgroundquestions have associated norms <strong>and</strong> fallunder the “Adaptive Functioning” domain<strong>of</strong> the TRF. The major portion <strong>of</strong> the TRFconsists <strong>of</strong> 100 items for the preschoolversion <strong>and</strong> 113 items for the school ageversion. These items describe problematicbehaviors <strong>and</strong> emotions that the teacherrates as being Not True, Somewhat True/Sometimes True, or Very True/Often True<strong>of</strong> the child. The problem behavior itemscover a broad array <strong>of</strong> both internalizing(e.g., anxiety, depression, somatic complaints)<strong>and</strong> externalizing (antisocial behavior,aggression, oppositionality) behaviors.As with the CBCL, the TRF now includesDSM-Oriented scales (6 for the school ageversion; 5 for the preschool version). Theonly scale difference is that the TRF doesnot include a Sleep Problems scale.Administration <strong>and</strong> ScoringThe TRF takes approximately 15–20 minto complete. The instructions to the teacherare printed on the front <strong>of</strong> the answer sheet.Scoring <strong>of</strong> the TRS can be done by h<strong>and</strong>using the TRF Pr<strong>of</strong>ile Sheets with separatepr<strong>of</strong>ile sheets available for boys <strong>and</strong> girls.However, a computer-scoring system isavailable that greatly facilitates scoring byautomatically calculating raw scale scores<strong>and</strong> converting them to norm-referencedscores appropriate for the child’s age <strong>and</strong>gender.Both the Pr<strong>of</strong>ile Sheets <strong>and</strong> the computer-scoringprogram provide raw scores<strong>and</strong> norm-referenced scores for severalscales. As with the CBCL, a Total Problemscore, which is an overall indicator <strong>of</strong>a child’s classroom adjustment, <strong>and</strong> twobroadb<strong>and</strong> scores consisting <strong>of</strong> Internalizing<strong>and</strong> Externalizing behaviors areincluded. These broad dimensions arefurther divided into the eight Syndromescales.The TRF allows for raw scores on allscales to be converted to T-scores <strong>and</strong>percentile ranks based on the st<strong>and</strong>ardizationsample. The T-scores are normalizedst<strong>and</strong>ard scores. That is, the raw scoredistributions are transformed to a normalizeddistribution. This procedure allowsT-scores on all scales to have similar dis-


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales163tributions <strong>and</strong> corresponding percentilesbased on the assumptions <strong>of</strong> a normaldistribution. However, as noted previouslyfor the CBCL, this transformationassumes that the dimensions assessed bythe scale should be normally distributed inthe general population, an assumption thatis questionable because most children tendto cluster in the normal end <strong>of</strong> the distribution.Norm-referenced scores are basedon gender <strong>and</strong> age-specific norms. In addition,as with the CBCL, T-scores on theTRF are truncated, such that the lowestscore provided is ≤ 50.NormingThe norming sample for the school-ageversion <strong>of</strong> the TRF consisted <strong>of</strong> 2,319 children,72% <strong>of</strong> whom were White. Fourteenpercent were identified as African American,<strong>and</strong> 7% were identified as Latino;thus, both ethnic minority groups appearto be underrepresented in this sample. TheTRF normative sample appears to be geographicallyrepresentative. Thirty-eightpercent <strong>of</strong> children in this sample werefrom an upper SES background, 46% froma middle SES background, <strong>and</strong> 16% froma low SES background.For the preschool version, the normingsample consisted <strong>of</strong> 1,192 children. Thissample was geographically diverse, butonly 10% <strong>of</strong> the sample came from a lowerSES background. The sample representedWhites <strong>and</strong> African Americans well (i.e.,48% <strong>and</strong> 36%, respectively), with only 8%<strong>of</strong> the sample identifying as Latino.For both versions <strong>of</strong> the TRF, the normativesample excluded children who hadreceived mental health or special educationservices within the preceding 12 months.Therefore, as with the other rating scales inthe Achenbach system, the sample shouldbe considered a normal comparison group,rather than one that is normative <strong>and</strong> representative<strong>of</strong> the general population.ReliabilityAchenbach <strong>and</strong> Rescorla (2000, 2001) providethree types <strong>of</strong> reliability information onthe TRF. Internal consistency estimates wereprovided for the Syndrome <strong>and</strong> DSM-Orientedscales. Coefficients indicated goodinternal consistency, ranging from 0.72 to0.95. For the preschool version, coefficientswere quite variable, ranging from 0.52 forSomatic Complaints to 0.96 for the AggressiveBehaviors scale. Test-retest reliabilityover an average <strong>of</strong> a 16-day interval is presentedon a sample <strong>of</strong> 44 children in the agerange <strong>of</strong> 6–18. The test-retest coefficientswere generally high (i.e., 0.80s <strong>and</strong> higher),with the exception <strong>of</strong> the Withdrawn/Depressed scale (r = 0.60) <strong>and</strong> the AffectiveProblems scale (r = 0.62). Four-month testretestreliability was variable, ranging from0.31 (Affective Problems) to 0.72 (Hyperactivity-Impulsivity).The 8-day test-retestreliability for the preschool version <strong>of</strong> theTRF was somewhat variable, ranging froma coefficient <strong>of</strong> 0.57 for Anxiety Problems to0.91 for Somatic Complaints (the scale withthe lowest internal consistency). Threemonthtest-retest reliability for the preschoolTRF was variable with coefficientsranging from 0.22 for Somatic Complaintsto 0.71 for Emotionally Reactive.Correlations between ratings from twodifferent teachers are provided for 88 children(Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). Thecorrelations were modest across scales, withthe mean coefficient for the full sample being0.49 for the Competence scales, 0.60 for theSyndrome scales, <strong>and</strong> .58 for the DSM-Oriented scales. Similar analyses for 102preschoolers revealed an overall mean coefficient<strong>of</strong> 0.62, with a range <strong>of</strong> 0.21 (SomaticComplaints) to 0.78 (Aggressive Behavior).ValidityThere is relatively limited validity informationavailable for the current versions<strong>of</strong> the TRF. The validity data reported inthe manuals (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000,


164 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales2001) mainly focus on the ability <strong>of</strong> thescales to differentiate non-referred fromclinical samples within gender. The TRFscales generally show such differential validity.However, the Somatic Complaints scale<strong>of</strong> both versions does not appear to consistentlydifferentiate the two groups (Achenbach& Rescorla, 2000, 2001). As notedabove, validity studies for the BASC-2-TRSdemonstrated good correspondence withthe TRF, especially for externalizing problems.An exception was the lack <strong>of</strong> correlationbetween scales on each form assessingsomatic complaints. Research has supportedthe predictive validity <strong>of</strong> the TRF at agethree in predicting problems, especiallyexternalizing problems, at age five (Kerret al., 2007). The TRF, like the CBCL,was based heavily on its well-researchedpredecessor. There is a wealth <strong>of</strong> researchon the previous versions <strong>of</strong> the TRF whichsupports its validity in (1) differentiatingclinic-referred children from non-referredchildren, (2) correlating with classroomobservations <strong>of</strong> children’s behavior, <strong>and</strong> (3)correlating with independent clinical diagnoses(see Achenbach, 1991; Casat, Norton,& Boyle-Whitesel, 1999; Piacentini, 1993).InterpretationInformation on the reliability <strong>and</strong> validity<strong>of</strong> the adaptive functioning component <strong>of</strong>the TRF is lacking; therefore, interpretations<strong>of</strong> these scales should be done verycautiously, if at all. Subsequently, althoughit may be useful to next consider the TRFTotal Problems score <strong>and</strong> composites,more specific information can be gleanedfrom interpretations <strong>of</strong> the eight syndromescales <strong>and</strong> the six DSM-Orientedscales. The reliability <strong>of</strong> these scales (withthe exception <strong>of</strong> the Somatic Complaintsscale) is good. However, because the initialdevelopment <strong>of</strong> the TRS item poolwas done in an attempt to be atheoretical,the item content <strong>of</strong> the TRS scales tendsto be more heterogeneous than other ratingscales that used a more explicit guidingtheory for scale development. For example,the Attention Problems scale consists <strong>of</strong>items traditionally associated with inattention(e.g., difficulty concentrating) but alsoincludes items associated with immaturity,overactivity, poor school achievement, <strong>and</strong>clumsiness. Therefore, it is imperativethat the items that led to a clinical scaleelevation be reviewed to underst<strong>and</strong> themeaning <strong>of</strong> the elevation. For example, achild may show an elevation on the AttentionProblems scale because <strong>of</strong> problemswith immaturity, clumsiness, or academicproblems, or a child may have an elevationdue to problems <strong>of</strong> inattention <strong>and</strong>/or hyperactivity. However, because <strong>of</strong> theunreliability <strong>of</strong> individual items, this itemlevelanalysis should be conducted onlywhen there is an elevation on the clinicalscale. The Attention-Deficit/HyperactivityProblems DSM-Oriented scale can bequite helpful in this type <strong>of</strong> scenario as itis more closely aligned with characteristicsindicative <strong>of</strong> ADHD.Interpretation <strong>of</strong> the Thought Problemsscale on both the TRF <strong>and</strong> CBCLdeserves several cautionary notes. Thisscale has an especially heterogeneous content,consisting <strong>of</strong> items describing obsessions(e.g., “Cannot get mind <strong>of</strong>f certainthoughts”), compulsions (e.g., “Repeatsacts over <strong>and</strong> over”), fears (e.g., “Fears certainanimals, situations, or places”), <strong>and</strong>psychotic behaviors (e.g., “Hears sounds orvoices that are not there”), many <strong>of</strong> whichare fairly ambiguous (e.g., strange behavior,strange ideas). For this scale, it shouldbe apparent that item level interpretation<strong>and</strong> integration with other informationcollected during the assessment are crucialbefore drawing conclusions.One final note is in order for interpretingTRS scales. Because the norm-referencedscores <strong>of</strong> the TRS are based on anormal sample <strong>and</strong> not a normative sample,it is recommended that a more conservativecut-<strong>of</strong>f score be used than would be


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales165Box 7.2Sample Case Using the CBCL <strong>and</strong> TRFDoug is an 11-year-old, fifth-grade boywho was referred for an assessment because<strong>of</strong> parental <strong>and</strong> teacher concerns about hisschool performance. He is suspected <strong>of</strong> havingsignificant attention problems. Doug also hassignificant trouble in peer <strong>and</strong> other relationships.He <strong>of</strong>ten fights <strong>and</strong> argues with peers,resulting in his <strong>of</strong>ten playing by himself.Doug has a history <strong>of</strong> significant medicaldiffi culties. He is reportedly the product <strong>of</strong>an at-risk pregnancy. Although he reportedlyreached most developmental milestoneswithin normal age limits, he has a history <strong>of</strong>motor delays. In second grade, he was diagnosedwith muscular dystrophy. He does nottolerate many foods well, <strong>and</strong> consequently,his appetite is poor.In the first grade, Doug was diagnosedwith a learning disability in reading. He is ina resource special education program. He isdescribed by his teacher as having significantsocial difficulties. He is reportedly <strong>of</strong>ten disrespectfultoward teachers <strong>and</strong> peers. His gradesdeteriorated significantly toward the end <strong>of</strong> thelast academic year. His teachers consider himto be a capable underachiever with behaviorproblems such as inattention, excessive talking,fighting, arguing, <strong>and</strong> poor work completion.Doug’s performance on intelligence <strong>and</strong>achievement testing indicate that his cognitivefunctioning is consistent with what would beexpected for his age, whereas his writing <strong>and</strong>reading skills were slightly below what wouldbe expected for his age.Doug’s ratings from the CBCL <strong>and</strong> TRFcompleted by his mother <strong>and</strong> teacher reflectthe multitude <strong>of</strong> his difficulties as follows:Comparisons across these two raters, behavioralobservations, <strong>and</strong> background informationindicate that Doug is experiencing difficultiesin a number <strong>of</strong> behavioral, emotional, <strong>and</strong>social domains. In particular, reports by Doug’smother <strong>and</strong> teacher <strong>of</strong> his tendency to daydream,his difficulty sustaining attention, hisimpulsive behavior, <strong>and</strong> his restlessness, as wellas the fact that such behaviors were reportedfor Doug at an early age, suggest that he meetscriteria for a diagnosis <strong>of</strong> Attention-DeficitScaleMother/Teacher(T)Withdrawn/Depressed 70/61Somatic Complaints 91/64Anxious/Depressed 79/55Social Problems 80/75Thought Problems 79/67Attention Problems 78/69Rule-Breaking Behav. 73/76Aggressive Behav. 85/65Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)- CombinedType. Ratings on the CBCL <strong>and</strong> TRF Attention-Deficit/HyperactivityProblems scale alsoindicated significant problems with behaviorsrelated to ADHD. In addition, Doug’sreported argumentativeness, disrespect towardothers, <strong>and</strong> tendency to break rules at home<strong>and</strong> at school indicate that he meets criteriafor Oppositional Defiant Disorder as well.Some concerns regarding depression werealso reported, particularly by Doug’s mother.However, these issues do not warrant anotherdiagnosis, but they do warrant continued monitoring<strong>and</strong> some interventions.Indications <strong>of</strong> thought problems were notcorroborated by other findings. The problemsreported by Doug’s mother on this scale(e.g., trouble sleeping, cannot’t get his mind <strong>of</strong>fcertain things) seemed particularly tied to hisreported problems with depression.CBCL <strong>and</strong> TRF Social Problems scoreswere corroborated by reports <strong>of</strong> his difficultygetting along with others. Doug’s social interactionskills are in need <strong>of</strong> intervention.Although the CBCL <strong>and</strong> TRF clearlyindicate some areas <strong>of</strong> concern <strong>and</strong> in need<strong>of</strong> intervention, this case also highlights theneed, more pressing in a case like this, tocomplement the CBCL <strong>and</strong> TRF with otherassessment strategies. In this case, backgroundinformation was particularly important forcorroborating rating scale information <strong>and</strong>indentifying treatment objectives.


166 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scalesthe case for other rating scales. Any elevations,regardless <strong>of</strong> the degree <strong>of</strong> elevation,should still be considered in conjunctionwith other assessment results (e.g., parentreport, self-report, history, observations,etc.). The sample case that follows gives abrief example <strong>of</strong> an interpretive approachto the CBCL <strong>and</strong> TRF (Box 7.2).Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesThe TRF remains one <strong>of</strong> the most widelyused <strong>of</strong> the teacher-completed behaviorrating scales. In addition to its popularity<strong>and</strong> familiarity with a large number <strong>of</strong>pr<strong>of</strong>essionals, the strengths <strong>of</strong> the TRFinclude:1. The large research literature on theTRF <strong>and</strong> its predecessors which demonstratesgood correspondence betweenthe TRF <strong>and</strong> other indicators <strong>of</strong> childfunctioning, particularly on externalizingbehaviors.2. The inclusion <strong>of</strong> DSM-Oriented scalesaids the clinician in interpreting teacherreports in terms <strong>of</strong> diagnostic categories.3. A larger normative sample than wasavailable for the previous versions <strong>of</strong> theTRF.Some weaknesses <strong>of</strong> the TRF include:1. An underrepresentation <strong>of</strong> Hispanics inthe normative sample.2. The exclusion <strong>of</strong> children with mentalhealth or special education services inthe normative sample, indicating thatsuch children are not represented.3. The questionable reliability <strong>and</strong> validity<strong>of</strong> the Somatic Complaints scale.4. A relatively limited assessment <strong>of</strong> adaptivefunctioning.<strong>Child</strong> Symptom Inventory-4(CSI-4)Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher ReportChecklistsThe <strong>Child</strong> Symptom Inventory-4 (CSI-4;Gadow & Sprafkin, 1998) is a st<strong>and</strong>ardizedrating scale designed to assess the symptoms<strong>of</strong> over a dozen childhood disorders. Thiscontent is unique from other rating scalesin that it is the only omnibus rating scalewhose entire content is explicitly tied to thediagnostic criteria specified in the DSM-IV(American Psychiatric Association, 1994).Therefore, its content reflects the researchthat went into developing these diagnosticcriteria, which is excellent for some disordersbut more suspect for others especiallyfor children (Widiger et al., 1998).The CSI-4 has both parent <strong>and</strong> teacherreport versions that contain analogousscale content, which enhances its usefulnessfor comparing <strong>and</strong> combining ratingsacross informants. The CSI-4 was designedfor use with children <strong>of</strong> ages 5–12, butthere is an analogous <strong>Adolescent</strong> SymptomInventory-4 for youth ages 12–18 (ASI-4;Gadow & Sprafkin, 1998) that has bothparent <strong>and</strong> teacher versions <strong>and</strong> an adolescentself-report checklist, the Youth SymptomInventory-4 (YSI-4). As part <strong>of</strong> thesame system, the Early <strong>Child</strong>hood Inventory-4(ECI-4; Gadow & Sprafkin, 1997)assesses DSM-IV symptoms in preschoolchildren <strong>of</strong> ages 3–5.The content <strong>of</strong> these forms is mostlyidentical; however, they also each includesome domains that may be particularlydevelopmentally relevant. For example, theASI-4 includes assessments <strong>of</strong> Antisocial<strong>Personality</strong> Disorder, Anorexia, <strong>and</strong> Bulimia.The ESI-4 omits items screening forpsychosis but includes items for SelectiveMutism, Reactive Attachment Disorder,sleep problems, <strong>and</strong> elimination problems.


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales167A fairly unique aspect <strong>of</strong> this system is theinclusion <strong>of</strong> a symptom checklist specificallyfor ADHD (ADHD-SC4). This inventoryincludes 50 items that assess the core symptoms<strong>of</strong> inattention <strong>and</strong> hyperactivity as wellas other areas <strong>of</strong> interest related to ADHD.More specifically, the ADHD-SC4 includesa Peer Conflict scale to assess the social difficultiesthat <strong>of</strong>ten accompany ADHD <strong>and</strong> aStimulant Side Effects Checklist as a meansto monitor side effects <strong>of</strong> medication a childmay be taking for the management <strong>of</strong> his/her ADHD symptoms.ContentThe CSI-4, because <strong>of</strong> its explicit link tothe DSMIV system for classifying mentaldisorders (Table 7.7), covers many symptomdomains that are not assessed by otherrating scales (e.g., tic disorders), especiallysymptoms <strong>of</strong> more severe types <strong>of</strong> childhoodpsychopathology (e.g., ObsessivecompulsiveDisorder, PosttraumaticStressDisorder, schizophrenia, autism, Asperger’sDisorder). As a result, the CSI-4 may beespecially useful in the assessment <strong>of</strong> moreseverely disturbed children. The items onthe CSI-4 were designed to approximatesymptoms from the DSM-IV with rephrasingdone to eliminate jargon, to emphasizeobservable behavior, rather than makinginferences about internal processes, <strong>and</strong> toeliminate descriptions <strong>of</strong> frequency (e.g.,“<strong>of</strong>ten” acts without thinking). The CSI-4is fairly long (i.e., 97 items for the parentform, 77 items for the teacher form), but thescales are grouped according to each individualdiagnosis <strong>and</strong>, as a result, the wholescale need not be given. Instead, symptoms<strong>of</strong> certain disorders can be selected basedon the specific purpose <strong>of</strong> the evaluation(e.g., Frick, Bodin, & Barry, 2000).Administration <strong>and</strong> ScoringThe 97 items on the CSI-4 are rated ona 0 (“Never”) to 3 (“Very Often”) scale.Table 7.6 Prevalence <strong>of</strong> DSM-IV Disorders in a Normal Sample using the CSI-4 ScreeningCriteriaParent ChecklistTeacher ChecklistDSM-IV Category Boys Girls Boys GirlsAttention-Deficit Hyper. (n = 134) (n = 129) (n = 662) (n = 661)Inattentive 6.4 2.4 11.2 4.2Hyperactive-Impulsive 4.1 3.2 3.5 0.5Combined 4.1 0.8 4.7 1.2Oppositional Defiant 9.2 7.0 6.3 1.8Conduct 6.8 2.3 3.5 1.1Generalized Anxiety 3.7 2.3 0.8 0.8Separation Anxiety 3.0 3.1Schizophrenia 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0Major Depression 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0Dysthymia 2.2 0.0 0.8 0.6Autism 0.7 0.8 0.4 0.3Asperger’s 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0


168 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating ScalesLike other rating scales, quantitative scalescores corresponding to each diagnosticcategory (e.g., Conduct Disorder) can bedetermined by simply summing the ratingsacross items, <strong>and</strong> this score is calledthe “symptom severity” index. However, a“symptom count” score can be used to moreclosely approximate the DSM-IV method<strong>of</strong> considering symptoms as either presentor absent. Using this method, any itemrated as being present “Often” or “VeryOften” is considered to indicate the presence<strong>of</strong> the symptom, <strong>and</strong> any item rated as“Never” or “Sometimes” is considered toindicate the absence <strong>of</strong> the symptom.NormingThe normative sample <strong>of</strong> the CSI-4 included552 parent ratings (272 boys, 280 girls) <strong>and</strong>1,323 teacher ratings (662 boys <strong>and</strong> 661 girls)in three states (Gadow & Sprafkin, 2002).The children were elementary school-age.<strong>Child</strong>ren receiving special education serviceswere not included, making this sample a normalrather than normative sample.In addition to being somewhat geographicallylimited, there was great overrepresentation<strong>of</strong> Caucasian children, particularlyfor the teacher rating sample, with thatsample being 95% Caucasian, 2.8% AfricanAmerican, <strong>and</strong> 0.7% Hispanic. Because <strong>of</strong>these limitations in the CSI-4 normativesamples, norm-referenced interpretationsshould only be made very cautiously. However,because the CSI-4 was not designedprimarily to be used as a norm-referencedinstrument but instead was designed as ascreener for DSM diagnoses, the more criticalpsychometric consideration is its reliability<strong>and</strong> validity for this purpose.ReliabilityOne study reporting on the reliability <strong>of</strong>the parent CSI-4 found moderate to goodinternal consistency for both symptomseverityscores <strong>and</strong> symptom-count scores(Sprafkin et al., 2002). More specifically,internal consistency coefficients rangedfrom a low <strong>of</strong> 0.45 for the symptom-severityindex for schizophrenia to 0.92 for symptomseverity <strong>of</strong> ADHD-PredominantlyInattentive Type. Four-month test-retestreliability coefficients ranged from 0.35 forMajor Depression to 0.88 for ADHD PredominantlyHyperactive-Impulsive Type,with all but two coefficients being 0.65 orhigher (Sprafkin et al.).Relatively limited information on thereliability <strong>of</strong> the teacher version <strong>of</strong> theCSI-4 is available. For example, the CSI-4manual describes test-retest reliability forthe ADHD <strong>and</strong> ODD categories during amedication trial for children with behavioralproblems. One-week test-retest coefficientsfor these two diagnoses averaged 0.62for ADHD <strong>and</strong> 0.90 for ODD (Gadow &Sprafkin, 1998).ValidityThere are several pieces <strong>of</strong> evidence for thevalidity <strong>of</strong> the CSI-4 as a screener for DSM-IV diagnoses in school-aged children. First,the preva lence <strong>of</strong> the diagnoses, based onthe symptom-count scoring method <strong>of</strong>the CSI-4 in the norm sample, seemed toapproximate those found in communitysamples <strong>of</strong> children using structured diagnosticinterviews (Frick & Silverthorn, 2001).These estimates, computed separately forboys <strong>and</strong> girls, are provided in Table 8.4.Second, when these prevalence estimateswere compared to a clinic-referred sample<strong>of</strong> school-aged children, the prevalence <strong>of</strong>DSM diagnoses was higher in the clinicreferredsample for almost all diagnoses.The exceptions were ADHD Hyperactive/Impulsive Type for both boys (7.5% clinicvs. 4.1% norms) <strong>and</strong> girls (4.7 vs. 3.2%),Asperger’s Disorder for both boys (2.7 vs.0%) <strong>and</strong> girls (1.3 vs. 0%), <strong>and</strong> Schizophreniafor boys (1.1 vs. 0%). The primaryconcern is the finding for the one ADHDsubtype, because the failure to find signifi-


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales169cant differences for the latter two disordersseems largely due to the very low base rate<strong>of</strong> these disorders in both samples.Third, <strong>and</strong> probably most importantly,Gadow <strong>and</strong> Sprafkin (1998) reportedon a clinic-referred sample <strong>of</strong> 101 referrals(between the ages <strong>of</strong> 6 <strong>and</strong> 12 years)to an outpatient child psychiatry service,in which they tested the correspondencebetween CSI-4 diagnostic cut-<strong>of</strong>fs <strong>and</strong>clinical diagnoses made by mental healthpr<strong>of</strong>essionals. In general, the sensitivity<strong>and</strong> specificity rates for the disordersassessed by the CSI-4 generally indicatedquite good correspondence with clinicaldiagnoses. This correspondence was especiallygood when parent <strong>and</strong> teacher ratingswere combined, such that a disorderwas considered present if either the parentor teacher ratings led to a CSI-4 screeningdiagnosis. For this multi-informant composite,the Sensitivity rates ranged from0.87 to 1.00, <strong>and</strong> the specificity rates rangefrom 0.40 to 0.92. For example, a diagnosis<strong>of</strong> Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD)showed a sensitivity rate <strong>of</strong> 0.93 indicatingthat, <strong>of</strong> those in the sample who hada clinical diagnosis <strong>of</strong> GAD, 93% crossedthe screening cut-<strong>of</strong>f for a diagnosis on theCSI-4. The specificity rate <strong>of</strong> 0.71 indicatesthat, <strong>of</strong> those without the diagnosis<strong>of</strong> GAD in the sample, 71% did not crossthe screening cut-<strong>of</strong>f on the CSI-4.Sprafkin <strong>and</strong> colleagues (2002) foundgood convergent validity for the CSIdomains (parent form) based on theirrelations with the CBCL Syndromescales. Of note, virtually all CSI domainswere moderately correlated with theAnxious/Depressed scale <strong>of</strong> the CBCL,which may speak more to the generaldistress nature <strong>of</strong> that CBCL scale thanthe lack <strong>of</strong> discriminative validity <strong>of</strong> theCSI domains. They also concluded thatthe CSI-4 is a good screener <strong>of</strong> a variety<strong>of</strong> child disorders based on the highcorrect classification rates found in theirsample.In a separate study, the teacher form<strong>of</strong> the CSI-4 showed similarly good diagnosticaccuracy with diagnoses made fromstructured interviews <strong>and</strong> moderate relationswith parent ratings (Gadow et al.,2004).It should be noted that the research onthe parent <strong>and</strong> teacher forms <strong>of</strong> the CSI-4far outpaces the research available on theircompanion measures, the ESI-4 <strong>and</strong> ASI-4. However, this issue is <strong>of</strong> less concerngiven the highly similar framework underwhich these measures were developed <strong>and</strong>the true intent <strong>of</strong> these measures (i.e., toscreen for symptoms included in a widelyused diagnostic nosology).InterpretationAlthough the CSI-4 content is designedto correspond to the symptoms <strong>of</strong> DSM-IV disorders, the authors <strong>of</strong> the scale arevery clear in stating that the scale shouldnever be used in isolation to make diagnoses(Gadow & Sprafkin, 1998). Instead,the CSI-4 is a screener that could indicatethe need for a more complete diagnosticassessment. Rather than being a significantlimitation, it highlights some very importantuses <strong>of</strong> the CSI-4. As mentioned inChapter 3, there is a great deal <strong>of</strong> overlap<strong>and</strong> co-occurrence among the variousforms <strong>of</strong> childhood disorders. The CSI-4provides an efficient way <strong>of</strong> screening fora large number <strong>of</strong> potential comorbiditiesthat can allow for a more focused <strong>and</strong>intensive assessment in the specific areas <strong>of</strong>concern indicated by this screening. Also,such a screening, because it is time- <strong>and</strong>cost-efficient, may be quite beneficial fordefining smaller samples at high risk forpsychopathology from larger non-referredsamples (see Frick et al., 2000).Given the fairly substantial limitationsin the normative samples for the CSI-4 <strong>and</strong>its companion measures, norm-referencedinterpretations are not recommended.Instead, the symptom-count method <strong>of</strong>


170 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scalesscoring is recommended to provide thebest approximation <strong>of</strong> DSM-IV disorders.Although the normative data suggest thatthe symptom-severity method <strong>of</strong> scoring issomewhat more reliable, it is not as consistentwith the structure <strong>of</strong> the DSM criteriathat relies on the presence or absence <strong>of</strong>symptoms to make diagnoses. Also, withoutgood normative data, it is difficult to judgewhen symptom severity should be considered“significant,” unless one is simply trying tomake relative comparisons between groups<strong>of</strong> children. In addition, the symptomcountmethod provides a very easy methodfor combining information from multipleinformants, which as the available dataclearly suggest also provides the best correspondenceto clinical diagnoses. Specifically,a symptom can be considered presentif endorsed by any informant (e.g., eitherteacher or parent), <strong>and</strong> the rate <strong>of</strong> symptomatologybased on this multi-informantprocedure can be compared to DSM-IVcriteria (see Piacentini, Cohen, & Cohen,1992).Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesStrengths <strong>of</strong> the CSI-4 system include:1. Its uniqueness in attempting to assesscontent that directly corresponds toDSM-IV classifications <strong>of</strong> childhoodpsychopathology.2. Efficiency in gaining diagnosis-relevantinformation.3. Good correspondence with clinicaldiagnoses, especially when using bothparent <strong>and</strong> teacher informants.Weaknesses <strong>of</strong> note include:1. The lack <strong>of</strong> a large normative base; thus,norm-referenced interpretations shouldnot be made from this rating scale system.2. A relative lack <strong>of</strong> research, particularlyon the ESI-4 <strong>and</strong> ASI-4, as well as theself-report component <strong>of</strong> this system.The CSI-4 <strong>and</strong> its related measures <strong>of</strong>fera potentially useful component to childassessment, particularly when preliminarydiagnoses are needed for reimbursement/insurancepurposes. However, asthe authors note, the CSI-4 (or any otherassessment technique) should not be usedas the sole criterion for making a clinicaldiagnosis. Instead, such decisions must bebased on a combination <strong>of</strong> many sources<strong>of</strong> information.Conners, 3rd Edition(Conners-3)Parent Rating ScaleThe Conners-3 (Conners, 2008a) ParentRating Scale (Conners-3-P) is the mostrecent revision to a widely used behaviorrating scale system. The Conners ParentRating Scale is designed similarly tothe BASC <strong>and</strong> Achenbach systems in thatit includes a number <strong>of</strong> clinically relevantdomains for which normative scores arederived. The parent rating scale is designedfor ages 6 through 18. The Long Formcontains 110 items <strong>and</strong> the Short Formcontains 45 items. There is also a 10-itemGlobal Index form. The Conners-3-Ptakes 10–20 min to complete, dependingon which form is used. The following discussionwill focus on the Long Form.As noted in Table 7.1, we recommendthe Conners Comprehensive BehaviorRating Scales (Conners, 2008b) for anassessment that covers externalizing,internalizing, <strong>and</strong> academic issues. However,as the information below indicates,the Conners-3 is unique in its detailed


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales171evaluation <strong>of</strong> ADHD <strong>and</strong> other externalizingissues.Scale ContentThe Conners-3-P includes five empiricallyderivedscales: Hyperactivity/Impulsivity,Executive Functioning, Learning Problems,Aggression, <strong>and</strong> Peer Relations. AnInattention scale developed theoreticallyis also available, as are five DSM-IV-TRSymptom scales for each <strong>of</strong> the DisruptiveBehavior Disorders (i.e., 3 ADHD subtypes,ODD, <strong>and</strong> CD). The Conners-3-Pincludes screening items for depression<strong>and</strong> anxiety, as well as impairment itemsfor home, school, <strong>and</strong> social relationships.Like the BASC, the Conners-3 includescritical items that may signal the need forfurther follow-up. These critical items areparticularly geared toward severe conductproblem behaviors (e.g., uses a weapon, iscruel to animals). Consistent with its predecessors,the Conners-3 includes a briefADHD Index. This scale is based on itemsthat best differentiate ADHD from nonclinicalsamples. As described in Chap. 6for the Conners-3 SR, the Conners-3-Phas three validity scales: Positive Impression(or “fake good”), Negative Impression(“fake bad”), <strong>and</strong> the Inconsistency Index.These scales are new to the Conners system.Two open-ended questions regardingother concerns <strong>and</strong> particular strengths/skills are also included. Detailed informationon the generation <strong>and</strong> selection <strong>of</strong>items is provided in the Conners-3 manual(Conners, 2008a).Administration <strong>and</strong> ScoringThe Conners-3-P uses a four-choiceresponse format where 0 = not at all true(never, seldom), <strong>and</strong> 4 = very much true(very <strong>of</strong>ten, very frequently). A Spanishtranslation is available. Both h<strong>and</strong> scoring<strong>and</strong> computer scoring are available.Raw scores are transformed to linearT-scores, meaning that each scale maintainsits natural distribution in the conversionto norm-referenced scores. Separatenorms are used for boys <strong>and</strong> girls, as is thecase for the other versions <strong>of</strong> the Conners-3.Norms are also computed by age.NormingThe normative sample <strong>of</strong> 1,200 cases wascollected mainly in the USA, with a smallnumber <strong>of</strong> cases coming from Canada.Recruitment <strong>of</strong> the normative sample wasaimed at reflecting US Census data regardingethnicity/race. Data reported by Conners(2008a) indicate that the normativesample closely reflects the Census statistics.This representativeness is a notableimprovement over previous versions <strong>of</strong> theConners rating scale, in that the previoussamples were predominantly Caucasian.As noted for the Conners-3 SR, the WesternUSA appears to have been somewhatunderrepresented. The majority <strong>of</strong> the parents(63.5%) in the normative sample hadat least some post-secondary education. Aclinical sample <strong>of</strong> 718 participants was alsocollected for validation purposes, with over35% <strong>of</strong> that sample being diagnosed withADHD or one <strong>of</strong> its subtypes.ReliabilityInternal consistency coefficients for thecontent <strong>and</strong> DSM scales <strong>of</strong> the Conners-3-P are all 0.80 <strong>and</strong> higher, <strong>and</strong> many are0.90 <strong>and</strong> higher for the overall sample. ThePeer Relations scale for girls had somewhatlower coefficients (i.e., 0.72 for 6–9-year olds; 0.78 for 10–13-year olds) Twoto four week test-retest coefficients weregood (i.e., all higher than .70). Interraterreliability for the parent form was alsogood, with adjusted rs all 0.74 <strong>and</strong> higherfor the content <strong>and</strong> DSM scales (see Conners,2008a).


172 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating ScalesValidityBecause the content scales were empirically-derived,it is not surprising thatconfirmatory factor analyses supportedthe five-factor model for those scales. Allintercorrelations among content <strong>and</strong> DSMscales were moderate to high in magnitude(i.e., ranging from 0.36 to 0.98). Correlationswith analogous scales from theteacher <strong>and</strong> self-report forms <strong>of</strong> the Conners-3were all moderate (i.e., rs = 0.49to 0.67). Criterion-related validity wasdemonstrated through moderate to highcorrelations between Conners-3-P scales<strong>and</strong> analogous scales from the BASC-2-PRS, CBCL, <strong>and</strong> BRIEF (see Conners,2008a). The associations betweenthe Conners-3 <strong>and</strong> CBCL were particularlyhigh. Differential validity evidencealso indicates that the Conners-3-P wassuccessful in distinguishing both a generalpopulation sample <strong>and</strong> within clinicalsamples. That is, scores on scales such asthose tied to ADHD tended to be elevatedfor clinical groups relative to non-clinicalgroups <strong>and</strong> higher for individuals withADHD relative to others within a clinicalpopulation. The correct classificationrate based on content <strong>and</strong> DSM scale elevationswere also relatively high (i.e., 57to 86%). More validity evidence for theShort Form <strong>and</strong> the Indexes are availablein the manual (Conners, 2008a).<strong>and</strong> (e) integration with other assessmentinformation.Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesSome <strong>of</strong> the strengths <strong>of</strong> the Conners-3-Pare:1. The improved representativeness <strong>of</strong>normative sample2. Availability <strong>of</strong> complementary teacher<strong>and</strong> self-report forms that provide acomprehensive assessment <strong>of</strong> externalizingproblems3. Good initial reliability <strong>and</strong> validity evidence4. Brevity <strong>of</strong> Short <strong>and</strong> Index FormsSome characteristics that may be consideredweaknesses are:1. Limited assessment <strong>of</strong> internalizingproblems <strong>and</strong> adaptive functioning (anissue that is addressed through use <strong>of</strong>the Conners CBRS)2. Uniform negative wording <strong>of</strong> items,which may result in a negative responseset3. A lack <strong>of</strong> available validity research conductedby persons other than the developersInterpretationAs discussed in Chap. 6, Conners (2008a)provides a clear step-by-step approach forinterpreting ratings on the various forms <strong>of</strong>the Conners-3. This approach involves (a)examining the validity scales; (b) evaluatingscale elevations; (c) examining the overallpr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong> scores (i.e., determining the constructsthat seem to be represented acrosselevations); (d) item-level interpretation,including critical items <strong>and</strong> screener items;Teacher Rating ScaleThe Teacher Rating Scale in the Conners-3system is very similar to the parentrating scale. In fact, the teacher ratingsscale <strong>and</strong> parent rating scale includethe same scales. The Long Form <strong>of</strong> theteacher rating scale is slightly longer thanthat <strong>of</strong> the parent rating scale (i.e., 115items), whereas the Short Form is slightlyshorter (i.e., 41 items). Two 10-itemHyperactivity Index forms are also available.The following discussion will focus


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales173primarily on the Long Form <strong>of</strong> the Conners-3-T.As with the self-report <strong>and</strong> parentforms <strong>of</strong> the Conners-3, we discussthe Conners-3-T because <strong>of</strong> its relativelyunique focus on ADHD <strong>and</strong> behavioralproblems. The companion teacher ratingscale from the Conners CBRS (Conners,2008b) provides a more extensive assessment<strong>of</strong> broader domains <strong>of</strong> functioning.Therefore, the selection <strong>of</strong> one set <strong>of</strong> ratingscales versus the other in the Connersfamily should be dictated by the purpose<strong>of</strong> the evaluation.ContentThe Conners-3-T has some item overlapwith the Conners-3 parent rating scale, butthere are also unique items in each form.The same four-point response scale usedfor the self-report <strong>and</strong> parent-report versions<strong>of</strong> the Conners-3 is also used for theteacher-report scale. As noted above, thescales are the same as those for the parentrating scale, including validity scales,impairment items, <strong>and</strong> critical items.Administration <strong>and</strong> ScoringThe Conners-3-T can be completed in10–20 min, or less if the Short Form is used.The Conners-3-T has both h<strong>and</strong>-scoring<strong>and</strong> computer-scoring formats that allowfor easy calculation <strong>of</strong> norm-referencedscores. As with the self-report <strong>and</strong> parentreport forms, only sex-specific T-scorescan be calculated. The scores are LinearT-scores <strong>and</strong> are based on each age group,which allows it to capture potential variabilityin discrete developmental stages.NormingThe norming process for the Conners-3-T was essentially the same as that usedfor the Conners-3-P <strong>and</strong> Conners-3 SR.Specifically, the norming sample for theConners-3-T consists <strong>of</strong> 1,200 teachersfrom throughout the USA, with a fewrespondents from Canada. Recruitment wasaimed at a sample that would reflect U.S.Census data on ethnicity/race. The studentsrated by teachers in the norming sampledo appear to match the Census data onethnicity/race (Conners, 2008a). However,the sample appears to be somewhat skewedtoward middle to high SES – based on parenteducation – as 76.9% <strong>of</strong> the studentsrated by teachers in the norming samplehad parents with at least some post-secondaryeducation. Almost as many cases came fromCanada as came from the western USA inthe Conners-3-T norming sample.ReliabilityInternal consistency coefficients for theteacher report version Conners-3 werequite high. Specifically, the coefficientsfor each <strong>of</strong> the content <strong>and</strong> DSM scaleswere 0.90 or higher, with the exception<strong>of</strong> the Conduct Disorder scale (0.77).Two- to four-week test-retest reliabilitycoefficients were also good, ranging from0.72 to 0.83 (see Conners, 2008a). Lastly,<strong>and</strong> perhaps particularly importantly forteacher ratings, interrater reliability coefficientsfor pairs <strong>of</strong> teacher raters weremoderate to high. The Peer Relations<strong>and</strong> Oppositional Defiant Disorder scaleshad the lowest adjusted coefficients (i.e.,0.52 <strong>and</strong> 0.55, respectively), whereas theHyperactivity/Impulsivity <strong>and</strong> ConductDisorder scales had the highest coefficients(i.e., 0.77). It should be noted thatthe lower coefficients from these analysesmay reflect less-than-ideal rater agreement,or they may reflect real differencesin a child’s behavior from one classroomcontext to another. Additional analyses,particularly in determining whetherteacher agreement might change as afunction <strong>of</strong> the child’s age, are needed.Validity


174 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating ScalesFactor analyses revealed a four-factorsolution for the Conners-3-T: Hyperactivity/Impulsivity,Aggression, Peer Relations,<strong>and</strong> a combined Learning Problems/Executive Functioning scale. Conners(2008a) also found support for consideringthe Learning Problems/ExecutiveFunctioning as consisting <strong>of</strong> two subscalesconsisting <strong>of</strong> items intended to loadon a Learning Problems <strong>and</strong> an ExecutiveFunctioning scale. As noted above,the Conners-3-T scales were moderatelycorrelated with the same scales fromthe parent <strong>and</strong> self-report versions. Thescales on the Conners-3-T were all moderatelyinterrelated. Criterion-relatedvalidity for the Conners-3-T was supportedthrough moderate to high correlationsbetween Conners-3-T scales <strong>and</strong>analogous scales on the BASC-2-TRS,Achenbach TRF, <strong>and</strong> BRIEF TeacherForm. Similar to the parent version <strong>of</strong>the Conners-3, the teacher version demonstratedgood differential validity inthat scales were elevated for individualsfrom a clinical sample relative to a generalsample, <strong>and</strong> scale scores tended todiffer within the clinical sample in intuitiveways. For example, ADHD scalescores tended to be higher for youthsdiagnosed with ADHD than for youthswith other difficulties who did not haveADHD diagnoses (see Conner, 2008a).InterpretationAt the very least, the Conners-3-T appearsto be useful as a screening for problems inclassroom adjustment, particularly in terms<strong>of</strong> learning or externalizing problems, <strong>and</strong>as part <strong>of</strong> a comprehensive assessmentbattery. The recommended approach forinterpreting the Conners-3-T mirrors thatdescribed for the Conners-3 SR <strong>and</strong> Conners-3-P.Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesThe strengths <strong>of</strong> the Conners-3-Tinclude:1. Content that allows for an extensiveassessment <strong>of</strong> ADHD symptoms <strong>and</strong>other behavioral problems.2. Good correspondence across scales onthe parent <strong>and</strong> teacher versions, whichfacilitates comparisons in a multi-informantassessment.3. The presence <strong>of</strong> several short screeningscales which may be more feasiblefor many teachers.Apparent weaknesses <strong>of</strong> the Conners-3-T include:1. Minimal assessment <strong>of</strong> depression <strong>and</strong>anxiety, as well as adaptive functioning.The Conners CBRS should be used ifextensive assessment <strong>of</strong> these domains isdesired.2. Lack <strong>of</strong> research on reliability <strong>and</strong> validityconducted by persons other than thedeve loper.3. The normative sample is not quite asdiverse as that for the parent <strong>and</strong> selfreportforms <strong>of</strong> the Conners-3, yet it isstill diverse in terms <strong>of</strong> race/ethnicity.<strong>Personality</strong> Inventory for<strong>Child</strong>ren-2 (PIC-2); StudentBehavior Survey (SBS)Parent Report PIC-2The <strong>Personality</strong> Inventory for <strong>Child</strong>ren-2[(PIC-2); Lachar & Gruber, 2001] is basedclosely on its predecessor, the PIC-R(Wirt, Lachar, Klinedinst, & Seat, 1990).The original development <strong>of</strong> the PIC followedclosely on the heels <strong>of</strong> the MMPI,with much <strong>of</strong> the early development worktaking place in the 1950s. The PIC-2 is a275-item rating scale designed for use with


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales175parents <strong>of</strong> children between the ages <strong>of</strong> 5<strong>and</strong> 19 years (Lachar & Gruber, 2001).Scale ContentThe PIC-2 scales, although revised, havea long clinical history. The PIC-2 includesscales that were developed via a mixture <strong>of</strong>empirical means with considerable use <strong>of</strong>external validation techniques <strong>and</strong> scalesTable 7.7 PIC-2 <strong>Clinical</strong> Scales, Subscales, <strong>and</strong> Internal Consistency CoefficientsCognitive Impairment (r = 0.87)Inadequate Abilities (r = 0.77)Others think my child is talentedMy child seems to underst<strong>and</strong> everything thatis said.Poor Achievement (r = 0.77)It is hard for my child to make goodgrades.Reading has been a problem for my child.Developmental Delay (r = 0.79)At one time my child had speech difficulties.My child could ride a tricycle by the age<strong>of</strong> 5.Impulsivity <strong>and</strong> Distractibility (r = 0.92)Disruptive Behavior (r = 0.91)My child jumps from one activity toanother.My child cannot keep attention on anything.Fearlessness (r = 0.69)My child will do anything on a dare.Nothing seems to scare my child.Delinquency (r = 0.95)Antisocial Behavior (r = 0.88)My child has been in trouble with thepolice.My child has run away from home.Dyscontrol (r = 0.91)When my child gets mad, watch out!Many times my child has become violent.Noncompliance (r = 0.92)My child <strong>of</strong>ten breaks the rules.My child tends to see how much he/she canget away with.Family Dysfunction (r = 0.87)Conflict among members (r = 0.83)There is a lot <strong>of</strong> tension in our home.Our family argues a lot at dinner time.Parent Maladjustment (r = 0.77)One <strong>of</strong> the child’s parents drinks too muchalcohol.The child’s parents are divorced or livingapart.Reality Distortion (r = 0.89)Developmental Deviation (r = 0.84)My child <strong>of</strong>ten gets confused.My child needs protection from everydaydangers.Hallucinations <strong>and</strong> Delusions (r = 0.81)My child thinks others are plotting againsthim/her.My child is likely to scream if disturbed.Somatic Concern (r = 0.84)Psychosomatic Preoccupation (r = 0.80)My child is worried about disease.My child <strong>of</strong>ten has an upset stomach.Muscular Tension <strong>and</strong> Anxiety (r = 0.68)Recently my child has complained <strong>of</strong> chestpains.My child <strong>of</strong>ten has back pains.Psychological Discomfort (r = 0.90)Fear <strong>and</strong> Worry (r = 0.72)My child will worry a lot before startingsomething new.My child is <strong>of</strong>ten afraid <strong>of</strong> little things.Depression (r = 0.87)My child has little self-confidence.My child hardly ever smiles.(Continues)


176 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating ScalesTable 7.7 (continued)Psychological Discomfort (r = 0.90)Sleep Disturbance/Preoccupation withDeath(r = 0.76)My child’s sleep is calm <strong>and</strong> restful.My child thinks about ways to kill himself/herself.Social Withdrawal (r = 0.81)Social Introversion (r = 0. 78)My child is usually afraid to meet newpeople.Shyness is my child’s biggest problem.Isolation (r = 0.68)My child does not like to be close withothers.My child <strong>of</strong>ten stays in his/her room forhours.Social Skill Deficits (r = 0.91)Limited Peer Status (r = 0.84)My child <strong>of</strong>ten brings friends home.(reversed)My child is very popular with other children.(reversed)Conflict with Peers (r = 0.88)My child seems to get along with everyone.(reversed)Other children make fun <strong>of</strong> my child’sideas.Note: From Lachar & Gruber (2001).developed through rational/ theoreticalapproaches.Many changes <strong>and</strong> improvements havebeen made in the PIC-2 scales. Contentoverlap was either reduced or eliminatedbetween scales, item-total correlation hadto be high, <strong>and</strong> validity scales were added(Lachar & Gruber, 2001). Scale content wasalso better articulated with that <strong>of</strong> the PIYin order to enhance score comparisons. ASpanish translation was developed as well.The PIC-2 also includes a 96-item shortform (the first 96 items <strong>of</strong> the St<strong>and</strong>ardForm) called the “Behavioral Summary.”An overview <strong>of</strong> the PIC-2 clinical scalesis provided in Table 7.7. In addition to thesescales, the PIC-2 provides three validityscales (i.e., Inconsistency, Dissimulation,<strong>and</strong> Defensiveness) <strong>and</strong> critical items.Administration <strong>and</strong> ScoringIt takes a parent about 40 min to completethe 275 true–false statements <strong>of</strong> the PIC-2. All administrations require at least twocomponents, an administration booklet<strong>and</strong> h<strong>and</strong>-scoring or computer-scoringanswer sheets. The h<strong>and</strong>-scoring processinvolves the use <strong>of</strong> four forms with a CriticalItems Summary Sheet as an option.The use <strong>of</strong> either PC or mail-in computerscoring limits the number <strong>of</strong> componentsto only two (administration booklet <strong>and</strong>answer sheet).NormingThe norming sample included 2,306 childrenin the kindergarten through 12thgrades. The normative sample appears torepresent 1998 US Census data – whichwere the data available at the time <strong>of</strong> thePIC-2 norming – well in terms <strong>of</strong> ethnicity,parents’ education level, <strong>and</strong> geographicregion <strong>of</strong> residence.Linear transformations <strong>of</strong> T-scores wereutilized. The range <strong>of</strong> derived scores islimited to T-scores based only on withinsexcomparisons. Therefore, as alluded toin the discussions <strong>of</strong> other tests, one is notable to determine how a child’s behaviorcompares to that <strong>of</strong> children in general.Percentile ranks are also not available.ReliabilityInternal consistency coefficients for thescales are for the most part acceptable <strong>and</strong>are shown in Table 7.7. The results <strong>of</strong> one-


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales177week test-retest studies are also generallysupportive (see Lachar & Gruber, 2001).Interrater reliability between mothers <strong>and</strong>fathers was generally very good, with coefficientsmostly 0.75 <strong>and</strong> higher for nonclinic-referredchildren. One exceptionwas the Somatic Complaints scale <strong>and</strong> itssubscales, with coefficients <strong>of</strong> 0.49 to 0.54(Lachar & Gruber, 2001).ValiditySeveral types <strong>of</strong> validity evidence arereported in the PIC-2 manual includingcriterion-related, differential diagnosis,<strong>and</strong> factorial validity. Factors correspondingto the Externalization, Internalization,Social Adjustment, <strong>and</strong> Total compositescores are described.The relations between PIC-2 scores <strong>and</strong>external indicators <strong>of</strong> adjustment are describedin detail in the manual (see Lachar & Gruber,2001). Some <strong>of</strong> the indicators include teacherSBS <strong>and</strong> child self-report PIY ratings. Unfortunately,such studies, by being limited tothe PIC “family” <strong>of</strong> measures, do not allowclinicians to determine the degree to whichPIC-2 results will differ from CBCL, BASC-2, MMPI-A, or other results. Evidence <strong>of</strong>this nature is important, as clinicians <strong>of</strong>tenuse multiple measures <strong>and</strong> frequently haveto describe their findings in comparison toprevious evaluation results. The extent <strong>of</strong>PIC-2 criterion-related validity evidence tobe found in the manual is sometimes difficultto discern. Considerable reference is made toSBS <strong>and</strong> PIY validity studies.<strong>Child</strong>ren with diagnoses in the clinicalsamples were used to compare PIC-2results for several diagnostic groups usingMANOVAs. Many significant effects werefound. However, sensitivity, specificity, <strong>and</strong>other typical indices <strong>of</strong> diagnostic accuracyare not provided.As is the case with the PIY, independentevidence <strong>of</strong> validity is difficult to obtain atthis time. Several aspects <strong>of</strong> validity remainto be assessed in order to support clinicians’use <strong>of</strong> the scale. First priority for furthervalidation is to assess the criterion-relatedvalidity <strong>of</strong> the PIC-2 with widely used scales,such as the CBCL <strong>and</strong> BASC-2 PRS becausemany clinicians will be faced with having tointerpret PIC-2 results in t<strong>and</strong>em with thesemeasures.InterpretationChapter 3 <strong>of</strong> the PIC-2 manual providesconsiderable guidance to the user. In fact,the sheer amount <strong>of</strong> tabular informationpresented is potentially overwhelming. Thefrequency <strong>of</strong> item endorsements for varioussamples, for example, is presented foreach scale. The value <strong>of</strong> such informationis questionable because it is based on theassumption that an item response is a reliable<strong>and</strong> valid indicator <strong>of</strong> some construct,which is a dubious assumption. Nevertheless,the manual provides numerous useful casestudies <strong>and</strong> correlates <strong>of</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>iles. In addition,the meaning <strong>of</strong> various T-scores forthe individual scales is thoroughly describedin an additional set <strong>of</strong> tables. Clinicians willprobably find these descriptions <strong>of</strong> T-scoreoutcomes to be valuable for deriving scoremeaning.Otherwise, we reiterate our recommendedsequential approach to interpretation(i.e., checking validity scales, criticalitems, scale elevations, subscale elevations,relevant item endorsements, consideringprimary vs. secondary concerns, integrationwith other information).Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesPIC-2 strengths include:1. A thorough manual by Lachar <strong>and</strong> Gruber(2001) that summarizes importantstudies <strong>of</strong> scale development.2. A great variety <strong>of</strong> subscale scores thatmay be <strong>of</strong> value for specialized uses.3. The inclusion <strong>of</strong> valuable interpretiveguidance in the manual.


178 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales4. Norming sample that closely matchescensus data at the time <strong>of</strong> the scale’sdevelopment.Weaknesses <strong>of</strong> the PIC-2 may include:1. Test length.2. A lack <strong>of</strong> criterion-related validity studies<strong>and</strong> shortage <strong>of</strong> validity studies independent<strong>of</strong> the test developers.3. Limited score options (i.e., absence <strong>of</strong>general norm-referenced comparisons<strong>and</strong> percentiles).The PIC-2 represents a significantupgrade <strong>of</strong> the PIC-R. The most importantimprovements are a reduction <strong>of</strong> itemoverlap between scales <strong>and</strong> the collection<strong>of</strong> new norms. Both independent validationresearch <strong>and</strong> clinical experience are necessaryto determine the ultimate utility <strong>of</strong> thescale.Teacher Report: The StudentBehavior SurveyThe Student Behavior Survey (SBS; Lachar,Wingenfeld, Kline, & Gruber, 2000) is theteacher version <strong>of</strong> the rating scale systemthat includes the parent-completed PIC-2youth self-report PIY. As a result, SBSrounds out a rating scale system with a long<strong>and</strong> distinguished history in the assessment<strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents by providing asource <strong>of</strong> information on a child’s classroomadjustment based on teacher report. TheSBS is not as long as its parent-report <strong>and</strong>self-report siblings, containing 102 itemsthat are rated on a four-point Likert scale.This rather moderate length allows mostteachers to complete the form easily in15–20 min. The scale has normative informationfor children <strong>of</strong> ages 5 through 18.ContentDespite being developed to complementthe PIC-2 <strong>and</strong> PIY scales, theSBS was not beholden to the item content<strong>of</strong> the parent-report <strong>and</strong> self-reportscales. Instead, the content <strong>of</strong> the SBSwas developed based on teacherendorsements <strong>of</strong> statements thatseem to reflect important dimensions<strong>of</strong> classroom adjustment. The content<strong>of</strong> the SBS can be divided into threemajor categories. The first category isAcademic Resources, which containsfour subscales: Academic Performance(eight items), Academic Habits (thirteenitems), Social Skills (eight items), <strong>and</strong>Parent Participation (six items). Thesesubscales are adaptive scales focusing onpotential strengths <strong>of</strong> the child in the academicenvironment, <strong>and</strong> therefore, itemson these subscales are worded in a positivedirection. The second category is AdjustmentProblems, which includes sevensubscales: Health Concerns (six items),Emotional Distress (fifteen items), UnusualBehavior (seven items), Social Problems(twelve items), Verbal Aggression (sevenitems), Physical Aggression (five items),<strong>and</strong> Behavior Problems (fifteen items).These two areas include the main clinicalscales <strong>of</strong> the SBS focusing on emotional,social, <strong>and</strong> behavioral areas <strong>of</strong> concern forthe child’s classroom adjustment.The third section is a Disruptive BehaviorDisorders category that includes threesubscales: Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity(16 items), Oppositional-Defiant (16items) <strong>and</strong> Conduct Problems (16 items).As the names <strong>of</strong> the subscales imply, thesescales were developed to provide a screeningfor the major disruptive behavior disordercategories specified in the DSM-IV.However, the individual items were notspecifically developed to tap DSM criteria.Instead, three clinicians chose itemsfrom the existing 102-item pool that werejudged to be most indicative <strong>of</strong> the DSM-IV criteria, a similar approach to thatemployed for the Achenbach measures


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales179(discussed earlier). This procedure led tosome criteria not being assessed (e.g., “Isspiteful <strong>and</strong> vindictive”) <strong>and</strong> other itemsincluded that are not part <strong>of</strong> the DSM criteria(e.g., “Demonstrates polite behavior<strong>and</strong> manners” reverse-scored). This issueis especially relevant to the Conduct Problemsscale, which is fairly divergent fromthe content <strong>of</strong> the DSM-IV definition <strong>of</strong>Conduct Disorder, including such items as“uses drugs or alcohol” <strong>and</strong> “preoccupiedwith sex.”Administration <strong>and</strong> ScoringThe items on the SBS are groupedaccording to their subscales, such thatthe 8 items for the Academic Performancesubscale are items 1 through 8,the 13 items for the Academic Habitssubscale are items 9 through 21, <strong>and</strong> soon. In addition, the subscale titles documentthis explicit grouping to the teacherraters. This is a somewhat unique formatin that other rating scales have items forthe subscale intermixed throughout thescale. There could be both positive <strong>and</strong>negative consequences <strong>of</strong> this format. Forexample, it makes scoring much easier <strong>and</strong>reduces the likelihood <strong>of</strong> clerical errors incomputing raw scores, because it is readilyapparent which items are included oneach subscale. Also, it makes inspection <strong>of</strong>items that led to subscale elevations a verysimple process. Alternatively, it opens thepossibility that teachers may be influencedby the name <strong>of</strong> the construct (e.g., socialskills) <strong>and</strong> rate children according to theiroverall perceptions <strong>of</strong> a child’s adjustmentfor that domain rather than basing theirratings on their perceptions <strong>of</strong> the individualbehaviors. For example, a teacherwho views a child as socially unskilledmay rate items under that heading asmore problematic than if he or she wasnot explicitly informed about the overalldomain being assessed.However, there is no empirical evidencethat this item format affects ratingsin any systematic way, <strong>and</strong> as mentionedpreviously, it greatly simplifies the scoringprocess. There are two “Auto-Score”forms for the SBS: one for children <strong>of</strong>ages 5–11 <strong>and</strong> one for adolescents <strong>of</strong> ages12–18. Raw scores are simply computedby summing the ratings within each <strong>of</strong>the 11 subscales included in the AcademicResources <strong>and</strong> Adjustment Problemdomains. Between the two sides <strong>of</strong> theratings is carbon paper that copies ratingson only those items that correspond to thethree disruptive behavior subscales. Rawscores are based on a sum <strong>of</strong> these items aswell. These 14 raw scores are then transferredto a cover Pr<strong>of</strong>ile page with separatecolumns for boys <strong>and</strong> girls. Thesepr<strong>of</strong>iles reflect a conversion to T-scores<strong>and</strong> show the relative elevations amongsubscales based on this norm-referencedscore. Importantly, the conversions <strong>and</strong>pr<strong>of</strong>iles can only be computed for separatemale <strong>and</strong> female norms, <strong>and</strong> not forboth sexes combined.NormsThe primary normative sample for the SBSincludes 2,612 children from regular educationclassrooms from 22 schools in 11states. The sample was fairly evenly dividedbetween boys <strong>and</strong> girls <strong>and</strong> had substantialrepresentation at each year <strong>of</strong> age from 5 to18. Also, the regional breakdown, parentaleducational level, <strong>and</strong> ethnic composition(e.g., 70% Caucasian, 15% African American,10% Hispanic American) was fairlyrepresentative <strong>of</strong> US Census Bureau statistics(see Lachar et al., 2000). The one relativelyminor exception was the somewhathigh rate <strong>of</strong> college graduates in this normsample (i.e., 35 vs. 26.9% cited for the USCensus).One <strong>of</strong> the unique features <strong>of</strong> the SBSis that, in addition to the regular educationnorm sample on which T-score conversa-


180 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scalestions were based, the manual also reportedon a large referred sample (n = 1,315) thatobtained teacher ratings on children from41 different sites in 17 states in the USA.These children included those in specialeducation classes, those referred to bothinpatient <strong>and</strong> outpatient mental healthclinics, <strong>and</strong> those referred to juvenile justicefacilities. This large sample allows for acomparison <strong>of</strong> the psychometric properties<strong>of</strong> the SBS in both a large normal sample<strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> in a large disturbed sample.Overall, each <strong>of</strong> the SBS scales differentiatedthe referred <strong>and</strong> normal samples withCohen’s d ranging from 0.23 (Parent Participation)to 0.98 (Academic Performance;see Lachar et al., 2000).ReliabilityThe information provided in the manual(Lachar et al., 2000) on the reliability <strong>of</strong> theSBS is exemplary. Internal consistency estimatesfor the 14 subscales across both thenormal <strong>and</strong> referred samples ranged from0.85 to 0.95, indicating uniformly excellentinternal consistency. Test-retestcorrelationsare provided for four samples <strong>of</strong>children ranging in age from 5 to 18 <strong>and</strong>with retest intervals ranging from 2 to 30weeks. Again, all scales showed quite goodtemporal stability, with the test-retest <strong>of</strong>the Unusual Behavior scale over a 20-weekperiod in adolescents being the only indexto be somewhat low (i.e., r = 0.29). A thirdtype <strong>of</strong> reliability, inter-rater agreement,was tested in two samples <strong>of</strong> 30 children,one sample including fourth <strong>and</strong> fifth graderegular education students <strong>and</strong> a secondsample including children (ages 5–12)receiving special education services. Thecorrelations between two teacher ratingsacross these samples ranged from 0.44 to0.91, with most indexes being above 0.70.ValidityThe dimensionality <strong>of</strong> the SBS was tested ina way that was somewhat different from thatreported for other behavioral rating scales.That is, rather than conducting a factoranalysis on the individual items, the itemsubscalecorrelations were compared foreach item’s correlation with the dimensionit is purported to assess <strong>and</strong> its correlationswith other dimensions. While this methodled to rationally derived scales that werefairly homogeneous in content, the decisionas to whether an item is “more strongly”associated with the dimension it is purportedto measure is somewhat subjective inthe absence <strong>of</strong> factor analysis. For example,“Blames others for own problems” is correlated0.79 with the Behavior Problems subscaleon which it is included, but it is alsocorrelated 0.76 with the Verbal Aggressionsubscale, 0.61 with the Physical Aggressionsubscale, <strong>and</strong> 0.54 with the Social Problemssubscale. The most problematic in this regardare the three Disruptive Behavior Scales,on which many items load equally high onall three dimensions, although this is likelydue to the nature <strong>of</strong> the criteria they weredesigned to assess, which tend to be substantiallyoverlapping (Frick et al., 1994).The manual <strong>of</strong> the SBS (Lachar et al.,2000) provides (1) the correlations <strong>of</strong> theSBS subscales with clinician ratings <strong>of</strong>adjustment problems in 129 primarilyclinic-referred children, (2) the correlationsamong SBS scores <strong>and</strong> parent- <strong>and</strong>self-report ratings using the PIC-2 <strong>and</strong>PIY, <strong>and</strong> (3) the correlations between theSBS <strong>and</strong> an early version <strong>of</strong> the ConnersRating Scale for teachers (see also Piseccoet al., 1999; Wingenfeld, Lachar, Gruber,& Kline, 1998).In general, these correlations supportthe convergent validity <strong>of</strong> the SBS scales,but like most rating scales, the divergentvalidity was less clear. That is, the SBS subscaleswere <strong>of</strong>ten correlated with the otherscales designed to measure similar constructs(i.e., convergent validity), but theywere also correlated with other dimensions


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales181<strong>of</strong> maladjustment as well. For example, theEmotional Distress subscale was significantlycorrelated with clinician ratings <strong>of</strong>psychological discomfort (r = 0.55), butthis subscale was also highly correlatedwith the ratings <strong>of</strong> disruptive behavior (r= 0.44). Again, this pattern is common forratings <strong>of</strong> children’s adjustment becausechildren with problems in one area <strong>of</strong>tenhave problems in many other areas <strong>of</strong>adjustment as well, <strong>and</strong> raters may alsodemonstrate response sets in that a childrating negatively in one area is rated similarlyin other areas. One notable weaknessuncovered in these validity analyses wasfor the Unusual Behavior subscale, whichseemed to be more strongly associatedwith measures <strong>of</strong> disruptive behaviors <strong>and</strong>ADHD than with more severe psychopathologyor reality distortion. For example,it was correlated 0.40 with clinician ratings<strong>of</strong> disruptive behavior but 0.25 with clinicianratings <strong>of</strong> serious psychopathology.Similarly, the Unusual Behavior subscalewas correlated at 0.41 with parent ratings<strong>of</strong> impulsivity <strong>and</strong> distractibility on thePIC-2, but at 0.27 with the Reality Distortionsubscale <strong>of</strong> the PIC-2.One additional set <strong>of</strong> validity analysesprovided in the manual were comparisonsbetween groups <strong>of</strong> children either diagnosedwith Disruptive Behavior Disordersby clinicians or children elevated on theHyperactivity Index in an earlier version<strong>of</strong> the Conners Rating Scale compared tocontrol children. As would be expected, theSocial Problem subscale, the three behaviorproblem subscales, <strong>and</strong> the disruptivebehavior disorder subscales all differentiatedchildren with behavior problems fromcontrol children. Also as expected, theacademic resources subscales tended to belower in groups <strong>of</strong> children with behavioralproblems, with the exception <strong>of</strong> the ParentParticipation subscale.InterpretationWithin the tradition <strong>of</strong> the PIC-2, which, inturn, was based on the MMPI tradition, themanual <strong>of</strong> the SBS provides a very detailedstep-by-step interpretative guide (Lacharet al., 2000). First, the manual recommendsexamining items for response adequacy,including ensuring that there are only a fewmissing responses. The one exception notedin the manual is that many teachers abovethe early elementary school grades may havedifficulty completing the Parent Participationscale because they are less likely to conversewith parents on a regular basis (Lacharet al., 2000). Also, it is important to notethat, unlike the PIC-2 <strong>and</strong> PIY, there are novalidity indexes on the SBS designed to helpin detecting potential threats to the quality<strong>of</strong> the teacher ratings. Second, <strong>and</strong> themain focus <strong>of</strong> the interpretative approach inthe manual, is a description <strong>of</strong> the characteristicsthat are <strong>of</strong>ten associated with childrenwho score in a given range on eachsubscale.These interpretive guidelines weredeveloped by correlating the T-scores onthe SBS subscales with descriptors providedby clinicians (n = 379), parents (n= 425), <strong>and</strong> students (n = 218). Descriptorsare provided for T-scores below 40for the academic resources subscales <strong>and</strong>for (1) T-scores between 60 <strong>and</strong> 70, <strong>and</strong>(2) T-scores above 70 for the adjustmentproblems scales. The authors note that thedescriptors for the higher elevations (above70) should be considered more definitivethan those between 60 <strong>and</strong> 70. The authorsclearly note, however, that all interpretations,even those above 70, should be consideredonly as “interpretative hypotheses,”<strong>and</strong> additional information (e.g., fromparent report, child self-report, <strong>and</strong> clinicalobservations) should be used to betterdetermine if these hypothetical descriptorsare appropriate for a given case.Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesThe strengths <strong>of</strong> the SBS include:


182 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales1. Content that includes a number <strong>of</strong> adaptivedimensions <strong>of</strong> classroom adjustment<strong>and</strong> a rather comprehensive assessment<strong>of</strong> conduct problems, including separatesubscales for verbal <strong>and</strong> physical aggression,<strong>and</strong> a general Behavior Problemssubscale.2. Fairly homogeneous subscale content,which greatly enhances the interpretation<strong>of</strong> scale elevations, as does thevery easy-to-use, step-by-step interpretiveguidelines, which provide the mostcommon characteristics for childrenwith specific scale scores.3. A large <strong>and</strong> representative norm sample.4. The evidence for subscale reliabilityusing both community <strong>and</strong> clinicreferredsamples is exemplary.All <strong>of</strong> these characteristics make the SBS avery useful tool for obtaining teacher ratings<strong>of</strong> classroom adjustment.Weaknesses <strong>of</strong> the SBS include:1. Limited research on the validity <strong>of</strong> theSBS scales <strong>and</strong> subscales2. A lack <strong>of</strong> cross-validation in other samples<strong>of</strong> the interpretative descriptorsprovided for children who score in aspecific range on each subscale need tobe cross-validated.3. The heterogeneous content <strong>of</strong> theUnusual Behavior subscale includessome items related to inattention(e.g., “Daydreams”) <strong>and</strong> some vaguebehaviors (e.g., “Behavior is strange<strong>and</strong> peculiar”). Early evidence suggestedthat it is more associated withBox 7.3Sample Case Using the PIC-2 <strong>and</strong> SBSRicky is a 7-year-old boy who was referred foran evaluation by his teacher because <strong>of</strong> concernsabout inattention, overactivity, <strong>and</strong> poorpeer interactions. Ricky was reportedly bornat home without the benefit <strong>of</strong> any medicalattentionMost <strong>of</strong> Ricky’s developmental milestoneswere delayed, especially language. Accordingto his mother, he did not speak his first wordsuntil the age <strong>of</strong> 2 years. He has reportedlydemonstrated some improvements in languagesince beginning speech therapy at age 4.According to his mother, as a toddler, Rickybegan demonstrating significant behavioralproblems including frequent temper tantrums,overactivity, <strong>and</strong> oppositional behavior. Suchbehavioral concerns continued when Rickybegan school. He has a history <strong>of</strong> getting introuble at school due tobecause <strong>of</strong> overactivity<strong>and</strong> defiance toward his teachers.Throughout the evaluation, Ricky demonstrateda short attention span <strong>and</strong> a highlevel <strong>of</strong> motor activity. He did not sit still <strong>and</strong>was easily distracted by other objects in theroom. When he was unable to testing objects<strong>and</strong> toys in the waiting room with him, Rickydisplayed tantrum behavior (e.g., kicking <strong>and</strong>crying).Ricky’s cognitive assessment results indicatedoverall functioning in the Low Averagerange, with his verbal skills being in theBorderline range. Tests <strong>of</strong> his achievementin reading <strong>and</strong> math indicated slightly belowaverage achievement relative to his sameagedpeers.PIC-2 Scale elevations were as follows:Impulsivity/Distractibility T = 80Delinquency T = 82Family Dysfunction T = 75Social Skill Deficits T = 68(Continues)


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales183Box 7.3 (Continued)SBS Scale elevations were as follows:Academic Habits (adaptive) T = 33Social Skills (adaptive) T = 32Emotional Distress T = 71Social Problems T = 67Unusual Behavior T = 63Verbal Aggression T = 80Physical Aggression T = 72Behavior Problems T = 82Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity T = 73Oppositional Defiant T = 73On rating scales, both Ricky’s mother<strong>and</strong> teacher reported significant concernsregarding inattention, overactivity, impulsivity,<strong>and</strong> externalizing behaviors. Thesebehavioral concerns have apparently beenpresent for some time. Furthermore, basedon background information as well as ratingsby Ricky’s mother <strong>and</strong> teacher, Ricky’sbehavioral problems are causing impairmentin his relationships with others <strong>and</strong> his abilityto perform required tasks in the classroom.Therefore, diagnoses <strong>of</strong> Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)-CombinedType <strong>and</strong> Oppositional Defiant Disorder arewarranted. The elevation on the SBS EmotionalDistress scale is consistent with reportsthat Ricky gets upset easily <strong>and</strong> throws tantrumsin the classroom. The slight elevationthe Unusual Behavior scale <strong>of</strong> the SBS completedby Ricky’s teacher appears consistentwith attention problems in that she reportedthat he daydreams <strong>and</strong> seems disoriented.In addition, Ricky’s mother indicated concernsregarding a high level <strong>of</strong> conflict inthe home, particularly between Ricky <strong>and</strong>his parents. This conflict appears to also berelated to Ricky’s behavioral problems, particularlyhis defiance.Recommendations for Ricky includedparental consultation with a mental healthspecialist to address his behavioral problems,consultation with a physician regardinga possible medication trial for hisADHD symptoms, <strong>and</strong> classroom accommodationsto help minimize the impact <strong>of</strong>Ricky’s behavioral concerns <strong>and</strong> inattentionon his academic <strong>and</strong> social functioning. Theresults <strong>of</strong> parent <strong>and</strong> teacher rating scales inthis case highlight the dissimilar structure <strong>of</strong>the PIC-2 <strong>and</strong> SBS. However, similar informationcan still be gleaned from these ratingscales that can aid in case conceptualization<strong>and</strong> recommendations.disruptive behavior dimensions thanwith indexes <strong>of</strong> more severe psychopathology<strong>and</strong> thought disturbances.4. The lack <strong>of</strong> direct correspondencebetween the three disruptive behaviordisorder subscales <strong>and</strong> DSM criteria. Thisis especially true for the Conduct Problemsscale, which appears quite divergentfrom the criteria for Conduct Disorder.In addition, there is no evidence for howwell the specific SBS subscales (e.g.,Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity) correspondto specific DSM-IV diagnoses(e.g., ADHD). As a result, the usefulness<strong>of</strong> SBS as a screener for specific DSM disordershas not been established.In addition to these issues, it is worth notingthat while SBS was developed to bepart <strong>of</strong> the assessment system that includesthe PIC-2 <strong>and</strong> PIY (reviewed previously)the item content <strong>and</strong> scale structure <strong>of</strong> theSBS is substantially different from these


184 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scalesother scales. The result is a tool that is veryrelevant for assessing children’s classroomfunctioning. However, it also makes it moredifficult to integrate information from thedifferent informants. A case example withPIC-2 <strong>and</strong> SBS data follows (Box 7.3).Sample Impairment-Oriented ScalesAs can be determined from the previous review,omnibus rating scales can provide invaluableinformation about a variety <strong>of</strong> domains <strong>of</strong>child functioning. This information, however,tends to describe functioning in terms<strong>of</strong> severity <strong>of</strong> problems <strong>and</strong>/or frequency <strong>of</strong>problems. Rating scales typically stop short <strong>of</strong>providing an indication as to what extent theproblems interfere with the child’s functioning.Information on impairment is <strong>of</strong>ten leftto the clinician to infer based on interview orother information. However, this informationis no less important for case conceptualization<strong>and</strong> treatment planning. In additionto assessing for impairment via structured orunstructured interviews, one may employan inventory to gather such information ina time-efficient manner <strong>and</strong> then follow-upaccordingly. A brief discussion <strong>of</strong> some suchinventories follows.Home Situations Questionnaire(HSQ) <strong>and</strong> School SituationsQuestionnaire (SSQ)The content <strong>of</strong> the Home Situations Questionnaire(HSQ; Barkley & Edelbrock,1987) <strong>and</strong> the School Situations Questionnaire(SSQ; Barkley & Edelbrock, 1987) ismarkedly different from the other ratingscales reviewed in this chapter. Rather thanhaving items that describe different types<strong>of</strong> child behaviors, these measures includesituations (e.g., while playing alone, whenvisitors are in the home, during individualdesk work, at recess, on the bus) in whicha child may have problems. That is, theHSQ <strong>and</strong> SSQ were not designed to assessspecific behaviors but to assess specificsituations in which problem behaviors canoccur. Therefore, these measures providean indication <strong>of</strong> the specific situations inwhich the child may demonstrate particulardifficulty or impairment.Both measures were designed to be completedin the same manner. The respondent(parent or teacher) rates whether ornot the child has any problem in a givensituation <strong>and</strong> then rates the severity <strong>of</strong> theproblem on a 1–9 scale. These measuresmay be used with a variety <strong>of</strong> clinical problems,as the respondent can be directed torespond as to whether or not the child “hasproblems” in the situations provided.The psychometric development <strong>of</strong> bothmeasures is limited. Normative informationis available from Altepeter <strong>and</strong> Breen (1989)as well as Barkley <strong>and</strong> Edelbrock (1987).However, norm-based comparisons maynot represent the best use <strong>of</strong> these tools.Factor analyses have revealed four factorsfor the HSQ (i.e., Non-Family Transactions,Custodial Transactions, Task-PerformanceTransactions, <strong>and</strong> Isolate Play) <strong>and</strong>three factors for the SSQ (i.e., UnsupervisedSettings, Task Performance, <strong>and</strong> SpecialEvents; Altepeter & Breen, 1989).The HSQ has demonstrated good testretestreliability <strong>and</strong> internal consistency(Altepeter & Breen, 1989). The number <strong>of</strong>problems <strong>and</strong> mean severity rating <strong>of</strong> theHSQ have been found to be related to ratings<strong>of</strong> impulsivity <strong>and</strong> hyperactivity (Altepeter& Breen, 1989). Test-retest reliability<strong>of</strong> the SSQ in a sample <strong>of</strong> 119 regular educationchildren was estimated at 0.68 for thenumber <strong>of</strong> problem situations <strong>and</strong> 0.78 forthe mean severity score (Barkley & Edelbrock,1987). Also, inter-rater agreement forthe SSQ was tested in a sample <strong>of</strong> 46 studentsages 8–17. The correlation between teacherswas 0.68 for the number <strong>of</strong> problem situations<strong>and</strong> 0.72 for the mean severity score(Danforth & DuPaul, 1996). Barkley <strong>and</strong>


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales185Edelbrock (1987) reported numerous significantcorrelations between the SSQ <strong>and</strong> ratingscale measures <strong>of</strong> externalizing behaviorproblems <strong>and</strong> evidence that the SSQ differentiateschildren with ADHD from childrenwithout ADHD. However, for both theHSQ <strong>and</strong> SSQ criterion-related validity evidenceis more difficult to operationalize, asthese measures have a different focus thanratings <strong>of</strong> symptoms or problems. Still, situationsin which the child has difficulties, asindicated on the HSQ <strong>and</strong> SSQ, can assistthe clinician in appropriately designing <strong>and</strong>prioritizing intervention strategies.<strong>Child</strong> Global <strong>Assessment</strong> Scale(CGAS)Another example <strong>of</strong> an assessment <strong>of</strong> impairmenttakes a different approach. The <strong>Child</strong>Global <strong>Assessment</strong> Scale (CGAS; Shafferet al., 1983) is an adaptation <strong>of</strong> an adult scaledesigned to assess overall level <strong>of</strong> impairmentat home, in school, or with friends. The scaleextends from a low <strong>of</strong> 1 (extremely impaired)to a high <strong>of</strong> 100 (no impairment). A parent,teacher, or interviewer is asked to rate thechild on this scale where deciles are accompaniedby a descriptor (e.g., 51–60, “somenoticeable problems”). Previous studies havedemonstrated some evidence <strong>of</strong> reliability<strong>and</strong> validity. A cut score is commonly usedin studies <strong>of</strong> child psychopathology (e.g.,CGAS 70 or below identifies a clinical case).The CGAS was used as one <strong>of</strong> the criteriafor validating the DSM-IV criteriafor the diagnosis <strong>of</strong> ADHD (Lahey et al.,1994). Lahey <strong>and</strong> colleagues used a CGASscore <strong>of</strong> 60 or less as an indication <strong>of</strong> significantimpairment associated with symptoms<strong>of</strong> ADHD. A noteworthy finding <strong>of</strong>this study was the differential results forthe parent <strong>and</strong> teacher CGAS scores. Theparent CGAS scores were significantlyrelated to symptoms <strong>of</strong> hyperactivity/impulsivity but not to inattention. TeacherCGAS scores were not significantly relatedto hyperactivity/impulsivity problems.These same teacher scores were, however,related to ratings <strong>of</strong> academic problems.The Lahey et al. investigation then usedthe relation between teacher <strong>and</strong> parentCGAS scores <strong>and</strong> inattention symptoms toshape the DSM-IV criteria for inattentionproblems associated with ADHD.The psychometric properties <strong>of</strong> theCGAS have been well-studied (see reviewby Schorre & V<strong>and</strong>vik, 2004). Of course,the accuracy <strong>of</strong> CGAS ratings (as is thecase for all ratings) depends heavily on therater’s knowledge <strong>of</strong> the child’s functioningin a variety <strong>of</strong> spheres (Weissman, Warner,& Fendrich, 1990). Can parents, for example,validly rate school <strong>and</strong> peer functioningas is required by the CGAS? Schorre<strong>and</strong> V<strong>and</strong>vik (2004) call for increased consistencyin how clinicians assess <strong>and</strong> thenrate impairment. Certainly consistency inconceptualizing constructs such as attentionproblems or depression aid in communication<strong>and</strong> treatment planning. Suchcould also be the case for assessing impairmentcaused by these problems.An additional consideration is whetherthe best approaches to assessing impairmentare already embedded in rating scalessuch as those reviewed in this chapter. Forinstance, a study by Bird et al. (1990) founda strong association between CGAS scores<strong>and</strong> the Total T-score <strong>of</strong> the CBCL. Themost impaired group had a mean Total T<strong>of</strong> 70, the next most impaired group hada mean <strong>of</strong> 67, the next group produced amean <strong>of</strong> 59, <strong>and</strong> the no-diagnosis groupmean was 53 (Bird et al.). <strong>Clinical</strong> elevationson st<strong>and</strong>ard rating scales may, then, providean indicator <strong>of</strong> impairment. However, Mash<strong>and</strong> Hunsley (2005) concluded that “<strong>Assessment</strong>s<strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents need t<strong>of</strong>ocus not only on specific disorders <strong>and</strong>problems but also on specific impairmentsthat may occur in the absence <strong>of</strong> a diagnosabledisorder” (p. 368). Therefore, it isquite likely <strong>and</strong> important that measures <strong>of</strong>impairment will see increasing use in clinicalassessment practice (Bird, 1999).


186 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating ScalesBox 7.4Assessing Change<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> change is important in clinicalpractice, particularly in light <strong>of</strong> increasingcalls for accountability for the delivery <strong>of</strong>health care. Throughout this text, our focusis primarily on assessment as a mechanism toprovide an answer to an initial referral question<strong>and</strong> treatment recommendations. Quiteobviously, however, assessment is an ongoingprocess throughout treatment as well. If for noother reason, assessment <strong>of</strong> change should beroutine because research has shown that suchevaluations lead to better fidelity to evidencebasedtreatments, <strong>and</strong> ultimately, to bettertreatment outcomes (Lambert et al., 2003).Some considerations <strong>of</strong> assessment tools asuseful for answering referral questions may notapply to assessments <strong>of</strong> change. Demonstration<strong>of</strong> utility for intervention planning <strong>and</strong> assessmentis a tricky endeavor since psychometricevidence <strong>of</strong> validity is not clearly applicable toquestions <strong>of</strong> treatment progress. A measuredesigned for evaluating change, for example, maynot need norms. Consequently, the quality <strong>of</strong> thenorming sample is not relevant. Moreover, a ratingscale that assesses a child’s tendency towardproblems in various areas may not be sensitiveto changes over a short period <strong>of</strong> time. Furthermore,it may not be necessary to interpret totalscores if one is interested only in change in theindividual behaviors. Perhaps ironically, goodtest-retest reliability may be undesirable in theassessment <strong>of</strong> session-to-session change. Therefore,ratings scales <strong>of</strong> the nature discussed in thischapter may not be suitable for assessing change– at least not in the short-term.Clinicians may wish to assess changethrough instruments that they develop for usewith a specific client with a specific treatmentplan. Such strategies, if true to the treatmenttargets, would have strong validity <strong>and</strong> utility.However, to the extent that st<strong>and</strong>ardized measurescan be used, a larger evidence base onmeaningful ways to assess change will develop.The Eyberg <strong>Child</strong> Behavior Inventory (ECBI;Eyberg & Ross, 1978) is an instrument that hasa long history <strong>of</strong> use for evaluating treatmentresults. It should be noted, however, that itsusefulness for assessing particular constructsis not well supported.The ECBI is a 36-item parent rating scaledesigned to assess behavior problems for children<strong>of</strong> age 2 through 17 years (Eyberg &Ross, 1978). Each item is rated in two ways:(1) a Likert-type scale that is used for markingfrequency, <strong>and</strong> (2) a dichotomous scale thatthe parent uses to identify if the issue is in facta problem.The ECBI has been used as a measure <strong>of</strong>conduct-problem behaviors (Burns & Patterson,1990), <strong>and</strong> it does possess some advantages,including the fact that it provides an indication<strong>of</strong> both frequency <strong>and</strong> severity for individualbehavior problems, which is not common forparent rating scales. This characteristic maymake the scale particularly useful for planning<strong>and</strong> evaluating treatment. The ECBI alsoproduces total scores for both the frequency(Intensity) <strong>and</strong> severity (Problem) measures.The available research suggests that theECBI is an example <strong>of</strong> an instrument that maybe useful for identifying treatment objectivesfor children referred for disruptive behaviorproblems <strong>and</strong> for evaluating response to treatment.Certainly, it was ahead <strong>of</strong> its time in itsamenability to evaluations <strong>of</strong> change. Whenthe ECBI is used for other purposes, such asnorm-referenced assessment <strong>of</strong> constructs,research to date does not reveal significantevidence <strong>of</strong> construct validity (using the termas outlined by Anastasi, 1988). The ECBI isan excellent example <strong>of</strong> a scale that has somevalue only in the h<strong>and</strong>s <strong>of</strong> the well-informedclinician who applies the scale only in circumstanceswhere it possesses empirical strengths.We recognize that the preponderance <strong>of</strong>our attention is devoted to other forms <strong>of</strong>assessment rather than assessment <strong>of</strong> change.Nevertheless, an increased research base onindividualized as well as large scale approachesto evaluating treatments is imperative. Wehave every reason to believe that discussions<strong>of</strong> evidence-based assessment will also includeassessments <strong>of</strong> treatment progress (e.g., Mash& Hunsley, 2005).


chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales187ConclusionsParent <strong>and</strong> teacher rating scales are nowcommon methods for assessing child problems.The quality <strong>of</strong> parent <strong>and</strong> teacherrating scales has improved considerably inrecent years. Routinely, scales have nationalnormative samples <strong>and</strong> provide expansiveinformation about their reliability <strong>and</strong>validity. In essence, rating scales providea time-efficient <strong>and</strong> reliable method forobtaining assessment information fromparents <strong>and</strong> teachers.We focused primarily on global scalesthat assess multiple domains <strong>of</strong> functioningbecause the nature <strong>of</strong> childhoodproblems is such that dysfunction in onedomain is <strong>of</strong>ten associated with problemsin other areas <strong>of</strong> functioning. Our review<strong>of</strong> rating scales was not intended to beexhaustive but was designed instead t<strong>of</strong>ocus on some <strong>of</strong> the most commonly usedscales <strong>and</strong> to illustrate what we feel aresome crucial areas to consider in evaluatingscales for use in a clinical assessment.Also, our overview was not intended toreplace a careful reading <strong>of</strong> the technicalmanuals <strong>of</strong> these scales but to highlightsome <strong>of</strong> the important features <strong>of</strong> thescales that might influence their use inclinical assessments.Furthermore, the ECBI (Box 7.4) is anexample <strong>of</strong> a parent rating scale that couldbe used to evaluate change. The OutcomesQuestionnaire-45 (OQ-45) is a questionnairethat has been used as a means to providetherapists with feedback from adultclients as <strong>of</strong>ten as after every session (Okiishiet al., 2006). The suitability <strong>and</strong> feasibility<strong>of</strong> such an approach with parents/child clients <strong>and</strong> in many clinical settingsis uncertain. Therefore, it is likely the casethat the clinician is routinely left to evaluatechange, whether formally or informally.This strategy has the advantage <strong>of</strong> beingexecuted by someone trained to define <strong>and</strong>detect the problems <strong>of</strong> focus. It has thedisadvantage <strong>of</strong> being utilized by the veryperson or persons trying to implement <strong>and</strong>demonstrate the effectiveness <strong>of</strong> their therapeuticstrategies. Research has increasinglyaddressed the implications <strong>of</strong> this approach(e.g., Lambert et al., 2003), but relativelylittle is known. Far less is known about theteacher assessment <strong>of</strong> changes in behavioral<strong>and</strong> emotional functioning during <strong>and</strong> followinginterventions. An exception wouldbe single-case designs tracking behavioralchanges resulting from classroom interventions;many times, these interventions areevaluated by school psychologists or othermental health pr<strong>of</strong>essionals.Chapter Summary1. Concerns about child self-reports <strong>and</strong>practicality have made parent <strong>and</strong>teacher rating scales commonplacein modern child assessment practice.These tools tend to be a very efficientmeans <strong>of</strong> gathering clinically relevantinformation.2. Research has indicated that the constructbeing evaluated <strong>and</strong> the child’sdevelopmental level influence ratingsprovided by parents <strong>and</strong> teachers <strong>and</strong>even the usefulness <strong>of</strong> such ratings.3. The Behavior <strong>Assessment</strong> System for<strong>Child</strong>ren (BASC-2) Parent RatingScales (PRS) <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales(TRS) have three forms <strong>of</strong> similar itemsthat span the preschool (2–5), child(6–11), <strong>and</strong> adolescent (12–21) ageranges. The PRS takes a broad sampling<strong>of</strong> a child’s behavior in home <strong>and</strong>community settings, whereas the TRSdoes the same for the school setting.4. The PRS <strong>and</strong> TRS were developedusing both rational/theoretical <strong>and</strong>empirical means in combination toconstruct the individual scales.


188 chapter 7 Parent <strong>and</strong> Teacher Rating Scales5. The BASC-2 measures include a relativelycomprehensive assessment <strong>of</strong> adaptivefunctioning.6. The Achenbach CBCL <strong>and</strong> TRF <strong>and</strong>their predecessors have long been consideredone <strong>of</strong> the premier rating scalemeasures <strong>of</strong> child psychopathology.7. The CBCL <strong>and</strong> TRF continue to bea preferred choice <strong>of</strong> many child cliniciansbecause <strong>of</strong> its history <strong>of</strong> successfuluse <strong>and</strong> popularity with researchers.8. The CBCL <strong>and</strong> TRF now includeDSM-Oriented scales that are moreclosely aligned to DSM criteria thanthe Syndrome scales <strong>of</strong> both measures.9. The CSI-4 is unique in its contentbeing explicitly tied to the diagnosticcriteria in DSM-IV. Thus, it provides ascreening <strong>of</strong> severe forms <strong>of</strong> childhood


C h a p t e r 8Behavioral ObservationsChapter Questionsl Why have direct observations <strong>of</strong>ten beenconsidered the st<strong>and</strong>ard by which otherassessment techniques are judged?l What are some <strong>of</strong> the characteristics <strong>of</strong>behavioral observations that limit theirusefulness in many clinical situations?l What are the basic components <strong>of</strong> observationalsystems?l What are some examples <strong>of</strong> observationalsystems that might be used as part<strong>of</strong> a clinical assessment <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong>adolescents?Direct observation <strong>of</strong> a child’s or adolescent’sovert behavior has held a reveredstatus in the clinical assessment <strong>of</strong> youth.Frequently, the validity <strong>of</strong> other methods<strong>of</strong> assessment is judged by their correspondencewith direct observations <strong>of</strong> behavior.In fact, behavioral observation is <strong>of</strong>tenviewed as synonymous with the practice <strong>of</strong>behavioral assessment (Shapiro & Skinner,1990). There are two primary reasons forthis importance provided to direct observations.First, as the term direct implies,observations <strong>of</strong> behavior are not filteredthrough the perceptions <strong>of</strong> some informant.Instead, the behaviors <strong>of</strong> the childare observed directly. As we have discussedin the chapters on behavior rating scales,information provided by others in thechild’s environment or by the child himselfor herself can be influenced by a host<strong>of</strong> variables <strong>and</strong> biases. This increases thecomplexity <strong>of</strong> interpreting these types <strong>of</strong>assessment by requiring assessors to accountfor these influences in their interpretations.Therefore, direct observations <strong>of</strong> behavioreliminate a great deal <strong>of</strong> the complexityin the interpretive process. Second, directobservations <strong>of</strong> behaviors frequently allow189P.J. Frick et al., <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Behavior,DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-0641-0_8, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010


190 chapter 8 behavioral observationsfor the assessment <strong>of</strong> environmental contingenciesthat are operating to produce,maintain, or exacerbate a child’s behavior.For example, direct observations can assesshow others respond to a child’s behavior, orthey can detect environmental stimuli thatseem to elicit certain behaviors. By placingthe behavior in a contextual framework,behavioral observations <strong>of</strong>ten lead to veryeffective environmental interventions.To illustrate this potential <strong>of</strong> behavioralobservations, Carroll, Houghton, Taylor,West, <strong>and</strong> List-Kerz (2006) conducted a study<strong>of</strong> 58 students (ages 8 to 11) in which twostudents, one with Attention Deficit HyperactivityDisorder (ADHD) <strong>and</strong> one withno disorder, were observed for 40 min. Aswould be expected, the children with ADHDshowed more <strong>of</strong>f-task behavior. However, theobservational system documented “triggers”to this <strong>of</strong>f-task behavior. Nearly, half <strong>of</strong> the<strong>of</strong>f-task behaviors <strong>of</strong> students with ADHDcould be attributed to environmental distractions,<strong>and</strong> over a quarter were precededby specific teacher behaviors.While these characteristics <strong>of</strong> behavioralobservations make their use an importantcomponent <strong>of</strong> many clinical assessments,we feel that the importance <strong>of</strong> directobservation is sometimes overstated. Likeany assessment technique, direct observationshave several limitations. One <strong>of</strong> theirmajor limitations is that direct observationsare <strong>of</strong>ten expensive <strong>and</strong> time consuming, ifone is to obtain high-quality information.Because <strong>of</strong> their cost, many assessors simplyeliminate this source <strong>of</strong> information fromtheir assessment battery. Alternatively,assessors may attempt less rigorous observationsthan are appropriate. For example,an assessor may observe a child interactingon a playground for a 20-min period<strong>and</strong> record the child’s behavior in a narrativeform, without clearly specifying whatbehaviors will be observed or how they willbe recorded. These informal observationsare dangerous if the assessor is unaware <strong>of</strong>the severe limitations <strong>and</strong> potential biasesin the data that are collected <strong>and</strong>, instead,interprets the data as if they were objective(see Harris & Lahey, 1982a).Another result <strong>of</strong> the costliness <strong>of</strong> directobservations is that the development <strong>of</strong>many obser vational systems has ignoredbasic psychometric considerations (Hartmann,Roper, & Bradford, 1979). In theprevious chapters on rating scales, wefocused a great deal <strong>of</strong> attention on the psychometricproperties <strong>of</strong> scales such as thedifferent types <strong>of</strong> reliability that have beenestablished, the information on the validity<strong>of</strong> the scales, <strong>and</strong> the normative base withwhich to compare scores. Because <strong>of</strong> costfactors, few observational systems haveestablished their reliability or validity inmultiple samples. An even more widespreadproblem for observational systems isthe lack <strong>of</strong> a representative normative samplethat would allow for a comparison <strong>of</strong> achild’s scores with those from the generalpopulation. As we have discussed in earlierchapters, having norm-referenced scores iscrucial in the clinical assessments <strong>of</strong> children<strong>and</strong> adolescents, given the rapid developmentalchanges they are experiencing.Even if one were to use an observationalsystem in the most sophisticated manner,direct observations are still limited by (1)the reactivity <strong>of</strong> the observational setting,(2) difficulties in obtaining an adequatesample <strong>of</strong> behaviors, <strong>and</strong> (3) an inabilityto detect internal events such as cognitions<strong>and</strong> emotions. Reactivity refers to awell-documented phenomenon that a personwill change his or her behavior whenit is being observed (Kazdin, 1981; Mash& Terdal, 1988). As a result, the sample<strong>of</strong> behavior may not be as objective as onewould hope. There is a significant amount<strong>of</strong> research on factors that influence thedegree <strong>of</strong> reactivity that results from directobservations (Harris & Lahey, 1982b;Kazdin, 1981). For example, the age <strong>of</strong> thechild can affect the degree <strong>of</strong> reactivity,with preschool children showing less reactivityto observation than older children


chapter 8 behavioral observations191(Keller, 1986). Also, steps can be taken toreduce reactivity during observation suchas allowing the child time to get used to(habituate to) the observational setting<strong>and</strong> reducing the conspicuousness <strong>of</strong> theobservational system (Keller, 1986). But,even under optimum conditions, reactivityis still likely to affect the results <strong>of</strong> theassessment to some degree.Another liability <strong>of</strong> direct observations isthe difficulty <strong>of</strong> obtaining an adequate sample<strong>of</strong> behaviors. There are several facetsto this issue. The first issue involves ensuringthat the sample <strong>of</strong> behavior is obtainedunder the most ecologically valid conditions;that is, under conditions that will generalizeto other times <strong>and</strong> situations. Although theissue <strong>of</strong> ecological validity is most importantfor observational systems that use contrived(analog) conditions (e.g., observing the childin a clinic playroom), it is also important inselecting the natural setting most appropriatefor conducting the observation. The secondissue is that, even if one selects the bestsetting, one must ensure that a large enoughtime frame is used, so that behaviors will berepresentative <strong>and</strong> generalizable to othertimes <strong>and</strong> settings. In the previous example<strong>of</strong> a child being observed in a playgroundsetting for a 20-min period, it cannot bedetermined how typical a child’s behaviorwas during this observational period. Heor she may have had an especially goodor especially problematic day on the playground.A third issue, which encompassesboth the selection <strong>of</strong> settings <strong>and</strong> adequacy<strong>of</strong> the observational period, is the difficultyin assessing many behaviors that are veryinfrequent (e.g., cruelty to animals, hallucinations,panic attacks) or by nature covert(e.g., stealing, lying). In most cases, onewould not ethically want to contrive a situationthat would prompt such behaviors, <strong>and</strong>the behaviors are <strong>of</strong>ten too infrequent to beobserved naturally occurring in the child’senvironment.A final issue in the use <strong>of</strong> behaviorobservations is the fact that observationsare limited to the assessment <strong>of</strong> overtbehaviors. They do not provide a meansfor assessing the cognitive, affective, <strong>and</strong>motivational components <strong>of</strong> a child’s functioning(Mash & Terdal, 1988). This doesnot negate the importance <strong>of</strong> having agood assessment <strong>of</strong> a child’s overt behaviorin making diagnostic <strong>and</strong> treatment decisions.However, it has become increasinglyclear that overt behavior is only onepiece <strong>of</strong> a complex puzzle. Research inseveral areas <strong>of</strong> child psychopathology hassupported the importance <strong>of</strong> intrap sychicvariables for both assessing (e.g., Frick,2006) <strong>and</strong> treating (e.g., David-Ferdon &Kaslow, 2008) children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.In summary, direct observations areaffected by some factors that <strong>of</strong>ten precludetheir use in many clinical settings <strong>and</strong>limit the usefulness <strong>of</strong> the data obtained.We spent a great deal <strong>of</strong> time reviewingthe factors that affect behavioral observations,not because <strong>of</strong> a bias against thisform <strong>of</strong> assessment, but because we havefound that assessors sometimes ignorethese issues. We feel that a clinical assessorshould be aware <strong>of</strong> these issues in decidingwhether or not direct observations shouldbe included in an assessment battery <strong>and</strong>should consider these issues when interpretingobservational data. However, theselimitations should not be considered anygreater than those associated with otherassessment techniques, <strong>and</strong> the limitationsmust be weighed against some veryimportant advantages <strong>of</strong> direct observation(e.g., elimination <strong>of</strong> reporter bias <strong>and</strong> readytranslation into environmental interventions).Direct observations can be an integralpart <strong>of</strong> many assessment batteries but,as is the case for all assessment techniques,they also have limitations in the informationthey provide in isolation.In the following section, we discussbasic issues in the development <strong>and</strong> use <strong>of</strong>observational systems. As was mentionedearlier in this chapter, many clinical assessorsuse informal observational techniques


192 chapter 8 behavioral observationsin their assessment battery without establishinga well-defined system. Unfortunately,the information obtained fromsuch systems is difficult to interpret.Unlike rating scales, there are few st<strong>and</strong>ardizedobservational systems that arereadily available for clinical use that havewell-established psychometric properties.Therefore, the next section focuseson basic considerations in designing anobservational system for one’s own clinicaluse. Following this discussion, someexamples <strong>of</strong> observational systems that arecommercially available or that have beenused in research are reviewed.Basics <strong>of</strong> ObservationalSystemsDefining Target BehaviorsThe basic components <strong>of</strong> observationalsystems can be broken down into the what,where, how, <strong>and</strong> by whom <strong>of</strong> the system.The first part <strong>of</strong> developing a system <strong>of</strong>direct observation involves defining whatbehaviors one wishes to observe. Definingthe behaviors <strong>of</strong> interest first involvesdeciding on the level <strong>of</strong> analysis one wishesto use (Barrios, 1993). Specifically, thelevel <strong>of</strong> analysis can be at the level <strong>of</strong> isolatedbehaviors, at the level <strong>of</strong> constellations<strong>of</strong> behaviors (syndromes), or at thelevel <strong>of</strong> interactions within a social unit.As an example <strong>of</strong> the social unit level <strong>of</strong>analysis, many observational systems allowfor the recording <strong>of</strong> how a child behavesin response to parental behavior <strong>and</strong> howa parent responds to a child’s behavior(Gelf<strong>and</strong> & Hartmann, 1984). Also, theexample given previously demonstratesan observational system that focuses onbehaviors by fellow students <strong>and</strong> teachersthat can influence the on-task behavior <strong>of</strong> astudent with ADHD (Carroll et al., 2006).Because these systems allow one to documentevents (stimuli) that elicit a behavior<strong>and</strong> responses to the behavior that mayhelp to maintain or increase it, this level<strong>of</strong> analysis provides important informationon potential targets <strong>of</strong> intervention. Anexample <strong>of</strong> a simple antecedent-behaviorconsequence(A-B-C) type <strong>of</strong> observationis provided in Box 8.1. From this example,it is clear that recording antecedents <strong>and</strong>consequences allows one to determine thesequence <strong>of</strong> events within which a behavioris embedded.After the level <strong>of</strong> analysis is chosen, onemust operationally define what behaviors,what constellation <strong>of</strong> behaviors, or whatantecedents/consequences will be observedwithin this window. These definitions aremade prior to beginning a direct observation<strong>and</strong> must be specified in objective <strong>and</strong>underst<strong>and</strong>able terms in order to reducethe potential for bias <strong>and</strong> increase the reliability<strong>of</strong> the observation. Some examples<strong>of</strong> target behaviors used in observationalsystems are described in Table 8.1.The target behaviors in Table 8.1 aresimply lists <strong>of</strong> behaviors from severaldomains that can be assessed by observationalsystems. In order to be reliable,coding systems must have very explicitdefinitions <strong>of</strong> each behavior. This is necessaryto reduce the possibility that theobserver will use subjective <strong>and</strong> idiosyncraticdefinitions <strong>of</strong> the behaviors,thereby making interpretations from theobservations difficult. Without such definitionsthe primary advantage <strong>of</strong> directobservations, objectivity, is severely compromised.One would think that behaviorssuch as those in Table 8.1 are easy todefine <strong>and</strong> that simple definitions wouldlead to different observers being able tocode the same behavior in the same way.Decades <strong>of</strong> research have found that thisis not true. To reliably code behaviors,one must develop very detailed definitions.Box 8.2 provides an example <strong>of</strong> avery detailed definition <strong>of</strong> behavior froma frequently used coding system.


chapter 8 behavioral observations193Box 8.1A Hypothetical Example <strong>of</strong> Simple A-B-C Observational System <strong>of</strong> an8-Year-Old Boy (B)Time/Setting Antecedent Behavior Consequence8:30/Math classcopyingfromboard8:35/Math classdoingseatwork8:55/Math classcompletingseatwork<strong>Child</strong> ignoreshimTeacher reprim<strong>and</strong>sBTeacher ignoresBTeacher puts B’sname on boardB takes pencil fromanother childB tears paper on child’sdeskB sulksB leaves seat to sharpenpencilB raises h<strong>and</strong>B gets out <strong>of</strong> seat <strong>and</strong> pullson teacher’s shirt to getattentionB starts to cry<strong>Child</strong> ignores him<strong>Child</strong> tells teacher <strong>and</strong>teacher reprim<strong>and</strong>s BTeacher allows B to eraseboardTeacher asks B to raise h<strong>and</strong>to leave seatTeacher continues to workwith other studentTeacher scolds B for leavingseat <strong>and</strong> places name onboard<strong>Child</strong> teases B<strong>Child</strong> teases B B tries to hit other child B sent to <strong>of</strong>ficeB returns toclassB sullen <strong>and</strong> refuses toworkTeacher allows B to collectassignmentsTable 8.1 Examples <strong>of</strong> Target Behaviors from Several Behavioral DomainsADHD(Carroll et al.,2006)ConductProblems(Patterson,1982)SocialCompetence(Dodge, 1983)Depression(Kazdin, 1988)Autism (Lord,Rutter, DiLavore,& Risi, 1999)Off task Noncompliance Solitary play Talking Asking for helpFidgeting Destructiveness Cooperative play Playing alone Symbolic playInappropriate Aggressive play Smiling Negativism Taking turnstalkingGazing around Insults/threats Compliments Frowning Reciprocal playOut <strong>of</strong> seat Aggression Rule making Complaining Telling a sequentialstoryLoud talking Arguing Turn taking WhiningTeasing


194 chapter 8 behavioral observationsBox 8.2Criteria for “Whine” from theDyadic Parent–<strong>Child</strong> Coding SystemDefinitionA whine consists <strong>of</strong> words uttered by thechild in a slurring, nasal, high-pitched, falsettovoice.ExamplesWhen can we go home?Mommy, I hurt my finger.I have to go to the bathroom. This is toohard.I don’t want to play this anymore.Guidelines1. The voice quality <strong>of</strong> the word or phraseis the primary distinguishing element forcoding whine.2. Each whined sentence constitutes a separatewhine. Whined phrases separatedfrom one another by a pause <strong>of</strong> 2 s orlonger are coded as separate whines.Examples:<strong>Child</strong>: I have a headache. I want to go home.(2 whine)<strong>Child</strong>: I don’t like the red blocks… 2-spause… <strong>and</strong> I don’t like the Legos.(2 whine)<strong>Child</strong>: Please let me take it home… 2-spause… Please. (2 whine)3. The content <strong>of</strong> the word or phrase maybe anything except smart talk.Examples:I don’t like this anymore.I hate you.I feel sick.You make me sick.You hurt my feelings.You’re a jerk.(whine)(smart talk)(whine)(smart talk)(whine)(smart talk)4. Whining is a verbal behavior <strong>and</strong> canoccur simultaneously with a nonverbaldeviant child behavior (destructive orphysical negative child).Decision Rules1. When uncertain as to whether the child’svoice quality is actually a whine or normalvoice quality, do not code whine.2. When uncertain as to whether a child’sverbalization is a whine, smart talk, or acry, code whine.3. When uncertain as to whether the deviantbehavior is a whine or a yell, code yell.Source: Summarized from the manual forthe Dyadic Parent–<strong>Child</strong> Interaction CodingSystem (Eyberg & Robinson, 1983) with theauthors’ permission.SettingOnce the target behaviors are defined,the next decision is to determine where toobserve these target behaviors. Naturalisticobservations involve observing the child inhis or her natural setting (e.g., in the classroom,at home). The kind <strong>of</strong> behaviors <strong>of</strong>interest (e.g., social interactions duringfree play) <strong>of</strong>ten determines what naturalsetting is best to conduct the observation(e.g., on the playground). In its purestform, naturalistic observations involveplacing no constraints on a child’s behaviorother than those naturally occurring in theobservational setting. However, sometimesit is necessary to place some restrictions onthe observational setting to enhance thequality <strong>of</strong> the observations. For example,an observer who is in the home <strong>of</strong> a childto observe parent–child interactions mayneed to place some constraints on the child<strong>and</strong> parents to ensure that there are sufficientopportunities to observe interactionsduring the observational session. Forexample, one may wish to place restrictionsthat parents <strong>and</strong> children must stayin the same room <strong>and</strong> that there is no talkingon the telephone, working on a computer,playing video games, or watchingTV. Another example is an observational


chapter 8 behavioral observations195system designed to observe a child’s anxiousbehavior. The observer may wish theteacher to “create” a situation that seems tolead to anxiety in the child, such as beingcalled on in class or taking a test, in orderto observe the child’s response.In Box 8.3, we provide an example<strong>of</strong> a study by Ostrov <strong>and</strong> Keating(2004) in which observations<strong>of</strong> aggressive behavior <strong>of</strong> preschool childrenwere observed in both free play<strong>and</strong> during several structured interactions.Both types <strong>of</strong> observations wereconducted in a naturalistic setting (i.e.,the child’s school). The use <strong>of</strong> differentobservational situations within the samestudy allowed the authors to determinethe types <strong>of</strong> settings in which aggression ismost likely to occur.Naturalistic observations are <strong>of</strong>tenpreferred because they generally providemore ecologically valid data. However,time <strong>and</strong> cost constraints may prevent onefrom conducting a naturalistic observation.For many clinical assessments, it is <strong>of</strong>tenimpossible for the assessor to make severalBox 8.3An Observational Study <strong>of</strong> Preschool AggressionOstrov <strong>and</strong> Keating (2004) reported a study<strong>of</strong> aggression in preschool children usingnaturalistic observations in the child’s schoolsetting. This study provides a good example<strong>of</strong> two common types <strong>of</strong> naturalistic observationaltechniques, one in which no restrictionsare placed in the natural setting (i.e., free play)<strong>and</strong> one in which the situation is structured(i.e., coloring task). The study involved 46children (mean age <strong>of</strong> 64 months) in ruralpreschools.What: The observation coding systemfocused behaviors in four main categories: (1)physical aggression: hitting, pushing, pulling,punching, forcibly taking objects; (2) verbalaggression: teasing, calling mean names, verbalthreats <strong>of</strong> harm, insults; (3) relational aggression:excluding from play group; spreadingrumors, withdrawing friendship; telling lies;ignoring peer; (4) number <strong>of</strong> male <strong>and</strong> femaleplaymates: number <strong>of</strong> children <strong>of</strong> each sex theobserved child directly interacted.Where: Free play observations were conductedduring regularly scheduled free playperiods in large indoor playrooms, in classrooms,<strong>and</strong> outdoors on the playground. Forthe coloring task, pairs <strong>of</strong> children were givena series <strong>of</strong> three pictures to color. However,the potential for mild conflict was introducedby providing one colorful crayon <strong>and</strong> onewhite one.How: Each observational session was 10min <strong>and</strong> every instance <strong>of</strong> the specified behaviorswere coded. Each child was observed forfive sessions. Behavioral counts were summedacross observational periods to determine ascore in each <strong>of</strong> the four behavioral categories.By Whom: Observers were three female<strong>and</strong> one male undergraduate students whowere trained on the observational system.Prior to conducting observations, observerswere introduced to the teacher <strong>and</strong> students<strong>and</strong> they spent a few days in the classroom tolet the students adjust to their presence.Results: Boys exhibited more physicalaggression but girls displayed more relationalaggression. Aggression was less overall <strong>and</strong>these gender differences were less pronouncedduring the coloring task. However, there wasfairly high stability in a child’s level aggressionacross contexts.Summarized from: Ostrov, J. M., &Keating, C.F. (2004). Gender differences inpreschool aggression during free play <strong>and</strong>structured interactions: An observationalstudy. Social Development, 13, 255–277.


196 chapter 8 behavioral observationshome visits to observe a child or adolescentinteracting with his or her parents. Also,for some behaviors, there may not be a way<strong>of</strong> obtaining unobtrusive observations ina child’s natural environment. As a result,the level <strong>of</strong> reactivity would be so high thatthe data would be meaningless. In additionto these more practical considerations,sometimes there is a need to exert morecontrol over the situation than is possible in anatural setting. For example, one may wishto observe a child’s activity level in a freeplay situation by determining how manytimes a child passes from one part <strong>of</strong> a roomto another. To code this reliably, one c<strong>and</strong>ivide the room into sections with tape<strong>and</strong> then code the number <strong>of</strong> times a childcrosses over a tape divider (Milich, 1984).This type <strong>of</strong> control (e.g., dividing theplayroom into grids) may not be feasiblein a child’s natural environment.For these reasons, it is sometimes necessaryor desirable to conduct analog observationsin a laboratory or clinic. Analog refersto the creation <strong>of</strong> a contrived setting thatapproximates the natural environment.Dividing a clinic playroom into grids toobserve a child’s activity level is one example<strong>of</strong> an analog setting. However, thekey to these observations is how well theanalog situation approximates the naturalenvironment. Staying with our example,it would be imperative that the playroombe similar to a play area that a child wouldbe in outside <strong>of</strong> the clinic (e.g., with ageappropriatetoys available). There aremany other examples <strong>of</strong> analog settings forbehavioral observations, but each involvesthe basic component <strong>of</strong> simulating a child’snatural environment in a clinic setting.Sometimes it is not feasible to have theclinic setting approximate the natural setting.In these cases, children may be askedto imagine themselves in a situation, <strong>and</strong>their behavior is observed in this role-playsituation. An area in which role play observationshave been frequently used is theassessment <strong>of</strong> children’s social competence(e.g., Bornstein, Bellack, & Hersen, 1977;Dodge, McClaskey, & Feldman, 1985). Forexample, Dodge et al. (1985) had childrenpretend that they were in certain social situations<strong>and</strong> then pretend that the assessorwas another child. An explicit coding systemwas developed to code the degree <strong>of</strong>social competence <strong>of</strong> a child’s behavior ineach <strong>of</strong> the imagined situations. An example<strong>of</strong> one <strong>of</strong> the role-play situations usedin this study is included in Box 8.4.Data CollectionThe next stage in developing an observationalsystem is to determine how one willcode the target behaviors in the selectedsetting. There are several data collectionmethods that can be used, with the method<strong>of</strong> choice depending on the characteristics<strong>of</strong> the behaviors <strong>of</strong> interest. Although thereare many variations <strong>of</strong> these basic data collectionmethods, the techniques can belargely placed into three categories: EventRecording, Duration Recording, <strong>and</strong> TimeSampling.Event RecordingEvent recording is the simplest <strong>of</strong> the datacollection methods. It involves recordingthe number <strong>of</strong> times that a target behavioroccurred during preset intervals orduring an entire observational session.This method was illustrated in the study<strong>of</strong> preschool aggression described in Box8.3 (Ostov & Keating, 2004). Due to itssimplicity, event recording is the mostfrequently used method <strong>of</strong> direct observation.However, to use event recording,target behaviors must have discrete beginnings<strong>and</strong> endings, such as hitting anotherchild, raising one’s h<strong>and</strong>, <strong>and</strong> asking toplay a game (Shapiro, 1987). In contrast,behaviors that are continuous <strong>and</strong> persistfor long periods <strong>of</strong> time are more difficultto code using event recording because it


chapter 8 behavioral observations197Box 8.4A Role-Play Situation from the Dodge Study <strong>of</strong> Social CompetenceSituation #2Let’s pretend that I’m playing blocks with some<strong>of</strong> my friends after lunch. We’re building a reallyneat house. You come in the schoolroom <strong>and</strong> seeus. Pretend that you really want to play blockswith us. what do you do <strong>and</strong> say?a. Was the child role playing?b. How competent was the response? Score:8-Complimentary or evaluative remark with are-quest to play: “Boy, that’s neat.Can I play?”6—A simple request to play: “I’d ask, MayI please play?”4—Rhetorical question or evaluativeremark: “What are you doing?” or “That’sneat.”2—Suggestion for different activity:“Want to play a game?”0—Aggressive responses: “I’d knock theblocks over”, “I would say nothing or sitdown at desk without speaking”: “I do’ntknow what to do”, or no answer.------------ ------------ ------------ ------------0 2 4 6 8VeryIncompetentSomewhatIncompetentNeither Competentnor IncompetentSomewhatCompetentVeryCompetentReproduced with permission <strong>of</strong> authors from the Scoring System for <strong>Child</strong> Role Plays: Role Playing Criteriaused in Dodge, K. A., McClaskey, C. L., & Feldman E. (1985). Situational approach to the assessment <strong>of</strong> socialcompetence in children. Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 53, 344–353.is difficult to distinguish the occurrence<strong>of</strong> one incident from the next. Examples<strong>of</strong> such continuous behaviors are <strong>of</strong>f-taskbehavior, talking out loud, <strong>and</strong> engagingin solitary play. Event recording is especiallyuseful for recording behaviors thatoccur only briefly <strong>and</strong> for recording lowfrequencybehaviors that only occur onceor twice in an observational period, such asswearing or hitting another child (Keller,1986; Shapiro, 1987).behavior. In duration recording, theobserver records the length <strong>of</strong> time fromthe beginning to the end <strong>of</strong> an instance <strong>of</strong>behavior. Duration <strong>and</strong> event recordingcan be combined to provide an even richersource <strong>of</strong> information (Shapiro, 1987). Forexample, in observing the temper tantrums<strong>of</strong> a young child, it may be helpful to recordnot only the frequency <strong>of</strong> tantrums withina given period, but to also record the duration<strong>of</strong> each tantrum episode.Duration RecordingFor some assessments, it may be moreimportant to know how long a behavioroccurs rather than the frequency <strong>of</strong> theTime SamplingIn both event <strong>and</strong> duration recording techniques,all instances <strong>of</strong> the behaviors arerecorded during the observational period.


198 chapter 8 behavioral observationsHowever, some behaviors occur too frequentlyto obtain accurate counts, or thereare no clear beginning or end points to thebehaviors, which prevents effective event<strong>and</strong> duration recording. For these types <strong>of</strong>behaviors, the observation period can bedivided into predetermined intervals, <strong>and</strong>the behaviors are simply coded as beingeither present or absent during each interval.Therefore, rather than yielding anexact count <strong>of</strong> the number <strong>of</strong> times that abehavior occurred during the observationalperiod, time sampling allows one to determinethe proportion <strong>of</strong> intervals in whichthe behavior occurred.Shapiro (1987) reviews three types <strong>of</strong>time-sampling techniques. In whole-intervalrecording one codes a behavior as presentonly if it occurs throughout a time interval.For example, an observational period in achild’s classroom can be broken down into20-s intervals <strong>and</strong> the number <strong>of</strong> intervalsin which the child remained on task forthe entire interval is recorded. In partialintervalrecording, one records whether ornot a behavior occurred at any time duringthe interval. Shapiro gives the example <strong>of</strong> ateacher dividing the day into 15-min segments<strong>and</strong> noting whether or not certainbehaviors occurred during each segment.A final type <strong>of</strong> time sampling is momentaryrecording, in which one records whether abehavior was present or absent only duringthe moment when a time interval ends.For example, when observing the degree<strong>of</strong> social withdrawal <strong>of</strong> a child, one maydivide an observational period into 60-sintervals <strong>and</strong> record whether or not a childwas engaged in interactions with otherchildren at the end <strong>of</strong> each interval.Selecting the ObserversAfter determining what behaviors will beobserved, where the observations will takeplace, <strong>and</strong> how the observations will beconducted, one still must determine whois best suited to conduct the observation.Having someone who is in the child’snatural setting (e.g., teacher or parent)conduct the observation is <strong>of</strong>ten useful innaturalistic observations because it helps tomaintain unobtrusiveness. However, it is<strong>of</strong>ten difficult to teach people in the child’snatural environment how to use a codingsystem <strong>and</strong> to ensure that it is being usedappropriately.In order to exert more control over theobservational methodology, many observationalsystems require rigorously trained<strong>and</strong> monitored observers. Barrios (1993)provides a summary <strong>of</strong> steps required intraining <strong>and</strong> monitoring observers (seeTable 8.2). As one can see from this summary,using specially trained observers isquite costly. Such stringent methodologyis feasible only when a large number <strong>of</strong>children are being observed with the sameobservational method. Therefore, thismethodology may not be optimal in manyclinical settings.One type <strong>of</strong> observation that is frequentlyused in clinical assessments involvestraining a person to observe his or her ownbehavior. The same steps <strong>of</strong> selecting targetbehaviors, determining the setting forthe observation, <strong>and</strong> determining how thetarget behaviors will be recorded are followed.However, in self-monitoring the childis trained to record his or her own behavior.Although self-monitoring has beenused largely with adults, children have usedself-monitoring systems to monitor suchdiverse behaviors as classroom attending,class attendance, talking out in class, roomcleaning, aggression, <strong>and</strong> inappropriateverbalizations (Mash & Terdal, 1988).One method <strong>of</strong> self-monitoring thathas begun to receive some attention inresearch is the use <strong>of</strong> electronic diaries.That is, with the advent <strong>of</strong> small computers<strong>and</strong> personal digital assistants (PDAs),children <strong>and</strong> adolescents can be taught to


chapter 8 behavioral observations199Table 8.2 Steps in Training <strong>and</strong> Monitoring ObserversStepOrientationEducationEvaluationApplicationRecalibrationTerminationDescriptionInforming observers <strong>of</strong> the importance <strong>of</strong> objective assessment in theunderst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> treating <strong>of</strong> childhood disorders. Informing observers <strong>of</strong>their duties <strong>and</strong> responsibilities, in particular their independent, unbiased, <strong>and</strong>faithful recording <strong>of</strong> the behavior <strong>of</strong> interest.Instructing observers in the response definitions <strong>and</strong> recording scheme throughthe use <strong>of</strong> written materials, filmed illustrations, <strong>and</strong> live demonstrations.<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> observers’ knowledge <strong>of</strong> the response definitions, coding system,<strong>and</strong> recording scheme through the use <strong>of</strong> written <strong>and</strong> oral examinations.Representation <strong>of</strong> materials until observers are thoroughly acquainted with allaspects <strong>of</strong> tracking <strong>and</strong> recording <strong>of</strong> the behaviors <strong>of</strong> interest.Graduated implementation <strong>of</strong> the observation system across a range <strong>of</strong> situations,beginning with analog ones <strong>and</strong> ending with actual setting <strong>of</strong> interest. Transitionfrom one situation to the next contingent upon observers achieving a criterionlevel <strong>of</strong> agreement <strong>and</strong> accuracy.<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> the accuracy <strong>and</strong> agreement <strong>of</strong> observers’ recordings in the setting<strong>of</strong> interest. Identification <strong>and</strong> correction <strong>of</strong> any breakdowns in the fidelity <strong>of</strong>observers’ recordings.Questioning observers as to the merits <strong>of</strong> the observation system. Informingobservers <strong>of</strong> their contributions to the underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> treating <strong>of</strong> the behaviors<strong>of</strong> interest. Reminding observers <strong>of</strong> the need to maintain confidentiality.Reproduced with permission from Barrios (1993). Direct observation. In T. H. Ollendick <strong>and</strong> M. Hersen (Eds.),H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong> adolescent assessment (pp. 140–164). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.carry a small PDA which cues the child torespond to certain prompts <strong>and</strong> questions.This allows the child to record responsesin real time (i.e., when it is actually occurring).For example, Whalen et al. (2006)had 52 children (mean age <strong>of</strong> 10.58) reporton their moods, behaviors, <strong>and</strong> socialcontexts every 30 min during nonschoolhours. The children carried a PDA thatbeeped every 30 min to signal it was timeto respond to certain questions. The children’sresponses were saved in the PDAfor later analyses. Twenty-seven <strong>of</strong> thechildren were diagnosed with ADHD <strong>and</strong>the results showed that these children hadmore behavioral problems, negative mood,<strong>and</strong> conflict with parents.Research suggests that children canself-monitor their behavior accuratelyif they are trained appropriately, have aclear <strong>and</strong> simple observational system,have an outside monitor <strong>of</strong> the accuracy<strong>of</strong> their recording, <strong>and</strong> are reinforced forthe accuracy <strong>of</strong> their recording (Keller,1986). The advantages <strong>of</strong> self-monitoringare that it is cost effective <strong>and</strong> is lessintrusive than many other forms <strong>of</strong> behavioralobservation. However, researchclearly suggests that children change theirbehavior as they become more aware <strong>of</strong>it through self-monitoring (Keller, 1986;Shapiro, 1987). Whereas this change inbehavior may be a beneficial aspect <strong>of</strong>self-monitoring in a treatment program,this reactivity limits the usefulness <strong>of</strong> selfmonitoringas a means <strong>of</strong> obtaining objectiveinformation on a child’s behavior forassessment purposes.


200 chapter 8 behavioral observationsExamples <strong>of</strong> ObservationalSystemsIn contrast to behavior rating scales, thereare few observational systems that arewidely used, have st<strong>and</strong>ardized procedures,<strong>and</strong> are readily available for clinical use. Inthis section, we review some notable exceptions.The goal <strong>of</strong> this overview is not toprovide an exhaustive list <strong>of</strong> observationalsystems but to provide a carefully selectedlist <strong>of</strong> observational techniques that varyin terms <strong>of</strong> target behaviors, settings <strong>of</strong>observation, method <strong>of</strong> data collection,<strong>and</strong> degree <strong>of</strong> training needed to reliablyuse the observational system. We feel that,even if one does not choose to use one <strong>of</strong>the specific systems discussed here, thesesystems provide concrete examples <strong>of</strong> some<strong>of</strong> the issues discussed in this chapter <strong>and</strong>therefore can serve as a guide for the development<strong>and</strong> use <strong>of</strong> other observational systems.Also, some additional observationalsystems that focus on a specific areas <strong>of</strong>adjustment are reviewed in other chapters.Achenbach System <strong>of</strong>Empirically Based <strong>Assessment</strong>:Direct Observation Form <strong>and</strong>Test Observation FormThe ASEBA system <strong>of</strong> assessments containstwo observational systems. The firstis the Direct Observation Form (DOF;Achenbach, 2001) which is designed foruse with children <strong>and</strong> adolescents ages 5 to14. It provides a method <strong>of</strong> coding observationsin academic classrooms <strong>and</strong> othergroup activities. The Test ObservationForm (McConaughy & Achenbach, 2004)is designed for use with children <strong>and</strong> adolescentsages 2 to 18 <strong>and</strong> allows for the coding<strong>of</strong> behavioral observations during theindividual administration <strong>of</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardizedability <strong>and</strong> achievement tests. Both <strong>of</strong> thesesystems are part <strong>of</strong> the ASEBA system <strong>and</strong>are designed to be interpreted in conjunctionwith the parent, teacher, <strong>and</strong> child selfreportversions <strong>of</strong> the ASEBA, all <strong>of</strong> whichhave been discussed in previous chapters.The DOF is designed to provide adirect observation <strong>of</strong> a child in a classroomor group setting during a 10-min period.There are three parts to the DOF. First,the observer is asked to write a narrativedescription <strong>of</strong> a child’s behavior throughoutthe 10-min observational period, notingthe occurrence, duration, <strong>and</strong> intensity<strong>of</strong> specific problems. Second, at the end <strong>of</strong>each minute the child’s behavior is codedas being on- or <strong>of</strong>f-task for 5 s. Third, atthe end <strong>of</strong> the 10-min period, the observerrates the child on 96 behaviors that mayhave been observed during the observationalperiod using a 4-point scale (from 0= behavior was not observed to 3 = definiteoccurrence <strong>of</strong> behavior with severe intensityor for greater than 3 min duration).The 96 problem behaviors on the DOFhave a high degree <strong>of</strong> item overlap withthe behaviors rated on the parent <strong>and</strong>teacher rating scales <strong>of</strong> the ASEBA system.Therefore, the DOF is nicely suitedfor a multimodal assessment <strong>of</strong> a child’sor adolescent’s emotional <strong>and</strong> behavioralfunctioning. Like the other parts <strong>of</strong> theASEBA system, the DOF can be used tocalculate a Total Problem score, which isa sum <strong>of</strong> the ratings <strong>of</strong> all 96 problems,two broadb<strong>and</strong> scales (Internalizing <strong>and</strong>Externalizing), <strong>and</strong> six narrowb<strong>and</strong> scales(Withdrawn-Inattentive, Nervous-Obsessive,Depressed, Hyperactive, Attentiondem<strong>and</strong>ing,Aggressive) (Achenbach,2001). The DOF does report norms from arelatively small sample <strong>of</strong> 287 nonreferredchildren (Achenbach, 2001).There is evidence that the DOF canbe used reliably by observers with minimaltraining. Inter-observer correlationshave been calculated on the Total Problemsscale in several samples. Correlationsbetween observers range from .96 in a


chapter 8 behavioral observations201residential treatment center (Achenbach,1986) to .92 in a sample <strong>of</strong> boys referredfor special services in school (Reed &Edelbrock, 1983) to.75 in a sample <strong>of</strong> outpatientreferrals to a child psychiatry clinic(McConaughy, Achenbach, & Gent, 1988).Inter-observer correlations for the On-taskscores were .71, .71, <strong>and</strong> .88 in the threesamples, respectively. Reed <strong>and</strong> Edelbrock(1983) reported inter-observer reliability intheir sample <strong>of</strong> 25 boys for a selected set <strong>of</strong>individual items from the DOF. In general,most items showed high inter-observercorrelations (most above .80), with theexceptions <strong>of</strong> Nervous, high-strung, ortense (.20); Picks nose, skin, or other parts<strong>of</strong> body (.52); <strong>and</strong> Compulsions, repeatsbehavior over <strong>and</strong> over (.53).Reed <strong>and</strong> Edelbrock (1983) reported thatthe Total Problems scale <strong>and</strong> On-task scoresfrom the DOF correlated in expected directionswith teacher ratings <strong>of</strong> total problems<strong>and</strong> adaptive behaviors. In addition, theDOF Total Problems scale <strong>and</strong> On-taskscores have been shown to differ in normal<strong>and</strong> disturbed children (McConaughyet al., 1988; Reed & Edelbrock, 1983). Interms <strong>of</strong> discriminating within disturbedchildren, the evidence for the DOF is lessclear. McConaughy et al. (1988) reportedthat the Total Problems, On-task, <strong>and</strong>Externalizing scores differentiated childrenclassified with internalizing or externalizingproblems. However, the internalizing scale<strong>of</strong> the CBCL-DOF did not demonstratediscriminant validity in this study.The TOF is designed to rate children’sbehavior during an individual st<strong>and</strong>ardizedtesting session. It has 125 items thatare rated on a four point scale (0 = “nooccurrence” – 3 = “definite occurrencewith severe intensity or 3 or more minutesduration”). Items are rated by the examinerimmediately after the testing session. Likethe DOF, the TOF has a strong overlap initems with other measures in the ASEBAsystem. Thus, a Total Problem, an Internalizing,<strong>and</strong> an Externalizing compositecan all be obtained from the TOF. Also,it includes five narrow b<strong>and</strong> scales: Withdrawn/Depressed,Language/ThoughtProblems, Anxious, Oppositional, <strong>and</strong>Attentional Problems.The TOF has a normative sample <strong>of</strong>3,943 children between the ages <strong>of</strong> 2 <strong>and</strong>18, most <strong>of</strong> which were obtained duringthe st<strong>and</strong>ardization <strong>of</strong> the Stanford-BinetIntelligence Scales-5th Edition (Roid,2003). In general, the TOF scales showgood test-retest reliability (.53–.87) over aperiod <strong>of</strong> 10 days, adequate interrater reliability(.42–.73), <strong>and</strong> good internal consistency(.74–.94) (McConaughy, 2005).Scores on the TOF are moderately correlatedwith corresponding parent completedASEBA (.27–.43) <strong>and</strong> teacher completedASEBA (.26–.38) scales (McConaughy,2005). Also, the TOF scales have differentiatedchildren with ADHD from normalcontrol children (McConaughy, 2005).In summary, the TOF <strong>and</strong> DOF areboth time-efficient observational systemsthat require minimal observer training <strong>and</strong>fit into a multimethod assessment system.Both observational systems have informationshowing some basic levels <strong>of</strong> reliability. Theyboth also provide norm-referenced scores,although the sample for the DOF norms isvery limited. Also, both systems have provento be useful for discriminating normal fromclinic-referred children. However, their abilityto differentiate within children with emotional<strong>and</strong> behavioral problems is less clear.Behavioral <strong>Assessment</strong>System for <strong>Child</strong>ren-StudentObservation SystemThe BASC-2-Student Observation System(BASC-2-SOS; Reynolds & Kamphaus,2004) is a commercially available, short (15-min) observational system that is designedfor use in a classroom setting. It is part <strong>of</strong>the comprehensive BASC-2 system, which


202 chapter 8 behavioral observationsincludes parent <strong>and</strong> teacher behavior ratingscales <strong>and</strong> a child self-report form, all<strong>of</strong> which have been discussed in previouschapters. The BASC-2-SOS defines 65specific target behaviors that are groupedinto 13 categories (4 categories <strong>of</strong> positive/adaptivebehaviors <strong>and</strong> 9 categories <strong>of</strong>problem behaviors). The 13 categories <strong>and</strong>examples <strong>of</strong> target behaviors in each categoryare provided in Table 8.3.Table 8.3 Behavioral Categories from theStudent Observation System <strong>of</strong> BASC2Category/DefinitionCategory/DefinitionResponse to Teacher/Lesson (appropriateacademic behaviorsinvolving teacheror class)Peer Interaction(appropriateinteractions withother students)Work on School Subjects(appropriateacademic behaviorsthat studentengages in alone)Transition Movement(appropriatenondisruptivebehaviors whilemoving from oneactivity to another)InappropriateMovement (inappropriatemotorbehaviors that areunrelated to classroomwork)Example <strong>of</strong> SpecificBehaviorsFollows directionsRaises h<strong>and</strong>Contributes to classdiscussionWaits for help onassignmentPlays with other studentsInteracts in friendlymannerShakes h<strong>and</strong> with otherstudentConverses with others indiscussionDoes seatworkWorks at blackboardWorks at computerPuts on/takes <strong>of</strong>f coatGets bookSharpens pencilWalks in lineReturns material usedin classFidgeting in seatPassing notesRunning aroundclassroomSitting/st<strong>and</strong>ing on top<strong>of</strong> deskCategory/DefinitionInattention(inattentive behaviorsthat are notdisruptive)Inappropriate Vocalization(disruptivevocal behaviors)Somatization(physical symptoms/complaints)Repetitive MotorMovements (repetitivebehaviors thatappear to have noexternal reward)Aggression (harmfulbehaviors directedat another personorproperty)Self-InjuriousBehavior (severebehaviors thatattempt to injureone’s self)Inappropriate SexualBehavior (behaviorsthat are explicitlysexual in nature)Bowel/BladderProblems (urinationor defecation)Example <strong>of</strong> SpecificBehaviorsDaydreamingDoodlingLooking around roomFiddling with objects/fingersLaughing inappropriatelyTeasingTalking outCryingSleepingComplaining <strong>of</strong> notfeeling wellFinger/pencil tappingSpinning an objectBody rockingHumming/singing tooneselfKicking othersThrowing objects atothersIntentionally rippinganother’s workStealingPulling own hairHead bangingBiting selfEating or chewingnonfood itemsTouching others inappropriatelyMasturbatingImitating sexual behaviorWets pantsHas bowel movementoutside toiletFrom C. R. Reynolds <strong>and</strong> R. W. Kamphaus (2004). Behaviorassessment system for children – 2nd edition(BASC-2). Circle Pines, MN: American GuidanceServices.The BASC-2-SOS was designed tobe completed during a 15-min observation<strong>of</strong> the child in an academic classroom.


chapter 8 behavioral observations203The behaviors during the observation arecoded in three parts. In Part A, each <strong>of</strong>the 65 behaviors are rated as being “NotObserved,” “Sometimes Observed” or“Frequently Observed.” Also, any behaviorjudged to be disruptive is noted. Part Buses a momentary time-sampling approachin recording data. The 15-min observationalperiod is divided into 30 intervals. Atthe end <strong>of</strong> each 30-sec interval the child’sbehavior is observed for 3-sec. A checklistallows the observer to mark each category<strong>of</strong> behavior that occurred during the3-sec observation interval. In Part C, theobserver is asked to describe the teacher’sinteractions with the student, focusing oncontingencies in the classroom that may beinfluencing the child’s behavior.The BASC-2-SOS is a simple <strong>and</strong> timeefficient observational system that assesses,through direct observation, many behaviorsthat are crucial for the clinical assessment<strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. It is one <strong>of</strong>the few direct observational systems commerciallyavailable. Also, the BASC-2-SOShas an electronic coding format in whichbehavioral observations can be recordeddirectly into a PDA device.However, the BASC2-SOS lacks anumber <strong>of</strong> crucial psychometric elements.First, there is no information onthe reliability <strong>of</strong> the system provided inthe manual. Establishing inter-observeragreement is a crucial component indeveloping an observational system, toensure that observations are objective <strong>and</strong>relatively free from bias. Second, there areno norms for the BASC-2-SOS, so normreferencedinterpretation <strong>of</strong> scores is notpossible. Third, there is limited informationon the validity <strong>of</strong> BASC-2SOS, suchas whether or not the BASC-2-SOS codecategories correlate with clinically importantcriteria (e.g., diagnoses, behaviorrating scales, response to intervention).Lett <strong>and</strong> Kamphaus (1997) reported thatscores on the BASC-2-SOS did differentiatechildren with ADHD from normalcontrol children. However, the limitedinformation on the psychometric properties<strong>of</strong> the BAS-2-SOS greatly limits itspotential contribution to many clinicalassessments.Behavioral Avoidance TestsBehavioral Avoidance Tests (BATs) havebeen used to observe a person’s behavioralresponse to anxiety-producing stimulisince the early 1900s (Jersild & Holmes,1935). Although there are many differentversions <strong>of</strong> BATs (e.g., Morris & Kratochwill,1983; Van Hasselt, Hersen, Bellack,Rosenblum, & Lamparski, 1979), they allinvolve exposing the child or adolescentto some feared stimuli (e.g., animal, dark,stranger, heights, blood), then requiringthe child to approach the feared stimuli ingraduated steps.BATs provide explicit <strong>and</strong> objective criteriafor observing a child’s behavioral reactionto the feared stimulus, such as howclosely the stimulus is approached, thenumber <strong>of</strong> steps in the gradual approachthat are taken, or the time spent touchingor h<strong>and</strong>ling the phobic stimulus. In quantifyingthese responses to anxiety-producingstimuli, BATs provide a measure <strong>of</strong> theseverity <strong>of</strong> a child’s anxiety <strong>and</strong> can helpdocument changes brought about by interventions(Vasay & Lonigan, 2000).Southam-Gerow <strong>and</strong> Chorpita (2007) providea good summary <strong>of</strong> the advantages <strong>and</strong>disadvantages <strong>of</strong> BATs. They describe theprimary disadvantage <strong>of</strong> BATs as the absence<strong>of</strong> a single st<strong>and</strong>ardized BAT. Instead, therehave been numerous different BATs developedthat vary widely on the number <strong>of</strong>steps in the graduated approach, the types <strong>of</strong>instructions given to the child, <strong>and</strong> how thefeared stimulus is presented. As a result, itis impossible to compare the findings acrossstudies <strong>and</strong> therefore it is impossible todevelop a significant body <strong>of</strong> knowledge onthe reliability, validity, <strong>and</strong> normative base


204 chapter 8 behavioral observationsfor any <strong>of</strong> the BATs. In addition, a hallmark<strong>of</strong> the BATs is the rigorous control overhow the feared object is presented to thechild <strong>and</strong> how the child approaches it. Thisdegree <strong>of</strong> control may prevent the behaviorobserved in the contrived setting from generalizingto the child’s natural environment.One final limitation <strong>of</strong> BATs is that they aremost commonly used to assess specific fears<strong>and</strong> are more difficult to design for assessingmore generalized anxiety (Vasay & Lonigan,2000).On the positive side, however, BATs arerelatively simple <strong>and</strong> time efficient in theiradministration. Many <strong>of</strong> the BATs havebeen shown to have good inter-observeragreement with minimal observer training<strong>and</strong> to be sensitive to treatment effects(Barrios & Hartmann, 1988; Southam-Gerow & Chorpita, 2007). Also, scoresfrom BATs are correlated with subjectiveratings <strong>of</strong> fear <strong>and</strong> with phobia diagnoses(Vasay & Lonigan, 2000). Most importantly,they provide one <strong>of</strong> the only methods<strong>of</strong> assessing the behavioral components(i.e., a child’s avoidance <strong>of</strong> a feared stimulus)<strong>of</strong> childhood anxiety.Conflict Negotiation TaskThe Conflict Negotiation Task wasdesigned to assess peer interactions, especiallythose interactions that may be associatedwith childhood depression (Rudolph,Hammen, & Burge, 1994). <strong>Child</strong>ren areobserved with an unfamiliar partner <strong>of</strong>the same age <strong>and</strong> gender. The system usesa task involving three points <strong>of</strong> potentialconflict <strong>of</strong> interest between the child <strong>and</strong>his or her partner. First, child dyads areplaced in a situation in which they areto build structures with colored blocksto match either <strong>of</strong> two models. Theyare informed that whoever constructs anidentical model would win a prize. Thedyad is given a set <strong>of</strong> blocks to share butthe number <strong>of</strong> blocks is only sufficient tobuild one complete model. Second, after10 min <strong>of</strong> observation, the dyad is askedto decide on how to distribute two prizes<strong>of</strong> unequal value. Third, the dyad participatesin a 5-min interview <strong>and</strong> the childwho received the less valuable prize isallowed to choose a new one.The interactions during this task arecoded by trained observers. Using an eventrecording system, behaviors are rated ona seven-point scale (0 = not at all present;4 = moderately present; 7 = to a largedegree present) <strong>and</strong> all ratings are scaledsuch that high scores indicate more negativepeer interactions. The behaviors aregrouped into four composites. Two compositesare related to broad dimensions <strong>of</strong>social behavior displayed by the child beingassessed. Conflict-resolution competenceincludes persistence in problem-solvingefforts, positive assertiveness, positive conflictmanagement, <strong>and</strong> general social competence.Emotional regulation includesconflict exacerbation, positive affect, <strong>and</strong>negative affect. The third composite consists<strong>of</strong> a dyadic quality code based on ratings<strong>of</strong> conflict or friction within the dyad,collaboration, problem-solving competence<strong>of</strong> the dyad, <strong>and</strong> mutuality/reciprocity.Finally, a peer response code is basedon the ratings <strong>of</strong> the peer’s behavior towardto the assessed child, including generalresponse valence (negative or positive) tothe target child, discomfort <strong>and</strong> embarrassmentin response to the target child, <strong>and</strong>emotional state at the end <strong>of</strong> the interaction.Rudolph et al. (1994) reported on theuse <strong>of</strong> this system in a sample <strong>of</strong> 36 children(20 girls <strong>and</strong> 16 boys) between theages <strong>of</strong> 7 <strong>and</strong> 13. The four composites fromthis observational system showed very highcorrelations between raters (.88 to .92)<strong>and</strong> the behaviors within each compositewere highly intercorrelated (.82 to .97).Most importantly, when the 36 childrenwere divided into those high on a measure<strong>of</strong> depression <strong>and</strong> those low on thismeasure, depressed children were rated as


chapter 8 behavioral observations205significantly less competent on all four <strong>of</strong>the composites. Specifically, children highon depression were observed to be significantlyworse in their conflict-resolutioncompetence, in their emotional regulation,in mutuality <strong>and</strong> cooperation duringthe interaction, <strong>and</strong> in the negative valenceplaced on the interactions by their partners(e.g., their partner being more uncomfortablein the interactions). The authors pointout that these data support the contentionthat children who score high on depressionhave significant interpersonal difficulties.Furthermore, the findings suggestthat observational systems <strong>of</strong> children’sinter actions with peers can be useful forassessing important aspects <strong>of</strong> these socialdifficulties.Dodge’s Observation <strong>of</strong> PeerInteractionsDodge (1983) developed a direct observationsystem that also assesses several components<strong>of</strong> peer interactions. However, thisobservation system focuses on behaviorsassociated with acceptance in a peer group.Dodge developed his system in a sample<strong>of</strong> 5-, 6-, 7-, <strong>and</strong> 8-year-old boys. <strong>Child</strong>renwere observed in 60-min play groups<strong>of</strong> eight boys each by three observers whowere stationed behind a one-way mirror.The boys wore numbered T-shirts to aid inquick identification by the observer. Eachobserver coded the behaviors <strong>of</strong> one boyfor a 6-min period <strong>and</strong> then coded a secondchild according to a prearranged schedule.There were 18 target behaviors <strong>of</strong> fivetypes that were defined for the observationsystem (see Table 8.4). A complex eventrecording system was used for the observations.Each time a target behavior wasobserved, the observer coded the time, thecontext (structured vs. unstructured), thetarget behavior observed, <strong>and</strong> the peer target(number <strong>of</strong> child). Observers receivedextensive training over a 4-week periodTable 8.4 Target Behaviors in the DodgeObservational System <strong>of</strong> Peer InteractionsBehavior CategorySolitary activeInteractive playVerbalizationsPhysical contactwith peersInteractions withadultTarget BehaviorsSolitary playWatching peersOn-task behaviorOff-task behaviorCooperative playAggressive playInappropriate play(e.g., st<strong>and</strong>ing ontable)Social conversationswith peersNorm-settingstatements (e.g., rulemaking)Hostile verbalizations(e.g., insults, threats)Supportive statements(e.g., compliments,<strong>of</strong>fers <strong>of</strong> help)Exclusions <strong>of</strong> peersfrom playExtraneousverbalization(e.g., laughs, cheers)HitsObject possession (e.g.,grabbing an objectfrom peer)Physically affectionatebehavior (e.g., holdingh<strong>and</strong>s, hugging)Social conversationwith adult leaderReprim<strong>and</strong>ed by adultgroup leaderFrom K. A. Dodge (1983). Behavioral antecedents<strong>of</strong> peer social status. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 54, 1386–1399.<strong>and</strong>, with this training, the observationalcodes showed quite high inter-observeragreement (Dodge, 1983). Across the 18target behaviors, 15 behaviors showedinter-observer agreement <strong>of</strong> 65% or better.


206 chapter 8 behavioral observationsThe three behaviors that showed poorinter-observer agreement were watchingpeers, norm-setting statements, <strong>and</strong> supportivestatements.Family Interaction CodingSystemOne <strong>of</strong> the most common uses <strong>of</strong> behavioralobservations is to observe parent–childinteractions, especially thoseassociated with childhood conduct problems(Frick & Loney, 2000). For example,Patterson <strong>and</strong> colleagues at the OregonSocial Learning Center have developeda direct observational system designed toassess children’s conduct problems in thehome <strong>and</strong> to assess the interactional patternsin which the conduct problems are<strong>of</strong>ten embedded. The Family InteractionCoding System (FICS; Patterson, 1982)is composed <strong>of</strong> 29 code categories thatinclude both child behaviors <strong>and</strong> parentalreactions to the child behaviors. These categoriesare summarized in Table 8.5. TheTable 8.5 Target Behaviors in the FamilyInteraction Coding SystemApproval High rate a Physicalnegative aAttention Humiliate a PhysicalpositiveComm<strong>and</strong> Ignore a ReceiveComm<strong>and</strong>negative a Laugh SelfstimulationCompliance Noncompliance a TalkCry a Negativism a Tease aDisapproval a Normative TouchDependency a No response Whine aDestructivenessPlay Yell aReproduced with author’s permission from G. R. Patterson(1982). Coerceive family process. Eugene, OR:Castalia.aDenotes aversive behaviors that are included in theTotal Aversive Behavior (TAB) score.goal <strong>of</strong> the FICS was to observe childreninteracting with family members in naturalhome settings. However, as describedby Patterson (1982), several restrictionshad to be made in the home for the observationalsessions. Specifically, to use theFICS, all family members must be presentduring the pre-arranged observation timeswith no guests present, <strong>and</strong> the family islimited to being present in two rooms <strong>of</strong>the house. There can be no telephone callsout (only brief answers to incoming calls)<strong>and</strong> no TV. Finally, there is to be no talkingto the observers during coding.The FICS was designed to have datacoded continuously <strong>and</strong> to provide asequential account <strong>of</strong> the interactionsbetween a child <strong>and</strong> other family members.The behavior <strong>of</strong> the child <strong>and</strong> the person(s)with whom the child interacts are coded insequence. After initial coding, many <strong>of</strong> thechild’s behaviors are summarized in a rateper-minutevariable that combines bothfrequency <strong>and</strong> duration <strong>of</strong> the behavior.However, the most frequently used scorefrom the FICS is the Total Aversive Behavior(TAB) score which is a sum <strong>of</strong> the number<strong>of</strong> aversive behaviors which occurredduring the observational session.Patterson (1982) describes a moderatelevel <strong>of</strong> inter-observer agreement for mostcode categories <strong>of</strong> the FICS, with the categories<strong>of</strong> Negativism <strong>and</strong> Self-Stimulationshowing the most questionable levels <strong>of</strong>agreement. One week test-retest reliability<strong>of</strong> the TAB was studied in a sample <strong>of</strong> 27boys <strong>and</strong> was found to be quite high (.78).The TAB was also found to discriminatebetween families <strong>of</strong> children referred forbehavior problems <strong>and</strong> nonreferred families<strong>and</strong> has proven to be sensitive to familyfocusedtreatment for children’s conductproblems (Patterson, 1982). Althoughmost <strong>of</strong> the individual code categories <strong>of</strong>the FICS can be coded consistently bytwo observers, psychometric informationis generally limited to the global index <strong>of</strong>aversive behavior, the TAB.


chapter 8 behavioral observations207In summary, the FICS is an example <strong>of</strong>an observation system designed to assess achild’s behavior in the home environment<strong>and</strong> to assess parent–child interactionalpatterns in which the behavior is embedded.The FICS is generally most usefulfor younger children (10 <strong>and</strong> under). Also,most <strong>of</strong> the psychometric informationavailable for the FICS is for the aversivebehaviors assessed by the TAB. This is notas severe a limitation as one might think,however, given that these aversive behaviorshave proven to be quite important tounderst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> treating children withconduct problems (Patterson, 1982).Structured Observation <strong>of</strong> Academic<strong>and</strong> Play SettingsMilich, Roberts, <strong>and</strong> colleagues (Milich,1984; Milich, Loney & L<strong>and</strong>au, 1982;Roberts, Ray, & Roberts, 1984) developedan observational system (StructuredObservation <strong>of</strong> Academic <strong>and</strong> Play Settings[SOAPS]) to assess behaviors associatedwith ADHD in a clinic playroomanalog setting. In this system, a clinicplayroom is designed with age-appropriatetoys, four tables, <strong>and</strong> a floor dividedinto 16 equal squares by black tape. Thechild is placed in two situations. Free Playinvolves the child being placed alone inthe room <strong>and</strong> allowed to play freely withthe toys. The Restricted Academic PlayroomSituation involves the child beingrequested (1) to remain seated, (2) tocomplete a series <strong>of</strong> academic tasks, <strong>and</strong>(3) not to play with any <strong>of</strong> the toys.Each observational situation lasts for 15min. A combination <strong>of</strong> event recording <strong>and</strong>time sampling is used in this observationsystem. Event recording is used to determinethe total number <strong>of</strong> grids crossedfor the entire observational period. Thatis, the number <strong>of</strong> times that a child movescompletely from one square <strong>of</strong> the dividedroom into another is counted. Eventrecording is also used to determine thenumber <strong>of</strong> times the child shifts his or herattention from one task to another duringthe entire observational period. A 5-sectime sampling procedure is used to observeother target behaviors. These include theproportion <strong>of</strong> 5-sec intervals that the childis out <strong>of</strong> his or her seat, fidgeting, noisy,<strong>and</strong> on task. In addition, a 5-sec time samplingis used to determine the number<strong>of</strong> intervals that the child was observedtouching forbidden toys. Also during theacademic task, the number <strong>of</strong> items completedis recorded.This observational system is useful inclinical assessments <strong>of</strong> ADHD for a number<strong>of</strong> reasons. First, it is a relatively easy touse observational system. As a result, highinter-observer reliability has been obtainedfor most categories with minimal observertraining. Second, categories from this systemhave been correlated with clinicians’diagnoses <strong>of</strong> ADHD, they have differentiatedADHD children from aggressive<strong>and</strong> other clinic-referred children (Milichet al., 1982), <strong>and</strong> they have been relativelystable over a 2-year period (Milich, 1984).Third, a modified version <strong>of</strong> this task hasbeen shown to be sensitive to treatmentwith stimulant medication (Barkley, 1988).ConclusionsIn this chapter, we have attempted to summarizeboth the advantages <strong>and</strong> disadvantages<strong>of</strong> direct observations <strong>of</strong> behavior aspart <strong>of</strong> a comprehensive clinical assessment.Although there are some limitations,direct observations are a useful componentto many assessment batteries. Probablythe biggest limitation to the clinical utility<strong>of</strong> direct observations is the time <strong>and</strong> costinvolved in conducting behavioral observationsappropriately. We have attempted tooutline some <strong>of</strong> the major considerations indeveloping <strong>and</strong> using observational systemsso that clinical assessors can (1) evaluate


208 chapter 8 behavioral observationsexisting observational systems appropriately,(2) develop their own observationalsystems as needed, <strong>and</strong> (3) recognize limitationsin the data provided by observationalsystems that are not developed <strong>and</strong>used in a sound manner.We concluded this chapter by providingan overview <strong>of</strong> several existing observationalsystems. This overview was notmeant to be exhaustive. The observationalsystems included were specifically chosento provide examples <strong>of</strong> the various domains<strong>of</strong> behavior that observational systems canassess, the different settings in which observationscan be conducted, <strong>and</strong> the variousmethodologies that can be employed. Two<strong>of</strong> the systems are commercially available(ASEBA-DOF/TOF; BASC-2-SOS)<strong>and</strong> cover a broad array <strong>of</strong> behaviors. Theother systems reviewed focus on more narrowlydefined dimensions <strong>of</strong> behavior suchas anxiety, social interaction, aggression, orADHD. Whether or not a clinical assessorchooses to use these specific systems,we feel that concrete examples <strong>of</strong> observationalsystems help to illustrate the uniquecontribution that behavioral observationscan make to an assessment battery.Chapter Summary1. Direct observations <strong>of</strong> children’s <strong>and</strong>adolescents’ behavior are an importantpart <strong>of</strong> an assessment because they providean objective view <strong>of</strong> behavior that isnot filtered through an informant.2. Direct observations are also helpful inassessing environmental contingenciesthat affect a child’s behavior.3. Direct observations also have a number<strong>of</strong> limitations:a. Conducting observations in a waythat provides valuable information is<strong>of</strong>ten an expensive <strong>and</strong> time-consumingprocess.b. Because <strong>of</strong> the cost <strong>of</strong> obtainingobservations, the development <strong>of</strong>observational systems <strong>of</strong>ten hasignored basic psychometric considerationssuch as testing the reliability <strong>of</strong>the system or developing an adequatenormative base.c. Even well-developed observationalsystems are subject to reactivity.That is, persons change their behaviorwhen they are aware <strong>of</strong> beingobserved, which reduces the validity<strong>of</strong> the observations.d. Other factors affecting the validity<strong>of</strong> observational systems include thedifficulty in observing an adequatesample <strong>of</strong> behavior <strong>and</strong> the inabilityto observe internal events.4. The basic components <strong>of</strong> observationalsystems include:a. What-defining target behaviors to beobserved.b. Where-selecting the most appropriatesetting in which to observe thebehaviorc. How-determining how the targetbehaviors will be codedd. Who-determining who shouldobserve the target behaviors5. In defining target behaviors, one mustconsider the level at which behaviorswill be defined <strong>and</strong> then clearly definethe behaviors to be observed.6. Observations conducted in a child’snatural environment have greater ecologicalvalidity but allow less controlover the observational setting thanobservations conducted in a laboratoryor analog setting.7. There are three basic ways in whichbehaviors can be recorded in an observationalsystem:a. Event recording-the number <strong>of</strong> timesa behavior occurred is recorded.


chapter 8 behavioral observations209b. Duration recording-the length<strong>of</strong> time from the initiation tothe desistance <strong>of</strong> a behavior isrecorded.c. Time sampling-behaviors arerecorded as to whether or not theyhave occurred during preset timeintervals.8. Observations can be conducted byoutside observers, people in a child’senvironment (e.g., parents, teachers),or by the child or adolescent himselfor herself.9. The ASEBA-DOF <strong>and</strong> ASEBA-TOFare two observational systems thatcan be used in conjunction with othercomponents <strong>of</strong> the ASEBA system.a. The DOF allows for a direct observation<strong>of</strong> a child’s classroom behaviorduring three to six 10-min observationalperiods.b. The TOF allows for a directobservation <strong>of</strong> a child’s behaviorduring individual st<strong>and</strong>ardizedtesting, <strong>and</strong> it has strong normativedata.c. Both the DOF <strong>and</strong> TOF have evidencesupporting their reliability<strong>and</strong> for differentiating referredfrom nonproblem children.10. The BASC-2-SOS is an observationalsystem used to assess classroom behaviorthat can be integrated with otherassessment components <strong>of</strong> the BASC-2system.a. It specifies 65 target behaviors <strong>and</strong>uses a momentary time-samplingprocedure for observations.b. There is no normative informationnor is there any information on thereliability or validity <strong>of</strong> this observationalsystem.11. BATs allow one to observe a child’sresponse to anxiety-provoking stimuli.12. Observational systems have also beendeveloped to assess peer interactions,parent–child interactions, <strong>and</strong> behaviorsassociated with ADHD.


C h a p t e r 9Peer-Referenced <strong>Assessment</strong>Chapter Questionsl What contributions can peer-referencedtechniques make to clinical assessments<strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents?l What are some ethical concerns in theuse <strong>of</strong> peer-referenced assessments?l What are the different types <strong>of</strong> peer-referencedassessments?l What do sociometric exercises measure<strong>and</strong> why might this be an importantcomponent <strong>of</strong> clinical assessments?l Besides social status, what other areas<strong>of</strong> a child’s behavioral, emotional, <strong>and</strong>social functioning can be assessed bypeer nomination techniques?Peer-referenced assessment strategies areassessment techniques in which a child oradolescent’s social, emotional, or behavioralfunctioning is assessed by obtainingthe perceptions <strong>of</strong> the child’s peers. One <strong>of</strong> themost common types <strong>of</strong> peer-referencedassessment is the sociometric assessment,in which the child’s acceptance in or rejectionby his or her peer group is determined.We discuss sociometric techniques in moredepth later in this chapter. However, sociometricassessment should not be consideredsynonymous with peer-referenced assessment.There are many aspects <strong>of</strong> a child’sadjustment, not just peer social status, thatcan be usefully assessed through the perceptions<strong>of</strong> a child’s peers. A sampling <strong>of</strong>the most common psychological domainssuitable for peer-referenced assessment<strong>and</strong> the different measurement strategiesare the focus <strong>of</strong> this chapter.The main reason for using peer-referencedassessment is that it provides importantinformation that cannot be obtainedfrom other sources. The importance <strong>of</strong> peerperceptions has both an intuitive <strong>and</strong> empiricalbasis. A child or adolescent’s social milieuis considered a major influence on a child’spsychological adjustment in most develop-211P.J. Frick et al., <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Behavior,DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-0641-0_9, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010


212 chapter 9 Peer-Referenced <strong>Assessment</strong>mental theories. Therefore, one <strong>of</strong> the mostdevastating effects <strong>of</strong> a child’s behavioraldisturbance is the effect that it may haveon his or her social environment (Mayeux,Bellmore, & Cillessen, 2007). Also, interventionsthat change many aspects <strong>of</strong> the child’sbehavior may not result in changes in thechild’s peer relationships (Hoza et al., 2005).For these reasons, underst<strong>and</strong>ing how achild is viewed by peers is critical in developinga complete picture <strong>of</strong> a child’s or adolescent’soverall psychological adjustment.Peer-referenced assessment, whether itfocuses specifically on a child’s social relationshipsor indirectly assess a child’s socialmilieu by determining how peers perceivea child’s emotional <strong>and</strong> behavioral functioning,allows for a better underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>of</strong> a child’s social network.The empirical literature supports thistheoretical emphasis on peer relationships.Parker <strong>and</strong> Asher (1987) conducted a metaanalyticreview <strong>of</strong> studies that have testedthe utility <strong>of</strong> peer relationships (primarilyacceptance <strong>and</strong> aggression) in predictinglater outcomes (dropping out <strong>of</strong> school,criminality, <strong>and</strong> psychopathology). Two <strong>of</strong>the major findings <strong>of</strong> this review were thatlow peer acceptance was consistently relatedto dropping out <strong>of</strong> school <strong>and</strong> that peerratedaggression was consistently relatedto delinquency. Similarly, in another study<strong>of</strong> 445 girls who were first studied at ages10 to 13, rejected peer status was relatedto increased risk for criminal <strong>of</strong>fending <strong>and</strong>alcohol abuse almost 40 years later (Zettergren,Bergman, & Wangby, 2006).This literature, therefore, clearly supportsthe importance <strong>of</strong> peer perceptions in predictingthe negative outcomes <strong>of</strong> a child <strong>and</strong>,hence, it illustrates the need to assess <strong>and</strong> tointervene in a child’s social milieu. Box 9.1summarizes several other interesting findingsfrom the Parker <strong>and</strong> Asher’s review that haveimplications for the use <strong>of</strong> peer-referencedtechniques in clinical assessments.The assertion that peer perceptionscannot be obtained by other methodscomes from the meta-analysis conductedby Achenbach, McConaughy, <strong>and</strong> Howell(1987). These authors calculated the averagecorrelations across the studies betweenpeer reports <strong>of</strong> social, emotional, or behaviorfunctioning with the reports <strong>of</strong> teachers<strong>and</strong> with the child’s self-report. Across23 studies reviewed, the average correlationacross all psychological domains was.44 between peer <strong>and</strong> teacher ratings, withthe correlation being somewhat higher forbehavioral (.47) problems than for emotional(.35) problems. Similarly, across 20 studies inwhich the correlation between peer ratings<strong>and</strong> the child’s self-report <strong>of</strong> adjustment wasdetermined, the average correlation was .26,again with the correlation for behavioral difficulties(.44) being somewhat higher thanfor emotional difficulties (.31). These datasuggest that there are substantial differencesbetween how peers rate children <strong>and</strong> howteachers rate children <strong>and</strong> how childrendescribe themselves. Therefore, to underst<strong>and</strong>a child’s peer network that is heavilyinfluenced by peer perceptions, these perceptionsmust be assessed directly.Ethics <strong>of</strong> Peer-ReferencedStrategiesDespite research on the unique <strong>and</strong> importantcontributions that peer-referencedtechniques can make to clinical assessments,these techniques are probably one<strong>of</strong> the least used assessment techniques<strong>of</strong> any reviewed in this book. The failureto include peer-referenced techniques inmany assessment batteries could be, inpart, due to the paucity <strong>of</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardized,well-normed, <strong>and</strong> readily available assessmentprocedures. This exclusion couldalso be due to the time-consuming nature<strong>of</strong> many <strong>of</strong> the peer-referenced techniquesused in the research literature. However,as we discuss later in this chapter, there


chapter 9 Peer-Referenced <strong>Assessment</strong>213Box 9.1Research Note: Meta-Analysis <strong>of</strong> the Association Between Peer Relationships<strong>and</strong> Later Psychological AdjustmentParker <strong>and</strong> Asher (1987) completed a comprehensivemeta-analytic review <strong>of</strong> the predictiveassociation between poor relationshipsin early to middle childhood <strong>and</strong> later (adolescent<strong>and</strong> adult) psychological adjustment.As mentioned in the text, this review clearlysupported the importance <strong>of</strong> a child’s socialcontext in general, <strong>and</strong> peer perceptions <strong>of</strong> achild specifically, in terms <strong>of</strong> predicting lateradjustment.However, in addition to illustrating theoverall importance <strong>of</strong> peer relationships toclinical assessments <strong>of</strong> children, this reviewhad several other interesting results that canguide the assessment process. For example,in terms <strong>of</strong> predictive accuracy, a consistentpattern <strong>of</strong> errors emerged in which peer-referencedprocedures tended to make few falsenegativeerrors in predicting which childrenwould have poor outcomes, but there weremany false-positive errors. That is, most childrenwho have problems later in life had peerrelationship problems. However, a large number<strong>of</strong> children with relationship problems donot show later difficulties. Knowledge <strong>of</strong> thistype <strong>of</strong> predictive relationship can be quitehelpful in interpreting peer-referenced assessments.The authors <strong>of</strong> this review also cautionusers <strong>of</strong> the literature to be aware <strong>of</strong>the fact that, despite knowing that thereis a predictive relationship between earlypeer relations <strong>and</strong> later adjustment, we donot know why this relationship exists. Forexample, it could be that because these childrenare excluded from normal patterns <strong>of</strong>peer interactions, they may also be excludedfrom normal socialization experiences <strong>and</strong>deprived <strong>of</strong> important sources <strong>of</strong> support.However, it is also possible that early forms<strong>of</strong> a pathological process that may emergemore fully in adulthood may have a negativeinfluence on early peer relationships.In essence, peer relationships could be anaccidental by-product <strong>of</strong> a pathological process<strong>and</strong> not really have a causal relationshipwith later adjustment.A final relevant point made by the authors <strong>of</strong>this review is the fact that future research shouldattempt to obtain a more comprehensive picture<strong>of</strong> children’s social relationships. For example,the authors found very limited data onshyness <strong>and</strong> social withdrawal in predicting lateroutcomes, with most studies relying on indices <strong>of</strong>acceptance <strong>and</strong> aggression as the primary aspects<strong>of</strong> social relationship to be studied. Other aspects<strong>of</strong> peer relationships that could be studied systematicallyinclude impulsive/hyperactivebehavior, bossy <strong>and</strong> dem<strong>and</strong>ing behaviors, <strong>and</strong>behaviors that define attributes that approximatehow children choose their friends (e.g., Is thischild fun to play with?).Source: Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1987). Peer relations <strong>and</strong> later personal adjustment: Are low-acceptedchildren at risk? Psychological Bulletin, 102, 357–389.are several relatively time-efficient peerreferencedprocedures that have been usedextensively in research which could add toa clinical assessment battery. Therefore,the low frequency <strong>of</strong> use is probably theresult <strong>of</strong> other considerations.One such consideration could be the intrusiveness<strong>of</strong> peer ratings. Peer-referencedassessment typically involves the use <strong>of</strong>many peer raters, making it more intrusivethan many other assessment techniques.For example, asking a teacher to complete abehavior rating scale adds only one person tothe assessment process. In contrast, havinga child’s class participate in a sociometricexercise involves 15–30 additional peoplein the assessment process.In addition to the sheer number <strong>of</strong>people that must be involved, the peopleinvolved are typically children who may


214 chapter 9 Peer-Referenced <strong>Assessment</strong>not appreciate the need for discretion<strong>and</strong> confidentiality. Therefore, specialprecautions to limit the intrusiveness<strong>of</strong> this intervention are essential. Thereare several necessary precautions forthe use <strong>of</strong> any peer-referenced strategy.First, it is important that both the parent<strong>and</strong> the child being assessed are clearlyinformed <strong>and</strong> give their consent to thepeer-referenced assessment (Gresham &Little, 1993). Second, peer-referencedtechniques should be designed to ensurethat the child’s classmates do not knowthat the assessment is focused on anyone individual (see Box 9.2 for an example<strong>of</strong> instructions provided in a sociometricexercise). Finally, administration <strong>of</strong> peerreferencedtechniques should be carefullymonitored to ensure that answersare not shared, <strong>and</strong> those involved in theassessment should be instructed on theimportance <strong>of</strong> the confidentiality <strong>of</strong> theirresponses after the assessment (McConnell& Odom, 1986).One <strong>of</strong> the most controversial aspects <strong>of</strong>peer-referenced strategies is the use <strong>of</strong> negativeratings from peers (e.g., nominations<strong>of</strong> children who are not liked or who areaggressive). Teachers <strong>and</strong> parents are <strong>of</strong>tenconcerned about the possibility that thesenegative ratings will lead to social rejection<strong>and</strong> other negative reactive effects. Fortunately,over 50 years <strong>of</strong> research usingpeer-referenced strategies has not foundany evidence for these negative effects(McConnell & Odom, 1986). In fact, thepotential for negative effects has been specificallytested (Hayvren & Hymel, 1984;Mayeux, Underwood, & Risser, 2007). Forexample, Mayeux, Underwood, <strong>and</strong> Risser(2007) completed a sociometric exercisewith 91 third graders <strong>and</strong> then interviewedthem <strong>and</strong> their teachers. Their results indicatedthat children were not hurt or upsetby the procedures nor did the participantsfeel that their peers treated them differentlyfollowing the testing.Despite the lack <strong>of</strong> evidence for reactiveeffects <strong>of</strong> negative ratings, it would benice to be able to use only positive ratings.Unfortunately, research has clearly shownthat negative ratings are not simply theopposite <strong>of</strong> positive ratings. Negative ratingsadd crucial additional information tothe assessment. For example, rejected childrenare not simply unaccepted children interms <strong>of</strong> peer status, but they are activelydisliked by their classmates (Coie, Dodge,& Copotelli, 1982). Therefore, one cannotsimply assess peer acceptance <strong>and</strong>then consider those low on acceptance asbeing rejected by peers. As we discuss laterin this chapter, this rejected status, whichrequires negative ratings to assess, is one<strong>of</strong> the most important indices <strong>of</strong> a child’ssocial status. Therefore, negative ratingsappear to be an important part <strong>of</strong> a peerreferencedassessment strategy. However,clinical assessors should be sensitive toconcerns about negative ratings <strong>and</strong> assureparents <strong>and</strong> teachers that appropriatesafe-guards are being used in the assessmentprocedures <strong>and</strong> explain to them thecritical need for this information in theevaluation.Types <strong>of</strong> Peer-ReferencedTechniquesPeer NominationsPeer nominations are the oldest <strong>and</strong> mostcommonly used form <strong>of</strong> peer-referencedassessment (Asher & Hymel, 1981).The procedure involves asking childrenin a classroom to select one or more <strong>of</strong>their peers who display a certain characteristic(e.g., liked, fights, cooperates, shy,leader). Although this procedure is relativelysimple <strong>and</strong> straightforward, thereare several variations <strong>of</strong> peer nominationprocedures. For example, there canbe a predetermined number <strong>of</strong> childrenthat can be selected in each category (fixedchoiceformat). Alternatively, the number<strong>of</strong> peers nominated in each category can


chapter 9 Peer-Referenced <strong>Assessment</strong>215Box 9.2Instructions for Two Peer-Referenced TechniquesAssessor-Administered Exercisewith Unlimited-Choice Format(Carlson, Lahey, & Neeper, 1984)After discussing the importance <strong>of</strong> confidentiality,sheets are provided to each child with the class role<strong>and</strong> each <strong>of</strong> 30 nomination categories.Now we’re ready to begin. We have writtendown lots <strong>of</strong> things kids do, <strong>and</strong> we would likeyou to check which kids in your class do thesethings. Look across the list <strong>of</strong> names alongthe top <strong>of</strong> the page until you find your name.We’ll ask you to tell us about what things youdo later, but for now put a line through yourname <strong>and</strong> draw a line down the column yourname is in so you will remember not to checkthese things for yourself. Go to the other page<strong>and</strong> do this every time you see your name.Now go back to the first page. See the numberone? After the number one, it says, “Thosewho are tall.” Now look across the names. Whois tall? Put an “X” under their name. Who isn’ttall? Put a “0” under their name. Go throughevery name one at a time <strong>and</strong> put an “X” underit if they are tall, <strong>and</strong> a “0” under it if they aren’ttall. Be sure to read every name on the top so youdon’t forget to check anyone. When you finishwith number one, you may turn back to the firstpage <strong>and</strong> wait for the rest <strong>of</strong> the group to finish.Following the completion <strong>of</strong> item one, allsubsequent items were completed in a similarmanner, with each item read aloud.Items1. Those who are tall2. Those who say they can beat everybody up3. Those who complain a lot4. Those who bother people when they are tryingto work5. Those who st<strong>and</strong> back <strong>and</strong> just watch otherswho are playing6. Those who get mad when they don’t gettheir way7. Those who start a fight over nothing8. Those who act like a baby9. Those who do not follow the rules when theyplay games10. Those who tell other children what to do11. Those who try to change the game when theyjoin in12. Those who help others13. Those who speak s<strong>of</strong>tly <strong>and</strong> are difficult tounderst<strong>and</strong>14. Those who do not pay attention whensomeone is talking to them15. Those who daydream a lot16. Those who keep talking even whensomeone is talking to them17. Those who ask a lot <strong>of</strong> questions whenthey join a group18. Those who always talk about themselveswhen they join the group19. Those who do not want your help even if you<strong>of</strong>fer it20. Those who do not know how to join in thegroup21. Those who show <strong>of</strong>f in front <strong>of</strong> the class22. Those who share their things23. Those who give in to others too much24. Those who are afraid to ask for help25. Those who <strong>of</strong>ten change the subject26. Those who are honest27. Those who do not try again when they lose28. Those who can not wait their turn29. Those who never seem to be happy30. Those who let others boss them around a lotTeacher-Administered Exercise withFixed-Choice Format (Strauss et al.,1988)A. Pass out paper to all <strong>of</strong> the children in theclass.(Continues)


216 chapter 9 Peer-Referenced <strong>Assessment</strong>Box 9.2 (Continued)B. Read the following statement to the children:“Class, I want us to do an exercise nowthat will help me learn more about you aspeople <strong>and</strong> about your friendships. It willjust take a couple <strong>of</strong> minutes <strong>and</strong> shouldbe fun, but if anyone does not want to takepart, you may feel free to sit quietly whilethe rest <strong>of</strong> us do the exercise. Because thequestions that I will ask are about privatefeelings, I want to ask you not to discussyour answers with each other <strong>and</strong> not to letyour neighbors see your paper.”C. Then ask the children to list the three childrenin the class that you…1. Like the most2. Like the least3. Think fight the most4. Think are the meanest5. Think are the most shyD. After the exercise is completed, collect thepapers. To protect the feelings <strong>of</strong> the children,you may wish to briefly look through the collectedpapers <strong>and</strong> state, “I haven’t had a chanceto look carefully at these, but it looks like everybodywas named as most liked by at least oneperson. That’s really nice.” Reiterate the needfor the children not to discuss their responseswith each other.E. Count the total number <strong>of</strong> children whoparticipated in the exercise, <strong>and</strong> the number<strong>of</strong> times the child being evaluated wasnamed in response to each <strong>of</strong> the five questions.Please write this information on theback <strong>of</strong> the page <strong>and</strong> mail it in the selfaddressedstamped envelope provided.F. Thank you again for your assistance. If theparent gives us permission to do so, a copy<strong>of</strong> the evaluation results will be sent to you.It is important to note that the fact that thischild is being evaluated does not necessarilymean that he or she has psychological problems;many <strong>of</strong> the children that we evaluateturn out to be perfectly normal.Note: Procedures are provided with permission <strong>of</strong> the authors.be left entirely to the child providing thenominations (unlimited-choice format). Anexample <strong>of</strong> instructions for both a fixedchoice<strong>and</strong> an unlimited-choice formatis provided in Box 9.2. Although someauthors prefer the unlimited-choice formatto avoid forcing a set number <strong>of</strong> childreninto categories, especially negativecategories (McConnell & Odom, 1986),there is little empirical evidence for any clearadvantage <strong>of</strong> one format over the other.A second dimension on which peer nominationprocedures can vary is the degree <strong>of</strong>explicitness that defines the nominatingpool. For example, some procedures simplyinstruct the children to consider any childin their class (Strauss, Lahey, Frick, Frame,& Hynd, 1988). In contrast, other nominationstrategies provide children with a roster<strong>of</strong> names from which to choose (Coie &Kupersmidt, 1983) or may even providepictures <strong>of</strong> classmates from which the raterselects nominees for the individual categories(Moore & Updegraff, 1964). The rule <strong>of</strong>thumb is that the younger the rater, the moreexplicit the definition <strong>of</strong> the nominatingpool should be. Also, if the pool <strong>of</strong> potentialnominees is not within a well-defined group(e.g., only part <strong>of</strong> a class is participating inthe procedure), then more explicit definitionsare required.Because <strong>of</strong> the difficulty <strong>and</strong> level <strong>of</strong>intrusiveness involved in collecting peernominations from entire classrooms,Prinstein (2007) compared two alternativemethods for obtaining peer perceptions.That is, this author compared nominationsobtained from a full sample <strong>of</strong> 232adolescents ages 15–17 years old, withthose obtained (a) using only a r<strong>and</strong>omlyselected subsample <strong>of</strong> 26 students <strong>and</strong> (b)using only two students in each class to


chapter 9 Peer-Referenced <strong>Assessment</strong>217rate other students. These students werechosen by their teachers as “social experts.”The correlations between nominationsobtained in the full classroom <strong>and</strong> thosecompleted by experts were generally quitehigh (r’s ranging from .55 to 93). Similarcorrelations were found between the fullclassroom procedure <strong>and</strong> the nominationscompleted by a r<strong>and</strong>om subsample(r’s ranging from .49 to 90). Thus, theseresults suggest that there may be someless cost intensive alternatives to obtainingpeer nominations, at least in classroomswith adolescent students.The typical level <strong>of</strong> interpretation <strong>of</strong> apeer nomination procedure is the number<strong>of</strong> times a child was nominated in a givencategory. This number is then comparedto a normative base for that particularprocedure to see if the child was nominatedat a level that is atypical for childrenhis or her age. However, there are someinstances where more complex combinationsor adjustments <strong>of</strong> peer nominationsare desired. For some purposes it may beuseful to compare the number <strong>of</strong> nominationsobtained by a child in one area withthe number <strong>of</strong> nominations that the samechild received in another area. For example,in sociometric techniques, one <strong>of</strong>tencompares the number <strong>of</strong> times a child wasnominated as Liked Least with the number<strong>of</strong> times he or she was nominated as LikedMost by classmates. This allows one todetermine the relative balance <strong>of</strong> two nominationcategories. However, to make suchcomparisons, the two scores should first beconverted to st<strong>and</strong>ard scores (e.g., Z-scores;Coie et al., 1982) to equate for possible differencesin the variance <strong>of</strong> the raw scores.A second type <strong>of</strong> conversion is warrantedif one wishes to compare nominations<strong>of</strong> one child with the nominations <strong>of</strong>another child from another nominatingpool (e.g., different class). To make thiscomparison the number <strong>of</strong> nominationsmust be adjusted to equate scores for differingclass sizes. For example, 5 nominations<strong>of</strong> Most Cooperative in a class <strong>of</strong> 12 shouldbe interpreted differently than 5 nominationsin a class <strong>of</strong> 30. As an example <strong>of</strong> thisconversion, Strauss et al. (1988) dividedthe number <strong>of</strong> nominations obtained by achild by the number <strong>of</strong> children participatingin the assessment. The quotient wasmultiplied by 23, so that the nominationswere all expressed in terms <strong>of</strong> a commonclass size <strong>of</strong> 23.SociometricsSociometric techniques focus on a specific,important aspect <strong>of</strong> a child’s peer relationships:a child’s social status. It answers thequestion <strong>of</strong> whether or not a child is liked<strong>and</strong> accepted by his or her peer group.Sociometrics do not assess specific behaviors<strong>of</strong> the child. It answers the question<strong>of</strong> whether the child is liked <strong>and</strong> not whatis the child like or why the child is liked(Asher & Hymel, 1981). Sociometric exerciseshave appeared in the research literaturesince the 1930s (see Gresham & Little,1993; Hughes, 1990), <strong>and</strong> the most commonlyused procedure has changed verylittle over this time. An example <strong>of</strong> thisbasic technique from Strauss et al. (1988)is provided in Box 9.2.Sociometric exercises can take the form<strong>of</strong> peer ratings, whereby peers rate a childon a Likert scale as to how well liked or dislikedhe or she is (Hamilton, Fuchs, Fuchs,& Roberts, 2000). However, the more commonmethod <strong>of</strong> obtaining sociometrics isthrough peer nominations. In this technique,children can be nominated by peersas a child who is Liked Most (sometimesdefined as Most like to have as a best friendor Most like to play with) <strong>and</strong>/or they can benominated by peers as a child who is LikedLeast (or alternatively, Least like to have as afriend or Least like to play with). Althoughthere is no definitive normative study thatspecifies exact cut-<strong>of</strong>fs for when nominationsare considered indicative <strong>of</strong> problems,


218 chapter 9 Peer-Referenced <strong>Assessment</strong>in a fixed response format allowing threenominations in each category in a class size<strong>of</strong> approximately 20 students, Liked Most(LM) nominations <strong>of</strong> less than two <strong>and</strong>nominations <strong>of</strong> Liked Least (LL) <strong>of</strong> greaterthan four are generally considered indicative<strong>of</strong> problems in peer relations (Dodge, Coie,& Brakke, 1982; Green, Vosk, Foreh<strong>and</strong>, &Beck, 1981; Strauss et al., 1988).A common way <strong>of</strong> interpreting sociometricnominations is by combining theLM <strong>and</strong> LL nominations into five distinctsocial status groups (Hughes, 1990). Asmentioned previously, when combiningLM <strong>and</strong> LL nominations, the nominationsshould first be converted to st<strong>and</strong>ardscores to equate for potential differencesin the variance <strong>of</strong> the two categories. Thefive groups are based on two differencescores. The social preference score is thedifference between LM <strong>and</strong> LL scores(LM − LL = Social Preference). High socialpreference scores indicate substantiallymore LM nominations than LL nominations.The social impact score is the sum <strong>of</strong>the LM <strong>and</strong> LL scores (LM + LL = SocialImpact). High social impact scores simplydetermine the number <strong>of</strong> nominations achild receives, regardless <strong>of</strong> whether theyare negative or positive. A combination <strong>of</strong>these scores leads to a child being consideredin one <strong>of</strong> several social status groups:Popular, Rejected, Neglected, Controversial,<strong>and</strong> Average. The method <strong>of</strong> combiningthese scores to determine a child’s socialstatus <strong>and</strong> two computational formulas thathave been used in research are provided inBox 9.3.Although there have been several variationsin the formulas for determining aBox 9.3Determining Social Status from Sociometric NominationsCategoryDescriptionComputational FormulaUsing St<strong>and</strong>ard Scores(Coie, Dodge,& Coppotelli, 1982)ComputationalFormula Using RawScores (Strausset al., 1988)PopularHigh social preferencescores (LM − LL) but fewLL nominationsLow social preferencescores with few LMnominationsLow social impact scores(LM + LL) <strong>and</strong> few nominationsin LM categoryHigh social impact scores<strong>and</strong> above-average LM<strong>and</strong> LL scoresAverage social preferencescores(1) ZLM − ZLL > 1(2) ZLM > 0(3) ZLL < 0(1) ZLM − ZLL < −1(2) ZLM < 0(3) ZLL > 0(1) ZLM + ZLL < −1(2) LM = 0(1). LM > 4.5(2) LL < 1.5Rejected(1) LM < 1(2) LL > 4.5Neglected(1) LM < 1.5(2) LL < 1.5Controversial(1) ZLM + ZLL > 1(2) ZLM > 0(3) ZLL > 0(1) ZLM − ZLL > −.5(2) ZLM − ZLL < .5AverageNote: .Both computational formulas are based on a fixed-choice format allowing for three nominations in boththe Like Most (LM) <strong>and</strong> Like Least (LL) categories. ZLM refers to LM nominations converted to st<strong>and</strong>ardZ-scores <strong>and</strong> ZLL refers to LL nominations converted to st<strong>and</strong>ard Z-scores. The unst<strong>and</strong>ardized formulas arebased on scores adjusted to a class size <strong>of</strong> 23.


chapter 9 Peer-Referenced <strong>Assessment</strong>219child’s social status, the validity <strong>of</strong> thesegroupings has been consistently shown inresearch (see Gresham & Little, 1993),including showing good convergence withstatistical methods for clustering childrenbased on peer nominations (Zettergren,2007). Further research has suggested thatthese nominations are quite stable overtime. Jiang <strong>and</strong> Cillessen (2005) conducteda meta-analysis <strong>of</strong> 77 studies <strong>of</strong> over 18,339participants <strong>and</strong> reported that the averagelevel <strong>of</strong> stability for social status over periods<strong>of</strong> less than 3 months ranged from r =.70 to r = .80. For periods <strong>of</strong> over 3 months,the average ratings ranged from .52 to .58.Importantly, research has suggested thatsociometric nominations can be influencedby the racial composition <strong>of</strong> a classroomwith social preference scores being higherwhen nominations are obtained from sameracepeers (Singleton & Asher, 1979). Jackson,Barth, Powell, <strong>and</strong> Lochman (2006)reported on 1,268 sociometric nominationsfrom children ages 9 to 11 years old across57 classrooms that ranged from 3 to 95%African-American. The results indicatedthat African-American students nominationswere more sensitive to the racial composition<strong>of</strong> the classroom than for Caucasian students.Specifically, African-American students’social preference scores <strong>and</strong> nominations forfighting <strong>and</strong> being a leader improved as thepercentage <strong>of</strong> African-American studentsin the classroom increased. The effects forCaucasian students was less clear, althoughthere was a small effect <strong>of</strong> Caucasian studentshaving lower preference scores if theclassroom was less than 33% Caucasian.Social status has also been associatedwith emotional <strong>and</strong> behavioral characteristics<strong>of</strong> the child. One <strong>of</strong> the most consistentfindings is that rejected children showhigher levels <strong>of</strong> aggressive <strong>and</strong> acting-outbehavior than nonrejected classmates(e.g., Dodge, 1983). However, neglectedstatus is also associated with problems inadjustment, most notably with anxiety(Strauss et al., 1988). Several behavioralcharacteristics are associated with popularchildren, including being more likely tocontribute to conversations during play,being more likely to engage in parallelplay, receiving <strong>and</strong> initiating more positivesocial behavior, <strong>and</strong> using effective peerentrystrategies (Gresham & Little, 1993).<strong>Child</strong>ren in the controversial status grouphave been less well studied. However, onestudy suggests that children in this socialstatus group tend to exhibit aggressive <strong>and</strong>disruptive behaviors, like the rejected children,but also tend to be viewed as sociallyskilled <strong>and</strong> as leaders, like the popularchildren (Coie et al., 1982).A distinction that research has increasinglyshown to be important is betweenwhether a child is well liked by their peers(i.e., accepted) <strong>and</strong> whether the child isviewed as “popular” (Prinstein, 2007). First,these ratings have only been modestly correlatedwith each other (Prinstein, 2007; Vaillancourt& Hymal, 2006). Second, ratings<strong>of</strong> greater peer acceptance have consistentlybeen related to more positive behavioral(e.g., less aggression; more prosocial behavior)<strong>and</strong> emotional (e.g., higher self-esteem)functioning (Gresham & Little, 1993; S<strong>and</strong>strom& Cillessen, 2006). However, childrenperceived by their peers as popular <strong>of</strong>tenshow more physical <strong>and</strong> relational forms <strong>of</strong>aggression (McDonald, Putallaz, Grimes,Kupersmidt, & Coie, 2007; Vaillancourt& Hymel, 2006). Third, peer popularityseems to be more strongly related to peervaluedcharacteristics such as power, physicalattractiveness, athleticism, <strong>and</strong> dressthan peer acceptance (Prinstein & Cillessen,2003; Vaillancourt & Hymel, 2006).From this research, it is clear that achild’s social status is intertwined with hisor her behavioral <strong>and</strong> emotional functioning,both in terms <strong>of</strong> current <strong>and</strong> futurefunctioning. Therefore, sociometrics cancontribute important information to manyclinical assessments by providing a reliablemethod <strong>of</strong> assessing a crucial aspect <strong>of</strong> achild’s social functioning.


220 chapter 9 Peer-Referenced <strong>Assessment</strong>Most sociometric exercises are conductedin school classrooms because theyprovide a very clear <strong>and</strong> well-defined referencegroup <strong>of</strong> peers from which to judgea child’s social acceptance or rejection.However, an important consideration inthe usefulness <strong>of</strong> data obtained from sociometricexercises conducted in classrooms is thelevel <strong>of</strong> student participation in the exercise.That is, there is evidence that evenmoderate declines in full classroom participationin the sociometric exercise canhave a major influence on the results. Forexample, Hamilton et al. (2000) comparedsociometric results using a peer-rating procedureacross varying levels <strong>of</strong> classroomparticipation. These authors reported that,even with a 75% rate <strong>of</strong> classroom participation,there were substantial differencesin the results compared to those obtainedfrom the full classroom. The results for25 <strong>and</strong> 50% participation rates were evenmore divergent from those obtained withfull participation. Importantly, the instabilityin the results across the varying levels<strong>of</strong> participation was greatest for childrenwith adjustment problems compared towell-adjusted children. Therefore, for childrenwith problems, who are <strong>of</strong>ten <strong>of</strong> mostinterest in sociometric exercises, participationrates seem to be especially importantfor interpreting the results. These findingshighlight the need to interpret informationfrom sociometric techniques in the context<strong>of</strong> the level <strong>of</strong> classroom participation inthe exercise. In Box 9.4 additional findingsfrom the Hamilton et al. (2000) study thathave potentially important implications forinterpreting information from sociometricexercises are summarized.Box 9.4Research Note: A Comparison <strong>of</strong> Sociometric Results Across Varying Levels<strong>of</strong> Classroom ParticipationHamilton et al. (2000) investigated the effects<strong>of</strong> different rates <strong>of</strong> classroom participationon peer ratings <strong>of</strong> social acceptance. Theseauthors reviewed 26 studies using sociometricratings to assess the social acceptance <strong>of</strong> childrenwith learning or behavioral disorders <strong>and</strong>found that the vast majority <strong>of</strong> studies did notreport the rate <strong>of</strong> participation in the sociometricexercise <strong>and</strong>, for those studies that did, therates varied from 67 to 100%. The potentialeffects <strong>of</strong> these varying rates was investigatedin 14 classrooms (grades 3 through 6) with full,or nearly full, rates <strong>of</strong> participation (i.e., 92 to100%). The authors used a group sociometricprocedure in which each student was providedwith a class roster. Each student’s name wasaccompanied by four circles closing (1) a smilingface to indicate that the student is liked, (2)a straight-mouthed face to designate that therater is indifferent to the student, (3) a frowningface to indicate that the student is disliked, <strong>and</strong>(4) a question mark to indicate that the rater isunsure <strong>of</strong> the student’s likability. Each studentreceived a percentage score for each <strong>of</strong> the fourcategories by dividing the number <strong>of</strong> responses(e.g., smiles) by the number <strong>of</strong> student ratersminus one, thereby creating percentage scoresfor each type <strong>of</strong> acceptance rating.To assess the effects <strong>of</strong> classroom participationrates, the classrooms were then sampledr<strong>and</strong>omly <strong>and</strong> repeatedly three times for eachlevel) so that 25, 50, <strong>and</strong> 75% <strong>of</strong> studentswere involved in the exercise. At each participationlevel, peer ratings were comparedagainst those obtained at the full level <strong>of</strong>participation. The results were quite consistentindicating that, as the rate <strong>of</strong> classroomparticipation decreased, ratings tended to beincreasingly divergent from those obtained atfull participation. Even the ratings at the 75%level were significantly different from theratings obtained at full participation (rangingfrom 3 to 18%). Importantly, the authorscompared the ratings <strong>of</strong> students with learning(Continues)


chapter 9 Peer-Referenced <strong>Assessment</strong>221Box 9.4 (Continued)disabilities <strong>and</strong> nondisabled students that wereunderachieving to average <strong>and</strong> high-achievingstudents. The decline in participation rateswas especially problematic for the ratings <strong>of</strong>the two groups with learning problems.The authors noted that the effects <strong>of</strong> classroomparticipation on this peer-rating proceduremay not generalize to the more commonpeer nomination procedure, although theymake the important point that participationeffects on nomination procedures need tobe tested in light <strong>of</strong> their results. They also<strong>of</strong>fer an interesting explanation for why participationrates may affect the ratings <strong>of</strong> lesswell-adjusted children to a greater degree.The authors suggest that the better-adjustedchildren may be viewed more similarly acrossclassmates, with a general consensus across studentson their likeability. In contrast, studentsmay have more “polarized” views <strong>of</strong> childrenwith problems in adjustment, with some classmatesliking them, others disliking them, <strong>and</strong>still others having a neutral view. As a result <strong>of</strong>this polarization, the ratings <strong>of</strong> these childrenmay be more dependent on which childrenare participating in the sociometric exercise.In actual practice, the problems in the accuracyin the peer ratings <strong>of</strong> disturbed childrenat lower levels <strong>of</strong> participation may be exacerbatedif these children tend to have disturbedfriends who may be less likely to volunteer toparticipate in the exercise. The authors concludeby noting that it is impossible to identifya specific “minimum rate” <strong>of</strong> participationthat should be obtained before the results <strong>of</strong>a peer-rating sociometric exercise are uninterpretable.However, they suggest that thesedata clearly indicate that assessors using sociometricratings need to recognize the potentialeffect <strong>of</strong> participation rates on the results <strong>of</strong>these exercises, especially when assessing childrenwith adjustment problems.AggressionAnother common aspect <strong>of</strong> a child’s functioningthat is assessed by peer nominationsis aggression. The typical format is to havea class nominate the children in the classwho “Fights most.” As with other nominationtechniques, the format can either be ina fixed-choice or unlimited-choice format.Peer nominations <strong>of</strong> aggression have beenshown to be correlated with a psychiatricdiagnosis <strong>of</strong> conduct disorder <strong>and</strong> thereforecan be considered an important indicator <strong>of</strong>the impairment associated with this syndrome(Walker et al., 1991). However, one <strong>of</strong> themost interesting <strong>and</strong> troubling characteristics<strong>of</strong> peer-nominated aggression is its stability.In their 5-year study, Coie et al., <strong>and</strong> Dodge(1983) found that peer nominations <strong>of</strong> “StartsFights” showed the most stability across the5years <strong>of</strong> any <strong>of</strong> the peer-nomination categoriesthat were obtained, exhibiting correlations<strong>of</strong> .83 between third <strong>and</strong> fourth grades <strong>and</strong>.84 across fifth <strong>and</strong> sixth grades. Huesmann,Eron, Lefkowitz, & Walder (1984) provideeven more dramatic evidence for the stability<strong>of</strong> peer nominations <strong>of</strong> aggression.These authors found that peer nominations<strong>of</strong> aggression at age 8 significantly predictedaggression 30 years later. Therefore, peernominations <strong>of</strong> aggression assess an aspect <strong>of</strong>problematic interpersonal functioning thatcan be highly stable for a child <strong>and</strong> is thus animportant target for intervention.Hyperactivity<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> inattention <strong>and</strong> motor hyperactivity,behaviors typically associated withADHD, has relied primarily on information


222 chapter 9 Peer-Referenced <strong>Assessment</strong>obtained from parents <strong>and</strong> teachers (Loeber,Green, Lahey, & Stouthamer-Loeber,1991). However, Schaughency <strong>and</strong> Rothlind(1991) provide some interesting datato suggest that peer nominations <strong>of</strong> inattentive<strong>and</strong> hyperactive behavior could aidin the assessment <strong>of</strong> ADHD. Specifically,these authors reported that peer nominations<strong>of</strong> “Can’t pay attention,” “Can’t waitturn,” <strong>and</strong> “Can’t sit still” correlated withteacher <strong>and</strong> observer measures <strong>of</strong> inattention<strong>and</strong> hyperactivity. In a second study,these authors reported that peer nominations<strong>of</strong> “Doesn’t pay attention” <strong>and</strong> “Can’tsit still” significantly discriminated betweenyoungsters diagnosed with ADHD fromother clinic-referred children.Although these results are promising<strong>and</strong> suggest that peer nominations can aidin the assessment <strong>of</strong> ADHD behaviors,there is no evidence that these peer nominationsshould take the place <strong>of</strong> parent <strong>and</strong>teacher ratings as a primary informationsource for these behavioral domains. Also,it is unclear whether or not peer nominationsadd anything to the assessment <strong>of</strong> ADHDbehaviors over the information providedby other assessment techniques.DepressionLefkowitz <strong>and</strong> Tesiny (1980) developed thePeer Nomination Inventory <strong>of</strong> Depression(PNID) to aid in the assessment <strong>of</strong> childhooddepression. A list <strong>of</strong> 13 depressionrelatedcategories was developed by nineexpert judges. These nomination categoriesare provided in Box 9.5. Lefkowitz <strong>and</strong>Tesiny found that the 2-month test-retestreliability <strong>of</strong> the individual depressionitems (mean r = .66) <strong>and</strong> the depressioncomposite for all 13 items (r = .79) wereacceptable. More importantly, the PNIDDepression composite was significantlyassociated with teacher (r = .41) <strong>and</strong> child(r = .23) ratings <strong>of</strong> depression. As was thecase with peer nominations <strong>of</strong> inattentionBox 9.5Depression Items on the Peer Nomination Inventory <strong>of</strong> DepressionProcedures<strong>Child</strong>ren were provided a class roster. Eachitem was read aloud twice, <strong>and</strong> children wereinstructed to draw a line through all the nameson their class roster “which best fit the question”(Lefkowitz & Tesiny, 1980, p. 45).Self-nominations were not permitted, butchildren could choose not to nominate anyonein a category.Depression ItemsWho <strong>of</strong>ten plays alone?Who thinks they are bad?Who doesn’t try again when they lose?Who <strong>of</strong>ten sleeps in class?Who <strong>of</strong>ten looks lonely?Who <strong>of</strong>ten says they don’t feel well?Who says they can’t do things?Who <strong>of</strong>ten cries?Who does not play?Who does not take part in things?Who does not have much fun?Who thinks others do not like them?Who <strong>of</strong>ten looks sad?The 13 items are only the depression itemson the PNID. The PNID includes 20 items,with additional items measuring happiness<strong>and</strong> popularity.Source: Lefsowitz, M. M., & Testiny, E. P. (1980). <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> childhood depression. Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong><strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 48, 43–50.


chapter 9 Peer-Referenced <strong>Assessment</strong>223<strong>and</strong> hyperactivity, peer assessment <strong>of</strong>depression is promising as a componentto a comprehensive evaluation but has notbeen sufficiently tested to determine itscontribution relative to more traditionalmeasures <strong>of</strong> depression.Other Peer-ReferencedTechniquesAlthough we have focused most <strong>of</strong> our discussionon peer nomination techniques, thereare other peer-referenced assessment strategiesthat have been used in research that maybe applicable to some clinical settings. Peerratings require children to rate on a Likerttypescale each member <strong>of</strong> their class or peergroup (Gresham & Little, 1993; McConnell& Odom, 1986). Like any rating scale, peerscales vary on the behavioral dimensionsincluded on the scale, the number <strong>of</strong> pointson the scale, <strong>and</strong> the behavioral descriptionsused as anchors on the scale. For example, arating scale for young children used a happyface to anchor the positive end <strong>of</strong> the continuum,a neutral face to anchor the middle,<strong>and</strong> a sad face to anchor the negative end <strong>of</strong>the continuum (Asher, Singleton, Tinsley, &Hymel, 1979).One <strong>of</strong> the most reliable forms <strong>of</strong> peerassessment, especially for very young childrenMcConnell & Odom, 1986), is thepaired comparison technique. In this procedure,photographs are taken <strong>of</strong> all thechildren in the class <strong>and</strong> photographs <strong>of</strong>each possible classmate dyad are paired.The rater is then asked to choose betweenthe two children in each dyad in referenceto some criterion (e.g., Fights, Liked,Shy, Cooperative, etc.). The salient cuesused in making the choices between peersmake this procedure much more reliablefor young children, especially <strong>of</strong> preschoolage. However, it is so labor-intensive thatit is not feasible for use in most clinicalassessments. As McConnell <strong>and</strong> Odom(1986) point out, for a class <strong>of</strong> 20 children,each child will have to make 171 selectionsfor each criterion.ConclusionsPeer-referenced assessment strategies sharethe common characteristic <strong>of</strong> having a childor adolescent evaluated on important psychologicaldimensions by his or her peers.Due to several practical <strong>and</strong> ethical considerations,peer-referenced strategies are notcommonplace in many clinical assessments.This is unfortunate because, if designedappropriately <strong>and</strong> with precautions taken toensure safe administration, peer-referencedassessment can provide an invaluable picture<strong>of</strong> child’s social context. This picture <strong>of</strong>a child’s social relationships is essential fortreatment planning given the importance<strong>of</strong> social relationships for a child’s current<strong>and</strong> future psychological adjustment.One <strong>of</strong> the most commonly used peerreferencedtechniques is the sociometricexercise. This peer nomination techniqueallows one to determine whether or nota child is accepted, rejected, or ignored(neglected) by his or her peer group. Thesedimensions <strong>of</strong> social status cannot be adequatelyassessed by other methods <strong>of</strong> assessment,such as teacher ratings or a child’sself-report. In addition, this crucial aspect<strong>of</strong> a child’s social context has been highlyrelated to emotional <strong>and</strong> behavioral disturbances.Therefore, sociometric assessmentis a type <strong>of</strong> peer-referenced assessment thatcould be an especially important component<strong>of</strong> many clinical evaluations.Chapter Summary1. Peer-referenced assessments provideinformation on how a child or adolescentis viewed by his or her peers <strong>and</strong>,thereby, provide important insights intoa child’s social milieu.


224 chapter 9 Peer-Referenced <strong>Assessment</strong>2. Peer-referenced assessments mustbe conducted in light <strong>of</strong> two importantethical issues: the importance <strong>of</strong>minimizing the intrusiveness <strong>of</strong> theassessment <strong>and</strong> the need to minimizepotential reactive effects <strong>of</strong> negativeratings by peers.3. Peer nominations are the most commonlyused forms <strong>of</strong> peer-referenced assessments.They involve having a child’s oradolescent’s peers select one or more childrenwho display certain characteristics.4. Sociometric exercises are a type <strong>of</strong> peernomination that determines whether achild is accepted, rejected, or neglected byhis or her peers.5. Aggression, hyperactivity, <strong>and</strong> depressionhave also been assessed through peernomination procedures. Unfortunately,the relative utility <strong>of</strong> peer-referencedassessments <strong>of</strong> these psychological dimensions,in comparison to other assessmentmodalities, has not been tested.6. In addition to peer nominations, perceptions<strong>of</strong> a child’s peers can be obtainedby having children rate each other alongcertain dimensions on a Likert-typescale.


C h a p t e r 1 0Projective TechniquesChapter Questionsl What are some <strong>of</strong> the key issues in thedebate over whether <strong>and</strong> how projectivesshould be used in clinical assessments?l What are some <strong>of</strong> the strengths <strong>and</strong>limitations <strong>of</strong> the clinical <strong>and</strong> psychometricapproaches to interpretation <strong>of</strong>projectives?l How would viewing projective techniquesfrom either a traditional projectionapproach or as a behavioral sampleinfluence the type <strong>of</strong> technique thatwould be used <strong>and</strong> the interpretationsthat would be made?l What are the basic interpretive strategiesfor inkblot techniques, thematic techniques,sentence-completion techniques,<strong>and</strong> projective drawing techniques?l What are some specific examples <strong>of</strong>administration, scoring, <strong>and</strong> interpretivesystems for each type <strong>of</strong> projective technique?The ControversySurrounding ProjectiveTechniquesNo type <strong>of</strong> assessment has engenderedas much controversy as projective techniques.For some, projectives are synonymouswith personality testing <strong>and</strong>provide some <strong>of</strong> the richest sources <strong>of</strong>clinical information on children <strong>and</strong>adolescents (Hughes, Gacono, & Owen,2007; Rabin, 1986; Weiner, 1986). Forothers, projective techniques typically225P.J. Frick et al., <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Behavior,DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-0641-0_10, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010


226 chapter 10 projective techniquesdo not meet even the minimum <strong>of</strong> basicpsychometric st<strong>and</strong>ards, <strong>and</strong> their use,therefore, detracts from the assessmentprocess <strong>and</strong> tarnishes the image thatpsychological testing has with other pr<strong>of</strong>essionals<strong>and</strong> with the general public(Anastasi, 1988; Gittelman-Klein, 1986;Hunsley & Bailey, 2001). In Box 10.1,we have attempted to summarize some <strong>of</strong>the major arguments made on either side<strong>of</strong> this debate.Our philosophy in writing this chapterwas not to espouse either <strong>of</strong> the strong viewson projective testing. Instead, our goal wasto provide the reader with an overview <strong>of</strong>this method <strong>of</strong> assessment that would allowfor an informed view <strong>of</strong> the appropriate role<strong>of</strong> projective techniques in clinical assessments.Too <strong>of</strong>ten in the past the debate overprojectives has focused on ideological arguments,or even on personal beliefs, withouta critical <strong>and</strong> scholarly examination <strong>of</strong> theactual issues involved. Therefore, the firstpart <strong>of</strong> this chapter focuses on what we feelare the major issues in the use <strong>of</strong> projectivesthat determine whether they should beused <strong>and</strong> how they should be used in clinicalassessments.Irrespective <strong>of</strong> one’s eventual st<strong>and</strong> onthe projective controversy, projective techniquesremain one <strong>of</strong> the most commonlyused methods <strong>of</strong> clinical assessment by psychologistsin general (Watkins, Campbell,Neiberding, & Hallmark, 1995) <strong>and</strong> by childpsychologists specifically (Hojnoski, Morrison,Brown, & Matthews, 2006). This fact isBox 10.1The Projective DebateProLess structured format allows clinician greaterflexibility in administration <strong>and</strong> interpretation<strong>and</strong> places fewer dem<strong>and</strong> characteristics thatwould prompt socially desirable responsesfrom an informant.Allows for the assessment <strong>of</strong> drives, motivations,desires, <strong>and</strong> conflicts that can affecta person’s perceptual experiences but are<strong>of</strong>ten unconscious.Provides a deeper underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> a personthan would be obtained by simply describingbehavioral patterns.Adds to an overall assessment picture.Helps to generate hypotheses regarding a person’sfunctioning.Non-threatening <strong>and</strong> good for rapportbuilding.Many techniques have a long <strong>and</strong> rich clinicaltradition.ConThe reliability <strong>of</strong> many techniques is questionable.As a result, the interpretations are morerelated to characteristics <strong>of</strong> the clinician thanto characteristics <strong>of</strong> the person being tested.Even some techniques that have good reliabilityhave questionable validity, especiallyin making diagnoses <strong>and</strong> predicting overtbehavior.Although we can at times predict things wecannot underst<strong>and</strong>, it is rarely the case thatunderst<strong>and</strong>ing does not enhance prediction(Gittelman-Klein, 1986).Adding an unreliable piece <strong>of</strong> information toan assessment battery simply decreases theoverall reliability <strong>of</strong> the battery.Leads one to pursue erroneous avenues in testingor to place undue confidence in a finding.Detracts from the time an assessor couldbetter spend collecting more detailed, objectiveinformation.<strong>Assessment</strong> techniques are based on an evolvingknowledge base <strong>and</strong> must continuallyevolve to reflect this knowledge.


chapter 10 projective techniques227not cited to defend their use. It is cited simplyto indicate that projective techniques area firmly entrenched part <strong>of</strong> the clinical assessmentprocess that shows no signs <strong>of</strong> changingin the near future.<strong>Clinical</strong> Techniqueor Psychometric Test?Much <strong>of</strong> the debate over the use <strong>of</strong> projectivetechniques comes from a confusion asto the most appropriate criteria with whichto judge the usefulness <strong>of</strong> projectives. Traditionalmethods <strong>of</strong> evaluating psychologicaltests are grounded in measurementtheory, which, as was discussed in Chap. 2,relies primarily on indexes <strong>of</strong> the reliability<strong>and</strong> validity <strong>of</strong> the scores that result fromthe test (Anastasi, 1988). When evaluatedon these terms, most projective techniqueshave not fared well (Hunsley & Bailey,2001). As Rabin (1986) states:“An aspect <strong>of</strong> projective tests that is not tobe overlooked is the frequent disappointment<strong>and</strong> disaffection with the adequacy,reliability, <strong>and</strong> validity <strong>of</strong> several projectivemethods. The psychologist, reared in the atmosphere<strong>of</strong> respect for science <strong>and</strong> for thepsychometric purity <strong>of</strong> his instruments, <strong>of</strong>tenfinds them wanting” (p. 8).One way in which these criticisms have beenaddressed has been through the development<strong>of</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardized administration, scoring,<strong>and</strong> interpretive procedures for certainprojective techniques which are designedto provide scores that meet traditional psychometricst<strong>and</strong>ards (Weiner, 2001). Twoexamples <strong>of</strong> such approaches that are frequentlyused for testing children <strong>and</strong> adolescentsare the Rorschach ComprehensiveSystem (Exner, 1974) <strong>and</strong> the RobertsApperception Test for <strong>Child</strong>ren (McArthur& Roberts, 1982). Both <strong>of</strong> these approachesto projective testing are discussed in moredetail later in this chapter. However, it isimportant to note that both systems sharethe goal <strong>of</strong> providing very clear <strong>and</strong> explicitguidelines on how the tests are to be given,scored, <strong>and</strong> interpreted. Such st<strong>and</strong>ardizationis a prerequisite to further psychometricevaluation.This method <strong>of</strong> addressing the criticisms<strong>of</strong> projective tests has not met with unanimousapproval. Instead, it has been arguedthat projective tests should not be evaluatedby traditional measurement theory<strong>and</strong> that any attempt at st<strong>and</strong>ardization willlimit the clinical utility <strong>of</strong> the technique.For example, Haak (1990) has argued that:“The problem with all <strong>of</strong> these st<strong>and</strong>ardizationefforts is the amount <strong>of</strong> destructionthey wreak on the essential nature <strong>of</strong> projectives.All such approaches result in a hugeloss <strong>of</strong> the rich <strong>and</strong> complex information thatis obtained by using the technique in the firstplace” (p. 149).This argument is based on the contentionthat projectives are part <strong>of</strong> the olderclinical tradition that seeks to describe theindividual person in depth, capturing all <strong>of</strong>his or her unique dispositions, motivations,conflicts, <strong>and</strong> desires. This is an idiographicapproach that is not concerned with howthe individual differs from the norm orhow his or her scores compare to those<strong>of</strong> some other reference group (e.g., thosewith diagnoses <strong>of</strong> depression). Instead, thegoal is simply to underst<strong>and</strong> the person’sunique qualities. In this conceptualization,“validity” takes on a very different meaningthan the one that is typically used inmeasurement theory. One is not concernedwith how a score compares to some objectivecriterion outside the person beingtested.For some psychologists this clinicalapproach might seem unscientific. However,all clinicians rely on intuition at somepoint in an evaluation to underst<strong>and</strong> thenuances <strong>of</strong> an individual case. Our science<strong>of</strong> human behavior is not at a point


228 chapter 10 projective techniqueswhere every clinical decision can be guidedby well-established principles, <strong>and</strong>, giventhe complexity <strong>of</strong> psychological functioning,such pure empiricism may never bepossible. Therefore, the clinical view <strong>of</strong>projectives considers these techniques asa structured way <strong>of</strong> obtaining these intuitions.By using this structure, one can usethe judgments <strong>of</strong> other experienced cliniciansas a guide to making interpretations.The importance <strong>of</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>ing thisdebate is not to decide which view is right.What is more crucial is for one to recognizethe two disparate ways <strong>of</strong> using projectivetechniques <strong>and</strong> the unique strengths<strong>and</strong> weaknesses <strong>of</strong> both. For example,using projectives as a psychometric techniqueallows one to compare a person’sscore with those from a normative group,or with those from some relevant clinicgroup, or with some other clinically importantcriterion (e.g., response to treatment).However, to use the scores in this way, onemust maintain rigorous st<strong>and</strong>ardization inprocedures <strong>and</strong> be willing to live within theconfines <strong>of</strong> the data that are available. Afrustrating aspect <strong>of</strong> clinical assessments isrealizing the limitations <strong>of</strong> what our assessmentscan provide.On the other h<strong>and</strong>, using projectives asa clinical tool allows one greater flexibilityin administration <strong>and</strong> interpretation.However, with this flexibility, the interpretationsthat result from the assessmentare much more susceptible to influencesthat are indiosyncratic to the assessor.Interpretations <strong>of</strong> the same case materialmay vary widely across clinicians. As such,interpretations should be clearly viewed asclinical impressions <strong>and</strong> not be evaluated inthe same way as empirically derived interpretations.In Box 10.2 we have provideda more detailed discussion <strong>of</strong> the importance<strong>of</strong> clearly defining one’s approachto projective assessment <strong>and</strong> then recognizingthe limitations inherent in eithermethod.Projection or BehavioralSample?Even more basic than the debate over themethod <strong>of</strong> interpretation is confusion overwhat psychological processes projectivetechniques are supposed to measure. Thecritical nature <strong>of</strong> this question is obviousfrom a psychometric viewpoint. Validity isthe critical property <strong>of</strong> a test <strong>and</strong> it is <strong>of</strong>tendefined as evidence that the test is measuringwhat it is supposed to measure (Anastasi,1988). Therefore, if it is unclear what a test issupposed to measure then it will be unclearas to what are the most appropriate methods<strong>of</strong> determining its validity.One dominant view <strong>of</strong> projective tests,which is the view that led to the name projective,is best described by Murray (1943):“There is the tendency for people to interpretan ambiguous human situation inconformity with their past experiences <strong>and</strong>present wants” (p. 1). This forms the basis<strong>of</strong> the projective hypothesis. The projectivehypothesis rests on the assumption thatpeople, in the absence <strong>of</strong> clear environmentaldem<strong>and</strong>s, will project basic aspects<strong>of</strong> themselves in their interpretations <strong>of</strong>environmental stimuli. Freudian theory,which dominated clinical psychology fordecades, heavily emphasized unconsciousconflict as the basic element <strong>of</strong> humanpersonality. Projection is seen by manyas being a window to these unconsciousdynamics (Rabin, 1986).However, there is a second view <strong>of</strong> projectives.Rather than seeing them as windowsto hidden or unconscious motives<strong>and</strong> drives, many assessors view projectivesas a behavioral sample. For example, Kn<strong>of</strong>f(1983) writes:“A student completes an incomplete sentenceblank with ‘I hate myself’ or ‘My father beatsme up all the time,’ <strong>and</strong> these hypotheses areconfirmed through self-injurious behavioror a physically abusive father, is this a hiddenaspect <strong>of</strong> personality? Or how is a student’s


chapter 10 projective techniques229Box 10.2Two Approaches to Projective Testing: You Cannot Have It Both WaysThe divergent approaches to the interpretation<strong>of</strong> projectives can be descriptively labeledas the psychometric approach <strong>and</strong> the clinicalapproach. The problem that arises in the use <strong>of</strong>projectives is that many clinical assessors aren’taware <strong>of</strong> the approach that they are using <strong>and</strong>therefore, do not recognize the limitations <strong>of</strong>their approach. To put it bluntly, many assessorswant the best <strong>of</strong> both worlds. They wantthe flexibility <strong>and</strong> the rich clinical informationafforded by the clinical approach, but they donot want to recognize the potential biases ininterpretation that are inherent in such usage.In contrast, many psychologists have foundnew promise in projective assessments withthe advent <strong>of</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardized administration<strong>and</strong> scoring procedures for some techniques.However, users <strong>of</strong> these systems are frustratedby the limited <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>ten confusing data-baseson which to base interpretations <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>ten slipback into making interpretations that are betterconsidered clinical intuitions. In this boxwe provide two examples <strong>of</strong> the confusionresulting from these differing approaches tointerpretation.In the clinical tradition, interpretations arebased on clinical judgment <strong>and</strong> experience.This is <strong>of</strong>ten considered bad practice, but wefeel that such clinical intuition is unavoidable<strong>and</strong> even desirable in any assessment enterprise.The problem arises when users fail torecognize the potential unreliability <strong>of</strong> theirclinical judgments. In fact, justification fortheir interpretation is <strong>of</strong>ten based on researchon the Exner Comprehensive System for Rorschachinterpretation, which has demonstratedacceptable levels <strong>of</strong> reliability for many scores(Hiller et al., 1999). Unfortunately, they usethis argument to justify the reliability <strong>of</strong> anyinterpretation they make from the Exner systemor to justify the reliability <strong>of</strong> their interpretationsfrom any projective technique. Thislatter practice would be analogous to assumingthat all self-report measures <strong>of</strong> anxietyhave the same psychometric properties <strong>and</strong>therefore can be interpreted in the same way.A second example comes from a commonpractice in using one <strong>of</strong> the newer st<strong>and</strong>ardizedsystems, like the Exner system for Rorschachinterpretation. Psychologists have enjoyed theincrease in reliability that such systems provide<strong>and</strong> which sets the stage for more empiricallybased interpretations. However, studies havenot always been able to show empirical supportfor some <strong>of</strong> the interpretations that have beenwell established in the clinical tradition (Carter& Daccy, 1996; Finch & Belter, 1993; Stredney& Ball, 2005). This has led some to the conclusionthat the richness <strong>of</strong> the Rorschach recordis simply too complex for current methodology(Finch & Belter, 1993). This implies that thescores cannot be tested adequately with currentresearch methodology. Because <strong>of</strong> this fact,these authors <strong>and</strong> others have recommendedthat one should use both the psychometric <strong>and</strong>clinical method <strong>of</strong> interpretation <strong>of</strong> Rorschachwhen using the Exner system.The problem arises when assessors makea clinical interpretation that does not haveempirical support but place undue confidencein this interpretation because they are using a“reliable <strong>and</strong> valid system.” This goes back toa basic psychometric principle. Tests or interpretivesystems are not themselves reliable<strong>and</strong>/or valid. The individual interpretationsthat one makes from them can be reliable <strong>and</strong>/or valid. Unfortunately, the Exner system, likemost <strong>of</strong> the interpretive systems for the projectivetechniques, encourages interpretationsbased on the clinical tradition, some <strong>of</strong> whichhave been supported in research <strong>and</strong> others <strong>of</strong>which have not garnered much research support.Users then are <strong>of</strong>ten unaware <strong>of</strong> the basis<strong>of</strong> their interpretations.response to a thematic approach which usesreal photographs depicting significant interpersonalsituations (i.e., peer group acceptance,attitudes toward school-work,reactions to new sibling) different from aninterview question asking how she/he is gettingalong with peers on the playground?”(p. 448).


230 chapter 10 projective techniquesIt is evident from this quotation that onecan view responses to projective tests assamples <strong>of</strong> behaviors from which onewould like to generalize to behaviors inother situations, outside <strong>of</strong> the testingenvironment. In fact, this type <strong>of</strong> interpretationunderlies Rorschach’s original development<strong>of</strong> the inkblot test <strong>and</strong> has been theguiding principle for Exner’s more recentsystem <strong>of</strong> interpretation. Rorschach, <strong>and</strong>later Exner (see Exner & Martin, 1983;Exner & Weiner, 1994), describe the inkblottests as a “perceptual test,” meaningthat a person’s perception <strong>of</strong> the inkblot isused as a sample <strong>of</strong> behavior with whichto generalize to the person’s perception <strong>of</strong>other, more clinically relevant, stimuli.These two competing views <strong>of</strong> what ismeasured by projectives have several importantimplications for the assessment process.As already mentioned, how one viewsthe process will determine what evidenceis used to establish the test’s validity. Forexample, if one views the test as a behavioralsample, then one would want evidencethat the behaviors obtained from the testare associated with behaviors outside thetesting situation. Alternatively, if one viewsprojectives as tapping unconscious conflicts,then the relationship to overt behavior isnot expected to be one-to-one, because thesame conflicts can be manifested in differentbehaviors (Koppitz, 1983). In this case,validity would be best established by showingthat responses on a projective techniqueare associated with other indicators <strong>of</strong>unconscious conflicts.Implicit in this discussion is the importantpoint that the way one views the psychologicalprocess that is being measuredby projective tests will determine the types<strong>of</strong> interpretations that will be made from achild’s or adolescent’s responses. A personviewing the results in terms <strong>of</strong> projectionwill make interpretations about drives <strong>and</strong>motivations. It is these types <strong>of</strong> predictionsthat one wishes to make. In contrast, a personviewing the results in terms <strong>of</strong> a sample<strong>of</strong> behavior will make interpretations aboutbehavioral tendencies that are likely to bemanifested in situations outside <strong>of</strong> the testingsituation. For example, if the Rorschachis used as a sample <strong>of</strong> perceptions, one wouldwish to make predictions about how theseperceptual tendencies will be manifested inother situations.The final impact <strong>of</strong> viewing projectivetechniques as either projection or a behavioralsample is its influence on the selection<strong>of</strong> the type <strong>of</strong> stimulus used. Specifically,if one is operating from the projectivehypothesis, one would want as ambiguousa situation as possible. For example,the Thematic Apperception Test (Murray,1943) contains a blank card that has nopicture on it <strong>and</strong> the person is required tomake up a story about this card. This is anexample <strong>of</strong> a very ambiguous situation withfew dem<strong>and</strong> characteristics, or very littlestimulus pull, that would guide a person’sresponse. This stimulus allows for the purestform <strong>of</strong> projection.In contrast, if one wishes to obtain abehavioral sample from the projective technique,high levels <strong>of</strong> stimulus pull may actuallybe beneficial. If one knows the dem<strong>and</strong>characteristics that promoted the response,then one would have some clue as to whatsituations one might generalize (i.e., oneswith similar dem<strong>and</strong> characteristics). Forexample, cards from the Roberts ApperceptionTest for <strong>Child</strong>ren (McArthur & Roberts,1982) were designed to pull for specificthemes (e.g., peer conflict, school problems,marital discord in parents). This is not adesirable property from the pure projectionviewpoint because it increases the dem<strong>and</strong>characteristics <strong>of</strong> the stimulus <strong>and</strong> therebylimits the degree <strong>of</strong> projection required.SummaryOur approach to the debate over the use<strong>of</strong> projective techniques is that assessorsshould use or not use projective testing


chapter 10 projective techniques231based on a careful consideration <strong>of</strong> criticalassessment issues. Assessors are <strong>of</strong>tenunclear about what approach to measurementthey are using (i.e., clinical or psychometric),<strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>ten make inappropriateinterpretations based on this confusion.Further, assessors are <strong>of</strong>ten unclear aboutwhat they are trying to measure with projectives,<strong>and</strong> again, this leads to confusionin interpretations or to the selection <strong>of</strong> atechnique that is not well-suited for theirpurpose. The rest <strong>of</strong> this chapter will highlightcharacteristics <strong>of</strong> specific projectivetechniques. However, these general issuesare paramount in underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> usingthese techniques; therefore, these issuesare revisited throughout this chapter withreference to specific techniques.Inkblot TechniquesOne <strong>of</strong> the most commonly used projectivetechniques is the ink-blot technique. Thestimulus is simply an inkblot, <strong>and</strong> the childis asked to interpret this ambiguous stimulusin some way. The best known <strong>of</strong> theinkblot techniques, the Rorschach, consists<strong>of</strong> ten cards with st<strong>and</strong>ardized inkblots.Although the Rorschach is <strong>of</strong>ten consideredsynonymous with inkblot techniques,a notable alternative is the Holtzman InkblotTechnique (HIT). This technique wasdeveloped to overcome psychometric limitationsin the Rorschach by constructing acompletely new set <strong>of</strong> inkblots (Holtzman& Swartz, 2003). The HIT consists <strong>of</strong> twoparallel forms, each <strong>of</strong> which contains 45inkblots.Volumes have been written on the differentinterpretive systems for inkblottechniques for children (e.g., Ames et al.,1974; Exner & Weiner, 1994; Holtzman& Swartz, 2003). The primary variationsamong these systems are along the dimensionsdiscussed in the introduction to thischapter: whether the inkblot test is viewedas a projective approach or as a behavioralsample, <strong>and</strong> whether a clinical or psychometricapproach to interpretation is taken.Rather than giving a superficial overview<strong>of</strong> the different approaches to interpretation,we will focus on one <strong>of</strong> the most commonlyused methods <strong>of</strong> interpretation forchildren: Exner’s Comprehensive System(ECS) for Rorschach interpretation. Thissystem attempts to integrate five majorapproaches to Rorschach interpretationinto a single Rorschach approach (Exner& Martin, 1983). Even limiting our focusto one system <strong>of</strong> interpretation does notallow us to do justice to the intricacies <strong>of</strong>Rorschach interpretation using the ECS;accordingly, the reader is referred to Exner<strong>and</strong> Weiner (1994) for a more in-depth discussion<strong>of</strong> this system <strong>of</strong> interpretation forassessing children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.The Exner ComprehensiveSystem for RorschachInterpretationProcess MeasuredThe ECS treats the Rorschach as a perceptual-cognitivetask. When viewed in thisway:“The Rorschach becomes a task to whichpeople respond by exercising their perceptual-cognitiveabilities <strong>and</strong> preferences. To articulatetheir answers, they must select parts<strong>of</strong> these variegated stimulus fields to whichthey wish to attend, use some mixture <strong>of</strong> thefeatures <strong>of</strong> the stimulus <strong>and</strong> their own needsto guide formulations, <strong>and</strong> identify objectsthat will give substance to their impressions.In short, they decide how to scan the stimulus,how to translate the stimulus input, <strong>and</strong> whatto report” (Exner & Weiner, 1982, p. 3).Weiner (1986) outlines four basic factorsthat influence a child’s response to theinkblot. First, the nature <strong>of</strong> the stimulusitself may lead the child to classify a blot


232 chapter 10 projective techniquesin a certain way. Although the inkblotswere designed to minimize stimulus pull,there are clearly some common or typicalanswers that are based on the specific features<strong>of</strong> the blots. Second, responses maybe influenced by concerns about making aparticular impression which could lead tosome censoring <strong>of</strong> responses in a sociallydesirable manner. Third, responses areinfluenced by personality traits that predisposea person to perceive the blots inidiosyncratic ways. Fourth, responses arepartly a function <strong>of</strong> situational psychologicalstates that affect a person’s perceptualexperience. Each <strong>of</strong> these factors providesan important context for interpreting achild’s response to the Rorschach inkblots.Administration <strong>and</strong> ScoringIn contrast to the complexity <strong>of</strong> Rorschachinterpretation, administration <strong>of</strong> the task isrelatively simple. The subject is h<strong>and</strong>ed theindividual cards with the inkblot stimuli <strong>and</strong>merely asked, “What might this be?” Theonly unacceptable response is, “It’s an inkblot.”If the child provides this answer, thenhe or she is encouraged to see it as somethingit’s not. All ten cards are administeredin this way, <strong>and</strong> the subject’s responses duringthis free association phase are recordedverbatim for later scoring. After the childresponds to all ten cards, the examiner entersthe inquiry phase. The assessor readministerseach card <strong>and</strong> reads to the child his or herinitial responses. The child is instructed toshow the examiner which part <strong>of</strong> the blotled to the response <strong>and</strong> what made himor her think it looked that way. The childis informed that the assessor would like tosee it “just the way you did” <strong>and</strong> severalst<strong>and</strong>ardized prompts are provided (e.g.,“What in the blot makes it look like that toyou?” Exner & Wiener, 1994). The child’sresponses during this inquiry phase are alsocoded verbatim to use in later scoring.The heart <strong>of</strong> the ECS is the extensive<strong>and</strong> detailed scoring procedure <strong>of</strong> a child’stest protocol (i.e., verbatim responses toRorschach cards). This system includesapproximately 90 possible scores. Thereare seven major categories <strong>of</strong> codes, whichare described in Table 10.1: Location,Determinants, Form Quality, OrganizationalActivity, Popularity, Content, <strong>and</strong>Special Scores. The ECS utilizes a StructuralSummary, which shows all <strong>of</strong> the possiblescores, plus various summary scoresthat are ratios, percentages, <strong>and</strong> other derivations<strong>of</strong> the individual scores that provideimportant information for interpretation.NormingThe best normative data on the Exnersystem come from a large (n = 1,870)nationwide sample <strong>of</strong> children between theages <strong>of</strong> 5 <strong>and</strong> 16 (Exner & Weiner, 1994).At each age in this 12-year age range, therewere at least 105 children. There was als<strong>of</strong>airly equal gender representation at eachage, <strong>and</strong> the inclusion <strong>of</strong> minority childrenwas at a proportion that approximatednational census data. The only weaknessevident in this normative data base wasthe overrepresentation <strong>of</strong> children fromhigher socio-economic strata (Exner &Weiner, 1994). From this normative sample,Exner <strong>and</strong> Weiner (1994) documentedseveral age-related trends in scores. Thesetrends are summarized in Table 10.2.Users <strong>of</strong> the ECS with children shouldbe aware <strong>of</strong> these developmental changesin the Rorschach responses <strong>and</strong> interpretscores within a normative perspective.The extensive normative database forchildren available with the ECS aids insuch inter pretations. This normative baseis one <strong>of</strong> the major reasons for the popularity<strong>of</strong> the ECS for use with children.However, it is important to note that theadequacy <strong>and</strong> consistency <strong>of</strong> the normativescores across different samples <strong>of</strong> children<strong>and</strong> adolescents has been questioned (Hunsley& DiGiulio, 2001; Meyer, Erdberg, &Shaffer, 2007).


chapter 10 projective techniques233Table 10.1 Summary <strong>of</strong> Scores Used in Exner’s Comprehensive System to Rorschach InterpretationCategories Description Examples <strong>of</strong> ScoresLocation Part <strong>of</strong> the blot used by respondent W = Whole blotD = Common areaDd = Uncommon AreaS = White spaceDeterminantForm QualityContentPopularOrganizationalActivitySpecial ScoresFeatures <strong>of</strong> the blot that contributed tothe formation <strong>of</strong> the responseMeasures the perceptual accuracy <strong>of</strong> theresponse (i.e., does the area <strong>of</strong> the blotreally conform to the child’s perception)Places into categories the variouspersons, places, <strong>and</strong> things that form thechild’s responseCodes the number <strong>of</strong> times the childgave a high-frequency (very common)response to a blotProvides an estimate <strong>of</strong> the efficiency<strong>of</strong> a child ’ s organization <strong>of</strong> the stimulusfieldDenotes unusual verbal material in achild ’ s responseF = FormC = ColorT = Texture/ShadingM = Human Movement+ = Superior–overelaborated0 = Ordinary–commonU = Unusual–rare but easy to see- = Minus-distorted, arbitrary, <strong>and</strong>unrealisticH = Whole HumanAn = AnatomyBl = BloodFi = FireFd = FoodHh = Household itemsP = Number <strong>of</strong> popular responsesgiven in the entire protocolZ score = Higher scores indicategreater organizationaleffortINCOM = IncongruousCombination-mergesdetails or images inunrealistically wayMOR = Morbid-response includesreferences to death orclear dysphoric feelingAG = Aggressive Movementresponseincludes action thatis clearly aggressiveReliabilityBecause <strong>of</strong> its explicit <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardizedadministration <strong>and</strong> scoring procedure, it isnot surprising that the ECS has proven tobe more reliable than many other inkblotinterpretive systems, showing high interrater<strong>and</strong> high test-retest reliability (Hilleret al., 1999). For example, in a sample <strong>of</strong> 258-year-old children, one-week test-retestcoefficients for individual <strong>and</strong> summary


234 chapter 10 projective techniquesTable 10.2 Age Trends in the Normative Data for the Exner Comprehensive System forRorschach InterpretationScoreLength <strong>of</strong> RecordLocationDevelopmentalQualityMovementDeterminantsChromatic ColorDeterminantsForm DimensionReflection ResponsesPopular ResponsesSpecial ScoresAge TrendYounger children tend to give fewer responses than older children. Protocols<strong>of</strong> less than 17 are not uncommon before age 15 <strong>and</strong> records <strong>of</strong> more than25 are unusual prior to age 13.Younger children (less than 11) give more responses that include the wholeblot rather than a specific area. More children, <strong>and</strong> especially very youngchildren, give at least one response that uses an infrequently identified area<strong>of</strong> the blot.Younger children frequently give many vague responses in which diffuseimpressions <strong>of</strong> the blot or blot area are given without clearly articulatingspecific outlines or structural features. Such vague responses account forone-third <strong>of</strong> the responses <strong>of</strong> children ages 5, 6, <strong>and</strong> 7.Younger children give few human movement responses. It is not unusual forthe responses <strong>of</strong> 5-, 6-, <strong>and</strong> 7-year-olds to have few or none such determinants,whereas it is uncommon for this to occur after age 11.It is not uncommon for children to give color responses that are notcreated based on the form features <strong>of</strong> the blot. The presence <strong>of</strong> such purecolor responses is <strong>of</strong>ten interpreted as indicative <strong>of</strong> poor affective regulation.About 70% <strong>of</strong> 5-year-olds, 35% <strong>of</strong> 8-year olds, 23% <strong>of</strong> 12-year olds, <strong>and</strong>8% <strong>of</strong> 16-year olds give at least one pure color response.Answers that include the impression <strong>of</strong> depth, distance, or dimensionalityincrease with age such that, by age 8, they appear at least once in over half<strong>of</strong> all subjects’ records.In contrast to responses <strong>of</strong> adults, images reported as reflections or mirrorimages, because <strong>of</strong> the symmetry <strong>of</strong> the blot are quite common in childprotocols. Such responses appear in about half <strong>of</strong> the protocols <strong>of</strong> childrenunder the age <strong>of</strong> 8. Although the incidence <strong>of</strong> such responses declines overtime, they are still found in about 25% <strong>of</strong> the protocols <strong>of</strong> 15-year olds.There is a steady increase in the number <strong>of</strong> popular responses with age,with adolescents giving approximately one-third more popular responsesthan children under age 8.Many <strong>of</strong> the special scores that document unusual verbalizations <strong>and</strong>cognitive slippage are more common in young children than in adolescents<strong>and</strong> adultsscores ranged from r = .49 to r = .95, witha mean coefficient <strong>of</strong> r = .84 (Exner &Weiner, 1994). In fact, the only coefficientto drop below r = .70 was the coefficientfor Inanimate Movement (r = .49).Validity <strong>and</strong> InterpretationsWhile Rorschach scores are reliable overshort time intervals, the stability <strong>of</strong> thescores over longer periods is lower thanthe stability <strong>of</strong> adult scores. Specifically,Exner, Thomas, <strong>and</strong> Mason (1985) tested57 children at 2-year intervals from age 8to 16. In general, most scores showed onlymoderate consistency over each two-yearinterval until the interval between the ages<strong>of</strong> 14 <strong>and</strong> 16. Some notable exceptions werethe fairly stable coefficients for the use <strong>of</strong>good form, the use <strong>of</strong> popular responses,


chapter 10 projective techniques235the number <strong>of</strong> active movement responses,<strong>and</strong> the use <strong>of</strong> shading features for makingdepth <strong>and</strong> dimension responses.The modest stability <strong>of</strong> Rorschachscores is not unique to this type <strong>of</strong>assessment, but is characteristic <strong>of</strong> mostassessment techniques in children (e.g.,McConaughy, Stanger, & Achenbach,1992). In fact, this is probably a positiveattribute <strong>of</strong> the scores because it suggeststhat the scores capture the rapid developmentalchanges experienced by children<strong>and</strong> adolescents. However, there is a tendencyto equate Rorschach responses withpersonality assessment, <strong>and</strong> to equate personalitywith stable dispositions. Thesefindings on the low stability <strong>of</strong> Rorschachscores clearly argue against making strongdispositional statements based on a child’sRorschach protocol.One common use <strong>of</strong> the ECS has beento assess childhood depression. Exner(1983) initially developed a DepressionIndex (DEPI) based on six scores froma child’s protocol. Unfortunately, theDEPI, based primarily on research inadults, showed very poor agreement withother measures <strong>of</strong> depression in children,which led Exner to revise the DEPI(Exner, 1990) in an effort to increase itscorrespondence with other measures <strong>of</strong>depression. Tests <strong>of</strong> the revised DEPIindex have also failed to find consistentassociations with other measures<strong>of</strong> depression (Archer & Krishamurthy,1997; Ball et al., 1991; Carter & Dacy,1996). These findings could be a function<strong>of</strong> inadequate methods <strong>of</strong> assessingchildhood depression in general, whichresults in the failure to have an appropriatest<strong>and</strong>ard with which to judge the Rorschach.Alternatively, Weiner (1986) hasargued that the Rorschach: “is a measure<strong>of</strong> personality processes, not diagnosticcategories… it can help to identify forms<strong>of</strong> psychopathology only to the extentthat they identify personality characteristicsassociated with the types <strong>of</strong> disorder”(p. 155). However, these findings suggestthat users <strong>of</strong> the DEPI from the ECSshould not expect the scores to be highlyrelated to other indexes <strong>of</strong> depression.An even more extreme caution is inorder for the Suicide Constellation for<strong>Child</strong>ren, a set <strong>of</strong> scores based on an indexused to assess for suicidal tendencies inadults (Exner, 1978). The suicide constellationwas developed by selecting the eightbest predictors <strong>of</strong> suicide from the ECSin a small sample (n = 39) <strong>of</strong> children whohad attempted or committed suicide withinfewer than 60 days after the Rorschach wastaken (Exner & Weiner, 1994). Unfortunately,the predictive validity <strong>of</strong> this indexhas not been replicated in other samples,making the interpretation <strong>of</strong> this indexquestionable at present (Allen & Hollifield,2003).Another common use <strong>of</strong> the Rorschachis in the detection <strong>of</strong> cognitive <strong>and</strong> perceptualirregularities that could be associatedwith schizophrenia (Weiner, 1986). Exner<strong>and</strong> Weiner (1994) outline four sets <strong>of</strong>Rorschach scores that can aid in this detection.First, disordered <strong>and</strong> illogical thoughtprocesses are the focus <strong>of</strong> several specialscores in the Exner system. For example,the Incongruous Combination score identifiesresponses that condense blot detailsor images into a single incongruous perceptin which the parts or attributes do notbelong together: “a person with the head<strong>of</strong> a chicken” (Weiner, 1986, p. 217). Second,perceptual inaccuracies are suggestedwhen the child has a protocol with manyresponses that do not correspond closelyto the form structure <strong>of</strong> the blot (i.e., poorform quality) or protocols with few commonor popular responses. Third, interpersonalinadequacies that are <strong>of</strong>ten associatedwith schizophrenia can be assessed inresponses involving human movement.Movement responses with poor form qualityare considered indicative <strong>of</strong> inaccurateor unrealistic interpretations <strong>of</strong> interpersonalsituations (Exner & Weiner, 1994).


236 chapter 10 projective techniquesFourth, the irregular content <strong>of</strong> a protocol,such as one with very violent (e.g., two boysstabbing each other in the chest) or verybizarre (e.g., flowers squirting poisonousgas) content can be considered suggestive<strong>of</strong> disturbed ideation that is <strong>of</strong>ten associatedwith schizophrenia.Exner <strong>and</strong> Weiner (1982) reported dataon 20 children (ages 9–16) reliably diagnosedwith schizophrenia <strong>and</strong> 23 nonschizophrenicchildren. These data indicated thatthe use <strong>of</strong> these indexes produced a highcorrect classification rate (90.7%). Thesepositive findings must be interpreted withthree cautions, however. First, indicators<strong>of</strong> perceptual disturbances for the Rorschachhave not always shown high correlationswith other behavioral indicators <strong>of</strong>thought disorders (Smith, Baity, Knowles,& Hilsenroth, 2001). Second, while theseindexes appear to be correlated with a diagnosis<strong>of</strong> schizophrenia, it is unclear howmuch utility these indexes possess over theactual behavioral symptoms <strong>of</strong> the disorder(Gittelman-Klein, 1986). In other words,there is no evidence to suggest that childrenwho show elevations on these indexes, butwho do not show overt behavioral manifestations<strong>of</strong> the disorder, are at risk for developingschizophrenia. Second, Gallucci(1989) studied a sample <strong>of</strong> 72 intellectuallygifted children <strong>and</strong> found elevated rates onthese indexes, compared to age norms, butno other signs <strong>of</strong> maladjustment in the children.Intellectually superior children mayprocess the Rorschach stimuli in nonconventionalways, but these differences shouldnot be considered indicative <strong>of</strong> a psychoticprocess. Similarly, Holaday (2000) reportedthat children <strong>and</strong> adolescents diagnosedwith post-traumatic stress disorder alsoreported significantly higher scores on theRorschach indicators <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia.Thus, indicators <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia on theRorschach appear to have a high rate <strong>of</strong>false-positives for thought disorders (i.e.,many children score high who do not haveother indicators <strong>of</strong> psychosis).EvaluationThese examples are only a small sample<strong>of</strong> the common uses <strong>of</strong> the ECS in clinicalassessments <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.However, these examples illustrate two issuesthat are important for using the Rorschachin the assessment <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.In general, Rorschach responses donot typically correspond closely to behaviorallybased diagnoses; therefore, use <strong>of</strong>the Rorschach for diagnostic purposes is notrecommended. Second, many <strong>of</strong> the Rorschachscores <strong>and</strong> indexes were developed<strong>and</strong> validated on adults. Unfortunately, theextension to children has not met with greatsuccess, as is evident from studies on the<strong>Child</strong>ren’s Depression Index <strong>and</strong> SuicideConstellation <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong>ren. Therefore, usersshould be wary about using adult-orientedsystems for interpreting the Rorschachresponses <strong>of</strong> children.Thematic (Storytelling)TechniquesThe second type <strong>of</strong> projective techniquefor children is the storytelling or thematicapproach. In this technique a child is showna moderately ambiguous picture or photograph<strong>and</strong> asked to tell a story about it. Forexample, the instructions to the RobertsApperception Test for <strong>Child</strong>ren (McArthur& Roberts, 1982) are:“I have a number <strong>of</strong> pictures I am going toshow you one at a time. I want you to makeup a story about each picture. Please tell mewhat is happening in the picture, what ledup to this scene, <strong>and</strong> how the story ends. Tellme what the people are talking about <strong>and</strong>feeling. Use your imagination <strong>and</strong> rememberthat there are no right or wrong answersfor the picture” (p. 7).Thematic tests have been a popular type <strong>of</strong>projective technique with children because


chapter 10 projective techniques237the storytelling format is usually nonthreatening<strong>and</strong> fun for them. However,it does require a significant level <strong>of</strong> verbalability in the child.There are many different thematic techniquesthat have been used with children<strong>and</strong> adolescents, which vary in the types <strong>of</strong>pictures that are used to promote children’sstories (see Kroon, Goudena, & Rispens,1998 for a review). One <strong>of</strong> the most commonlyused techniques for children <strong>and</strong>adolescents is the Thematic ApperceptionTechnique (TAT; Murray, 1943). The TATconsists <strong>of</strong> 31 cards with black-<strong>and</strong>-whitepictures <strong>of</strong> primarily adult figures involvedin some relatively ambiguous action orinteraction. Because the TAT was designedprimarily for use with adults <strong>and</strong> containedadult pictures, the <strong>Child</strong>ren’s ApperceptionTest (CAT; Bellak & Bellak, 1949)was developed especially for children. Theoriginal CAT contains ten cards with picturesdepicting animal figures, although alater CAT-H was developed with humanfigures. The pictures on both the CAT <strong>and</strong>the CAT-H were designed to elicit typicalchildhood conflicts that are predicted frompsychodynamic theory (e.g., sibling rivalry,oedipal urges, toileting concerns). Anotherthematic test heavily influenced by psychodynamictheory is the Blacky Picture Test(Blum, 1950). The Blacky Picture Testconsists <strong>of</strong> 11 cartoons whose central figureis a dog named Blacky. Like the CAT, thepictures were designed to depict psychosexualconflicts common in children.Two apperception tests, the SchoolApperception Method (Solomon & Starr,1968) <strong>and</strong> the Michigan Picture Test-Revised (Hutt, 1980), were designed morespecifically for use in educational settings.Another apperception test designed specificallyfor use with children is the RobertsApperception Test for <strong>Child</strong>ren (RATC;McArthur & Roberts, 1982). The RATC isquite explicit in the themes assessed by thestimulus pictures. Unlike many other thematictechniques, the themes the pictureswere designed to assess are not specific topsychodynamic theory. Also, the RATC isone <strong>of</strong> the few thematic techniques thatincludes an explicit scoring system. TheRATC is reviewed in greater detail later inthis chapter.Several thematic approaches havespecific sets <strong>of</strong> pictorial stimuli for specificgroups <strong>of</strong> children. This is based onresearch showing that children providegreater verbalization on thematic apperceptiontechniques when the stimulusmaterial more closely matches their ethnicity,gender, <strong>and</strong> age (Constantino &Malgady, 1983). For example, the TAT<strong>and</strong> RATC contain some pictures that aregender-specific <strong>and</strong> the RATC containssupplementary pictures depicting AfricanAmerican children (McArthur & Roberts,1982). Of particular note, the Tell-Me-A-Story technique (TEMAS: Constantino,Malgady, & Rogler, 1988) is a thematicapperception test that was specificallydesigned to be a culturally sensitive testfor inner-city children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.The TEMAS involves 23 brightly coloredcards depicting inner-city themes involvingpeer <strong>and</strong> family interactions. Thereare 11 sex-specific cards <strong>and</strong> two parallelsets for minority (depicting Hispanic<strong>and</strong> African-American characters) <strong>and</strong>nonminority children. Also, unique tothe TEMAS are separate norms for Caucasian,African-American, Puerto Rican,<strong>and</strong> other Hispanic children across threeage groups (5–7, 8–10, 11–13). However,these norms are based on a rather limitedsample (n = 642) <strong>of</strong> children from publicschools in New York City (see Flanagan &DiGiuseppe, 1999).General Interpretation <strong>of</strong>Thematic TechniquesOne important issue in the use <strong>of</strong> thematictechniques is the lack <strong>of</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardizedadministration or scoring procedures for


238 chapter 10 projective techniquesmost systems. Of the 12 thematic apperceptiontests used for children <strong>and</strong> adolescentsreviewed by Kroon et al. (1998), only5 had st<strong>and</strong>ardized <strong>and</strong> objective methods<strong>of</strong> scoring children’s responses. For example,assessors administering the TAT <strong>of</strong>tenselect certain cards to administer. Further,there are many different systems forobtaining scores but most assessors do notuse any systematic scoring system. Giventhe lack <strong>of</strong> consistency in administration<strong>and</strong> scoring, it is not surprising that evidencefor the reliability <strong>and</strong> validity <strong>of</strong>thematic techniques is limited. Thematictechniques are <strong>of</strong>ten interpreted within anidiographic or clinical tradition in whichclinical impressions <strong>of</strong> an individual childare obtained through an analysis <strong>of</strong> thechild’s stories.<strong>Clinical</strong> interpretation <strong>of</strong> a child’s storyis typically based on two broad aspects <strong>of</strong>a child’s response. The first step is a processinterpretation. In this part <strong>of</strong> the interpretation,one notes such characteristics <strong>of</strong> thestories as how elaborate the stories were,whether the stories were coherent <strong>and</strong> tiedto the stimulus card, <strong>and</strong> whether therewere any specific cards for which the childhad difficulty formulating a story. This type<strong>of</strong> interpretation can be used to determinehow invested the child or adolescent was inthe assessment process, whether there wereany potential disturbances or idiosyncrasiesin thought processes, <strong>and</strong> whether therewere any specific types <strong>of</strong> stimuli that eliciteddefensive reactions from the child.The second part <strong>of</strong> the interpretiveprocess is a content analysis. <strong>Child</strong>ren’sstories are typically analyzed for (1) thecharacteristics <strong>of</strong> the hero or main character(e.g., motives, needs, emotions, selfimage),(2) forces that affect the hero inhis or her environment (e.g., rejection bypeers, punitiveness from parents, frighteningforces, support by parent, affectionfrom sibling), (3) the coping or problemsolvingstrategies used by the hero (e.g.,aggression, compromise, nurturance), <strong>and</strong>(4) the outcomes <strong>of</strong> the story (e.g., positiveor negative, outcomes brought aboutby hero or someone in his or environment,outcomes are realistic). The content analysisshould determine whether there are anyconsistent themes in a child’s story, especiallythemes that transcend the stimuluspull <strong>of</strong> a card. For example, an aggressivestory provided for a card that shows twochildren fighting is less diagnostic than astory with an aggressive theme based on apicture <strong>of</strong> two people sitting next to eachother in a park.Roberts ApperceptionTechnique for <strong>Child</strong>renThe Roberts Apperception Techniques for<strong>Child</strong>ren (RATC; McArthur & Roberts,1982) is one example <strong>of</strong> a thematic techniquethat was explicitly designed for usewith children <strong>and</strong> is one <strong>of</strong> the few storytellingprocedures with an explicit <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardizedscoring system. This instrumentillustrates some major components in theinterpretive process <strong>of</strong> thematic techniques.ContentThe RATC is intended for use with children<strong>and</strong> adolescents <strong>of</strong> ages 6–15. Thereis a st<strong>and</strong>ard set <strong>of</strong> 27 stimulus cardsdepicting common situations, conflicts,<strong>and</strong> stresses in children’s lives (McArthur& Roberts, 1982). Eleven cards have parallelmale <strong>and</strong> female versions, <strong>and</strong> there is asupplementary set <strong>of</strong> stimulus cards featuringAfrican-American children. A description<strong>of</strong> the RATC cards <strong>and</strong> the themes thecards were designed to elicit are providedin Table 10.3.Administration <strong>and</strong> ScoringThe administration procedures <strong>of</strong> theRATC are quite simple (the instructionsgiven to the child were provided earlierin this chapter). The RATC provides


chapter 10 projective techniques239Table 10.3 Depictions in the Stimulus Cards from the Roberts Apperception Test for<strong>Child</strong>renCard Number Description Common Themes1 (B & G) Both parents discussing something withchildElicits themes <strong>of</strong> family confrontation<strong>and</strong> stories in which parents aregiving advice or punishing a child2 (B & G) Mother hugging child Elicits themes <strong>of</strong> maternal support<strong>and</strong> dependency needs in relation toa material figure3 (B & G) <strong>Child</strong> working on homework Elicits themes related to child’sattitude to school, teachers, tests,<strong>and</strong> homework4 One child st<strong>and</strong>ing over another childin prone position5 (B & G) Parents are shown in an embrace withchild looking on6 (B & G) Two white children are shown interactingwith a black childElicits themes with aggression,accidents, <strong>and</strong> illnessesElicits themes related to a child’sattitude toward parental displays <strong>of</strong>affectionElicits themes related to peerinteractions <strong>and</strong> racial attitudes7 (B & G) <strong>Child</strong> sitting up in bed awake Elicits themes <strong>of</strong> anxiety <strong>and</strong> baddreams8 Both parents speaking to male <strong>and</strong> femalechild9 <strong>Child</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ing with clenched fists over achild sitting on the ground10 (B & G) Mother holding baby with childlooking onElicits themes related to familydiscussions, such as around disciplineor planning a family activityElicits themes related to peeraggressionElicits themes <strong>of</strong> sibling rivalry <strong>and</strong>attitudes toward the birth <strong>of</strong> a newsibling11 <strong>Child</strong> cowering with h<strong>and</strong>s in front <strong>of</strong> face Elicits themes <strong>of</strong> fear <strong>and</strong> anxiety12 (B & G) Adult male glaring at a distressed adultfemale with child looking on13 (B & G) <strong>Child</strong> preparing to throw chair onto theground14 (B & G) <strong>Child</strong> with paint on h<strong>and</strong>s has put h<strong>and</strong>printson wall with mother looking on indistress15 Adult female in bathtub with male childlooking through door16 (B & G) <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> father in a discussion whilefather looks at a paperElicits themes <strong>of</strong> parental conflict<strong>and</strong> parental depressionElicits themes <strong>of</strong> anger <strong>and</strong> aggressivefeelingsElicits themes <strong>of</strong> maternal limit setting<strong>and</strong> child wrongdoingElicits attitudes toward sexuality <strong>and</strong>nudityElicits themes <strong>of</strong> father-child relationships<strong>and</strong> paternal approvalNote: B & G = Separate cards for girls <strong>and</strong> boys.Source: McArthur, D. S., & Roberts, G. E. (1982). Roberts Apperception Test for <strong>Child</strong>ren. Los Angeles: WesternPsychological Service.


240 chapter 10 projective techniquesexplicit instructions for scoring the storiesprovided by a child. Each story is scored on16 coding categories. There are 8 Adaptivecategories, 5 <strong>Clinical</strong> categories, <strong>and</strong> 3 categorieslabeled Indicators. A description <strong>of</strong>these categories is provided in Table 10.4.As evident from this box, the RATC codingcategories are quite similar to traditionalcontent areas used to interpret otherthematic techniques. Scores used in interpretationsfrom the RATC are the totalnumber <strong>of</strong> times a given code was presentacross all stories. This allows one to determineconsistent themes (high scores withina category) across stories.NormingOne objective <strong>of</strong> the authors <strong>of</strong> the RATCwas to develop a st<strong>and</strong>ardized scoringsystem so that normative data could begenerated <strong>and</strong> used by other users <strong>of</strong> thesystem (McArthur & Roberts, 1982). Theimportance <strong>of</strong> age-specific normative datain the interpretation <strong>of</strong> projective testswas already discussed in the previous sectionon the Rorschach. Unfortunately, thenormative data provided in the RATCmanual are minimal. The normative sampleon which norm-referenced scores arebased consisted <strong>of</strong> 200 school children:20 boys <strong>and</strong> 20 girls in the age ranges <strong>of</strong>6–7 <strong>and</strong> 8–9 <strong>and</strong> 30 boys <strong>and</strong> 30 girls inthe age ranges <strong>of</strong> 10–12 <strong>and</strong> 13–15. Notonly is the size <strong>of</strong> the sample small, butits representativeness is also questionable.The sample was taken from three schooldistricts in southern California. Althoughthe manual states that an effort was madeto select children from lower, middle, <strong>and</strong>upper socioeconomic family backgrounds(McArthur & Roberts, 1982), there is noevidence to show that this goal was met,nor is there any information given onthe ethnic makeup <strong>of</strong> the sample. Finally,comparisons <strong>of</strong> scores from this normativesample to other non-referred samples<strong>of</strong> children have shown very differentdistributions <strong>of</strong> scores (Bell & Nagle,1999). Therefore, the norm-referencedscores provided in the RATC manual are<strong>of</strong> questionable utility.ReliabilityA positive outcome <strong>of</strong> the explicit scoringsystem was an increase in reliabilitycompared to other thematic approacheswithout st<strong>and</strong>ardized administration orscoring procedures. The manual reportsthat 17 doctoral-level raters averaged 89%agreement on three RATC protocols, <strong>and</strong>8 master’s-level clinicians reached 84%agreement. Evidence for the split-halfreliability <strong>of</strong> the RATC was less impressive,however. Acceptable reliability(above .70) was found for only 6 <strong>of</strong> the 13adaptive <strong>and</strong> clinical scales: Limit Setting,Unresolved, Resolution 2, Resolution 3,Problem Identification, <strong>and</strong> Support.Validity <strong>and</strong> InterpretationsThe increase in reliability afforded by theRATC scoring system has set the stage forthe development <strong>of</strong> a database with which tojudge the instrument’s validity. However, theextent <strong>of</strong> this database is presently quite limited<strong>and</strong> the findings mixed. For example,the manual reported comparisons between200 clinic-referred children <strong>and</strong> the 200well-adjusted children in the st<strong>and</strong>ardizationsample (McArthur & Roberts, 1982). In thisbroad test, all eight <strong>of</strong> the adaptive scales <strong>and</strong>all three indicators differed between the twogroups. In contrast, the only clinical scaleto show differences between groups wasthe Rejection scale. Thus, the clinical scalesfailed to differentiate maladjusted from welladjustedchildren, which does not bode wellfor the likelihood <strong>of</strong> these scales accomplishingthe more difficult task <strong>of</strong> differentiatingtypes <strong>of</strong> problems within clinic-referredchildren.


chapter 10 projective techniques241Table 10.4 Pr<strong>of</strong>ile Scales <strong>and</strong> Indicators from the Roberts Apperception Test for <strong>Child</strong>renDescription <strong>of</strong>Scale Purpose Scoring CriteriaReliance onOthers (A)Designed to measure the adaptive capacity to usehelp to overcome problemMain character (1) seeks assistance for h<strong>and</strong>ling problem or completingtasks, (2) asks permission, or (3) asks for approval or material objectsSupport-Other (A) Reflects tendency to support others Main character (1) gives object or does something requested or (2)gives emotional support or encouragementSupport-<strong>Child</strong> (A) Measures self-sufficiency, maturity, assertiveness,<strong>and</strong> positive affectLimit Setting (A) Measures the extent to which parent placesreasonable <strong>and</strong> appropriate limits on childProblemIdentification (A)Measures child ’ s ability to articulate problemsituationsResolution-1 (A) Measures child’s tendency to seek easy orunrealistic solutions to problemsResolution-2 (A) Measures a child’s tendency to use constructiveresolutions to problemsResolution-3 (A) Measures a child’s tendency to use constructiveresolutions to problemsAnxiety (C) Measures a child’s tendency to interpret situationsas dangerous <strong>and</strong> fearfulMain character (1) shows appropriate self-confidence, assertiveness,self-reliance, perseverance, or delay <strong>of</strong> gratification or (2) experiencespositive emotionsStory describes some appropriate disciplinary action or constructivediscussion following child wrongdoingCharacter in story states a problem or obstacle or experiences contradictoryfeelingsStory involves a situation in which a problem is solved without anyclear mediating process or by some unrealistic or imaginary processStory involves constructive resolution <strong>of</strong> internal feelings, externalproblem, or conflicted interpersonal relationship; solution is limitedto present situation without an explanation <strong>of</strong> the process involved inworking through the problemSame as Resolution-2, except the story explains how the characterworked through problemStory involves (1) character showing apprehension, self-doubt, or guiltor (2) themes <strong>of</strong> illness, death, or accidentsAggression (C) Measures a child’s aggressive impulses Story involves angry feelings, physical or verbal attack, or destruction<strong>of</strong> objectsDepression (C) Measures a child’s tendency to depression Story involves dysphoric feelings, giving up, or vegetative symptoms <strong>of</strong>depression(Continues)


242 chapter 10 projective techniquesTable 10.4 (Continued)Description <strong>of</strong>Scale Purpose Scoring CriteriaRejection (C) Measures issues that a child might haveconcerning separation <strong>and</strong> rejectionUnresolved (C) Measures a tendency toward having an externallocus <strong>of</strong> control or an inability to control one’semotionsAtypical Response Measures distortions in a child’s thoughtprocesses or unusual ideationMaladaptiveOutcome (I)Measures poor problem-solving abilities or thepresence <strong>of</strong> a pessimistic or hopeless cognitive styleRefusal (I) Measures a lack <strong>of</strong> investment in the task orextreme defensiveness to certain stimuliStory involves physical separation, rejection, dislike <strong>of</strong> another person,needs unmet by others, or racial discriminationStory involves an emotional reaction without a resolutionStory involves a distortion <strong>of</strong> stimulus card, is an illogical story,involves homicidal or suicidal ideation/action, involves death, orinvolves child abuseStory involves characters that behave inappropriately to solve a problemor when the story ends with the main character dying<strong>Child</strong> refuses to respond or stops in the middle <strong>of</strong> a story <strong>and</strong> refuses togo onNote: Descriptions <strong>of</strong> scoring criteria are not full criteria <strong>and</strong> should not be used in place <strong>of</strong> the explicit criteria provided in the RATC manual. Manual also provides concreteexamples <strong>of</strong> each criterion. (A) = Adaptive scale, (C) = <strong>Clinical</strong> scale, (I) = Indicator.Source: McArthur, D. S., & Roberts, G. E. (1982). Roberts Apperception Test for <strong>Child</strong>ren. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.


chapter 10 projective techniques243EvaluationBecause <strong>of</strong> the lack <strong>of</strong> validity evidence,the RATC should not be used in diagnosticdecision-making, as is the case for otherthematic approaches. Instead, the RATCshould be used as a method <strong>of</strong> obtainingclinical impressions, with a consideration<strong>of</strong> all the strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses <strong>of</strong> thismethod <strong>of</strong> interpretation. However, unlikemany other thematic techniques, theexplicit scoring system <strong>of</strong> the RATC has ledto a reliable scoring procedure that sets thestage for further validation to guide interpretations.Another caution in interpretingthe RATC stems from the poor normativebase from which norm-referenced scoresprovided in the RATC are derived (Bell& Nagle, 1999). These scores should heregarded as suspect until further informationbecomes available from larger <strong>and</strong>more representative samples <strong>of</strong> children<strong>and</strong> adolescents.Sentence CompletionTechniquesAnother type <strong>of</strong> projective technique thatis frequently used in the clinical assessment<strong>of</strong> children is the sentence-completiontechnique (SCT). The sentence-completionmethod involves providing the child,either orally or in writing, with a number<strong>of</strong> incomplete sentence stems such as, “Myfamily is…” or “I am most ashamed <strong>of</strong>….”As is evident from these examples, thestimulus employed in sentence-completiontechniques is much less subjective than theother projective methods reviewed in thischapter. That is, the sentence stems have ahigh degree <strong>of</strong> stimulus pull in promptingcertain types <strong>of</strong> answers. As a result, manyhave debated whether sentence-completionmethods should even be considered projective,given their more objective nature(Hart, 1986).Although SCTs clearly require a differentlevel <strong>of</strong> inference than other projectives,they probably are closer to other projectivesin design <strong>and</strong> interpretation than toself-report rating scales. However, a decisionas to whether or not to use an SCT ina clinical assessment goes back to our initialdiscussion <strong>of</strong> projective techniques in general.If one wishes to interpret projectivesas a behavioral sample, then the objectivenature <strong>of</strong> the SCT <strong>and</strong> the lower level <strong>of</strong>inference required is a distinct advantage.In contrast, if one wishes to enhance projection,then the strong stimulus pull <strong>of</strong> theSCT is less desirable.To illustrate the diversity in how SCTsare used, Holaday, Smith, <strong>and</strong> Sherry (2000)surveyed a r<strong>and</strong>om sample (n = 100) <strong>of</strong> members<strong>of</strong> the Society for <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong><strong>and</strong> they obtained a 60% responserate to their survey. On questions related towhy <strong>and</strong> how they used SCTs in their clinicalpractice; the only response endorsed bythe majority <strong>of</strong> respondents (67%) was as“part <strong>of</strong> a more comprehensive assessmentbattery.” A substantial minority were splitin endorsing “to determine personalitystructure” (i.e., as a projective test) <strong>and</strong> inendorsing “as a structured interview” (i.e.,as a behavioral sample) to describe theiruse <strong>of</strong> SCTs, with 30% <strong>and</strong> 25%, respectively,endorsing these uses. Interestingly,28% endorsed the use “to obtain quotablequotes” as a justification for inclusion <strong>of</strong>SCTs in their assessment battery, suggestingthat the information provided by theSCT is <strong>of</strong>ten used to obtain examples toillustrate findings from other assessmentprocedures (e.g., clinical diagnoses).ContentFeatures <strong>of</strong> SCTsDespite a common format, there arenumerous SCTs available that vary in theircontent, length, complexity, <strong>and</strong> purpose.The Rotter Incomplete Sentence Blank


244 chapter 10 projective techniques(Rotter & Rafferty, 1950) is one <strong>of</strong> the oldest<strong>and</strong> most common <strong>of</strong> the SCTs. It wasoriginally developed for use with adults<strong>and</strong> consists <strong>of</strong> 40 items designed to elicitinformation on psychosexual conflicts. Itis available in three forms, <strong>and</strong> the authorsprovide a quantitative scoring procedurethat can be used to determine the degree <strong>of</strong>conflict present in each response. Anothercommonly used SCT is the Hart SentenceCompletion Test for <strong>Child</strong>ren (HSCT).The 40-item HSCT was developed specificallyfor use with children, <strong>and</strong> the contentwas designed to elicit children’s perceptions<strong>of</strong> family, peers, school, <strong>and</strong> self (Hart,1986). There are numerous other SCTprocedures that are beyond the scope <strong>of</strong>this chapter to discuss in detail (see Haak,2003; Holaday et al., 2000 for reviews).AdministrationAdministration <strong>of</strong> SCTs is straightforward<strong>and</strong> typically includes instructions whichinform children that they are to completethe sentences in whatever mannerthey choose, <strong>and</strong> that there are no right orwrong answers.There are three important dimensionson which the administration procedures<strong>of</strong> SCTs differ (Hart, 1986). First, SCTs<strong>and</strong> users <strong>of</strong> SCTs can differ on whetherthe sentence stems are to be read aloudto the child or adolescent or whether thechild being assessed is to be asked to readthe questions <strong>and</strong> respond privately. Thechoice <strong>of</strong> which administration format touse is partly a function <strong>of</strong> the child’s readinglevel <strong>and</strong> age, with assessors tendingto read sentence stems to children more<strong>of</strong>ten than to adolescents (Holaday et al.,2000). However, the verbal interchangethat results from the reading <strong>of</strong> questionsto a child can also provide an assessor withadditional information (e.g., a child’s affectiveresponse to a sentence stem, a child’sapparent motivation toward the task) onwhich to evaluate his or her responses.Second, some users <strong>of</strong> SCTs request thatthe child answer as quickly as possible bysaying the first thing that comes to his orher mind in an effort to elicit spontaneous<strong>and</strong> unguarded responses. In contrast,other users attempt to promote deliberationby telling the subjects that they cancomplete the sentences in any way theylike <strong>and</strong> that the purpose <strong>of</strong> the test is tobetter underst<strong>and</strong> their real feelings. Thefirst use <strong>of</strong> the SCT is typically preferredif the goal <strong>of</strong> administration is projection.The second administration procedure istypically preferred if the goal is to obtain abehavioral sample.Third, SCTs differ in whether or notthey include an inquiry process. In theinquiry phase, children are asked to explaintheir responses in more depth. This questioninghelps the assessor determine why achild may have completed the sentence ina particular way. This information is especiallyuseful for responses that are unusual,ambiguous, or diagnostically important(Hart, 1986). Because <strong>of</strong> the importantclinical information obtained by thisinquiry, it is <strong>of</strong>ten an integral part <strong>of</strong> theadministration <strong>of</strong> SCTs for many assessors(Haak, 2003).InterpretationAs with most projective techniques, thereis great variability in how SCTs are scored<strong>and</strong> interpreted. Many SCTs do not haveexplicit scoring or interpretive guidelines<strong>and</strong>, even for those that do, many users donot follow them in practice. For example,<strong>of</strong> the 60 respondents from the survey <strong>of</strong>users <strong>of</strong> SCTs conducted by Holaday et al.(2000), only 17% <strong>of</strong> those respondentswho said they use SCTs in the assessment<strong>of</strong> children stated that they score the testaccording to a manual or according to theauthors’ instruction, <strong>and</strong> 27% <strong>of</strong> thoserespondents who said they use SCTs withadolescents reported doing so. In fact,25% <strong>of</strong> respondents did not even know


chapter 10 projective techniques245the name <strong>of</strong> the SCT that they used inpractice <strong>and</strong> 13% reported that they writetheir own sentence stems to address theirclient’s needs.However, interpretation <strong>of</strong> SCTs typicallyrelies on an analysis <strong>of</strong> the manifestcontent <strong>of</strong> a child’s responses. As was thecase with the thematic techniques, anassessor would analyze a child’s responsefor consistent themes that might provideclues to the child’s emotional adjustmentor his or her perceptions <strong>of</strong> certain personsor situations. For example, positiveresponses to stems designed to assess perceptions<strong>of</strong> parents (My father is the best;What I like best about my father is he isnice) are thought to be an indication <strong>of</strong> apositive father-child relationship. This isan example <strong>of</strong> the low level <strong>of</strong> inferencethat is <strong>of</strong>ten applied to the interpretation<strong>of</strong> SCTs.Some assessors also analyze the process <strong>of</strong>a child’s responses, such as whether they arecomplex, whether they are perseverative,whether they are expressive <strong>and</strong> imaginative,<strong>and</strong> whether they are coherent <strong>and</strong> related tothe sentence stem (Haak, 2003). This type <strong>of</strong>analysis can provide the assessor with insightas to how invested a child was in the task <strong>and</strong>some possible clues about a child’s thoughtprocesses. Box 10.3 provides an overview <strong>of</strong>the most common approaches to interpretingresponses on SCTs.EvaluationAs is evident from the discussion <strong>of</strong> SCTsto this point, most systems do not haveexplicit <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardized administration,scoring, <strong>and</strong> interpretive procedures.These decisions are <strong>of</strong>ten left to the judgment<strong>of</strong> the assessor, who can be guidedby the advice <strong>of</strong> the authors <strong>of</strong> the SCT orother experienced clinicians (e.g., Haak,2003). As a result, most SCTs can be consideredto fall in the clinical tradition <strong>of</strong>projectives, with most techniques failingto have well-established psychometricproperties (Anastasi, 1988). Of particularconcern is the lack <strong>of</strong> a normative base toguide the interpretation <strong>of</strong> SCTs in children<strong>and</strong> adolescents. Also, most SCTswere initially developed for adults, so thecontent is <strong>of</strong>ten inappropriate for children.Drawing TechniquesA final popular approach to projective testingwith children is through the interpretation<strong>of</strong> children’s drawings. The popularity<strong>of</strong> drawing techniques in the assessment <strong>of</strong>children can be attributed to two factors.First, unlike other projective techniquesthat require substantial verbal ability <strong>of</strong>tenexceeding the capacity <strong>of</strong> some very youngchildren, drawing techniques are primarilynonverbal. Second, most children are familiar<strong>and</strong> comfortable with drawing, so it isan enjoyable assessment context for a child.Koppitz (1983) writes:“Drawing is a natural mode <strong>of</strong> expression forboys <strong>and</strong> girls. It is a nonverbal language <strong>and</strong>form <strong>of</strong> communication; like any other language,it can be analyzed for structure, quality,<strong>and</strong> content” (p. 426).From this description, it is evident thatthe interpretation <strong>of</strong> children’s drawings isbased on the same assumptions that underliethe interpretation <strong>of</strong> other projectiveapproaches; namely, that drawings containnonverbal clues <strong>and</strong> symbolic messagesabout a child’s self-concept, motivations,concerns, attitudes, <strong>and</strong> desires (Cummings,1986).Kn<strong>of</strong>f (2003) provides a general frameworkfor administering <strong>and</strong> interpretingdrawings. Specifically, administrationinvolves two phases. During the performancephase, the child is provided with thenecessary materials to complete the task(e.g., paper; crayons) <strong>and</strong> asked to draw


246 chapter 10 projective techniquesBox 10.3Research Note: Interpretive Approaches to Sentence Completion TechniquesThe author <strong>of</strong> the Hart Sentence CompletionTest for <strong>Child</strong>ren (Hart, 1986) providedan interesting summary <strong>of</strong> various interpretiveapproaches to sentence completion techniques(SCTs) in the clinical assessment <strong>of</strong>children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.Strategy 1The most common interpretive approachto SCT is to review each item’s content <strong>and</strong>obtain clinical impressions about a child’s personalitydynamics. The assessor searches forpatterns, clues, <strong>and</strong> thought processes <strong>and</strong> generateshypotheses consistent with the assessor’sview <strong>of</strong> human behavior. This approach <strong>of</strong>tenleads to different interpretations <strong>of</strong> the sameset <strong>of</strong> responses by different clinicians. What isviewed as important or diagnostic will dependon the assessor’s theoretical orientation.Strategy 2The next approach places sentence stems intoclusters with similar item content that arejudged to elicit similar psychological information.The assessor determines if there areimportant patterns <strong>of</strong> responses within a cluster<strong>of</strong> items. However, like the first strategy,the interpretations are heavily dependent onthe assessor’s orientation. Which items determinea meaningful cluster <strong>and</strong> what constitutesan important pattern <strong>of</strong> responses withina cluster are based on an assessor’s theoreticalorientation.Strategy 3The third approach is exemplified by theRotter Incomplete Sentence Blank (Rotter& Rafferty, 1950) <strong>and</strong> is based on psychodynamictheory. Each response is analyzedaccording to the degree <strong>of</strong> intrapsychic conflictevident in the response. It is a quantitativescoring system in which the severity <strong>of</strong>the conflict is rated as negative, neutral, orpositive.Strategy 4The fourth approach involves comparingresponses on an SCT to some predeterminedcriteria. This approach attempts to limit theunreliability inherent in the other strategiesby minimizing the reliance on the assessor’stheoretical orientation. An example <strong>of</strong> thisapproach is the Hart Sentence CompletionTest for <strong>Child</strong>ren (Hart, 1972). In a st<strong>and</strong>ardizationsample, a large pool <strong>of</strong> responseswas obtained for each sentence stem. Theresponses were placed into positive, negative,<strong>and</strong> neutral categories by expert judges. Ineach rating category, representative responseswere identified for each sentence stem to aidassessors in making their determination <strong>of</strong> thevalence <strong>of</strong> a child’s response.Source: Hart, D. H. (1986). “The Sentence Completion Techniques,” in II. M. Kn<strong>of</strong>f (Ed.), The <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong><strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> <strong>Personality</strong>, New York: Guilford.specific pictures. During the inquiry phase,a series <strong>of</strong> questions are asked to clarifythe persons <strong>and</strong> objects in the picture, tounderst<strong>and</strong> their actions <strong>and</strong> motives, <strong>and</strong>to have the child describe in more detailwhy he or she chose to draw the picturein the way he or she did. Both the drawingitself <strong>and</strong> the child’s description <strong>of</strong> it areused to generate hypotheses about potentialthemes that may provide insight into thechild’s emotional (e.g., anxiety) <strong>and</strong> social(e.g., family relations) functioning.Draw-a-Person TechniqueOne <strong>of</strong> the most popular drawing techniquesfor children is the Draw-a-PersonTechnique (DAPT), made popular by aseminal publication by Koppitz (1968).In this technique a child is simply given a


chapter 10 projective techniques247paper <strong>and</strong> lead pencil <strong>and</strong> asked to draw apicture <strong>of</strong> a whole person. It is left up tothe child the type <strong>of</strong> person to be drawn(e.g., age, gender, race, context <strong>of</strong> figure).After finishing this first drawing, the childis given another sheet <strong>of</strong> paper <strong>and</strong> askedto draw another person <strong>of</strong> the opposite sexfrom the first drawing.Koppitz (1968) provides one <strong>of</strong> the mostexplicit guides to interpreting children’s figuredrawings. She organizes her approacharound three basic questions. The firstquestion is, How did the child draw the figure?Such content analysis is the focus notonly <strong>of</strong> the Koppitz system, but <strong>of</strong> mostinterpretive systems <strong>of</strong> children’s drawing.In the Koppitz system, the figure is viewedas reflecting a child’s self-concept. Koppitzdeveloped a series <strong>of</strong> 30 Emotional Indicators(EI) that were rare in children’s drawings,that were independent <strong>of</strong> age, <strong>and</strong> thatdifferentiated undisturbed from maladjustedchildren. Examples <strong>of</strong> EI in the Koppitz systeminclude poor integration <strong>of</strong> parts, slanting<strong>of</strong> figure by 15° or more, omission <strong>of</strong>mouth, body, or limbs, <strong>and</strong> monster or grotesquefigures. Figure size is another EI thatis not only a part <strong>of</strong> the Koppitz system, butis included in many interpretive systems <strong>and</strong>is considered to be a key indicator <strong>of</strong> a child’sself-esteem. Small figures are interpreted asindicating low self-esteem (2 in. or less inheight) <strong>and</strong> large expansive figures (9 in. ormore in height) are interpreted as indicatinghigh levels <strong>of</strong> self-esteem. Box 10.4 summarizes,in more detail, Koppitz’s EI.Box 10.4Further Discussion <strong>of</strong> the Koppitz Emotional Indicators for Human FigureDrawingsAs mentioned in the text, the writings <strong>of</strong> Koppitz(1968, 1983) have been quite influentialin the interpretation <strong>of</strong> human figure drawingsfor children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. A key element toher projective interpretation <strong>of</strong> drawings is thepresence or absence <strong>of</strong> 30 Emotional Indicators(EI). Koppitz’s El were chosen based on(1) their utility in differentiating disturbedfrom nondisturbed children, (2) their lowprevalence (less than 6%) in the drawings <strong>of</strong>nondisturbed children, <strong>and</strong> (3) their occurringindependent <strong>of</strong> age.The EIs can be divided into three broad categories: Quality Signs, Special Features, <strong>and</strong>OmissionsQuality Signs Special Features OmissionsPoor integration <strong>of</strong> parts Tiny head No eyesShading <strong>of</strong> face Crossed eyes No noseShading <strong>of</strong> h<strong>and</strong>s & neck Presence <strong>of</strong> teeth No mouthAsymmetry <strong>of</strong> limbs Short arms No bodySlanting figures Long arms No armsTiny figure Arms clinging to body No legsBig figure Big h<strong>and</strong>s No feetTransparencies <strong>of</strong> major body parts H<strong>and</strong>s cut <strong>of</strong>f No neckLegs pressed togetherGenitalsMonster/grotesque figuresMultiple figures drawn spontaneouslyClouds(Continues)


248 chapter 10 projective techniquesBox 10.4 (Continued)Koppitz explains that the EI are notscores but are clinical signs that mayreveal underlying attitudes <strong>and</strong> characteristics<strong>of</strong> the child (Koppitz, 1983). Thereis evidence that the EIs occur at a greaterfrequency in the drawings <strong>of</strong> emotionallydisturbed than nondisturbed children (seeFinch & Belter, 1991). However, Koppitzdescribes the difficulty in interpreting EIsfor the individual child:“There is no relationship between an EI<strong>and</strong> overt behavior. For instance, long arms<strong>and</strong> big h<strong>and</strong>s both reflect aggressiveness<strong>and</strong> anger, yet children who show these twoEI on their drawings may act very differently.One boy may reveal his anger by refusingto do his homework or by truantingfrom school, another child may be physicallyaggressive to peers, while a third childmay withdraw <strong>and</strong> soil himself when angry.The Human Figure Drawings indicate thatall three pupils are angry; the youngsters’behaviors demonstrate how they expressthis anger. It is also important to recognizethat different EI can reflect the same attitude.Thus, a girl may show acute anxietyby shading the body <strong>and</strong> face <strong>of</strong> her HumanFigure Drawing <strong>and</strong> by omitting thearms. When she makes another drawingsome time later, she may omit the figure’snose <strong>and</strong> h<strong>and</strong>s <strong>and</strong> may draw a dark cloudabove the figure. Similarly, a single EI mayhave different meanings depending on thesituation. For example, a tiny figure mayreflect underlying timidity or shyness, orit may indicate withdrawal or depression.The true meaning <strong>of</strong> a given EI can only bedetermined by other aspects <strong>of</strong> the personalitybattery, from observing the child indifferent settings, <strong>and</strong> from studying his orher developmental <strong>and</strong> social background”(Koppitz 1983, p. 423).The second question around whichthe Koppitz interpretation is organized is,Whom does the child draw? Most childrentend to draw figures that are <strong>of</strong> the samegender as themselves (Cummings, 1986;Finch & Belter, 1993). Based on these findings,Koppitz considered a child’s drawingan opposite-sex figure on his or her firstdrawing to be diagnostic, either <strong>of</strong> problemsin gender identity or as a reflection<strong>of</strong> loneliness <strong>and</strong> isolation. There is also atendency to view the figure as an indicator<strong>of</strong> the child’s image <strong>of</strong> his or her own body(Cummings, 1986).The final question in the Koppitzinterpretive system is, What is the childtrying to express via the drawing? A child’sself-figure may reflect his or her self-perceptions,or a drawing <strong>of</strong> someone elsemay reflect attitudes or conflicts towardthis person. Koppitz notes that a child’sdrawing may (1) be a reflection <strong>of</strong> a child’swish, fantasy, or ideal; (2) be an expression<strong>of</strong> real attitudes or conflicts; or (3)be a mixture <strong>of</strong> both. To help clarify thisissue, many assessors note either a child’sspontaneous verbalizations about a figureor ask the child to tell a story about thefigure. As noted above, the assessor mayfollow up with an inquiry phase in whichhe or she asks specific questions about thefigure such as, Who is he/she? or Whomwere you thinking about while you weredrawing? or What is he/she thinkingabout? or How does he/she feel?House–Tree–PersonA second projective drawing technique isthe House-Tree-Person (HTP) technique(Cummings, 1986). In this technique thechild is asked to draw a house, a tree, <strong>and</strong>a person. The order is always the same <strong>and</strong>the drawings are done on separate sheets<strong>of</strong> paper. After the drawing, children are


chapter 10 projective techniques249asked a series <strong>of</strong> questions to give them anopportunity to describe <strong>and</strong> interpret theobjects that were drawn (Cummings, 1986;Koppitz, 1983). According to one <strong>of</strong> theoriginators <strong>of</strong> the HTP technique, the threefigures give insight into different facets <strong>of</strong> achild’s functioning (Hammer, 1958). Thehouse is thought to elicit feelings associatedwith the child’s home situation <strong>and</strong> familialrelationship. In contrast, the tree is thoughtto elicit deeper <strong>and</strong> unconscious feelingsabout the child <strong>and</strong> his or her relationshipswith the environment. Unlike the self-portrait,the tree is thought to have less pull forconscious self-descriptions <strong>and</strong> therefore toinvolve a greater level <strong>of</strong> projection. And,finally, the drawing <strong>of</strong> a person is thoughtto reflect more <strong>of</strong> a conscious or semiconsciousview <strong>of</strong> the child’s self, the child’sideal self, or a significant other.Kinetic Family DrawingA third common projective drawing techniquethat is used in the assessment <strong>of</strong> children<strong>and</strong> adolescents is the Kinetic FamilyDrawing. In this technique a child is askedto “Draw a picture <strong>of</strong> everyone in yourfamily, including you, doing something”(Burns & Kaufman, 1970, p. 5). Theseinstructions emphasize the family engagingin some activity, hence the term kinetic.As was the case for the HTP technique,there is an inquiry phase in which a childis asked to describe <strong>and</strong> explain his or herdrawing (Cummings, 1986). The first part<strong>of</strong> the inquiry typically involves the childexplaining who each figure is (e.g., name,relationship to the child, age). The child isthen asked to describe all the figures, whatthey are doing in the picture, how they arefeeling, <strong>and</strong> what they are thinking about.After these initial descriptive questions, thechild is asked to tell a story about the drawing,saying what happened immediatelybefore the actions depicted in the drawingtook place <strong>and</strong> what happens next. Finally,the child is asked to describe anything thathe or she would change about the pictureif he or she could.The popularity <strong>of</strong> the KFD lies in itsability to assess a child’s perceptions <strong>of</strong> hisor her family in a fun <strong>and</strong> nonthreateningway. Burns <strong>and</strong> Kaufman (1970) outlinea three-part interpretive process that isheavily dependent, not just on the drawing,but on the inquiry phase that follows.The first part <strong>of</strong> the interpretive processis the analysis <strong>of</strong> the actions portrayed inthe drawing. They not only refer to themovements between people but the energy(e.g., avoidance, conflict, nurturance) <strong>and</strong>emotion (e.g., love, anxiety, anger) capturedin the picture. The next part <strong>of</strong> theinterpretive process deals with the style<strong>of</strong> the family drawing. Style refers to thepatterns <strong>of</strong> interactions among significantfamily members <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>ten reflects a child’sdefense system (e.g., denial, isolation). Thefinal stage <strong>of</strong> the interpretation is the symbolicinterpretation, which is analogous tothe content interpretations <strong>of</strong> other projectivedrawing techniques.Psychometric Cautions forDrawing TechniquesAs with most other projective techniques,the best method <strong>of</strong> validating projectivedrawings has been hotly debated. Ina review <strong>of</strong> the psychometric properties<strong>of</strong> drawing techniques, Cummings (1986)found that the lack <strong>of</strong> explicit scoring<strong>and</strong> interpretive guidelines for projectivedrawings has caused most systems to havepoor reliability. Even for those systems inwhich high reliability estimates have beenobtained, correlations between drawings<strong>and</strong> other measures <strong>of</strong> a child’s adjustmenthave not been consistently shown (Joiner,Schmidt, & Barnett, 1996). Most studieshave found that clinicians are unable todistinguish clinically identified childrenfrom nonclinical controls using projective


250 chapter 10 projective techniquesdrawings. Of great concern is the use <strong>of</strong>projective drawings to detect child sexualabuse. Summaries <strong>of</strong> this research have notbeen able to find indicators from drawingsthat have consistently <strong>and</strong> reliably differentiatedabused from non-abused childrenacross multiple samples (Garb, Wood, &Nezworski, 2000).This inability to demonstrate thevalidity <strong>of</strong> projective drawings has led someauthors to suggest that the use <strong>of</strong> drawingsin clinical assessments <strong>of</strong> children is unethical(Martin, 1983). This strong stancehas sparked a lively debate (Kn<strong>of</strong>f, 1983).It is clear that content analyses <strong>of</strong> drawingshave rarely been shown to predictovert behavior, yet many users still try tomake behavioral predictions (e.g., aggression,anxiety, history <strong>of</strong> sexual abuse) fromdrawing techniques. A quote from Cummings’s(1986) review <strong>of</strong> projective drawingresearch seems to summarize a sensibleway to view assessment with projectivedrawings <strong>and</strong> possibly a good way to viewprojective testing in general:“The greatest value associated with projectivedrawings does not lie in the graphicsymbols represented on the paper. Rather,the value <strong>of</strong> the technique may be in thepractitioner’s opportunity to observe the examinee’sbehavior while drawing. Drawingsprovide a nonthreatening beginning pointwhich should lead to an in-depth exploration<strong>of</strong> attitudes, feelings, <strong>and</strong> beliefs via thesynthesis <strong>of</strong> direct interviews, third-partyinterviews, observations, <strong>and</strong> test data” (pp.238–239).ConclusionsIn this chapter we have outlined some <strong>of</strong> themajor issues in the debate <strong>of</strong> when <strong>and</strong> howto use projective testing in the clinical assessment<strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. As withmost assessment techniques, the problemwith projective testing lies not in the techniquesthemselves, but in the inappropriatepurposes for which they are <strong>of</strong>ten employed.This issue is exacerbated in the use <strong>of</strong> projectivetechniques because <strong>of</strong> wide-spreaddisagreement over the basic nature <strong>of</strong> thesetechniques. There is considerable debateover which psychological processes theyare designed to measure, <strong>and</strong> there is lack<strong>of</strong> agreement over what method <strong>of</strong> interpretation(e.g., clinical or psychometric) ismost appropriate for a given technique. Themost important goal <strong>of</strong> this chapter was toprovide the reader with a clear discussion <strong>of</strong>these issues so that projectives can be usedappropriately, with the assessor clearly recognizingthe limitations <strong>of</strong> whichever interpretiveapproach is used.We have also summarized some <strong>of</strong> themajor methods <strong>of</strong> projective testing that areused with children. We have discussed inkblottechniques, story-telling techniques,sentence-completion techniques, <strong>and</strong> projectivedrawings. Space limitations preventan exhaustive review <strong>of</strong> specific techniques<strong>and</strong> interpretive systems. However, we haveattempted to provide selected examples <strong>of</strong>each type <strong>of</strong> projective method as a basis fordeveloping greater expertise in the use <strong>and</strong>interpretation <strong>of</strong> these techniques throughfurther didactic <strong>and</strong> clinical training.Chapter Summary1. Projective techniques have been thefocus <strong>of</strong> much controversy. However,they remain the most frequently usedmethod <strong>of</strong> psychological assessment.2. Much <strong>of</strong> the debate over projectivetechniques stems from the confusionover what the projective techniqueswere designed to measure <strong>and</strong> how tobest evaluate their usefulness.(a) The first area <strong>of</strong> confusion is whetherprojective techniques obtain samples<strong>of</strong> behavior or whether they assessunconscious personality dynamics.


chapter 10 projective techniques251(b) The second focus <strong>of</strong> debate iswhether projectives are ways <strong>of</strong>obtaining highly individualizedclinical impressions or whether theyare psychometric tests that shouldbe evaluated by traditional indexes<strong>of</strong> reliability <strong>and</strong> validity.3. The Exner Comprehensive Systemprovides a structured method foradministering, scoring, <strong>and</strong> interpretingresponses to Rorschach inkblots.(a) The inkblots are administered intwo phases: a free association phase<strong>and</strong> an inquiry phase.(b) Detailed scoring <strong>of</strong> responses provides90 possible scores to be used ininterpretation.(c) Normative samples <strong>of</strong> children havedocumented several age trends inchildren’s Rorschach responses.(d) Scores from the Exner system haveproven to be reliable.(e) The validity <strong>of</strong> scores for children hasnot been well established, although ithas been difficult to determine themost appropriate way <strong>of</strong> testing thevalidity <strong>of</strong> Rorschach scores.4. Thematic story-telling techniques providea child with a relatively ambiguouspicture <strong>and</strong> require that the child “makeup a story” about the picture.5. Most interpretive systems <strong>of</strong> thematictests use a two-part interpretation <strong>of</strong> achild’s stories. The first step interpretsthe process <strong>of</strong> a child’s stories (e.g.,coherence <strong>of</strong> stories) <strong>and</strong> the second stepinterprets the content <strong>of</strong> the stories.6. A popular thematic test for use withpreadolescent children is the RATC.(a) The RATC contains pictures depictingcommon situations that childrenexperience <strong>and</strong> provides a st<strong>and</strong>ardscoring system for children’sresponses.(b) The explicit scoring instructionsallow for reliable scoring <strong>of</strong> children’sresponses.(c) The st<strong>and</strong>ardization sample onwhich norm-referenced scores arebased is quite small <strong>and</strong> its representativenessis questionable.(d) Existing evidence for the validity <strong>of</strong>RATC scores is quite limited.7. Sentence-completion techniques providethe child with a sentence stem<strong>and</strong> require the child to complete thesentence.8. Most sentence-completion techniquesdo not have st<strong>and</strong>ardized scoring proceduresfor interpreting children’sresponses. It is left to clinical judgmenthow to interpret the content <strong>of</strong> theresponses.9. Drawing techniques, such as theDraw-a-Person Technique, the House-Tree-Person, <strong>and</strong> the Kinetic FamilyDrawings are popular for assessingchildren because drawing is a familiar<strong>and</strong> enjoyable exercise for children.10. Despite their popularity, scoresderived from children’s drawingshave not been highly associated withother indicators <strong>of</strong> a child’s emotional,behavioral, or social functioning.


C h a p t e r 1 1Structured Diagnostic InterviewsChapter Questionsl What are the major differences betweenstructured <strong>and</strong> unstructured interviews?l What are the major similarities <strong>and</strong>differences among the most commonstructured diagnostic interviews used toassess children?l What information can diagnostic interviewsprovide that cannot be obtainedfrom other assessment techniques?l What are some important guidelines forthe appropriate use <strong>of</strong> diagnostic interviewsin the assessment <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong>adolescents?History<strong>Clinical</strong> interviews have a prominent placein the history <strong>of</strong> psychological assessment.The face-to-face verbal dialog betweenassessor <strong>and</strong> client is the prototypicalformat for most clinical enterprises. Historically,the most common type <strong>of</strong> clinicalinterview has been the unstructuredinterview. In the unstructured interview,the interviewer determines what questionsshould be asked, how the questions shouldbe framed, what follow-up questionsshould be asked, <strong>and</strong> what are acceptableresponses from the client. This unstructuredformat is quite consistent with theclinical approach to assessment, which wasdiscussed in the previous chapter on projectivetechniques. It allows the assessmentto be tailored to the individual needs <strong>of</strong> theclient <strong>and</strong> relies heavily on the individualclinician’s orientation <strong>and</strong> expertise.However, the unreliability inherent inunstructured interviews generates somesignificant problems. The results <strong>and</strong> interpretation<strong>of</strong> such interviews tend to behighly idiosyncratic to the clinician conductingthe interview. This unreliability isespecially problematic for research. Hence,many clinical assessors have developed more253P.J. Frick et al., <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Behavior,DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-0641-0_11, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010


254 chapter 11 structured diagnostic interviewsstructured diagnostic interview schedulesthat provide a clear <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardized formatfrom which to conduct the clinical interview.This st<strong>and</strong>ardization helps address thekey problems associated with unstructuredinterviews; however, one must recall thatthese interviews are valid only for certainpurposes (e.g., collecting data on specificsymptoms) <strong>and</strong> relatively less valid for others(e.g., treatment planning for a specificclient; Mash & Hunsley, 2005).Initially, most <strong>of</strong> these instruments weredesigned for use with adults <strong>and</strong> were primarilyused in research. Two <strong>of</strong> the betterknown early interview schedules were theFeighner Research Diagnostic Criteria(Feighner et al., 1972), which later becamethe NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule(DIS; Robins et al., 1981), <strong>and</strong> the Schedulefor Affective Disorders <strong>and</strong> Schizophrenia(SADS; Endicott & Spitzer, 1978).Over the past two decades, structureddiagnostic interviews have moved frombeing strictly research instruments to beinga part <strong>of</strong> many clinical assessments. Inaddition, several interview schedules havebeen developed for use with children <strong>and</strong>adolescents. This chapter focuses on theseinterview schedules <strong>and</strong> their potential rolein the clinical assessment <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong>adolescents.OverviewStructured diagnostic interviews consist <strong>of</strong>a set <strong>of</strong> questions that the assessor asks theinformant (e.g., parent or child). There areexplicit guidelines on how responses are tobe scored. Interview questions generallystart with a stem question (e.g., Have youbeen involved in many physical fights?).If the stem is answered affirmatively, thenfollow-up questions are asked to determineother relevant parameters such asfrequency (e.g., How many fights have youbeen in, in the past year?), severity (e.g.,Have you ever used a weapon in a fight?),onset (e.g., When was the first time yougot into a fight?), <strong>and</strong> impairment (e.g.,Has fighting caused problems for you atschool, home, or with kids your age?). Anexample <strong>of</strong> the question format from theNIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedulefor <strong>Child</strong>ren-Version 4 (DISC-IV; Shafferet al., 2000) is provided in Box 11.1.Box 11.1Example <strong>of</strong> the DISC-IV QuestionFormatThe following is an example <strong>of</strong> the stem/follow-up question format used by mostdiagnostic interview schedules. This questionwas taken from the questions assessing MajorDepressive Disorder from the DISC-IV(Shaffer et al., 2000).“I’m now going to ask some questionsabout feeling sad <strong>and</strong> unhappy.”1. In the last year, was there a time whenyou <strong>of</strong>ten felt sad or depressed for a longtime each day?IF YES,A. Was there a time in the last year whenyou felt sad or depressed for a longtime each day?IF NO, GO TO QUESTION 2B. Would you say that you felt that wayfor most <strong>of</strong> the day?C. Was there a time when you felt sad ordepressed almost every day?IF NO, GO TO QUESTION 2IF YES,D. In the last year, were there twoweeks in a row when you felt sad ordepressed almost every day?IF NO, GO TO QUESTION 2E. When you were sad or depressed,did you feel better if something goodhappened or was about to happen toyou?F. Now, what about the last four weeks?Have you felt sad or depressed?


chapter 11 structured diagnostic interviews255Commonalities Across InterviewSchedulesIn Table 11.1, we summarize the basiccharacteristics <strong>of</strong> some <strong>of</strong> the most commonlyused diagnostic interviews for children.It should be noted that the interviewsare continuously updated to keep up withchanging diagnostic criteria or modifiedfor some specific application (e.g.,outcome measure for treatment <strong>of</strong> childhooddepression, screening device for anepidemiological study <strong>of</strong> child disorders).Therefore, many <strong>of</strong> the interviews havemultiple versions <strong>and</strong> are continually beingrevised. The contents <strong>of</strong> these interviewsare all based on the criteria specified in theDiagnostic <strong>and</strong> Statistical Manual <strong>of</strong> MentalDisorders, starting with the third edition(DSM-III; American Psychiatric Association,1980) <strong>and</strong> continuing through itsmost recent revision (DSM-IV-TR; AmericanPsychiatric Association, 2000).Which DSM disorders are assessed isgenerally quite similar across the differentinterview schedules. All <strong>of</strong> the interviews canbe used to assess for the disruptive behaviordisorders, affective disorders, <strong>and</strong> anxietydisorders in children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. Each<strong>of</strong> these interviews also allows for at leasta brief screening for schizophrenia. Themajority <strong>of</strong> structured interviews also allowfor an assessment <strong>of</strong> substance use, elimination,<strong>and</strong> eating disorders. Tic disordersare covered exclusively by CAPA, DISC-IV,<strong>and</strong> K-SADS, whereas the ADIS containsunique screening questions for mental retardation,learning disorder, <strong>and</strong> somat<strong>of</strong>ormsymptomatology. The CAPA is unique in itsdetailed assessment <strong>of</strong> sleep disorder symptoms.Most interviews organize questions bydiagnosis. However, the Interview Schedulefor <strong>Child</strong>ren <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong>s (ISCA;Sherrill & Kovacs, 2000) provides a symptom-orientedinterview format with itemsclustered by content (e.g., impaired concentration)<strong>and</strong> topic area (e.g., mental status)rather than specific diagnostic criteria.In order to promote multi-informantassessments, most interviews contain parallelforms to ask identical questions toboth the child <strong>and</strong> a parent. There is evenan experimental teacher version <strong>of</strong> theDISC 2.3, an earlier version <strong>of</strong> the DISC,that was used in the DSM-IV field trialsfor the disruptive behavior disorders(Frick et al., 1994).A recent trend in the most widely usedstructured interview schedules is the development<strong>of</strong> computer-administered versions.Currently, the DISC-IV, the DICA,the CAPA, <strong>and</strong> the Dominic-R all haveformats that can be administered by computer.The computer format was designedto enhance the reliability <strong>of</strong> the interviewsby increasing the ease <strong>of</strong> administration<strong>and</strong> data collection. Similar to the pencil<strong>and</strong>-paperversion, the computer format isdesigned to be administered by an examiner.The examiner reads items from thecomputer screen <strong>and</strong> enters the patient’sresponses. The computer quickly scores<strong>and</strong> stores responses, selects the appropriatefollow-up questions, <strong>and</strong> skips out <strong>of</strong>diagnostic sections when the respondentfails to meet a certain threshold <strong>of</strong> severity. Also,the computer-administered interviews haveprograms that quickly score the interview<strong>and</strong> provide various summary scores (e.g.,symptom indexes, number <strong>of</strong> diagnosticthresholds met) that aid in the interpretation<strong>of</strong> the results.Most <strong>of</strong> the interviews were designedto assess children <strong>and</strong> adolescents betweenthe ages <strong>of</strong> 8 <strong>and</strong> 17. Some interviewsreport applicability to younger childrenwith parents as informants (e.g., Vallaet al., 2000). However, there is someevidence that the reliability <strong>of</strong> children’sself-report on diagnostic interviews islow before age 9 (Edelbrock et al., 1985;Hodges & Zeman, 1993). The length <strong>of</strong>time that it takes to administer a diagnosticinterview is heavily dependent on thechild being assessed. Because <strong>of</strong> the stem/follow-up question format, children with


256 chapter 11 structured diagnostic interviewsTable 11.1 Overview <strong>of</strong> Structured Diagnostic Interviews for <strong>Child</strong>renInterview Time Ages Time Frame Informants CommentsAnxiety Diagnostic Interviewfor DSM-IV (ADIS;Albano & Silverman,1996; Silverman, Saavedra,& Pina, 2001)<strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong>Psychiatric <strong>Assessment</strong>(CAPA; Angold & Costello,2000)<strong>Child</strong>ren’s Interview forPsychiatric Symptoms(ChIPS; Weller et al.,2000)Diagnostic Interview for<strong>Child</strong>ren <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong>s(DICA; Reich, 2000;Reich et al., 1982)Diagnostic InterviewSchedule for <strong>Child</strong>ren(DISC-IV; Shaffer et al.,1993, 2000)60–90min60–120min30–60min60–120min90–120min7–17 Present <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> Parent Semi-structured interview focusing on childhoodanxiety disorders. Categorical <strong>and</strong> dimensionalsymptom assessment with diagnostic summary thatplaces diagnoses in hierarchical format. Support forreliability <strong>and</strong> validity for anxiety disorder diagnoses.Limited validity data for other diagnostic categories.9–17 Present <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> Parent Semi-structured interview with flexibility in wording<strong>and</strong> in probing for relevant symptoms. Can beused by trained lay interviewers. Includes Likerttypesymptom severity scores. Separate section forimpairment. Good reliability except for modestestimates for ODD <strong>and</strong> CD. Some validity evidencefor diagnoses relative to external criteria.6–18 Present <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> Parent Highly structured format. Can be used by trained layinterviewers. Brief with broad symptom coverage.Simplified language for use with younger children.Limited research, but some evidence <strong>of</strong> good sensitivity<strong>and</strong> specificity.6–18 Lifetime <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> Parent Structured with changes toward more flexible format.Good reliability with some evidence <strong>of</strong> validity.Evidence supporting use categorically <strong>and</strong> dimensionally.Used cross-culturally <strong>and</strong> with a variety <strong>of</strong>populations.6–17 Present (separatelifetime module)<strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> parent (experimentalteacher versionavailable)Highly structured format designed for use by trainedlay interviewers. Good reliability at symptom <strong>and</strong>diagnosis level. Covers many diagnostic domains.Frequently used inepidemiological studies. Diagnoses from parentversion related to greater impairment.


chapter 11 structured diagnostic interviews257Dominic-R (Valla,Bergeron, & Smolla,2000)Pictorial Instrument for<strong>Child</strong>ren <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong>s(PICA-III-R; Ernst,Cookus, & Moravec,2000)Schedule for AffectiveDisorders <strong>and</strong> Schizophreniafor School-Aged<strong>Child</strong>ren (K-SADS;Ambrosini, 2000; Chamberset al., 1985; Tillmanet al., 2003)15–25min40–60min6–11 Present <strong>Child</strong> Highly structured pictorial interview with verbalsymptom descriptions. Designed for use by lay interviewerswith minimal training. Promising reliability<strong>and</strong> validity data. Should be interpreted with cautionif used with young children.6–16 Present <strong>Child</strong> Semi-structured pictorial interview with verbalsymptom descriptors. Designed for use by experiencedclinicians. Categorical <strong>and</strong> dimensional symptomassessment <strong>of</strong> 14 diagnostic categories. Shouldbe interpreted with caution if used with youngchildren. Limited psychometric data. Authors notethe need for update to reflect DSM-IV criteria.90 min 6–18 Present <strong>and</strong> lifetime(in separate<strong>and</strong> combinedinterview formats)<strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> Parent Semi-structured interview originally designed t<strong>of</strong>ocus on depression. Variety <strong>of</strong> versions that varyin length. Low level <strong>of</strong> structure necessitates useby clinicians. Includes ratings <strong>of</strong> symptom severity.Widely used in a variety <strong>of</strong> populations with goodpsychometric evidence.


258 chapter 11 structured diagnostic interviewsmore symptoms will require more interviewtime because <strong>of</strong> the need to ask morefollow-up questions. However, as is evidentfrom Box 11.1, the average time toadminister the interviews does not varymuch across the different schedules <strong>and</strong>lasts typically from 60 to 90 min.Major Sources <strong>of</strong> VariationAcross Interview SchedulesFrom the previous discussion it is clear thatthe various interview schedules probablyhave more similarities than differences.However, one <strong>of</strong> the major differencesacross schedules is the degree <strong>of</strong> structureinherent in the interview format. All <strong>of</strong> theinterviews provide some degree <strong>of</strong> structure<strong>and</strong> give guidelines for st<strong>and</strong>ardizedadministration <strong>and</strong> scoring. However, thereis substantial variation in the amount <strong>of</strong>“leeway” given the assessor across the variousinterviews. For example, the K-SADSis one <strong>of</strong> the least structured <strong>of</strong> the interviews.The original manual for administrationincludes the following instructions:“The K-SADS supplies a series <strong>of</strong> questionsaddressed to the child for each item to berated. The aim is not to oblige the rater toask all <strong>of</strong> the questions. They serve as a guidefor questions which have been found mosthelpful <strong>and</strong> informative. The rater shouldask as many questions as necessary to arriveat a well-documented rating. Needless tosay, probing should be as neutral as possible<strong>and</strong> leading questions should be avoided”(Puig-Antich & Chambers, 1978, p. 2).In contrast, the DISC was designed to havea high degree <strong>of</strong> structure in administration.The manual for its administrationincludes the following instructions:“The DISC symptom questions are designedto be read exactly as written. There is verylimited scope for independent questioning.DO NOT deviate from the prescribedquestion sequence. DO NOT make up yourown questions because you think you have abetter way <strong>of</strong> getting at the same information,or because you think the question ispoorly worded” (Fisher et al., 1992, p. 31).The trade-<strong>of</strong>f between leeway <strong>and</strong> structureis obvious. Less structure allows theassessor to tailor the interview accordingto the needs <strong>of</strong> the individual client. However,these interviews generally require agreater degree <strong>of</strong> experience to administer<strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>ten have lower levels <strong>of</strong> reliability(Gutterman, O’Brien, & Young, 1987;Hodges & Zeman, 1993).Another major variation among thestructured interviews for children is thetime frame used to assess symptoms <strong>and</strong>diagnoses. All <strong>of</strong> the interviews assesswhether problems are currently evident.This is called a present episode frame <strong>of</strong> reference.Most interviews consider present episodesto be within the previous six months,although in some instances the time framemay be as short as within the last two weeks(e.g., ISCA for emotional disorders) or aslong as within the last year (e.g., DISC-IVfor Conduct Disorder). Of note, the CAPArestricts the assessment <strong>of</strong> symptoms tothe previous three months due to concernswith the reliability <strong>of</strong> memory in children<strong>and</strong> adults over longer time intervals. Similarly,the Dominic-R does not obtain anytemporal information, such as onset <strong>and</strong>duration <strong>of</strong> symptoms, given concerns as tothe validity <strong>of</strong> such information in youngchildren. Nevertheless, the major source<strong>of</strong> variation is whether or not interviewschedules are limited to present episodes.A number <strong>of</strong> interviews restrict the focus<strong>of</strong> assessment to the present episode timeframe (e.g., CAPA <strong>and</strong> ChIPS). However,an increasing number <strong>of</strong> interviews providefor the assessment <strong>of</strong> both present <strong>and</strong> lifetimediagnoses. For example, the DISC-IV providesa more recently incorporated wholelifemodule assessing for whether or not a


chapter 11 structured diagnostic interviews259child has exhibited symptoms <strong>of</strong> diagnosessince age five but prior to the current year(Shaffer et al., 2000). Similarly, there arelifetime formats for both the ISCA <strong>and</strong>K-SADS. The DICA is unique in its exclusivefocus on lifetime diagnoses.A third source <strong>of</strong> variation within theinterview schedules is the answer format. Formost interviews, the interview responses arecoded into a categorical format (yes, no).This categorical format is consistent withthe DSM orientation in which symptomsare considered either present or absent.In contrast, the ADIS, CAPA, ISCA,PICA-III-R, <strong>and</strong> K-SADS have answerformats that can be placed on a Likert-typescale that allows one to rate the severity <strong>of</strong>a symptom. While this format makes itmore difficult to translate responses intoDSM diagnoses, it does not create an artificialdichotomy between the presence orabsence <strong>of</strong> a symptom <strong>and</strong> allows symptomscores to reflect gradations in severity.Evaluation <strong>of</strong> DiagnosticInterviewsAdvantagesStructured interviews share with behaviorrating scales the goal <strong>of</strong> obtaining adetailed description <strong>of</strong> a child’s emotions<strong>and</strong> behaviors from multiple informants.The logical question is: what advantages dothe time-consuming structured interviews<strong>of</strong>fer in comparison to the more time-efficientbehavior rating scales? Some <strong>of</strong> themore important advantages <strong>of</strong> structureddiagnostic interviews follow.(1) Structured interviews are useful inobtaining important parameters <strong>of</strong> achild’s behavior that are not typicallyassessed by most behavior rating scales.Specifically, most interview schedulesprovide questions that elicit informationon the duration <strong>of</strong> a child’s behavioraldifficulties <strong>and</strong> the age at which theproblems began to emerge. This temporalinformation allows one to take adevelopmental perspective in underst<strong>and</strong>inga case, a perspective that hasproven to be crucial for assessing manyforms <strong>of</strong> childhood psychopathology(e.g., Silverthorn & Frick, 1999).(2) Interviews also allow one to determinethe temporal sequencing among behaviors.For example, it is important in theassessment <strong>of</strong> childhood depression todetermine whether periods <strong>of</strong> sadnessoccurred contiguously with other behaviorsassociated with depression, such assleep disturbances, eating disturbances,or thoughts <strong>of</strong> death (Kazdin, 1988).(3) Most structured interviews assess thelevel <strong>of</strong> impairment associated withbehaviors being reported. Most interviewshave questions that elicit informationon the degree to which a child’sdifficulties are affecting his or herfunctioning in major life arenas (e.g., athome, at school, <strong>and</strong> with peers).(4) Diagnostic interviews also enhancethe correspondence between assessmenttechniques <strong>and</strong> diagnostic criteria. As mentionedpreviously, the most commonstructured interviews were specificallydesigned <strong>and</strong> revised to correspond tothe changing DSM system. Therefore,the usefulness <strong>of</strong> the interview is, inpart, dependent on the usefulness <strong>of</strong>the diagnostic definitions that are beingassessed.This tie between assessment <strong>and</strong> diagnosiscan be advantageous for several reasons.First, it promotes revisions <strong>of</strong> the interviewsto correspond with advances in ourknowledge <strong>of</strong> the basic characteristics <strong>of</strong>child <strong>and</strong> adolescent psychological disorders.Second, it allows one to make adiagnosis based on strict adherence todiagnostic criteria. Either due to theoretical,empirical, or practical reasons


260 chapter 11 structured diagnostic interviews(e.g., insurance reimbursement), manyclinicians attempt to make DSM diagnosesas a result <strong>of</strong> their assessments.Too <strong>of</strong>ten, diagnoses are made based oninformation (e.g., rating scales, projectivetests) that do not directly assess the diagnosticcriteria or through techniques thatare unsystematic in their assessment <strong>of</strong>symptomatology <strong>and</strong> associated features(e.g., unstructured interviews; Klein et al.,2005). As a result, the meaning <strong>of</strong> thediagnosis is ambiguous. Structured diagnosticinterviews do not have this problem.They tend to result in more reliablediagnoses (Silverman & Ollendick, 2005)<strong>and</strong> can actually provide important informationthat may alter diagnostic impressions<strong>and</strong> treatment recommendations(Kashner et al., 2003).(5) Diagnostic interviews are also helpfulin training clinical assessors. As assessorsare developing their competence ininterviewing, it is <strong>of</strong>ten helpful to havean explicit format from which to conductthe interview. It gives the assessora good way to learn the basic characteristics<strong>of</strong> childhood emotional <strong>and</strong>behavioral disorders. After being trainedin administration procedures <strong>and</strong> afterconducting several interviews withactual clients, assessors <strong>of</strong>ten begin tointernalize the diagnostic criteria for themost common disorders <strong>of</strong> childhood.This knowledge can then be applied insituations in which a structured interviewis not possible.DisadvantagesDiagnostic interviews also have a number<strong>of</strong> weaknesses <strong>of</strong> which the clinician shouldbe aware.(1) The time consuming nature <strong>of</strong> theinterviews coupled with some questionas to whether they provide incrementalvalidity in the assessment <strong>of</strong> somedisorders such as ADHD (Pelhamet al., 2005) suggest that structuredinterviews may not be practical oruseful in many situations.(2) In addition, structured interviewsdepend on DSM criteria, which is astrength for assessing well-validatedsyndromes but a weakness for assessingdisorders with a weak empirical basis.(3) Diagnostic interviews are subject tothe same potential reporter biases thatwere discussed in previous chapters onself-report inventories <strong>and</strong> parent <strong>and</strong>teacher behavior rating scales.(4) Making differential diagnoses with theassistance <strong>of</strong> structured interviews stilldoes not directly translate to plans forintervention (Mash & Hunsley, 2005).(5) There is great difficulty in makingnorm-referenced interpretations frominterviews. <strong>Clinical</strong>ly significant levels <strong>of</strong>symptoms are <strong>of</strong>ten based on DSM criteriarather than based on a comparisonwith a representative normative sample.Therefore, the appropriateness <strong>of</strong> clinicalelevations for a given age dependson the appropriateness <strong>of</strong> the diagnosticcriteria for that age. For example, Barkley(1997) has questioned the validity<strong>of</strong> applying the same diagnostic criteria(i.e., the same number <strong>of</strong> symptoms) forADHD across the early-childhood <strong>and</strong>adolescent years based on a documenteddecline in hyperactivity <strong>and</strong> impulsivitysymptoms as children move into adolescence.As a result, the uniform diagnosticthreshold, while appropriate forelementary school-aged children, maybe too conservative for adolescents.Several community studies using anearly version <strong>of</strong> DISC (e.g., Anderson,et al., 1987; Costello, 1989) allow one toview the base rates <strong>of</strong> disorders assessedby the DISC in community samples<strong>of</strong> school-age children. Such work hascontinued with the current versionwith various populations (e.g., Roberts,


chapter 11 structured diagnostic interviews261Roberts, & Xing, 2007). Although thesestudies provide some information onhow a child who meets criteria wouldcompare to others in the general population,this type <strong>of</strong> normative informationis still much more limited than thetype provided by other assessment techniques,most notably behavior ratingscales.(6) Another weakness <strong>of</strong> most diagnosticinterviews is the failure to provide aformat for obtaining information from achild’s teacher. This source <strong>of</strong> informationis crucial in the clinical assessment<strong>of</strong> elementary school-aged children(Loeber et al., 1991). As a result, informationfrom teachers must be obtainedby some other method, thereby makingit difficult to determine if discrepanciesbetween a teacher’s report <strong>and</strong> thereport <strong>of</strong> others are due to real differencesin a child’s classroom behavior orwhether they are due to differences inthe assessment format.Recommendations for Use<strong>of</strong> Structured InterviewsBased on the strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses <strong>of</strong>structured interviews, several recommendationscan be made on their appropriateuse in the clinical assessment <strong>of</strong> children<strong>and</strong> adolescents. First, like any assessmenttechnique, the diagnostic interviewshould never be used alone in aclinical evaluation. It should be one part<strong>of</strong> a comprehensive assessment battery.For example, information from diagnosticinterviews should be supplemented byassessment techniques that provide betternorm-referenced scores (e.g., behavior ratingscales) <strong>and</strong> by assessment techniquesthat provide information on a child’s classroomfunctioning (e.g., behavioral observationsin the school).In addition, the diagnoses derived fromthe diagnostic interviews should be viewedwithin the context <strong>of</strong> the overall assessment.A diagnosis can be viewed similarly to theway an elevation on a behavior rating scaleis interpreted. Specifically, it is one piece <strong>of</strong>information that needs to be integrated withother sources <strong>of</strong> information to develop agood case formulation. Stated simply, diagnosesbased solely on diagnostic interviewsshould not be considered final clinical diagnoses.Such final diagnoses should be basedon an assessor’s integration <strong>of</strong> multiplesources <strong>of</strong>. A case example in which a diagnosticinterview was used as part <strong>of</strong> a comprehensiveassessment battery is provided inBox 11.2.The child’s age is also an importantconsideration in the use <strong>of</strong> structuredinterviews. Generally, the reliability formost interview schedules is low beforethe age <strong>of</strong> 9 for child self-report (Hodges& Zeman, 1993). It seems that the structured,face-to-face dialogue is not appropriatefor assessing very young children.Several interviews have been developedusing pictorial stimuli, rather than relyingpurely on question-<strong>and</strong>-answer format, inan effort to increase the reliability <strong>of</strong> theinterview for younger children.A description <strong>of</strong> two such pictorial interviewsis provided in Box 11.3.Another important issue in interpretinginformation obtained from st<strong>and</strong>ardizedinterview schedules is the consistent findingthat, if the interview is repeated, parents<strong>and</strong> children uniformly report fewer symptomson the second administration (Jensen,Watanabe, & Richters, 1999; Piacentini,et al., 1999). For example, Piacentini, et al.(1999) found that when the DISC-IV wasreadministered to the same sample <strong>of</strong> 245parent–child pairs (age 9–18) 12 days later,parent-reported symptoms dropped 42%,<strong>and</strong> child-reported symptoms dropped 58%,The reasons for this symptom attenuationhave not been conclusively shown. However,they can include (1) sensitization to clinical


262 chapter 11 structured diagnostic interviewsBox 11.2Case Example: The DISC-IV in the Evaluation <strong>of</strong> a 9-Year-Old Girl withAttention-Deficit Hyperactivity DisorderAlexis who is 9 years <strong>and</strong> 3 months old wasreferred for a comprehensive psychologicalevaluation by her parents upon the recommendation<strong>of</strong> her teachers. Her teachers hadreported to Alexis’ parents that she was havingdifficulty paying attention <strong>and</strong> was daydreaming,interrupting others, <strong>and</strong> making carelessmistakes in her work. Her parents requesteda comprehensive evaluation to determine theseverity <strong>and</strong> possible cause <strong>of</strong> these difficulties<strong>and</strong> to get recommendations for possibleinterventions to aid in her school adjustment.Alexis’s background, developmental, <strong>and</strong>medical history were unremarkable. Duringthe testing, Alexis had great difficulty concentrating<strong>and</strong> was easily distracted. She was alsovery fidgety <strong>and</strong> restless. Intellectually, Alexishad much better verbal comprehension abilities,especially in the area <strong>of</strong> verbal reasoning,than nonverbal perceptual-organizational abilities.Consistent with her verbal abilities, Alexisscored in the above average range on measures<strong>of</strong> reading <strong>and</strong> math achievement.Alexis’s emotional <strong>and</strong> behavioral functioningwere assessed through the use <strong>of</strong> structuredinterviews con-ducted with Alexis, her parents,<strong>and</strong> her teachers as well as through ratingscales completed by her parents <strong>and</strong> teacher.The structured interviews were the parentversion <strong>of</strong> the DISC-IV <strong>and</strong> the experimentalteacher version used in the DSM-IV fieldtrials (Frick et al., 1994). The child version wasgiven to Alexis.The following is an excerpt from the report<strong>of</strong> Alexis’s evaluation that illustrates how informationfrom the DISC-IV was integrated withother assessment information.The only problematic areas that emergedfrom this assessment <strong>of</strong> Alexis’ emotional <strong>and</strong>behavioral functioning were significant problems<strong>of</strong> inattention, disorganization, impulsivity, <strong>and</strong>overactivity that seem to be causing her significantproblems in the classroom. On the DISC-IV, Alexis’s parents agreed that such difficultieswere noticeable by the time she was age 5. Alexisherself endorsed many symptoms <strong>of</strong> inattention,but not overactivity, on the DISC-IV. Alexis’steachers described her as being very restless <strong>and</strong>fidgety, being easily distractible, having very disorganized<strong>and</strong> messy work habits, having a hardtime completing tasks, <strong>and</strong> making many carelessmistakes. Results from teacher rating scalesfurther suggested that these behaviors are moresevere than would be typical for children Alexis’sage. These behaviors are consistent with a diagnosis<strong>of</strong> Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder(ADHD). Also consistent with this diagnosis werethe age <strong>of</strong> onset <strong>of</strong> these symptoms as reported byAlexis’s parents. These behaviors associated withADHD seem to be causing significant problemsfor Alexis in school, affecting the amount <strong>and</strong>accuracy <strong>of</strong> her schoolwork.issues leading to a heightened threshold forsymptom reporting, (2) a circumscribedfocus on only the interval between assessmentperiods, (3) statistical regression tothe mean, <strong>and</strong> (4) knowledge that brevity<strong>of</strong> responding will shorten the duration <strong>of</strong>the interview (Piacentini et al., 1999). Thissymptom attenuation is not much <strong>of</strong> an issuein most clinical uses <strong>of</strong> structured interviewsthat do not involve multiple administrations.However, there is also evidence thatthe number <strong>of</strong> symptoms reported declineswithin an interview schedule, such thatparents <strong>and</strong> child tend to report more symptomsfor diagnoses assessed early in theinterview, even if the order <strong>of</strong> assessment isvaried (Jensen et al., 1999).This type <strong>of</strong> symptom attenuation withinan interview is <strong>of</strong> much greater concernbecause it clearly can influence the resultsfrom a typical clinical use <strong>of</strong> structuredinterviews. In an attempt to solve this problem,Edelbrock, Crnic, <strong>and</strong> Bohnert (1999)modified the administration <strong>of</strong> DISC-2.3


chapter 11 structured diagnostic interviews263Box 11.3Research Note: Developing Structured Interviews for Young <strong>Child</strong>renTo increase the usefulness <strong>of</strong> structured interviewsfor assessing younger children, severalauthors have attempted to develop interviewsthat involve pictorial content to eitherreplace or augment the typical question-<strong>and</strong>answerformat for structured interviews. Oneexample <strong>of</strong> this approach is the Dominic-R byValla et al. (2000). The Dominic-R is designedto assess DSM-III-R criteria for anxiety disorders,mood disorders, <strong>and</strong> the disruptivebehavior disorders in children ages 6–11.The interview involves pictures <strong>of</strong> a childnamed Dominic facing situations that arecommon in children’s daily lives. The picturesare accompanied by questions about the visualimage (e.g., “Do you feel sad <strong>and</strong> depressedmost <strong>of</strong> the time, like Dominic?”). There is aversion <strong>of</strong> the Dominic-R, the “Terry Questionnaire,”that employs an African Americancharacter named “Terry,” <strong>and</strong> there are translations<strong>of</strong> the Dominic-R in French, Spanish,<strong>and</strong> German. There is also an InteractiveDominic Questionnaire that is a CD-ROMbasedinteractive cartoon. The Dominic-Rtakes about 15 to 25 minutes to complete<strong>and</strong> is highly structured, which allows it to beadministered by lay interviewers. The reliability<strong>of</strong> the Dominic-R was assessed in a sample<strong>of</strong> 340 community children aged 6 to 11, <strong>and</strong>it revealed reliability coefficients that weremuch improved over other structured interviewswith very young children. An adolescentversion has also been developed (Smolla et al.,2004).For the child version, test-retest reliabilityover 7 to 12 days for diagnoses from the Dominic-Rranged from a kappa <strong>of</strong> .44 to a kappa<strong>of</strong> .69, with most being above .60 (Valla et al.,1997). Also, the diagnoses based on the Dominic-Rwere strongly associated with diagnosesmade by experienced clinicians with kappa valuesranging from .64 to .88 (Valla et al., 2000).Furthermore, research has shown the Dominic-Rto accurately designated children meetingDSM-IV criteria for Conduct Disorder(Arseneault et al., 2005). Therefore, it appearsthat the combination <strong>of</strong> pictorial stimuli <strong>and</strong>verbal stimuli can enhance the reliability <strong>and</strong>validity <strong>of</strong> responses in young children.A similar assessment system, described byErnst, Cookus, <strong>and</strong> Moravec (2000), is calledthe Pictorial Instrument for <strong>Child</strong>ren <strong>and</strong><strong>Adolescent</strong>s (PICA-III-R). The PICA-III-R isa semi-structured interview that includes 137pictures assessing anxiety disorders, mood disorders,disruptive behavior disorders, psychoticdisorders, <strong>and</strong> substance abuse. Therefore,the content <strong>of</strong> the PICA-III-R is somewhatbroader than that <strong>of</strong> the Dominic-R. Like theDominic-R, the PICA-III-R combines pictorialstimuli with verbal questions. However,the verbal questions on the PICA-III-R aremore extensive, <strong>of</strong>ten including many followupquestions (e.g., “Do you ever get like him(e.g., sad)?” “How much?” “Do people tellyou that you look sad?”). Also, the PICA-III-R verbal questions are not meant to be readverbatim. The type <strong>and</strong> degree <strong>of</strong> questioningis left somewhat up to the interviewer. Because<strong>of</strong> all <strong>of</strong> these characteristics, the PICA-III-Rmay be more useful for older childrenwith more severe forms <strong>of</strong> psychopathologycompared to the Dominic-R, <strong>and</strong> itmust be administered by a experienced clinician.Ernst et al. (2000) reported that, ina sample <strong>of</strong> 51 inpatient children <strong>and</strong> adolescents(aged 6 to 15), the PICA-III-Rscales were generally internally consistent(i.e., most over .80), although the internalconsistency <strong>of</strong> the Mania (.69) <strong>and</strong> Obsessive-Compulsive symptoms (.54) were somewhatlow. There is minimal information on thevalidity <strong>of</strong> the PICA-III-R other than the factthat scores from this interview changed overthe course <strong>of</strong> hospitalization for the inpatients,which presumably reflected improvementbrought about by treatment.Both the Dominic-R <strong>and</strong> PICA-III-Rprovide examples <strong>of</strong> ways to enhance theusefulness <strong>of</strong> structured interviews in obtainingchild self-report. It is important to note,however, that both <strong>of</strong> these interviews arein the very early stages <strong>of</strong> development, <strong>and</strong>much more information is needed on their(Continues)


264 chapter 11 structured diagnostic interviewsBox 11.3 (Continued)reliability across samples <strong>and</strong> their validity forassessing DSM diagnoses.Finally, another example <strong>of</strong> a uniqueapproach to the assessment <strong>of</strong> young childrenis the Berkeley Puppet Interview (see Measelle,Ablow, Cowan, & Cowan, 1998). Thisinterview calls for two puppets to make statementsabout themselves <strong>and</strong> then ask the askthe child a question with two choices basedon the same attribute (e.g., “Are you good atmaking friends or are you not good at makingfriends?”). This interview includes academic,social, <strong>and</strong> symptom-related (i.e., aggressionhostility<strong>and</strong> depression-anxiety) domains.Some evidence <strong>of</strong> convergent validity has beenfound based on ratings by parents <strong>and</strong> teachers(Measelle et al., 1998) <strong>and</strong> the interviewhas been able to differentiate clinic-referredfrom community samples <strong>of</strong> children (Ablowet al., 1999). However, much more researchon the validity, feasibility, <strong>and</strong> utility <strong>of</strong> thisassessment tool is needed.to produce more stable symptom endorsementsthat are less susceptible to attenuationeffects. They included a more detailedintroduction with an overview <strong>of</strong> all areasto be covered on the interview, <strong>and</strong> theyprovided definitions <strong>of</strong> key concepts usedthroughout the interview. Furthermore,they employed a flexible order <strong>of</strong> administrationthat allowed parents to select theorder in which the various diagnoses wereassessed. Using this methodology in a sample<strong>of</strong> 24 parent-child pairs with childrenranging in age from 6 to 15, there was veryminimal reduction in the number <strong>of</strong> symptomsreported when the interviews wererepeated one week later. However, none<strong>of</strong> the st<strong>and</strong>ardized administration proceduresthat accompany structured interviewschedules provide for this type <strong>of</strong> administration,<strong>and</strong> therefore, the possible dropin number <strong>of</strong> symptoms reported for disordersassessed later in the interviews mustbe considered when interpreting the resultsfor individual children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.A final consideration in using structuredinterviews concerns when to administerdiagnostic interviews in the assessment battery.There is no research on this issue, <strong>and</strong>these recommendations come from clinicalexperience. Diagnostic interviews shouldnot be the first assessment administeredto parents. The structured format doesnot facilitate the development <strong>of</strong> rapportbetween the interviewer <strong>and</strong> parent, <strong>and</strong>some parents become frustrated in tryingto fit their main concerns <strong>and</strong> descriptions<strong>of</strong> their child’s behavior into the confines <strong>of</strong>the interview. Therefore, it is <strong>of</strong>ten helpfulto precede diagnostic interviews with lessstructured questions that allow parents toexpress, in their own words, their concernsfor their child. However, for children <strong>and</strong>adolescents, we actually find that the structuredformat enhances rapport in manycases. <strong>Child</strong>ren <strong>of</strong>ten enter the assessmentsituation nervous because they are unsureabout what is expected <strong>of</strong> them. The clear<strong>and</strong> explicit response format <strong>of</strong> diagnosticinterviews makes the dem<strong>and</strong>s <strong>of</strong> the situationapparent for the child <strong>and</strong> therebyreduces his or her anxiety in many cases.Up to this point, we have tried to give anoverview <strong>of</strong> structured interviews, lookingat the various formats that are available,highlighting some <strong>of</strong> the advantages <strong>and</strong>disadvantages <strong>of</strong> using interview schedulesin a clinical assessment, <strong>and</strong> providingguidelines for appropriate use. In the nextsection, we provide a more in-depth lookat one particular interview schedule, theDISC-IV. We chose the DISC-IV as a prototypicalexample <strong>of</strong> a structured interview


chapter 11 structured diagnostic interviews265because it is one <strong>of</strong> the most widely usedinterview schedules for children <strong>and</strong> adolescents,<strong>and</strong> it has been one <strong>of</strong> the mostsystematically developed. However, onemust be aware that the DISC-IV is one <strong>of</strong>the most structured interview schedules,<strong>and</strong> therefore, it has all <strong>of</strong> the advantages<strong>and</strong> disadvantages that accompany a highdegree <strong>of</strong> structure.Focus on the NIMHDiagnostic InterviewSchedule for<strong>Child</strong>ren (DISC-iv)DevelopmentThe original version <strong>of</strong> the DISC (DISC-1; Costello, 1983; Costello et al., 1984)was designed to be a downward extension<strong>of</strong> the adult-oriented Diagnostic InterviewSchedule (Robins et al., 1981). TheDISC-1 was developed as part <strong>of</strong> an initiativeby the NIMH Division <strong>of</strong> Biometry<strong>and</strong> Epidemiology that focused onobtaining a greater underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> theprevalence <strong>of</strong> childhood mental disorders(Shaffer et al., 2000). The DISC-1 wasdesigned for use in epidemiological studies<strong>and</strong> was explicitly tied to the version <strong>of</strong>the DSM being used at the time (DSM-III;American Psychiatric Association, 1980).In 1985, Dr. David Shaffer at the NewYork State Psychiatric Institute <strong>and</strong> his colleaguesundertook a revision <strong>of</strong> the interviewto (1) improve its reliability for usewith children <strong>and</strong> for use by lay interviewers<strong>and</strong> (2) provide diagnostic compatibilitywith the DSM-III-R (American PsychiatricAssociation, 1987) <strong>and</strong> anticipated DSM-IV <strong>and</strong> ICD-10 criteria (Fisher et al., 1992).Modifications to the DISC have beengreatly informed by field-testing conductedas part <strong>of</strong> a large NIMH-funded multisitestudy titled the Methods for the Epidemiology<strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> MentalDisorders (MECA) study.The current version <strong>of</strong> the DISC, theDISC-IV (Shaffer et al., 2000), assessesapproximately 30 diagnoses <strong>of</strong> childhood<strong>and</strong> adolescence <strong>and</strong> is fully compatiblewith the DSM-IV (American PsychiatricAssociation, 2000) <strong>and</strong> ICD-10 (WorldHealth Organization, 1993) classificationsystems. The development <strong>of</strong> the DISChas exp<strong>and</strong>ed to other languages <strong>and</strong> toalternative administration formats (i.e.,computerized).Structure <strong>and</strong> ContentThe DISC-IV (Shaffer et al., 2000) contains358 core questions <strong>and</strong> approximately1,300 questions that are asked contingenton a child’s responses to the core questions.There are two parallel versions <strong>of</strong>the DISC-IV, the youth version (DISC-Y),to be administered to children <strong>of</strong> ages <strong>of</strong>9–17, <strong>and</strong> the parent version (DISC-P), tobe administered to the parents <strong>of</strong> children<strong>of</strong> ages 6–17. There is also an experimentalteacher version (DISC-T), which wasdeveloped for use in the DSM-IV field trials(Frick et al., 1994). The DISC-IV wasdesigned with a primary focus on currentpsychological functioning. It assesses forsymptoms occurring within two overlappingtime intervals: the past twelve months<strong>and</strong> the past four weeks. The DISC-IV containsan optional whole life module designedto measure symptoms occurring as early asage 5. An alternative present-state versionhas been developed targeting only the fourweektime interval (see Shaffer et al., 2000).The DISC-IV is organized in “diagnosticmodules.” There are six modulesthat comprise sets <strong>of</strong> related disorders. Asummary <strong>of</strong> the modules is provided inBox 11.4. For each diagnosis, DISC-IV isdesigned to obtain information about thepresence <strong>of</strong> symptoms included in DSM


266 chapter 11 structured diagnostic interviewsBox 11.4Organization <strong>and</strong> Content <strong>of</strong> theDISC-1VModuleAnxietyMoodPsychosisDisruptiveBehaviorSubstanceUseMiscellaneousSource: Shaffer et al., 2000.Disorders CoveredSocial Phobia SeparationAnxiety Specific PhobiaPanicAgoraphobiaGeneralized AnxietySelective MutismObsessive-CompulsivePosttraumatic StressMajor Depressive/DysthymicMania/HypomaniaSchizophreniaAttention-Deficit/HyperactivityDisorderOppositional Defiant DisorderConduct DisorderAlcohol Abuse/DependenceNicotine DependenceMarijuana Abuse/DependenceOther SubstanceAbuse/DependenceAnorexia Nervosa/BulimiaNervosaElimination DisordersTic DisordersPicacriteria. If a certain threshold, usuallybelow DSM diagnostic threshold, is met,the questions regarding the age <strong>of</strong> firstonset, impairment, <strong>and</strong> past treatment areasked. (See Box 11.1 for an example <strong>of</strong> theDISC-IV question format.)AdministrationThe DISC-IV was designed to be administeredby interviewers without clinicalexperience after approximately two to sixdays <strong>of</strong> training. Use <strong>of</strong> the computerizedDISC-IV is accompanied by less stringenttraining requirements. Training includes (1)instruction on st<strong>and</strong>ard DISC-IV administrationprocedures, (2) viewing an actualadministration <strong>of</strong> the DISC-IV, <strong>and</strong> (3)supervised practice in administration witha confederate in a controlled situation.At the beginning <strong>of</strong> the interview, theinterviewer completes an introductorymodule consisting <strong>of</strong> several pieces <strong>of</strong>demographic information (e.g., age <strong>and</strong>sex <strong>of</strong> child) that are necessary to properlyadminister the interview. The intervieweralso establishes a time line with the intervieweeto assist in his or her recall for theonset <strong>and</strong> duration criteria contained in theinterview. The time line establishes salientevents (e.g., birthdays, vacations, start <strong>of</strong> theschool year, holidays) that occurred in theyear preceding the interview. These anchorshelp the child or parent remember the timeframe for diagnostic questions.The verbal instructions given to therespondent are semi-structured. That is,several points that must be covered areprovided, but verbatim instructions arenot required. The points include:1. There are no right or wrong answers.The best answer is the one that tells themost about the child.2. The informant should try to answer“yes” or “no” to each question.3. The time frame is within the last year,unless otherwise specified.4. Some <strong>of</strong> the questions on the form willbe left out.5. Some questions maybe asked morethan once.6. It is possible to take breaks, if needed.Unlike the instructions, the administration<strong>of</strong> the actual DISC-IV questionsis quite structured. The questions aredesigned to be read exactly as written <strong>and</strong>in the sequence prescribed. Interviewersare explicitly instructed not to makeup their own questions or to ask for anexample unless it is requested in the inter-


chapter 11 structured diagnostic interviews267view format. If a respondent does notunderst<strong>and</strong> the question, the interviewershould repeat the question, emphasizingthe words that seemed to cause confusion.The interviewer is not allowed to answerinterpretive questions for the respondent(e.g., “What do you mean by <strong>of</strong>ten?” or “Isone or two times considered frequent?”).The interviewer is instructed to simply askthe respondent to interpret the question“whichever way s/he thinks is best.”ReliabilityThe DISC-IV (<strong>and</strong> other interview schedules)relies heavily on psychometric dataderived from prior versions <strong>of</strong> the instrument.Initial reliability data on the DISC-2.3was obtained from a series <strong>of</strong> articles by Shaffer<strong>and</strong> colleagues (Piacentini et al., 1993;Schwab-Stone et al., 1993; Shaffer et al.,1993). These authors tested the psychometricproperties <strong>of</strong> the DISC-2.3 in a sample<strong>of</strong> 75 clinic-referred children ages 11–17. In41 cases, the child <strong>and</strong>/or parent were reinterviewedone to three weeks later by a secondinterviewer. There were sufficient casesto calculate the test-retest reliability for fiveDSM-III-R diagnoses (i.e., Attention-DeficitHyperactivity Disorder, Oppositional DefiantDisorder, Conduct Disorder, MajorDepression, <strong>and</strong> Separation Anxiety Disorder).The kappa statistics for the test-retestagreement are reported in Table 11.2. Alsoreported in this table are the intraclass correlations,which provide an estimate <strong>of</strong> thetest-retest reliability <strong>of</strong> the symptom clustersthat form the criteria for DSM-III-R diagnoses.Importantly, the tendency for parents<strong>and</strong> children to report substantially fewersymptoms on repeated administrations <strong>of</strong>structured interview can substantially reducetest-retest coefficients. Therefore, Table 11.2also includes another index <strong>of</strong> reliability, theCronbach’s alpha, as an estimate <strong>of</strong> the internalconsistency <strong>of</strong> the symptom clusters.On a diagnostic level, all diagnosesshowed relatively high test-retest agreementTable 11.2 Test–Retest <strong>and</strong> Internal ConsistencyEstimates from the DISC-2.3Kappa ICC AlphaParent only (n = 39)Attention-Deficit .55 .87 .87HyperactivityOppositional.88 .82 .75DefiantConduct .87 .86 .56Major Depression .72 .82 .88Separation Anxiety .77 .61<strong>Child</strong> Only (n = 41)Attention-Deficit.72 .83HyperactivityOppositional.16 .44 .67DefiantConduct .55 .60 .59Major Depression .77 .68 .85Separation Anxiety .72 .66 .71Combined Parent<strong>and</strong> <strong>Child</strong> (n = 37)Attention-Deficit .56 .84HyperactivityOppositional.59 .65DefiantConduct .50 .80Major Depression .71 .66Separation Anxiety .80 .72Note: Kappa is the agreement between diagnosesat Time 1 <strong>and</strong> diagnoses at Time 2 with a 1- to3-week interval between interviews. ICC is the intraclasscorrelation between symptoms at Time 1 <strong>and</strong>Time 2. Alpha is Chronbach’s alpha calculated forthe symptom at Time 1.Source: M. Schwab-Stone,P. Fisher, J. Piacentini, D. Shaffer, M. Davies, &M. Briggs (1993). “The Diagnostic InterviewSchedule for <strong>Child</strong>ren-Revised Version (DISC-R): II.Test–Retest Reliability,” Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy<strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 32, 651–657.for the parent interview, the child interview,<strong>and</strong> the combined parent <strong>and</strong> child interview.When combining a parent <strong>and</strong> child report,a symptom was considered present if eitherparent or child endorsed it. The one exceptionto the generally good reliability was thelow reliability <strong>of</strong> the Oppositional DefiantDisorder diagnosis by child report. Another


268 chapter 11 structured diagnostic interviewsexception was the low internal consistency<strong>of</strong> the Conduct Disorder symptoms. However,the low internal consistency <strong>of</strong> thesesymptoms is not surprising for two reasons.First, only three symptoms are required forthe diagnosis <strong>of</strong> Conduct Disorder <strong>and</strong> most<strong>of</strong> these symptoms tend to have a relativelylow base rate. Second, these symptoms tendto be indicative <strong>of</strong> discrete—perhaps independent—problembehaviors or delinquentacts, making internal consistency estimatesa less than optimal method <strong>of</strong> determiningthe reliability <strong>of</strong> this symptom domain.Although these reliability data are basedon an earlier version <strong>of</strong> the DISC, the DISC2.3, Shaffer et al. (2000) provide initial reliabilityestimates for the DISC-IV from asample <strong>of</strong> 84 parents <strong>and</strong> 82 children, ages9–17, selected from several outpatient psychiatricclinics. These data were derivedfrom the computer-administered version <strong>of</strong>the DISC-IV, the C-DISC-IV. Interviewswere conducted by lay interviewers with anaverage retest interval <strong>of</strong> seven days. Thepreliminary findings are consistent with theresults for the DISC-2.3, with kappa coefficientsranging from .43 (Conduct Disorder)to .96 (Specific Phobia) for the parent report<strong>and</strong> from .25 (Simple Phobia) to .92 (MajorDepressive Episode) for the child report.ValidityThe validity <strong>of</strong> diagnostic interviews is<strong>of</strong>ten assessed by comparing the results <strong>of</strong>structured interviews to diagnoses made byexperienced clinicians. For example, Piacentiniet al. (1993) reported moderate tostrong agreement between the results <strong>of</strong>DISC interviews <strong>and</strong> clinician diagnoseswhen the Parent DISC-2.3 was used (averagekappa = .50) but low agreement based onthe <strong>Child</strong> DISC-2.3 (average kappa = .34).Combining the two interviews gave agreementestimates between those <strong>of</strong> eitherinformant alone (average kappa = .41).These authors reported that most <strong>of</strong> thecases with disagreements between clinici<strong>and</strong>iagnoses <strong>and</strong> the DISC-2.3 were cases thatwere close to the diagnostic threshold. Forexample, several disagreements emergedin which children had seven symptoms <strong>of</strong>ADHD (rather than the required eightsymptoms in DSM-III-R criteria) <strong>and</strong> werenot given the diagnosis according to theDISC-2.3 but were given the diagnosis <strong>of</strong>ADHD by the clinician.However, another study (Lewczyk et al.,2003) found relatively poor correspondencebetween diagnoses based on the DISC-IV<strong>and</strong> diagnoses by clinicians. The DISC-IVyielded higher rates <strong>of</strong> diagnoses on anxietydisorders, ODD, CD, <strong>and</strong> ADHD, butclinician diagnoses <strong>of</strong> depression were morecommon.Friman et al. (2000) provided a uniqueinvestigation <strong>of</strong> the predictive validity<strong>of</strong> the DISC by comparing interviewdata to behavioral observations recordedin a residential treatment program. Theresearchers examined both convergent <strong>and</strong>discriminant validity across a lengthy timeinterval (i.e., one year). Validity data wereobtained on 369 children, aged 9–17, whowere administered a computerized version<strong>of</strong> the DISC-Y 2.3, the C-DISC-Y 2.3,upon enrollment in the residential program<strong>and</strong> at a one year follow-up. Diagnoses <strong>of</strong>both Oppositional Defiant Disorder <strong>and</strong>Conduct Disorder were compared to dailyobservations <strong>of</strong> disruptive behavior thatwere coded by program staff <strong>and</strong> summedto form monthly behavior ratings <strong>of</strong> bothoppositional <strong>and</strong> conduct problem behaviors.Youth meeting criteria for a DISC-2.3diagnosis <strong>of</strong> ODD or CD upon enrollmentexhibited significantly greater observedbehavioral difficulties on program entrythan youth not meeting a diagnosis foreither disorder. Furthermore, change indiagnostic status across the two assessmentperiods predicted changes in observeddisrupted behavior across the same timeinterval. For example, youth who met criteriafor an ODD/CD diagnosis at Time


chapter 11 structured diagnostic interviews2691 but not at Time 2 were characterizedby a downward pattern <strong>of</strong> observed antisocialbehavior in the months separatingthe interviews. This is contrasted withyouth whose observed antisocial behaviorincreased as they moved from no diagnosisat Time 1 to diagnosis at Time 2.As further evidence for the validity <strong>of</strong> theDISC interview, Edelbrock <strong>and</strong> Costello(1988) found strong associations betweenthe diagnoses <strong>of</strong> ADHD, Conduct Disorder,<strong>and</strong> Depression/Dysthymia from theDISC-P <strong>and</strong> the Hyperactive, Delinquent,<strong>and</strong> Depressed scales <strong>of</strong> a previous version<strong>of</strong> the <strong>Child</strong> Behavior Checklist (Achenbach& Edelbrock, 1983) in a sample <strong>of</strong>270 clinic-referred children between theages <strong>of</strong> 6 <strong>and</strong> 16. High rates <strong>of</strong> agreementwere also found between the originalDISC-P <strong>and</strong> the CBCL in another study<strong>of</strong> 40 psychiatric referrals <strong>and</strong> 40 pediatricreferrals (Costello, Edelbrock, & Costello,1985). In contrast, the relation between theCBCL <strong>and</strong> the child version <strong>of</strong> the DISCtended to be much lower. However, it isimpossible to determine whether the lowcorrelations with the DISC-C were dueto differences in informants (i.e., parentcompletedCBCL <strong>and</strong> child-respondentDISC) or to differences in the assessmentinstruments themselves.ConclusionsStructured diagnostic interviews havebecome an important part <strong>of</strong> many clinicalassessments <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.Like behavior rating scales, diagnostic interviewsprovide a reliable means <strong>of</strong> assessing achild’s emotional <strong>and</strong> behavioral functioning.In this chapter, we have attempted tohighlight the advantages <strong>and</strong> disadvantages<strong>of</strong> using diagnostic interviews. Diagnosticinterviews enhance clinical assessments byproviding a format for determining howlong a child’s problems have been occurring,for determining the temporal sequencing <strong>of</strong>behaviors, <strong>and</strong> for estimating the degree<strong>of</strong> impairment associated with a child’semotional or behavioral problems. Theseimportant parameters <strong>of</strong> a child’s emotional<strong>and</strong> behavioral functioning are <strong>of</strong>ten notassessed by other assessment modalities.In addition, diagnostic interviews are typicallytied to the most recent revisions <strong>of</strong> theDSM, which closely links assessment withthis system <strong>of</strong> classification.On the negative side, diagnostic interviewsare <strong>of</strong>ten time intensive, <strong>and</strong> they typicallydo not provide any norm-referencedinformation on a child’s functioning abovethat which is accorded by DSM criteria. Inaddition, diagnostic interviews typically donot include a format for obtaining informationfrom a child’s teacher, <strong>and</strong> their reliabilityin obtaining self-report information foryoung children (i.e., below age 9) is somewhatquestionable, although pictorial orother formats provide a promising method<strong>of</strong> enhancing their usefulness in this youngage group. As a result <strong>of</strong> these weaknesses,diagnostic interviews are best used as part<strong>of</strong> a more comprehensive assessment battery.We have attempted to provide guidelinesfor their use in this capacity. We havealso attempted to provide an overview <strong>of</strong> themost commonly used diagnostic interviewsfor assessing children <strong>and</strong> adolescents, highlightingthe major commonalities <strong>and</strong> differencesacross interviews. We concludedthe chapter with a more detailed discussion<strong>of</strong> the DISC-IV as an example <strong>of</strong> a typicaldiagnostic interview schedule designed foruse with children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.Chapter Summary1. Structured diagnostic interviews consist<strong>of</strong> a set <strong>of</strong> questions to be asked <strong>of</strong> achild or adolescent with explicit guidelineson how the youth’s responses areto be scored.


270 chapter 11 structured diagnostic interviews2. Most <strong>of</strong> the commonly used structuredinterviews are designed to assess diagnosticcriteria from one <strong>of</strong> the recent versions<strong>of</strong> the Diagnostic <strong>and</strong> Statistical Manual<strong>of</strong> Mental Disorders. Therefore, if a goal<strong>of</strong> the evaluation is to make or clarify aDSM diagnosis, structured interviews arean important assessment tool.3. Interviews vary on the degree <strong>of</strong> structureinherent in the interview format<strong>and</strong> whether or not the interview assessesonly current episodes <strong>of</strong> the disorders.4. Structured interviews, like behavior ratingscales, obtain detailed descriptions<strong>of</strong> a child’s emotions <strong>and</strong> behaviors frommultiple informants.5. Unlike rating scales, however, structuredinterviews allow for the assessment<strong>of</strong> important parameters <strong>of</strong> a child’sbehavior, such as the duration <strong>of</strong> thebehavioral difficulties, the temporalsequencing among problems, <strong>and</strong> thedegree <strong>of</strong> impairment associated withthe difficulties.6. Most structured interviews are timeconsuming <strong>and</strong> are not good for makingnorm-referenced interpretations.7. Diagnoses derived from structured interviewsshould be viewed within the context<strong>of</strong> other assessment instruments.8. <strong>Child</strong> self-report from diagnosticinterviews is typically not reliablebefore age 9.9. The NIMH Diagnostic InterviewSchedule for <strong>Child</strong>ren-Version IV(DISC-IV) is a prototypical structureddiagnostic interview for use with children<strong>and</strong> adolescents.a. The DISC is highly structuredso that it can be administered bytrained lay interviewers.b. There are child, parent, <strong>and</strong> experimentalteacher versions <strong>of</strong> theDISC.c. The DISC contains 358 core questions<strong>and</strong> 1,800 follow-up questions that areasked contingent on a child’s responsesto the core questions.d. The DISC is organized in six modules:(1) Anxiety Disorders, (2) MoodDisorders, (3) Psychosis, (4) DisruptiveBehavior Disorders, (5) Alcohol<strong>and</strong> Substance User Disorders, <strong>and</strong>(6) Miscellaneous Disorders (i.e.,Eating Disorders, Elimination Disorders,Tic Disorders).10. Diagnostic interviews should only beused as part <strong>of</strong> a more comprehensiveassessment battery.


C h a p t e r 1 2Assessing Family ContextChapter Questionsl Why is assessing a child’s family contextimportant in the clinical assessment <strong>of</strong>children’s emotional <strong>and</strong> behavioral functioning?l What dimensions <strong>of</strong> a child’s family contextare most important to the assessmentprocess?l How can one assess these importantdimensions <strong>of</strong> family functioning?IntroductionIn the summary <strong>of</strong> research on childhoodpsychopathology provided in Chap. 3, one<strong>of</strong> the more important findings was thatchildren’s <strong>and</strong> adolescents’ emotional <strong>and</strong>behavioral functioning was heavily influencedby the dem<strong>and</strong>s <strong>and</strong> stressors theyexperienced in their environment. As aresult, to truly underst<strong>and</strong> a child’s or adolescent’spsychological adjustment, onemust not limit the assessment to obtainingcharacteristics <strong>of</strong> the youth but must alsoassess the important contexts that shape achild’s or adolescent’s behavior. There is nocontext more important to underst<strong>and</strong>ing achild or adolescent than the family context.Causal Role <strong>of</strong> FamilialInfluencesResearch on childhood psychopathologyconsistently suggests that factors within thefamily play a major causal role in the development<strong>of</strong> personality <strong>and</strong> psychopathology(Erickson, 1998). At times the causal role <strong>of</strong>271P.J. Frick et al., <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Behavior,DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-0641-0_12, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010


272 CHAPTER 12 assessing family contexta child’s family has been overemphasized,either by ignoring the potential effects thata child or adolescent can have on the family(Pardini, 2008), or by ignoring the factorsthat can influence both the family <strong>and</strong> child(Frick & Jackson, 1993). These caveatsindicate that a child’s family context is onlyone piece <strong>of</strong> a very complex puzzle in underst<strong>and</strong>ingthe psychological adjustment <strong>of</strong> achild or adolescent. However, research alsosuggests that it is a very important piece <strong>of</strong>the puzzle. Family factors play an integralrole in the causal theories <strong>of</strong> many types<strong>of</strong> problems across many theoretical orientations.Box 12.1 provides examples <strong>of</strong>three different types <strong>of</strong> childhood problemsfrom three different theoretical perspectives,all <strong>of</strong> which emphasize the familycontext as a causal agent in a child’s adjustmentproblems. Therefore, if the goal<strong>of</strong> an assessment is to uncover the possiblecauses <strong>of</strong> a child’s or adolescent’s emotionalor behavioral difficulties <strong>and</strong>, thereby, pointthe way to important treatment goals, theassessment <strong>of</strong> family functioning is criticalto the assessment process.Box 12.1<strong>Child</strong>hood Problems in a Family Context: Examples <strong>of</strong> Three Problems <strong>and</strong> ThreeTheoretical Orientations<strong>Child</strong>hood Anxiety: An OperantPerspectiveOne way <strong>of</strong> conceptualizing the causes <strong>of</strong>childhood anxiety has been from an operantperspective. In this model a child receivesreinforcement from his or her environmentthat functions to maintain <strong>and</strong> increase theanxious behavior. Often this reinforcementis provided in the family context. A goodexample is provided by Ross (1981) <strong>of</strong> an8-year-old girl, Valerie, with school phobia.Valerie refused to attend school, <strong>and</strong> an assessment<strong>of</strong> her home context revealed that theconsequences <strong>of</strong> her refusal to go to schoolwere indeed quite positive. Instead <strong>of</strong> goingto school, Valerie was able to sleep an hourlater than her three siblings who were attendingschool. Until her mother left for work,she was allowed to follow the mother aroundthe house <strong>and</strong> then was taken to a neighbor’shouse for the day. At the neighbor’s houseValerie was free to do whatever she pleasedfor the rest <strong>of</strong> the day, such as playing games<strong>and</strong> making occasional trips to a corner storewhere she bought c<strong>and</strong>y, gum, <strong>and</strong> s<strong>of</strong>t drinks.It was clearly a comfortable routine for bothchild <strong>and</strong> mother, who thereby avoided Valerie’stemper tantrums.<strong>Child</strong>hood Aggression: A Social LearningPerspectiveGerald Patterson (Patterson, 1982) <strong>and</strong>his colleagues at the Oregon Social LearningCenter have developed a social learningmodel for the development <strong>of</strong> aggression.In this model, family interactions provide atraining ground for a child to learn coercivemethods <strong>of</strong> controlling interactions with others.Through analyses <strong>of</strong> micro-social interactionsbetween parents <strong>and</strong> children, Pattersonoutlines the development <strong>of</strong> a coercive cyclethat develops between parent <strong>and</strong> child <strong>and</strong>which escalates through aversive conditioning.The cycle starts when a parent makes adem<strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong> a child <strong>and</strong> a child reacts aversively(e.g., whines, becomes defiant). Rather thanpushing the child, the parent withdraws thedem<strong>and</strong>, which reinforces the child’s aversiveresponse. During the next phase <strong>of</strong> the cycle,the parent again makes a dem<strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong> the childbut decides not to give in to the child’s aversivebehaviors. As the child becomes more aversive(e.g., temper tantrum), the parent becomesmore aversive (e.g., yelling, spanking). As theparent becomes more aversive, the child eventuallycomplies, which reinforces the parent’sincrease in aversive behavior.(Continues)


CHAPTER 12 assessing family context273Box 12.1 (Continued)This cycle repeats itself over <strong>and</strong> overagain, leading to each party reinforcingincreasing levels <strong>of</strong> aversiveness in the other.This training in coercive responses is thencarried over by the child into other settingswith other people (e.g., teachers, peers). It isevident that the cycle is transactional: that is,both the child <strong>and</strong> the parent contribute tothe escalating cycle. However, for the purpose<strong>of</strong> the current discussion, it is evident howimportant the child’s family environment,especially parent–child interactions, is to thedevelopment <strong>of</strong> aggression within this theoreticalframework.Eating Disorders: A Family SystemsPerspectiveSargent, Liebman, <strong>and</strong> Silver (1985) describefamily characteristics that provide a context inwhich the psychological features <strong>of</strong> anorexia nervosafit <strong>and</strong> are adaptive.Families <strong>of</strong> a person with anorexia have beenfound to have parents who are overinvolved intheir child’s life. This overinvolvement preventsthe child with anorexia from perceiving her ownsensations, including hunger. It also prevents thechild from developing a sense <strong>of</strong> self-competence<strong>and</strong> the ability to use problem-solving skills. Asthe anorexia worsens, the family becomes moreprotective <strong>and</strong> involved <strong>and</strong> further inhibits theaffected child from acting more maturely <strong>and</strong>adaptively. Families <strong>of</strong> a child with anorexia alsotend to have difficulty resolving conflict. As aresult <strong>of</strong> unresolved marital conflict, the parentshave difficulty collaborating to h<strong>and</strong>le thechild’s symptoms <strong>and</strong> actually counteract eachother in their attempts. These are just a few <strong>of</strong>the family dynamics that family system theoristshave proposed to explain the development<strong>and</strong> maintenance <strong>of</strong> anorexia nervosa in a child.However, it clearly illustrates the primary role<strong>of</strong> the family context for underst<strong>and</strong>ing a childwith an eating disorder.Family History <strong>and</strong> DifferentialDiagnosesTwo other facts emerge from research onchildhood psychopathology that point tothe importance <strong>of</strong> assessing a child’s familycontext in clinical assessments. First,childhood emotional <strong>and</strong> behavioral problemstend to be rather amorphous, lackingclear boundaries, more so than is the casein adult psychopathology (see Lilienfeld,2003). Stated another way, children withproblems <strong>of</strong>ten have multiple types <strong>of</strong> problems,<strong>and</strong> it is <strong>of</strong>ten difficult to know whatis primary <strong>and</strong> what might be secondary.Second, there seems to be a parent–childlink to many types <strong>of</strong> psychopathology,with parents <strong>and</strong> children showing similarpatterns <strong>of</strong> adjustment (McMahon &Dev Peters, 2002). Taking these two factstogether, assessment <strong>of</strong> the adjustment <strong>of</strong>parents in whom the type <strong>of</strong> problem maybe more clearly defined may provide cluesto the primary problem <strong>of</strong> the child.An example from research on childhoodaffective disorders illustrates the use<strong>of</strong> family history data in making a differentialdiagnosis. Prior to adolescence, thediagnosis <strong>of</strong> a bipolar affective disorder isdifficult to make. But research suggeststhat a significant proportion <strong>of</strong> childrenwith a depressive disorder will develop abipolar disorder later in life (Geller, Fox,& Fletcher, 1993). Geller et al. foundthat obtaining a family psychiatric historyhelped to predict which children with adepressive disorder were at most risk fordeveloping a bipolar disorder. Specifically,the presence <strong>of</strong> a family history <strong>of</strong> a bipolardisorder significantly predicted which <strong>of</strong>the children with depression would laterbegin to cycle between manic <strong>and</strong> depressivestates. This study also illustrates theimportant treatment implications for mak-


274 CHAPTER 12 assessing family contexting differential diagnoses. <strong>Child</strong>ren whowere depressed <strong>and</strong> had a family history<strong>of</strong> bipolar disorders were more likely tohave manic behaviors develop followingtreatment with anti-depressant medicationthan were the depressed children withouta family history <strong>of</strong> bipolar disorder. A casestudy in which family history informationwas used in making a differential diagnosisis provided in Box 12.2.Interpreting InformationProvided by the ParentMany <strong>of</strong> the assessment techniques discussedthroughout this book rely on thereport <strong>of</strong> family members in the assessment<strong>of</strong> child or adolescent adjustment.As a result, another important reason forassessing a child’s family context is thatfactors within the family can affect theBox 12.2Family History <strong>and</strong> Differential Diagnosis: A Case Study <strong>of</strong> a 7-Year-Old Girl withSocial PhobiaClaire is a 7-year, 2 month-old girl who was inthe middle <strong>of</strong> the first grade when her teacherrecommended that she be tested at an outpatientmental health clinic. Her teacher was concernedthat she might have Attention DeficitHyperactivity Disorder. Claire seemed bright<strong>and</strong> capable <strong>of</strong> learning <strong>and</strong>, in fact, performedquite well in one-on-one situations with theteacher or teacher’s aide. However, in the generalclassroom setting, Claire rarely finished herwork. She was <strong>of</strong>ten noted to be staring <strong>of</strong>f intospace <strong>and</strong> she had to be constantly redirectedback to her work. Her teacher emphaticallystated that Claire was not a behavior problem.In fact, Claire was quite quiet <strong>and</strong> reserved <strong>and</strong>even had difficulty asking for help when it wasneeded.There were several differential diagnosesthat were considered in the psychologicalevaluation <strong>of</strong> Claire. A psychoeducationalevaluation that included an intelligence test<strong>and</strong> an academic screener indicated that Clairewas quite capable <strong>of</strong> learning at or above alevel expected for her age. Therefore, herproblems in school did not seem to be causedby the presence <strong>of</strong> an intellectual deficit or alearning disability. However, the differentialdiagnosis between an attention deficit disorder<strong>and</strong> an anxiety disorder was more difficult,as she exhibited many behaviors consistentwith both types <strong>of</strong> problems. Several pieces<strong>of</strong> information helped make the decision thatClaire’s primary problem was one <strong>of</strong> anxiety,<strong>and</strong> particularly, social anxiety.First, Claire’s attentional difficulties tendedto be much milder than would be expected forchildren with attention deficit hyperactivitydisorder, as indicated by structured interviewsconducted with Claire’s mother <strong>and</strong> teacher<strong>and</strong> rating scales completed by her mother<strong>and</strong> teacher. Second, Claire showed a number<strong>of</strong> other symptoms <strong>of</strong> anxiety in social situations.For example, she refused to go to Sundayschool at church <strong>and</strong> to other social activities(e.g., parties) <strong>and</strong> she had one good friend in theneighborhood but would only spend time withher if they were alone. Third, Claire’s motherhad a history <strong>of</strong> agoraphobia that had led toseveral lengthy periods in which she could notleave the house because <strong>of</strong> her fears.In this example a family history <strong>of</strong> anxietywas just one piece <strong>of</strong> the assessment thathelped to make the differential diagnosis.However, it seemed to be an important piece.The diagnosis itself ended up being importantbecause rather than treatment focusingon Claire’s attentional problems, a treatmentstrategy that used systematic desensitizationto social situations was implemented, withClaire’s teacher reporting dramatic improvementsin Claire’s school performance by theend <strong>of</strong> the year.


CHAPTER 12 assessing family context275information provided by family memberson the child’s adjustment. To appropriatelyinterpret the information obtained fromparents <strong>and</strong> other family members, onemust underst<strong>and</strong> those factors that couldinfluence a parent’s accuracy in providinginformation on a child. For example,a noncustodial parent involved in a custodydispute may try to inflate the problems<strong>of</strong> a child in an effort to get a morefavorable court decision. In contrast, thecustodial parent may have motivations topresent the child in a more positive light.A second example would be parents whoare trying to have their child placed in aresidential treatment center <strong>and</strong> who mayinflate problems in an effort to justify thisplacement. These are just two examples <strong>of</strong>a myriad <strong>of</strong> familial factors that can affecthow one interprets the information providedby family members.De Los Reyes <strong>and</strong> Kazdin (2005) providea review <strong>of</strong> several family factors thatcan influence parent ratings <strong>of</strong> the childincluding family stress, the parent–childrelationship, <strong>and</strong> the level <strong>of</strong> marital discord.De Los Reyes <strong>and</strong> Kazdin (2005) alsonote several aspects <strong>of</strong> parental adjustmentthat can influence how parents rate theirchildren’s adjustment. One area that hasbeen the focus <strong>of</strong> substantial research is onthe effects <strong>of</strong> depression on parents’ report<strong>of</strong> their child’s adjustment. There havebeen numerous studies that have called intoquestion the accuracy <strong>of</strong> depressed mothers’reports about their children’s behavior (seeDe Los Reyes & Kazdin, 2005 <strong>and</strong> Richters,1992 for reviews). This research hassuggested that depressed mothers reportmore problems in their children than arereported by nondepressed parents <strong>and</strong>teachers <strong>and</strong> more than are detected usingdirect behavioral observations <strong>of</strong> the children.These findings have led many authors toconclude that parents’ depression leads toa distorted view <strong>of</strong> the children’s behavior.We summarize Richter’s (1992) review <strong>and</strong>critique <strong>of</strong> this literature in Box 12.3.Box 12.3Research Note: Depressed Mothers as Informants About Their <strong>Child</strong>ren: ACritical ReviewJohn Richters (1992) conducted a critical review<strong>of</strong> the research on the effects <strong>of</strong> depression ona parent’s rating <strong>of</strong> a child’s behavior. Richterscited 17 studies that have been published callinginto question the accuracy <strong>of</strong> depressed mothers’reports. In general, depressed mothers havetended to report more behavior problems intheir children than the level reported by teachers,fathers, or children <strong>and</strong> greater than thatobserved in behavioral observations. All <strong>of</strong> thesestudies led researchers to the conclusion thatdepressed mothers’ perceptions <strong>of</strong> their children’sbehavior were biased by their own level <strong>of</strong>depression. However, Richters’s critical review<strong>of</strong> several methodological <strong>and</strong> interpretive problemsthat have plagued this body <strong>of</strong> researchcalls into question this depressive bias theory.The first major problem in these studies wasthe fact that most <strong>of</strong> the comparisons betweenmothers <strong>and</strong> other informants used measuresthat were discordant on either the types <strong>of</strong>behaviors assessed or the situation in whichthe behaviors were assessed. The best examplewas the frequent comparison between mothers’<strong>and</strong> teachers’ ratings on a behavior rating scalethat had a different item content. In this case,both the behaviors assessed <strong>and</strong> the situationin which the behaviors were being observedwere discordant. As a result, it is unclearwhether the differences between mothers <strong>and</strong>the other raters were due to maternal depressionor to differences in the behaviors <strong>and</strong>/or situations being assessed. Only 27% <strong>of</strong> thecomparisons between depressed mothers <strong>and</strong>(Continues)


276 CHAPTER 12 assessing family contextBox 12.3 (Continued)other informants used ratings that were bothbehaviorally <strong>and</strong> situationally concordant.The second pervasive problem in this literatureis the fact that most <strong>of</strong> the studies (94%)did not demonstrate that the mothers’ overreportingwas systematically related to maternaldepression. Twenty-four percent <strong>of</strong> the studiessimply documented that depressed mothersreported more behavior problems in theirchildren than did nondepressed mothers.Seventy-one percent indicated that maternaldepression predicted variance in mothers’ ratings<strong>of</strong> their children that was not accountedfor by criterion ratings provided by otherinformants. Richters argues that the mostdirect evidence for the depression distortionhypothesis would be if mother-criterion disagreementswere systematically related (correlated)with measures <strong>of</strong> maternal depression.The third problem discussed by Richters isthat most <strong>of</strong> the studies (94%) focused onlyon maternal depression. It is well establishedthat depression is related to other factorswithin the individual (e.g., other forms <strong>of</strong>psychopathology) <strong>and</strong> the environment (e.g.,marital satisfaction). Therefore, it is unclearwhether or not mothers’ disagreements withinformants were due to the depression or toother aspects <strong>of</strong> the mothers’ adjustment <strong>and</strong>/or concomitant stressors in the family environment.As a result <strong>of</strong> these problems, Richters suggestedthat we must be cautious in acceptingthe depression distortion hypothesis until morerefined research is conducted. In fact, Richterscites five studies that used better methodology<strong>and</strong> found that depressed mothers agreedwith other informants as well or even betterthan nondepressed mothers. However, theseare only a few studies, <strong>and</strong> they are not withoutflaws themselves. At this point, however,clinical assessors should at least be aware <strong>of</strong> theissues, many <strong>of</strong> which are unresolved, in thisvery important body <strong>of</strong> research.Source: Richters, J. E. (1992). Depressed mothers as informants about their children: A critical review <strong>of</strong> theevidence for distortion. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 485–499.Like parental depression, there is alsoevidence that parental anxiety may influencea parent’s report <strong>of</strong> childhood problems(Briggs-Gowan, Carter, & Schwab-Stone,1996). Frick <strong>and</strong> colleagues (Frick, Silverthorn,& Evans, 1994) found that in a sample<strong>of</strong> 41 clinic-referred children between the ages<strong>of</strong> 9 <strong>and</strong> 13, mothers tended to report moresymptoms <strong>of</strong> anxiety disorders than did thechild. This overreporting was systematicallyrelated to anxiety in the mother. Specifically,the more anxious the mother, the greater theoverreporting <strong>of</strong> anxiety in the child. Theseauthors also reported that maternal anxietywas not associated with overreporting <strong>of</strong>other types <strong>of</strong> maladjustment but seemed tobe more specifically related to anxiety. Thispattern <strong>of</strong> results would be consistent withthe possibility that anxious mothers projecttheir anxiety symptoms onto their reports <strong>of</strong>anxiety in their children.Assessing Family Functioning:General IssuesTo this point we have discussed several reasonswhy assessing the family is an importantpart <strong>of</strong> clinical assessments <strong>of</strong> a child oradolescent. In this section we discuss morespecifically what areas <strong>of</strong> family functioningshould be assessed <strong>and</strong> how this can beaccomplished. However, before discussingspecific areas <strong>and</strong> techniques, two generalpoints deserve mention.First, many <strong>of</strong> the behavior rating scalesthat were reviewed in previous chaptershave subscales that assess various aspects <strong>of</strong>a child’s family context. For example, theparent-completed <strong>Personality</strong> Inventoryfor <strong>Child</strong>ren-2 (PIC-2; Lachar & Gruber,2001) <strong>and</strong> the child self-report <strong>Personality</strong>Inventory for Youth (PIY; Lachar & gruber,


CHAPTER 12 assessing family context2771994) both include a Family Relations scale.Included in this scale are items assessingmarital stability, consistency in discipline,emotional tone <strong>of</strong> family, community connectedness,<strong>and</strong> parental adjustment. TheMMPI-A contains a supplementary contentscale, the <strong>Adolescent</strong>-Family Problems scale(Archer, 1992), which includes 35 itemsassessing an adolescent’s perceptions <strong>of</strong> familyconflict, level <strong>of</strong> love <strong>and</strong> acceptance inthe home, family communication, <strong>and</strong> emotionalsupport provided by the family. TheBASC-2 Self-Report Scale (Reynolds &Kamphaus, 2004) contains a Relations withParents scale that assesses a child’s perceptions<strong>of</strong> being important in the family, thequality <strong>of</strong> parent–child interactions, <strong>and</strong> thedegree <strong>of</strong> parental trust <strong>and</strong> concern. All <strong>of</strong>these scales provide a time-efficient screening<strong>of</strong> many important aspects <strong>of</strong> a child’sfamily environment. The main drawbackis that each scale combines many differentaspects <strong>of</strong> a child’s family environment, makingit impossible to uncover specific areas <strong>of</strong>strength <strong>and</strong>/or dysfunction that could beimportant in underst<strong>and</strong>ing a child <strong>and</strong> inmaking treatment recommendations.This criticism leads to our next generalcomment for assessing a child’s family context.What areas to assess <strong>and</strong> how rigorousthe assessment should be within theseareas will vary depending on the purpose<strong>of</strong> the evaluation. In the sections that followwe make the case that several aspects<strong>of</strong> the family should be routinely assessed:parenting style <strong>and</strong> parenting practices, parentingstress, marital conflict, <strong>and</strong> parentaladjustment. The depth <strong>of</strong> the assessmentin each area <strong>and</strong> which additional areas <strong>of</strong>family functioning should be assessed willvary depending on the individual case. Forexample, the assessment <strong>of</strong> a child by aschool psychologist to document emotional<strong>and</strong> behavioral factors that might be impairingacademic performance may include onlyminimal assessment <strong>of</strong> the child’s perception<strong>of</strong> the family environment <strong>and</strong> only as it mayinfluence his or her behavior in the classroom.In contrast, an assessment designedto assess a child’s adjustment to a recentparental divorce in order to make treatmentrecommendations on factors that could aidin the child’s post-divorce adjustment mayinclude a substantial family component. Thisassessment will most likely include obtainingextensive information on the level <strong>of</strong> parentalconflict <strong>and</strong> level <strong>of</strong> parental cooperation inchild-related issues, as these factors are crucialto underst<strong>and</strong>ing a child’s adjustment todivorce (Amato & Keith, 1991).Another type <strong>of</strong> assessment that requiresvery detailed <strong>and</strong> somewhat specializedassessment <strong>of</strong> family functioning is in thecase <strong>of</strong> known or suspected child abuse.A recommended assessment strategy forcases <strong>of</strong> child abuse is summarized in Box12.4. These examples illustrate the pointthat how intensive the assessment <strong>of</strong> familyfactors will be <strong>and</strong> which familial factorswill be assessed may vary somewhat fromcase to case.General Considerationsin Assessing FamilyFunctioningIn the subsequent sections, we reviewseveral critical areas <strong>of</strong> family functioningthat we feel are particularly importantin the clinical assessments <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong>adolescents. In each case, we provide abrief overview <strong>of</strong> the research supportingthe importance <strong>of</strong> each aspect <strong>of</strong>family functioning for underst<strong>and</strong>ing achild’s adjustment. This is followed by asummary <strong>of</strong> some commonly used measuresto assess that domain <strong>of</strong> functioning.It is important to note that most <strong>of</strong>the assessment methods that were chosenfor review were parent-report or childreportmeasures <strong>of</strong> family functioning.This was done for several reasons. First,these methods typically are the most time


278 CHAPTER 12 assessing family contextBox 12.4Research Note: A <strong>Child</strong> Abuse <strong>and</strong> Neglect <strong>Assessment</strong> StrategyCrooks <strong>and</strong> Wolfe (2007) outline a conceptualmodel to guide assessments <strong>of</strong> child abuse <strong>and</strong>neglect. Their model emphasizes the need tounderst<strong>and</strong>, not only the abusive behavior <strong>of</strong>the parent, but the family context in which theabuse takes place. They note that “the impact<strong>of</strong> maltreatment depends on not only theseverity <strong>and</strong> chronicity <strong>of</strong> the abusive eventsthemselves but also how such events interactwith the child’s individual <strong>and</strong> family characteristics”(p. 646). As result, an assessmentmust focus on a myriad <strong>of</strong> individual, familial,<strong>and</strong> cultural factors that research has relatedto child abuse <strong>and</strong> neglect, as well as the possibleprotective factors that can reduce theimpact <strong>of</strong> these risk factors.On the basis <strong>of</strong> this view <strong>of</strong> child abuse<strong>and</strong> neglect, Crooks <strong>and</strong> Wolfe suggest thatmost assessments need to be comprehensive<strong>and</strong> need to address the following generalpurposes: (1) identify the general strengths<strong>and</strong> needs <strong>of</strong> the family system; (2) assessparental responses to the dem<strong>and</strong>s <strong>of</strong> childrearing;(3) identify the needs <strong>of</strong> the child; <strong>and</strong>(4) assess parent–child relationship <strong>and</strong> abusedynamics. A summary <strong>of</strong> the important assessmentobjectives that follow from these overallgoals is provided below. Interested readers arereferred to the Crooks <strong>and</strong> Wolfe chapter inwhich they provide recommendations for specifictechniques to accomplish each <strong>of</strong> thesegoals. Many <strong>of</strong> these techniques are reviewedin other chapters <strong>of</strong> this text.Goal 1: Identify General Strengths <strong>and</strong>Problem Areas <strong>of</strong> Family SystemA. Family Background1. Parental history <strong>of</strong> rejection <strong>and</strong> abuseduring own childhood.2. Discipline experienced by parents duringown childhood.3. Family planning <strong>and</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> childrenon the marital relationship.4. Parents’ preparedness for <strong>and</strong> sense <strong>of</strong>competence in child rearing.B. Marital Relationship1. Length, stability, <strong>and</strong> quality <strong>of</strong> maritalrelationship.2. Degree <strong>of</strong> conflict <strong>and</strong> physical violencein marital relationship.3. Support from partner in child rearing.C. Areas <strong>of</strong> Perceived Stress <strong>and</strong> Supports1. Employment history <strong>and</strong> satisfaction <strong>of</strong>parents.2. Economic stability <strong>of</strong> family.3. Social support for parents, both within <strong>and</strong>outside the family (e.g., number <strong>and</strong> quality<strong>of</strong> contacts with extended family, neighbors,social workers, <strong>and</strong> church members).D. Parental Physical <strong>and</strong> Mental Health1. Recent or chronic health problems2. Drug <strong>and</strong> alcohol use3. Emotional disturbance <strong>and</strong> social dysfunctionGoal 2: Assess Parental Responsesto <strong>Child</strong>-Rearing Dem<strong>and</strong>sA. Emotional Reactivity <strong>of</strong> Parent1. Parents’ perception <strong>of</strong> how abused childdiffers from siblings <strong>and</strong> other children.2. Parents’ feelings <strong>of</strong> anger <strong>and</strong> loss <strong>of</strong>control when interacting with child.3. Typical methods <strong>of</strong> coping with arousalduring stressful episodes.B. <strong>Child</strong>-Rearing Methods1. Appropriateness <strong>of</strong> parental expectationsfor child behavior, given child’sdevelopmental level.2. Typical methods used by parents forcontrolling/disciplining the child.3. Willingness <strong>of</strong> parents to learn newmethods <strong>of</strong> discipline.4. Parents’ perception <strong>of</strong> effectiveness <strong>of</strong>discipline strategies.5. <strong>Child</strong>’s response to discipline attempts.(Continues)


CHAPTER 12 assessing family context279Box 12.4 (Continued)Goal 3: Identify Needs <strong>of</strong> the <strong>Child</strong>A. <strong>Child</strong> Social, Emotional <strong>and</strong> BehavioralFunctioning1. Behaviors that may place this child atrisk for abuse.2. Problems in adjustment resulting fromabuse <strong>and</strong> living in family with multiplestressors.B. <strong>Child</strong> Cognitive <strong>and</strong> Adaptive Abilities1. Identify child’s developmental level<strong>and</strong> coping capacity to determine mostappropriate method <strong>and</strong> level <strong>of</strong> intervention.2. Determine if abuse or chronic familystressors have led to cognitive delaysor delays in the child’s development <strong>of</strong>adaptive behaviors.3. <strong>Child</strong>’s attributions for the abuse <strong>and</strong>reaction to family difficulties.Goal 4: Assessing Parent–<strong>Child</strong> Relationship<strong>and</strong> Abuse DynamicsA. Risk <strong>of</strong> parent for future abuse <strong>and</strong>neglect.B. The quality <strong>of</strong> the parent-relationships.C. Parental empathy toward children’s feelingsSource: Crooks, C. V., & Wolfe, D. A. (2004). <strong>Child</strong> abuse <strong>and</strong> neglect. In E. J. Mash & R.A. Barkley (Eds.),<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> childhood disorders (4th ed., pp. 639–684). New York: Guilford Press.efficient method for collecting informationon the child’s family. Second, thesemeasures are <strong>of</strong>ten st<strong>and</strong>ardized <strong>and</strong> easilyobtainable. Third, these rating scalestend to have the best normative data thatallow for interpretations <strong>of</strong> scores basedon some comparison group. Thus, theserating scales tend to be the most useful inmany clinical assessments.However, such assessment methods arenot without limitations. Morsbach <strong>and</strong>Prinz (2006) reviewed eight measures thatuse parent-report <strong>of</strong> their own parentingbehaviors. Their evaluation <strong>of</strong> these measuressuggests that most demonstratedacceptable internal consistency (.70 <strong>and</strong>above). Further, most measures showedmoderate concordance between parent <strong>and</strong>child ratings <strong>of</strong> parenting (.23–37) withsomewhat higher concordance betweenreports <strong>of</strong> the two parents <strong>and</strong> betweenparent report <strong>and</strong> observations <strong>of</strong> parentingbehavior. However, these authorsnoted that parents are <strong>of</strong>ten asked to makeestimates <strong>of</strong> high-frequency behaviors (e.g.,yelling) over long periods <strong>of</strong> time (e.g., 6months) which may make accurate reportsdifficult. Further, they noted that many <strong>of</strong>the questions deal with sensitive issues thatmay not be socially desirable <strong>and</strong> may beconsidered intrusive by parents. As a result,such questions could result in biases intheir responses. Finally, these authors alsonoted that many scales <strong>of</strong>ten include vaguequantifiers (e.g., frequently, sometimes,never) that may also influence the accuracy<strong>of</strong> parents’ responses.Thus, although we have chosen to focuslargely on ratings scales for the assessment<strong>of</strong> family functioning for the reasons notedabove, it is important to recognize the limitationsin the information obtained by thisassessment format. As noted by McMahon<strong>and</strong> Frick (2007), information obtainedby these measures should be interpretedwith other assessment information, suchas interviews <strong>and</strong> behavioral observations,whenever possible.


280 CHAPTER 12 assessing family contextParenting Styles<strong>and</strong> PracticesThere is broad consensus that parentingbehaviors exert a significant influence onchild development. There is less consensusregarding the specific aspects <strong>of</strong> parentingthat are most crucial to child adjustment.However, Darling <strong>and</strong> Steinberg (1993)provide a good context for conceptualizingparenting <strong>and</strong> its effect on child <strong>and</strong> adolescentadjustment.Darling <strong>and</strong> Steinberg (1993) divide parentinginto two main components: parentingstyles <strong>and</strong> parenting practices. Theseauthors define parenting styles as “a constellation<strong>of</strong> attitudes toward the child thatare communicated to the child <strong>and</strong> createan emotional climate in which the parents’behaviors are expressed” (p. 493). Theseauthors use Baumrind’s (1971) typologyto exemplify parenting style. Baumrinddivides parenting styles into three types.The authoritarian style is characterized by arule-adherence orientation that de-emphasizesautonomy <strong>and</strong> emotional support. Thepermissive style is a child-centered style inwhich child autonomy is <strong>of</strong> primary importance<strong>and</strong> rules <strong>and</strong> dem<strong>and</strong>s are minimal.The authoritative style is characterized byemotional support <strong>and</strong> respect for appropriateautonomy in the child but in the context<strong>of</strong> clearly defined <strong>and</strong> consistently enforcedrules. It is this last parenting style, Authoritative,that research has consistently linkedto healthier child adjustment.In contrast to parenting style, parentingpractices are defined as the techniquesused by the parent to socialize their child<strong>and</strong> enforce rules. For example, a specificdiscipline practice (e.g., degree <strong>of</strong> corporalpunishment), use <strong>of</strong> positive parenting strategies(e.g., praise <strong>and</strong> reward for appropriatebehavior), consistency in parenting, <strong>and</strong>appropriate supervision <strong>and</strong> monitoring <strong>of</strong>a child’s behavior by a parent are all examples<strong>of</strong> parenting practices that have beenlinked to child adjustment (Frick, 1994).The unique contribution <strong>of</strong> Darling <strong>and</strong>Steinberg’s model <strong>of</strong> parenting is not onlyits explicit distinction between parentingstyle <strong>and</strong> parenting behaviors but also itsclear specification <strong>of</strong> how these factorsinteract to influence child development.Specifically, parenting style provides a contextin which parenting behaviors influencea child’s development. As a result, the sameparenting behavior may have differenteffects on a child depending on the parentingstyle. For example, there is a generallyaccepted association between adolescents’school performance <strong>and</strong> their parents’involvement in their schooling. However,the effectiveness <strong>of</strong> parents’ school involvementin facilitating academic achievementhas been found to be greater among parentswho have an authoritative parentingstyle than among parents who show anauthoritarian parenting style (Steinberg,Lamborn, Dornbusch, & Darling, 1992).The implications <strong>of</strong> this model <strong>of</strong> parentingare important for clinical assessments<strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. Itsuggests that, to underst<strong>and</strong> the effects <strong>of</strong>parenting on a child or adolescent’s development,one must assess both parentingstyle <strong>and</strong> parenting practices. In the followingsections, we provide a review <strong>of</strong> somemeasures that have been used to assessboth parenting style (The Family EnvironmentScale – Moos & Moos, 1986) <strong>and</strong>parenting practices (Alabama ParentingQuestionnaire – Shelton, Frick, & Wootton,1996; the Parenting Scale – Arnold,O’Leary, Wolff, & Acker, 1993; DyadicParent–<strong>Child</strong> Interaction Coding System;Eyberg, Nelson, Duke, & Boggs, 2005).Family Environment Scale-Second Edition (FES)The FES (Moos & Moos, 1986) is a90-item true-false questionnaire that iswidely used to assess persons’ perceptions<strong>of</strong> their family environment. It is one <strong>of</strong> themost widely used instruments for assessing


CHAPTER 12 assessing family context281family processes (Piotrowski, 1999). It hasbeen used to assess family functioning ina wide variety <strong>of</strong> cultures (Bao-Yu & Lin-Yan, 2004; Teufel-Shone et al., 2005) <strong>and</strong> infamilies <strong>of</strong> children with a range <strong>of</strong> adjustmentproblems including Attention DeficitHyperactivity Disorder (Pressman et al.,2006), anxiety disorders (Suveg, Zeman,Flannery-Schroeder, & Cassoano, 2005)<strong>and</strong> affective disorders (Belardinelli et al.,2008).The FES can be completed by the parent<strong>and</strong>/or child (over 11 years). There arethree forms <strong>of</strong> the FES. We focus on theReal Form (Form R) <strong>of</strong> the FES, whichmeasures the respondent’s actual perceptions<strong>of</strong> the family environment. However,there are also two special forms <strong>of</strong> theFES. The Ideal Form (Form I) allows therespondent to answer items in terms <strong>of</strong> thetype <strong>of</strong> family he or she would ideally like.The Expectations Form (Form E) allowsthe respondent to answer items in terms<strong>of</strong> what he or she expects family environmentsto be like.ContentThe FES is divided into ten subscalesfrom three domains: Relationships, PersonalGrowth, <strong>and</strong> System Maintenance. Adescription <strong>of</strong> the ten subscales within thesedomains is provided in Table 12.1. Theitem content was primarily developed onthe basis <strong>of</strong> family systems theory. This isevident from the emphasis on family structure<strong>and</strong> organization <strong>and</strong> the focus on thetransactional patterns between members <strong>of</strong>Table 12.1 Subscales <strong>of</strong> the Family Environment ScaleDimension Subscale Description <strong>of</strong> Item ContentRelationship Cohesion Commitment, help, <strong>and</strong> support providedby family membersExpressivenessExtent to which family members areencouraged to express feelingsConflictAmount <strong>of</strong> anger, aggression, <strong>and</strong> conflictamong family membersPersonal growth Independence Extent to which self-sufficiency, assertiveness,<strong>and</strong> independence are encouraged inthe familyAchievement orientation Extent to which activities <strong>of</strong> familymembers are achievement-oriented <strong>and</strong>competitiveIntellectual-cultural orientation Degree <strong>of</strong> interest in political, social, <strong>and</strong>cultural activitiesActive-recreational orientation Emphasis placed on participation in social<strong>and</strong> recreational activitiesMoral-religious emphasis Importance placed on ethical <strong>and</strong> religiousissuesSystem maintenance Organization Importance placed on having a clear familystructure <strong>and</strong> well-defined rolesControlDegree to which rules <strong>and</strong> procedure forfamily are explicitSource: Moos & Moos (1986).


282 CHAPTER 12 assessing family contextthe family in the FES item content. Also, asevident from the content, the FES is bestthought <strong>of</strong> as a measure <strong>of</strong> parenting styleor family climate, rather than <strong>of</strong> specificparenting practices.NormsThe FES manual reports information on alarge normative sample that included 1,125families from all regions <strong>of</strong> the country,single-parent <strong>and</strong> multi-generational families,families drawn from ethnic minoritygroups, <strong>and</strong> families <strong>of</strong> all age groups(Moos & Moos, 1986). It is unclear howrepresentative the sample is on each <strong>of</strong>these variables. However, the authors notethat 294 families from the normative samplewere drawn r<strong>and</strong>omly from specifiedcensus tracts in the San Francisco area, <strong>and</strong>the means <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations <strong>of</strong> FESscales did not differ between this group <strong>and</strong>the rest <strong>of</strong> the normative sample.Although the normative group includesfamilies <strong>of</strong> all age groups, it is notable thatthe majority <strong>of</strong> the normative samples wasbased on the reports <strong>of</strong> adults, with muchless data available on the reports <strong>of</strong> children<strong>and</strong> adolescents. This is important becausethe authors found small but systematic differencesbetween the scales completed byparents <strong>and</strong> adolescents (Moos & Moos,1986). Specifically, adolescents perceivedless emphasis on cohesion, expressiveness,independence, <strong>and</strong> intellectual/religiousorientation <strong>and</strong> more emphasis on conflict<strong>and</strong> achievement than did their parents.Reliability <strong>and</strong> ValidityThe ten subscales <strong>of</strong> the FES generallyhave been shown to have acceptable levels<strong>of</strong> reliability in many samples. Themanual reports internal consistency estimatesin a large community sample (n= 1,067) ranging from a = 61 to a = 78(Moos & Moos, 1986). Two-month testretestreliability in a smaller communitysample (n = 47) ranged from r = 68 to r =86. One note <strong>of</strong> caution for the reliability<strong>of</strong> the FES is finding that the reliability<strong>of</strong> the scales may be lower in adolescentsamples (Boyd, Gullone, Needleman, &Burt, 1997).Moos <strong>and</strong> Moos (1986) provide a goodsummary <strong>of</strong> over 100 research articles usingthe FES, which attests to its correlationwith other measures <strong>of</strong> family functioning,its ability to differentiate distressed fromnondistressed families, <strong>and</strong> its sensitivityto treatment effects. Many more studieshave been published since this review(Piotrowski, 1999).As noted above, the FES has beenwidely used with several different clinicalpopulations <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.For example, using the FES, Suveg et al.(2005) noted that mothers <strong>of</strong> children withan anxiety disorder showed less emotionalexpressiveness than non-clinic referredchildren (ages 8–12). As another example,Pressman et al. (2006) found that families<strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents with AttentionDeficit Hyperactivity disorder reportedhigher rates <strong>of</strong> family conflict on the FES.Finally, Lucia <strong>and</strong> Breslau (2006) reportedthat higher levels <strong>of</strong> family cohesion, asmeasured by the FES when children wereage 6, were associated with fewer emotional<strong>and</strong> attentional problems in thechildren at age 11.Probably the biggest threat to the validity<strong>of</strong> the FES is the failure to validate thescale structure through factor analyses.The scales were designed primarily on thebasis <strong>of</strong> content <strong>and</strong> face validity. Unfortunately,factor analyses have generallyisolated anywhere from two (e.g., Fowler,1982) to six (Sanford, Bingham, & Zucker,1999) or seven (Robertson & Hyde, 1982)factors on the FES. No study has providedconvincing evidence supporting the tenscalestructure that is the basis for mostinterpretations from the FES.


CHAPTER 12 assessing family context283Alabama ParentingQuestionnaire (APQ)The APQ (Shelton et al., 1996) is a measure<strong>of</strong> parenting behavior that was developedfor use with parents <strong>of</strong> elementary-schoolagedchildren <strong>and</strong> adolescents (6 to 17years old). However, it has been used insamples as young as ages 3 <strong>and</strong> 4 (Clerkin,Marks, Policaro, & Halperin, 2007; Dadds,Maujean, & Fraser, 2003) with some modification<strong>of</strong> its content. It consists <strong>of</strong> 42items that are presented in both globalreport (i.e., questionnaire) <strong>and</strong> telephoneinterview formats, <strong>and</strong> there are separateversions <strong>of</strong> each format for parents <strong>and</strong>children. Also, Elgar, Waschbusch, Dadds,<strong>and</strong> Sivaldason (2007) have developed a9-item short version <strong>of</strong> the scale for use asa brief screener.Most <strong>of</strong> the research using the APQ hasused the questionnaire formats. Items onthis format are rated on a five-point Likerttypefrequency scale <strong>and</strong> ask the informanthow frequently each <strong>of</strong> the various parentingpractices typically occurs in the home.On the telephone interview format, fourinterviews are conducted with parents <strong>and</strong>children with at least 3 days between eachinterview. The informant is asked to reportthe frequency with which each parentingpractice has occurred over the previous 3days <strong>and</strong> responses for each item are averagedacross the four interviews.ContentThe content <strong>of</strong> the APQ was developedto assess the five dimensions <strong>of</strong> parentingthat have been most consistently relatedto behavior problems in youth: Involvement,Positive Parenting, Poor Monitoring/Supervision,Inconsistent Discipline,<strong>and</strong> Corporal Punishment (Shelton et al.,1996). It also includes several other itemsassessing “other discipline practices,” suchas use <strong>of</strong> time out or taking away privileges.The items on the APQ <strong>and</strong> its subscales areprovided in Table 12.2. The items used onthe 9-item short version are also designatedin this table (Clerkin et al., 2007). Studiesusing the APQ <strong>of</strong>ten have used scores fromthe individual scales (e.g., Frick, Christian,& Wootton, 1999) or they have used composites<strong>of</strong> these scales (e.g., Frick, Kimonis,D<strong>and</strong>reaux, & Farrell, 2003). ThereTable 12.2 Subscales <strong>of</strong> the Alabama Parenting QuestionnaireInvolvement1. You have a friendly talk with your child4. You volunteer to help with special activities that your child is involved in (such as sports, boy/girlscouts, church youth groups)7. You play games or do other fun things with your child9. You ask your child about his/her day in school11. You help your child with his/her homework14. You ask your child what his/her plans are for the coming day15. You drive your child to a special activity20. You talk to your child about his/her friends23. Your child helps plan family activities26. You attend PTA meetings, parent/teacher conferences, or other meetings at your child’s school(Continues)


284 CHAPTER 12 assessing family contextTable 12.2 (Continued)Positive parenting2. You let your child know when he/she is doing a good job with something a5. You reward or give something extra to your child for obeying you or behaving well13. You compliment your child when he/she does something well a16. You praise your child if he/she behaves well a18. You hug or kiss your child when he/she has done something well27. You tell your child that you like it when he/she helps around the housePoor monitoring/supervision6. Your child fails to leave a note or to let you know where he/she is going a10. Your child stays out in the evening past the time he/she is supposed to be home a17. Your child is out with friends you do not know a19. Your child goes out without a set time to be home21. Your child is out after dark without an adult with him/her24. You get so busy that you forget where your child is <strong>and</strong> what he/she is doing28. You don’t check that your child comes home from school when he/she is supposed to29. You don’t tell your child where you are going30. Your child comes home from school more than an hour past the time you expect him/her32. Your child is at home without adult supervisionInconsistent discipline3. You threaten to punish your child <strong>and</strong> then do not actually punish him/her a8. Your child talks you out <strong>of</strong> being punished after he/she has done something wrong a12. You feel that getting your child to obey you is more trouble than it’s worth22. You let your child out <strong>of</strong> a punishment early (like lift restrictions earlier than you originally said) a25. Your child is not punished when he/she has done something wrong31. The punishment you give your child depends on your moodCorporal punishment33. You spank your child with your h<strong>and</strong> when he/she has done something wrong35. You slap your child when he/she has done something wrong38. You hit your child with a belt, switch, or other object when he/she has done something wrongOther discipline practices34. You ignore your child when he/she is misbehaving36. You take away privileges or money from your child as a punishment37. You send your child to his/her room as a punishment39. You yell or scream at your child when he/she has done something wrong40. You calmly explain to your child why his/her behavior was wrong when he/she misbehaves41. You use time out (make him/her sit or st<strong>and</strong> in corner) as a punishment42. You give your child extra chores as a punishmentaItems that are included in the 9-item short screening version <strong>of</strong> the APQ (Clerkin et al., 2007).


CHAPTER 12 assessing family context285are two common ways <strong>of</strong> forming compositescores. The first way is to st<strong>and</strong>ardizethe scores (e.g., create z-scores) <strong>and</strong> thencombine the Involvement <strong>and</strong> PositiveParenting scales into a Positive ParentingComposite <strong>and</strong> the Poor Monitoring/Supervision, Inconsistent Discipline, <strong>and</strong>Corporal Punishment scales into a NegativeParenting Composite. The secondmethod for forming composites from theAPQ is to create a single DysfunctionalParenting composite by st<strong>and</strong>ardizing allfive scales, inversely scoring the positiveparenting scales, <strong>and</strong> summing all fivedimensions.NormsOne <strong>of</strong> main limitations in the APQ is thelack <strong>of</strong> norm-referenced scores that can beused to interpret the scales. However, thereare now two studies that provide scoresfrom fairly large samples <strong>of</strong> non-referredchildren, from which cut-scores can bedeveloped. The first study was conductedwith 1,402 children ages 4 to 9 (Elgar et al.,2007) in Australia <strong>and</strong> the second studywas conducted with 1,219 German schoolchildren ages 10 to 12 (Essau, Sasagawa, &Frick, 2006).Reliability <strong>and</strong> ValidityAs noted previously, most <strong>of</strong> the publishedresearch using the APQ to date has utilizedthe global report formats <strong>of</strong> the scale,with the exception <strong>of</strong> Shelton et al. (1996).Across studies, the reliability <strong>and</strong> stability <strong>of</strong>APQ scores have generally been acceptablewith several notable exceptions. First, theinternal consistency <strong>of</strong> the short three-itemCorporal punishment scale has <strong>of</strong>ten beenquite low on all formats. Second, the internalconsistency <strong>of</strong> the Poor Monitoring/Supervision scale has been low in the interviewformat (all alphas below .50 – Sheltonet al., 1996). Third, Frick et al. (1999)showed poor reliability <strong>of</strong> the child-reportformats in very young children (below age9) (see also Shelton et al., 1996).Several studies have provided factorialsupport for the five dimensions aroundwhich the scale was developed (Elgar et al.,2007; Essau et al., 2006). Also, parent ratingson the APQ are significantly associatedwith observations <strong>of</strong> parenting behaviorin 4- to 8-year-old boys (Hawes & Dadds,2006). However, the most common use <strong>of</strong>the APQ has been to study parenting infamilies <strong>of</strong> children with conduct problems.An association between APQ scales <strong>and</strong> conductproblems has been reported in community(Dadds et al., 2003), clinic-referred(Frick et al., 1999; Hawes & Dadds, 2006),<strong>and</strong> inpatient samples (Blader, 2004), as wellas in families with deaf children (Brubaker& Szakowski, 2000) <strong>and</strong> families with substance-abusingparents (Stanger, Dumenci,Kamon, & Burstein, 2004). Also, thesestudies have documented this relationshipin samples as young as age 4 (Dadds et al.,2003; Hawes & Dadds, 2006) <strong>and</strong> as old asage 17 (Frick et al., 1999).Importantly, Frick et al. (1999) reportedsome differences in which dimensions <strong>of</strong>parenting were most strongly associatedwith conduct problems at different ages,with Inconsistent Discipline being moststrongly associated in young children (ages6–8), Corporal Punishment being moststrongly associated in older children (ages9–12), <strong>and</strong> Involvement <strong>and</strong> Poor Monitoring/Supervisionbeing most stronglyrelated in adolescents (ages 13–17).Although the most common use <strong>of</strong> theAPQ has been to assess parenting in families<strong>of</strong> youth with conduct problems, it hasbeen used to assess family correlates toanxiety disorders (Pfiffner & McBurnett,2006) <strong>and</strong> to assess parenting in families <strong>of</strong>depressed parents (Cummings, Keller, &Davies, 2005). Finally, several studies haveused the APQ scales to test changes in parentingbehaviors following treatment (e.g.,Feinfield & Baker, 2004; Hawes & Dadds,2006).


286 CHAPTER 12 assessing family contextParenting Scale (PS)The PS (Arnold et al., 1993) is anothercommonly used measure <strong>of</strong> parenting. It isa 30-item parent report scale that focusesspecifically on parents’ attitudes <strong>and</strong> beliefsabout discipline. The items are all rated ona seven-point scale in which the parent isasked to estimate the probability with whichthey would use a particular discipline strategy(e.g., when my child misbehaves, I spank, slap,grab, or hit my child). It was originally developedfor use with young children (ages 18–48months; Arnold et al., 1993) <strong>and</strong> has primarilybeen used to assess parenting in preschoolchildren. However, there is evidence for itsutility in samples <strong>of</strong> children as old as 11 years<strong>of</strong> age (Prinzie, Onghena, & Hellinckx, 2007;Steele, Nesbitt-Daly, Daniel, & Foreh<strong>and</strong>,2005).ContentThe PS items can be grouped into threedimensions: Laxness, Overreactivity, <strong>and</strong>Verbosity. There has been some debateover the appropriateness <strong>of</strong> this three scalestructure because it has obtained factoranalytic support in some studies (i.e., Arney,Rogers, Baghurst, Sawyer, & Prior, 2008;Arnold et al., 1993; Rhoades & O’Leary,2007) but not in others (Prinzie et al., 2007;Reitman et al., 2001; Steele et al., 2005).The difference in the factor analyses typicallyinvolves whether the Verbosity factoremerges as a separate dimension. Also,Rhoades <strong>and</strong> O’Leary (2007) developed aPS-Should scale that was designed to assesshow parents believe they “should disciplinetheir children,” rather than assessing theirreport <strong>of</strong> actual discipline practices.NormsOne <strong>of</strong> main limitations in the PS is thelack <strong>of</strong> norm-referenced scores that canbe used to interpret the scales. However,there are several studies that provide scoresfrom fairly large samples <strong>of</strong> non-referredchildren from which cut-scores can bedeveloped. Arney et al. (2008) provideddata from 1,656 mothers <strong>of</strong> children (ages3–5) from South Australia <strong>and</strong> Prinzieet al. (2007) provided data from 596 mothers<strong>and</strong> 559 fathers <strong>of</strong> children ages 5 to11 years in Belgium. Finally, Rhoades <strong>and</strong>O’Leary (2007) provided data on 453 families<strong>of</strong> children ages 3 to 7 years from thenortheast United States.Reliability <strong>and</strong> ValidityMost studies <strong>of</strong> the PS show adequateinternal consistency <strong>and</strong> test-retest reliabilityfor the PS (e.g., Arnold et al., 1993;Rhoades & O’Leary, 2007) with the exception<strong>of</strong> the Verbosity scale (Arney et al.,2008). Further, the PS has been shownto be correlated with other measures <strong>of</strong>parenting practices (Rhoades & O’Leary,2007; Steele et al., 2005) <strong>and</strong> has beenassociated with measures <strong>of</strong> adjustmentproblems in children (Arney et al., 2008;Prinzie et al., 2007). Scores on the PS havealso been shown to be sensitive to effects<strong>of</strong> interventions designed to improve parentingbehaviors ( S<strong>and</strong>ers, Markie-Dadds,Tully, & Bor, 2000).Dyadic Parent–<strong>Child</strong> InteractionCoding System (DPICS)The DPICS (Eyberg et al., 2005) is a highlystructured coding system designed to assessmaternal behaviors <strong>and</strong> parent–child interactionsin several st<strong>and</strong>ard settings. In contrastto the other measures <strong>of</strong> parenting thathave been reviewed, the DPICS is an observationalsystem. It has typically been usedto code parent–child interactions <strong>of</strong> preschoolchildren (e.g., Eisenstadt, Eyberg,McNeil, Newcomb, & Funderbunk, 1993;Robinson & Eyberg, 1981). Parents <strong>and</strong>


CHAPTER 12 assessing family context287children are observed in two 5-min periods,typically in a clinic playroom setting.In the <strong>Child</strong> Directed Interaction (CDI)the parent is instructed to allow the child tochoose any activity <strong>and</strong> to play along withthe child. In the Parent Directed Interaction(PDI) the parent is instructed to selectan activity <strong>and</strong> to keep the child playingaccording to parental rules.ContentThe 5-min interactions are videotaped forlater coding. The DPICS includes a detailedmanual for coding several parent <strong>and</strong> childbehaviors. The system codes 12 parent <strong>and</strong>14 child behaviors. A summary <strong>of</strong> thesebehaviors included in the DPICS is providedin Table 12.3. In addition to discrete behaviors,several additional categories are includedin the DPICS to code sequences <strong>of</strong> behaviors.Parental responses (i.e., ignores or responds)to child’s defiant behavior <strong>and</strong> child responses(i.e., compliances, non-compliances, or noopportunity) to parental comm<strong>and</strong>s arecoded. The coding system is a continuousfrequency count <strong>of</strong> all behaviors observedduring the 5-min interaction periods.NormsThe normative information available onthe DPICS is quite limited. Robinson <strong>and</strong>Eyberg (1983) provide data on 22 familieswith children between the ages <strong>of</strong> 2 <strong>and</strong>7. The sample was primarily two-parentfamilies (73%) <strong>and</strong> highly educated (mean<strong>of</strong> 15.2 years <strong>of</strong> education for parents). As aresult, the generalizability <strong>of</strong> this informationto other samples is questionable.Reliability <strong>and</strong> ValidityNot surprisingly, given the very detailedbehavioral descriptions provided by theDPICS manual, trained observers havebeen able to achieve quite high interraterreliability with the DPICS. In a sampleTable 12.3 Categories from the DyadicParent–<strong>Child</strong> Interaction Coding SystemMaternal behaviors1. Praise(a) Labeled praise(b) Unlabeled praise2. Comm<strong>and</strong>(a) Direct comm<strong>and</strong>s(b) Indirect comm<strong>and</strong>s3. Other verbalizations(a) Descriptive/reflective questions(b) Descriptive/reflective statements(c) Irrelevant verbalization(d) Verbal acknowledgment4. Responses to child behavior(a) Physical positive(b) Ignore(c) Critical statement(d) Physical negative<strong>Child</strong> behaviors1. Deviant(a) Whine(b) Cry(c) Smart talk(d) Yell(e) Destructive(f) Physical negative2. Response to comm<strong>and</strong>s(a) Compliance(b) Noncompliance(c) No opportunitySource: Eyberg & Robinson (1983).<strong>of</strong> 42 families (20 clinic-referred <strong>and</strong> 22normal control) the mean interrater reliabilityfor parent behaviors was .91 <strong>and</strong>for child behaviors was .92 (Robinson& Eyberg, 1981). In addition, DPICSscores have been shown to differentiatefamilies <strong>of</strong> clinic-referred children withconduct problems from families <strong>of</strong> normalcontrol children (Eyberg et al., 2005;Robinson & Eyberg, 1981). Scores from


288 CHAPTER 12 assessing family contextthe DPICS have also been shown to besensitive to interventions for families <strong>of</strong>children with behavior problems (Eisenstadtet al., 1993; Eyberg & Robinson,1982; Hembree-Kigin & McNeil, 1995).Parenting StressThe second dimension <strong>of</strong> family functioningthat is critical to assess in most clinicalassessment <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents isparental stress. A high level <strong>of</strong> stress caninfluence children’s adjustment in a number<strong>of</strong> ways, one <strong>of</strong> which is by making itmore difficult for a parent to use optimalparenting strategies (Whiteside-Mansellet al., 2007). For example, elevated stresscan lead to lower levels <strong>of</strong> parental warmth<strong>and</strong> higher rates <strong>of</strong> harsh parenting (Dopke,Lundahl, Dunsterville, & Lovejoy, 2003;Haskett, Ahern, Ward, & Allaire, 2006).There are two types <strong>of</strong> measures that canbe used in clinical assessment <strong>of</strong> children<strong>and</strong> adolescents. The first are measures <strong>of</strong>general life stress (e.g., life event scales) <strong>and</strong>the second are measures <strong>of</strong> stress specific toparenting. Examples <strong>of</strong> general measures <strong>of</strong>stress include the Life Experiences Survey(Sarason, Johnson, & Siegel, 1978) <strong>and</strong> theFamily Events List (Patterson, 1982). Suchmeasures <strong>of</strong> general stress have proven to beimportant for underst<strong>and</strong>ing children withbehavior problems (Johnston, 1996; Snyder,1991) <strong>and</strong> they have been related to abusivebehavior in parents (Whipple & Webster-Stratton, 1991). However, in the sectionsbelow, we focus on two measures <strong>of</strong> stressmore specifically related to parenting.<strong>Child</strong> Abuse PotentialInventory-Second EditionThe CAPI (Milner, 1986) is a 160-itemrating scale completed by a child’s parent.As the name implies, the CAPI was originallydeveloped to assess dimensions<strong>of</strong> parental behavior that have provento be risk factors for physical abuse <strong>of</strong>children. However, the CAPI assessesmultiple areas <strong>of</strong> family functioning thatare important in many clinical assessments.Further, several <strong>of</strong> the scales focusdirectly on stressors related to parentinga child or adolescent. The items requirea third-grade reading level to complete<strong>and</strong> each item is presented in a forcedchoice,agree-disagree format. The fullform takes approximately 15 min to complete.However, a brief 24-item version<strong>of</strong> the scale has been developed <strong>and</strong> hasproven to be highly correlated with thefull version (Ondersma, Chaffin, Mullins,& LeBreton, 2005).ContentThe CAPI contains three validity scales: Lie,R<strong>and</strong>om Response, <strong>and</strong> Inconsistency. TheLie scale was designed to detect tendenciesto distort responses in a socially desirablemanner. Both the R<strong>and</strong>om Response <strong>and</strong>Inconsistency scales were designed to detecthaphazard or r<strong>and</strong>om responses to itemswithout regard to item content. To test theusefulness <strong>of</strong> these validity scales, Milner <strong>and</strong>Crouch (1997) had two groups <strong>of</strong> parents, 106community volunteer parents <strong>and</strong> 80 parentsattending a program for parents at risk forabuse, complete the CAPI in several differentways: answering honestly, answering in a wayto make themselves “look good,” answeringin a way to make themselves “look bad,” <strong>and</strong>answering inconsistently. These differinginstructions did affect how parents answeredthe CAPI questions, suggesting that parentscan intentionally distort their ratings. Withthe exception <strong>of</strong> detecting the faking-badcondition (58% correct identification), theCAPI validity indexes were good at detectingmost <strong>of</strong> the other response conditions, rangingfrom 82 to 100% correct identificationacross both samples <strong>of</strong> parents.There are six primary scales <strong>of</strong> theCAPI that are combined into a compositeAbuse scale. The items were developedfrom an extensive review <strong>of</strong> the child


CHAPTER 12 assessing family context289abuse <strong>and</strong> neglect literature. The Distressscale assesses parental anger, frustration,impulse control, anxiety, <strong>and</strong> depression.The Rigidity scale assesses parents’ flexibility<strong>and</strong> realism in their expectations<strong>of</strong> children’s behavior. It includes suchitems as “A child should never disobey,”“A child should always be neat,” <strong>and</strong>“A child should never talk back.” TheUnhappiness scale assesses a parent’s degree<strong>of</strong> personal fulfillment as an individual, asa parent, as a marital/sex partner, <strong>and</strong> as afriend. Problems with <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> Self is ascale with items tapping parents’ perceptions<strong>of</strong> their child’s behavior <strong>and</strong> theirperceptions <strong>of</strong> their own self-concept as aparent. The last two scales, Problems withFamily <strong>and</strong> Problems with Others, assessthe level <strong>of</strong> family conflict in the extendedfamily <strong>and</strong> the level <strong>of</strong> conflict with personsoutside the family or community agencies.NormsNormative information is available inthe CAPI manual (Milner, 1986) from asample <strong>of</strong> 836 parents, child care workers,<strong>and</strong> parent aides from Florida, California,North Carolina, Hawaii, Oklahoma,Illinois, New York, <strong>and</strong> West germany.It is unclear how this normative samplewas selected, <strong>and</strong> the representativeness<strong>of</strong> this sample in terms <strong>of</strong> parental education,socio-economic status, <strong>and</strong> ethnicityis also unclear. This is crucial informationbecause there is evidence that familyfunctioning can vary as a function <strong>of</strong> thesevariables (Maccoby & Martin, 1983). Asa result, lack <strong>of</strong> accessible information onthe normative sample hinders the abilityto make norm-referenced interpretationsfrom the CAPI scales.Reliability <strong>and</strong> ValidityThe CAPI manual provides evidence thatthe composite Abuse scale <strong>and</strong> the Distress<strong>and</strong> Rigidity scales exhibit acceptable internalconsistency <strong>and</strong> temporal stability. Thereliability <strong>of</strong> the four other individual scalestends to be more inconsistent across samples.In terms <strong>of</strong> validity, there is evidence thatCAPI scores are associated with documentedrisk factors for child abuse (Budd, Heilman,& Kane, 2000; Grietens, De Haene, &Uyteeborek, 2007; Haskett, Scott, & Fann,1995). Also there is evidence that the compositeAbuse scale can successfully discriminatebetween proven abusers <strong>and</strong> controlsubjects (Milner & Wimberley, 1980), <strong>and</strong>this extends across cultural groups (Haz &Ramirez, 1998). Finally, the Abuse scalehas proven to be sensitive to the effects <strong>of</strong>intervention with high-risk parents (Wolfe,Edwards, Manion, & Koverola, 1988).Therefore, it appears that the CAPI providesa reliable method <strong>of</strong> assessing dysfunctionalelements <strong>of</strong> a child’s family environment,including several aspects <strong>of</strong> parental stressthat are associated with child abuse. In addition,the composite Abuse scale does seemto be an index <strong>of</strong> risk for abuse, although itis important to recognize that many parentswho score high on the CAPI have no documentedevidence <strong>of</strong> abuse in the home (i.e.,false positive) (Haz & Ramirez, 1998).Parenting Stress Index-SecondEdition (PSI)The PSI (Abidin, 1986) is unique in itsfocus specifically on stressors related toparenting. It was primarily designed toassess the family context <strong>of</strong> preschoolchildren between the ages <strong>of</strong> 1 <strong>and</strong> 4,although it has been used in older samples<strong>of</strong> pre-adolescent children. Completion<strong>of</strong> the PSI requires at least a fifth-gradeeducation. It contains 151 items <strong>and</strong>generally takes 20–30 min to complete.A short-form <strong>of</strong> the PSI has been developedwith 36 items (Abidin, 1995).ContentThe items <strong>of</strong> the full PSI are divided intotwo main categories: <strong>Child</strong> Domain (47


290 CHAPTER 12 assessing family contextitems) <strong>and</strong> Parent Domain (54 items). The<strong>Child</strong> Domain consists <strong>of</strong> items that assessqualities <strong>of</strong> a child that make it difficult forparents to fulfill their parental role. TheParent Domain assesses sources <strong>of</strong> stress<strong>and</strong> disability related to parental functioning.Table 12.4 provides a summary <strong>of</strong> thescales that constitute the <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> Parentdomains. The PSI also allows for the computation<strong>of</strong> a composite score that providesan overall indicator <strong>of</strong> the amount <strong>of</strong> stressin the parent–child system.Table 12.4 Item Content <strong>of</strong> the Parenting Stress Index-Second EditionScale Items Characteristics <strong>of</strong> High Scorers<strong>Child</strong> domain 47 <strong>Child</strong> displays qualities that make it difficult for the parent to fulfillparenting rolesAdaptability 11 <strong>Child</strong> shows inability to change from one task to another withoutemotional upset, avoids strangers, is overreactive to changes in routine<strong>and</strong> difficult to calmAcceptability 7 <strong>Child</strong> is not as attractive, intelligent, or pleasant as the parent hadhoped or expectedDem<strong>and</strong>ingness 9 <strong>Child</strong> is very dem<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> parents’ time <strong>and</strong> energy, with patternssuch as frequent crying, frequent requests for help, <strong>and</strong> frequent minorproblem behaviorsMood 5 <strong>Child</strong> is frequently unhappy, sad, <strong>and</strong> cryingDistractibility/HyperactivityReinforcesparent9 <strong>Child</strong> displays overactivity, restlessness, distractibility, <strong>and</strong> short attentionspan, fails to finish things, <strong>and</strong> shifts from one activity to another6 Interactions between child <strong>and</strong> parent fail to produce good feelings inthe parent; associated with parental feelings <strong>of</strong> rejection <strong>and</strong> poor selfconceptas parentParent domain 54 Indicates significant stress on the parent–child system that is related todimensions <strong>of</strong> parental functioningDepression 9 Parent reports significant feelings <strong>of</strong> depression <strong>and</strong> guilt. High scoresmay prevent parent from mobilizing sufficient levels <strong>of</strong> psychic <strong>and</strong>physical energy to fulfill parenting responsibilitiesAttachment 7 Parent does not feel emotional closeness to child <strong>and</strong> parent perceivesan inability to accurately read <strong>and</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> child’s feelings <strong>and</strong> needsRestriction <strong>of</strong>roleSense <strong>of</strong>competence7 Parents feel that parental role restricts their freedom <strong>and</strong> impairs theirattempts to maintain own identity13 Parents do not feel that they can adequately fulfill their parental roleseither because <strong>of</strong> a lack <strong>of</strong> knowledge <strong>of</strong> child development or a limitedrange <strong>of</strong> child-management skillsSocial isolation 6 Parents perceive themselves as socially isolated from their peers,relatives, <strong>and</strong> other social support systemsRelationshipwith spouse7 Parents perceive that they do not receive emotional <strong>and</strong> physical supportfrom their spouse in area <strong>of</strong> child managementParent health 5 Parents report a deterioration in physical health that is impacting theirability to fulfill parental responsibilitiesSource: Abidin (1986).


CHAPTER 12 assessing family context291The 36-item short form <strong>of</strong> the scalehas been tested <strong>and</strong> items form two relativelydistinct factors <strong>of</strong> parental distress<strong>and</strong> dysfunctional parent child interactions(Haskett et al., 2006). Importantly, thecorrelation between the total scores on theshort <strong>and</strong> long form is generally quite high(e.g., r = 87; Abidin, 1995).NormsThe normative sample for the PSI consisted<strong>of</strong> 534 parents <strong>of</strong> children referredto a small group <strong>of</strong> pediatric clinics in Virginia;the median age <strong>of</strong> the children was9 months (SD = 23.2 months). The representativeness<strong>of</strong> the normative sample isone <strong>of</strong> the major weaknesses <strong>of</strong> the scale.The sample consisted <strong>of</strong> primarily White(92%), highly educated (1/3 with collegedegrees) parents from central Virginia.Thus, the use <strong>of</strong> norm-referenced scoresfor families that do not match these characteristicsis questionable.Reliability <strong>and</strong> ValidityThe manual <strong>of</strong> the PSI provides convincingevidence for the internal consistency<strong>and</strong> temporal stability <strong>of</strong> the threecomposite scores: Total Stress, ParentDomain, <strong>and</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Domain. The reliabilitycoefficients for the individual scales,however, are much more variable <strong>and</strong>typically exhibit relatively low reliabilityestimates. The manual provides one <strong>of</strong>the best summaries <strong>of</strong> the extensive use <strong>of</strong>the PSI in research on the family context<strong>of</strong> preschoolers (see also Abidin, Flens, &Austin, 2006 for an updated review). Ingeneral, the PSI scales have been correlatedwith other measures <strong>of</strong> family functioning(Pinderhughes, Dodge, Bates,Petit, & Zelli, 2000), including correlationswith observations <strong>of</strong> parentingbehavior (Bigras, LaFreniere, & Dumas,1996). Also, the PSI has differentiatedfamilies who are experiencing major stressorsfrom nonstressed families (Holden& Banez, 1996; Whiteside-Mansell et al.,2007) <strong>and</strong> has proven sensitive to treatmenteffects (Nixon, Sweeney, Erickson,& Touyz, 2003).Also, factor analyses generally supportthe broad Parent <strong>and</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Domainsfor grouping the various PSI subscales,although some studies have provided supportfor a third Parent–<strong>Child</strong> InteractionDomain, which includes the subscales<strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Acceptability, <strong>Child</strong> ReinforcesParent, <strong>and</strong> Parent Attachment to <strong>Child</strong>(Hutcheson & Black, 1996; Solis & Abidin,1991). Further, Bigras et al. (1996)reported that, in a sample <strong>of</strong> 218 mothers<strong>of</strong> preschoolers, the Parent <strong>and</strong> <strong>Child</strong>Domains predicted parental, familial,<strong>and</strong> child outcomes different from thoseobtained from other sources. Specifically,the Parent Domain was more strongly<strong>and</strong> independently associated with measures<strong>of</strong> marital adjustment <strong>and</strong> maternaldepression, whereas the <strong>Child</strong> Domainwas more strongly <strong>and</strong> independentlyassociated with child difficulties reportedby the mother <strong>and</strong> children’s problemsobserved during parent–child interactions.These results are important in suggestingthat the two domains are valid inassessing somewhat independent dimensions<strong>of</strong> family functioning.Marital ConflictThere is a long history <strong>of</strong> research showinga link between divorce <strong>and</strong> childbehavior problems. The most comprehensivesummary <strong>of</strong> this research comes fromAmato <strong>and</strong> Keith (1991). These authorsconducted a meta-analysis <strong>of</strong> 92 publishedstudies <strong>of</strong> the impact <strong>of</strong> divorce on a child’spsychological well-being. The combinedsamples from the 92 studies involvedover 13,000 children. This meta-analysisrevealed that divorce consistently had a


292 CHAPTER 12 assessing family contextnegative impact on several types <strong>of</strong> childwell-being (e.g., conduct problems, schoolachievement, social adjustment, <strong>and</strong> selfconcept).These studies suggested that therelationship between divorce <strong>and</strong> psychologicaldifficulties in children was greatestwithin the 2 years immediately followinga divorce.The meta-analysis also provided intriguingdata in support <strong>of</strong> the theory that it is theconflict that occurs between parents before<strong>and</strong> during the separation that has the mostdetrimental impact on a child’s adjustment(see also Emery, 1982). Whereas children<strong>of</strong> divorced families tended to have pooreradjustment than children in low- conflict,intact families, children in intact, high-conflicthomes tended to have the poorest adjustment<strong>of</strong> all three groups. Also consistentwith this perspective, several studies foundthat less conflict <strong>and</strong> better divorce cooperationbetween parents predicted better postdivorceadjustment for children.The implications <strong>of</strong> these findings areimportant to clinical assessments <strong>of</strong> children<strong>and</strong> adolescents. They suggest that it isnot simply enough to determine the maritalstatus <strong>of</strong> a child’s parents for underst<strong>and</strong>ingthe potential impact <strong>of</strong> the parents’ maritalrelationship on the child. A more importantfocus <strong>of</strong> assessment is the overt conflictbetween parents that is witnessed by thechild.There are several marital inventoriesthat are frequently used in research <strong>and</strong>clinical practice, <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>ten included in theclinical assessment <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents(McMahon & Frick, 2007). TheMarital Adjustment Test (MAT; Locke &Wallace, 1959) <strong>and</strong> the Dyadic AdjustmentScale (DAS; Spanier, 1976) are two selfreportinstruments that have been shownto produce reliable scores <strong>and</strong> differentiatepersons in distressed <strong>and</strong> nondistressedmarriages. However, these inventoriestend to focus on general marital satisfactionrather than on overt conflict per se.The O’Leary-Porter Scale (OPS; Porter &O’Leary, 1980) is a brief rating scale thatfocuses on overt marital conflict <strong>and</strong>, evenmore specifically, on marital conflict thatis witnessed by the child or adolescent. Asa result, the OPS is uniquely suited for usein clinical assessments <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.O’Leary-Porter Scale (OPS)The OPS (Porter & O’Leary, 1980) wasdeveloped specifically for studying the associationbetween marital adjustment <strong>and</strong> childbehavior problems. The OPS is a 20-itemself-report inventory within which areembedded nine items that assess the degree<strong>of</strong> marital conflict witnessed by the child.A parent rates on a five-point frequencyscale (Never to Very Often) how <strong>of</strong>ten thechild witnesses arguments between himselfor herself <strong>and</strong> the spouse over money, discipline,wife’s role in family, <strong>and</strong> personalhabits <strong>of</strong> the spouse. Two questions also askfor overall estimates <strong>of</strong> the amount <strong>of</strong> verbal<strong>and</strong> physical hostility between spouses thatis witnessed by the child.There is little normative data on theOPS. However, 2-week test-retest reliabilityin a sample <strong>of</strong> 14 families was found tobe quite high (r = 92) (Porter & O’Leary,1980). These authors also reported thatthe OPS was correlated with several types<strong>of</strong> maladjustment in children. Highlightingthe importance <strong>of</strong> focusing specificallyon overt conflict, these authors found thatthe OPS was more consistently associatedwith child adjustment difficulties thanwas a measure <strong>of</strong> general marital satisfaction(i.e., the MAT). This associationbetween scores on the OPS has been replicatedin other studies (Foreh<strong>and</strong>, Long,& Hedrick, 1987; Mann & MacKenzie,1996). Thus, it appears that the OPS capturesthe critical component <strong>of</strong> maritaldiscord in terms <strong>of</strong> its detrimental effecton child adjustment.


CHAPTER 12 assessing family context293Parental AdjustmentIn the introduction to this chapter, wediscussed several ways in which assessingparental psychiatric adjustment is criticalto the clinical assessment <strong>of</strong> children. Wediscussed two areas <strong>of</strong> research, one onparental depression <strong>and</strong> another on parentalanxiety, that suggest that informationobtained from a parent must be interpretedin light <strong>of</strong> the parent’s level <strong>of</strong> emotionaldistress. We also discussed the importance<strong>of</strong> obtaining a family psychiatric history formaking differential diagnoses <strong>and</strong> treatmentrecommendations. In this section, weprovide a brief overview <strong>of</strong> basic researchshowing the link between parent <strong>and</strong> childadjustment difficulties that can aid theclinical assessor in structuring assessments<strong>and</strong> making appropriate interpretationsfrom the assessment information.Parental DepressionOne type <strong>of</strong> parental adjustment that has awell-documented link to child developmentis parental depression. Studies have foundthat between 40% (Orvaschel, Walsh-Allis,& Ye, 1988) <strong>and</strong> 74% (Hammen et al.,1987) <strong>of</strong> the children <strong>of</strong> depressed parentsexhibit significant adjustment problems.Depression in parents places children atrisk for a number <strong>of</strong> problems spanningacademic, social, emotional, <strong>and</strong> behavioraldomains (Downey & Coyne, 1990).Therefore, it seems that parental depressionis a nonspecific risk factor for problemsin children. That is, it is not specificallyrelated to the development <strong>of</strong> a single type<strong>of</strong> child behavior problem.There are two possible exceptions tothis non-specific relationship, both relatedto subtypes within affective disorders.First, Weissman, Warner, Wickramaratne,& Prus<strong>of</strong>f (1988) found some preliminaryevidence that early-onset recurrent depression(depression that has its initial onsetbefore adulthood) might have a more specificlink with childhood depression. Second,family histories <strong>of</strong> bipolar disordersin parents predict a risk for subsequentbipolar disorder in adolescents with achildhood-onset <strong>of</strong> depressive symptoms(Geller et al., 1993).Goodman <strong>and</strong> Gotlib (1999) reviewedthe literature on the risk for problems inadjustment in children <strong>of</strong> depressed mothers.They outline four possible mechanismsto explain this risk: (a) an inherited predispositiontransmitted from parent to child;(b) failure <strong>of</strong> the child to develop appropriateemotional regulation strategies;(c) exposure to negative maternal moods,thoughts, <strong>and</strong> behaviors; <strong>and</strong> (d) exposureto a high rate <strong>of</strong> stressors associated withmother’s depression (e.g., higher rates <strong>of</strong>marital conflict). These links clearly illustratethe need to assess parental depressionto underst<strong>and</strong> several potential causal factorsthat could help to explain a child’s oradolescent’s problems in adjustment.Parental Substance AbuseA comprehensive review <strong>of</strong> the literaturefound that, like parental depression, parentalalcoholism is associated with a number<strong>of</strong> child adjustment problems. West <strong>and</strong>Prinz (1987) reported studies finding anassociation between parental alcohol abuse<strong>and</strong> the following problems in their children:hyperactivity, conduct problems,delinquency, substance abuse, intellectualimpairment, somatic problems, anxiety,depression, <strong>and</strong> social deficits. Like depression,some <strong>of</strong> the lack <strong>of</strong> specificity in itseffect on child adjustment may be due to afailure to define subgroups within parentswho abuse substances (Frick, 1993). Alternatively,West <strong>and</strong> Prinz reviewed severalstudies suggesting that the effects <strong>of</strong> havinga substance-abusing parent on a child’sadjustment may be mediated through the


294 CHAPTER 12 assessing family contextimpact on the home environment <strong>and</strong>the impact on parent <strong>and</strong> child interactions.Consistent with this view, parentalsubstance use has been linked to a host <strong>of</strong>problematic parent practices, includinghigher rates <strong>of</strong> abuse (Ondersma, 2007;Walsh, MacMillan, & Jamieson, 2003).Parental Antisocial BehaviorThe intergenerational link to antisocialbehavior is a consistent finding in research<strong>and</strong> one that has long intrigued social scientists<strong>and</strong> policy-makers alike (see Frick& Loney, 2002, for a review). Early studiestended to focus on the intergenerationallink to criminality. This research foundthat the link was independent <strong>of</strong> socioeconomicstatus, neighborhood, <strong>and</strong> intelligence(Glueck & Glueck, 1968). Morerecent studies have focused on psychiatricdefinitions <strong>of</strong> antisocial disorders. As instudies <strong>of</strong> criminality, children diagnosedwith antisocial disorders are significantlymore likely to have parents with antisocialdisorders than are children without conductproblems (Frick et al., 1992; Monuteaux,Faraone, Gross, & Biederman, 2007).An important methodological pointin the more recent family history studieswas the fact that each used clinic controlgroups <strong>and</strong> found that histories <strong>of</strong> antisocialdisorders in parents were specificto conduct problems in children. That is,children with conduct problems not onlyhad higher rates <strong>of</strong> parental antisocial disorder(APD) than normal controls, butthey also had higher rates <strong>of</strong> parental APDthan clinic-referred children with otherproblems in adjustment (Frick et al., 1992).Therefore, unlike parental depression <strong>and</strong>substance abuse, parent antisocial behaviorappears to have a more specific relationshipto a particular child problem (i.e., conductproblems).Frick <strong>and</strong> Loney (2002) reviewed datasupporting several potential mechanismsto explain this link including an inheriteddisposition passed from parent to child,parental modeling <strong>of</strong> antisocial behaviors,<strong>and</strong> disruptions in the family caused bythe parent’s antisocial behavior. In support<strong>of</strong> at least some inherited predisposition,Tapscott, Frick, Wootton, <strong>and</strong> Kruh (1996)showed that a paternal history <strong>of</strong> antisocialpersonality disorder was associated with ahigher rate <strong>of</strong> Conduct Disorder in theirbiological <strong>of</strong>fspring, even if the father hadno contact with the child since the firstyear <strong>of</strong> life.Parental ADHDThere is evidence that parents <strong>and</strong> otherbiological relatives <strong>of</strong> children with ADHDshow more attentional problems (Albert-Corush, Firestone, & Goodman, 1986) <strong>and</strong>a higher rate <strong>of</strong> ADHD (Faraone, Biederman,Keenan, & Tsuang, 1991). However,these studies may have underestimatedthe link between parent <strong>and</strong> child ADHDby studying the parents’ current adjustment.Given that 30–50% <strong>of</strong> children withADHD may not be diagnosed with thisdisorder as adults (Barkley, Fischer, Smallish,& Fletcher, 2002), it may be that many<strong>of</strong> the parents <strong>of</strong> ADHD children exhibitedADHD as a child but are not currentlyshowing symptoms.To test this possibility, Frick et al. (1991)studied the childhood histories <strong>of</strong> parents <strong>of</strong>clinic-referred children. A child’s biologicalparent reported on whether or not he or shehad problems associated with ADHD beforethe age <strong>of</strong> 18 <strong>and</strong> then completed a similarfamily history questionnaire for all firstdegreerelatives. <strong>Child</strong>ren with ADHD weremore likely to have mothers, fathers, <strong>and</strong>other biological relatives who also exhibitedADHD as children than were other clinicreferredchildren. In fact, approximately75% <strong>of</strong> the 103 children with ADHD hadone biological relative with a significant history<strong>of</strong> ADHD (27% <strong>of</strong> mothers <strong>and</strong> 44% <strong>of</strong>fathers) <strong>and</strong> 46% had two biological relativeswith a significant history <strong>of</strong> ADHD. This


CHAPTER 12 assessing family context295study suggests that an assessment <strong>of</strong> parents’childhood histories <strong>of</strong> behavior problemscould aid in the assessment <strong>of</strong> ADHD inchildren.Parental AnxietyAnother type <strong>of</strong> problem that appears tohave a familial link is anxiety. Last, Hersen,Kazdin, Francis, <strong>and</strong> Grubb (1987) reportedthat in their sample <strong>of</strong> children with an anxietydisorder (n = 58), 83% <strong>of</strong> the childrenhad a mother with a lifetime history <strong>of</strong>anxiety disorders. Furthermore, 57% hada mother experiencing significant levels <strong>of</strong>anxiety concurrently with the child. Both<strong>of</strong> these proportions were significantlygreater than what was found in parents <strong>of</strong>clinically referred children without anxietydisorders.Importantly, Frick et al. (1994) foundsimilar results but also found that the linkbetween mother <strong>and</strong> child anxiety couldnot solely be attributed to anxious mothersreporting more anxiety in their children.All <strong>of</strong> the children in the Frick et al.study who self-reported an anxiety disorderhad a mother with a history <strong>of</strong> an anxietydisorder. Further, there are a number <strong>of</strong>studies suggesting that parental anxietycan influence the attachment between theparent <strong>and</strong> child (Costa & Weems, 2005)<strong>and</strong> can lead to parenting behaviors (e.g.,overprotectiveness, failure to encourageindependence) (Dadds, Barrett, Rapee, &Ryan, 1996) that can place a child at riskfor anxiety <strong>and</strong> other problems in adjustment.Thus, it is clear from these findingsthat parental anxiety is an important area tobe assessed in clinical assessments <strong>of</strong> anxiouschildren.Parental SchizophreniaAnother type <strong>of</strong> maladjustment with aclear familial link is schizophrenia. <strong>Child</strong>ren<strong>of</strong> one schizophrenic parent appear tohave a 10 to 15% likelihood <strong>of</strong> developingschizophrenia; the children <strong>of</strong> two schizophrenicparents have about a 25 to 46%risk (Gottesman, McGuffin, & Farmer,1987). These rates <strong>of</strong> disorder in <strong>of</strong>fsprings<strong>of</strong> schizophrenic parents are striking giventhat the prevalence <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia in thegeneral population is between 1 <strong>and</strong> 10per 1,000 individuals (Helzer & Pryzbeck,1988). However, parents who have anotherrelative with schizophrenia are <strong>of</strong>ten quiteconcerned over the risk for their children,on the basis <strong>of</strong> the evidence for a familialtransmission. Therefore, it is <strong>of</strong>ten importantfor clinical assessors to also view theserisks from the point <strong>of</strong> view that the vastmajority <strong>of</strong> children with a schizophrenicrelative, even if that relative is a parent, donot develop schizophrenia.<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Family HistoryFrom this very brief overview <strong>of</strong> the familiallink to childhood disorders it is evidentthat obtaining a family history is a criticalcomponent <strong>of</strong> most clinical assessments <strong>of</strong>children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. However, likeall aspects <strong>of</strong> the assessment process, whatareas to be assessed <strong>and</strong> the depth at whichthey will be assessed depend on the individualcase. In some cases a screening forpsychiatric disorders in a child’s relativescan be conducted as part <strong>of</strong> an unstructuredinterview followed by a more indepthfamily history assessment only if thisis judged to be warranted from the initialscreening. In other cases a more detailed<strong>and</strong> structured assessment may be neededfrom the outset.It is beyond the scope <strong>of</strong> this book tocover assessment <strong>of</strong> adult psychopathologyin great detail. However, it is importantto note that one can assess a widerange <strong>of</strong> problems in parents or otherrelatives through omnibus rating scalesor structured interviews. For example, theNEO-<strong>Personality</strong> Inventory (NEO-PI;Costa & McCrae, 1985) <strong>and</strong> the Minnesota


296 CHAPTER 12 assessing family contextMultiphasic <strong>Personality</strong> Inventory-SecondEdition (MMPI-2; Hathaway & McKinley,1989) are two widely used <strong>and</strong> readily availableobjective personality inventories thatcover a number <strong>of</strong> areas <strong>of</strong> psychologicalfunctioning. There are also numerousstructured diagnostic interviews that areavailable, like the NIMH Diagnostic InterviewSchedule-Third Edition (DIS-IIIA:Helzer & Robins, 1988) <strong>and</strong> the StructuredInterview for DSM-IV (SCID; First,Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1995).When considering what type <strong>of</strong> assessmentmay be needed, it is important tonote that structured interviews tend t<strong>of</strong>ocus on more severe pathology, assessingfor diagnosable disorders, than the objectivepersonality inventories do. In addition,the structured interviews tend to be moreamenable to the family history method<strong>of</strong> assessment, in which a family memberreports on him- or herself <strong>and</strong> other relativeswho cannot be assessed directly.There may be some assessments whena more focused family history is deemedappropriate, such as when one wants t<strong>of</strong>ocus on some specific domain <strong>of</strong> parentaladjustment. For example, the Beck DepressionInventory (BDI; Beck, Steer, & Garbin,1988) <strong>and</strong> the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory(Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1970)are brief screening measures for depression<strong>and</strong> anxiety, respectively that are <strong>of</strong>tenused to assess parental adjustment in clinicreferredchildren. These are just a few <strong>of</strong>a host <strong>of</strong> domain-specific rating scalesthat can be used to assess a specific area<strong>of</strong> adjustment in a child’s parent or otherrelatives (see McMahon & Frick, 2007 forothers).ConclusionsIn this chapter we discussed several reasonswhy the assessment <strong>of</strong> the family environmentis critical to most clinical assessments<strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. Family factors<strong>of</strong>ten play a critical causal role in child maladjustment<strong>and</strong> familial factors can aid inmaking differential diagnoses <strong>and</strong> treatmentrecommendations, two important goals <strong>of</strong>many clinical assessments. Further, factorswithin the family are <strong>of</strong>ten important forinterpreting information provided by members<strong>of</strong> the family on a child’s or adolescent’sadjustment.There are a large number <strong>of</strong> dimensions<strong>of</strong> family functioning that can influencechild adjustment. We focused on fourdimensions that we think are especiallyimportant to assess in most clinical assessments<strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents: parentingstyles <strong>and</strong> practices, parenting stress,marital conflict, <strong>and</strong> parental adjustment.For each <strong>of</strong> these dimensions, we reviewedthe research linking them to child adjustment<strong>and</strong> then provided several methodsfor assessing them in clinical assessments.Chapter Summary1. There is no context more important tounderst<strong>and</strong>ing a child’s emotional <strong>and</strong>behavioral functioning than underst<strong>and</strong>ingthe child’s family context. This isbecause <strong>of</strong> the following factors:(a) Familial influences <strong>of</strong>ten play majorcausal roles in a child’s or adolescent’spsychological difficulties.(b) A family psychiatric history can beinstrumental in making differentialdiagnoses.(c) Underst<strong>and</strong>ing a child’s or adolescent’sfamily context can help tointerpret information provided bymembers <strong>of</strong> the family.(d) Underst<strong>and</strong>ing a child’s family contextcan help to determine the mostimportant targets for intervention.2. Many behavior rating scales reviewedin previous chapters, such as the PIC-2,the PIY, the MMPI-A, <strong>and</strong> the BASC-


CHAPTER 12 assessing family context2972-SRP, have scales that assess variousaspects <strong>of</strong> a child’s family context.3. Research suggests that two criticaldimensions <strong>of</strong> family functioningrelated to child adjustment are parentingstyle, which is the emotional climateprovided by the parents, <strong>and</strong> parentingpractices, which are techniques used byparents to socialize their children <strong>and</strong>enforce rules.4. The Family Environment Scale-Second Edition (FES) is a commonlyused measure <strong>of</strong> parenting style <strong>and</strong>the emotional climate <strong>of</strong> the family.The FES is divided into subscales fromthree domains: Relationships, PersonalGrowth, <strong>and</strong> Systems Maintenance.5. The Alabama Parenting Questionnaire(APQ) <strong>and</strong> the Parenting Scale (PS)are measures that focus more specificallyon parenting behaviors, such asparents’ discipline strategies.6. The Dyadic Parent <strong>Child</strong> InteractionCoding System (DPICS) is an observationalcoding system designed toassess parent <strong>and</strong> child behaviors in twost<strong>and</strong>ardized parent–child interactiontasks.7. The amount <strong>of</strong> stress experienced byparents is also important to child adjustment.Scales can assess general stressorsor stressors specific to parenting.8. The <strong>Child</strong> Abuse Potential Inventory-Second Edition (CAPI) was developedto assess parents’ risk for abusing theirchildren <strong>and</strong> includes several scalesrelated to family stress.9. The Parenting Stress Index-SecondEdition (PSI) focuses specifically onstressors related to parenting.10. The level <strong>of</strong> marital discord <strong>and</strong>overt marital conflict in the home hasproven to be important in underst<strong>and</strong>ingchildren’s functioning.11. Assessing parental adjustment canprovide critical information in clinicalassessments, especially family histories<strong>of</strong> depression, anxiety, antisocial behavior,attention deficit disorder, substanceabuse, <strong>and</strong> schizophrenia.


C h a p t e r 1 3History TakingChapter Questionsl What are the unique contributions <strong>of</strong> historicalinformation to the child assessmentprocess?l What are the typical domains, variables,or behaviors, assessed by such strategies?l What structured <strong>and</strong> unstructured historytaking methods are available?l How should clinicians go about collectingcomprehensive historical informationefficiently?The Role <strong>of</strong> History Taking in<strong>Child</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>History taking, <strong>of</strong>ten through a clinicalinterview is central to the purpose <strong>of</strong>child assessment. Indeed, it is perhaps theessential component <strong>of</strong> child psychologicalassessment, as a good history enables theclinician to conceptualize a case by providinginformation about the developmentalcourse <strong>of</strong> the child’s difficulties, the specificpresentation <strong>of</strong> the individual child’sdifficulties, risk <strong>and</strong> protective factors, <strong>and</strong>the important contextual influences on thechild’s functioning. Such factors are notroutinely assessed by rating scales, selfreportinventories, or other widely usedmeasures. Indeed, it is impossible to conceive<strong>of</strong> a competent assessment that wouldnot include history taking in some form.Although it is at least as crucial as othertypes <strong>of</strong> information for child assessment,historical information is <strong>of</strong>ten underutilizedby clinical assessors. This oversight isespecially regrettable given the widespreadavailability <strong>of</strong> history-taking measures orguides <strong>and</strong> the ease with which they can beused. It appears that history taking is simplynot as well entrenched in child behavioral299P.J. Frick et al., <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Behavior,DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-0641-0_13, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010


300 chapter 13 history takingassessment, as it is in medical assessment,where history taking (interviewing) is centralto making initial diagnostic decisions.Instead, many clinicians may focus on thecurrent presentation <strong>of</strong> symptoms or problems,as well as behavioral observations(which is important information), to theexclusion <strong>of</strong> gathering information aboutthe child’s developmental history.On the other h<strong>and</strong>, clinicians may centeran entire assessment around history takingthrough an unstructured clinical interviewwhich is necessarily idiosyncratic by child<strong>and</strong> clinician. Such approaches to assessmentfail to gather specific information onthe symptomatology <strong>and</strong> the child’s functioningrelative to developmental normsthrough norm-referenced assessment.Therefore, as with every other methoddiscussed in this text, history taking should beconsidered part, but not all, <strong>of</strong> a comprehensiveassessment. In short, we consider historytaking necessary but not sufficient. There arenumerous reasons for taking histories as part<strong>of</strong> any child assessment. A few <strong>of</strong> the variablesthat are uniquely assessed by history takinginclude age <strong>of</strong> onset, course/prognosis, etiology,previous treatment history, <strong>and</strong> otherrelevant background information.Age <strong>of</strong> OnsetMost rating scales <strong>and</strong> self-report inventoriesdo not directly assess the age <strong>of</strong>onset <strong>of</strong> problems. Age <strong>of</strong> onset is particularlyimportant in child assessmentas it affects diagnosis directly. In the case<strong>of</strong> mental retardation, for example, age<strong>of</strong> onset must occur during the developmentalperiod (i.e., prior to age 18). Furthermore,the age <strong>of</strong> onset is crucial forthe diagnosis <strong>of</strong> ADHD, autistic disorder,learning disabilities, <strong>and</strong> many otherchildhood disorders (APA, 2000).Most psychologists have seen cases<strong>of</strong> suspected mental retardation with areported age <strong>of</strong> onset in the early 20s.Similarly, children are <strong>of</strong>ten referred forADHD with an age <strong>of</strong> onset after age 7.Sometimes, however, the age <strong>of</strong> onset isunclear or disputed, <strong>and</strong> the typically lessstructured format <strong>of</strong> history taking is idealfor clarifying the onset issue for a particularchild.As one example, a mother may bringin a college freshman because he is suddenlyhaving problems in school. On arating scale or other more structuredassessment format, the clinician may betempted to accept this current time as theage <strong>of</strong> onset. In the less structured history-takinginterview, one can pursue theissue <strong>of</strong> onset more thoroughly by askingthe mother if her son had ever receivedspecial education services, had a history <strong>of</strong>difficulty concentrating or finishing tasks,had been prescribed medication for attentionproblems, or had received tutoring inan early grade. The clinician may also askif there were any teacher complaints inearly elementary school concerning attentionproblems.It is not unusual for a clinician to find amuch earlier age <strong>of</strong> onset than that initiallystated by a child, teacher, parent, or otherinformant. If, in this sample case, the collegestudent did experience some difficultiesin the first grade that were associatedwith inattention <strong>and</strong> hyperactivity, <strong>and</strong>he also struggled to pass both the ninth<strong>and</strong> twelfth grades, then the clinician mayinvestigate more carefully the diagnosis<strong>of</strong> ADHD with its associated early age <strong>of</strong>onset. A more extended case study is providedin Box 13.1.Course/PrognosisThe course <strong>of</strong> a child’s difficulties refers tothe assessment <strong>of</strong> the developmental trajectory<strong>of</strong> symptomatology. Differencesin the stability <strong>of</strong> symptoms can have substantialimplications for differential diagnosis.For example, if a parent describes achild as having had severe hyperactivityin first grade that was not apparent in second


chapter 13 history taking301Box 13.1Case Example <strong>of</strong> the Use <strong>of</strong> History TakingJames is a 15-year-old male who was referredby his father for suspected ADHD. Thereferral for evaluation was also supported byhis high school teachers.According to James’s father, his school difficultiesbecame pronounced about 3 years ago.He rarely does homework <strong>and</strong> is described byhis teachers as exhibiting hyperactivity, impulsivity,<strong>and</strong> inattention. He is also describedas the class clown by his teachers. A recentclassroom observation found that James wasgrossly inattentive in comparison to the otherchildren. He, for example, was the only onewho briefly rolled around on the floor in therear <strong>of</strong> the room during part <strong>of</strong> a teacher’s lecture.Previously, norm-referenced behaviorratings by four <strong>of</strong> his teachers identified himas highly hyperactive <strong>and</strong> inattentive.Being mindful <strong>of</strong> the late onset <strong>of</strong> James’symptoms, the clinician asked James’s fatherfor more detail about the onset <strong>of</strong> his problems.His father could not identify significantsymptomatology prior to age 12. It wasalso discovered that James’s parents wentthrough a contentious divorce concurrentlywith the onset <strong>of</strong> symptoms. It was furtherdisclosed that James recently has threatenedto attempt suicide on several occasions. Aninterview with James subsequently revealedsignificant evidence <strong>of</strong> depression with suicidalideation.In this case, history taking, by clarifyingthe age <strong>of</strong> onset, made a significant contributionto the conceptualization <strong>of</strong> James’s difficulties<strong>and</strong> intervention design. Failure touse history taking to rule out ADHD in thiscase could have been disastrous, as poor historytaking could have resulted in perhaps noassessment <strong>of</strong> suicidality <strong>and</strong> in a failure totreat his depression.grade but then emerged again in the fourthgrade, the diagnosis <strong>of</strong> ADHD may be calledinto question. Such a pattern would signalthat contextual variables or other difficultiesmight be related to the emergence <strong>of</strong> symptoms.Similarly, if a child displays symptoms<strong>of</strong> autism that seem to spontaneously remit<strong>and</strong> reappear, then the diagnosis <strong>of</strong> autismbecomes questionable. Furthermore, a previoushistory <strong>of</strong> episodes <strong>of</strong> depression placean individual at significantly greater risk forfuture such episodes (Klein, Dougherty, &Olino, 2005). Thus, history taking is criticalfor underst<strong>and</strong>ing an individual’s risk fordepression <strong>and</strong> to plan for the most appropriateinterventions.Evaluation <strong>of</strong> course also provides evidence<strong>of</strong> the effectiveness <strong>of</strong> previous interventionstrategies. A child’s, or family’s,failure to respond to previous interventionssuggests that different <strong>and</strong>/or moreintensive interventions may be warranted.The trajectory <strong>of</strong> behavioral excesses ordeficits is uniquely accessed via history taking.Subtleties <strong>of</strong> this nature are difficult toassess even with many <strong>of</strong> the structureddiagnostic interviews. However, clinicianswho are aware <strong>of</strong> the influence <strong>of</strong> courseon case conceptualization, treatment planning,<strong>and</strong> prognosis are more likely to beadept at gathering <strong>and</strong> integrating suchinformation into their assessments.EtiologyEtiology refers to the likely presumed cause<strong>of</strong> a child’s difficulties. The assessment <strong>of</strong>etiology is crucial in that it has importantimplications for treatment. The discoverythat a child’s learning difficulties inmathematics did not begin until the ninthgrade may very well rule out a learningdisability, depending on other aspects <strong>of</strong>


302 chapter 13 history takingthe client’s history. If the clinician thendiscovers that the child was for the firsttime placed in the advanced section <strong>of</strong> amathematics class, then curriculum placementbecomes a potential etiologicalagent. A simple change in classes may bean effective intervention for this suspectedmathematics learning disability. Such aparsimonious intervention may not havebeen tried, however, without importanthistorical information that led the clinicianto a potential etiology.Knowledge <strong>of</strong> etiology, in general, is<strong>of</strong> potential importance for at least fourreasons (AAMR, 1992).1. The etiology may be associated withother problems (e.g., Down Syndrome)that may impair other aspects <strong>of</strong> functioning- physical, social, etc.2. The etiology may be amenable to treatment/intervention.3. Knowledge <strong>of</strong> etiology may lead to thedesign <strong>of</strong> prevention programs for certainetiologies.4. Etiologies may be useful for forminghomogeneous groups for research ortreatment.It is apparent that part <strong>of</strong> this assessmentshould include a history <strong>of</strong> other symptomatologyto assess for comorbidity, as wellas issues that could have actually precipitatedcurrent difficulties (e.g., a history <strong>of</strong>depression that now manifests partly asdifficulty concentrating).In addition, attempting to determine etiologyfor a specific client helps clarify both caseconceptualization to answer the referralquestion <strong>and</strong> the recommended interventionstargeting the presumed etiologies.After all, these activities are the primarypurpose <strong>of</strong> child assessment, <strong>and</strong> historytaking is valuable to these ends.Unfortunately, for most childhoodproblems the range <strong>of</strong> potential single<strong>and</strong> multiple etiologies is <strong>of</strong>ten extensive.The potential etiologies associatedwith depression, for instance, are multitudinous.A non-exhaustive list <strong>of</strong> thesefactors is shown in Table 13.1. Moreover,multiple etiologies may be interactingto produce symptoms <strong>of</strong> a problemsuch as depression. It would not be unusual,for example, for a depressed child to beaffected by parental depression, poverty,<strong>and</strong> the death <strong>of</strong> a friend, all <strong>of</strong> which mayrequire simultaneous <strong>and</strong>/or coordinatedintervention.Commonly used history-taking formsmay not fully address the various etiologiesassociated with a problem or set <strong>of</strong> problems.Clinicians <strong>of</strong>ten have to use theirknowledge <strong>of</strong> child development, psychopathology,<strong>and</strong> other areas <strong>of</strong> psychologicalresearch to go beyond the st<strong>and</strong>ardquestions included on a history form (i.e.,branching) to rule out important high-frequencyetiologies.This process <strong>of</strong> branching from a historyform is difficult for even the mostsavvy clinician. For the majority <strong>of</strong> examiners,the most realistic option will be toassess history over the course <strong>of</strong> two ormore assessment sessions. A second sessioncould be as simple as a telephone callthat allows the clinician to rule out an etiologythat has been hypothesized basedon previous history, assessment data,or other information. As Green (1992)notes: “Time between sessions is <strong>of</strong>tenan important diagnostic <strong>and</strong> therapeutically” (p. 460), <strong>and</strong> taking such time wouldallow the clinician to be more accurate inhis/her case conceptualization. However,it is acknowledged that many practitionersare working in settings or undercircumstances in which assessments areexpected to be completed very quickly.Therefore, it is important that the clinicianbe highly knowledgeable aboutetiology at the outset <strong>of</strong> the assessment,approach the history taking interviewaccordingly, <strong>and</strong> still take the necessarytime to not hastily attempt to answer areferral question.


chapter 13 history taking303Table 13.1 Selected Etiologies <strong>of</strong> DepressionMedical disorders associated with depressionAutoimmune disorders: systemic lupus, rheumatoid disease, sarcoidosisCancers: head, pancreas, gastrointestinal, lung, renalCentral nervous system diseases: Parkinson’s disease, degenerative dementias, normal-pressurehydrocephalus, subarachnoid hemorrhages. Huntington’s disease, reversible dementias, focal lesions(nondominant), stroke, head traumaEndocrinopathies/metabolic disorders: hypothyroidism, Addison’s disease, Cushing’s disease, pituitarytremors, diabetes, hyperparathyroidism, porphyriaIntoxications: lead, mercury, thalliumOccult infections: genitourinary tract, liverViral infections: influenza, viral pneumonia, mononucleosis, hepatitisDrugs associated with major depression or dysthymiaAnticancer: vincristine, vinglastineAntihypertensives: reserpine, methyldopa, propanolol, guanethidine, hydralazine, clonidineAnti-infectives: cycloserineAnti-Parkinson agents: levodopa, arnantadine, carbidopaCorticosteroidsHormones: estrogens, progesteronePsychotropicsOther psychiatric disorders that may confound the diagnosis <strong>of</strong> depressionAnxiety disorders: panic disorders, phobic disordersObsessive-Compulsive DisorderSleep disorders: narcolepsy, sleep apneaPsychotic disordersEating Disorders: Anorexia Nervosa, bulimiaVague somatic complaintsDementiaSexual dysfunctionsFamily Psychiatric HistoryAs noted in the previous chapter, a specificetiological agent <strong>of</strong> increasingimportance is familial psychiatric history.Research has underscored the importance<strong>of</strong> family psychiatric history as a risk factorfor similar problems in <strong>of</strong>fspring <strong>and</strong>as a variable that may contraindicatesome treatment options (see Klein et al.,2005). School screening is an example <strong>of</strong>the application <strong>of</strong> family psychiatric historytaking. Prekindergarten screeningprograms <strong>of</strong>ten ask questions aboutchild behavior, <strong>and</strong> the inclusion <strong>of</strong> familyhistory information may trigger preventionor intervention efforts. If, for example,a father reports a history <strong>of</strong> separationanxiety as a young child, then follow-upcould be planned to monitor this father’s<strong>of</strong>fspring upon entry into kindergarten.While some minor separation anxietysymptoms are common at this age, if achild with family resemblance for suchproblems displays some difficulties, thenearlier <strong>and</strong> more aggressive interventionmay be warranted. The importance<strong>of</strong> family history is illustrated in the casestudy provided in Box 13.2.


304 chapter 13 history takingBox 13.2A Case Study Illustrating the Importance <strong>of</strong> Family HistoryBradford is a 10-year-old male who wasreferred for school difficulties. He has reportedlyalways had school problems, but theyhave worsened in the fifth grade. His teacherscomplain that he is unmotivated, moody,irritable, <strong>and</strong> oppositional.His father stated that he has had difficultygetting Bradford to complete homework. Thesehomework sessions usually turn into powerstruggles, <strong>and</strong> Bradford ends up crying. He alsoreportedly seems unusually emotional in comparisonto his younger brother. According to hismother, he angers quickly <strong>and</strong> cries easily.Bradford’s developmental history is unremarkable,with the exception <strong>of</strong> the continuingproblems with work completion. Recently,however, he has had problems interacting withhis peers. He cannot identify a best friend, <strong>and</strong>he is less frequently invited over to other children’shomes, according to his mother.<strong>Assessment</strong> results did not reveal any problems<strong>of</strong> note. His intelligence <strong>and</strong> academicachievement scores ranged from average toabove average, with his scores in math in theLow Average range. From his teachers’ report,it appears that Bradford’s difficulty with completingwork has contributed to his fallingbehind somewhat in math.Parent <strong>and</strong> teacher rating scales did not produceany significant T-scores. His self-reportscores on internalizing scales such as anxiety <strong>and</strong>depression were slightly elevated (in the 60s).Bradford’s developmental history <strong>and</strong> testresults make his case conceptualization difficult.The results suggest that he has internalizingproblems, but he does not display symptomatologythat approaches the severity necessary tomeet diagnostic criteria for depression, dysthymia,or anxiety disorders. The clinician beganto question the parents’ reason for the referral.In particular, it was difficult to discern why theparents were so concerned about what appearsto be a circumscribed problem with homeworkcompletion <strong>and</strong> achievement motivation. Theclinician discovered the real reason for the parents’referral at the feedback session.After the clinician presented the findings,recommended behavioral intervention forhomework incompletion, <strong>and</strong> did not makea diagnosis, Bradford’s father (who had notattended the previous assessment session)disclosed his concern about Bradford’s internalizingsymptoms, saying that Bradford hadinherited his father’s susceptibility to depression.Bradford’s father then disclosed that hehad not wanted the clinician to know about hisprevious <strong>and</strong> ongoing treatment, so as to notbias the clinician. Bradford’s dad then revealedthat he has suffered from depression since childhood<strong>and</strong> has found it difficult to control. Hehas reportedly been involved in psychotherapyat various times, including currently. He hasalso received pharmacological treatment fordepression <strong>and</strong> is currently taking Prozac. Hereported that he has been hospitalized previouslybecause <strong>of</strong> suicidal ideation. He also saidthat although he is a successful businessman, hehas difficulty functioning without ongoing psychotherapy<strong>and</strong> medical management. He alsorevealed that his father suffered from depression<strong>and</strong> eventually committed suicide.These belated but important historicalfindings certainly affected the clinician’s conceptualization<strong>of</strong> the case <strong>and</strong> interventionplanning. The clinician changed his recommendationsto include psychotherapy as afurther assessment tool <strong>and</strong> as a means fordesigning preventive interventions.One <strong>of</strong> the methodological problemsassociated with family health <strong>and</strong> psychiatrichistory taking is that <strong>of</strong> underreporting<strong>of</strong> illness. More reliable information may beobtained by directly interviewing each familymember, a method known as family study(Rende & Weissman, 1999). Numerous interviewmethods exist for conducting a familystudy <strong>of</strong> adults including the DiagnosticInterview for Genetic Studies (DIGS; Nurnbergeret al., 1994) <strong>and</strong> the Family HistoryScreen (Weissman et al., 2000). Apart froma specific focus on family psychiatric history,some methods exist that may help a clinicianorganize information about the child’s familystructure <strong>and</strong> history. A brief discussion <strong>of</strong>genograms as one such method is presentedin Box 13.3.


Box 13.3Genograms, not unlike pr<strong>of</strong>ile plots on measuressuch as the MMPI-A or intelligencetests, present information graphically in amanner that can be quickly interpreted by thetrained pr<strong>of</strong>essional. In its most widely usedform, the genogram is essentially a family treethat allows the pr<strong>of</strong>essional to document theclient’s family structure (i.e., parents, siblings,gr<strong>and</strong>parents, etc.), the relationships amongfamily members, critical events, as well as anyparticular variables <strong>of</strong> interest (e.g., psychiatrichistory, genetic history).Several volumes on genograms exist (e.g.,McGoldrick, Gerson, & Shellenberger, 2008).The uses <strong>of</strong> genograms vary widely includingas a means for obtaining medical history (seePapadopoulos, Bor, & Stanion, 1997), forunderst<strong>and</strong>ing an individual’s cultural context(Shellenberger et al., 2007), <strong>and</strong> to aid inclinical supervision (Aten, Madson, & Kruse,2008). For clinical assessment, Papadopouloset al. (1997) emphasize that genograms arenot a quantitative technique with an objectiveinterpretative strategy. Instead, genogramsprovide a visual representation <strong>of</strong> family historythat can be used, in conjunction with305other information, to allow the clinician todevelop hypotheses about the case.In addition to gathering information toaid in case conceptualization <strong>and</strong> answeringa referral question, genograms may also beuseful to convey etiological theories to theclient/parent about the presenting problems(Papadopoulos et al., 1997). For example,visually depicting an extensive family history<strong>of</strong> depression (or depicting protective factorsin the family context) may help parents reconceptualizetheir child’s problems.Of course, to take advantage <strong>of</strong> the presumedbenefits <strong>of</strong> genograms, including efficiency<strong>of</strong> information gathering, one must bewell-trained at constructing <strong>and</strong> interpretinggenograms <strong>and</strong> must still conduct a clinicalinterview in a manner that will obtain thedesired information. Genograms are not amenableto the same tests <strong>of</strong> reliability, validity, <strong>and</strong>utility <strong>of</strong> many <strong>of</strong> the other tools described inthis text. Therefore, the decision <strong>of</strong> whether ornot to routinely employ such a strategy comesdown to the clinician’s training, comfort, <strong>and</strong>perception <strong>of</strong> a genogram’s usefulness.A common set <strong>of</strong> symbols, <strong>and</strong> a very basicgenogram example is given below:


306 chapter 13 history takingPrevious <strong>Assessment</strong>/Treatment/InterventionA child’s history <strong>of</strong> previous intervention/treatment <strong>and</strong> assessment <strong>of</strong> these factors isprimarily available through history taking.Similarly, previous assessment results mayaffect interpretation <strong>of</strong> current findings.A clinician, for example, may encountera child who has received psychotherapyfor depression for 2 years, pharmacologicaltreatment for 3 months, <strong>and</strong> partialhospitalization for 6 months prior tothe current evaluation. If the child isdemonstrating symptomatology duringthe current evaluation that ismore severe than previously noted,the child’s clinician is more likely toadvise aggressive treatment, perhaps evenhospitalization. The need for aggressivetreatment, however, may not appear compellingwithout the knowledge <strong>of</strong> the previousintervention failures.Information about previous assessmentalso allows clinicians to gauge the accuracy<strong>of</strong> their current findings. A clinician canvalidate their current findings by comparingthem to the previous assessmentresults. A child, who was diagnosed withan anxiety disorder 2 months earlier, maydemonstrate significant anxiety as indicatedby current MMPI-A <strong>and</strong> RCMASresults. This congruence between current<strong>and</strong> recent findings may lend some validityto the psychologist’s current results.A lack <strong>of</strong> congruence between the recent<strong>and</strong> current test findings, however, can beequally insightful. In this situation, an advisedfirst step would be to check the scoring <strong>of</strong> thecurrent measures. Previous diagnoses mayalso help guide further assessment. If a childhas been diagnosed previously with ADHD,then the examiner may wish to exp<strong>and</strong> theassessment to other forms <strong>of</strong> disruptivebehavior, given the high comorbidity amongsuch problems (American Psychiatric Association,2000). Knowledge <strong>of</strong> the ADHDdiagnosis <strong>and</strong> its associated features <strong>and</strong>comorbidities gives the clinician a specifichypothesis totest with further assessmentresults <strong>and</strong> a potentially valuable set <strong>of</strong> targetsfor intervention planning.Contextual FactorsAs noted above, clinicians should be preparedto branch <strong>of</strong>f <strong>of</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard historyquestions or forms to assess issues that areclient-specific <strong>and</strong> highly important to acomprehensive case conceptualization.For even a novice clinician, it is readilyapparent that contextual factors in a child’shistory may exert great influence on his/her present functioning. Such factorsinclude family relationships, divorce, moves,socioeconomic status, ethnicity, culture,neighborhood environment, <strong>and</strong> traumaticevents (see Dodge & Pettit, 2003). Forexample, it is one thing to determine that achild’s present academic difficulties are thecause <strong>of</strong> a parent’s concerns <strong>and</strong> the reasonfor referral. It is quite another to learn thatthere were no such difficulties for the childuntil after his/her parents divorced.A more complex contextual factor that mayplay a direct role in the design <strong>of</strong> interventionsis that <strong>of</strong> the goodness-<strong>of</strong>-fit betweenthe child’s characteristics <strong>and</strong> the contexts inwhich he/she is expected to function. In particular,contexts in which the child spendsmore time or that are more proximal likelywill exert a stronger positive or negativeinfluence (Cicchetti & Toth, 1998). If a childis prone to problems with anxiety <strong>and</strong> hisor her parents likewise openly worry aboutmany things <strong>and</strong> interact with the child inan emotional manner, the child’s anxiety islikely to be exacerbated. Being in the sameclass as another anxious student with whomthe child does not interact will not have aninfluence on the child’s anxiety. Treatmentrecommendations will likely include suggestionsfor the parents in managing their ownanxiety <strong>and</strong>/or in altering their communicationstrategies with the child.


chapter 13 history taking307These contextual factors should be routinelyincluded as part <strong>of</strong> history takingeven if there is no indication <strong>of</strong> adversecontextual factors at the outset <strong>of</strong> theassessment. The influence <strong>of</strong> context canalso be protective, <strong>and</strong> thus, these variablesmay also serve as strengths around whichinterventions can be built.ContentThere is considerable overlapping contentamong history measures althoughvarying degrees <strong>of</strong> emphasis exist. Some<strong>of</strong> the most common areas <strong>of</strong> inquiryinclude (adapted from the Department<strong>of</strong> Psychiatry <strong>and</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychiatry, TheInstitute <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry, <strong>and</strong> the MaudesleyHospital <strong>of</strong> London, 1987):ComplaintsPresent illnessFamily historyMotherFatherSiblingsOther relativesFamily atmospherePersonal historyEarly developmentBehavior <strong>and</strong> temperamentSchoolOccupationAdolescenceSexual historyMedical historyPrevious psychiatric problemsAlcohol, tobacco, <strong>and</strong> drug useAntisocial behaviorCurrent life situationDevelopmental delays or disabilities<strong>Personality</strong>Attitudes toward others in social, family,<strong>and</strong> sexual relationshipsAttitudes toward selfMoral <strong>and</strong> religious attitudes <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardsMoodLeisure activities <strong>and</strong> interestsFantasy lifeReaction pattern to stressA history that is <strong>of</strong> interest to a certainspecialty practice may have a differingemphasis. The content <strong>of</strong> a history thatmay be taken by a neurologist or neuropsychologistis described by Teodori (1993)as including the following topics:Birth historyDevelopmental historySchool historyIQ test resultsSocial historyNutritional historyHistory <strong>of</strong> exposure to chemicals or toxinsHistory <strong>of</strong> other family illnessesOther medical illnessesMedicationsHospitalizationsSurgeryReview <strong>of</strong> systems inquiring about allother aspects <strong>of</strong> the child’s physical functioningPrevious recordsFormatsHistory taking is typically conducted viatwo formats: an interview <strong>and</strong> a writtenform. While history taking research hasfocused on the interview, there are <strong>of</strong>tenoccasions where a written form may beused, particularly with the parents <strong>of</strong> children.Parents are already accustomed tocompleting such forms while waiting inphysicians’ <strong>of</strong>fices. Increasingly, parentsare also accustomed to completing ratingscales in psychologists’ <strong>of</strong>fices.Most history-taking forms, however,require considerable English language fluencyon the part <strong>of</strong> parents. Technical topics,such as the type <strong>of</strong> special education programthat their child attends or a previous medicalcondition, are difficult for even the mostsophisticated parents. Regardless, parents


308 chapter 13 history takingmay be able to complete part <strong>of</strong> the history<strong>and</strong> thereby save the examiner interviewingtime. <strong>Assessment</strong> systems such as the BASC(Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004) which was discussedin previous chapters on rating scales,also include forms for history taking (e.g.,BASC-2 Structured Developmental History;SDH). An example <strong>of</strong> a history form isshown in Fig. 13.1. Of course, history formswill vary greatly in their depth <strong>and</strong> breadth.


chapter 13 history taking309


310 chapter 13 history taking


chapter 13 history taking311


312 chapter 13 history takingThe important issue is to cover essential historyinformation as part <strong>of</strong> the assessment<strong>and</strong> to ideally do so in an efficient manner.Forms, such as the one shown in Fig. 13.1,allow the clinician to gain such informationbut also require the clinician to follow upbased on the parent’s responses.Many clinicians, however, prefer a completelyinterview format because it allowsthem to use a branching procedure to pursuetopics that are <strong>of</strong> particular relevance toissues <strong>of</strong> onset, course, etiology, <strong>and</strong> previoustreatment history among other factorswithout a potentially cumbersome formthat includes some irrelevant informationfor a particular child. A sample scenario isa case where an 8-year-old child is referredbecause <strong>of</strong> a decline in academic achievement.The examiner may discover that thechild’s performance in school was exemplaryuntil the current academic year, whenthe child missed 35 days <strong>of</strong> school because<strong>of</strong> an automobile accident. The examiner, inthis case, will likely branch to ask questionsabout the child’s length <strong>of</strong> absence from theschool <strong>and</strong> the extent <strong>of</strong> documented headinjury. New <strong>and</strong> detailed information aboutthese problems may pr<strong>of</strong>oundly influenceintervention planning. Special educationmay be placed in abeyance in favor <strong>of</strong> remedialcoursework <strong>and</strong> monitoring to see if thechild recovers his premorbid level <strong>of</strong> functioningwith little intervention, as would<strong>of</strong>ten be predicted. Hence, the interview orpartial interview format allows the clinicianthe flexibility necessary for thorough exploration<strong>of</strong> variables that may impact interventionrather than have the parent spendtime on a number <strong>of</strong> history questions thatare not central to the referral concern. Itmust be emphasized that such interviewsare, by nature, clinician- <strong>and</strong> client-specific,so psychometric properties, particularlyreliability, are not meaningful in evaluatinga clinician’s interview as part <strong>of</strong> theassessment process. Despite their lack <strong>of</strong>amenability to empirical evaluations, asnoted above, interviews are indispensable


chapter 13 history taking313in psychological assessment. History formsmay be quite useful for efficiency <strong>and</strong>research purposes, but in clinical assessmentthey should be augmented by a directclinical interview.The face-to-face approach <strong>of</strong> the interviewformat may <strong>of</strong>fer additional advantagesbeyond the possibility for branching.See, for example, Green’s (1992) advice onobserving nonverbal cues during historytaking given in Box 13.4.ConclusionsThis chapter puts forth the argument thathistory taking is a central, but <strong>of</strong>ten overlookedcomponent, <strong>of</strong> child assessment.Various reasons for making history takingmore central to the child assessmentprocess are <strong>of</strong>fered with the focus on theunique contributions that history takingmakes to the process. Specifically, historytaking is ideal for assessing factors such asage <strong>of</strong> onset, etiology, course, <strong>and</strong> previousassessment/intervention outcomes.Psychologists are advised to selectappropriate history forms <strong>and</strong> practice thebranching procedures necessary for integratingpsychological science with childhistory. In fact, history taking provides anideal venue for the integration <strong>of</strong> psychopathology,child development, <strong>and</strong> cognitivescience literatures, among others,with child characteristics. Empirical evidenceshould shape the domains coveredin st<strong>and</strong>ard history-taking procedures;however, the process will, by nature, beclient-specific.Box 13.4Observing Nonverbal Cues During History TakingGreen (1992) provides some advice to pediatriciansregarding behaviors to observeduring the interviewing process. This compendiumis equally useful to the psychologistwho, while interviewing, is developinghypotheses about family dynamics <strong>and</strong> otherfactors that impact child development. Thefollowing is an adapted list <strong>of</strong> behaviors forwhich psychologists should observe duringhistory taking with a caregiver informant orset <strong>of</strong> informants.l Perspiration, blushing. or paling; controlled,uneven, or blocked speech; plaintivevoice; talking in a whisper; gait;posture; tics; affirmative nodding; negativeshaking <strong>of</strong> the headl Frequent swallowing tenseness, fidgeting,preoccupied air, avoidance <strong>of</strong> eye contact,social distancel A sudden glance at the interviewer orsomeone else in the family precipitated bya statement or questionl Clenching, rubbing, wringing h<strong>and</strong>s,searching or nail bitingl Dress <strong>and</strong> personal groomingl Reddening <strong>of</strong> eyes or cryingl Frowns, smilesl Affect inappropriate to ideation (e.g., aparent smiles when talking about a child’ssevere behavior problems)l Interactions among parents, child, <strong>and</strong>examinerl Developmentally inappropriate behavior(e.g., older child on parent’s lap)l The way in which the infant or young childis held or helped during the interviewl The parent’s ability to have child respondto a parent’s requestl Adopted from Green (1992), p. 455.


314 chapter 13 history takingChapter Summary1. Most rating scales <strong>and</strong> self-report inventoriesdo not directly assess the age <strong>of</strong>onset <strong>of</strong> problems as well as do historytaking methods.2. The use <strong>of</strong> history taking to clarify age<strong>of</strong> onset is crucial for the diagnosis <strong>and</strong>conceptualization <strong>of</strong> a number <strong>of</strong> childhooddisorders.3. Etiology refers to the likely presumedcause <strong>of</strong> a child’s difficulties.4. Knowledge <strong>of</strong> etiology is <strong>of</strong> potentialimportance for at least four reasons:(a) The etiology may be associatedwith other problems (e.g., DownSyndrome) that may impair otheraspects <strong>of</strong> functioning - physical,social, or other domains.(b) The etiology may be amenable totreatment/intervention.(c) Knowledge <strong>of</strong> etiology may lead tothe design <strong>of</strong> prevention programs.(d) Etiologies may be useful for forminghomogeneous groups for research oradministrative purposes.5. Research indicates a number <strong>of</strong> importantareas to cover in child <strong>and</strong> adolescenthistory taking. Clinicians shouldbe aware <strong>of</strong> these areas <strong>and</strong> use the“branching” technique to appropriatelyfollow up on any areas <strong>of</strong> concern for theclient.


C h a p t e r 1 4Adaptive Behavior ScalesChapter Questionsl What is meant by the term adaptivebehavior?l What are the typical domains <strong>of</strong> adaptivebehavior scales?l How is adaptive behavior different fromother constructs such as personality orintelligence?l What are some <strong>of</strong> the most popularadaptive behavior scales?History <strong>of</strong> the ConstructThe adaptive behavior construct traces itsroots to early work in mental retardation,which, in turn, is linked to the roots <strong>of</strong>intellectual assessment (Kamphaus, 2001).Although intelligence tests contributedmightily to the recognition <strong>of</strong> the mentalretardation syndrome, Doll (1940) notedthat intelligence measures lacked sufficientbreadth for assessing all <strong>of</strong> the relevantdomains <strong>of</strong> behavior that needed tobe considered in treatment <strong>of</strong> individualswith mental retardation.Doll drew on his experience as a psychologistat the Vinel<strong>and</strong> State TrainingSchool in New Jersey, where he wascharged with the assessment <strong>and</strong> rehabilitation<strong>of</strong> individuals with mental retardation.He noted that such individuals notonly lacked intellectual abilities necessaryfor academic attainment, but they also<strong>of</strong>ten appeared to lack day-to-day livingskills needed for independent functioning.In Doll’s terminology, they lacked socialmaturity.In order to intervene <strong>and</strong> improvea child’s social maturity, Doll created ascale to assess specific behaviors that were315P.J. Frick et al., <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Behavior,DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-0641-0_14, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010


316 chapter 14 Adaptive Behavior Scalesdeemed necessary for successful living.The Vinel<strong>and</strong> Social Maturity Scale (Doll,1953) was the first <strong>of</strong> its genre. It includedseveral sets <strong>of</strong> items that assessed variousaspects <strong>of</strong> social maturity including locomotion,social skills, <strong>and</strong> grooming. TheVinel<strong>and</strong> yielded a score that was roughlyparallel to a composite score <strong>of</strong>fered bymany intelligence tests <strong>of</strong> the day. His totalscore was dubbed a “Social Quotient.” TheVinel<strong>and</strong> became the premier measure <strong>of</strong>adaptive behavior up to the present day,<strong>and</strong> Doll’s pioneering work became thebasis for all subsequent measures.The AAIDD CriteriaThe American Association on Intellectual<strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities (AAIDD),formerly the AAMR, bases its criteria for anintellectual disability, or mental retardation,on deficits in both intellectual functioning<strong>and</strong> adaptive behavior (see Luckassonet al., 2002), similar to previous criteria(e.g., AAMR, 1992). The current criteriathat indicate that three broad areas <strong>of</strong> adaptivebehavior should be assessed list tendomains <strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior that should beassessed for the purposes <strong>of</strong> mental retardationdiagnosis <strong>and</strong> intervention planning.These domains are: conceptual skills (e.g.,language, reading, writing, money skills),social skills (e.g., interpersonal, responsibility,avoiding victimization, obeying rules),<strong>and</strong> practical skills (e.g., maintaining a safeenvironment, self-care such as dressing, eatingmeals, hygiene).It should be noted that competence inthese areas <strong>of</strong> function may provide a basis<strong>of</strong> intervention for children with otherproblems such as autism, ADHD, learningproblems, or anxiety. In fact, the assessment<strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior has been referred to asan essential part <strong>of</strong> assessments for autism(Ozon<strong>of</strong>f, Goodlin-Jones, & Solomon,2005). Adaptive functioning has also beendescribed as one factor that can help differentiatemental retardation from learningdisabilities, in that the former would bemarked by broad deficits in adaptive functioning,<strong>and</strong> the latter would be marked bymore specific deficits (Fletcher, Francis,Morris, & Lyon, 2005). The acquisition<strong>and</strong> demonstration <strong>of</strong> adaptive skills havealso been identified as an important bufferagainst the development <strong>of</strong> psychopathology.For example, social competencies mayallow a child to resist being overwhelmedby negative life stressors (Tanaka & Westerman,1988). In this context, adaptive behaviorscales may serve an important functionin the assessment <strong>of</strong> all children referred forevaluation, not just those who are suspected<strong>of</strong> having an intellectual disabilityDefining Adaptive BehaviorAdaptive behavior has been defined as “theperformance <strong>of</strong> the daily activities that arerequired for social <strong>and</strong> personal sufficiency”(Sparrow, Cicchetti, & Balla, 2005, p. 6).Hence, adaptive behavior is the antithesis <strong>of</strong>most <strong>of</strong> the behavioral constructs discussedin this book in that it deals with behavioralcompetencies <strong>and</strong> their absence as opposedto assessing behavioral problems.Most definitions <strong>of</strong> adaptive behaviorhave some core similarities including thepremise that it is an age-related construct.Specifically, adaptive behavior increaseswith age in the absence <strong>of</strong> interfering circumstances,much as academic achievementsaccrue over time. Adaptive behaviorsare identified by the st<strong>and</strong>ards <strong>of</strong> others<strong>and</strong> the social context in which the childfunctions (DeStefano & Thompson, 1990).Finally, adaptive behavior assessmentfocuses on typical behavior as opposed toability. This assessment emphasis is alsoconsistent with the assessment <strong>of</strong> otherdevelopmental accomplishments such asacademic achievements.


chapter 14 Adaptive Behavior Scales317Relationship to IntelligenceThe choice <strong>of</strong> informant is apparently amoderator variable affecting the correlation<strong>of</strong> measures <strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior <strong>and</strong>intelligence (Kamphaus, 2001).Correlations between intelligence <strong>and</strong>adaptive behavior measures are modest,.40 to .60 (DeStefano & Thompson,1990) indicating some overlap but independence.This modest relation, however,is an oversimplification. The correspondencebetween the two constructs hasbeen found to be higher in individualswith pervasive developmental disorderswho also have deficits in intelligence thanin individuals with pervasive developmentaldisorders who do not have intellectualdeficits (Bolte & Poustka, 2002). In bothgroups, adaptive behavior across domainswas below average. In addition, moderatecorrelations between adaptive behavior<strong>and</strong> intelligence have been found for juvenile<strong>of</strong>fenders (Hayes, 2005). The informant<strong>and</strong> domain <strong>of</strong> adaptive behaviorassessed also seem to affect the relation.Specifically, when teachers are used asinformants, the correlation between intelligence<strong>and</strong> adaptive behavior increases,<strong>and</strong> domains that assess communication<strong>and</strong> functional academic skills tend to correlatehigher with intelligence test results(Kamphaus, 1987).Hence, if one sees a stronger relationshipbetween intelligence <strong>and</strong> adaptive behavioras desirable, an emphasis on the assessment<strong>of</strong> functional academics rated by teacherswill provide adaptive behavior results <strong>of</strong>desired value. If, however, one views theadaptive behavior construct as separate orcomplementary to intelligence assessment,parent ratings <strong>of</strong> less academically relatedskills (e.g., socialization) should be sought.In essence, where possible, an evaluatorshould seek information from both parents<strong>and</strong> teachers in a variety <strong>of</strong> adaptive behaviordomains.Otherwise, Harrison (1990), who hasmade numerous contributions to the adaptivebehavior assessment literature, favors the use<strong>of</strong> parents as informants. She observes:“The third-party method <strong>of</strong> administration isparticularly appropriate for the assessment <strong>of</strong>adaptive behavior. Because adaptive behavioris generally conceptualized as the daily activitiesin which a per-son engages to take care<strong>of</strong> himself or herself <strong>and</strong> get along with otherpeople, the information sup-plied by a thirdparty will be more valid than the direct administration<strong>of</strong> tasks. The third-party methodalso allows for the assessment <strong>of</strong> individualswho cannot participate in the administration <strong>of</strong>many tests, such as the severely h<strong>and</strong>icapped<strong>and</strong> young children” (pp. 472–473).The modest to small relation <strong>of</strong> adaptivebehavior to intelligence is punctuated in astudy by Szatmari et al. (1993). This studyfollowed 129 children <strong>of</strong> extremely lowbirthweight (501–1,000 g) to the ages <strong>of</strong> 7or 8 years <strong>and</strong> compared their performanceto that <strong>of</strong> a control group. They found significantdecrements in intelligence for thelow birthweight group but no significantdeficits for adaptive behavior. Such resultsindicate that different mechanisms affectthe development <strong>of</strong> intelligence <strong>and</strong> adaptivebehavior skills.While the low to moderate relationbetween intelligence <strong>and</strong> adaptive behaviorscales is well documented (Kamphaus,2001), the relation between adaptive behavior<strong>and</strong> academic achievement is <strong>of</strong> considerableimportance given that academicachievement is <strong>of</strong>ten the criterion variable<strong>of</strong> interest to child clinicians. di Sibio(1993) examined the relation <strong>of</strong> adaptivebehavior to achievement after the varianceattributable to intelligence was removed.While intelligence scores were morehighly correlated with achievement, themeasure <strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior in that studyadded 11% <strong>of</strong> the variance to the prediction


318 chapter 14 Adaptive Behavior Scales<strong>of</strong> achievement beyond that predicted byintelligence. The results <strong>of</strong> this study supportthe importance <strong>of</strong> assessing adaptive behavior<strong>and</strong> intervening in order to enhance achild’s behavioral competencies which, inturn, provide another approach to enhancingacademic achievement.Uses <strong>of</strong> Adaptive BehaviorScalesTraditionally, adaptive behavior scaleshave been considered central to the diagnosis<strong>of</strong> mental retardation (DeStefano& Thompson, 1990). Although this useis circumscribed, it is an important onegiven the epidemiology <strong>of</strong> mental retardation<strong>and</strong> the importance <strong>of</strong> accuratediagnosis that includes a comprehensiveassessment <strong>of</strong> adaptive functioning.It has also been argued that assessment<strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior has been includedin order to reduce the number <strong>of</strong> falsepositive diagnoses <strong>of</strong> mental retardationoccurring based solely on IQ scores (seeGreenspan, 2006).Perhaps as importantly, as the counterpoint<strong>of</strong> behavior problem scales, adaptivebehavior scales hold unique potential forintervention design based on assessmentresults. Adaptive behavior scales measurekey skills that contribute to a child’s successfulfunctioning in a variety <strong>of</strong> environments.That is, adaptive behaviorscales serve a valuable function for theclinician in that they pinpoint specificskills that a child has not acquired, whichthen may serve as the focus <strong>of</strong> treatmentefforts.Adaptive behavior scales are particularlyuseful in educational settings where theirresults can be integrated with the objectives<strong>of</strong> individualized educational plans(IEPs). For instance, adaptive behaviorscales can be used to identify social skills<strong>and</strong> other target behaviors for classroomintervention planning.Characteristics <strong>of</strong> AdaptiveBehavior ScalesDomains AssessedAdaptive behavior scales are analogous tomeasures <strong>of</strong> behavior problems in that thedomains assessed vary somewhat from testto test. In Table 14.1 we illustrate the contentfor three measures <strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior.Domains assessing various aspects<strong>of</strong> independent functioning/daily living,social skills, <strong>and</strong> communication skills arecommon to many tests. These domainsare reflective <strong>of</strong> those in the intellectualdisability criteria described above. Thedomains <strong>of</strong> behavior that are assessed bya particular test are influenced by its agerange. <strong>Child</strong>ren’s tests, for example, mayplace less emphasis on occupational skills,independent living, <strong>and</strong> interpersonal relationshipsthan measures that are moreconcerned with assessing adult adaptation.Measures <strong>of</strong> adult functioning requiredomains aimed at assessing occupationalskills, whereas the child’s analog is schoolfunctioning. The domains <strong>of</strong> an adaptivebehavior scale, thus, become an importantconsideration in test selection. A client’sage, the institution’s treatment program,<strong>and</strong> other factors may also influence testselection.In a 1993 study, Widaman, Stacy, <strong>and</strong>Borthwick-Duffy applied multitrait/multimethodmatrix procedures to the identification<strong>of</strong> major domains <strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior.The participants for this study were 157persons with moderate, severe, <strong>and</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>oundmental retardation. The authorsfound clear evidence for the existence <strong>of</strong>four major domains: cognitive competence,social competence, social maladaptation,<strong>and</strong> personal maladaptation. These resultssuggest that, when assessing individualswith mental retardation, measures <strong>of</strong> atleast these four domains would be desirable.Fortunately, the majority <strong>of</strong> adaptive


chapter 14 Adaptive Behavior Scales319Table 14.1 Overview <strong>of</strong> Three Measures <strong>of</strong> Adaptive BehaviorTest Name Age Range Scales/Domains Administration TimeVinel<strong>and</strong> AdaptiveBehavior Scales,2nd editionBirth through 90 Communication (receptive, expressive, written); dailyParent/caretaker <strong>and</strong> classroom living skills (personal, domestic, community); socialization(interpersonal relationships, play <strong>and</strong> leisureRating forms; survey <strong>and</strong>exp<strong>and</strong>ed interview formstime, coping skills); motor (gross, fine); maladaptivebehavior (internalizing, externalizing, other)20–60 minScales <strong>of</strong> IndependentBehavior: RevisedAdaptive Behavior<strong>Assessment</strong> System-IIInfancy through adulthood Motor skills (gross motor, fine motor); social interaction<strong>and</strong> communication skills (social interaction, languagecomprehension, language personal expression);personal living skills (eating <strong>and</strong> meal preparation,toileting, dressing, personal self-care, domestic skills);community living skills (time <strong>and</strong> punctuality, money<strong>and</strong> value, work skills, home/community orientation)Birth to 89 years Communication, community use, functional academics,Separate forms for 0–5; 5–21;home living, health <strong>and</strong> safety, leisure, self-care,<strong>and</strong> 16–89self-direction, social, workBroad independence scale,45–60 min; short form scale,10–15 min; early developmentscale, 10–15 min15–20 min to administer


320 chapter 14 Adaptive Behavior Scalesbehavior scales measure these domains <strong>and</strong>,in many cases, additional areas as well.Norm vs. Criterion ReferencingPrior to the publication <strong>of</strong> the revisedVinel<strong>and</strong> in 1984, there were no nationallynormed adaptive behavior scales. Severalscales possessed local or regional norms,<strong>and</strong> many were created locally <strong>and</strong> interpretedinformally. Unfortunately, many <strong>of</strong>these scales were used for making normreferenceddecisions such as determiningwhether or not a child had adaptive behaviordeficits that were significant enough to warrantthe diagnosis <strong>of</strong> mental retardation.Among the adaptive behavior assessmentquestions most frequently posed bypsychologists are the following:1. Does the child have adaptive behaviordeficits that are significant enough towarrant the diagnosis <strong>of</strong> mental retardation?2. What are the adaptive behavior deficitsthat most influence the child’s adjustment<strong>and</strong> therefore, require intervention?3. What are the adaptive behavior strengthsdisplayed by the child?Questions 2 <strong>and</strong> 3 are less likely torequire norm referencing, althoughit may still be <strong>of</strong> some benefit. Inorder to answer these questions, theclinician could make intraindividual comparisons<strong>and</strong>/or gauge deficits on the basis<strong>of</strong> how the deficits impair adaptation toparticular environments (e.g., failure t<strong>of</strong>ollow rules in games).Choice <strong>of</strong> InformantThe Vinel<strong>and</strong> Social Maturity Scale representsone <strong>of</strong> the first scales in psychologyto place a premium on parents as informants.This approach stood in stark contrastto the popularity at the time <strong>of</strong> usingdirect measures <strong>of</strong> child behavior, such asintelligence tests.Modern adaptive behavior scales suchas the Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2 (Sparrow, Cicchetti, &Balla, 2005) <strong>and</strong> Scales <strong>of</strong> IndependentBehavior-Revised (Bruininks, Woodcock,Weatherman, & Hill, 1996) still emphasizethe use <strong>of</strong> parents as informants. Formost scales in which parents serve as informants,maternal reports are used nearlyexclusively for item development, scaling,<strong>and</strong> norming. Although fathers are usedless frequently, there are no systematicdata currently available that clarify the differencesbetween mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers asinformants regarding adaptive behavior.Secondarily, teachers <strong>of</strong>ten serve as raters<strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior. However, teachershave different views <strong>of</strong> a child’s adaptivebehavior because <strong>of</strong> the varied dem<strong>and</strong>s <strong>of</strong>school <strong>and</strong> home settings. Domains thatare commonly included on parent scales<strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior, such as toileting,bathing, dressing, budgeting, <strong>and</strong> healthcare are impractical for teachers. Likewise,parents have difficulty reporting onfunctional academics in reading, writing,calculation, <strong>and</strong> some vocational skills.The differing dem<strong>and</strong>s <strong>of</strong> school <strong>and</strong>home environments virtually ensure thata clinician will have an incomplete underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>of</strong> a child’s adaptive behavior ifeither teacher or parent adaptive behaviorratings are not used.Other caregivers also serve as importantinformants regarding a child’s adaptivebehavior. Caregivers may include psychiatricaides in hospitals, nurses, mental healthassistants, nannies, gr<strong>and</strong>parents, teacheraides, work supervisors, or others whohave sustained nearly daily contact with achild or adolescent. These individuals may,in some circumstances, fulfill parent orteacher roles <strong>and</strong> therefore may be competentprimary or secondary informants forsuch scales. Even when parents or teachers


chapter 14 Adaptive Behavior Scales321complete an adaptive behavior scale, othercaregiver information may be <strong>of</strong> value. Anadolescent’s work supervisor is one example<strong>of</strong> an informant who may contributeunique <strong>and</strong> important information to thepool <strong>of</strong> assessment data gathered on adaptivefunctioning.Finally, the child as an informant shouldnot be overlooked. In other words, thechild may be tested directly in order toassess adaptive behavior, although this is aless popular option with many clinicians.Perhaps one reason for its lack <strong>of</strong> popularityis a dearth <strong>of</strong> available instruments. The<strong>Child</strong>ren’s Adaptive Behavior Scale (CABS;Richmond & Kicklighter, 1979) is one <strong>of</strong>the few direct measures <strong>of</strong> adaptive behavioravailable. The CABS uses an individualtesting format to assess Language Development,Independent Functioning, FamilyRole Performance, Economic/VocationalActivity, <strong>and</strong> Socialization. Obviously,these domains are common to many types<strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior measures.As is the case with behavior rating scales,there is <strong>of</strong>ten a high level <strong>of</strong> disagreementbetween the informants who give informationor adaptive behavior scales. Parents<strong>and</strong> teachers have been found to generallyagree on overall adaptive behavior estimates(e.g., Harrison & Oakl<strong>and</strong>, 2003).However, they may disagree considerablyat the scale or domain levels (Shaw, Hammer,& Lel<strong>and</strong>, 1991). Self- <strong>and</strong> caregiverreports have been found to sometimesagree on adaptive behavior ratings but todisagree considerably on ratings <strong>of</strong> maladaptivebehavior (Voelker et al., 1990).Factors such as child age <strong>and</strong> length <strong>of</strong>involvement in a treatment program mayalso affect agreement between informants(see Shaw et al., 1991).Administration FormatAdaptive behavior scales are commonlyadministered using a checklist or semistructuredinterview technique, except, <strong>of</strong>course, in the case <strong>of</strong> direct assessment <strong>of</strong>the child. The checklist format is equivalentto the approach used for parent orteacher ratings <strong>of</strong> behavior problems discussedin previous chapters. The semistructuredinterview method espoused byDoll (1953) <strong>and</strong> popularized by Sparrow,Balla, <strong>and</strong> Cicchetti (1984) differs substantiallyfrom rating scale methods.The semi-structured interview technique,however, requires a high level <strong>of</strong>clinical skill. The clinician has to make theinterview conversation-like, topical, <strong>and</strong>empathic, while at the same time collectingthe necessary information to allow foraccurate rating (scoring) <strong>of</strong> individual items.In addition, younger children could havea difficult time articulating their skills invarious adaptive behavior domains. Therefore,when using self-report procedures foradaptive behavior, a semi-structured interviewformat is not recommended. Therating scale method is more time-efficient<strong>and</strong> practical in that the clinician does noteven need to be present for the administration<strong>of</strong> the scale.However, the semi-structured interviewtechnique has many virtues, including thefollowing:l Allowing the examiner to clarify questionsfor the informant by providing examples,<strong>and</strong> so forthl Contributing to the establishment <strong>of</strong>rapport between clinician <strong>and</strong> parentbecause <strong>of</strong> the conversation-like nature<strong>of</strong> the interactionl Mitigating against response sets such asfake good or fake badl Permitting the assessment <strong>of</strong> adaptivebehavior, despite poor English-languagereading skillsThe semi-structured interview techniquecan be easily mastered with practice. Sometechniques for mastering the techniquefollow.


322 chapter 14 Adaptive Behavior Scales1. Begin by asking general questions <strong>and</strong>then proceed to the specific informationneeded to score items. If one startedwith the Communication domain <strong>of</strong> theVinel<strong>and</strong>-2, for example, a good startingquestion for a parent <strong>of</strong> a toddler mightbe something like, “Tell me about some<strong>of</strong> the things that Tom is saying thesedays.”2. Ask for examples <strong>of</strong> day-to-day behaviorbecause adaptive behavior scales aredesigned to assess typical behavior ratherthan ability (see Sparrow et al., 2005). Afollow-up expressive language questionmight be, “Tell me the words that youcan remember Tom saying today.”3. Become very familiar with the interviewitems <strong>and</strong> scoring criteria for specificitems in order to ensure that adequateclarification is sought.4. Conduct the interview topically. Forexample, ask all <strong>of</strong> the items regardingtelephone skills (answering appropriately,states telephone number, uses payphone, etc.) before proceeding to thenext topic.5. Pursue questioning until you have aclear picture <strong>of</strong> the child’s day-to-daybehavior. Once you have achieved thisportrait, you can confidently rate thechild’s behavior on individual items.Omnibus Adaptive BehaviorScalesVinel<strong>and</strong> Adaptive BehaviorScales, 2nd edition (TheVinel<strong>and</strong>-2; Sparrow, Cicchetti,& Balla, 2005)The Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2 (Sparrow et al., 2005) isthe latest version <strong>of</strong> an assessment toolfor adaptive behavior that traces its rootsto the originator <strong>of</strong> the adaptive behaviorconstruct, Edgar Doll. Doll created thefirst widely used scale <strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior,the Vinel<strong>and</strong> Social Maturity Scale. TheVinel<strong>and</strong>-2 <strong>and</strong> its predecessor (Sparrow,Balla, & Cicchetti, 1984) represent a substantialrevision, adaptation, <strong>and</strong> extension<strong>of</strong> Doll’s original scale.The Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2 consists <strong>of</strong> a family <strong>of</strong>scales each <strong>of</strong> which possesses characteristicsthat make it well-suited for particularpurposes. The Vinel<strong>and</strong> componentsinclude the following: The Teacher RatingForm assesses adaptive functioning in theclassroom for children ages 3½ through 18.The Survey Interview Form is administeredto parent/caretakers <strong>of</strong> individual frombirth to age 90 in semi-structured interviewformat (an Exp<strong>and</strong>ed Interview Form isalso available). New to the Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2 is aParent/Caregiver Rating Form that allowsparents to rate adaptive behavior items in arating scale format.The Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2 has many uses in additionto its popularity as a tool in assessments<strong>and</strong> diagnoses <strong>of</strong> mental retardation.The Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2 provides a rather comprehensiveassessment <strong>of</strong> an individual’sbehavioral competencies <strong>and</strong> can be usedto assess treatment progress, as well as todetermine treatment goals.ContentAside from re-st<strong>and</strong>ardization, the mostsubstantial change in the Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2 fromthe previous Vinel<strong>and</strong> is in the expansion<strong>of</strong> the number <strong>of</strong> items in each domain<strong>and</strong> the inclusion <strong>of</strong> the Parent/CaretakerRating Form (Sparrow et al., 2005). TheVinel<strong>and</strong>-2 includes the same domains asits predecessor: Communication, DailyLiving Skills, Socialization, Motor Skills,<strong>and</strong> Maladaptive Behavior. The MotorSkills domain is designed for ages frombirth through 6 years <strong>and</strong> for older individualswith motor h<strong>and</strong>icaps. The MaladaptiveBehavior domain is essentially abehavior problems checklist that assesses


chapter 14 Adaptive Behavior Scales323very severe difficulties such as problems inpublic, sexual misbehavior, self-injuriousbehavior, bedwetting, <strong>and</strong> truancy. Each <strong>of</strong>the domains consists <strong>of</strong> following content:CommunicationReceptiveExpressiveWrittenDaily Living SkillsPersonalDomesticCommunitySocializationInterpersonal RelationshipsPlay <strong>and</strong> Leisure TimeCoping SkillsMotor SkillsGrossFineMaladaptive BehaviorInternalizingExternalizingOther (e.g., wets bed)Administration <strong>and</strong> ScoringThe Vinel<strong>and</strong> Survey form uses Doll’s wellknownsemi-structured interview technique,a method that is <strong>of</strong>ten not followedloyally by Vinel<strong>and</strong> users. The techniquehas many advantages; unfortunately, its disadvantagesare more salient. Said simply,mastering the semi- structured interviewtechnique is not easy.A central problem with the method isthe necessity <strong>of</strong> organizing the interviewtopically, while the items are placed onthe response form by difficulty order. Forexample, several items <strong>of</strong> the Daily LivingSkills domain <strong>of</strong> the Survey Form haveto do with telephone skills – answering,dialing, <strong>and</strong> so on. The semi-structuredinterview technique involves obtainingadequate information to score these items,even though they are scattered throughoutthe record form. This central contradiction<strong>of</strong> simultaneously interviewing topicallyas well as having items ordered byincreasing difficulty undoubtedly leads toroutine usage <strong>of</strong> the Vinel<strong>and</strong> as a ratingscale despite the fact that this is a violation<strong>of</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardized procedure. Two aspects<strong>of</strong> the Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2 speak to the tendencyfor clinicians to have difficulty with thesemi-structured interview approach: (a)icons are now shown on the record formsuch that interviewers can more easily spotitems that go together (e.g., a book iconsignals items that have to do with readingskills); <strong>and</strong> (b) the Parent/Caretaker RatingForm <strong>and</strong> the Teacher Rating Formnow make it possible for the clinician toobtain Vinel<strong>and</strong> ratings without the use <strong>of</strong>an interview. Nevertheless, given the manyadvantages <strong>of</strong> the semi-structured interviewtechnique cited earlier, it behoovesclinicians to acquire this unique skill.Vinel<strong>and</strong> interpretation features st<strong>and</strong>ardscores based on a mean <strong>of</strong> 100 <strong>and</strong>st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation <strong>of</strong> 15 for the AdaptiveBehavior Composite <strong>and</strong> for each <strong>of</strong> theDomain scores. A new feature is the use<strong>of</strong> “v-scale” scores for subdomains. Thesescores have a mean <strong>of</strong> 15 <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>arddeviation <strong>of</strong> 3.NormingThe Survey Form norming sample <strong>of</strong> theVinel<strong>and</strong> was collected on the basis <strong>of</strong>2001 US Census Bureau statistics (CurrentPopulation Survey, 2001). The samplewas made up <strong>of</strong> 3,687 subjects from birththrough 90 years. The sample appears tobe representative <strong>of</strong> the larger populationin terms <strong>of</strong> ethnicity, SES, geographicregion, <strong>and</strong> disability status. <strong>Clinical</strong> samples<strong>of</strong> individuals with ADHD, autism(both with <strong>and</strong> without language), hearingimpairment, <strong>and</strong> “emotional or behavioraldisturbance” were included in the st<strong>and</strong>ardization<strong>and</strong> subsequent studies (Sparrowet al., 2005, p. 91).


324 chapter 14 Adaptive Behavior ScalesReliabilityReliability analyses indicate good internalconsistency for the Adaptive BehaviorComposite <strong>and</strong> the domain scores, withcoefficients generally .90 <strong>and</strong> higher forthe former <strong>and</strong> .80 <strong>and</strong> higher for thelatter. Subdomain internal consistencycoefficients likewise tended to be .80 <strong>and</strong>higher, with the exception <strong>of</strong> the Receptive(communication), Personal (dailyliving skills), <strong>and</strong> Play <strong>and</strong> Leisure time(socialization) which were somewhatlower (see Sparrow et al., 2005).Test-retest reliability (approximately2–5-week interval) coefficients werealso good, <strong>and</strong> the parent forms <strong>of</strong> theVinel<strong>and</strong>-2 demonstrated adequate intervieweragreement on the Survey InterviewForm <strong>and</strong> adequate interrater reliabilityacross parents on the Parent/CaregiverForm (Sparrow et al.).ValidityMany aspects <strong>of</strong> validity are addressedby the authors <strong>of</strong> the Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2 (Sparrowet al., 2005). The content <strong>of</strong> theVinel<strong>and</strong>-2 was designed to be reflective<strong>of</strong> AAIDD <strong>and</strong> DSM-IV-TR criteriafor adaptive functioning, as outlined fordiagnoses <strong>of</strong> intellectual disabilities ormental retardation. In addition, the rawscores increase lawfully from age to agelending credence to the argument thatthe Vinel<strong>and</strong> measures adaptive behavioras a developmental phenomenon.In other words, with development, anindividual should acquire more adaptiveskills, <strong>and</strong> this phenomenon is reflectedin the Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2 raw score data.Differential validity studies describedby the authors note that the Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2Adaptive Behavior Composite <strong>and</strong> domainscores differentiated among individualswith mild, moderate, or severe mentalretardation. Similarly, the scores for individualswith autism who also had verbalskills were higher than those for individualswith autism without verbal skills (Sparrowet al.).Convergent validity was examinedby the associations between Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2domains <strong>and</strong> analogous scales on theBASC-2. In general, parent reportedVinel<strong>and</strong>-2 domains were moderatelycorrelated with similar parent reportBASC-2 scales (e.g., Communication withFunctional Communication). These coefficientswere generally higher for individualsages 12–18 than for younger subjects.Similar results were found for analyses <strong>of</strong>the Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2 <strong>and</strong> the Adaptive Behavior<strong>Assessment</strong> System-II (ABAS-II; seebelow), with moderate correlations foundacross age groups for analogous domains<strong>and</strong> subdomains.Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesThe Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2 <strong>and</strong> its predecessors benefitfrom a long history <strong>of</strong> successful use<strong>and</strong> numerous research investigations. TheVinel<strong>and</strong>’s noteworthy strengths includethe following:1. Multiple components that are useful fora variety <strong>of</strong> diagnostic <strong>and</strong> interventionplanning purposes2. A supportive research base that suggeststhat the Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2 possesses expectedcorrelations with measures <strong>of</strong> similar(convergent validity) constructs3. An exhaustive item pool which allowsfor the ready identification <strong>of</strong> treatmentgoals <strong>and</strong> objectives4. A large national normative sample <strong>and</strong>several local norm samples, which makeit particularly well suited for diagnosticdecision making5. Modifications <strong>of</strong> the new version whicheases administration in either semistructuredinterview or rating scaleformats


chapter 14 Adaptive Behavior Scales325Among the Vinel<strong>and</strong>’s weaknesses are thefollowing:1. The considerable training that isrequired to properly use the semi-structuredinterview technique2. Relatively little research on theVinel<strong>and</strong>-2 which is <strong>of</strong>fset to someextent by research on its predecessors<strong>and</strong> its familiarity to many clinicians.3. More limited research on the classroomversion, particularly regarding its utilityfor educational interventions.In Box 14.1 we provide a case exampleillustrating the use <strong>of</strong> the Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2.Reason for ReferralJoan, a 12-year-old girl, was referred forevaluation by her father to determine ifshe is receiving the education services thatshe needs in school. Joan is a sixth-gradespecial education student who is currentlyin a fifth-grade inclusion classroom whereBox 14.1A Case Example Use in theVinel<strong>and</strong>-2Name: JoanAge: 12 yearsGrade: 6Evaluation ProceduresDifferential Abilities Scale (DAS)Woodcock Johnson Tests <strong>of</strong> Achievement,3rd edition (WJ-III)Behavior <strong>Assessment</strong> System for <strong>Child</strong>ren(BASC-2): Parent Rating Scales(PRS), Teacher Rating Scales (TRS), <strong>and</strong>Self-Report <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> (SRP)Vinel<strong>and</strong> Adaptive Behavior Scales, 2ndedition (Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2): Classroom RatingScale, Parent/Caretaker Rating Scaleshe reportedly is receiving extra help fromher teacher <strong>and</strong> an aide throughout theday. Her reading skills are reportedly wellbelow what would be expected for hergrade level. According to Joan’s mother,her school has recommended retainingJoan in the sixth grade next year, <strong>and</strong> Joan’sfather would like a second opinion.Background InformationAccording to her mother, Joan has lived withher biological parents <strong>and</strong> paternal gr<strong>and</strong>parentsfor the last 10 years. Joan’s motheris a legal secretary, <strong>and</strong> her father works ata lumberyard. Joan reportedly has no othersiblings. At birth, Joan weighed 7 lb, 11 oz<strong>and</strong> was described as a healthy baby. Joan’sdevelopment during infancy was reportedlynormal, <strong>and</strong> she did not have any seriousdiseases or other difficulties.Mrs. Jordan indicated that Joan isinvolved with her church youth group <strong>and</strong>the local 4H Club. She enjoys caring forher pets which include a rabbit, goat, <strong>and</strong>two dogs. Joan also reportedly enjoys makingcrafts with her gr<strong>and</strong>mother.Joan’s teacher <strong>and</strong> aide were interviewed.Both stated that they are impressed<strong>and</strong> pleased with Joan’s progress, sincethe beginning <strong>of</strong> the school year. Shewas described as a nice kid who workshard. Reading reportedly continues to beJoan’s weakest area, <strong>and</strong> math is a relativestrength. According to her teacher, Joan iseasily frustrated with reading assignments.She is described as a sight reader. Joan’ssocialization skills in the classroom <strong>and</strong>the community have reportedly improved.Her teachers <strong>and</strong> mother noted that shegets along well with her peers <strong>and</strong> adults inher environment.According to her mother, the schoolteam has recommended placing Joan ina sixth-grade inclusion class next year inorder to give her another year to developbefore starting middle school. Althoughher socialization skills have improved, they


326 chapter 14 Adaptive Behavior Scalesreportedly believe that she needs the additionalyear to mature. Joan’s father reportsthat Joan would prefer to go to the middleschool next year with her friends.Previous EvaluationJoan was reportedly in a speech programuntil third grade for fluency <strong>and</strong> articulationdifficulties. She continues to haveproblems with expressive speech, but herteachers report that this is not a significantproblem. Joan is described as speakingrelatively clearly in class. Joan was recentlyprescribed glasses, but reportedly she failsto wear them in class, <strong>and</strong> she did not wearthem during this evaluation.Joan was initially evaluated when shewas in the first grade. From that evaluation,she was diagnosed as mildly intellectuallydelayed <strong>and</strong> placed in a self-containedclassroom. Joan’s Mental Processing Compositeon the Kaufman <strong>Assessment</strong> Batteryfor <strong>Child</strong>ren, 2nd edition (KABC-2)at that time was in the Borderline range(MPC = 70). She was re-evaluated threeyears later, <strong>and</strong> her Full Scale score on theWechsler Intelligence Scale for <strong>Child</strong>ren-Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) was also in theBorderline range (FSIQ = 74). Her AdaptiveBehavior Composite on the Vinel<strong>and</strong>Adaptive Behavior Scale was also in theBelow Average range (ABC = 72).Behavioral Observations DuringTestingJoan was appropriately dressed <strong>and</strong> wellgroomedduring the evaluation. No auditoryor motor abnormalities were noted.She also did not appear to have any visualdifficulties, but she was not wearing herprescribed corrective lenses. Rapport waseasily established; however, Joan was nottalkative during the evaluation, <strong>and</strong> she didnot initiate conversation. At times, she wasdifficult to underst<strong>and</strong> because she mumbled.However, she willingly answered theexaminer’s questions <strong>and</strong> smiled frequently.She appeared to be nervous in the beginning,but she became more relaxed, as suggestedby more frequent smiling <strong>and</strong> relaxedbody posture, as testing progressed. Joanwas cooperative <strong>and</strong> polite. Although Joanworked hard during testing, she becamefrustrated easily, especially during readingexercises. When frustrated, Joan tended torespond more impulsively, shift <strong>of</strong>ten in herseat, rub her face, avoid eye contact, <strong>and</strong>adopt a flat affect.Observations during testing suggest thatJoan’s self-report personality test resultsare somewhat suspect. Her limited readingabilities <strong>and</strong> verbal expression skills, aswell as her impulsivity on tasks requiringreading limits the potential value <strong>of</strong> thosefindings.Test Results <strong>and</strong> InterpretationPsychoeducational <strong>Assessment</strong>: Cognitivefunctioning was assessed using the DAS.She obtained a General Conceptual Abilityscore on the DAS <strong>of</strong> 59. There is a 90%probability that Joan’s DAS General ConceptualAbility score falls between 54 <strong>and</strong>64, indicating that her performance meetsor exceeds less than 1% <strong>of</strong> her peers. Bothher Verbal <strong>and</strong> Nonverbal scores were alsoin the Significantly Below Average range,but her Spatial score was somewhat higher,falling in the Significantly Below Averageto Moderately Below Average range. Theseresults indicate that her intellectual functioningis below that <strong>of</strong> her same-aged peers.These results are also highly consistentwith Joan’s performance on academicachievement measures. St<strong>and</strong>ard scores onthe Kaufman Tests <strong>of</strong> Educational Achievement(KTEA) ranged from 48 (ReadingComprehension) to 64 (Math Applications),which correspond to percentile ranks <strong>of</strong>less than 1. All <strong>of</strong> the academic skill areasassessed were significantly below average.In general, these results are supported by


chapter 14 Adaptive Behavior Scales327Joan’s prior evaluations, curriculum-basedmeasures taken at her school, <strong>and</strong> teacherreports.Adaptive Behavior: The Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2 wascompleted by Joan’s mother, who indicatedthat Joan’s overall adaptive behavioris significantly below average for her age.Her Socialization <strong>and</strong> Daily Living Skillsdomain scores were in the Below Averagerange, suggesting that Joan has some difficultycommunicating her needs <strong>and</strong> ideas<strong>and</strong> caring for herself relative to her sameagedpeers. Her rating for the CommunicationDomain, which was well below average,suggests that Joan has substantial difficultyexpressing her ideas verbally or in writtenform <strong>and</strong> in underst<strong>and</strong>ing what she readsor hears from others. Her teacher’s ratingson the Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2 were higher in eachdomain, indicating that her overall adaptivebehavior in the classroom is slightly belowthat <strong>of</strong> her same-aged peers. Joan’s teacherrated her in the Average range in Daily LivingSkills <strong>and</strong> Socialization. Her rating inthe Communication Domain was in theBelow Average range.The results <strong>of</strong> the Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2 suggestthat the following behaviors should beconsidered as objectives for intervention:Communication: Reading simple stories,printing three- <strong>and</strong> four-word sentences,reading vocabulary <strong>of</strong> at least tenwords, <strong>and</strong> writing vocabulary <strong>of</strong> at leastten wordsDaily Living Skills: Dressing appropriatelyfor the weather, avoiding individualswith contagious diseases, telling time by5-min segments, caring for her hair <strong>and</strong>fingernails, <strong>and</strong> using household cleaningproductsSocialization: Controlling anger whendenied own way <strong>and</strong> attending after schoolor evening activities with same age/gradepeersThe results suggest that Joan’s adaptivefunctioning may be better at schoolthan at home. However, her overall adaptivefunctioning is still below what wouldbe expected for her age, particularly in thearea <strong>of</strong> Communication. Her intellectualfunctioning <strong>and</strong> academic achievement aresubstantially below what is expected for herage. The results indicate that Joan meetscriteria for a diagnosis <strong>of</strong> Mild MentalRetardation.Reports by Joan’s mother <strong>and</strong> teacheron the BASC-2 <strong>of</strong> poor functional communication,poor adaptability to change, <strong>and</strong>difficulties with self-care (parent report)are consistent with their reports <strong>of</strong> Joan’sadaptive functioning on the Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2<strong>and</strong> with her diagnosis <strong>of</strong> Mild MentalRetardation. Joan’s teacher reported on amoderate level <strong>of</strong> concerns on the LearningProblems scale <strong>of</strong> the TRS, which isconsistent with Joan’s history <strong>of</strong> academicdifficulties.General Recommendations1. From the results <strong>of</strong> the current evaluation,it is apparent that Joan shouldreceive resource educational servicesthrough her school. Joan should not beexpected to make one year <strong>of</strong> academicprogress for each year <strong>of</strong> attendance inschool. Therefore, slow academic attainmentby itself may not warrant retentionin grade. Joan’s parents <strong>and</strong> school <strong>of</strong>ficialsshould communicate regularly as tothe appropriate placement <strong>and</strong> accommodationsfor Joan.2. Joan’s parents are advised to read someliterature on other children functioningat Joan’s intellectual level to underst<strong>and</strong>how she learns, what expectationsare reasonable for her, what her needsare, <strong>and</strong> how to meet those needs.3. The focus <strong>of</strong> academics for Joan shouldbe to help her develop minimal competencyin independent living skills, includingvocational skills.4. Joan should receive vocational counselingto guide her educational program.


328 chapter 14 Adaptive Behavior Scales5. Joan may need to be reevaluated forthe speech program at school given hercontinued difficulties with communication<strong>and</strong> the difficulty <strong>of</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>ingher at times during the presentevaluation.6. If a computer is available, Joan coulduse s<strong>of</strong>tware designed to increase herreading, math, <strong>and</strong> spelling skills. Shecould work on the computer alone orwith a teacher. Computer usage wouldincrease her independence, give her theattention that she needs, <strong>and</strong> provideindividualized instruction.7. Joan should be allowed as much as possibleto be around her regular classroompeers (i.e., education in the least restrictiveenvironment possible), so that shecan learn socialization, coping, <strong>and</strong>behavioral skills by observing <strong>and</strong> interactingwith them.Scales <strong>of</strong> Independent Behavior:Revised (SIB-R; Bruininks, Woodcock,Weatherman, & Hill, 1996)The SIB-R in many ways resembles theVinel<strong>and</strong>. It is a broad-based assessment <strong>of</strong>adaptive behavior spanning an age range <strong>of</strong>infancy through adulthood.ContentThe SIB-R includes 14 subscales that aresubsumed under four clusters (i.e., MotorSkills, Social Interaction <strong>and</strong> CommunicationSkills, Personal Living Skills, <strong>and</strong>Community Living Skills). These clustersare summarized by a Broad Independencescale composite score. The clusters <strong>and</strong>their subscales are the following:Motor SkillsGross MotorFine MotorSocial Interaction <strong>and</strong> CommunicationSkillsSocial InteractionLanguage ComprehensionLanguage ExpressionPersonal Living SkillsEating <strong>and</strong> Meal PreparationToiletingDressingPersonal Self-CareDomestic SkillsCommunity Living SkillsTime <strong>and</strong> PunctualityMoney <strong>and</strong> ValueWork SkillsHome/Community OrientationAn Early Development scale is providedon the SIB-R for assessing the adaptive skills<strong>of</strong> infants <strong>and</strong> young children. This scalemay also be <strong>of</strong> benefit for the assessment<strong>of</strong> significantly impaired individuals withdevelopmental ages <strong>of</strong> 8 or lower.Flexible use <strong>of</strong> the SIB-R is furtherenhanced by the availability <strong>of</strong> an IndividualPlan Recommendation form. Thisform aids the process <strong>of</strong> intervention planningthrough needs identification <strong>and</strong>progress monitoring.Another valuable feature <strong>of</strong> the SIB-Ris the provision <strong>of</strong> a short form that can beadministered in 10–15 min. The short formconsists <strong>of</strong> items from the longer scale thatgive a quick indication <strong>of</strong> the overall presenceor absence <strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior deficits.A low score on this scale would triggerthe administration <strong>of</strong> the complete form oranother adaptive behavior scale.A Problem Behaviors scale is also included.This scale measures eight areas <strong>of</strong> problembehavior:Hurtful to SelfHurtful to OthersDestructive to PropertyDisruptive BehaviorUnusual or Repetitive HabitsSocially Offensive Behavior


chapter 14 Adaptive Behavior Scales329Withdrawal or Inattentive Behavior;Uncooperative BehaviorAn additional advantage <strong>of</strong> this scale isthat problem behaviors are not only ratedaccording to frequency but also by severity.The severity ratings provide the clinicianwith more guidance to follow in a problem-solvingassessment paradigm (Bruininkset al., 1996).ReliabilitySplit-half coefficients for the clusters arereported in the test manual to be in the.80s to low .90s (Bruininks et al., 1996).The Broad Independence scale reliabilityestimates were commonly in the mid tohigh .90s. Test–retest reliabilities for theclusters <strong>and</strong> the Broad Independence scalewere similarly high.Administration <strong>and</strong> ScoringThe SIB-R is administered via structuredinterview or checklist in approximately45–60 min. The questions are merely posedto a parent, teacher, or other caregiver asthey are written. Respondents are givenfour response options for each item:1. Never or rarely, even if asked2. Does, but not well, or about 1/4 <strong>of</strong> thetime; may need to be asked3. Does fairly well, or about 3/4 <strong>of</strong> thetime; may need to be asked4. Does very well, or always or almostalways; without being askedThe items are worded as precise behavioralobjectives that require little comprehensionon the part <strong>of</strong> the informant.The SIB-R <strong>of</strong>fers a large <strong>and</strong> usefulvariety <strong>of</strong> derived scores includingst<strong>and</strong>ard scores, percentile ranks, <strong>and</strong>age equivalents. The more novel scoresinclude training implication ranges, a relativemastery index, <strong>and</strong> a support score,although more traditional scores <strong>of</strong> differentadaptive behavior domains are alsoavailable.NormingA sample <strong>of</strong> 2,182 subjects was used fornorming the SIB-R (Bruininks et al., 1996).The age range <strong>of</strong> the sample extended frominfancy through adulthood.ValidityDeStefano <strong>and</strong> Thompson (1990) laudedthe original SIB for its evidence <strong>of</strong> contentvalidity by the observation: “The SIBshows good content validity, in that itsstructure <strong>and</strong> content cover a broad range<strong>of</strong> skills <strong>and</strong> traits included in current models<strong>of</strong> adaptive <strong>and</strong> maladaptive behavior”(p. 461).Roberts et al. (1993) conducted a correlationalstudy <strong>of</strong> the previous SIB <strong>and</strong>Vinel<strong>and</strong> Survey Form that demonstratesconsiderable criterion related validity forboth instruments. This study involved 1284-year olds to 7-year olds with developmentaldisabilities. Both tests were foundto produce one large factor. Similarly,McGrew, Bruininks, <strong>and</strong> Thurlow (1992)found the SIB to correlate significantly withcommunity adjustment for 239 adults withmild to severe levels <strong>of</strong> mental retardation.While the SIB-R is not as well-researchedas its predecessor or the Vinel<strong>and</strong>, the fewstudies available show good evidence <strong>of</strong>criterion-related validity.InterpretationAs with the other adaptive behavior scalesreviewed in this chapter, we recommendinterpretation <strong>of</strong> the SIB-R first at the compositelevel <strong>and</strong> then at the cluster level.In most cases, the composite will give anaccurate overall picture <strong>of</strong> the individual’sadaptive functioning. However, there may


330 chapter 14 Adaptive Behavior Scalesbe relative strengths or weaknesses acrossclusters (<strong>and</strong> perhaps on specific items/skills) that would inform intervention ina meaningful way. It is good practice forclinicians to consider what specific sets <strong>of</strong>skills led to particular strengths or weaknessesin areas <strong>of</strong> adaptive functioningto illustrate the skills that the client hasacquired or struggled to acquire.Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesThe SIB-R possesses many admirabletraits that make it a very practical tool. Itsstrengths include the following:1. Flexibility <strong>of</strong> administration with theavailability <strong>of</strong> both short <strong>and</strong> long forms2. An objective item-scoring scheme thateases scoring3. Its link to other Woodcock-Johnsontests, which foster transfer <strong>of</strong> training tothe SIB-R.4. Broad item coverage (i.e., content validity)The weaknesses <strong>of</strong> the SIB-R includethe following:1. Relatively complex scoring algorithms(which are eased considerably by theuse <strong>of</strong> its accompanying Compuscores<strong>of</strong>tware program)2. A lack <strong>of</strong> factor-analytic <strong>and</strong> criterionrelatedvalidity studies conducted bythose other than the test developersAdaptive Behavior <strong>Assessment</strong>System, 2nd edition (ABAS-II;Harrison & Oakl<strong>and</strong>, 2003)The ABAS-II, a newer adaptive behaviorscale, is organized in a manner that isclosely related to DSM <strong>and</strong> AAIDD criteria.It is a comprehensive system that isdesigned to assess individuals from 5 to 89years <strong>of</strong> age (Harrison & Oakl<strong>and</strong>).ContentThe ABAS assesses ten constructs including:CommunicationCommunity UseFunctional AcademicsHome/School Living (denoted as“Home” on the Parent Form <strong>and</strong>“School” on the Teacher Form)Health <strong>and</strong> SafetyLeisureSelf-CareSelf-DirectionSocial Work (only completed if the individualhas a full or part-time job)The ABAS-II includes various forms thatassess the ten skill areas. Specifically theABAS-II consists <strong>of</strong> a parent <strong>and</strong> a teacher/daycare provider form for ages 0–5, separateparent <strong>and</strong> teacher forms for ages 5–21, <strong>and</strong>an adult form, completed by a caregiver, forages 16 through 89 years.Administration <strong>and</strong> ScoringThe ABAS-II is a rating scale that appearsto be straightforward to use. According tothe manual (Harrison & Oakl<strong>and</strong>, 2003),an informant can rate the child in 15–20min. Items are organized by skill area inorder <strong>of</strong> difficulty, which probably reducesadministration time. The work area isskipped for most children, for example,<strong>and</strong> many skills listed are so far beyond thecompetencies <strong>of</strong> most children that theycan quickly be answered with zero.The item response scale is 0 = is not able;1 = never when needed; 2 = sometimes whenneeded; 3 = always or “almost always” whenneeded; <strong>and</strong> a last category is “check if youguessed.” This latter category may helpexaminers determine the amount <strong>of</strong> confidenceto place in a rater’s responses.Item scores merely need to be summedto produce raw scores by skill area. The rawscores are then transferred to the back <strong>of</strong>the record form to conduct table conver-


chapter 14 Adaptive Behavior Scales331sions to st<strong>and</strong>ard scores for each scale (M =10, SD = 3) <strong>and</strong> a “General Adaptive Composite”(M = 100, SD = 15). Percentile ranks<strong>and</strong> confidence intervals are also <strong>of</strong>fered.NormingThe ABAS was normed on a total sample<strong>of</strong> 7,370 individuals with demographic samplingbased on 1999 US Census estimates.General national norms are <strong>of</strong>fered as wellas numerous validity studies <strong>of</strong> individualswith disabilities. Stratification variablesfor the sample included geographic region,parental educational attainment, sex, <strong>and</strong>race/ethnicity. The norming sample mayhave included a slight under-representation<strong>of</strong> children with disabilities (see Richardson& Burns, 2005).ReliabilityReliability <strong>of</strong> the ABAS-II was evaluatedvia internal consistency, test–retest, <strong>and</strong>inter-rater reliability. Reliability coefficientswere generally quite high with theinternal consistency coefficients for theGeneral Adaptive Composite <strong>and</strong> the skillareas being all around .90 <strong>and</strong> the interraterreliability coefficients were generallyin the .80s. In addition, cross-informantcoefficients across forms <strong>and</strong> skill areaswere good (i.e., approximately .70; Harrison& Oakl<strong>and</strong>, 2003).ValidityThe ABAS-II has been described as havinggood content validity on the basis <strong>of</strong>its theoretical foundation in diagnosticcriteria for mental retardation (Richardson& Burns, 2005). Confirmatory factoranalyses support the factor structure <strong>of</strong> theABAS-II, <strong>and</strong> the manual presents goodevidence <strong>of</strong> both convergent <strong>and</strong> divergentvalidity (Harrision <strong>and</strong> Oakl<strong>and</strong>, 2003).In addition, the differential validity <strong>of</strong> theABAS-II was demonstrated for childrenin a variety <strong>of</strong> diagnostic categories comparedto matched controls. Richardson <strong>and</strong>Burns (2005) suggest that more research isneeded concerning the applicability <strong>of</strong> variousitems <strong>of</strong> the ABAS-II cross-culturally.The same could be said for other measures<strong>of</strong> adaptive functioning.InterpretationABAS-II interpretive guidelines are <strong>of</strong>feredin the manual along with a few case studies.The interpretive suggestions are basic,clear, <strong>and</strong> sensible. We again recommend atop-down approach to interpretation (i.e.,composite to items) with skill examplesbeing provided to illustrate the client’srelative strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses. In addition,corroborating evidence is needed onadaptive functioning such that clinicians donot hastily use a pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong> scores to make adiagnostic decision (Harrison & Oakl<strong>and</strong>,2003).Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesThe ABAS-II is a viable alternative to theVinel<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> SIB-R given its ease <strong>of</strong> use<strong>and</strong> linkage to the AAIDD <strong>and</strong> DSM-IVcriteria. Noteworthy strengths include thefollowing:1. A close link to diagnostic criteria2. Ease <strong>of</strong> administration <strong>and</strong> h<strong>and</strong> scoring3. Inclusion <strong>of</strong> numerous validity studiesin the manualSome potential weaknesses <strong>of</strong> the ABAS-IImay include the following:1. Inability to gather unsolicited clinicalinformation as it is <strong>of</strong>ten available in asemi-structured interview2. Lack <strong>of</strong> validation research by othersthan the test developers3. Slight underrepresentation <strong>of</strong> childrenwith disabilities in the norming sample.


332 chapter 14 Adaptive Behavior ScalesMeasuring Social SkillsA universally accepted definition <strong>of</strong> adaptivebehavior remains elusive resulting inscales that may have a variety <strong>of</strong> domains(Merrell & Popinga, 1994). The development<strong>and</strong> revision <strong>of</strong> adaptive behaviorinstruments <strong>of</strong> late have resulted in measuresthat reflect AAIDD criteria for mentalretardation. However, any one <strong>of</strong> thosedomains (e.g., daily living skills, socialskills) can be assessed in a more detailedfashion than is typically the case with adaptivebehavior scales. One aspect <strong>of</strong> adaptivebehavior, social competence, or socialskills is <strong>of</strong> such importance for a variety <strong>of</strong>child outcomes that scales which measurethis construct exclusively are available.Social Skills Rating SystemThe SSRS (Gresham & Elliott, 1990) is acomprehensive measure <strong>of</strong> social skills thatincorporates multiple domains <strong>and</strong> raters.Teacher, parent, <strong>and</strong> student forms areprovided for measuring a variety <strong>of</strong> socialskills across settings. Although the SSRSalso measures externalizing, internalizing,<strong>and</strong> hyperactivity problem behaviors,social skills are the focus <strong>of</strong> the system. Theteacher form <strong>of</strong> the SSRS also includes arating <strong>of</strong> academic competence.ContentThe SSRS differs from other crude assessments<strong>of</strong> social skills or omnibus rating scales(e.g., BASC-2) by assessing multiple domains<strong>of</strong> social skills. The domains correspondingto each form are shown in Table 14.2. A totalscore is also available for each form.Administration <strong>and</strong> ScoringEach <strong>of</strong> the SSRS forms is straightforwardto use. The parent <strong>and</strong> teacher forms aresomewhat unique rating scales in comparisonto many <strong>of</strong> the measures reviewed thusfar. In addition to requiring the rater toassess the frequency <strong>of</strong> a behavior, the rateris also asked to indicate the importance <strong>of</strong>the behavior for a child’s development.The availability <strong>of</strong> these importance ratingsallows the clinician to better prioritizebehaviors for intervention.A full range <strong>of</strong> scores are <strong>of</strong>fered by theSSRS, including st<strong>and</strong>ard scores, percentileranks, <strong>and</strong> a behavior level. Behavior levelsare somewhat like stanines in that they divideup portions <strong>of</strong> the distribution <strong>of</strong> scores intothree levels where > + l sd = more,


chapter 14 Adaptive Behavior Scales333average. Such indices are available for eachdomain <strong>and</strong> the total score.NormingThe SSRS norming samples consisted <strong>of</strong>4,170 children, 1,027 parents, <strong>and</strong> 259 teacherswho completed forms in 1988 (Gresham& Elliott, 1990). The sample used for theStudent Form is well-described in themanual. On the other h<strong>and</strong>, the Parent <strong>and</strong>Teacher Form samples are described in onlya few paragraphs. The characteristics <strong>of</strong> thechildren rated on the Teacher Form, such astheir SES <strong>and</strong> ethnicity, are not given. TheSES <strong>of</strong> the parent sample is heavily skewedtoward high levels <strong>of</strong> SES. The lack <strong>of</strong>detailed information given for norming <strong>of</strong>the Teacher <strong>and</strong> Parent Forms makes it difficultto evaluate the quality <strong>of</strong> the norms <strong>and</strong>to make recommendations for their use.ReliabilityThe reliability estimates for the Teacher<strong>and</strong> Parent forms reported in the SSRSmanual are generally adequate. Mean coefficientalpha reliability estimates for theTeacher Form subscales were in the high.80s <strong>and</strong> .90s. Parent Form coefficients areslightly lower with more coefficients in the.70s. Student Form reliabilities were generallylow. Coefficients for the Cooperation,Assertion, <strong>and</strong> Self-Control scales did notexceed .70. The test-retest coefficients forthe Student Form were even more disappointing,with none <strong>of</strong> the values exceeding.70, including the Total Scale value,which was only .68 (Gresham & Elliott,1990). Moderate correlations between parent<strong>and</strong> teacher ratings on the SSRS havebeen found (Ruffalo & Elliott, 1997).ValidityConducting criterion-related validity studieswith the SSRS may be difficult because<strong>of</strong> debate about the appropriate criterion.There is not a clear criterion in theassessment <strong>of</strong> social skills. Some sense <strong>of</strong>criterion-related validity may be gainedfrom the studies reported in the manual(Gresham & Elliott, 1990).A study <strong>of</strong> the SSRS Teacher Form correlationswith the Social Behavior <strong>Assessment</strong>(SBA) lends some criterion-related validationto the Teacher Form. The majority <strong>of</strong>correlations were significant for a sample <strong>of</strong>79 cases, suggesting that the two measuresshare considerable overlap. The Cooperationsubscale <strong>of</strong> the Teacher Form was mosthighly correlated with scores from the SBA.This scale correlated -.70 with the Interpersonaldomain <strong>of</strong> the SBA <strong>and</strong> -.73 with theTask-Related domain <strong>of</strong> the SBA.A study <strong>of</strong> the relation <strong>of</strong> the TeacherForm to the Achenbach <strong>Child</strong> BehaviorChecklist-Teacher Report Form (CBCL-TRF) provided mixed support for theProblem Behavior scales. The ExternalizingScale correlated .69 with its counterparton the CBCL, but the Teacher Formcorrelated only .33 with its CBCL counterpart(Gresham & Elliott, 1990).Similar results were obtained for theSSRS Parent Form. In a study <strong>of</strong> 45 parentratings <strong>of</strong> children, the Externalizing Scalecorrelated .70 with the Externalizing score<strong>of</strong> the CBCL-PRF. Again, however, theInternalizing Scale correlated only .50 withthe Internalizing Scale <strong>of</strong> the CBCL-PRF(Gresham & Elliott, 1990). These studies <strong>of</strong>the Problem Behaviors scales <strong>of</strong> the SSRSsuggest that the Internalizing Scale measuressomething different from the correspondingscale <strong>of</strong> the widely used Achenbach.The factor analyses provided in theSSRS manual provide limited insights intothe underlying traits assessed by the SSRSprimarily because <strong>of</strong> the methods used.Principal components were extracted, butfactor analysis was not conducted. Furthermore,the components were apparentlyderived solely on the basis <strong>of</strong> empiricalmethods. Subsequent research has founda poor fit <strong>of</strong> the factor structure describedin the manual for parent ratings <strong>of</strong> elementaryschool-aged children (Van Horn et al.,


334 chapter 14 Adaptive Behavior Scales2007). In particular, Van Horn et al. (2007)found better support for a shorter version<strong>of</strong> the SSRS with the same domains as traditionallyused in the SSRS (Gresham &Elliott, 1990). Importantly, that longitudinalstudy also suggested that the structure<strong>of</strong> the SSRS may vary across ethnic groups<strong>and</strong> that the SSRS may evaluate a somewhatdifferent construct for 3rd grade childrenthan for children in kindergarten. In otherwords, SSRS scores may not be a good wayto track changes in social skills over timebecause the scores may change in meaningover time (Van Horn et al., 2007). Similarly,Whiteside, McCarthy, <strong>and</strong> Miller (2007)found a lack <strong>of</strong> support for the proposedfactor structure <strong>of</strong> the SSRS for elementaryschool-aged children. However, in support<strong>of</strong> the SSRS’s overall construct validity, theyfound that scores on the SSRS were significantlyrelated to a history <strong>of</strong> peer problems.Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesThe SSRS provides a unique assessment toolfor child clinicians in that it is a thoroughmethod for assessing behaviors that are <strong>of</strong>tenlabeled as social skills. Furthermore, althoughthe norming <strong>of</strong> the SSRS is not ideal, it is farsuperior to other measures that do not possessadequate norms <strong>and</strong> yet are used to makenorm-referenced decisions. Other strengths <strong>of</strong>the SSRS include the following:1. An attempt at a multi-domain assessment<strong>of</strong> social skills2. The use <strong>of</strong> multiple informants3. An integrated method <strong>of</strong> interpretation<strong>and</strong> intervention planningSome <strong>of</strong> the weaknesses <strong>of</strong> the SSRSinclude the following:1. Inadequately described norm samplesfor the Teacher <strong>and</strong> Parent Forms2. Poor reliabilities for some <strong>of</strong> the subscales,especially those on the Student Form3. Potential differences in meanings <strong>of</strong>scores across ethnic groups <strong>and</strong> developmentalstages (Van Horn et al., 2007)4. Emerging research that does not supportthe SSRS factor structure described inthe manual5. Less than adequate criterion-relatedvalidity for the Internalizing scale <strong>of</strong> theProblem Behaviors domainConclusionsAdaptive functioning is increasingly beingrecognized as an important, if not essential,aspect <strong>of</strong> child assessments. Although Dollintroduced this intuitive concept in the1930s, adaptive behavior was not formallyincluded as central to the mental retardationdiagnostic process until the 1950s.In addition to its inclusion in diagnosticcriteria, adaptive functioning also hasan important role in intervention planning.To this end, components <strong>of</strong> adaptivebehavior scales are now included onmany behavior rating scales, such as theAchenbach <strong>and</strong> BASC-2, <strong>and</strong> these componentsare summarized in Box 14.2.Anotherinteresting trend is for adaptive behaviorscales to be single-domain measures. TheSSRS is an excellent example <strong>of</strong> the trendtoward developing assessment measures <strong>of</strong>what may be called sub-constructs <strong>of</strong> adaptivebehavior.This chapter highlighted several <strong>of</strong> themost popular adaptive behavior scales, butthe reader should be aware that there are asubstantial number <strong>of</strong> such scales available.Chapter Summary1. Edgar Doll first discussed the construct<strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior by drawingon his experience as a psychologist at


chapter 14 Adaptive Behavior Scales335Box 14.2Adaptive Behavior as a Component <strong>of</strong> Omnibus Rating ScalesWith recent research <strong>and</strong> development inthe assessment <strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior, it is sufficientto say that this construct is no longerconsidered a mere afterthought in childassessment or as a secondary component <strong>of</strong>evaluations for mental retardation. Instead,adaptive behavior is viewed as an importanttarget <strong>of</strong> assessment for describing anindividual’s strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses <strong>and</strong>for designed skill-based interventions. Thisincreased recognition <strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior asa central aspect <strong>of</strong> child assessments is evidentin recent changes to omnibus rating scales.For example, although the original BASC(Reynolds & Kamphaus, 1992) was uniqueat the time in its multi-domain assessment<strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior, the BASC-2 (Reynolds& Kamphaus, 2004) has an exp<strong>and</strong>ed set <strong>of</strong>adaptive behavior domains. The BASC-2 sawthe addition <strong>of</strong> a Functional Communication(parent <strong>and</strong> teacher) <strong>and</strong> Activities <strong>of</strong> DailyLiving (parent) scale. The reader will notethat these scales in particular seem consistentwith adaptive behavior domains included inthe measures reviewed in this chapter. Theother adaptive scales (i.e., Adaptability – parent<strong>and</strong> teacher; Study Skills – teacher; SocialSkills – parent <strong>and</strong> teacher; Leadership – parent<strong>and</strong> teacher; Self-reliance – self; Selfesteem– self; Relations with Parents – self;Interpersonal Relations – self) may overlap tosome extent with Socialization, but they mayalso provide some unique information (e.g.,one’s ability to adjust to change in the case <strong>of</strong>the Adaptability scale). Research is quite scanton these scales to date. Initial validation <strong>of</strong> theBASC-2 found that the adaptive scales weregenerally negatively correlated with clinicalscales <strong>of</strong> the BASC-2, as well as other measures(Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004). Such resultsdo not provide information as to the potentialclinical utility <strong>of</strong> each individual scale. Thatis, although the psychometric properties <strong>of</strong>these scales are adequate, interpretation <strong>and</strong>clinical use might be best at the item levelin terms <strong>of</strong> pinpointing specific behavioralconcerns in the areas <strong>of</strong> adaptability, communication,self-care, <strong>and</strong> social skills. Onthe basis <strong>of</strong> state <strong>of</strong> the relevant research, aclinician cannot say with much confidence,for example, what a low (or high) score onthe Leadership scale <strong>of</strong> the BASC-2-PRSreally means. However, some items (e.g., “Iscreative;” “Is good at getting people to worktogether.”) may highlight specific strengths<strong>of</strong> the child or areas that may need someimprovement.The BASC-2 is not the only omnibus scalethat directly assesses adaptive behavior. TheAchenbach <strong>and</strong> PIC/SBS/PIY (see Chaps. 6<strong>and</strong> 7) systems also have strategies for assessingstrengths <strong>and</strong> socialization. In the case <strong>of</strong>the former, these are limited to a few screenertypeitems regarding overall competencies. Inthe case <strong>of</strong> the latter, these are framed in terms<strong>of</strong> social problems, the absence <strong>of</strong> which wouldimply adaptive social functioning. Therefore,the assessment <strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior <strong>of</strong>fered onscales such as the BASC-2 or Achenbach cannotbe considered substitutes for more detailedevaluations such as through the Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2or ABAS-II. If clinicians are routinely usingomnibus rating scales as part <strong>of</strong> comprehensiveassessments, then at the very least theywill now have the ability to efficiently screenfor deficits in adaptive functioning that canbe more thoroughly evaluated through meanssuch as those described in this chapter.the Vinel<strong>and</strong> State Training School,where he was charged with the assessment<strong>and</strong> rehabilitation <strong>of</strong> individualswith mental retardation. He notedthat such individuals not only lackedintellectual abilities that were necessaryfor academic attainment, but theyalso <strong>of</strong>ten appeared to lack day-to-dayliving skills needed for independentfunctioning.


336 chapter 14 Adaptive Behavior Scales2. The American Association on Intellectual<strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities(AAIDD), formerly the AAMR, liststhree broad domains (i.e., conceptualskills, social skills, <strong>and</strong> practical skills) <strong>of</strong>adaptive behavior that should be evaluatedin assessments <strong>of</strong> mental retardation(see Luckasson et al., 2002). In addition,adaptive functioning is an importantconstruct for case conceptualization<strong>and</strong> treatment planning for a number <strong>of</strong>problems (e.g., autism, ADHD).3. Adaptive behavior has been defined asthe performance <strong>of</strong> the daily activitiesthat are required for social <strong>and</strong> personalsufficiency.4. Adaptive behavior scales serve a valuablefunction for child clinicians in that theypin-point specific deficits that a child hasnot acquired, which in turn may serve asthe focus <strong>of</strong> treatment efforts.5. Widaman, Stacy, <strong>and</strong> Borthwick-Duffy(1993) found clear evidence for the existence<strong>of</strong> four major adaptive behaviordomains: cognitive competence, socialcompetence, social maladaptation, <strong>and</strong>personal maladaptation. Most present daymeasures <strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior includedomains consistent with these findings.6. Parents, teachers, <strong>and</strong> other caregiversare most <strong>of</strong>ten used as informants foradaptive behavior scales. <strong>Child</strong>ren mayalso be used as self-informants for someadaptive behavior scales, although sucha strategy has some obvious limitations.7. Correlations between intelligence <strong>and</strong>adaptive behavior measures are modest(i.e., .40 to .60), indicating some overlapbut also substantial independence.8. Adaptive behavior scales are commonlyadministered using a checklist or semistructuredinterview technique.9. The semi-structured interview techniquerequires a high level <strong>of</strong> clinicalskill. The clinician has to make theinterview conversation-like, topical,<strong>and</strong> empathic, while at the same timecollecting the necessary informationto allow for accurate rating (scoring)<strong>of</strong> individual items.10. The Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2 is probably the mostwell-known <strong>and</strong> widely used tool forassessing adaptive behavior for individualsfrom birth through adulthood.There is an extensive body <strong>of</strong> researchon its predecessor, <strong>and</strong> a number <strong>of</strong>changes were made to the present versionto facilitate administration, scoring,<strong>and</strong> interpretation.11. The Scale <strong>of</strong> Independent Behavior(SIB-R) in many ways resembles theVinel<strong>and</strong>. It is a broad-based assessment<strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior spanningan age range <strong>of</strong> infancy throughadulthood.12. The ABAS-II is closely aligned withDSM <strong>and</strong> AAIDD criteria. So far, ithas demonstrated good reliability <strong>and</strong>validity.13. One aspect <strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior,social competence or social skills, is<strong>of</strong> such importance that scales thatmeasure this construct exclusively areavailable.14. The Social Skills Rating System(SSRS) is a comprehensive measure <strong>of</strong>social skills which incorporates multipledomains <strong>and</strong> raters. Teacher, parent,<strong>and</strong> student forms are providedfor measuring a variety <strong>of</strong> social skillsacross settings.15. The assessment <strong>of</strong> adaptive behavioris increasingly being viewed as a centralcomponent <strong>of</strong> child assessment.Exp<strong>and</strong>ed components <strong>of</strong> adaptivebehavior scales are now being includedon omnibus rating scales such as theAchenbach <strong>and</strong> BASC-2.


P a r t I I IAdvanced Topics


C h a p t e r 1 5Integrating <strong>and</strong> Interpreting<strong>Assessment</strong> InformationChapter QuestionsIntroduction• What are some reasons why assessmentinformation may differ depending onwho is providing it?• What are the issues involved in usingsimple (equal weighing) or complex(unequal weighing) methods for combininginformation from different sources?• How might age <strong>of</strong> the child <strong>and</strong> type <strong>of</strong>behavior affect the agreement betweendifferent sources on a child’s or adolescent’sadjustment?• What steps should one go through tointegrate information from differentsources, from different methods, <strong>and</strong>across different areas <strong>of</strong> functioning?Throughout this book we have emphasizedthe need for a comprehensive evaluationwhen testing children <strong>and</strong> adolescentsin most circumstances. Comprehensivemeans that (1) many areas <strong>of</strong> functioningshould be assessed, (2) assessments shouldbe conducted using multiple techniques,<strong>and</strong> (3) assessments should obtain informationfrom many sources (e.g., child, parent,teachers, peers). If one follows thisadvice, one is confronted with a dizzyingarray <strong>of</strong> information gathered during theassessment. In the previous chapters wediscussed each individual component <strong>of</strong>the assessment in isolation. However, the339P.J. Frick et al., <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Behavior,DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-0641-0_15, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010


340chapter 15 integrating <strong>and</strong> interpreting assessment informationmost important <strong>and</strong> most difficult part <strong>of</strong>the evaluation is the integration <strong>of</strong> thisinformation into a clear case formulationthat answers the referral questions <strong>and</strong>points the way to the most appropriateintervention.Although we discuss integrating informationacross multiple domains <strong>of</strong> functioning<strong>and</strong> across multiple assessment techniques,a main focus <strong>of</strong> this chapter is on integratinginformation from different informants.This is a particularly difficult endeavorbecause one is theoretically obtaining thesame information on a child’s adjustmentfrom different sources. One might expectthat there would be a high degree <strong>of</strong> correlationbetween information provided bydifferent informants. Unfortunately, thisis not the case. A large body <strong>of</strong> researchhas indicated that there are generally quitesmall correlations between different informantsproviding the same type <strong>of</strong> information(Achenbach, McConaughy, & Howell,1987; De Los Reyes & Kazdin, 2005).One explanation for this low rate <strong>of</strong>agreement between informants is that differentpeople see a child in different settings.That is, the low correlations betweeninformants may reflect real differences ina child’s behavior across different settings.The meta-analysis conducted by Achenbachet al. (1987) provides some support forthis possibility. Across the 119 studies thatthey reviewed, the average correlation inratings between informants who see a childin different settings (e.g., parent/teacher)was .28. In contrast, the average correlationbetween informants who see a childin the same setting (e.g., pairs <strong>of</strong> parents orpairs <strong>of</strong> teachers) was .60. This substantialincrease in agreement between informantswho see a child in the same setting providesevidence that the lack <strong>of</strong> informant agreementreflects, at least in part, real situationalvariability in children’s behavior.Konold, Walthall, <strong>and</strong> Pianta (2004)also provide evidence to support the contentionthat much <strong>of</strong> the cross-informantdisagreement is due to situational specificbehaviors in children across settings. Specifically,these authors obtained parent <strong>and</strong>teacher ratings <strong>of</strong> young children’s behavior.They reported that the general form<strong>and</strong> factor loadings for the ratings werevery similar across mother, father, <strong>and</strong>teacher ratings, even though the correlationsamong the ratings from each informantwere fairly low. Thus, the low level<strong>of</strong> agreement appeared to be more likelydue to situation-specific behavior, than dueto the manner in which the behaviors weremeasured.This situational variability has importantimplications for clinical assessments.It suggests that to make sense <strong>of</strong> informationon a child’s emotional <strong>and</strong> behavioralfunctioning, a clinical assessor must alsoassess the contextual dem<strong>and</strong>s that mightinfluence a child’s behavior <strong>and</strong>, thereby,account for the discrepant information oneis obtaining. For example, a clinical assessormight be quite puzzled over a teacher’sreport <strong>of</strong> significant problems <strong>of</strong> inattention,disorganization, <strong>and</strong> hyperactivityat school, when it appears to be in starkcontrast to only minimal problems beingreported by a child’s parent. However, furtherassessment <strong>of</strong> the home context mayindicate that the parents do not put manydem<strong>and</strong>s for sustained attention, organization,or sitting still on the child at home. Asa result, the behaviors are not problematicin this setting.Another important aspect <strong>of</strong> the lack <strong>of</strong>agreement between informants is determiningthe level <strong>of</strong> analysis at which agreement isbeing measured. Individual behaviors tendto show less consistency across informantsthan broader dimensions or composites <strong>of</strong>multiple behaviors. A good example <strong>of</strong> thisphenomenon comes from a study by Biederman,Keenan, <strong>and</strong> Faraone (1990). In aclinic sample <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents,the average correlation between parent <strong>and</strong>teacher reports <strong>of</strong> individual symptoms <strong>of</strong>Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder


chapter 15 integrating <strong>and</strong> interpreting assessment information341(ADHD) on a structured interview wasquite low (r = .21). However, these authorsfound that, if one looked at the diagnosticlevel, the level <strong>of</strong> agreement between parents<strong>and</strong> teachers was quite high. If a parentreported that a child had ADHD onthe structured interview, there was a 90%probability that the child’s teacher wouldreport the presence <strong>of</strong> the disorder.There are two ways one can conceptualizethis issue. On a psychometric level,classic measurement theory states that anaggregate measure shows higher reliabilitythan its individual components (Peterson,Kolen, & Hoover, 1989). Alternatively, thesame issue can be framed as another aspect <strong>of</strong>the situational specificity <strong>of</strong> behavior. Thatis, the same underlying construct or psychologicaldimension may be present acrosssituations, but the behavioral manifestationmay change, depending on the situationaldem<strong>and</strong>s. If one frames it in this way, theimplication for clinical assessment is thesame as that discussed previously. To underst<strong>and</strong>the different behavioral manifestations,one must underst<strong>and</strong> the dem<strong>and</strong>s <strong>of</strong>the different contexts under consideration.Karver (2006) provided data to suggestthat some <strong>of</strong> the disagreement betweeninformants is related to the types <strong>of</strong> behaviorsbeing assessed. Using 20 judges forratings <strong>of</strong> 59 child behaviors on 11 dimensions,three primary dimensions <strong>of</strong> behavioraccounted for 43% <strong>of</strong> the level agreementbetween parent <strong>and</strong> child ratings. The firsttwo dimensions relate to the saliency <strong>of</strong> thebehavior to the parent or child (e.g., ease <strong>of</strong>recall <strong>of</strong> the behavior, objective nature <strong>of</strong>the behavior, perceived seriousness <strong>of</strong> thebehavior). The third dimension involvedhow observable the behavior was <strong>and</strong> howwilling the parent or child would be toreport the behavior accurately because <strong>of</strong>social desirability.One final issue on informant disagreementis the fact that some apparent disagreementmay be an artifact <strong>of</strong> differentmeasurement techniques used across informants.An example is a case in which achild is showing indications <strong>of</strong> depressionon a projective technique but this is notreported by parent <strong>and</strong> teacher on ratingscales. A clinical assessor may consider thisa disagreement across informants. In fact,this is more a function <strong>of</strong> the lack <strong>of</strong> agreementacross methods, because studies haveshown that the agreement between projective<strong>and</strong> objective self-report measures isgenerally low (Ball, et al., 1991). However,even within the same method, what isassessed may differ across informants. Forexample, some rating scales include verydifferent items for parents <strong>and</strong> teachers(Richters, 1992). As a result, it becomesunclear whether disagreements are due toactual differences in perceptions betweenparents <strong>and</strong> teachers or whether they aredue to different questions being asked <strong>of</strong>the two informants.Considering these issues, clinical assessorsshould first try to determine if seeminglydiscrepant information can beattributed solely to varying situationaldem<strong>and</strong>s, the level <strong>of</strong> analysis, the types <strong>of</strong>behaviors being assessed, or to differencesin the assessment strategy. If the discrepantinformation can be explained by these factors,one has gone a long way in explainingthe reasons for a child’s problems <strong>and</strong> oneusually has obtained important informationfor designing treatment. For example, if achild is displaying highly anxious behaviorin the school setting only, one should lookfor what aspects <strong>of</strong> the school environmentare leading to the anxiety (e.g., social-evaluativesituations). In doing this, one haspointed the way to one route <strong>of</strong> intervention.That is, to systematically desensitizethe child to the anxiety-provoking stimuli.Unfortunately, in many situations one cannotbe confident that any <strong>of</strong> these factorsare contributing to the discrepant information.In such cases, one is confronted withthe difficult decision <strong>of</strong> what to do withconflicting information in developing a caseformulation.


342chapter 15 integrating <strong>and</strong> interpreting assessment informationIntegrating Informationacross InformantsSimpler May be BetterTo deal with this issue <strong>of</strong> conflicting information,clinical assessors have <strong>of</strong>ten developedtheir own algorithm or methods <strong>of</strong>deciding how to weigh the reports <strong>of</strong> differentinformants. In Box 15.1 we summarizethe results <strong>of</strong> a survey <strong>of</strong> child mentalhealth pr<strong>of</strong>essionals in which respondentsrated the importance <strong>of</strong> different informantsfor assessing various types <strong>of</strong> childhoodpsychopathology. This provides agood illustration <strong>of</strong> common practice inweighing different informants. In Box 15.2we summarize an explicit diagnostic decisiontree that was developed to st<strong>and</strong>ardizethe diagnostic decision-making process.This decision tree was designed for usewith structured diagnostic interviews. Wefeel that these two pieces <strong>of</strong> research illustratethe problems one faces when attemptingto systematically integrate discrepantBox 15.1A Survey <strong>of</strong> Mental Health Pr<strong>of</strong>essionals’ Use <strong>of</strong> Different Informants to Assess<strong>Child</strong>hood PsychopathologyLoeber, Green, <strong>and</strong> Lahey (1990) conducteda survey <strong>of</strong> 105 members <strong>of</strong> the Society forResearch in <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychopathologyassessing their perceptions <strong>of</strong> therelative usefulness <strong>of</strong> pre-pubertal children,their mothers, <strong>and</strong> their teachers as informantson emotional <strong>and</strong> behavioral problemsin children. Two-thirds <strong>of</strong> the respondents hadPh.D’s <strong>and</strong> one-third had M.D.’s. Fifty-ninepercent described themselves as child researchers<strong>and</strong> clinicians, 34% described themselves asresearchers only, 1.9% described themselves asclinicians only, <strong>and</strong> 5% described themselves inan “other” category. Respondents rated the utility<strong>of</strong> information provided by mothers, teachers,<strong>and</strong> 7–12-year-old children on a 0 (notuseful) to 3 (very useful) scale. There were 44behaviors rated covering the domains <strong>of</strong> inattention/hyperactivity,oppositional problems,conduct problems, anxiety, <strong>and</strong> depression.These authors found that the perceivedrelative usefulness <strong>of</strong> children, mothers, <strong>and</strong>teachers varied as a function <strong>of</strong> the domainbeing assessed. Specifically, teachers were ratedas more useful than both parents <strong>and</strong> childrenin the assessment <strong>of</strong> inattention/hyperactivity.In contrast, parents were rated as more usefulthan teachers <strong>and</strong> children in assessing severeantisocial behavior <strong>and</strong> aggression. Both parents<strong>and</strong> teachers were rated as more useful inassessing children’s oppositional behavior thanchildren themselves, although there was no differencein the perceived usefulness <strong>of</strong> parents<strong>and</strong> teachers within this domain. In general,parents <strong>and</strong> children were judged more usefulin assessing internalizing problems (anxiety<strong>and</strong> depression) than teachers. Somewhatsurprisingly, parents were judged significantlymore useful than children in assessing internalizingproblems.In sum, these authors found that there weresystematic preferences for certain informantsover others in assessing childhood psychopathology,<strong>and</strong> these preferences varied as a function<strong>of</strong> the behavioral domain being assessed.These authors correctly point out that theresults were limited to mental health pr<strong>of</strong>essionals’perceptions <strong>of</strong> pre-pubertal (ages 7–12)children. The results may have been different,especially in the relative importance placed onchild self-report, if the assessment <strong>of</strong> psychopathologyin adolescents had been the focus <strong>of</strong>the survey (see for example Cantwell, Lewinsohn,Rhode, & Seeley, 1997).Source: Loeber, R., Green, S. M., & Lahey, B. B. (1990). Mental health pr<strong>of</strong>essionals’ perception <strong>of</strong> the utility <strong>of</strong>children, mothers, <strong>and</strong> teachers as informants on childhood psychopathology. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology,19, 136–143.


chapter 15 integrating <strong>and</strong> interpreting assessment information343Box 15.2An Attempt to Develop Rules for Combining Multiple Sources <strong>of</strong> Information on<strong>Child</strong>hood PsychopathologyThe struggle to develop st<strong>and</strong>ardized methods<strong>of</strong> integrating information from multipleinformants is exemplified in an article by Reich<strong>and</strong> Earls (1987). These researchers set out todevelop “a replicable strategy to make psychiatricdiagnoses when reports are obtainableindependently from parents <strong>and</strong> children” (p.601). The authors clearly state that the goalwas not to achieve perfect parent-child agreementbut to learn how to evaluate these differentsources <strong>of</strong> information. The followingis a summary (see appendix <strong>of</strong> research articlefor full criteria) <strong>of</strong> the decision tree used bythese authors to make diagnoses according tothe DSM-III (American Psychiatric Association,1980). We present this approach not somuch as a recommended decision tree, but asan example <strong>of</strong> strategies that assessors havedeveloped to resolve the problem <strong>of</strong> discrepantinformation.1. Attention-Deficit Disorder(a) Minimum <strong>of</strong> eight symptoms fromparent report, at least six symptomsfrom child report, <strong>and</strong> an age <strong>of</strong> onsetbefore 7.(b) Evidence <strong>of</strong> impairment in schoolfrom teacher’s report either <strong>of</strong> significantinattention, disruptive behavior,academic underachievement, or peerdifficulties.(c) If parent reports six or seven symptoms<strong>and</strong> child report shows four or five,diagnosis can be made with compellingevidence from teacher.2. Oppositional Disorder(a) Minimum <strong>of</strong> two symptoms from bothparent <strong>and</strong> child <strong>and</strong> a report <strong>of</strong> at least6 months duration.(b) Teacher indicates a pattern <strong>of</strong> negative<strong>and</strong> defiant behavior.3. Conduct Disorder(a) Two or more symptoms from eitherparent or child.(b) Confirmation <strong>of</strong> antisocial behaviorfrom at least one other source.4. Major Depression(a) For children over 13, a diagnosis canbe made from child report alone if (1)child reports dysphoric mood or anhedoniafor 2 weeks or longer; (2) at leastfour vegetative symptoms from DSM-III criteria are present for 2 weeks orlonger; (3) there is evidence <strong>of</strong> impairment,such as grades dropping, irritability,social withdrawal, etc.(b) A diagnosis <strong>of</strong> depression would normallynot be made by parents reportalone unless child report was justunder threshold.5. Separation Anxiety <strong>and</strong> Overanxious Disorder(a) Diagnosis can be made from eitherparent or child report alone only if theother informant provides some evidence<strong>of</strong> anxiety <strong>and</strong>/or depressive symptoms.Source: Reich, W., & Earls, E. (1987). Rules for making psychiatric diagnoses in children on the basis <strong>of</strong>multiple sources <strong>of</strong> information: Preliminary strategies. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 15, 601–616.assessment information either in clinicalpractice or in clinical research (see alsoSmith, 2007).Piacentini, Cohen, <strong>and</strong> Cohen (1992)provide an interesting discussion on developingideal weighing systems for combininginformation across multiple informants.These authors label systems in which onesource <strong>of</strong> information is weighed moreheavily than others (e.g., teachers’ report<strong>of</strong> inattention given more weight than parents’report) as complex schemes. In contrast,simple schemes are those where informationfrom all sources is weighed equally. These


344chapter 15 integrating <strong>and</strong> interpreting assessment informationsimple schemes can also be called the either/or rule, since a finding is considered significantif it is reported by any informant (e.g.,parent, child, or teacher).These authors provide both a theoretical<strong>and</strong> an empirical rationale for whysimple schemes are either as good as orbetter than complex schemes. First, theseauthors argue that most weighing systemsrely on clinical judgment, in the absence <strong>of</strong>any clear research base to guide decisions.They cite numerous findings that “expertjudgments” are “almost always inferior toequal weighing in predicting outcomesbased on multiple variables” (p. 54). Thisrationale, however, would only hold forweighing systems based on clinical judgment.What about empirically derivedweighing systems?The authors cite a study in which a complexmethod <strong>of</strong> combining information wasdeveloped on the basis <strong>of</strong> logistic regressionanalyses. When Bird, Gould, <strong>and</strong> Staghezza(1992) compared their empirically derivedweighing procedure with a simple either/orscheme, there were no differences in howwell either system predicted clinicians’ diagnoses.Moreover, weighing systems basedon regression analyses from one sampleare likely to underperform simple schemeswhen applied in a different sample, as theweighing system is at least partially dependenton the idiosyncratic characteristics <strong>of</strong>the sample. In summary, Piacentini, Cohen,<strong>and</strong> Cohen (1992) provide compellingarguments for the use <strong>of</strong> simple schemes incombining discrepant reports.Although in principle this approachseems sound <strong>and</strong> we advocate that itshould be the primary or default optionfor most clinicians, there are several issuesthat one needs to consider when using thisapproach. First, this approach assumesthat false positives (i.e., a clinically significantfinding from an informant that is nottrue) are rarer <strong>and</strong> less harmful than falsenegatives. For many clinical situations, thisemphasis on ensuring that children in need<strong>of</strong> treatment are not missed is appropriate.However, we also feel that such a decisionshould not be made blindly. Cliniciansshould always be careful in diagnosing <strong>and</strong>classifying childhood problems <strong>and</strong> carefullyconsider both positive <strong>and</strong> negativeconsequences <strong>of</strong> any decision (see Chap. 3for issues in diagnosis <strong>and</strong> classification).Therefore, one must carefully consider therisks <strong>and</strong> benefits <strong>of</strong> both false positives<strong>and</strong> false negatives when developing caseformulations.A second issue was raised by Piacentini,Cohen, <strong>and</strong> Cohen (1992), who noted thatsimple schemes are better only if informantsare asked to provide “only that informationthey would ordinarily be expectedto know” (p. 59). In essence, this impliesthat there will be certain informants whohave better knowledge <strong>of</strong> a child’s adjustmentthan others. For example, teachersmay not have knowledge about certainbehaviors that occur outside <strong>of</strong> the schoolsetting, such as firesetting, cruelty to animals,or needing to sleep with parents. Inaddition to the type <strong>of</strong> adjustment beingassessed, we feel that there are other factorsthat may make some informants betterthan others in a given case. In the nextseveral sections we discuss these factorsthat may affect the quality <strong>of</strong> informationobtained from specific sources. From theoutset we should note that most <strong>of</strong> thesefactors largely will affect the planning <strong>of</strong>the evaluation rather than the interpretation.Specifically, many <strong>of</strong> the factors willaffect the choice <strong>of</strong> informant <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong> themethod used to obtain information.Informant Discrepancies<strong>and</strong> the Type <strong>of</strong> ProblemThe survey by Loeber, Green, <strong>and</strong> Lahey(1990), cited in Box 15.1, indicated thatclinicians tend to weigh adult informants(e.g., parents, teachers) more heavily forobservable behaviors <strong>and</strong> tend to weigh


chapter 15 integrating <strong>and</strong> interpreting assessment information345child self-report more heavily for emotionalproblems. The logical basis to thisargument is that emotional distress islargely an internal subjective event that ismore difficult to tie to behavioral referentsthat can be observed by others. However,there is a growing body <strong>of</strong> research thathas largely supported this method <strong>of</strong> clinicalpractice.For example, mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers showa higher level <strong>of</strong> agreement in rating children’sobjective, observable, behaviorproblems than in rating less observableemotional problems (Christensen, Mergolin,& Sullaway, 1992). Also, researchhas found that adults tend to report moreconduct problems <strong>and</strong> overactivity thanis reported by child self-report (Kashani,et al., 1985; Reich & Earls, 1987), <strong>and</strong>children tend to report more emotionalsymptoms than do parents or teachers(Bird, Gould, & Stagheeza, 1992; Cantwellet al., 1997; Edelbrock et al., 1986). Thisdifferential prevalence in reporting wouldsupport the common clinical practice<strong>of</strong> emphasizing adult informants in theassessment <strong>of</strong> externalizing problems <strong>and</strong>emphasizing children in the assessment <strong>of</strong>internalizing problems, if one assumes thatfalse positives are rare. This view is consistentwith the either/or approach discussedin the previous section.However, there are other studies thathave attempted to go beyond simply documentingdifferences in prevalence to testthe differential validity <strong>of</strong> various informantson child adjustment. These studiestest the differential validity <strong>of</strong> various informantsacross behavioral domains usingsome external <strong>and</strong> clinically importantcriteria. A good example <strong>of</strong> this approachis a study by Loeber et al. (1991). In a clinicreferred sample <strong>of</strong> boys between the ages<strong>of</strong> 6 <strong>and</strong> 13, these authors tested the correlationsamong parent, child, <strong>and</strong> teacherreports <strong>of</strong> disruptive behaviors <strong>and</strong> severalimportant impairment criteria (e.g.,school suspensions, police contacts, graderetention, special education placement)assessed one year later. Supporting commonclinical practice, teachers’ report <strong>of</strong>inattentive/hyperactive behaviors werethe best predictors <strong>of</strong> impairment oneyear later. For conduct problem behaviors,parental report tended to show themost consistent prediction <strong>of</strong> impairment,although teachers’ <strong>and</strong> children’s informationalso had utility in predicting schoolsuspensions. These findings should beinterpreted in light <strong>of</strong> the age <strong>of</strong> the samplestudied. Specifically, this study focused onelementary school-age children. As we willdiscuss in more detail in the next section,there is some evidence that the age <strong>of</strong> thechild may influence the importance <strong>of</strong> differentsources <strong>of</strong> information. For example,the importance <strong>of</strong> child report for assessingantisocial behavior, especially covert antisocialbehavior (e.g., lying, stealing), mayincrease in adolescents, while the importance<strong>of</strong> parental report may decrease asparents’ knowledge <strong>of</strong> their child’s behaviorsdecreases (e.g., Cantwell, et al., 1997;Elliott, Huizinga, & Ageton, 1985).The validation <strong>of</strong> different informantsrequires the measurement <strong>of</strong> some independent<strong>and</strong> clinically important criteriaagainst which to judge the usefulness <strong>of</strong>information provided. This has been veryproblematic for testing the differentialvalidity <strong>of</strong> informants’ reports <strong>of</strong> internalizingchild behavior because it is difficultto determine what should be the validatingcriteria. One notable exception was a studyby Frick, Silverthorn, <strong>and</strong> Evans (1994)that used a maternal history <strong>of</strong> anxiety asa way <strong>of</strong> validating informants’ reports<strong>of</strong> childhood anxiety. Specifically, theseauthors studied the correlations amongparent, child, <strong>and</strong> teacher reports <strong>of</strong> anxietyin children <strong>and</strong> a history <strong>of</strong> an anxietydisorder in mother. In the sample <strong>of</strong> childrenages 9–13, these authors found thatteacher report <strong>of</strong> anxiety was not relatedto a family history <strong>of</strong> anxiety, but bothparent <strong>and</strong> child reports <strong>of</strong> anxiety were.


346chapter 15 integrating <strong>and</strong> interpreting assessment informationHowever, there was additional support forthe relative importance <strong>of</strong> children’s selfreport<strong>of</strong> anxiety. <strong>Child</strong>ren’s self-reports<strong>of</strong> anxiety disorders were always accompaniedby confirmation by their parents.However, parental report was not alwaysconfirmed by the child, <strong>and</strong> parent–childdisagreements on the presence <strong>of</strong> an anxietydisorder were systematically related tothe parents’ own level <strong>of</strong> anxiety. Theseauthors concluded that there was evidencefor parental projection <strong>of</strong> their own anxietyonto their reports <strong>of</strong> anxiety in their children.This provides some preliminary supportfor the relative emphasis on children’sself-report <strong>of</strong> anxiety, at least in this agegroup (9–13 years) <strong>of</strong> children.Informant Discrepancies<strong>and</strong> Age <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong>Another factor that might affect the quality<strong>of</strong> information provided by different informantsis the age <strong>of</strong> the child or adolescentbeing assessed. First, one would expectthat, as a child grows older, parents wouldhave less knowledge <strong>of</strong> the child’s emotions<strong>and</strong> behaviors, especially as parent–child relationships change in adolescence(Paik<strong>of</strong>f & Brooks-Gunn, 1991). Similarly,as a child leaves early elementary school,the likelihood <strong>of</strong> a single teacher spendinga great deal <strong>of</strong> time with a child decreases.Third, as a child develops cognitively, heor she may become better able to reporton such abstract concepts as emotions<strong>and</strong> thoughts. As a result <strong>of</strong> these factors,one would expect that the importance <strong>of</strong>parents <strong>and</strong> teachers as informants woulddecrease with age <strong>and</strong> the importance <strong>of</strong>children’s self-report would increase.Edelbrock et al. (1985) tested thesehypothesized age effects on parent <strong>and</strong>child reports using structured diagnosticinterviews. Using two-week test-retestcorrelations as the index <strong>of</strong> reliability, theseauthors found that the reliability <strong>of</strong> parentreport decreased with age as predicted,although the reliability generally remainedat an acceptable level into adolescence.<strong>Child</strong>ren’s self-report showed a more dramaticage trend. <strong>Child</strong>ren’s self-report onthe structured interviews showed a clearincrease in reliability with age. Importantly,the reliability <strong>of</strong> child self-report onthe structured interviews was quite poorbefore age 9. Evidence for the predicteddecrease in reliability <strong>of</strong> teacher’s reportalso was obtained from another source.Specifically, in the initial development<strong>of</strong> the Behavioral <strong>Assessment</strong> System for<strong>Child</strong>ren, it was found that the reliabilityfor the Teacher Report Form decreasedwith age (Reynolds & Kamphaus, 1992).One question in interpreting theseresults is whether the changes in reliabilityacross age are confined to structuredtechniques, like structured interviews<strong>and</strong> behavior rating scales. At least thefindings for children’s self-report are notconfined to structured techniques. Thereliability <strong>of</strong> children’s responses to projectivetechniques has also been foundto increase with age (Exner, Thomas, &Mason, 1985). Therefore, the hypothesizedage-related changes in the reliability<strong>of</strong> various informants have generallybeen supported in research across a number<strong>of</strong> assessment domains. These findingson the limited reliability <strong>of</strong> childself-report for very young children maynot be surprising for many clinicianswho work with young children. However,clinical assessors who work largelywith older children, adolescents, oradults tend to use a traditional approachto assessment that relies heavily on selfreport<strong>and</strong> may be uncomfortable with areduced role <strong>of</strong> self-report in the assessment<strong>of</strong> young children.A final issue related to informant discrepancies<strong>and</strong> age <strong>of</strong> the child relates tothe level <strong>of</strong> agreement between informantsother than the child (e.g. parent <strong>and</strong> teachers).In their meta-analysis <strong>of</strong> 119 studies,Achenbach et al. (1987) reported that parent<strong>and</strong> teacher ratings were in greater


chapter 15 integrating <strong>and</strong> interpreting assessment information347agreement for younger children than foradolescents. As noted above, part <strong>of</strong> thisage trend may be due to adolescents spendingless time with teachers <strong>and</strong> parents<strong>and</strong>, as a result, these informants may haveless complete information on the adolescent’sadjustment. However, Achenbachet al. also suggest that younger children’sbehavior may be more cross-situationallyconsistent. Importantly, however, theseage related findings have not always beenconsistently reported across samples (DeLos Reyes & Kazdin, 2005).Informant Discrepancies: OtherFactorsIn addition to type <strong>of</strong> behaviors <strong>and</strong> age <strong>of</strong>the child, there are other factors that mightinfluence the report <strong>of</strong> various informants<strong>and</strong> which therefore should be consideredwhen interpreting discrepant information.In a previous chapter we discussed factorswithin the family, such as parental adjustment<strong>and</strong> marital conflict, which may affectthe information provided by the parent on achild’s adjustment. For example, Foley et al.(2005) reported on a sample <strong>of</strong> 2,798 twinsages 8–17. They found that maternal alcoholism<strong>and</strong> marital difficulties were characteristic<strong>of</strong> cases in which an anxiety disorderwas reported by mothers but not children.Also, informants might have differingmotivations, both conscious <strong>and</strong> unconscious,that can affect the informationthey provide (Karver, 2006). For example,a child may not want to admit to problembehavior or a teacher may be interested ingetting a child placed outside <strong>of</strong> his class.Several rating scales discussed in previouschapters include validity scales that attemptto detect such response sets <strong>and</strong> aid in theinterpretation <strong>of</strong> information provided.It is also important to note testing conditionswhen interpreting the report <strong>of</strong> differentinformants. For example, a childmay have been administered a self-reportquestionnaire after a long testing session<strong>and</strong> it is obvious in watching him completethe questionnaire that he is not reading theitems carefully.SummaryFrom this discussion it is clear that,although the simple scheme <strong>of</strong> equallyweighing the report <strong>of</strong> different informantsusing an either/or approach is a good startingpoint, a clinical assessor cannot use thisapproach blindly. There are numerous factorsthat must be considered in trying toexplain seemingly discrepant informationfrom different sources.The previous discussion outlines some<strong>of</strong> the more important issues that havebeen uncovered in research that can helpguide clinical decision making. However,the final case formulation that results fromthe integration <strong>of</strong> multiple types <strong>of</strong> assessmentinformation involves a number <strong>of</strong>complex clinical decisions. To aid in thisprocess, in Box 15.3 we provide a summary<strong>of</strong> an article by Nezu <strong>and</strong> Nezu (1993) thatoutlines (1) some <strong>of</strong> the common cognitivestrategies that are used by people inmaking decisions but which could lead toerrors in clinical reasoning <strong>and</strong> (2) a generalorientation to clinical reasoning thatcan minimize the effects <strong>of</strong> these errorsin clinical judgment. Using this generalproblem orientation as a basis, the followingsection outlines a step-by-step strategythat can help to guide the clinician in theintegration <strong>of</strong> assessment information.A Multistep Strategy forIntegrating InformationThe following strategy assumes one hasconducted a comprehensive clinical evaluation<strong>of</strong> the child or adolescent. The prerequisitesare (1) having information ona child’s adjustment from various sources


348chapter 15 integrating <strong>and</strong> interpreting assessment informationBox 15.3Research Note: Common Errors in <strong>Clinical</strong> Reasoning <strong>and</strong> a Problem-SolvingModel for Developing a Case FormulationNezu <strong>and</strong> Nezu (1993) conceptualize a clinicalassessment as a method <strong>of</strong> translating a client’scomplaints <strong>of</strong> distress into a meaningful set <strong>of</strong>target problems <strong>and</strong> treatment goals. This iswhat we have referred to throughout this bookas developing a case formulation. Nezu <strong>and</strong>Nezu conceptualize this process as delineatinginstrumental outcomes (target problems)that are believed to directly or indirectly relateto a desired outcome for the client (treatmentgoals). For clinicians to use assessment informationin delineating instrumental outcomes,they must go through a complex problemsolvingprocess. Nezu <strong>and</strong> Nezu first presentseveral common cognitive strategies that canlead to errors in the problem-solving process.Next, these authors outline an orientation toproblem-solving that should limit these errorsin clinical reasoning.Errors in <strong>Clinical</strong> Reasoning1. The availability heuristic occurs when cliniciansattempt to estimate the probability<strong>of</strong> an event on the basis <strong>of</strong> the ease withwhich examples <strong>of</strong> that event come tomind. Nezu <strong>and</strong> Nezu use the example <strong>of</strong> aclinician who may overestimate the risk <strong>of</strong>suicide in a new client, if the clinician wasrecently involved in the case <strong>of</strong> a client whocommitted suicide but was judged to be <strong>of</strong>low risk.2. The representative heuristic occurs when aschema is accessed by a given characteristicto the exclusion <strong>of</strong> other schemas.For example, the symptom <strong>of</strong> sadness mayautomatically lead the clinician to considermajor depression (i.e., diagnostic schema)to the exclusion <strong>of</strong> other possible diagnosticschemas (e.g., medically related mooddisorder, personality disorders).3. The anchoring heuristic occurs when predictionsor decisions are overly dependenton initial impressions <strong>and</strong> later informationis discounted, even if the new informationis in disagreement with the initialimpressions. For example, if a depressedclient discusses interpersonal problemsin the initial meeting with the clinician,the anchoring heuristic would lead theclinician to select poor social skills as theprimary target <strong>of</strong> intervention to the exclusion<strong>of</strong> other information (e.g., presence<strong>of</strong> negative cognitions) obtained in subsequentassessment.4. Confirmatory search strategies involve usingprocedures that seek only to confirm initialimpressions <strong>and</strong> failing to seek disconfirmingevidence. For example, a child referredfor problems <strong>of</strong> inattention <strong>and</strong> motor restlessnessis assessed only for other symptoms<strong>of</strong> ADHD, without considering other possiblereasons for his or her symptoms (e.g.,learning disability, depression, anxiety).Problem OrientationNezu <strong>and</strong> Nezu propose that a systematicproblem-solving approach to clinical decisionmakinghelps to limit the use <strong>of</strong> these cognitiveheuristics. The key to this approach is theclinician’s problem orientation. This refers tothe clinician’s overall theoretical orien tationfor viewing problem behavior, which definesthe proper content <strong>and</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> assessment.These authors recommend an orientation <strong>of</strong>planned critical multiplism, which assumes thatclinical outcomes are brought about by a multiplicity<strong>of</strong> interacting factors. The basis <strong>of</strong>this problem orientation is that:“A variety <strong>of</strong> biological (e.g., genetic, neurochemical,physical), psychological (e.g.,affective, cognitive, behavioral) <strong>and</strong> social(e.g., social <strong>and</strong> physical environment) variablescan serve to act <strong>and</strong> interact as casualagents or maintaining factors. Such variablescan influence the pathogenesis <strong>of</strong> a symptom,disorder or both in either a proximal (e.g.,(Continues)


chapter 15 integrating <strong>and</strong> interpreting assessment information349Box 15.3 (Continued)immediate antecedent stimulus) or distal (e.g.,develop-mental history) manner” (p. 256).Nezu <strong>and</strong> Nezu state that even if this modelis incorrect in a given case (i.e., there is a unitarycause), by starting with the planned criticalmultiplism orientation, the clinician is lesslikely to selectively focus on any single typeor class <strong>of</strong> instrumental outcomes. As a result,this problem orientation limits the potentialfor cognitive biases.Source: Nezu, A. M. & Nezu, C. M. (1993). Identifying <strong>and</strong> selecting target problems for clinical interventions:A problem-solving model. Psychological <strong>Assessment</strong>, 5, 254–263.<strong>and</strong> using multiple methods; (2) havinginformation on the child’s developmental,medical, <strong>and</strong> psychiatric history; (3) havinginformation on multiple areas <strong>of</strong> functioning,such as information on academiccapabilities <strong>and</strong> peer relations, in additionto information on multiple areas <strong>of</strong> behavioral<strong>and</strong> emotional functioning; <strong>and</strong> (4)having information on the important contexts(e.g., home, school, work) in which achild functions. Once this information isobtained, the following multistep procedurecan be used to develop a case formulation.In Boxes 15.4 <strong>and</strong> 15.5 we providetwo case examples that illustrate this interpretiveprocess.Step 1: Document All<strong>Clinical</strong>ly SignificantFindings Regarding the<strong>Child</strong>’sAdjustmentThe first step <strong>of</strong> the procedure follows theeither/or rule. The assessor sifts throughall <strong>of</strong> the information on a child’s or adolescent’sadjustment from all sources <strong>and</strong>methods <strong>and</strong> determines if there are anysignificant findings, such as norm-referencedelevations on rating scales, diagnosesbased on structured interviews, or clinicallysignificant material from projective tests.As discussed previously, this is a very sensitiveprocedure that can result in high rates<strong>of</strong> significant findings. We feel that it is agood starting point in the interpretive process.However, we also feel that stopping atthis point would result in an unacceptablyhigh rate <strong>of</strong> false positives. The subsequentsteps help to increase the specificity <strong>of</strong> theprocess.Step 2: Look for ConvergentFindings acrossSources <strong>and</strong> MethodsAfter noting all <strong>of</strong> the significant findingsacross methods <strong>and</strong> sources, the nextstep is to isolate areas that are consistentacross the various pieces <strong>of</strong> assessmentinformation. In looking at cross-method<strong>and</strong> cross-informant convergence, it is recommendedthat one takes a closer look atthe assessment information <strong>and</strong> does notsimply determine whether or not a scorehas crossed some threshold or elevation.We have discussed at several points in thisbook the arbitrary nature <strong>of</strong> many clinicalcut-<strong>of</strong>fs, such as elevations on a scale<strong>of</strong> a behavior rating scale or a diagnosis ona structured interview. How different isa child with a T-score <strong>of</strong> 69 from a childwith a T-score <strong>of</strong> 70 on a given scale? Howdifferent is a child with seven symptoms <strong>of</strong>ADHD from a child with eight?


350chapter 15 integrating <strong>and</strong> interpreting assessment informationBox 15.4Case Study to Illustrate the Multistep Interpretive Procedure: A 7-Year-Old Boywith Behavior ProblemsJake was a 7-year, 1-month-old boy who wasreferred to an outpatient psychological clinicfor a comprehensive psychological evaluationby his mother. His mother reported that Jakewas having great difficulty completing schoolwork <strong>and</strong> that he was extremely oppositional<strong>and</strong> hard to manage at home. Jake was administereda comprehensive battery <strong>of</strong> tests thatincluded (1) an unstructured clinical interview;(2) an extensive family background assessment;(3) a psychoeducational assessment; (4)an assessment <strong>of</strong> his emotional <strong>and</strong> behavioralfunctioning through structured interviewsconducted with his mother <strong>and</strong> teacher <strong>and</strong>rating scales completed by his mother, histeacher, <strong>and</strong> by Jake himself; (5) a sociometricexercise conducted with his class; <strong>and</strong> (6) anobjective assessment <strong>of</strong> his classroom performance(e.g., amount <strong>of</strong> work completed, accuracy<strong>of</strong> work).Step 1: Document All <strong>Clinical</strong>lySignificant Findings on the<strong>Child</strong>’s AdjustmentOn a structured interview, the DiagnosticInterview Schedule for <strong>Child</strong>ren-Version4 (DISC-IV; Shaffer et al., 2000), Jake metcriteria for ADHD according to both hismother <strong>and</strong> his teacher. On the <strong>Child</strong> BehaviorChecklist (CBCL: Achenbach, 2001), hehad T-scores above 70 on both the ThoughtProblem scale <strong>and</strong> the Attention Problemscale according to the father <strong>and</strong> T-Scores<strong>of</strong> 70 <strong>and</strong> 67 on the Attention problem <strong>and</strong>Thought Problem scales, respectively, accordingto his mother’s report. On the teachercompletedComprehensive Behavior RatingScale for <strong>Child</strong>ren (CBRSC; Neeper, Lahey,& Frick, 1990), he obtained a T-score <strong>of</strong> 71on the Daydreams scale <strong>and</strong> a T-score <strong>of</strong> 65on the Motor Hyperactivity scale.Also on the DISC-IV, Jake met criteriafor oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD)according to his mother’s report. Similarly, heobtained T-scores above 70 on the AggressiveBehavior scale <strong>of</strong> the CBCL by both mother<strong>and</strong> father reports.A final significant finding was the report <strong>of</strong>a sufficient number <strong>of</strong> depressive symptomsto meet criteria for current Major Depressionaccording to maternal report on the DISC-IV.Step 2: Look for ConvergentFindings across Sources <strong>and</strong>MethodsIt was clear that there were convergent findingsfor significant problems <strong>of</strong> inattention–disorganization <strong>and</strong> impulsivity–hyperactivityassociated with ADHD, according to parents<strong>and</strong> teachers <strong>and</strong> across structured interviews<strong>and</strong> rating scales. However, for the oppositionalbehaviors, the teacher report gave no indications<strong>of</strong> these behaviors in the school environmenteither on rating scales or on structuredinterviews. For the parental report <strong>of</strong> depression,although Jake’s teacher did not reportsymptoms <strong>of</strong> depression <strong>of</strong> sufficient severityto warrant a diagnosis solely on her report, shedid endorse several symptoms, such as feelingsad <strong>and</strong> irritable, feeling bad about himself, <strong>and</strong><strong>of</strong>ten seeming as if he was about to cry. However,on the <strong>Child</strong>ren’s Depression Inventory(CDI; Kovacs, 1991), Jake did not report significantfeelings <strong>of</strong> depression.Step 3: Try to ExplainDiscrepanciesIn talking to Jake’s teacher, it appeared thatshe had a very structured classroom with ateacher’s aide who implemented behavioralprograms for students. Jake’s parents weredivorced, <strong>and</strong> his mother seemed quite distressedover Jake’s behavior. As a result, sheseemed to be very unsure <strong>of</strong> how to discipline(Continues)


chapter 15 integrating <strong>and</strong> interpreting assessment information351Box 15.4 (Continued)Jake, leading to a great deal <strong>of</strong> inconsistency<strong>and</strong> sometimes harshness in her disciplineattempts. These differences in teacher <strong>and</strong>parent h<strong>and</strong>ling <strong>of</strong> Jake’s behavior seemed toexplain the fact that he was not showing significantoppositional behaviors at school. Whilethere was some support for the depressivebehaviors by Jake’s teacher, Jake did not reportsuch behaviors. However, given Jake’s youngage, there was some concern over his ability toaccurately report on internal feelings using thestructured CDI.Step 4: Developing a Pr<strong>of</strong>ile<strong>and</strong> Hierarchy <strong>of</strong> ProblemAreasJake exhibited a common pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong> showingboth ADHD <strong>and</strong> ODD. However, there wereseveral reasons why ADHD was consideredprimary. First, the ADHD behaviors had beenpresent throughout Jake’s life, <strong>and</strong> they werethe only behaviors shown at both home <strong>and</strong>school. Second, Jake’s teacher reported thatJake’s grades were primarily affected by hisfailure to complete work or his hasty completion<strong>of</strong> work that led to careless errors. Thisteacher report was consistent with a psychoeducationalassessment that revealed an IQ inthe average range <strong>and</strong> academic achievementalso in an age-appropriate range. A sociometricexercise indicated that Jake was dislikedby a significant number <strong>of</strong> his classmates(5 out <strong>of</strong> a class <strong>of</strong> 17 nominated him as “likedleast”). Therefore, the ADHD was consideredto be causing a high level <strong>of</strong> impairment forJake, educationally <strong>and</strong> socially. Third, Jake’sbiological father reported that he had had significantproblems with inattention <strong>and</strong> hyperactivityas a child.Jake’s depressive features were also consideredto be secondary to the ADHD. Hismother reported that they started after thepast semester in school, when he had receivedhis worst grades ever which led to high rates<strong>of</strong> conflict at home.Step 5: Determine CriticalInformation to Place in theReportJake’s mother had seen a counselor for 6months following her divorce from Jake’sfather, 3 years prior to the evaluation. Thispiece <strong>of</strong> information did not seem to be crucialfor underst<strong>and</strong>ing our case formulation <strong>of</strong>Jake. Also, because the psychoeducational testingdid not reveal many significant strengthsor weaknesses in cognitive processing or invarious areas <strong>of</strong> academic achievement, thisinformation was only briefly summarized,indicating that it was not suggestive <strong>of</strong> thepresence <strong>of</strong> a cognitive deficit or a learningdisability.Box 15.5Case Study to Illustrate Multistep Interpretive Procedure: A 14-Year-Old Boywith a Language Disorder <strong>and</strong> Social PhobiaJarrod was 14 years, 2 months old when hisschool referred him for a comprehensive psychologicalevaluation. They were concernedabout Jarrod’s inability to complete schoolwork<strong>and</strong> his extreme shyness. His teachers reportedthat Jarrod rarely initiated conversations witheither peers or teachers <strong>and</strong> responded to otherpeople with only one-word answers. Jarrod hadbeen receiving resource help at school in a learningdisabilities classroom for the past 2 years.(Continues)


352chapter 15 integrating <strong>and</strong> interpreting assessment informationBox 15.5 (Continued)Jarrod was administered a comprehensivebattery <strong>of</strong> tests that included an unstructuredclinical interview, an extensive familybackground assessment, a psychoeducationalassessment, assessment <strong>of</strong> his emotional <strong>and</strong>behavioral functioning through structuredinterviews conducted with his mother <strong>and</strong>Jarrod <strong>and</strong> rating scales completed by Jarrod<strong>and</strong> his mother.Step 1: Document All <strong>Clinical</strong>lySignificant Findings on the<strong>Child</strong>’s AdjustmentOn structured interviews, Jarrod met criteriafor Social Phobia on the DISC-IV by both hisown self-report <strong>and</strong> the report <strong>of</strong> his mother.In the psychoeducational assessment, Jarrodscored 25 points lower in his Verbal IQ thanon his Performance IQ. This verbal deficit wasconsistent with previous testing conducted byhis school. In addition to the verbal weakness,Jarrod scored poorly on tests <strong>of</strong> processingspeed.Step 2: Look for ConvergentFindings across Sources <strong>and</strong>MethodsAlthough not in the clinically significantrange, there was support for Jarrod’s socialanxiety <strong>and</strong> social withdrawal on parent- <strong>and</strong>child-completed rating scales. On the CBCLcompleted by Jarrod’s mother, the Withdrawalscale had a T-score <strong>of</strong> 68. On the self-report<strong>Personality</strong> Inventory for Youth (PIY; Lachar& Gruber, 1994), Jarrod obtained a T-score<strong>of</strong> 65 on the Social Withdrawal scale. Thiswas consistent with reports from his teachers<strong>and</strong> from behavioral observations during testingthat indicated that Jarrod was very quiet<strong>and</strong> withdrawn. The other main areas <strong>of</strong> convergencewere that Jarrod did not show significantbehavioral problems <strong>and</strong> his parentsseemed to provide a warm <strong>and</strong> stable homeenvironment.Step 3: Try to ExplainDiscrepanciesIn this case the only discrepancy was the somewhatmilder level <strong>of</strong> social withdrawal beingreported at home than was being reported byteachers at school, although clearly the shynesswas still evident at home. This was likelydue to Jarrod’s higher level <strong>of</strong> comfort withfamily members, which made him more willingto converse with family members.Step 4: Developing a Pr<strong>of</strong>ile<strong>and</strong> Hierarchy <strong>of</strong> ProblemAreasIt seemed as though Jarrod had an expressivelanguage disorder <strong>and</strong> a deficit in processingspeed. As a result, Jarrod was likely to be uncomfortableconversing in social situations, leadingto the high rate <strong>of</strong> social withdrawal. Thelanguage disorder <strong>and</strong> processing speed deficitwere judged to be primary in this case becausethey had been chronic throughout his life <strong>and</strong>Jarrod did not show a high rate <strong>of</strong> anxiety inother situations that did not require a great deal<strong>of</strong> verbal expression.Step 5: Determine CriticalInformation to Place in theReportA great deal <strong>of</strong> the information on Jarrod’sfamily background, current family functioning,<strong>and</strong> functioning in other behavioral, <strong>and</strong>emotional areas (e.g., conduct <strong>and</strong> depression)were only summarized briefly in the report.They were felt to be important to documentJarrod’s many psychological strengths, but adetailed discussion <strong>of</strong> these areas was not feltto be needed in the written report.


chapter 15 integrating <strong>and</strong> interpreting assessment information353Therefore, when looking for convergentpieces <strong>of</strong> evidence, one should lookat the full range <strong>of</strong> scores, problems, orsymptoms. For example, suppose a childreports a high level <strong>of</strong> anxiety on a selfreportquestionnaire, passing a clinical cut<strong>of</strong>fpoint as required in Step 1. However,on a parent-structured interview there areno diagnoses <strong>of</strong> anxiety disorders reported,but the parent has reported three symptoms<strong>of</strong> overanxious disorder <strong>and</strong> two symptoms<strong>of</strong> separation anxiety disorder. Further, ateacher rated the child with a T-score <strong>of</strong>64 on the anxiety scale <strong>of</strong> a behavior ratingscale. If one just viewed scores thatexceeded a clinical cut-<strong>of</strong>f, this patternwould look like the child’s self-report <strong>of</strong>anxiety was unsupported by other informants,when, in fact, we would argue thatthere is fairly consistent support for thepresence <strong>of</strong> anxious behaviors, althoughthe anxiety is not being perceived as severeby parents <strong>and</strong> teachers.Step 3: Try to ExplainDiscrepanciesThe complexity <strong>of</strong> the assessment processis dramatically reduced when assessmentinformation is consistent across methods<strong>and</strong> sources. This is the ideal casethat is the dream <strong>of</strong> every clinical assessor.Unfortunately, existing research <strong>and</strong>clinical experience suggest that this is notlikely to happen <strong>of</strong>ten. In most cases, therewill be numerous discrepancies between theinformation provided by the differentsources after steps 1 <strong>and</strong> 2. At this stage,one should take the information discussedin previous sections <strong>of</strong> this chapter <strong>and</strong> tryto develop explanations for the discrepancies.Can the discrepancies be explainedby different dem<strong>and</strong>s in the various settingsin which a child is observed? Can thediscrepancies be explained by differencesin the measurement techniques used orby certain characteristics/motivations <strong>of</strong>the informants? Can the discrepanciesbe explained by differing knowledge <strong>of</strong>the child’s behavior across informants? Asmentioned previously, if one can answerthese questions <strong>and</strong> account for discrepantinformation, one has gone a long wayin developing a good case formulation <strong>and</strong>developing goals for treatment.Step 4: Develop a Pr<strong>of</strong>ile<strong>and</strong> Hierarchy <strong>of</strong>Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesBoth research <strong>and</strong> clinical practice indicatethat children rarely have problems thatare specific to one area. As a result, thenext step in the interpretive process is todevelop the pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong> a child’s or adolescent’sstrengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses across thedifferent domains <strong>of</strong> psychological functioningthat have been assessed. In addition,we feel that this process needs to gobeyond simply documenting the differentareas <strong>of</strong> strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses, to alsoprioritizing the different areas <strong>of</strong> concern.This prioritization should be both a conceptual<strong>and</strong> practical endeavor.Conceptually, one should consider whatproblematic area may be primary <strong>and</strong>which areas may be secondary, with secondarybeing defined as areas that seem to belargely a result <strong>of</strong> some other primary factor.For example, for a child who becomesdepressed because <strong>of</strong> his/her frequentschool suspensions <strong>and</strong> police contacts forantisocial behavior, the depression maybest be considered secondary to the antisocialbehavior.Practically, one needs to consider whatarea should be the primary focus <strong>of</strong> intervention.This may follow closely with thedetermination <strong>of</strong> primary <strong>and</strong> secondaryproblems, where intervention targeting<strong>of</strong> primary areas (e.g., antisocial behavior)


354chapter 15 integrating <strong>and</strong> interpreting assessment informationmay also alleviate problems considered tobe secondary (e.g., depression). However,these two levels <strong>of</strong> analysis do not necessarilyhave to be congruent. For example,a child with ADHD may have developedconduct problems secondary to the ADHDbecause <strong>of</strong> the effects <strong>of</strong> the child’s behavioron parent–child interactions. However,given the current problems causedby the conduct disturbance <strong>and</strong> the relation<strong>of</strong> these problems to future problemsin adjustment, intervention targeting theconduct problems may be the primarytreatment recommendation, rather thantreatment for ADHD.Most assessors would agree with theneed to prioritize problem areas, but mostwould also agree that this is difficult toaccomplish with any degree <strong>of</strong> reliability<strong>and</strong> validity. We must admit that prioritizingareas <strong>of</strong> need <strong>and</strong> determining primary<strong>and</strong> secondary areas <strong>of</strong> dysfunction are<strong>of</strong>ten largely made on the basis <strong>of</strong> clinicaljudgment in the absence <strong>of</strong> a clear researchbase to guide this process. However, wefeel that there are three primary factorsto consider in prioritizing areas within achild’s pr<strong>of</strong>ile.First, one should look at the degree <strong>of</strong>impairment associated with different areas<strong>of</strong> dysfunction. As a st<strong>and</strong>ard part <strong>of</strong> anyassessment, one should measure the degreeto which various behaviors are affecting achild’s functioning in at least three majorlife areas: in school, with peers, <strong>and</strong> athome. Then several questions can be askedin an effort to determine if some problemareas are contributing more to impairmentthan others. Are the problematic areas differentiallyaffecting a child’s ability to learnor to perform well academically? Has oneor more <strong>of</strong> the areas significantly affecteda child’s ability to form or maintain meaningfulrelationships with same-age peers? Isany problem area causing a higher level <strong>of</strong>conflict with parents <strong>and</strong>/or siblings thanothers? This method <strong>of</strong> prioritization isone <strong>of</strong> the most important factors clinically,because areas that cause the greatest degree<strong>of</strong> impairment typically are the most importanttargets <strong>of</strong> interventions.Second, one can look at the temporalsequencing <strong>of</strong> when problem behaviorsdeveloped to help to determine what mightbe primary or secondary. Did symptoms <strong>of</strong>depression develop after a chronic pattern<strong>of</strong> antisocial behavior? A good example <strong>of</strong>the therapeutic implications <strong>of</strong> temporalsequencing comes from a study in whichchildren with major depression who onlyshowed conduct problems after the onset<strong>of</strong> the depression showed a significantreduction <strong>of</strong> conduct problems after thedepression was successfully treated (Puig-Antich et al., 1978).Third, viewing family history data canhelp in making a determination <strong>of</strong> primary<strong>and</strong> secondary areas <strong>of</strong> disturbance. As wediscussed in Chap. 12, in the section onassessing a child’s family history, informationon parental psychiatric history can<strong>of</strong>ten aid in making differential diagnoses.This is largely due to the fact that (1)adult psychological disturbance tends to bemore easily defined <strong>and</strong> (2) many forms <strong>of</strong>childhood disturbance show a familial link.As a result, assessing parental adjustmentpatterns can provide clues as to what is theprimary area <strong>of</strong> dysfunction in their childor adolescent.Step 5: DetermineCritical Information toPlace in the ReportIn the next chapter we provide a detaileddiscussion <strong>of</strong> report writing. However, itis important to note that the last stage <strong>of</strong>integrating information is a filtering process.After one has developed a pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong>the child that explains the referral problems<strong>and</strong> suggests treatment goals, it is likely thatmany pieces <strong>of</strong> information gathered will


chapter 15 integrating <strong>and</strong> interpreting assessment information355have either minimal or no bearing on thefinal case conceptualization. A good assessorwill limit the amount <strong>of</strong> unessentialinformation that is collected in an evaluationby carefully tailoring the evaluation tothe needs <strong>of</strong> an individual child. However,much information cannot be determined tobe irrelevant until after it is collected.Many clinicians feel that, if informationwas collected, it should be included in thewritten <strong>and</strong>/or oral report <strong>of</strong> the assessmentresults. Our feeling is that too muchirrelevant information detracts from thecase formulation, making it unduly confusing.Therefore, we feel that the final stepin the interpretive process is determiningwhat information is essential for underst<strong>and</strong>ingthe case formulation <strong>and</strong> subsequentrecommendations for treatment<strong>and</strong> what information should be omittedor discussed only minimally in the report.This final step sets the stage for the writing<strong>of</strong> a clear, concise, <strong>and</strong> readable summary<strong>of</strong> the assessment results.ConclusionsIn this chapter we discuss some <strong>of</strong> the issuesinvolved in integrating assessment informationacross informants, across methods,<strong>and</strong> across psychological domains. One <strong>of</strong>the more difficult aspects <strong>of</strong> this endeavoris integrating discrepant information fromdifferent informants on the same dimension<strong>of</strong> functioning. We discussed evidencethat simple decision rules that equallyweigh information given by various informantsmay be as good as or better thanmore complex methods <strong>of</strong> differentiallyweighing information provided by variousinformants. However, this simple ruleneeds to be qualified by whether the informantis knowledgeable about the child’sbehavior <strong>and</strong> whether the informant canreport the information effectively. Thesetwo factors can be influenced by manyfactors, such as age <strong>of</strong> the child <strong>and</strong> thespecific behavioral domain being assessed.We conclude the chapter by providing amulti-step procedure for integrating thevarious pieces information from a comprehensiveevaluation into a clear caseformulation that answers the referral question<strong>and</strong> points the way to a recommendedcourse <strong>of</strong> intervention.Chapter Summary1. Because most clinical assessments <strong>of</strong> children<strong>and</strong> adolescents must be comprehensive,one <strong>of</strong> the most difficult parts<strong>of</strong> the assessment process is integratingassessment information into a clear caseformulation that points the way to themost appropriate interventions.2. The most difficult task is integratinginformation on the same behavior providedby different informants. Researchindicates that poor agreement amonginformants is the rule, rather than theexception.(a) These discrepancies may reflect realdifferences in a child’s functioningin different contexts (e.g., home,school, clinic). Therefore, assessingthe characteristics <strong>of</strong> the differentcontexts can help to explain somediscrepancies.(b) Some discrepancies may be an artifact<strong>of</strong> the level <strong>of</strong> analysis. Forexample, individual symptoms maybe very discrepant across informants,whereas the presence <strong>of</strong> a disordermay show greater concordance.(c) The level <strong>of</strong> agreement can berelated to certain characteristics <strong>of</strong>the behaviors being assessed, suchas the salience <strong>of</strong> the behavior tothe informant, how observable thebehavior is to others, <strong>and</strong> how willingthe informant is to report on thebehavior.


356chapter 15 integrating <strong>and</strong> interpreting assessment information(d) Discrepancies can also be an artifact<strong>of</strong> different measurement techniquesused to obtain information from differentinformants.3. Unfortunately, in many cases it is difficultto attribute discrepant informationsolely to varying situational dem<strong>and</strong>s, tothe level <strong>of</strong> analysis, to the type <strong>of</strong> behaviorbeing assessed, or to differences inassessment strategy. In these cases thereis evidence that a simple either/or systemfor combining information worksbest. That is, a finding is considered significantif reported by any informant.(a) This approach assumes that falsepositives are rarer <strong>and</strong> less harmfulthan false negatives.(b) This approach also assumes thatinformants are only asked to provideinformation that they should beexpected to know.4. The quality <strong>of</strong> information providedby different informants seems to varydepending on the domain <strong>of</strong> functioningbeing assessed <strong>and</strong> the age <strong>of</strong> thechild being assessed.(a) Research suggests that teacherreport <strong>of</strong> inattentive-hyperactivebehaviors, parent report <strong>of</strong> conductproblems, <strong>and</strong> child report <strong>of</strong> emotionalproblems seem to be relativelymost important in preadolescentchildren.(b) The usefulness <strong>of</strong> child self-reportincreases with age <strong>and</strong> the usefulness<strong>of</strong> teacher report decreases withage. Age-related changes in parentalreport are less consistently found,although there are some indicationsthat the usefulness <strong>of</strong> parental reportalso decreases somewhat as the childenters adolescence.5. Other factors, such as a parent’s emotionaladjustment, the degree <strong>of</strong> maritalconflict, idiosyncratic motivations <strong>of</strong> aninformant, <strong>and</strong> testing conditions, caninfluence informant agreement.6. A multistep procedure for integratingdiverse assessment information is recommended.Step 1: Document all clinically significantfindings on the child’sadjustment.Step 2: Look for convergence acrosssources <strong>and</strong> methods.Step 3: Attempt to explain discrepancies.Step 4: Develop a pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>and</strong> hierarchy<strong>of</strong> findings.Step 5: Determine what critical informationto place in the formalreport.


C h a p t e r 1 6Report WritingChapter Questionsl How does the call for evidence-basedassessment lend itself to report writing?l How does a clinician clarify the referralquestion?l What are the common mistakes madein report writing?l How should assessment results bereported to parents in conferences?Reporting Problems<strong>and</strong> ChallengesPresenting assessment results orally or inwriting can be a foreboding task. However,this process is central to assessment. Themost sophisticated, accurate, <strong>and</strong> comprehensivecase conceptualization is uselessif the key figures in a child’s life (e.g., parents,teachers) are unaware <strong>of</strong> the results orrecommendations from an assessment ordo not fully underst<strong>and</strong> them. Aside fromappropriately <strong>and</strong> accurately conveyingresults <strong>and</strong> recommendations, clinicians arealso <strong>of</strong>ten faced with fears <strong>of</strong> litigation <strong>and</strong>insecurities about their interpretive skill.Thus, a chapter on report writing is crucialfor an assessment text. As clinicians knowwell, their written products can carry a greatdeal <strong>of</strong> importance, <strong>and</strong> if done well, theycan facilitate positive outcomes for a child.On the other h<strong>and</strong>, if a report is faulty (i.e.,inaccurate, unclear, full <strong>of</strong> errors), it willmake no impact aside from being a negativereflection on the clinician who wrote it.Effective psychological report writing istaking on increased importance for practicingpsychologists. Psychological reports aremade available to parents, judges, lawyers,357P.J. Frick et al., <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Behavior,DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-0641-0_16, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010


358 chapter 16 report writing<strong>and</strong> other non-psychologists, creating theopportunity for improper interpretation <strong>of</strong>the results by untrained individuals. Morepositively, psychological reports remainparticularly useful to other clinicians whoevaluate a child who has previously beenseen by a psychologist. A previous psychologicalreport can provide a valuable baselineagainst which a clinician can gaugeresponse to treatment, the emergence <strong>of</strong>a comorbid problem, <strong>and</strong> other factors. Aprevious diagnosis <strong>of</strong> Conduct Disorder,for example, may encourage the evaluatingpsychologist to screen for depressionbecause <strong>of</strong> known comorbidity (DeBaryshe,Patterson, & Capaldi, 1993). A cliniciancan significantly enhance the quality <strong>of</strong>work conducted by a successor through theproduction <strong>of</strong> an articulate written report.Despite its importance, the topic <strong>of</strong>report writing is relatively neglected in theresearch literature (Ownby & Wallbrown,1986). While a number <strong>of</strong> works are availableon this topic (e.g., Braaten, 2007; Lichtenberger,Mather, Kaufman, & Kaufman,2004; Tallent, 1993), little research has beenconducted to assess the effects <strong>of</strong> reportwriting on important outcomes such as thelikelihood that a recommendation will befollowed (Ownby & Wallbrown, 1986).Ownby <strong>and</strong> Wallbrown (1986) draw severaldiscouraging conclusions. They concludeas follows on psychological reports:l Considered useful to some extent byconsumers such as psychiatrists <strong>and</strong>social workersl Frequently criticized by these pr<strong>of</strong>essionalgroups on both content <strong>and</strong> stylisticgroundsl May (or may not) make substantial contributionsto patient managementIn addition to the opinions <strong>of</strong> psychiatrists<strong>and</strong> social workers, a number <strong>of</strong> studies haveassessed teachers’ satisfaction with psychologicalreports <strong>and</strong> have found that they arefrequently dissatisfied with them (Ownby& Wallbrown, 1986). One can get a sense<strong>of</strong> why teachers <strong>and</strong> other pr<strong>of</strong>essionals aredissatisfied with psychological reports byreading the following excerpt that was takenverbatim from a report. All <strong>of</strong> the conclusionsdrawn by the evaluator in this case areon the basis <strong>of</strong> one test requiring the child tosimply reproduce nine designs with pencil<strong>and</strong> paper.We quote:The Bender-Visual Motor Gestalt test suggestsdelinquency <strong>and</strong> an acting out potential. He isanxious, confused, insecure <strong>and</strong> has a low selfesteem.He may have difficulties in interpersonalrelationships <strong>and</strong> tends to isolate himself whenproblems arise…. [He] also seems to have a lot<strong>of</strong> anxiety <strong>and</strong> tension over phallic sexuality <strong>and</strong>may be in somewhat <strong>of</strong> a homosexual panic.This clinician was apparently using a cookbookapproach to interpretation, conveyingno clear evidence to support his/hercase conceptualization. A report like this is<strong>of</strong> no help to anyone, especially not to thechild being evaluated.One <strong>of</strong> the difficulties with report writingis that different audiences require differentreports. For example, a psychometricsummary (i.e., a portion <strong>of</strong> the report thatpresents only test scores <strong>and</strong> is usually givenat the beginning or as an appendix at the end<strong>of</strong> a report) given without context is likely tobe <strong>of</strong> little use to parents but <strong>of</strong> great potentialuse to colleagues <strong>and</strong> perhaps teachers.An important decision that each psychologistmust make prior to report writing isto determine the primary audience for thereport. For example, a psychometric summarymay be <strong>of</strong> minimal use to parentswho have contracted with the psychologistin private practice for an evaluation. Inthis case, it is more sensible to present testresults in context in order to communicateeffectively with the parents. A psychometricsummary is more in order in a treatmentteam situation, where it is imperative that apsychologist communicate effectively withknowledgeable colleagues.


chapter 16 report writing359In most situations, we recommend thatthe clinician attempt to make his or herreports accessible <strong>and</strong> useful to all pertinentaudiences so that interested parties do nothave different reports for the same assessment<strong>of</strong> the same child. Our discussion willfocus mainly on the expectation that onereport will be made available to parents,teachers, physicians, etc., with the underst<strong>and</strong>ingthat certain presentations <strong>of</strong> results<strong>and</strong> interpretations will be most useful tocertain audiences.Report Writing as Part <strong>of</strong>Evidence-Based <strong>Assessment</strong>Report writing has not specifically beenaddressed in recent writings on evidencebasedassessment <strong>of</strong> children. Instead, thediscussion has focused on the use <strong>of</strong> tools<strong>and</strong> methods that are valid <strong>and</strong> that demonstrateclinical utility (see Mash & Hunsley,2005). We feel that the move towardevidence-based assessment should, <strong>and</strong>will, be reflected in the reports that resultfrom psychological assessments.Mash <strong>and</strong> Hunsley (2005) point out thatevidence-based assessment is not meant toreplace the clinician or the clinician’s judgment.Similarly, the clinician will continue tobe a key figure in assessment reports. That is,it is unlikely <strong>and</strong> undesirable that reports willbe completely boiler plate endeavors thatdo not allow for flexibility based on the particularassessment approach used, the client’sparticular presenting problems, or the needs<strong>of</strong> the client <strong>and</strong> allied pr<strong>of</strong>essionals.In contrast, not unlike a scientific manuscript,reports from an evidence-basedapproach to assessment can be seen as themeans by which a client’s history <strong>and</strong> difficultiesare described, results are obtained<strong>and</strong> interpreted, <strong>and</strong> suggestions for future(treatment) approaches to the difficultiesare discussed.The main sections <strong>of</strong> most psychologicalreports are discussed later in this chapter, butin many ways, they are analogous to sections<strong>of</strong> journal articles in psychology <strong>and</strong> otherscientific fields. For example, the “ReferralQuestions” section is essentially a statement<strong>of</strong> the evaluation’s purpose. “BackgroundInformation” in a report is similar to a literaturereview in a research article, whereinthe previously noted issues are mentioned<strong>and</strong> the current questions or problems arepresented to the audience. The “<strong>Assessment</strong>Procedures” or Psychometric Summary providesthe methods used in trying to addressthe referral question(s). The results <strong>of</strong> thecurrent assessment begin to be addressedfully in the “Behavioral Observations” section.This section provides a context for theassessment results, particularly any testingthat occurred directly with the client. Theanalogy in a scientific manuscript would beinitial analyses that point to any variablesthat need to be controlled or any conditionsthat might call some results into question.Similarly, testing conditions or client factors(e.g., child was sick on the day <strong>of</strong> testing)could be important information for interpretingassessment results. The “<strong>Assessment</strong>Results <strong>and</strong> Interpretation” sectionsare ideally a mix <strong>of</strong> what might be found inthe “Results” <strong>and</strong> “Discussion” sections <strong>of</strong>a scientific article. In the report, the clinicianshould not present data with no interpretation,<strong>and</strong> the clinician should not makeinterpretations without clearly providingthe data on which they were made. Recommendations,which are critical in an assessmentreport, allow the clinician to suggestwhat should be done in the future to addressthe problem. Researchers routinely do thisas well in their published manuscripts.Pointing to consistencies between assessmentreports <strong>and</strong> scientific manuscripts isan oversimplification <strong>of</strong> the report writingprocess in some ways. Nevertheless, manyclinicians-in-training are also well-versedin research methodology <strong>and</strong> writing, <strong>and</strong>this analogy may serve to make reportwriting seem less nebulous <strong>and</strong> daunting.The collection <strong>of</strong> background information,the scoring <strong>of</strong> measures, <strong>and</strong> inter-


360 chapter 16 report writingpretation <strong>of</strong> results that occur in researchare essentially the processes that take placein evidence-based assessment <strong>and</strong> reportwriting. The gathering <strong>and</strong> explaining <strong>of</strong>evidence allow the clinician to clearly presenta case conceptualization (theory) <strong>of</strong> theclient’s difficulties that is grounded in data,as opposed to the approach evidenced inthe quote earlier in this chapter.Pitfalls <strong>of</strong> Report WritingComplaints about psychological reports persist.Norman Tallent (1993) wrote a l<strong>and</strong>marktextbook on report writing in which hesummarized the literature on the strengths<strong>and</strong> weaknesses <strong>of</strong> reports as identified bypsychologists’ colleagues in mental healthcare, most notably social workers <strong>and</strong> psychiatrists.Some <strong>of</strong> the highlights <strong>of</strong> Tallent’sreview are outlined in the next section.Vocabulary ProblemsThe problem <strong>of</strong> using vague or impreciselanguage in report writing is commonplace.The colloquial term used to describe suchlanguage is psychobabble. Siskind (1967), forexample, studied the level <strong>of</strong> agreementbetween psychologists <strong>and</strong> psychiatrists indefining words such as the following:AbstractAffectiveAggressionAnxietyBizarreBrightCompulsiveControlDefenseDependentDepressiveEmotionalHostilityImmaturityImpulsiveNormal ConstrictionThe results <strong>of</strong> the study showed very littlecorrespondence between the definitions<strong>of</strong>fered by the two groups <strong>of</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>essionals.We suspect that without clear descriptors<strong>of</strong> problems (e.g., hyperactivity, depression),such disagreement among consumers<strong>of</strong> reports persists today.Tallent (1993) refers to one aspect <strong>of</strong> thisproblem with language as exhibitionism,which seems to be a frequent criticism <strong>of</strong>reports, particularly on the part <strong>of</strong> other psychologists.One commentator stated, “Theyare written in stilted psychological terms toboost the ego <strong>of</strong> the psychologist” (p. 33).Some other pertinent observations byTallent (1993) on the use <strong>of</strong> language by psychologistsin reports are paraphrased below:l They include complex (meaningless)words that are <strong>of</strong>ten used to add lengthto the report.l They are written in esoteric languageunderstood by the psychologist only.For example, it may be said that clientsmanifest overt aggressive hostility in animpulsive manner – when, in fact, theypunch you on the nose.l They are not frequently enough writtenin lay language. In particular, scores areover- emphasized, <strong>and</strong> the fit between theresults <strong>and</strong> the child’s actual behavior isunder-emphasized. Tallent (1993) arguesthat an excessive focus on multiple scoresor indices may be a method to cover upthe clinician’s lack <strong>of</strong> true underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>of</strong> the assessment findings.l They include language that is so vague<strong>and</strong> unclear that it cannot be falsified orconsidered wrong.These latter two points are critical if reportsare to address the referral question in a mannerthat is amenable to subsequent, appropriateintervention. Of course, psychologycannot be singled out as the only pr<strong>of</strong>essionwith a preference for its own idiosyncraticterminology, as anyone who reads a physician’sreport or a legal contract will attest.Perhaps psychologists can, however, lead theway toward competent reporting <strong>of</strong> findings.


chapter 16 report writing361Faulty InterpretationFaulty interpretations may be made on thebasis <strong>of</strong> personal ideas, biases, <strong>and</strong> idiosyncrasies(Tallent, 1993). The problem ismost readily seen when the psychologistis clearly using the same theories or drawingthe same conclusions in every report. Apsychologist may conclude that all children’sproblems are due to poor ego functioning,neuropsychological problems, orfamily system failure. A psychologist whoadheres exclusively to behavioral principles,for example, will attribute all childproblems to faulty reinforcement histories.The savvy consumer <strong>of</strong> this psychologist’sreports will eventually become wary <strong>of</strong> thepsychologist’s conclusions, as the relevance<strong>of</strong> the favored theory to some cases is questionable.One can imagine the skepticismthat may be engendered by a psychologistwho concludes that a child whose schoolperformance has just deteriorated subsequentto a traumatic head injury merelyneeds more positive reinforcement to bringhis grades up to pre-trauma levels.Problems may also occur if a psychologistdraws conclusions that are clearly in conflictwith the data collected for a child. A psychologistmay decide not to make a diagnosis,in seeming contrast to rating scale findings<strong>of</strong> significant T-scores on the majority <strong>of</strong>scales. If a clear argument for resolving thisincongruity is not made, the consumer <strong>of</strong> thereport may well suspect biases. The psychologistwho routinely does not reconcile highT-scores with a lack <strong>of</strong> a diagnosis may soonbe labeled as unwilling to diagnose regardless<strong>of</strong> assessment results. The reverse situationcan also be problematic, wherein thepsychologist makes a diagnosis without anyclear indications <strong>of</strong> significant symptomatologyor impairment. Teachers, pediatricians,or other referral sources who receive thisinterpretation consistently from the samepsychologist may eventually pay more attentionto the data presented in the reports <strong>and</strong>ignore the psychologist’s conclusions, or theymay simply refer elsewhere.Report LengthPsychologists, more so than other groups,complain about the excessive length <strong>of</strong> reports(Tallent, 1993). However, length may not bethe real issue. Perhaps long reports are usedto disguise incompetence, fulfill needs foraccountability, or impress others. The possibilitythat length is a cover for other ills is<strong>of</strong>fered in the following example:A business executive likes to relate the anecdoteabout the occasion when he assigned anew employee to prepare a report for him. Indue time, a voluminous piece <strong>of</strong> writing wasreturned. Dismayed, the executive pointed outthat the required information could be presentedon one, certainly not more than two, pages.But sir, pleaded the young man, I don’t knowthat much about the matter you assigned me to(Tallent, 1988, p. 72).It may also be worth considering that theTen Comm<strong>and</strong>ments are expressed in 297words, the Declaration <strong>of</strong> Independence isin 300 words, <strong>and</strong> the Gettysburg Addressis in 266 words.Number ObsessionThe clinician must always keep clearly inmind that the child is the lodestar <strong>of</strong> theevaluation, <strong>and</strong> the numbers obtainedfrom personality tests <strong>and</strong> the like are onlyworthy <strong>of</strong> emphasis if they contribute tothe underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> the child being evaluated.One way to think <strong>of</strong> the scores is asa means to an end, with the end being betterunderst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> the child. The samenumbers for two children can mean two quitedifferent things. Just as a high temperaturereading can be symptomatic <strong>of</strong> a host <strong>of</strong>disorders from influenza to appendicitis,so, too, a pathognomic behavioral sign canreveal a host <strong>of</strong> possible conditions.One horrendous error <strong>of</strong>ten madewhen reporting test scores is a psychologistreporting a score <strong>and</strong> then saying thatit is invalid. Then why report it (Tallent,


362 chapter 16 report writing1993)? If a test score is invalid, how doesit serve the child to have this score as part<strong>of</strong> a permanent record? Reporting apparentlyinvalid scores is akin to a physicianmaking a diagnostic decision on the basis<strong>of</strong> a fasting blood test when the patientviolated the fasting requirements. In alllikelihood, the flawed results would notbe reported; rather, the patient would berequired to retake the test. We suggestthat one does not have to report scores for a testjust because it was administered. This stanceapplies to scores that are deemed invalid orcircumstances in which the psychometricsunderlying the scores are questionable. Inthese situations, disregarding the informationfrom the measure or providing onlydescriptions <strong>of</strong> the responses may betterinform case conceptualization.Failure to Address ReferralQuestionsTallent (1993) points out that psychologiststoo <strong>of</strong>ten fail to dem<strong>and</strong> clear referral questions,<strong>and</strong> as a result, their reports appearvague <strong>and</strong> unfocused. This very obviouspoint is all too frequently overlooked.Psychologists should insist that referralsources present their questions clearly, <strong>and</strong>if not, the psychologist should meet withthe referring person to obtain further detailon the type <strong>of</strong> information that is expectedfrom the evaluation (Tallent, 1993). Manyagencies use referral forms to assist in thisprocess <strong>of</strong> declaring assessment goals. Aform similar to those used by hospitals isshown in Fig. 16.1, <strong>and</strong> one suitable for useby school systems is given in Fig. 16.2.On occasion, the referral question(s)can be insidious <strong>and</strong>, consequently, placethe psychologist in the position <strong>of</strong> disappointingthe referral source before theevaluation is even initiated. Under thesecircumstances, the psychologist may feelhelpless or even betrayed because <strong>of</strong> thenegative reaction <strong>of</strong> the referral sourceto the presentation <strong>of</strong> results <strong>and</strong> recommendations.Psychologists may <strong>of</strong>ten needto pursue the true referral question. Someexamples <strong>of</strong> stated <strong>and</strong> true referral questionsare shown below:Stated ReferralQuestionA child’s teacherwants to know ifchild has ADHDA parent wantsto know why achild is failing inschoolA psychiatristwants to knowif a child isdepressedA psychologistwants to knowif the child isneurologicallyimpairedTrue Referral QuestionThe teacher is convincedthat the child has ADHD<strong>and</strong> expects the psychologistto confirm itThe parent thinks thechild is depressed <strong>and</strong>would like her to be onmedicationThe psychiatrist has madethe diagnosis <strong>of</strong>depression <strong>and</strong> has placedthe child on medication.The referral was madesimply because a secondopinion is required forreimbursement purposesThe psychologist isseeking a diagnosis <strong>of</strong>traumatic brain injury inorder to bolster her courttestimonyIn all <strong>of</strong> these scenarios, it wouldbehoove the psychologist to clearly determinethe referral source’s actual needs <strong>and</strong>/or desires early on in the referral/evaluationprocess <strong>and</strong> then determine the mostappropriate way to proceed.The Consumer’s ViewVirtually no recent research has beenconducted on the consumer’s view <strong>of</strong> psychologicalreports. One study evaluatedteacher preferences for <strong>and</strong> comprehension<strong>of</strong> varying report formats (Wiener, 1985).This study required a group <strong>of</strong> elementaryschool teachers to read <strong>and</strong> rate their comprehension<strong>of</strong> <strong>and</strong> preferences for threedifferent reports for the same child.


chapter 16 report writing363Patient Name John Doe __________________________________________________________________Medical Record Number 00071103 ____________________ _____________________________________Attending Physician LymanType <strong>of</strong> Consultation:Patient is a 13 year-old with Type I Diabetes who has poor adherence to treatment regimen. Parents areconcerned that John is aware <strong>of</strong> the risks <strong>of</strong> th is poor adherence but seems apathetic. Patien t’s affect isflat <strong>and</strong> may be de pressed. Pati ent to be discharg ed fr om hospital following psych. consul tation.Results <strong>of</strong> Consultation: Patient appears depressed <strong>and</strong> seems knowledgeable about diabetes<strong>and</strong> his diabetes regimen. In particular, his parents noted that he appears sad most <strong>of</strong> the time,lacks energy, has reduced his contact with friends, <strong>and</strong> does not seem interested in activities thathe used to enjoy. Rating scales completed by patient <strong>and</strong> his mother showed moderate levels <strong>of</strong>depression. Family history <strong>of</strong> depression is significant. Outpatient therapy is recommended <strong>and</strong>has been scheduled to begin in 1 week.Signed _____________________________________Title ______________________ DateFigure 16.1Sample referral form consultation used by hospitalsThe three reports used were a shortform, a psychoeducational report, <strong>and</strong> aquestion-<strong>and</strong>-answer format. The shortformreport was one page, single-spaced.It used some jargon, such as acronyms, toshorten length; conclusions were drawnwithout reference to a data source; <strong>and</strong>recommendations were given withoutelaboration. The psychoeducationalreport format was three <strong>and</strong> a half single-spacedpages. It used headings suchas Reason for Referral, Learning Style,Mathematics, Conclusions, <strong>and</strong> Recommendations.Observations were statedin behavioral terms with examples usedfreely. Recommendations were given<strong>and</strong> elaborated, <strong>and</strong> acronyms <strong>and</strong> otherjargon were only used when they weredefined in text. The question-<strong>and</strong>-answerreport was similar to the psychoeducationalreport in many ways, but it did notuse headings per se. This report listedreferral questions <strong>and</strong> then answeredeach question in turn. This report wasfour <strong>and</strong> a half pages long.Amazingly, in this study, the participantspreferred length. First, teacherscomprehended the two longer reportsbetter. Second, <strong>of</strong> the two longer reports,the teachers preferred the question-<strong>and</strong>-


364 chapter 16 report writingStudent's Name Jane Smith_____________________________ Date <strong>of</strong> Referral 10/11/07Referring School Stuart Elem.______ Age 8 _ Grade 2 _____ Grades Repeated N/AIs the student now receiving speech therapy? ___ Yes x noNever Sometimes OftenExpressive Language (problems in grammar, limited vocabulary)Receptive Language (comprehension not following directions)Speech (poor enunciation, lisps, stutters, omits sounds, infantile speech)xxxGross Motor Coordination (eye-h<strong>and</strong>, manual dexterity)xVisual (cannot see blackboard, squints, rubs eyes, holds book too close) xHearing (unable to discriminate sounds, asks to have instructionsrepeated, turns ear to speaker, <strong>of</strong>ten has earaches)Health (example: epilepsy, respiratory problems, etc.)xxMedications (yes) (no) (Type)Overly energetic, talks out, out <strong>of</strong> seat: SometimesVery quiet, uncommunicative: OftenActing out (aggressive, hostile, rebellious, destructive, cries easily): SometimesInattentive (short attention span, poor on task behavior): OftenDoesn ' t appear to notice what is happening in the immediateenvironment:SometimesPoor Peer Relationships (few friends, rejected, ignored, abused by peers): OftenACADEMIC PROBLEMSReading (word attack, comprehension): OftenWriting (illegible, reverses letters, doesn't write): OftenSpelling (cannot spell phonetically, omits or adds letters): OftenMathematics (computation, concepts, application): NeverSocial Science, Sciences (doesn't h<strong>and</strong>le concepts, doesn't underst<strong>and</strong>rerelationships, poor underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> cause <strong>and</strong> effect): NeverSignature <strong>and</strong> position <strong>of</strong> referring personFigure 16.2Referral for consultation used by schoolsanswer report over the psychoeducationalreport. The short form was clearly leastpreferred. These are intriguing results inthat they hint that length may be overratedas a problem in report writing <strong>and</strong> thatteachers may prefer a question-<strong>and</strong>-answerreport format. This finding is interestingbecause this format is rarely used in reportsfrom clinical assessments.Do parents have different preferencesfrom teachers? In a follow-up study withparents using the same methodology, Wiener<strong>and</strong> Kohler (1986) found that teachers<strong>and</strong> parents have similar preferences. Inthis second study, the same three reportformats were used. As was the case withteachers, parents comprehended the twolonger reports significantly better than


chapter 16 report writing365the short-form report. An interestingadditional finding was that parents with acollege education comprehended reportsbetter than parents with only a high schooldiploma. Parents also tended to prefer thequestion-<strong>and</strong>-answer format to the othertwo formats, although the difference inpreference scores between the psychoeducational<strong>and</strong> question-<strong>and</strong>-answer reportsfailed to reach statistical significance.The results <strong>of</strong> these two studies suggestthat the two most frequent consumers <strong>of</strong>child <strong>and</strong> adolescent psychological reports,parents <strong>and</strong> teachers, consider the clarity<strong>of</strong> reports to be more important than theirabsolute length. They also show a preferencefor reports that have referral questionsas their focus. Cognizance <strong>of</strong> thesetwo findings may benefit psychologists whowrite reports for children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.Suggested PracticesReport Only PertinentInformationOne <strong>of</strong> the most difficult decisions to makewhen writing a report involves gauging therelevance <strong>of</strong> information to include (Teglasi,1983). Clinicians happen onto a great deal <strong>of</strong>information during the course <strong>of</strong> an evaluation,some <strong>of</strong> which is tangential. Say, forexample, a child is referred for an evaluation<strong>of</strong> ADHD. During the course <strong>of</strong> an interviewwith the child’s father, he recounts at lengthhis disappointment with his wife. He tells theclinician that she is dating other men, <strong>and</strong> hebelieves that she is not spending adequatetime with their children. When writing thereport on this case, the clinician has to determinewhether or not this information is pertinentto the ADHD evaluation.Clinicians must think critically aboutthe in-formation that they include inreports <strong>and</strong> consider its relevance to thecase. As discussed by Lichtenberger et al.(2004), the objectives <strong>of</strong> psychologicalreports are to “answer the referral questions;describe the person; organize thedata; <strong>and</strong> recommend interventions” (p. 3).If information is not relevant to theseobjectives, <strong>and</strong> it is very personal, the psychologistshould consider carefully thedecision to invade a family’s privacy byincluding such information in the report.Define Abbreviations <strong>and</strong>AcronymsAcronyms are part <strong>of</strong> the idiosyncraticlanguage <strong>of</strong> psychological assessment.They can greatly facilitate communicationamong psychologists, but theyhinder communication with non-psychologists.Psychologists, just like otherpr<strong>of</strong>essionals, need to use nontechnicallanguage to communicate with parents,teachers, <strong>and</strong> other colleagues in themental health field. A pediatrician wouldnot ask a mother if her child had an emesis;rather, the physician would inquirewhether or not the child vomited.When writing a report, psychologistsshould limit their use <strong>of</strong> acronyms<strong>and</strong> should define any acronym used in areport. Use <strong>of</strong> the acronym SAD for separationanxiety disorder, for example, withoutdefining, is questionable practice.Emphasize Words Ratherthan NumbersParticularly in the test results section <strong>of</strong> areport, clinicians must resist a temptationto focus exclusively on numbers (i.e., testscores). Lichtenberger et al. noted that“some evaluators spend too much timewriting about the obtained test scoresrather than about what these scores mean”(p. 5). Words <strong>of</strong>ten communicate moreeffectively than numbers because theycommunicate more directly <strong>and</strong> in a moreaccessible manner to a variety <strong>of</strong> audiences.The typical question <strong>of</strong> a referral source has


366 chapter 16 report writingnothing to do with the obtained T-scoresbut, rather, the psychologist’s interpretation<strong>of</strong> these scores. Most laypersons willnot underst<strong>and</strong> the T-score metric but canmore easily grasp clinician’s interpretation<strong>of</strong> norm-referenced scores.Reduce Difficult WordsThe issue <strong>of</strong> using simple language isby now obvious. The difficult part forreport writers is following through onthis advice. Consider the following twoparagraphs, which differ greatly. The firstexcerpt uses vocabulary that is unnecessarilycomplex for most consumers <strong>of</strong>reports. The second example is a rewrite<strong>of</strong> the first paragraph that uses a morepractical vocabulary level.There is also evidence from the test data to suggestthat Pam is obdurate in response to anxiety.She may also tend to be very concrete <strong>and</strong> notnotice some <strong>of</strong> the subtleties <strong>of</strong> interpersonaldiscourse. Given these idiosyncrasies, shemay find it difficult to generate effective socialproblem-solving strategies <strong>and</strong> mechanisms forcoping with life’s stressors.The next paragraph tries to communicatemore clearly by using, among other things,simpler language.Pam responds to stress by withdrawing fromothers (e.g., going to her room or leaving agroup <strong>of</strong> friends on a social outing), whichseems to be the only method she uses for dealingwith stress. She also has trouble underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>and</strong> responding to messages given byothers in social situations (e.g., body languageor verbal hints). Because <strong>of</strong> these behavior patterns,Pam has trouble making friends.Related to the use <strong>of</strong> difficult words is theissue <strong>of</strong> using the correct person. We haveoccasionally seen reports where instead<strong>of</strong> using the child’s name, he or she wasreferred to as “the child” or “the subject.”This usage sounds too mechanistic <strong>and</strong>impersonal for a psychological report.In most cases, the use <strong>of</strong> the child’s name isbetter. It is important to also clearly differentiateamong sources <strong>of</strong> information <strong>and</strong>between data gathered during the assessment<strong>and</strong> the clinician’s interpretations.Jargon or convoluted writing makes theseimportant distinctions difficult to be madeout by the reader.Briefly Describe theInstruments UsedIn many cases, it is safe to assume that thereader <strong>of</strong> the report has little knowledge <strong>of</strong>the tests being used. When practical, we suggestthat report writers describe the nature<strong>of</strong> the assessment devices being used.The naive reader <strong>of</strong> a report will also behelped by descriptors <strong>of</strong> the nature <strong>of</strong> a scaleor subscale that is being discussed. Thisobservation is particularly true for scalesthat are not adequately described by theirnames. Depression scales are a good example<strong>of</strong> scales that may be perceived inappropriately.The label depression could conjureup a variety <strong>of</strong> images in a report reader’smind including the image <strong>of</strong> a child that isincapacitated by sadness. It may well be thata Depression score indicates significancebut, de-pending on the items endorsed, maynot warrant the formal diagnosis <strong>of</strong> depression.In this case, the clinician should try todescribe the nature <strong>of</strong> the scale content <strong>and</strong>/or its interpretive meaning in order to discouragemisuse <strong>of</strong> results.Edit the ReportWe have found that a number <strong>of</strong> ourstudents do not take a critical eye towardediting their own work <strong>and</strong> not just interms <strong>of</strong> grammar <strong>and</strong> spelling. Editingis necessary to ensure the most accuratecommunication in the least amount <strong>of</strong> space.


chapter 16 report writing367Tallent (1988) provides the followingexcellent example <strong>of</strong> how an editor (<strong>and</strong>the articulate psychologist) thinks:There is the tale <strong>of</strong> the young man who went intothe fish business. He rented a store, erected asign, FRESH FISH SOLD HERE, <strong>and</strong> acquiredmerch<strong>and</strong>ise. As he was st<strong>and</strong>ing back admiringhis market <strong>and</strong> his sign, a friend happened along.Following congratulations, the friend gazed at thesign <strong>and</strong> read aloud, FRESH FISH SOLD HERE.Of course it’s here. You wouldn’t sell it elsewhere,would you? Impressed with such astuteness, theyoung man painted over the obviously superfluousword. The next helpful comment had to dowith the word sold. You aren’t giving it away.Again impressed, he eliminated the useless word.Seemingly that was it, but the critic then focusedon the word fresh. You wouldn’t sell stale fish,would you? Once more our hero bowed to thestrength <strong>of</strong> logic. But finally he was relieved thathe had a logic-tight sign for his business; FISH.His ever alert friend, however, audibly sniffingthe air for effect, made a final observation: Youdon’t need a sign (p. 88).Psychologists do not need to engage in suchsevere editing, but they should at least makean attempt to think critically about their wordusage in order to reduce report, sentence,<strong>and</strong> paragraph length. Judicious editing cango a long way toward clarifying meaning in areport. Sometimes new clinicians are not usedto critiquing their own writing. One readilyavailable option is to have a colleague readreports. Confidentiality, however, should bekept in mind if an editor is used.Use Headings <strong>and</strong> Lists FreelyHeadings <strong>and</strong> lists can enhance the clarity<strong>of</strong> communication (Harvey, 1989). If,for example, a clinician draws a number <strong>of</strong>conclusions about a child, the conclusionscan sometimes lose their impact if they areembedded in paragraphs.As one would predict, the use <strong>of</strong> headings<strong>and</strong> lists to excess has a downside. A reportthat uses too many lists, for example, appearsstilted, <strong>and</strong> it may not communicate all <strong>of</strong>the texture <strong>and</strong> subtleties <strong>of</strong> the child’s performance.Report writers should considerusing additional headings if a section <strong>of</strong> theirreport stretches for nearly a page (singlespaced)without a heading. Clinicians shouldconsider lists if they want to add impact tostatements <strong>and</strong>/or conclusions.Use Examples <strong>of</strong> Behaviorto Clarify MeaningBecause there is some disagreement regardingthe meanings <strong>of</strong> particular words,report writers should clarify their meaningin order to ensure accuracy. Words thatmay conjure up a variety <strong>of</strong> interpretationsinclude anxiety, cooperation, dependent, hyperactive,<strong>and</strong> low self-esteem. One way to fosterclarity is to use examples (i.e., behavioralreferents) <strong>of</strong> the child’s behavior. Here, forexample, are two ways to say that a child,Emilio, was anxious.Emilio exhibited considerable anxiety duringthe testing.Or, alternatively:Emilio appeared anxious during the testing. Hefrequently asked whether or not he had solvedan item correctly. He occasionally looked atthe ticking stopwatch during an item <strong>and</strong> thenhurried, <strong>and</strong> his face became flushed when itwas obvious to him that he did not know theanswer to a question.An additional benefit <strong>of</strong> using examples<strong>of</strong> behavior generously is that it forces thepsychologist to consider the extent <strong>of</strong> supportingevidence for a conclusion abouta child’s behavior. If a psychologist writesthat a child is anxious but cannot think <strong>of</strong>behaviors to help explain this, then theconclusion should not be drawn, as it isinsupportable by evidence.Direct quotes, to some extent, are alsohelpful for clarifying meaning. If a clinician


368 chapter 16 report writingconcludes that an adolescent is suicidal, aquote from the child may help clarify thisstatement considerably. The child mayhave said, “I thought about taking somepills once” or “I feel like I want to run outin front <strong>of</strong> a car tonight <strong>and</strong> if that doesn’twork, I will steal my father’s gun <strong>and</strong> killmyself.” These statements convey varyingdegrees <strong>of</strong> suicidal intent that are mostclearly differentiated by quotes.Reduce Report LengthTallent (1988) gives the following instancesas indications <strong>of</strong> undue length:l The psychologist is concerned that ittook too long to write it (we might addthat most reports will seem that way forbeginning clinicians, but the time to writereports should decline with experience).l The psychologist has difficulty organizingall <strong>of</strong> the details for presentation.l Some <strong>of</strong> the content is not clear or useful.l The detail is much greater than can beput to good use.l Speculations are presented without agood rationale for them.l The writing is unnecessarily repetitious.l The organization is not tight.l The reader is irritated by the length orreads only a few sections such as the Summaryor Recommendations sections.The issue <strong>of</strong> length is primarily a concern<strong>of</strong> other psychologists, <strong>and</strong> it is intertwinedwith other issues, such as clarity. Hence,the psychologist in training should notassume that shorter is better. Quality maybe a more important issue than quantity.At this early point in training, the newreport writer should keep the issue <strong>of</strong>length in mind while writing reports. Concernsabout length, however, should neverinterfere with the need to portray a child’sperformance accurately.Check ScoresAn all-too-frequent <strong>and</strong> grievous error is tore-port scores that are incorrect. Computerizedscoring represents a breakthroughthat limits errors. In fact, if the facilities areavailable, we suggest that each test protocolthat is scored by h<strong>and</strong> be checked againstcomputer scoring. If this is not possible,the test scores should at least be doublecheckedprior to finalizing a report.One way <strong>of</strong> checking scores is to bealert to in-consistencies. If, for example, anadjudicated adolescent who was referredfor conduct problems obtains an elevatedT-score on depression measures <strong>and</strong> no elevationson conduct problem scales, then thescore should be double-checked to see if ascoring error is the source <strong>of</strong> the incongruity.If a score doesn’t seem sensible, then theclinician should always check for a scoringerror in order to rule out this possibility.Check Grammar <strong>and</strong> SpellingAnother problem with reports that detractsfrom the credibility <strong>of</strong> the clinician is thepresence <strong>of</strong> spelling errors. Clinicians arestrongly advised to take the time to electronically<strong>and</strong> visually check their spelling<strong>and</strong> grammar.Adapting Reports toAudience <strong>and</strong> SettingThere is probably no optimal report format.Psychologists <strong>of</strong>ten find that theyhave to adapt their reports to meet theneeds <strong>of</strong> an ever-changing audience. Audienceshave varying characteristics, such asliteracy levels, <strong>and</strong>, more importantly, theyhave differing referral questions.In a school setting, many referrals are forlearning problems. Teachers may also beseeking information to assist them in curriculumdecisions. These are very different


chapter 16 report writing369referral questions than those that may be <strong>of</strong>interest in other settings. In a psychiatrichospital setting, issues such as suicide potential,safety, <strong>and</strong> coping strategies may be <strong>of</strong>greater concern. These questions are verydifferent than those <strong>of</strong> the school setting,requiring a focus on topics such as diagnosis<strong>and</strong> implications for pharmacological treatment.Parents are yet another audience withspecific questions. When conducting anevaluation for parents in a private practicesetting, the emphasis may be on advisingthe parents on what they can do to effectchange in their child’s behavior.The report excerpts used throughoutthis book were taken from a variety <strong>of</strong> settingswith differing referral questions. Thereader is advised to think carefully aboutthe needs <strong>of</strong> referral sources when readingthese examples <strong>and</strong> writing reports.Referral QuestionsThis section is crucial because the referralquestions dictate the design <strong>of</strong> the evaluation.This section is <strong>of</strong>ten brief but shouldbe descriptive so that the purpose <strong>of</strong> theevaluation is clear. The referral sourceshould also be stated (Lichtenberger et al.,2004). The lack <strong>of</strong> clear referral questionsmay lead to consumer or referral sourcedissatisfaction with the report. As notedpreviously, psychologists may have to speakmore than once with the referral source toclarify the nature <strong>of</strong> the question(s). Thereferral questions should be stated in terms<strong>of</strong> specific examples <strong>of</strong> the child’s difficultiesrather than general labels (e.g., “hyperactivity,”“academic problems,” “anxiety”).This section may also indicate (briefly) theduration, severity, <strong>and</strong>/or frequency <strong>of</strong> theproblem.The Sections<strong>of</strong> the PsychologicalReportIdentifying InformationMost report formats provide some identifyingin-formation on the top <strong>of</strong> thefirst page <strong>of</strong> the re-port. This section caninclude information such as name <strong>of</strong> thechild, age, grade, birth date, <strong>and</strong> perhapsthe name <strong>of</strong> the school or agency wherethe child is currently attending or beingserved. Also, most reports indicate that thereport con-tent is confidential.<strong>Assessment</strong> ProceduresThis section typically lists the assessmentmethods (both quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative)<strong>and</strong> tests that were used in the evaluation.Evaluation procedures can, <strong>and</strong> frequentlydo, include inter-views, reviews <strong>of</strong> records,<strong>and</strong> classroom or other observations.Background InformationThis section should include all <strong>of</strong> the pertinentinformation that may affect interpretation<strong>of</strong> a child’s scores. The key word hereis pertinent. The clinician should report onlyinformation that is relevant to the currentevaluation, not in-formation that is superfluousor an undue invasion <strong>of</strong> privacy (Teglasi,1983). Material should only be included if ithas some potential impact on the interpretation<strong>of</strong> the child’s scores in order to answerthe referral question(s). While parentaloccupation <strong>and</strong> marital status are generallyprivate subjects, these may be importantpieces <strong>of</strong> information, given what is currentlyknown about the effects <strong>of</strong> parentalvariables on child functioning. Lichentengeret al. (2004) provide a user-friendly <strong>and</strong> sensiblesummary <strong>of</strong> the types <strong>of</strong> informationto include in this section, as well as tips toprovide the information clearly.The report writer should also be clearabout the sources <strong>of</strong> information. If thefather views his son as lazy, then this statementshould be attributed to the father.


370 chapter 16 report writingStatements that could be used for makingsuch attributions include the following:According to…His father/mother stated…His mother’/father’s opinion is…His teacher’s view <strong>of</strong> the situationHer guidance counselor reported that…His parole <strong>of</strong>ficer acknowledges that…If care is not taken to make clear the sources<strong>of</strong> information, questions may arise at thetime when feedback is given to involvedparties.Sensitive background informationshould also be corroborated or excludedfrom the report if it is inflammatory <strong>and</strong>cannot be corroborated. For example, a5-year-old may say something like “Mymother shoots people,” <strong>and</strong> later, the psychologistdiscovers that the child’s motheris a police <strong>of</strong>ficer.Previous assessment results should alsobe included in this section (Teglasi, 1983).Also, previous experiences with psychologicalor educational interventions shouldbe noted here. The clinician may alsorefer the reader to a previous evaluation.Referring to previous evaluations, withoutfully recapitulating them, can substantiallyreduce written report bulk.Behavioral ObservationsIn this section, the behaviors that the childexhibits during the assessment are recorded.When writing this section, the number <strong>of</strong>observations made, the setting where theobservations were made (e.g., school, clinic,etc.), <strong>and</strong> the person who made the observationsshould be identified (Teglasi, 1983). Abrief description <strong>of</strong> the setting, particularlyif the report writer is describing classroomobservations, is also appropriate. Domainsthat routinely should be covered include“physical appearance, ease <strong>of</strong> establishing<strong>and</strong> maintaining rapport, response to failures/successes,response to encouragement,attention span, language style, distractibility,activity level, anxiety level, mood, impulsivity/reflectivity,problem-solving strategy,attitude toward the testing process, attitudetoward examiner, attitude toward self,unusual mannerisms or habits <strong>and</strong> validity<strong>of</strong> test results in view <strong>of</strong> behaviors” (Lichtenbergeret al., 2004, p. 60).Care should be taken not to confuseobservations with interpretations. In otherwords, it is appropriate, for example, tostate that the child appeared motivated toperform well, but such a statement shouldbe accompanied by the behaviors that ledto this assertion.<strong>Assessment</strong> Results<strong>and</strong> InterpretationThis section is where the test results for thechild are reported. Some report writers preferto integrate the results from various measuresinto a single section. Still others opt todivide this section into subsections accordingto domains assessed. The domains mayinclude: cognitive/intellectual, academicachievement, adaptive behavior, visual/motor, <strong>and</strong> behavioral/personality. This lattersection is <strong>of</strong> primary interest for this text.Organization within the behavioral/personality section can be according totheoretical orientation, training, or otherpreferences <strong>of</strong> the psychologist. We happento recommend that this section beorganized from the most important constructto least important, such that allevidence from multiple tools regardingthe most important domain <strong>of</strong> functioningfor the client (e.g., depression) is discussedfirst, followed by other comorbidissues, rather than presenting informationby each individual measure <strong>and</strong> then tryingagain to integrate the information fromvaried sources. This approach puts thefocus, in our view, where it belongs: theconstructs/domains <strong>of</strong> functioning, not thetests. Most importantly, this section shouldprovide coherent interpretations <strong>of</strong> resultsthat relate logically to one another <strong>and</strong> to


chapter 16 report writing371other sections, such as sections devotedto providing diagnostic considerations.Hence, this section should not simplyreport numerical findings that are devoid<strong>of</strong> interpretation.Diagnostic ConsiderationsThe decision about whether or not toinclude a separate portion dealing withdiagnostic issues is likely influenced by setting<strong>and</strong> referral questions. Nonmedicalsettings, for example, may discourage theinclusion <strong>of</strong> a discussion <strong>of</strong> this nature inthe psychologist’s report. The omission <strong>of</strong>such a section may be in keeping with interdisciplinaryapproaches to making classification/diagnostic/eligibilitydecisions.The format for this section can be inlists or in paragraphs. A psychologist maysimply list diagnoses in a manner consistentwith the DSM-IV multiaxial approach.Others prefer to use a paragraph or twoto more fully explain the rationale for oragainst making certain diagnoses.SummaryThe final section <strong>of</strong> the report is intended togive an overview <strong>of</strong> the major findings. Thisreview helps to ensure that the reader underst<strong>and</strong>sthe major points made in the report.A rule <strong>of</strong> thumb for writing summaries is touse one sentence to summarize each section<strong>of</strong> the report. In addition, a sentence shouldbe devoted to each major finding presentedin the test results section. In some cases, onesentence can be used to summarize multiplefindings <strong>and</strong> recommendations.One <strong>of</strong> the common pitfalls <strong>of</strong> preparingsummaries is including new information inthe summary section. If a clinician introducesa new finding in the summary, the reader islost. The reader has no idea as to the sourceor rationale behind the conclusion. We suggestthat students read their draft summariescarefully <strong>and</strong> check every conclusion made inthe summary against the body <strong>of</strong> the report.SignaturesReports typically require signatures attestingto their authenticity. An important component<strong>of</strong> this seemingly unimportant aspect<strong>of</strong> the report is the necessity for cliniciansto use titles that represent them accurately.Some states, for ex-ample, do not have specialtylicensure, <strong>and</strong> the use <strong>of</strong> a title such asLicensed Pediatric Psychologist is not appropriate.In this case, a more generic term suchas Licensed Psychologist should be used,especially if the psychologist lacks evidence<strong>of</strong> board certification <strong>of</strong> specialty training.Students should also be careful to representthemselves accurately. A title suchas Practicum Student, Intern, Trainee, orsomething similar should be used. Psychologicalcustom also dictates the inclusion <strong>of</strong>the highest degree obtained by the clinician.RecommendationsRecommendations should be specific <strong>and</strong>clear (Teglasi, 1983). A recommendationfor individual psychotherapy may be difficultto carry out, for example, if the specificproblems that need to be addressed<strong>and</strong> other aspects <strong>of</strong> the recommendationare not made explicit. Some reasons thatrecommendations are not subsequentlyfollowed may have to do with how they arecommunicated. Recommendations shouldbe understood by the individuals who willimplement them, developmentally appropriatefor the child, <strong>and</strong> practical, <strong>and</strong>should avoid being unnecessarily complex(Lichtenberger et al., 2004).Some recommendations may also bedifficult to communicate succinctly inwriting. Therefore, one approach may beto include h<strong>and</strong>-outs for treating certainproblems that are much more specificthan can be included in the typical recommendationsection <strong>of</strong> a report. A h<strong>and</strong>outdetailing some specific recommendationsfor a teacher responding to inattentivebehaviors in the classroom may be morevaluable to the teacher than an abbreviated


372 chapter 16 report writingrecommendation. In almost all cases, theclinician should relay recommendations inperson to psychiatrists, teachers, parents,<strong>and</strong> other colleagues (Teglasi, 1983) inorder to ensure that they are followed.Psychometric SummarySome clinicians include a listing <strong>of</strong> all <strong>of</strong>the child’s obtained scores with the report.While this summary will be <strong>of</strong> limitedvalue to the less knowledgeable reader, itmay be <strong>of</strong> great value to another clinicianwho reviews the report. This summaryis best placed on a separate sheet(s)<strong>of</strong> paper, which makes it convenient forthe clinician to be selective about whoreceives the summary. Some psychologistsmay prefer to not send the summaryto parents <strong>and</strong> virtually always send it toother psychologists.The Report Writing Self-TestA report-writing self-test is provided in Fig.16.3. This checklist allows the psychologistto periodically <strong>and</strong> quickly review principles<strong>of</strong> re-port writing.Item True False1. Was the report edited?TF2. Are unnecessary invasions <strong>of</strong> privacy avoided?TF3. Is the referral question(s) explicitly stated?TF4. Is the referral question answered?TF5. Does the report emphasize words over numbers?TF6. Can a person with a high school education underst<strong>and</strong> the wording used? TF7. Is the report brief enough that major findings are not lost?TF8. Arethe conclusions drawn without undue hedging?TF9. Do the conclusions fit the data?TF10. Are invalid results omitted?TF11. Are percentile ranks included for the benefit <strong>of</strong> parents <strong>and</strong> clients?TF12. Were spelling <strong>and</strong> grammar checked?TF13. Aresupporting data integrated with conclusions?TF14. Are the recommendations clear <strong>and</strong> specific?TF15. Are headings <strong>and</strong> lists added to enhance space?TF16. Are acronyms defined <strong>and</strong> not overused?TF17. Is the summary free <strong>of</strong> new information?TF18. Were scores double-checked?TF19. Are examples <strong>of</strong> behavior used to clarify meaning?TF20. Are test instruments described adequately?TF21. Is the rationale for diagnoses provided?TF22. Is a conference scheduled to accompany the written report?23. Was written parental consent obtained prior to releasing the reportto interested agencies or parties?TTFF24. Is a feedback session scheduled with the child or adolescent?TF25. Are the type <strong>and</strong> paper <strong>of</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>essional quality (e.g., laser-quality print)? TFFigure 16.3Report writing self-test


chapter 16 report writing373Communicating ResultsOrallyParent ConferencesFor the purposes <strong>of</strong> this text, parent is usedgenerically to include any consistent caregiverin the child’s life. Examples <strong>of</strong> suchcaregivers include stepparents, residentialcaretakers, <strong>and</strong> gr<strong>and</strong>parents, among others.Imparting assessment results to parentsre-quires considerable savvy, as the individualdifferences between families aremyriad. Because <strong>of</strong> this diversity, there isnot a singular methodology that will beeffective with all parents. This section willpresent some ideas for sharing results withparents or other caregivers. However, it isvital for the psychologist to remain flexiblein order to adapt the format <strong>of</strong> the feedbacksession to the needs <strong>of</strong> the parent, othercaregiver, or family, as well as the setting.In an old but insightful article, Ricks(1959) summarized the heart <strong>of</strong> the parentconference dilemma.The audience <strong>of</strong> parents to which our testbasedinformation is to be transmitted includesan enormous range <strong>and</strong> variety <strong>of</strong> minds <strong>and</strong>emotions. Some are ready <strong>and</strong> able to absorbwhat we have to say. Reaching others may beas hopeless as reaching watchers with an AMradio broadcast. Still others may hear what wesay, but clothe the message with their own specialneeds, ideas, <strong>and</strong> predilections (p. 4).Regardless <strong>of</strong> the potential pitfalls, parentsmust be informed <strong>of</strong> the results <strong>of</strong> apsychological evaluation <strong>of</strong> their child (thelegal, ethical, <strong>and</strong> regulatory m<strong>and</strong>ates forthis practice are given in Chapter 4).Some helpful suggestions for communicatingtest results to parents are given next:1. Avoid excessive hedging or deceit. Theproblem with hedging or failing toreport bad news is that many parentssense this deceit <strong>and</strong> respond to thepsychologist with appropriate mistrust.Honesty is also easily sensed by parents,which ultimately enhances the credibility<strong>of</strong> the psychologist.2. Use percentile ranks heavily whendescribing norm-referenced test results.This metric is easier for parents tounderst<strong>and</strong> than other norm-referencedscores.3. Instead <strong>of</strong> lecturing, allow parentsopportunities to participate by askingabout topics such as their opinion <strong>of</strong>the results <strong>and</strong> how they fit with theirknowledge <strong>of</strong> their child. Moreover,listening carefully to parents helps thepsychologist determine the psychologicalneeds <strong>of</strong> the parents that are relevantto the evaluation. Similarly, it is essentialthat the parents be given frequentopportunities to ask questions (Lichtenbergeret al., 2004).4. Anticipate questions prior to the interview<strong>and</strong> prepare responses. How woulda psychologist answer the question, “Willmy daughter outgrow her ADHD?”Psychologists can gauge the probabilitythat such questions will arise by listeningcarefully in the intake interview <strong>and</strong>throughout the assessment process.5. Schedule adequate time for the interview.Parent conferences <strong>of</strong>ten becomemore involved than one has planned.Adequate time allows the psychologisttime to use counseling skills to bringa parent feedback conference to adequateclosure. Ideally, 1–2 h could beallocated for such a parent session. If asession ends early, then the psychologistis the recipient <strong>of</strong> a precious gift– extra time.6. It is <strong>of</strong>ten helpful to seek practice communicatingwith parents from a variety<strong>of</strong> back-grounds. Some parents can beaddressed as if they are colleagues, whileothers may have only a limited grasp<strong>of</strong> the issues being discussed. Translators,ministers, teachers, trusted family


374 chapter 16 report writingfriends, <strong>and</strong> others may serve as allies inthe feedback process.7. Avoid questionable <strong>and</strong>/or overlyexplicit predictions (Kamphaus, 2001).Phrases to be avoided would be statementslike, “She will never go to college,”or “She will always have troublewith school.” These types <strong>of</strong> statementscan be <strong>of</strong>fensive to parents, not to mentioninaccurate.8. Use good, basic counseling skills. Everyparent likes to talk about the trials <strong>and</strong>successes <strong>of</strong> raising a child. Give parentsat least some opportunity to do this, as itallows you to show interest in the childby listening to the parent’s perspective.9. Do not engage in counseling that isbeyond your level <strong>of</strong> expertise. Parentsare <strong>of</strong>ten very eager to obtain advice froma pr<strong>of</strong>essional. It is inappropriate (<strong>and</strong>unethical by most stan-dards) for a psychologistto provide services for whichhe or she is not trained. If, for ex-ample,a parent requests marital counseling<strong>and</strong> you have no training in this area,you should inform the parent <strong>of</strong> thisfact <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>fer a referral. In fact, the psychologistis wise to have referral sourcesreadily available for such eventualities.10. Be aware that some parents are notready to accept some test results. Parentsmay impugn your skills becausethey cannot accept the fact that theirchild has a severe h<strong>and</strong>icap. They mayleave the session angry, <strong>and</strong> you mayfeel inept. The idea that every parentconference will end on a happy note isunrealistic. Examine your skills criticallyin response to parent feedback,but realize that some parents simplywill not accept the results because <strong>of</strong>their own personal issues. An example<strong>of</strong> such a situation may involve a parentwith the same h<strong>and</strong>icapping conditionas the child. If a parent was labeledh<strong>and</strong>icapped <strong>and</strong> ridiculed by peers, heor she may become defensive <strong>and</strong> angryat the suggestion that his or her childmay have a h<strong>and</strong>icap. The session withsuch a parent will likely end on a tensenote. In many <strong>of</strong> these cases, however,the parent will adapt <strong>and</strong> accept thenews after developing the psychologicalresources to cope with the attendantstresses. The psychologist mayfind this same parent to interact morepositively in the next encounter.11. Maintain a positive tone throughoutthe session <strong>and</strong> discuss the child’sstrengths <strong>and</strong> competencies.Teacher ConferencesMany <strong>of</strong> the principles used in parentconferences also apply to teachers. Severalnuances, however, will be outlined in thefollowing suggestions:1. Do not monopolize a teacher’s breakfrom teaching. Some teachers get fewbreaks in a day. Most get a brief lunch,when they prefer to unwind with colleagues<strong>and</strong> prepare for the remainder<strong>of</strong> the day. A clinician is unlikely to comm<strong>and</strong>a teacher’s undivided attentionduring such breaks. If a teacher has anadditional free period, it may be a goodtime for a conference. After school isfrequently the best time to get a teacher’sundivided attention for a meeting.Teachers are generally very busy people,so the pace <strong>of</strong> the meeting will bequicker than is the case for parents.2. Teachers are interested in schoolingissues. The diagnosis <strong>of</strong> Conduct Disorderis <strong>of</strong> less concern to teachersthan getting specific recommendationsfor helping the child in the classroom(Teglasi, 1983). If a psychologist is nottrained <strong>and</strong>/or has little experience inteacher consultation, the assistance <strong>of</strong>someone like a qualified school psychologistshould be enlisted to assist withthe teacher conference.


chapter 16 report writing375In any assessment with a school-agedchild, the clinician should be prepared toconduct a teacher conference, or at thevery least be available to answer any questions<strong>and</strong> facilitate the implementation<strong>of</strong> classroom-based interventions. Such aconference is desirable because teachersare usually involved somehow in the treatment<strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.<strong>Child</strong> FeedbackProviding assessment feedback to a childis <strong>of</strong>ten overlooked, but it is important todo so in most cases because the child willlikely begin some interventions or experiencesome changes in his or her environmentdirectly related to the assessment.The major decision that a clinician needsto make before giving feedback to a childregards the type <strong>of</strong> information that isappropriate for a child’s developmentallevel. Clearly, the kind <strong>of</strong> feedback given toparents is inappropriate for a 5- or 6-yearold,who may have extraordinary difficultyunderst<strong>and</strong>ing the concept <strong>of</strong> a percentilerank. A child this age, however, may beable to underst<strong>and</strong> the consequences <strong>of</strong>the evaluation. In this situation, the childmay be able to underst<strong>and</strong> something like:“Remember those tests I gave you? Well,some <strong>of</strong> them seemed hard for you. Because<strong>of</strong> this, I suggested to your parents that yoube helped after school. So now, you will begoing to visit a teacher after school whowill help you with schoolwork.”The older the child, the more similarthe feedback session becomes to the onefor parents. One dramatic difference,however, is that negative feedback to achild or adolescent can have the opposite<strong>of</strong> the intended effect. That is, in mostcases, the goal is to improve variables suchas peer-related social skills. A child who istold that he or she has poor social skillsmay decide to stop trying to interact withpeers. In some cases, the clinician’s honestycould harm the child. A few optionsare available in cases where a clinician isconcerned about such negative consequences.One option is to have someonewho knows the child well <strong>and</strong> has a positiverelationship with him or her help thepsychologist communicate the results in anon-threatening way to the child. A goodperson to fill this role is a teacher, otherpr<strong>of</strong>essional caregiver, or possibly a parent.A second possibility, if applicable, isto have the child’s primary therapist orcounselor eventually share the results withthe child in a counseling session, when heor she could help the child cope with theresults in a supportive setting.In most cases involving feedback tochildren or adolescents, it is advisable toconsult with a fellow pr<strong>of</strong>essional (e.g.,teacher, counselor, speech therapist, etc.)who knows the child extremely well. Thiscolleague can help the psychologist gaugethe ability <strong>of</strong> the child to deal appropriatelywith the assessment results <strong>and</strong> associatedinterventions.ConclusionsReport writing <strong>and</strong> oral reporting are central,not ancillary, considerations in the assessmentprocess. The most insightful <strong>and</strong> elegant<strong>of</strong> evaluations is lost if not translatedto usable information in written reports<strong>and</strong> intervention planning meetings.Unfortunately, these central assessmentskills are under-emphasized in the training<strong>of</strong> clinicians who are left to acquire theseskills through trial-<strong>and</strong>- error. Cliniciansare advised to seek out expert supervisionin this area, if it is not readily <strong>of</strong>fered. Inaddition, enlisting the aid <strong>of</strong> a competenteditor can markedly enhance the quality<strong>of</strong> written work. Writing is not easy. Writingskills, however, can be acquired <strong>and</strong>improved with diligence <strong>and</strong> patience.


376 chapter 16 report writingChapter Summary1. Psychological reports are frequently madeavailable to parents, judges, lawyers, <strong>and</strong>other non-psychologists, creating theopportunity for improper interpretation<strong>of</strong> the results by untrained individuals.2. Psychological reports can be useful toother clinicians who evaluate a childwho has previously been seen by a psychologist.3. Different audiences require differenttypes <strong>of</strong> written reports.4. Some <strong>of</strong> the common problems withreport writing include the following:(a) Vocabulary problems(b) Faulty interpretation(c) Report length(d) A number emphasis(e) Failure to address referral questions5. Some research has shown that teachersprefer a question-<strong>and</strong>-answer reportformat.6. Parents also tend to prefer a question<strong>and</strong>-answerformat to other formats,although the difference in preferencescores between the psychoeducational <strong>and</strong>question-<strong>and</strong>-answer reports in one studyfailed to reach statistical significance.7. Suggested report writing practicesinclude the following:(a) Report only pertinent information(b) Define abbreviations <strong>and</strong> acronyms(c) Emphasize words rather than numbers(d) Reduce difficult words(e) Describe the tests used(f) Edit the report at least once(g) Use headings <strong>and</strong> lists freely(h) Use examples <strong>of</strong> behavior to clarifymeaning(i) Reduce report length(j) Check scores(k) Check spelling <strong>and</strong> grammar8. Psychological reports <strong>of</strong>ten includesome or all <strong>of</strong> the following headings:(a) Identifying Information(b) <strong>Assessment</strong> Procedures(c) Referral Question(s)(d) Background Information(e) Behavioral Observations(f) <strong>Assessment</strong> Results <strong>and</strong> Interpretation(g) Diagnostic Considerations(h) Summary(i) Signatures(j) Recommendations(k) Psychometric Summary9. Hints for communicating test resultsto parents include the following:(a) Be direct <strong>and</strong> honest(b) Use percentile ranks heavily whendescribing test results(c) Allow parents opportunities toparticipate(d) Anticipate questions prior to theinterview <strong>and</strong> prepare responses(e) Schedule adequate time for theinterview(f) Practice communicating with parentsfrom a variety <strong>of</strong> backgrounds(g) Avoid questionable predictions(h) Use good, basic counseling skills toconvey difficult information(i) Do not engage in counseling thatis beyond your level <strong>of</strong> expertise(j) Be aware that some parents are notready to accept some <strong>of</strong> the conclusions<strong>of</strong>fered10. Teacher conferences are important forensuring cooperation with recommendations.11. The major decision that a clinicianneeds to make before giving feedbackto a child regards the type <strong>of</strong> informationthat is appropriate for the child’sdevelopmental level.


C h a p t e r 1 7<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> AttentionDeficit Hyperactivity <strong>and</strong>Disruptive Behavior DisordersChapter QuestionsIntroductionl What are some findings from researchon Attention-Deficit HyperactivityDisorder (ADHD) that have importantimplications for designing clinicalassessments for children suspected <strong>of</strong>having ADHD?l What are some practical guidelines fordesigning an assessment battery forchildren suspected <strong>of</strong> having ADHD<strong>and</strong> interpreting the assessment results?l What are the implications <strong>of</strong> research onchildhood conduct problems for designingclinical assessments for children withthese problems?l What basic questions should beaddressed in clinical assessments <strong>of</strong> childrenwith conduct problems?This chapter is the first <strong>of</strong> a series <strong>of</strong>chapters focusing on the assessment <strong>of</strong>several specific types <strong>of</strong> childhood emotional<strong>and</strong> behavioral problems. Thesechapters are designed to help an assessorapply information on the various assessmentstrategies discussed in previouschapters to the assessment <strong>of</strong> some <strong>of</strong> themore common types <strong>of</strong> psychopathologyexhibited by children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.We start with the assessment <strong>of</strong> disorders,sometimes called externalizing behaviors(Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1978) ordisorders <strong>of</strong> undercontrol (Quay, 1986).It is appropriate to start our syndromeby-syndromediscussion with this class<strong>of</strong> disorders because they tend to be themost common reason for referral to child377P.J. Frick et al., <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Behavior,DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-0641-0_17, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010


378CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivitymental health clinics (Frick & Kimonis,2008).This predominance in clinic referrals isout <strong>of</strong> proportion to the prevalence <strong>of</strong> thesedisorders in the general population, whereemotional difficulties are <strong>of</strong>ten as prevalent(Costello, Egger, & Angold, 2005). Thishigh referral rate for disruptive behaviordisorders is likely due to two factors.First, unlike adult mental health referrals,children <strong>and</strong> adolescents are rarely selfreferred.Instead, they are <strong>of</strong>ten referred bysignificant others (e.g., parents, teachers,physicians) in their environment. Second,disruptive behavior disorders, as the nameimplies, are syndromes <strong>of</strong> behavior thatcause significant disruptions in a child’senvironment, <strong>of</strong>ten directly affecting thoseresponsible for referring a child for assessment<strong>and</strong> treatment. Thus, anyone workingin a clinical setting with children <strong>and</strong>adolescents must have a firm underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>of</strong> these behavioral disorders.To reiterate a common theme <strong>of</strong> thisbook, our recommendations for assessingchildren with ADHD <strong>and</strong> the disruptivebehaviors disorders are based on researchon the basic characteristics <strong>of</strong> these disorders.In each <strong>of</strong> the following sectionswe first provide a brief discussion <strong>of</strong> themost clinically relevant research findings<strong>and</strong> then discuss specific recommendationsfor assessment procedures based onthese findings. We divide our discussioninto two sections corresponding to themajor subdivisions within the externalizingdisorders. The first section involvesa discussion <strong>of</strong> a syndrome <strong>of</strong> behaviorsinvolving inattention-disorganization <strong>and</strong>impulsivity-motor hyperactivity labeledas Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder(ADHD) by the DSM-IV-TR (AmericanPsychiatric Association, 2000). Thesecond section focuses on conduct problems<strong>and</strong> aggression subsumed under thecategories <strong>of</strong> Oppositional Defiant Disorder(ODD) <strong>and</strong> Conduct Disorder (CD)(American Psychiatric Association, 2000).Attention-DeficitHyperactivity DisorderClassification <strong>and</strong> SubtypesHistoryThe syndrome <strong>of</strong> ADHD has long beenrecognized in the medical <strong>and</strong> psychologicalliterature, with some descriptionsdating back well over 100 years (Smith,Barkley, & Shapiro, 2007). However, overthe years there has been considerable disagreementover what are the core features<strong>of</strong> the disorder. As a result <strong>of</strong> this confusion,there have been numerous changes indiagnostic definitions (see Frick & Lahey,1991; Smith et al., 2007). This evolutionin our conceptualization <strong>of</strong> ADHDis reflected in changes in the diagnosticcriteria for ADHD over the most recentrevisions <strong>of</strong> the Diagnostic <strong>and</strong> StatisticalManual <strong>of</strong> Mental Disorders.The second edition <strong>of</strong> the DSMincluded a syndrome <strong>of</strong> the HyperkineticReaction <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong>hood to emphasize thebelief that motor hyperactivity is the corefeature <strong>of</strong> the disorder (DSM-II; AmericanPsychiatric Association, 1968). In thethird edition <strong>of</strong> the DSM the disorder wasreconceptualized to emphasize deficits insustained attention, acquiring the termAttention Deficit Disorder (DSM-III;American Psychiatric Association, 1980).Also in the DSM-III, it was explicitly recognizedfor the first time that childrencould have attention deficits in the absence<strong>of</strong> motor hyperactivity. The next revision,DSM-III-R, s<strong>of</strong>tened this emphasis onattention deficits, placing it on equal footingwith motor hyperactivity in its definition<strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit HyperactivityDisorder (American Psychiatric Association,1987). The DSM-III-R criteria alsodid away with subtypes based on the presenceor absence <strong>of</strong> motor activity.Before discussing the most recent revision<strong>of</strong> the DSM (DSM-IV-TR; American


CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivity379Psychiatric Association, 2000), it is importantto highlight two main sources <strong>of</strong> variationin these conceptualizations <strong>of</strong> thedisorder. First, a main source <strong>of</strong> debatehas been over what are the core features <strong>of</strong>the disorder. For example, DSM-III proposedthree core dimensions <strong>of</strong> behaviorassociated with attention deficit disorders:(1) inattention (e.g., very distractible, difficultyfinishing things), (2) impulsivity(e.g., acts without thinking, interrupts others,loses things, makes careless mistakes),<strong>and</strong> (3) hyperactivity (e.g., fidgety <strong>and</strong> restless,running around <strong>and</strong> climbing excessively).In contrast, DSM-III-R eliminatedany distinctions among these behaviors<strong>and</strong> considered all three dimensions to beindicative <strong>of</strong> a single domain <strong>of</strong> behavior.Research has fairly consistently suggestedthat the best method for conceptualizingthe symptoms is somewhere inbetween. Specifically, factor analyses havegenerally been able to document two partiallyindependent dimensions <strong>of</strong> behavior:inattention/disorganization <strong>and</strong> impulsivity/overactivity(Lahey, Carlson, & Frick,1997). The behaviors that are generallyconsidered to be the core features <strong>of</strong>ADHD are listed in Table 17.1.The second source <strong>of</strong> contention evidentthrough the revisions <strong>of</strong> the DSMis the debate over whether there are validsubtypes <strong>of</strong> ADHD. DSM-III proposed theexistence <strong>of</strong> two types <strong>of</strong> attention deficitdisorder: Attention Deficit Disorder withoutHyperactivity (ADD/WO) <strong>and</strong> AttentionDeficit Disorder with Hyperactivity(ADD/H). These subtypes shared the corefeatures <strong>of</strong> inattention <strong>and</strong> impulsivity butdiffered on the presence <strong>of</strong> motor hyperactivity.Lahey et al. (1997) summarize asignificant body <strong>of</strong> research attesting tothe validity <strong>of</strong> this distinction. <strong>Child</strong>renwith ADD/H tend to exhibit more conductproblems, to be more impulsive, <strong>and</strong>to be more socially rejected than childrenwith ADD/WO. In contrast, children withADD/WO tend to be more sluggish <strong>and</strong>drowsy (<strong>of</strong>ten described as unmotivated),to be more anxious <strong>and</strong> shy, <strong>and</strong> to be morelikely to show an optimal response to lowdoses <strong>of</strong> stimulant medication than childrenwith ADD/H. Based on their review<strong>of</strong> the research, these authors concludedTable 17.1 Core Dimensions <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivity DisorderInattention-DisorganizationDifficulty organizing things a,bDifficulty finishing tasks a,bDifficulty following through on instructions a,bOften loses things a,bEasily distracted a,bOften does not listen a,bDifficulty concentrating <strong>and</strong> sustaining attention a,bMisses details <strong>and</strong> makes careless mistakes aOften avoids or dislikes tasks requiring sustainedmental effort aOften forgetful aNeeds a lot <strong>of</strong> supervision bImpulsivity-HyperactivityExcessive running <strong>and</strong> climbing a,bDifficulty playing quietly a,bExcessive talking a,bFrequently interrupts <strong>and</strong> intrudes a,bAlways on the go a,bExcessive fidgeting <strong>and</strong> squirming a,bDifficulty staying in seat a,bDifficulty waiting turn a,bFrequently blurts out answers a,bFrequently calls out in class baSvmptoms included in the DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) criteria for ADHD, althoughwording may not be exactly as that included in the manual. b Behaviors included in factor analyses reviewed byLahey et al., 1997.


380CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivitythat the decision to eliminate the subtypesin the DSM-III-R was not consistent withthis body <strong>of</strong> research.DSM-IV-TRThe DSM-IV-TR definition <strong>of</strong> ADHD wasdesigned to reflect research findings onboth <strong>of</strong> these issues (American PsychiatricAssociation, 2000). First, there are twosymptom lists, which closely correspond tothe two dimensions <strong>of</strong> behavior describedin Table 17.1. Second, the DSM-IV-TRrecognizes the existence <strong>of</strong> subtypes basedlargely on the presence <strong>of</strong> hyperactivity.There is an ADHD Predominantly InattentiveType to designate children withproblems <strong>of</strong> inattention <strong>and</strong> disorganizationbut without problems <strong>of</strong> impulsivity<strong>and</strong> overactivity. In addition, there are theADHD Predominantly Hyperactive Type<strong>and</strong> ADHD Combined Type to designatechildren with significant problems <strong>of</strong>impulsivity-hyperactivity, either in isolationfrom or in combination with problems<strong>of</strong> inattention <strong>and</strong> disorganization.While these subtypes seem to match theresearch findings summarized previously,it is important to note that the stability <strong>of</strong>these ADHD subtypes over time is questionable.Specifically, in a sample <strong>of</strong> 118children with ADHD who were ages 4 to6 at the start <strong>of</strong> the study, Lahey, Pelham,Loney, Lee, <strong>and</strong> Wilcutt (2005) reportedthat it was not unusual for children tochange in their subtype <strong>of</strong> ADHD over the8-year study period. For example, 37% <strong>of</strong>children with Combined Type <strong>and</strong> 50% <strong>of</strong>the children with Predominantly InattentiveType met criteria for a different subtypeat least twice during the study period.<strong>Child</strong>ren with the Hyperactive Type werethe most likely to shift subtypes, with mostshifting to the Combined Type at somepoint during the study.An inspection <strong>of</strong> the symptoms includedin the DSM-IV-TR criteria for ADHD (seeTable 17.1) indicate that the individualbehaviors that form the diagnostic criteriafor this disorder are behaviors that arequite common to some degree in normallydeveloping children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. Thisis one <strong>of</strong> the issues that has led to seriousconcerns over the potential overdiagnosis<strong>of</strong> the disorder <strong>and</strong> concomitant overuse <strong>of</strong>stimulant medication to treat it (Angold,Erkanli, Egger, & Costello, 2000; Jensenet al., 1999). There are two critical issuesrelated to these concerns. First, at present,there is little empirical evidence tosupport the concerns about overdiagnosis<strong>and</strong> overmedication, although clearly thisis a very difficult issue on which to obtaingood data (Jensen et al., 1999). Second, thesymptoms <strong>of</strong> most childhood disorders,not just ADHD, are not qualitatively differentfrom normal behaviors shown bychildren (e.g., sadness as a symptom <strong>of</strong>depression). This is not to imply that this isnot an important issue in assessment but toillustrate that it is not specific to ADHD.It relates to the important issue raised inChap. 3 that classification systems mustclearly define what parameters are mostimportant for differentiating disordered(i.e., clinically impairing) manifestations<strong>of</strong> the symptoms from more normal manifestations.The DSM-IV-TR includes severalsuch parameters for the diagnosis <strong>of</strong>ADHD, <strong>and</strong> it is imperative that assessorssystematically assess these parameters toavoid overdiagnosis.The first parameter is the frequency <strong>and</strong>severity <strong>of</strong> the symptoms. The DSM-IV-TRsets six symptoms <strong>of</strong> either inattention-disorganizationor impulsivity-hyperactivityas the diagnostic threshold for the disorder.This level <strong>of</strong> severity was chosen basedon evidence that it seemed to designate alevel <strong>of</strong> symptomology that predictedclinically significant levels <strong>of</strong> psychosocialimpairment (e.g., poor academic performance,social rejection) for elementaryschool-aged children (Lahey, Applegate,McBurnett, et al., 1994). Using this diagnosticthreshold, 3–7% <strong>of</strong> children would


CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivity381typically meet the diagnostic criteria forADHD (American Psychiatric Association,2000). It is important to note that theappropriateness <strong>of</strong> this threshold has beenquestioned for young preschool children asbeing too liberal, because many very youngchildren show high rates <strong>of</strong> these behaviors<strong>and</strong> eventually outgrow them (Campbell,1990), <strong>and</strong> for adolescents <strong>and</strong> youngadults as being too conservative, becausethe frequency <strong>and</strong> severity <strong>of</strong> many <strong>of</strong> thesymptoms seem to decline in adolescence(Barkley, 1997a).The second parameter that differentiatesnormal <strong>and</strong> abnormal patterns <strong>of</strong> inattention,impulsivity, <strong>and</strong> overactivity is theonset <strong>and</strong> duration <strong>of</strong> the symptoms. DSM-IV-TR specifies that “some hyperactive-impulsiveor inattentive symptoms must havecaused impairment before age 7 years”(American Psychiatric Association, 2000,p. 92). This criterion is consistent with theconceptualization that ADHD is a lifelongpattern <strong>of</strong> maladaptive behavior <strong>and</strong> not atransient reaction to a specific stressor orto the dem<strong>and</strong>s <strong>of</strong> a particular developmentalstage (Barkley, 1997a). While theage <strong>of</strong> onset criterion is consistent withthis conceptual framework, there are severalpractical problems in using this criterionin clinical assessments. First, it is <strong>of</strong>tendifficult to gain accurate accounts <strong>of</strong> whensymptoms became problematic, especiallywhen assessing adolescents <strong>and</strong> adults,which involves recall <strong>of</strong> events over a longperiod <strong>of</strong> time (Barkley, 1997a).Second, it is not uncommon for many<strong>of</strong> the symptoms <strong>of</strong> ADHD, especially theinattention ones, to only become problematiconce the dem<strong>and</strong>s for sustainedattention <strong>and</strong> organization increase in laterelementary school years (Lahey et al., 2005;Loeber, Green, Lahey, Christ, & Frick,1992). This developmental change in inattentionsymptoms is likely the reason thatthe age <strong>of</strong> 7 onset criterion may be particularlyproblematic for the PredominantlyInattentive Type <strong>of</strong> ADHD. Specifically,in a sample <strong>of</strong> 380 clinic-referred children(mean age 8.7 years) who met DSM-IV criteriafor ADHD, almost all <strong>of</strong> those whomet the symptom cut-<strong>of</strong>f for the PredominantlyHyperactive-Impulsive Type <strong>and</strong>the Combined Type met the age <strong>of</strong> 7 onsetcriterion (Applegate et al., 1997). In contrast,only 48% <strong>of</strong> those children with PredominantlyInattentive Type met this age <strong>of</strong>onset criterion <strong>and</strong> those who did not meetthe criterion did not differ from those thatdid on several important validity indexes,including level <strong>of</strong> impairment <strong>and</strong> level <strong>and</strong>type <strong>of</strong> comorbidity with other disorders.Therefore, the validity <strong>of</strong> the onset criterionfor this ADHD subtype was questionable.Third, it is important to recognizethat, while the core deficits underlying thesymptoms <strong>of</strong> ADHD may be stable acrossdevelopment, how these deficits are manifestedin symptoms <strong>and</strong> secondary characteristicsmay change across development(Barkley, 1997a). In Table 17.2, we providea brief summary <strong>of</strong> some <strong>of</strong> these developmentalchanges in the symptom patterns<strong>and</strong> secondary characteristics acrossdevelopment. One criticism <strong>of</strong> the DSM-IV-TR definition <strong>of</strong> ADHD that includesthe same number <strong>and</strong> types <strong>of</strong> symptomsfor children, adolescents, <strong>and</strong> adults is thatthis static definition may not capture thesedevelopmental changes adequately (Barkley,1997a).A third parameter in the DSM-IV-TRdefinition <strong>of</strong> ADHD that is importantfor separating normative from disorderedlevels <strong>of</strong> attention <strong>and</strong> overactivity is thespecification <strong>of</strong> cross-situational consistency<strong>of</strong> symptoms. That is, to be diagnosed withADHD, a child must show impairmentrelated to the symptoms in two or moresettings. This criterion is consistent withthe conception that ADHD should not besolely a function <strong>of</strong> a single set <strong>of</strong> environmentalcircumstances (e.g., a disorganizedclassroom, a chaotic home environment)<strong>and</strong> clearly suggests that an adequate assessment<strong>of</strong> ADHD must involve an assessment


382CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit HyperactivityTable 17.2 Developmental Changes in the Core Features <strong>and</strong> Secondary Characteristics Associated with ADHD Across the LifespanAge Primary Characteristics Secondary FeaturesPreschool Restlessness, excessive activity, difficulty remaining seated,<strong>and</strong> acts without thinkingElementary-school age Poor attention span, distractibility, impulsivity, difficultyremaining seated, <strong>and</strong> can’t play quietlyAdolescence Poor attention span, distractibility, inability to finish things,careless mistakes, <strong>and</strong> lack <strong>of</strong> forethought <strong>and</strong> planningYoung adults Restlessness, distractibility, difficulty completing work,carelessness, <strong>and</strong> lack <strong>of</strong> forethought <strong>and</strong> planningNoncompliance, accidental injuries, aggression, <strong>and</strong>problems in toilet trainingDifficulty following rules, immaturity, social rejection,needing a lot <strong>of</strong> supervision, failure to do householdchores, <strong>and</strong> poor school performancePoor school performance (dropping out), lowself-esteem, depression, substance use, delinquency,family conflict, needless risk taking, automobileaccidents, teenage pregnancy, <strong>and</strong> rebelliousnessPoor educational <strong>and</strong> occupational performance,depression, substance use, poor social/marital adjustment,<strong>and</strong> poor anger managementSources: Summarized from Barkley, 1997a; Barkley, Guevremont, Anastopoulos, & Fletcher, 1993; DuPaul & Stoner, 1994; Nadeau, 1995; Smith et al., 2007; Wender, 1995.


CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivity383<strong>of</strong> the child in several different settings.However, although this criterion appearsquite basic, it is difficult to use in clinicalassessments for several reasons. For example,it is clear that children with ADHDwill show variations in the level <strong>and</strong> severity<strong>of</strong> their behavior problems dependingon the dem<strong>and</strong>s <strong>of</strong> the situation (e.g., time<strong>of</strong> day, level <strong>of</strong> structure, <strong>and</strong> complexity<strong>of</strong> the task) (Barkley, 1997a). Therefore,an assessor should not interpret the crosssituationalcriterion to imply that a childwith ADHD must show the same level <strong>and</strong>severity symptoms across different settingswith different dem<strong>and</strong>s. Instead, one mustconsider whether the child shows similarbehaviors in situations with equivalentdem<strong>and</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> such judgments are very difficultto make. For example, if a child ishaving trouble associated with ADHD atschool but not at home, the clinical assessormust judge whether or not this is due tothe fact that dem<strong>and</strong>s for sustained attention<strong>and</strong> sitting still are not placed on thechild at home.Taken together, the level <strong>and</strong> severity<strong>of</strong> symptoms, their presence over extendedperiods <strong>of</strong> time, <strong>and</strong> their cross-situationalconsistency all are critical components <strong>of</strong>defining ADHD <strong>and</strong> must be systematicallyassessed in making this diagnosis. Inherentin each <strong>of</strong> these criteria is that they designatechildren who show some significantlevel <strong>of</strong> impairment in their psychosocialfunctioning due to the symptoms associatedwith ADHD. On the simplest, butpossibly the most important level, it is thissignificant impairment in functioning (e.g.,causing problems in school work, causingsocial rejection) that is most important indifferentiating normative <strong>and</strong> disorderedlevels <strong>of</strong> the symptoms that are part <strong>of</strong>the ADHD definition. Furthermore, thedegree <strong>of</strong> impairment in family functioning,peer relationships, <strong>and</strong> academic functioning,are <strong>of</strong>ten the main reasons that a childwith ADHD is referred for an evaluation<strong>and</strong> they are some <strong>of</strong> the best predictors<strong>of</strong> a child’s long-term adjustment (Pelham,Fabiano, & Massetti, 2005).Comorbidities<strong>Child</strong>ren with ADHD are an excellentexample <strong>of</strong> the fact that children with problemsin one area <strong>of</strong> adjustment are at risk forproblems in other areas as well. Problemsthat <strong>of</strong>ten co-occur with ADHD are quiteimportant clinically. They <strong>of</strong>ten cause moredisruptions for the child <strong>and</strong> predict pooreroutcomes than the primary ADHD symptomsthemselves (Frick & Lahey, 1991). Asa result, the secondary features are <strong>of</strong>ten amajor focus <strong>of</strong> intervention (Pelham et al.,2005).Conduct Problems/AggressionThe most common co-occurring problemsexperienced by children with ADHDare conduct problems <strong>and</strong> aggression,with research suggesting that 60–75% <strong>of</strong>children referred to clinics with ADHDshow significant levels <strong>of</strong> these problems(Hinshaw, 1987). It is <strong>of</strong>ten these conductproblems that lead to a great deal <strong>of</strong> disruptionfor children with ADHD, leading tomultiple disciplinary confrontations withparents <strong>and</strong> teachers, school suspensions,<strong>and</strong> problems in peer relations. In addition,these conduct problems are <strong>of</strong>ten predictive<strong>of</strong> poor outcomes in adolescence <strong>and</strong>young adulthood, especially for predictingdelinquency <strong>and</strong> substance abuse (Fischer,Barkley, Fletcher, & Smallish, 1993; Mannuzza,Gittelman-Klein, Konig, & Giampino,1989).Other ComorbiditiesAnother condition that <strong>of</strong>ten occurs withADHD is academic underachievement ora learning disability, both <strong>of</strong> which arefrequently defined as school achievementbelow a level predicted by a child’s age


384CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivity<strong>and</strong> intellectual level. Approximately 30%<strong>of</strong> children with ADHD show such learningproblems (Frick, Lahey, Christ, Loeber,& Green, 1991; Massetti et al., 2008).In addition, children with ADHD tendto show a high rate <strong>of</strong> conflict with peers(Mikami & Hinshaw, 2003), with parents(Johnston & Mash, 2001), <strong>and</strong> withteachers (Cunningham & Boyle, 2002).And not surprisingly, given the amount<strong>of</strong> difficulty <strong>and</strong> conflict the ADHD child<strong>of</strong>ten experiences in his or her environment,children with ADHD <strong>of</strong>ten showhigh rates <strong>of</strong> anxiety (Tannock, 2000) <strong>and</strong>low self-esteem that persist throughoutchildhood <strong>and</strong> into adolescence (Fischeret al., 1993).Conceptual ModelThere have been numerous theories <strong>of</strong>ADHD that differ in terms <strong>of</strong> identifyingthe “core deficit” that underlies thesymptoms <strong>of</strong> ADHD <strong>and</strong> the etiologicalfactors that lead to this deficit. A growingnumber <strong>of</strong> researchers in this area havebegun to focus on a failure in a child’sinhibition system that influences his orher ability to regulate attention, actions,<strong>and</strong> emotions (e.g., Nigg, 2006; Whalen& Henker, 1998). One <strong>of</strong> the more influential<strong>and</strong> best articulated <strong>of</strong> such theoriesis one proposed by Barkley (1997b),which defines “behavioral inhibition” asthe capacity to inhibit motivated behaviors,either prior to their initiation oronce they are initiated, which creates adelay between an impulse <strong>and</strong> action. Thisdelay allows the child to “think through”his or her actions <strong>and</strong> allows the behaviorto be self-directed <strong>and</strong> guided by thedem<strong>and</strong>s <strong>of</strong> any given situation. A deficitin this inhibition system would make itdifficult for a child to sustain his or herattention on a single task, it would makeforesight <strong>and</strong> planning difficult, <strong>and</strong> itwould make it difficult for the child toinhibit impulses for motor movement,thereby accounting for the core symptoms<strong>of</strong> the disorder. Barkley also outlineshow such a deficit could account formany <strong>of</strong> the other characteristics typicallyfound in people with ADHD, such as apoor sense <strong>of</strong> time, poor emotional selfcontrol,deficits in problem solving, <strong>and</strong>an inability to modulate behavior basedon changing situational dem<strong>and</strong>s.While there is a growing consensus that adeficit in the inhibitory control <strong>of</strong> behaviormay be a primary or at least an importantdeficit in children in ADHD, it is less clearwhat could cause this deficit to develop.Many theories focus on structural neurologicalabnormalities in parts <strong>of</strong> the nervoussystem involved in inhibitory control<strong>of</strong> behavior (Castellanos et al., 2002). Othertheories focus on abnormalities in the functioning<strong>of</strong> these neurological regions withstudies examining the cerebral blood flow<strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adults with ADHD consistentlyshowing areas <strong>of</strong> decreased activityin the prefrontal regions <strong>of</strong> the brain(Hendren, DeBacker, & P<strong>and</strong>ina, 2000).There is evidence that these neurologicalabnormalities can result from anumber <strong>of</strong> different influences. Specifically,there is evidence that ADHD symptomsare highly heritable <strong>and</strong>, as a result,these neurological abnormalities may beinherited (Waldman & Gizer, 2006). Inaddition, the neurological abnormalitiescould result from trauma to the developingnervous system such a prenatalexposure to alcohol or other drugs, birthtrauma, or exposure to environmentaltoxins (e.g., lead) (Smith et al., 2007). Itis important to note that, although mosttheories <strong>of</strong> ADHD emphasize potentialneurological underpinnings to the disorder,there is currently no neurological testthat has proven to be useful in diagnosingADHD. Instead, the diagnosis relieson a careful assessment <strong>of</strong> the behaviorallybased diagnostic criteria usinga process outlined in the next section<strong>of</strong> this chapter. Although social experiencescan influence the development <strong>of</strong>


CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivity385behavioral inhibition <strong>and</strong> can changebrain functioning (Cicchetti & Walker,2001), most theories have not emphasizedenvironmental factors as primarycausal agents in the development <strong>of</strong> thecore symptoms <strong>of</strong> ADHD. Instead, mosttheories emphasize the role <strong>of</strong> environmentalfactors in determining how thecore deficit is expressed. For example, achild with problems in behavioral inhibitionwill be very difficult to socialize butsome parents will be better than othersin working with such a child to developcompensatory strategies to minimize theeffects that the inhibitory deficit mayhave on the child’s academic <strong>and</strong> psychosocialfunctioning.As a result, environmental factors canplay a large role in the development <strong>of</strong>some <strong>of</strong> the problems (e.g., poor schoolperformance, social rejection, conductproblems) that can develop secondarily tothe ADHD symptoms (Frick, 1994; Johnston& Mash, 2001). Therefore, althoughpsychosocial influences may not be integralto many causal theories <strong>of</strong> ADHD, itis still important to carefully assess a child’spsychosocial context to (1) determine thedegree to which the problems associatedwith ADHD have negatively impacteda child’s functioning <strong>and</strong> (2) guide interventionsdesigned to reduce or preventmany <strong>of</strong> the secondary characteristics <strong>and</strong>co-occurring problems in adjustment that<strong>of</strong>ten develop in children with ADHD(Pelham et al., 2005).Implications for <strong>Assessment</strong>In Table 17.3, we summarize the mainimplications <strong>of</strong> research on ADHD fordesigning an appropriate assessment batteryfor children or adolescents suspected<strong>of</strong> having ADHD. In addition, in Boxes17.1 <strong>and</strong> 17.2, we provide two case examples<strong>of</strong> a typical ADHD assessment battery,with Box 17.1 describing the assessment<strong>of</strong> a child with ADHD-Combined Type<strong>and</strong> Box 17.2 describing the assessment<strong>of</strong> a child with ADHD-PredominantlyInattentive Type.Table 17.3 The Nature <strong>of</strong> ADHD <strong>and</strong> Implications for <strong>Assessment</strong>Focus <strong>of</strong> ResearchClassification <strong>and</strong> presence<strong>of</strong> subtypesPresence <strong>of</strong> multiplecomorbiditiesPotential alternative causesImplications for <strong>Assessment</strong>Assess for presence <strong>of</strong> two core dimensions <strong>of</strong> behavior inattention/disorganization <strong>and</strong> impulsivity/overactivityAssess for subtypes based on the presence <strong>of</strong> impulsivity/overactivityAssess duration to determine if behaviors are chronic <strong>and</strong> stableAssess situational variability <strong>of</strong> behaviorsAssess level <strong>of</strong> impairment associated with symptomsAssess for the presence <strong>of</strong> conduct problems/aggressionAssess for the presence <strong>of</strong> learning problemsAssess anxietyAssess self-esteemAssess social relationships <strong>and</strong> peer social statusAssess level <strong>of</strong> parent–child <strong>and</strong> teacher–child conflictsObtain a developmental <strong>and</strong> medical historyAssess for intellectual <strong>and</strong> learning deficitsAssess for emotional difficulties


386CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit HyperactivityBox 17.1Case Study: Evaluation <strong>of</strong> an 8-Year-Old Girl with Attention-Deficit HyperactivityDisorder-Combined TypeClaire was 8 years, 9 months old when hermother referred her to an outpatient mentalhealth clinic for a comprehensive psychologicalevaluation. Claire’s mother was concernedabout Claire’s aggressive behavior, describingClaire as having an “uncontrollable temper”<strong>and</strong> being very defiant. Maternal report alsoindicated that Claire has had very inconsistentacademic performance throughout her first 3years <strong>of</strong> school, primarily because she failedto complete work <strong>and</strong> made a lot <strong>of</strong> carelessmistakes. To illustrate the effect <strong>of</strong> Claire’scarelessness on her school performance, hermother described an incident the previousschool year in which Claire rushed through anarithmetic test <strong>and</strong> completed all the questionsas addition, even though half <strong>of</strong> the problemswere subtraction. Claire reportedly knew howto do subtraction problems.<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Core SymptomsThe core symptoms <strong>of</strong> ADHD were assessedthrough a structured diagnostic interview conductedwith Claire’s mother <strong>and</strong> her teacher(DISC-IV; Shaffer et al., 2000 <strong>and</strong> behaviorrating scales completed by her mother (CBCL;Achenbach, 2001) <strong>and</strong> her teacher (CBRSC;Neeper, Lahey, & Frick, 1990). On the DISC-IV, Claire’s mother <strong>and</strong> teacher both reportedsignificant problems <strong>of</strong> inattention-disorganization,such as difficulty sustaining attention, havingdifficulty finishing tasks, <strong>of</strong>ten losing things,frequently making careless mistakes, <strong>and</strong> havingvery messy work habits. In addition, theseproblems in attention were accompanied bysignificant problems <strong>of</strong> impulsivity <strong>and</strong> motorhyperactivity. Both mother <strong>and</strong> teacher indicatedthat Claire frequently interrupted others;<strong>of</strong>ten talked out in class; was very fidgety <strong>and</strong>restless in class; <strong>and</strong> could not stay in her seat,either in class or at home to eat dinner.Consistent with a diagnosis <strong>of</strong> ADHD-Combined Type, Claire’s mother reported onthe structured interview that these problemshave been evident since very early in Claire’slife, especially the motor overactivity. In fact,maternal report indicated that Claire had beenasked to leave two preschools because she wastoo “rambunctious” <strong>and</strong> could not sit still.Claire’s teacher indicated that these symptoms<strong>of</strong> ADHD were currently interfering with herschool performance to a substantial degree.On the Academic Performance Rating Scale(APRS: DuPaul, Rapport, & Perriello, 1991).Claire’s teacher indicated that she was turningin less than half <strong>of</strong> the work required bythe class <strong>and</strong> it <strong>of</strong>ten was inaccurate, despiteClaire knowing the material.Finally, parent <strong>and</strong> teacher report on theomnibus rating scales suggested that the coreADHD behaviors were more severe thanwould be expected in children her age. Usingthe age- <strong>and</strong> gender-specific norms <strong>of</strong> CBCL,Claire had elevations on both the AttentionProblems (T-score <strong>of</strong> 75) <strong>and</strong> Thought Problems(T-score <strong>of</strong> 76) scales. Similarly, Claire’steacher on the CBRSC rated her as elevated onthe Motor Hyperactivity scale, with a T-score<strong>of</strong> 79 based on the entire normative sample(across ages <strong>and</strong> gender). Whereas the CBCLelevations may have been spuriously high dueto comparisons restricted to girls, the elevationon the CBRSC based on a comparison toboth boys <strong>and</strong> girls clearly indicated that herbehavior was more severe than is typical forchildren her age.<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> ComorbiditiesLike many children with ADHD-CombinedType, Claire also exhibited significant conductproblems. On the DISC-IV, her parent <strong>and</strong>teacher described Claire as showing frequenttemper tantrums, <strong>of</strong>ten arguing with adults,<strong>of</strong>ten refusing adults’ requests, blaming othersfor her mistakes, <strong>and</strong> being grouchy <strong>and</strong>easily annoyed. These behaviors were ratedas severe on both parent <strong>and</strong> teacher ratingscales, with T-scores above 70 being obtainedon the Delinquent <strong>and</strong> Aggressive Behaviorscales <strong>of</strong> the parent-completed CBCL <strong>and</strong> the(Continues)


CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivity387Box 17.1 (Continued)Oppositional-Conduct Disorders scale <strong>of</strong> theteacher-completed CBRSC.On the CBCL Social Problems scale <strong>and</strong> theCBRSC Social Competence scale, mother <strong>and</strong>teacher also indicated that Claire’s behavioralproblems seemed to be affecting her peer relations.She was described by parent <strong>and</strong> teacheras being bossy <strong>and</strong> domineering in peer interactions,which had led to difficulties in makingfriends. However, a sociometric exercisedid not indicate that Claire’s social status wasnegatively affected by this behavior. In a class<strong>of</strong> 13, she was nominated as “Liked most” by 3children <strong>and</strong> “Liked least” by only 2 children.A psychoeducational evaluation did notreveal any significant learning problems.Claire’s intelligence scores were in the averageto high average range in both verbal <strong>and</strong>nonverbal abilities. She also obtained agest<strong>and</strong>ardscores in the high average range onthe individually administered achievementtest. Thus, there were no indications <strong>of</strong> cognitivedeficits, <strong>and</strong> her achievement scoresindicated that she seemed to be learning ator above a level expected for her age.Ruling Out Alternative CausesClaire’s birth, medical, <strong>and</strong> developmentalhistory did not suggest the presence <strong>of</strong> anymedical or neurological disorder. As mentionedpreviously, the psychoeducationalevaluation did not reveal any cognitive orlearning problems that could account for thebehaviors. An unstructured clinical interviewdid reveal that Claire reported being sexuallyabused over a period <strong>of</strong> 1 month duringthe previous summer by a paternal uncle.This alleged abuse had been reported to thelocal child protection agency, <strong>and</strong> Claire hadbeen seen at the community mental healthcenter for 3 months following the incident.However, it did not appear that Claire’s difficultiescould be solely accounted for byan emotional reaction to sexual abuse, forseveral reasons. First, the ADHD behaviorswere more severe than would be expectedfrom such a reaction, <strong>and</strong> they clearly predatedthe alleged abuse incident. Second, shedid not show any other signs <strong>of</strong> anxiety <strong>and</strong>depression that would suggest a significantdegree <strong>of</strong> emotional distress.Box 17.2Case Study: Evaluation <strong>of</strong> an 8-Year-Old Boy with Attention-Deficit HyperactivityDisorder-Predominantly Inattentive TypeSean was 8 years, 1 month when his teacherreferred him to a child mental health clinicfor a comprehensive psychological evaluation.Sean was failing most subjects in the thirdgrade <strong>and</strong> his teacher attributed this poor performanceprimarily to problems in concentration.At home, Sean’s mother also reportedthat he had difficulty completing things, was<strong>of</strong>ten daydreaming, <strong>and</strong> seemed to have littlemotivation for anything.<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Core SymptomsThe core ADHD behaviors were assessed bystructured interviews (DISC-IV) completedby Sean’s parent <strong>and</strong> teacher <strong>and</strong> by parent(CBCL) <strong>and</strong> teacher (CBRSC, APRS)behavior rating scales. On structured interviews,both parent <strong>and</strong> teacher reported thatSean showed significant problems <strong>of</strong> inattention<strong>and</strong> disorganization, such as beingvery distractible, frequently daydreaming,having difficulty finishing tasks, <strong>of</strong>ten seemingunmotivated, <strong>and</strong> seeming very sluggish<strong>and</strong> drowsy. Although Sean was described byhis mother as somewhat fidgety, neither hismother nor his teacher reported significantproblems <strong>of</strong> impulsivity or overactivity.Consistent with these reports on the structuredinterviews, Sean was rated as showing(Continues)


388CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit HyperactivityBox 17.2 (Continued)attentional problems on parent <strong>and</strong> teacherrating scales out <strong>of</strong> a normative range. On theCBCL completed by his mother, Sean hada T-score <strong>of</strong> 69 on the Attention Problemsscale. On the teacher-completed CBRSC, hehad T-scores <strong>of</strong> 76 <strong>and</strong> 71 on the SluggishTempo <strong>and</strong> Daydreams scales, respectively.He also had a T-score <strong>of</strong> 69 on the Inattention-Disorganizationscale <strong>of</strong> the CBRSC.Consistent with a diagnosis <strong>of</strong> ADHD-InattentiveType, his teacher’s rating <strong>of</strong> MotorHyperactivity on the CBRSC was within anormative range (T-score <strong>of</strong> 57).<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> ComorbiditiesOn parent <strong>and</strong> teacher structured interviews,Sean was reported as having some signs <strong>of</strong> mildanxiety, including frequent stomachaches, selfconsciousbehaviors, <strong>and</strong> concerns about hisappearance. These symptoms did not seemsevere enough to warrant a diagnosis <strong>of</strong> an anxietydisorder <strong>and</strong> they did not appear out <strong>of</strong> agenormativeranges on the CBCL, on the CBRSC,or on the self-report Revised <strong>Child</strong>ren’s ManifestAnxiety Scale (Reynolds & Richmond, 1985).A psychoeducational assessment did notreveal any evidence <strong>of</strong> a learning disability.Sean’s scores on both in st<strong>and</strong>ardized intelligencetest <strong>and</strong> achievement test were allwithin age-appropriate limits. An assessment<strong>of</strong> Sean’s peer relations did not indicateany problems in this psychological domain,based on parent <strong>and</strong> teacher rating scales <strong>and</strong>a sociometric exercise conducted with Sean’sclassmates. Similarly, there were no indicationsfrom any assessment source that Seanexhibited significant conduct problems.Ruling Out Alternative CausesA birth, medical, <strong>and</strong> developmental historyobtained from Sean’s mother did not revealany indications <strong>of</strong> significant medical or neurologicalproblems. He reportedly had somedifficulties breathing immediately followingbirth, but he was quickly stabilized with oxygen.Also, he had some mild allergies to dust<strong>and</strong> pollen, but these were not severe enoughto warrant medication.Although Sean exhibited some anxiety symptoms,they did not seem severe enough to becausing his problems in attention. Also, his anxietyseemed to be focused largely around school(e.g., stomachaches on school days, worry abouttests). Therefore, it seemed more likely thatSean’s anxiety was secondary to the academicproblems caused by his attentional difficulties.Assessing Core BehaviorsGuided by the research on classification <strong>of</strong>ADHD, the first goal <strong>of</strong> the assessment isto assess the core features <strong>of</strong> ADHD. Many<strong>of</strong> the behavior rating scales (Chaps. 6 <strong>and</strong>7) <strong>and</strong> structured interviews (Chap. 11) discussedin previous chapters provide scales orsections that assess for these core behaviors.These behaviors can also be assessed throughbehavioral observations (Chap. 8; see alsoPelham et al., 2005; Smith et al., 2007).In selecting specific scales to assess thecore features <strong>of</strong> ADHD, several key factorsshould be considered. First, one should payclose attention to item content. Many measuresare based on outdated conceptualizations<strong>of</strong> ADHD or the scales were formedstrictly on the basis <strong>of</strong> statistical covariation,without any guiding theory to help in scaledefinition. As a result, many scales that purportto measure behaviors associated withADHD also include behaviors that are notcurrently viewed as being part <strong>of</strong> the primarysymptom clusters. Unfortunately,these less relevant behaviors are intermixedwith core behaviors, <strong>and</strong>, as a result, manymeasures have no pure indicator <strong>of</strong> ADHDsymptoms. Examples <strong>of</strong> several commonlyused omnibus rating scales with subscalesthat assess ADHD behaviors are presentedin Box 17.3 to illustrate this point. Evidentfrom this table is the fact that a child mayhave elevations on many scales that purportto measure ADHD without actually showingthe core features <strong>of</strong> the disorder. Therefore,


CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivity389when selecting a scale to use in an assessment<strong>of</strong> possible ADHD, an important considerationis whether it contains scales thatare largely composed <strong>of</strong> ADHD symptoms.Further, when interpreting scale elevationson a given rating scale, one must be surethat the elevations were actually due to thebehaviors associated with ADHD.Second, many measures simply do nothave sufficient coverage <strong>of</strong> the core ADHDbehaviors to aid in the differentiation <strong>of</strong> thetwo subtypes <strong>of</strong> ADHD. Often behaviorsindicative <strong>of</strong> inattention-disorganization areintermixed with impulsive <strong>and</strong> overactivebehaviors, providing no method <strong>of</strong> determiningsubtypes. For example, from Box 17.3one notices that the ASEBA (Achenbach,2001) Attention Problems scale includesitems associated with inattention-disorganization<strong>and</strong> impulsivity-motor hyperactivity.As a result, use <strong>of</strong> this overall clinical scaledoes not aid in distinguishing subtypes <strong>of</strong>ADHD. Typically, structured interviews thatare tied to diagnostic classification systems<strong>and</strong> are updated as the classification systemis updated have the best symptom coverage.Structured interviews also allow one to determinethe duration <strong>and</strong> stability <strong>of</strong> ADHDbehaviors, which, as discussed previously, iscrucial in the assessment <strong>of</strong> ADHD.Box 17.3Commonly Used Behavior Rating Scales with Subscales Related to ADHD:An Illustration <strong>of</strong> Item HeterogeneityMany <strong>of</strong> the more commonly used parent<strong>and</strong> teacher rating scales have very heterogeneousitem content with respect toADHD. Most <strong>of</strong> the scales that purportto measure constructs related to ADHDcontain a large number <strong>of</strong> behaviors notconsidered to be part <strong>of</strong> the core symptoms<strong>of</strong> ADHD (see Table 17.1). Toillustrate this, we list the item content <strong>of</strong>relevant subscales from a few commonlyused behavior ratings scales <strong>and</strong> highlightin boldface type the items that are notdirectly tied to the two core dimensions<strong>of</strong> ADHD.Achenbach System for Empirically Based <strong>Assessment</strong> (Achenbach, 2001)Cross Informant ScalesAttention ProblemsAttention Deficit Hyperactivity Problems(9 items) (7 items)Acts too youngFails to finish thingsFails to finish thingsDifficulty concentratingDifficulty concentratingCan’t sit stillCan’t sit stillActs impulsivelyOften confusedInnattentiveDaydreamsTalks too muchActs impulsivelyLoudPoor school performance ---------------------Innattentive14% not part <strong>of</strong> core ADHD symptoms------------------------44% not part <strong>of</strong> coreADHD symptoms


390CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit HyperactivityBehavior <strong>Assessment</strong> System for <strong>Child</strong>ren – 2nd Edition (Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004)Parent Rating ScaleTeacher Rating ScaleAttention Problems(6 items)Hyperactivity(8-items)Attention Problems(7 items)Hyperactivity(11 items)Pays attention Cannot wait turn Pays attention Cannot wait turnHas a short attention Acts without thinking Has a short attention Acts without thinkingspanHas poor self-control spanListens to directions Interrupts parents Listens to directions Has poor self-controlwhen they are on the Does not pay attentionto lectures doingSeeks attention whilephoneschoolworkPays attention whenspoken toListens carefullyIs easily distracted------------------0% not pare <strong>of</strong> coreADHD symptomsActs out <strong>of</strong> controlInterrupts others whenthey are speakingFiddles with thingswhile at mealsDisrupts other children’sactivities------------------50% not part <strong>of</strong> coreADHD symptomsListens carefullyIs easily distractedIs easily distractedfrom class work------------------0% not part <strong>of</strong> coreADHD symptomsActs out <strong>of</strong> controlInterrupts others whenthey are speakingDisrupts the schoolwork<strong>of</strong> other childrenDisrupts other children’sactivitiesHas trouble stayingseatedIs overly activeCalls out in class------------------45% not part <strong>of</strong> coreADHD symptomsConners Rating Scales – 3rd Edition (Conners, 2008)ParentInattention (10 items)Has trouble staying focused on one thingat a timeHas a short attention spanAvoids or dislikes things that take a lot <strong>of</strong> effort<strong>and</strong> are not funHas trouble concentratingDoesn’t pay attention to details; makescareless mistakesHas trouble changing from one activityto anotherTeacherInattention (11 items)Has a short attention spanDoesn’t pay attention to details;makes careless mistakesGives up easily on difficult tasksIs sidetracked easilyAvoids or dislikes things that take a lot <strong>of</strong> effort<strong>and</strong> are not fun(Continues)


CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivity391Conners Rating Scales – 3 (Continued)ParentInattention (10 items) cont.Inattentive, easily distractedGives up easily on difficult tasksHas trouble keeping his/her mind on workor play for long10% not part <strong>of</strong> core ADHD symptomsTeacherInattention (11 items) cont.Gets boredHas trouble concentratingInattentive, easily distractedHas trouble changing from one task toanotherHas trouble keeping his/her mind on work or play-------------------------------------------------------for long 18% not part <strong>of</strong> core ADHD symptoms-------------------------------------------------------Hyperactivity/Impulsivity (14 items)FidgetingBlurts out answers before the question hasbeen completedIs constantly movingExcitable, impulsiveGets over-stimulatedBlurts out the first thing that comes to mindHas difficulty waiting for his/her turnRuns or climbs when he/she is not supposed toIs noisy <strong>and</strong> loud when playing our using freetimeLeaves seat when he/she should stay seatedHyperactivity/Impulsivity (18 items)Leaves seat when he/she should stay seatedGets overly excitedFidgets or squirms in seatRestless or overactiveBlurts out answers before the question has beencompletedExcitable, impulsiveActs as if driven by a motorRuns or climbs when he/she is not supposed toTalks out <strong>of</strong> turnInterrupts others (e.g., butts into conversations orgames)Fidgets or squirms in seatRestless or overactiveIs noisy <strong>and</strong> loud when playing or using free timeInterrupts others (for example, butts into conversationsor games)Gets over-stimulated or “wound up”Talks too muchFidgeting---------------------------------------------------7% not part <strong>of</strong> core ADHD symptoms Is constantly movingGets up <strong>and</strong> moves around during lessonsHas difficulty waiting for his/her turnTalks non-stop-----------------------------------------------------11% not part <strong>of</strong> core ADHD symptoms(Continues)


392CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivity<strong>Personality</strong> Inventory for <strong>Child</strong>ren -2(Lachar & Gruber, 2001)Impulsivity <strong>and</strong> Distractibility (27 items)My child’s manners sometimes embarrass meSchoolteachers complain that my child cannotsit stillMy child’s behavior <strong>of</strong>ten makes othersangryMy child <strong>of</strong>ten does not finish things thathe/she startsMy child jumps from one activity to anotherMy child <strong>of</strong>ten acts without thinkingMy child is <strong>of</strong>ten restlessI cannot get my child to do his/her schoollessonsMy child <strong>of</strong>ten forgets to do thingsMy child <strong>of</strong>ten nags <strong>and</strong> bothers otherpeopleMy child cannot wait for things like otherchildren doMy child cannot keep attention on anythingMy child does not learn from his/hermistakesMy child is almost always on time <strong>and</strong>remembers what he/she is supposed to doMy child cannot sit still in school because<strong>of</strong> nervousnessThe school says that my child needs helpin getting along with other childrenMy child usually runs rather than walksMy child tends to swallow food withoutchewing itRecently the school has sent home notesabout my child’s bad behaviorMy child seems more clumsy than otherchildren his/her ageMy child will do anything on a dareMy child brags about being sent to theprincipal at schoolNothing seems to scare my childMy child likes to show <strong>of</strong>fMy child tends to bragMoney seems to be my child’s biggestinterest--------------------------------------------63% not part <strong>of</strong> core ADHD symptomsStudent Behavior Survey (Lachar, Wingenfeld,Kline, & Gruber, 2000)Attention Deficit Hyperactivity (16 items)Completes class assignmentsDemonstrates logical approach to learningFollows teacher’s directionsMaintains alert <strong>and</strong> focused attentionPersists even when activity is difficultRemembers teacher’s directionsStays seatedWaits for turnWorks independently without disturbingothersListens to other studentsDaydreamsInterrupts othersImpulsiveMisbehaviors unless closely supervisedOveractiveTalks excessively--------------------------------------------25% not part <strong>of</strong> core ADHD symptoms


CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivity393There are several ratings scales that havebeen designed solely to assess behaviorsassociated with ADHD <strong>and</strong> provide muchmore extensive coverage <strong>of</strong> behaviors associatedwith this disorder. They include theADHD Rating Scale IV (DuPaul, Power,Anastopoulos, & Reid, 1998) <strong>and</strong> the DisruptiveBehavior Disorders Rating Scale(Pelham et al., 2005), both <strong>of</strong> which weredesigned to provide a complete coverage<strong>of</strong> the diagnostic criteria for ADHD. TheBrown Attention Deficit Disorder Scalesfor <strong>Child</strong>ren (Brown, 2001) <strong>and</strong> the AttentionDeficit Disorder Evaluation Scales(ADDES-3; McCarney, 2004) also provideextensive coverage <strong>of</strong> ADHD symptoms,although they were not explicitly tied toDSM-IV-TR criteria.Third, when selecting measures toassess the core features <strong>of</strong> ADHD, onemust obtain information from multiplesources (parents <strong>and</strong> teachers). This helpsone determine the situational variability<strong>of</strong> behaviors. Also, if the core features <strong>of</strong>ADHD can be assessed through multiplemodalities (e.g., rating scales <strong>and</strong> behavioralobservations), this negates the needto rely on any single imperfect assessmentinstrument. Therefore, one must choosea set <strong>of</strong> assessment instruments that providesa multi-informant <strong>and</strong> multi-methodassessment <strong>of</strong> the core ADHD behaviors.Fourth, one should have informationthat allows comparison to age norms. Mostdefinitions <strong>of</strong> ADHD either implicitly orexplicitly state that the symptoms shouldbe inconsistent with a child’s developmentallevel (American Psychiatric Association,2000). Therefore, an assessment mustprovide information that allows one tocompare a child’s behaviors to the behaviors<strong>of</strong> other children <strong>of</strong> a similar developmentallevel. Typically, behavior ratingscales are best suited for this task because<strong>of</strong> their extensive normative base. However,one important caution is in order inusing norm-referenced rating scales. Manyscales (e.g., ASEBA) <strong>of</strong>ten only providenorm-referenced scores broken down byage <strong>and</strong> gender. Definitions <strong>of</strong> ADHD donot make the restriction that the behaviorsbe inconsistent for a child’s gender. Infact, it is well accepted that boys are fourto six times more likely to show ADHDthan girls (American Psychiatric Association,2000). Using gender-specific normsignores this widely found <strong>and</strong> acceptedgender ratio <strong>and</strong> artificially equates thenumber <strong>of</strong> girls <strong>and</strong> boys with significantlevels <strong>of</strong> ADHD behaviors. This will leadto more girls <strong>and</strong> fewer boys being consideredto have significant ADHD behaviorsthan if cross-gender norms are used.Fifth, researchers studying children withADHD <strong>and</strong> attempting to define the coredeficit that may underlie this disorder (e.g.,response inhibition, sustained attention)have frequently used laboratory measures <strong>of</strong>inattention <strong>and</strong> impulsivity in their assessments.These measures place children inst<strong>and</strong>ardized conditions <strong>and</strong> attempt toquantitatively measure their inattentiveness,impulsiveness, or related behaviorsunder these conditions. For example, one<strong>of</strong> the most frequently used laboratorymeasures for the assessment <strong>of</strong> ADHD isthe Continuous Performance Test (CPT).There are many variations <strong>of</strong> the CPTbut a prototypical CPT is the one developedby Gordon (1983). In the GordonCPT, a child is told to view a screen onwhich numbers are presented. The childis instructed to press a button each time apredetermined number is presented. Tworesponses are measured. Omissions are thenumber <strong>of</strong> times the designated numberis presented to the child <strong>and</strong> the child failsto press the button. Number <strong>of</strong> omissionsis considered to be a measure <strong>of</strong> sustainedattention, especially increases in omissionsover time. Commissions occur when thechild incorrectly presses the button whenthe designated number is not presented <strong>and</strong>are considered a measure <strong>of</strong> impulsivity.There are many variations <strong>of</strong> the CPT(e.g., Conners, 1995), with some CPTtasks presenting auditory rather thanvisual stimuli <strong>and</strong> other tasks presenting


394CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivitydistracters (e.g., numbers flashing oneither side <strong>of</strong> the target stimuli that needto be ignored). Also, in addition to theCPT, there are many other laboratorymeasures that have been used in researchwith children who have ADHD. Rapport,Chung, Shore, Denney, <strong>and</strong> Isaacs (2000)review 142 studies using over 40 differentlaboratory measures to assess characteristicsassociated with ADHD. They identifyseveral characteristics <strong>of</strong> the tasks thatmost consistently differentiate childrenwith ADHD from other children. First,the tasks that most consistently showgroup differences rely on recognition,recall, or both (e.g., the letter or word thata child is supposed to look for is not continuouslydisplayed for the child), <strong>and</strong> they<strong>of</strong>ten involve some speed <strong>of</strong> processingcomponent. Also, most <strong>of</strong> the tasks thatreliably differentiate children with ADHDfrom other children place special dem<strong>and</strong>son a child’s working memory <strong>and</strong> each<strong>of</strong> the tasks is experimentally paced, notallowing the child to control the speed atwhich stimuli are presented. Rapport et al.(2000) provide an interesting discussionas to how these different task parametersmay provide clues to the specific deficitsdisplayed by children with ADHD.It is evident from this rather extensiveliterature that these tasks have been quiteuseful in studying children with ADHD.These tasks also have a number <strong>of</strong> appealingqualities for clinical assessments aswell. For example, they can help bridgethe gap between assessment instrumentsbeing used in research that guide our currentconceptualizations <strong>of</strong> ADHD <strong>and</strong>those commonly used in clinical practiceto make the diagnosis (Frick, 2000). Evenmore appealing, however, is that these laboratorytasks provide a potential means <strong>of</strong>assessing the symptoms <strong>of</strong> ADHD that arenot based on the perceptions <strong>of</strong> others thatcould be biased.Unfortunately, despite this promisefor their clinical utility, there are a number<strong>of</strong> limitations in their developmentthat make their clinical usefulness in thediagnosis <strong>of</strong> ADHD somewhat limited atpresent (Pelham et al., 2005; Smith et al.,2007). For example, the primary validation<strong>of</strong> these tasks has been to differentiateADHD children from non-ADHDchildren or to show the effects <strong>of</strong> stimulantmedication on the task performance<strong>of</strong> children with ADHD (Rapport et al.,2000). Unfortunately, both types <strong>of</strong> studiesrely on group-level data that are difficultto translate into findings that aremeaningful for interpreting an individualchild’s score (e.g., How many children withADHD do not score high on the laboratorymeasure? What percentage <strong>of</strong> treatedchildren show the response to medicationon the task?). Furthermore, it is not clearwhether performance differences on thesetasks are specific to children with ADHDor whether children with other types <strong>of</strong>behavioral or emotional disturbances alsoshow similar problems in performance.Without such information, it is difficultto use these measures to make differentialdiagnoses between ADHD <strong>and</strong> otherforms <strong>of</strong> psychopathology.Finally, scores from the laboratorymeasures generally show low correlationswith behavioral observations <strong>and</strong> parent<strong>and</strong> teacher reports (e.g., interviews <strong>and</strong>rating scales) <strong>of</strong> ADHD symptoms (Barkley,1991; DuPaul, Anastopoulos, Shelton,Guevremont, & Metevia, 1992). Furthermore,there is very little information as towhether or not the laboratory measuresadd any clinically useful information tothese more ecologically valid measures<strong>of</strong> behavior (Pelham et al., 2005; Rapportet al., 2000). For example, if a childis reported as showing significant problemswith ADHD symptoms accordingto parent <strong>and</strong> teacher reports <strong>and</strong> basedon behavioral observations in the classroom,interventions for his behavioralproblems in his natural environment willlikely be the same, irrespective <strong>of</strong> his performanceon the laboratory task. Becausetreatment decisions are largely based on


CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivity395the more ecologically valid measures, therole <strong>of</strong> laboratory measures in clinicalassessments is uncertain at this point. InBox 17.4, we summarize Barkley’s (1991)review <strong>of</strong> laboratory measures <strong>of</strong> ADHDsymptoms, which provide a more indepthdiscussion <strong>of</strong> these measures’ ecological<strong>and</strong> incremental validity.Box 17.4Research Note: Ecological Validity <strong>of</strong> Laboratory Measures <strong>of</strong> Attention <strong>and</strong>ImpulsivityBarkley (1991) provides a critical discussion<strong>of</strong> laboratory measures (LM) used to assessattention <strong>and</strong> impulsivity, two aspects <strong>of</strong> thecore symptoms <strong>of</strong> ADHD. The focus <strong>of</strong> much<strong>of</strong> Barkley’s discussion is on reaction timetasks (RTT), continuous performance tasks(CPT), <strong>and</strong> the Matching Familiar FiguresTest (MFFT). Although Barkley also discussesanalogue observations <strong>of</strong> motor activity, thesewere discussed previously in Chap. 8.Concept <strong>of</strong> Ecological ValidityThe first issue addressed in this article is theconcept <strong>of</strong> ecological validity. Barkley definedecological validity as “the degree to whichLMs represent the actual behaviors <strong>of</strong> interest(i.e., inattention <strong>and</strong> impulsivity) as they occurin naturalistic settings” (p. 150). This aspect<strong>of</strong> validity is crucial for clinical assessments. Incontrast, most research projects have the goal <strong>of</strong>determining the “core deficit” in ADHD, whichmay not be manifested in the natural setting.Therefore, a LM “need not be ecologically validto be useful in research on ADHD” (p. 151).Evidence for Ecological ValidityOne type <strong>of</strong> research on the ecological validity<strong>of</strong> LMs consists <strong>of</strong> studies testing group differenceson LMs between ADHD <strong>and</strong> controlchildren. In general, Barkley’s review suggestedthat all <strong>of</strong> the LMs have consistently differentiatedADHD from normal control children.However, most studies indicate that these effectsare weakened or altogether eliminated whendifferences in intellectual level are controlled.Also, LMs do not differentiate ADHD childrenfrom other clinic-referred children. As a result,scores on the LMs are not useful in makingdifferential diagnoses within clinic referrals.A second type <strong>of</strong> research investigates theeffect <strong>of</strong> stimulant medication on a child’s performanceon the LMs. Barkley’s conclusion wasthat the research evidence was mixed. Severalstudies found significant effects for stimulantmedication on commonly used LMs, whereasmany studies found no such reliable effect.A third type <strong>of</strong> research investigates thecorrelations between scores on LMs <strong>and</strong> parent<strong>and</strong> teacher ratings <strong>of</strong> ADHD behavior.Studies have generally found significant butmodest (.21–.51) correlations between LMmeasures <strong>of</strong> attention <strong>and</strong> impulsivity <strong>and</strong>parent <strong>and</strong> teacher ratings <strong>of</strong> behaviors consideredto be indicative <strong>of</strong> these constructs.ConclusionsBarkley concludes that the ecological validity <strong>of</strong>LMs should be considered limited based on theexisting research. As a result, clinical researchshould avoid using LMs as the “gold st<strong>and</strong>ard”against which other measures <strong>of</strong> attention <strong>and</strong>impulsivity are judged. For clinical assessments,one should recognize the limited usefulness <strong>of</strong>LMs in a battery <strong>of</strong> assessment tests. Most clinicaldecisions are based on the more ecologicallyvalid measures <strong>of</strong> parent <strong>and</strong> teacher report <strong>of</strong> achild’s behavior in the naturalistic setting. Barkleyconcludes, “Where LM results conflict withthose obtained from other sources, such as parent<strong>and</strong> teacher behavior ratings, history, <strong>and</strong>observations in natural settings, the LM resultsshould probably be disregarded in favor <strong>of</strong> thesemore ecologically valid sources” (p. 173). As aresult, the incremental benefit <strong>of</strong> adding LMsto an assessment battery seems to be minimal.Source: Barkley, R. A. (1991). The ecological validity <strong>of</strong> laboratory <strong>and</strong> analogue assessmentmethods <strong>of</strong> ADHD. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 19, 149–178.


396CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit HyperactivityOne final note is in order for assessing thecore features <strong>of</strong> ADHD. Despite widespreadcautions against the practice (e.g., Barkley, 1990;Kamphaus, 2001; Kaufman, 1994), clinical assessorscontinue to use the Freedom from Distractibility(FD) factor from the Wechsler IntelligenceScales for <strong>Child</strong>ren (Wechsler, 1991) as an indicator<strong>of</strong> the presence <strong>of</strong> ADHD symptoms. Thisinappropriate use <strong>of</strong> FD is largely maintainedsimply because <strong>of</strong> the name given to the factor,which is a significant source <strong>of</strong> distress to the personwho first applied the FD label to the WISCsubtests. Alan Kaufman states, “The label shouldhave been trashed years ago. I cringe whenever Iread it” (Kaufman, 1994, p. 212).As Kaufman goes on to explain, theuse <strong>of</strong> the FD as a measure <strong>of</strong> inattentionor distractibility is inappropriatebecause these scales are affected by multipleemotional <strong>and</strong> behavioral factors, notjust distractibility. To illustrate the point,Kaufman reviewed 19 studies in which theFD was lower in samples <strong>of</strong> children withlearning disabilities, children with leukemia,children with emotional difficulties,heterogeneous psychiatric samples <strong>of</strong>inpatient or outpatient children, childrenwith autism, children with schizophrenia,children with Conduct Disorder, <strong>and</strong> childrenwith muscular dystrophy. The authorthen makes the cogent point that the FDfactor is important because <strong>of</strong> its robustability to differentiate the abnormal (medically,behaviorally, <strong>and</strong> educationally) fromthe normal population, but it is basicallymeaningless for identifying a specific type<strong>of</strong> exceptionality, such as ADHD.Assessing Comorbid ProblemsMany <strong>of</strong> the problems that <strong>of</strong>ten co-occurwith ADHD can be assessed in conjunctionwith the assessment <strong>of</strong> the core ADHDbehaviors. For example, many <strong>of</strong> the omnibusrating scales <strong>and</strong> structured interviewsdiscussed in previous chapters includeitems that assess for conduct problems,anxiety, depression, self-esteem, <strong>and</strong> socialcompetence. Like the ADHD behaviorsthemselves, potential co-occurring problemsare best assessed through multipleinformants <strong>and</strong> using multiple formats.Given the overlap with learning problems,psycho-educational testing should also be apart <strong>of</strong> most ADHD assessments. Learningdifficulties are not reliably assessed throughrating scales, interviews, behavioral observations,or projective testing. As a result,st<strong>and</strong>ardized intelligence <strong>and</strong> achievementtests are <strong>of</strong>ten required as part <strong>of</strong> a comprehensiveevaluation for ADHD.Some omnibus behavior rating scalesinclude items that assess the degree <strong>of</strong> familyconflict <strong>and</strong> other aspects <strong>of</strong> a child’s familycontext. However, as discussed in Chap. 12,some assessments may require a more indepthassessment <strong>of</strong> specific areas <strong>of</strong> familyfunctioning that are better obtained throughmethods that specifically focus on the child’sfamily context. Each aspect <strong>of</strong> family functioningthat was highlighted in Chap. 12(parenting styles <strong>and</strong> behaviors, parentingstress, marital conflict, <strong>and</strong> parental adjustment)is important in underst<strong>and</strong>ing thefamily context <strong>of</strong> a child with ADHD.Assessing Potential AlternativeCausesOne <strong>of</strong> the most difficult aspects <strong>of</strong> assessingfor ADHD is ruling out alternative causes forthe symptoms. Probably, the most importantpiece <strong>of</strong> information for this purpose hasalready been discussed. If one has adequatelydetermined that the symptoms <strong>of</strong> ADHD are<strong>of</strong> sufficient number, severity, <strong>and</strong> duration<strong>and</strong> cause enough impairment to warrant adiagnosis, most <strong>of</strong> the alternative explanationsfor symptoms can be ruled out as a soleexplanation for the behaviors. Many <strong>of</strong> thealternative explanations for ADHD symptomsresult in behaviors similar to ADHD,but <strong>of</strong> lower intensity <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong> shorter durationthan is typical for children with ADHD.Given that some medical <strong>and</strong> neurologicaldisorders can manifest in problems <strong>of</strong>


CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivity397inattention-disorganization <strong>and</strong> impulsivityhyperactivity,<strong>and</strong> given that such behaviorscan be side effects <strong>of</strong> certain medications, itis important that a thorough developmental<strong>and</strong> medical history be obtained on a childwhen assessing for ADHD. When the historyis suggestive <strong>of</strong> the possible presence<strong>of</strong> a medical or neurological disorder, thechild can be referred to a physician for amore comprehensive medical <strong>and</strong>/or neurologicalexam. However, most experts onADHD feel that a full medical exam neednot be a st<strong>and</strong>ard part <strong>of</strong> a diagnostic assessment<strong>of</strong> ADHD, because the diagnosis isprimarily based on behavioral data (Barkley,1997a; Pelham et al., 2005).The greatest difficulty in ruling outalternative explanations for ADHD symptomsis the problem <strong>of</strong> determining whatis primary <strong>and</strong> what is secondary. Basedon the research on comorbidities, weknow that ADHD children are at risk formany other problems in adjustment, most<strong>of</strong> which can also mask as ADHD (e.g.,learning disabilities, emotional disorders).The distinction between primary <strong>and</strong> secondaryis largely a clinical one, based ona complex weighing <strong>of</strong> various pieces <strong>of</strong>assessment information. To summarize oursuggestions from Chap. 15, several types<strong>of</strong> information may be helpful in makingthis difficult case formulation. Consideringthe level <strong>of</strong> impairment associated withdifferent areas <strong>of</strong> dysfunction (i.e., whichproblem areas seem to be causing the mostproblems for the child) <strong>and</strong> the temporalsequencing <strong>of</strong> problem behaviors (i.e.,which problems seem to predate others)can help in distinguishing primary or secondaryareas <strong>of</strong> dysfunction. In addition,viewing family history data <strong>and</strong> determiningif a child might be at risk for a certaintype <strong>of</strong> problem given its occurrence inrelatives may also aid in making differentialdiagnoses. Specifically, for ADHD,determining if a child’s parents or otherfirst-degree relatives had childhood histories<strong>of</strong> ADHD symptoms can providevaluable information in making an ADHDdiagnosis. In contrast, a positive familyhistory for bipolar illness might warrantfurther assessment to determine if a child’smotor restlessness <strong>and</strong> problems <strong>of</strong> impulsivecontrol are early indicators <strong>of</strong> an affectivedisturbance (Smith et al., 2007).ADHD in the Schools: SpecialEducation PlacementADHD is a psychological syndrome thatrequires collaboration between pr<strong>of</strong>essionalsacross multiple specialties (i.e., psychology,education, medicine) for assessment<strong>and</strong> treatment. <strong>Clinical</strong> assessors must beable to effectively utilize the expertise <strong>of</strong>other pr<strong>of</strong>essionals <strong>and</strong> tailor their assessmentsto provide useful information tomany different disciplines. Because ADHD<strong>of</strong>ten has its most dramatic <strong>and</strong> noticeableeffect on a child in the school setting, collaborationwith educators is particularlycrucial. It is essential that assessments forADHD be designed to provide informationhelpful to educators in developingappropriate interventions.We feel strongly that not all childrenwith ADHD require special educationplacement. There is ample evidence thatsome children with ADHD can successfullyfunction in a regular education classroomwith medication, with specific modifications<strong>of</strong> the classroom environment, <strong>and</strong>/or with structured behavioral interventionsdesigned to reduce the disruptive behaviors(see Abramowitz & O’Leary, 1991). However,designing a successful interventionprogram for a child with ADHD requires aclear documentation <strong>of</strong> a child’s strengths<strong>and</strong> weaknesses (behaviorally, emotionally,cognitively, <strong>and</strong> academically). Simplyknowing that a child has ADHD gives educatorsonly limited information on whichto base interventions, given the greatdiversity within children with ADHD. As


398CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivitya result, clinical assessments should clearlyoutline an individual child’s competencies<strong>and</strong> deficits, with suggestions on how theseabilities will influence a child’s functioningin a classroom setting.Unfortunately, some children withADHD may require more intensive educationalservices, such as those <strong>of</strong>fered inspecial education programs. To serve childrenin special education programs, schoolsystems must operate under federal <strong>and</strong>state guidelines, the former <strong>of</strong> which mayvary in implementation from state to state<strong>and</strong> the latter <strong>of</strong> which may vary both incontent <strong>and</strong> implementation. It is imperativethat clinical assessors underst<strong>and</strong> theguidelines under which a school system isoperating, in order to design assessments<strong>and</strong> make recommendations that enhancea school’s ability to appropriately meet theeducational needs <strong>of</strong> a child.The primary piece <strong>of</strong> federal legislationthat guides provision <strong>of</strong> special educationservices to children <strong>and</strong> adolescents is theIndividuals with Disabilities Act (IDEA),which was originally passed in 1975 asPublic Law 94–142 <strong>and</strong> amended <strong>and</strong>renamed in 1990 <strong>and</strong> 2004. IDEA, <strong>and</strong> themore recent IDEIA, m<strong>and</strong>ates that childrenwith disabilities be provided speciallydesigned instruction <strong>and</strong> related services inthe least restrictive environment (i.e., closecontact with children without disabilities)necessary for a child to learn. Providingservices to children with ADHD underIDEA has been a source <strong>of</strong> contention inmany school systems, because ADHD isnot listed as one <strong>of</strong> the disabilities explicitlycovered under IDEA. However, manychildren with ADHD have secondary featuresthat may allow them to be servedunder IDEA guidelines, such as speech orlanguage impairments, emotional disturbance,or specific learning disabilities. Infact, most <strong>of</strong> the children with ADHD whorequire intensive special education servicesdo so, not because <strong>of</strong> the ADHD itself,but because <strong>of</strong> the additional disruptionscaused by these secondary features.However, it is possible that some childrenwith ADHD may need special educationservices but do not qualify underIDEA guidelines. These children can beserved under a civil rights law, Section 504.Section 504 is part <strong>of</strong> the RehabilitationAct Amendments <strong>of</strong> 1973 (PL93–112) <strong>and</strong>was designed to protect individuals withh<strong>and</strong>icaps. Section 504 specifically m<strong>and</strong>atesthat,No otherwise qualified individual with h<strong>and</strong>icapsin the United States, shall, solely by reason<strong>of</strong> her or his h<strong>and</strong>icap, be excluded fromparticipation in, denied the benefits <strong>of</strong>, or besubjected to, discrimination under any programor activity receiving Federal financial assistance(29 U.S.C. Sec. 794).It is generally accepted that ADHD qualifiesas a h<strong>and</strong>icap under Section 504 (Madsen,1990) <strong>and</strong>, therefore, schools mustmake accommodations for the individualneeds <strong>of</strong> children with ADHD under thisstatute. Unfortunately, unlike IDEA, Section504 does not allocate federal fundingfor educational interventions.This discussion <strong>of</strong> special education lawsmay seem irrelevant or at least peripheralfor clinical assessors who operate outside <strong>of</strong>the school system. In fact, many such assessorsprefer to remain ignorant <strong>of</strong> such legalguidelines, so as not to be confined to thelimits delineated in such laws. However,if one wants to aid a child with ADHD inreceiving needed educational services, oneshould underst<strong>and</strong> the legal guidelines soas to be able to work collaboratively withschool personnel in developing an appropriateeducational plan.Conduct ProblemsClassification <strong>and</strong> DiagnosisLike ADHD, conduct problems in childrenrepresent a critical mental health concern.These problems are highly disruptive


CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivity399to others in a child’s environment can bepredictive <strong>of</strong> problems later in life, includingcriminal behavior (Frick & Kimonis,2008). Also like ADHD, there is considerableagreement that children with conductproblems are a heterogeneous group (Frick,2006). Therefore, a substantial body <strong>of</strong>research has been directed at determiningthe most appropriate method <strong>of</strong> classifyingconduct problems into meaningful subtypes.The most commonly used method <strong>of</strong>classifying conduct problems in childrenis a two-dimensional approach originallydescribed in DSM-III <strong>and</strong> continuedwith some modifications in the laterrevisions <strong>of</strong> this manual. This systemdivides conduct problems into two syndromes:Oppositional Defiant Disorder(ODD) <strong>and</strong> Conduct Disorder (CD).ODD refers to a pattern <strong>of</strong> negativistic,oppositional, <strong>and</strong> stubborn behaviors,whereas CD refers to more severeantisocial <strong>and</strong> aggressive behaviors thatinvolve serious violations <strong>of</strong> others’rights (e.g., aggression, destruction <strong>of</strong>property) or deviations from major ageappropriatenorms (e.g., running awayfrom home, truancy). A summary <strong>of</strong> thebehaviors indicative <strong>of</strong> these two dimensionsis provided in Table 17.4.A detailed discussion <strong>of</strong> the validity <strong>of</strong>the ODD/CD distinction is beyond thescope <strong>of</strong> this chapter (see McMahon &Frick, 2007). However, the relationshipbetween ODD <strong>and</strong> CD behaviors is importantfor a number <strong>of</strong> reasons. First, thereappears to be a hierarchical relationshipbetween the two diagnoses. That is, mostchildren with the more severe symptoms<strong>of</strong> CD also show the symptoms <strong>of</strong> ODD(Lahey & Loeber, 1994; Spitzer, Davies,& Barkley, 1990). However, the converseis not true. There are many children withODD who do not show the more seriousconduct problems associated withCD. Second, there seems to be a developmentalrelationship between ODD <strong>and</strong>CD. A 3-year longitudinal study <strong>of</strong> clinicreferredboys found that 82% <strong>of</strong> the newcases <strong>of</strong> CD (n = 22) that emerged duringthe study period had received a diagnosis<strong>of</strong> ODD in the preceding year (Lahey,Loeber, Quay, Frick, & Grimm, 1992).Therefore, ODD behaviors can be viewedas a risk factor for the development <strong>of</strong> themore severe CD.Table 17.4 Two-Dimensional Classification <strong>of</strong> Conduct ProblemsOppositional Defiant DisorderConduct DisorderLoses temperBullies or intimidates othersArgues with adultsInitiates physical fightsActively defies adultsHas been physically cruel to othersRefuses adults’ requests or rules Steals things <strong>of</strong> nontrivial valueDeliberately annoys others Forced someone into sexual activityBlames others for mistakes Stays out after dark without parental permission, before age 13Is angry <strong>and</strong> resentfulLies to obtain goods or favorsIs spiteful <strong>and</strong> vindictiveHas been physically cruel to animalsIs touchy <strong>and</strong> easily annoyed Has deliberately destroyed others’ propertyHas set fires with intention <strong>of</strong> causing serious damageHas run away from home overnight more than onceIs <strong>of</strong>ten truant from school, beginning before age 13Has broken into someone’s house, building, or car


400CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit HyperactivityComorbiditiesThe most common problem co-occurringwith conduct problems is ADHD. In ameta-analytic study <strong>of</strong> community studies,Waschbusch (2002) reported that 36% <strong>of</strong>boys <strong>and</strong> 57% <strong>of</strong> girls with conduct problemshad comorbid ADHD. When studyingpre-adolescent children <strong>and</strong> childrenreferred to clinics, the rate is even higher<strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>ten ranges from 75 to 90% (Abik<strong>of</strong>f& Klein, 1992; Hinshaw, 1987). Thisdegree <strong>of</strong> overlap has led to a debate as towhether or not ADHD <strong>and</strong> conduct problemsshould even be considered separatepsychological domains (see Rutter, 1983).We feel that the research indicates thatthese domains are at least partially independent(Frick, 1994; Hinshaw, 1987).Research does suggest that the development<strong>of</strong> ADHD usually precedes the development<strong>of</strong> conduct problems (Waschbusch,2002) <strong>and</strong> it <strong>of</strong>ten signals the presence <strong>of</strong> amore severe <strong>and</strong> more chronic form <strong>of</strong> conductproblems in children (Frick & Loney,1999). Also, there is growing evidence thatconduct problems improve when childrenwith ADHD have been treated with stimulantmedication (Hinshaw, 1991). Therefore,clinical assessments <strong>of</strong> children withconduct problems should routinely assessfor the presence <strong>of</strong> ADHD.<strong>Child</strong>ren with conduct problems als<strong>of</strong>requently have a comorbid anxiety disorder,<strong>and</strong> this seems to be especially thecase for girls (Loeber & Keenan, 1994).<strong>Child</strong>ren with conduct disorders alsoshow a high rate <strong>of</strong> depression (Harrington,Fudge, Rutter, Pickles, & Hill,1991). Importantly, there is evidence thatchildren with both conduct problems <strong>and</strong>depression show a high rate <strong>of</strong> suicidalideation (Capaldi, 1992). The combination<strong>of</strong> suicidal ideation, depression, <strong>and</strong>poor impulse control that <strong>of</strong>ten is presentin children with conduct problemshas been associated with increased risk forsuicide (Shaffer, Garl<strong>and</strong>, Gould, Fisher,& Trautman, 1988). Finally, childrenwith CD are at higher risk for substanceabuse, especially those children with bothADHD <strong>and</strong> CD (Lynskey & Fergusson,1995; Thompson, Riggs, Mikulich, &Crowley, 1996).Research also indicates that approximately20–25% <strong>of</strong> children with CD areunderachieving in school relative to a levelpredicted by their age <strong>and</strong> intellectualabilities (Frick et al., 1991). The reasonfor this association is not clear, possiblybecause the mechanisms involved maydiffer depending on the age <strong>of</strong> the samplestudied (Hinshaw, 1992). For example, inelementary school-age samples, much <strong>of</strong>the overlap between CD <strong>and</strong> academicunderachievement seems to be due tothe presence <strong>of</strong> ADHD (Frick et al.,1991). However, learning difficultiesseem to predict adolescent-onset conductproblems independent <strong>of</strong> other factors(Hinshaw, 1992). Despite the lack <strong>of</strong> adefinitive explanation for the correlationbetween learning <strong>and</strong> conduct problems,the simple fact that they co-occur so consistentlywarrants the assessment <strong>of</strong> learningproblems when assessing children <strong>and</strong>adolescents with conduct problems.Correlates with PotentialCausal RolesMost researchers agree that conduct problemsare the result <strong>of</strong> a complex interaction<strong>of</strong> multiple causal factors (Frick, 2006).Identifying the important causal agents<strong>and</strong> how they interact to cause conductproblems is still an area in need <strong>of</strong> moreresearch. Past research has uncovered severalfactors that are associated with conductdisorders <strong>and</strong> that likely play a role in theirdevelopment. These factors can be summarizedin five categories: biological factors,cognitive correlates, family context,social ecology, <strong>and</strong> peers. The research onthe biological correlates <strong>of</strong> conduct problemsin children, while crucial for developingcausal theories, is not reviewed here


CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivity401because the current state <strong>of</strong> knowledge isnot sufficiently developed to have clearimplications for assessment (see Dodge &Pettit, 2003; Lahey, Hart, Pliszka, Applegate,& McBurnett, 1993; Raine, 2002).In contrast, there are several aspects <strong>of</strong>the child’s cognitive <strong>and</strong> learning styles thathave been associated with conduct problems<strong>and</strong> aggression that may be importantto the assessment process (see Frick& Loney, 2000). First, in general, childrenwith conduct disorders tend to score lowon intelligence tests, especially in the area<strong>of</strong> verbal intelligence (Loney, Frick, Ellis,& McCoy, 1998; M<strong>of</strong>fitt, 1993). Second,many children with serious conduct problemstend to show a learning style that ismore sensitive to rewards than punishments.This has been labeled as a rewarddominantresponse style that could explainwhy many <strong>of</strong> these children persist in theirmaladaptive behaviors, despite the threat<strong>of</strong> serious potential consequences (Fisher& Blair, 1998; O’Brien & Frick, 1996).Third, many children with conduct problemsshow deficits in their social cognition,which is the way they interpret social cues<strong>and</strong> use them to respond in social situations.For example, children with conductproblems are more likely to attribute hostileintent to the actions <strong>of</strong> peers <strong>and</strong> areless able to develop nonaggressive responsealternatives in situations involving peerconflict, both <strong>of</strong> which could make thechild more likely to respond aggressivelyin social situations (Crick & Dodge, 1996;Dodge & Pettit, 2003).While these cognitive correlates haveplayed an important role in many theories<strong>of</strong> how conduct disorders develop,<strong>and</strong> they are important targets <strong>of</strong> interventionfor many treatment programs(e.g., Lochman, Wells, & Lenhart, 2008),there is probably no set <strong>of</strong> correlates thathas been as important to theory <strong>and</strong> treatmentas family dysfunction. There seemto be at least three dimensions <strong>of</strong> familyfunctioning that are consistently relatedto childhood conduct problems: parentalpsychiatric adjustment, marital instability/divorce, <strong>and</strong> parental socialization practices(see Frick, 1994). A meta-analysis <strong>of</strong>the research on the relationship betweenfamily functioning <strong>and</strong> conduct disordersin youth found that parental socializationpractices were especially important(Loeber & Stouthamer-Loeber, 1986). Tobe specific, parental involvement in theirchild’s activities, parental supervision <strong>of</strong>their child, <strong>and</strong> the use <strong>of</strong> harsh or inconsistentdiscipline tended to show the strongestrelationships with conduct problemsin children <strong>of</strong> all the variables included inthe meta-analysis.Another clinically important class <strong>of</strong>correlates is comprised <strong>of</strong> factors withinthe child’s larger social ecology that mayplay a causal role in the development <strong>of</strong>conduct problems. One <strong>of</strong> the most consistentlydocumented <strong>of</strong> these correlateshas been low socio-economic status (Frick,Lahey, Hartdagen, & Hynd, 1989). However,several other ecological factors, many<strong>of</strong> which are related to low socio-economicstatus, such as poor housing, poor schools,<strong>and</strong> disadvantaged neighborhoods, havealso been linked to the development <strong>of</strong> conductproblems in children (see Frick, 1998;Peeples & Loeber, 1994). In addition, thehigh rate <strong>of</strong> violence, witnessed by childrenwho live in impoverished inner-cityneighborhoods, has also been linked to thedevelopment <strong>of</strong> conduct problems (Os<strong>of</strong>sky,Wewers, Hann, & Fick, 1993).Finally, research has documented a relationshipbetween peer rejection in elementaryschool <strong>and</strong> the later development <strong>of</strong>conduct problems (R<strong>of</strong>f & Wirt, 1984). Inaddition, peer rejection in elementary schoolis predictive <strong>of</strong> an association with a deviantpeer group (i.e., one that shows a highrate <strong>of</strong> antisocial behavior <strong>and</strong> substanceabuse) in early adolescence (Fergusson,Swain, & Horwood, 2002). This relationshipis important because association witha deviant peer group leads to an increase


402CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivityin the frequency <strong>and</strong> severity <strong>of</strong> conductproblems (Patterson & Dishion, 1985) <strong>and</strong>it has proven to be a strong predictor <strong>of</strong>later delinquency <strong>and</strong> other negative outcomes,such as substance abuse (Dishion,Capaldi, Spracklen, & Li, 1995; Fergussonet al., 2002). Therefore, peer rejection maybe directly related to the development <strong>of</strong>conduct problems but also may indirectlyinfluence conduct problems by increasingthe chance that the child or adolescent willassociate with deviant peers.Conceptual ModelFrom the preceding section, it is clear thatthere has been a great deal <strong>of</strong> research documentingmany characteristics <strong>of</strong> childrenwith severe conduct problems that areimportant to consider in conducting assessmentswith these children. Unfortunately,there has not been much agreement as to agood framework for organizing these manydiverse characteristics into a clear conceptualframework for underst<strong>and</strong>ing howconduct problems develop. Many theoristshave tended to focus on either one correlate(e.g., community violence) or oneclass <strong>of</strong> correlates (e.g., family dysfunction)without attempting to integrate the manydiverse <strong>and</strong> potentially interacting influencesthat play a role in the development<strong>of</strong> conduct problems. Others have viewedthese problems from a “cumulative risk”perspective, which acknowledges that anysingle factor will be limited for explainingthe development <strong>of</strong> conduct problems <strong>and</strong>that their development in any child is likelythe result <strong>of</strong> the additive influence <strong>of</strong> manydifferent causal factors (Loeber, Burke,Lahey, Winters, & Zera, 2000).An alternative framework that is gainingsupport in research is a “developmentalpathway approach,” which proposesthat children may develop conduct problemsthrough many different causal trajectories,each involving somewhat differentinteractions <strong>of</strong> causal processes (Frick,2006). This approach recognizes that, forany child, the development <strong>of</strong> serious conductproblems is likely the result <strong>of</strong> multipleinteracting causal factors. In addition,a developmental framework explicitlyrecognizes that there may be distinct subgroups<strong>of</strong> children with severe conductproblems with different causal processesunderlying their behavior. This approachis consistent with a long history <strong>of</strong> tryingto divide antisocial <strong>and</strong> delinquent youthinto distinct subgroups that differ in terms<strong>of</strong> behavior, associated characteristics,outcomes, <strong>and</strong> response to treatment (seeFrick & Marsee, 2006 for a review).Consistent with this research, the DSM-IV-TR specifies two subtypes <strong>of</strong> ConductDisorder (CD; American PsychiatricAssociation, 2000). <strong>Child</strong>ren in the childhood-onsetsubtype begin showing severeconduct problems prior to adolescence,<strong>of</strong>ten as early as preschool or early elementaryschool, <strong>and</strong> their behavioral problemsincrease in rate <strong>and</strong> severity over thechildhood years (Lahey & Loeber, 1994).In contrast, youth in the adolescent-onsetsubtype do not show significant behavioralproblems in childhood, but begin to exhibitsignificant conduct problems as they enteradolescence (M<strong>of</strong>fitt, 1993, 2003). One<strong>of</strong> the key differences between these twogroups <strong>of</strong> antisocial youth is that the childhood-onsetgroup is much more likely tocontinue to show antisocial <strong>and</strong> criminalbehavior into adulthood compared to theadolescent-onset group (Frick & Loney,1999; M<strong>of</strong>fitt, 2003). However, in additionto the differences in prognosis, researchhas uncovered several other characteristicsthat could suggest that the causal processesunderlying the antisocial behavior <strong>of</strong> thetwo groups are also different.Specifically, children in the childhoodonsetgroup are characterized by moreaggression, higher rates <strong>of</strong> cognitive (e.g.,lower verbal intelligence) <strong>and</strong> neuropsychological(e.g., executive functioning


CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivity403deficits) dysfunction, more disturbances intheir autonomic nervous system functioning,<strong>and</strong> more severe problems <strong>of</strong> impulsecontrol, <strong>of</strong>ten leading to higher rates <strong>of</strong>diagnosis <strong>of</strong> Attention-Deficit HyperactivityDisorder than children with the adolescent-onsetpattern <strong>of</strong> CD (Frick, 2006;M<strong>of</strong>fitt, 1993; 2003). The fewer pathogenicbackground factors, as well as thebetter adult outcome for the adolescentonsetsubtype, suggests that their conductproblems may be an exaggeration <strong>of</strong> a normativepattern <strong>of</strong> rebellious <strong>and</strong> antisocialbehavior related to the important tasksinvolved in identity development that takeplace in adolescence (Frick, 2006; M<strong>of</strong>fitt,2003; Silverthorn & Frick, 1999). In contrast,the childhood-onset group appearsto show a more severe type <strong>of</strong> dysfunctionthat extends beyond a single developmentalstage.Importantly, research has also indicatedthat there are some important distinctionsthat can be made within this childhoodonsetgroup in terms <strong>of</strong> the types <strong>of</strong> dysfunctionalprocesses that may be operating.The distinction is based on differentiatingbetween children who show a callous <strong>and</strong>unemotional interpersonal style <strong>and</strong> thosewho do not. Callous-unemotional traitsrefer to a lack <strong>of</strong> guilt over misdeeds, a lack<strong>of</strong> empathy toward others, <strong>and</strong> other deficitsin their emotional responses (Frick &Dickens, 2006; Frick & White, 2008).<strong>Child</strong>ren with conduct problems, whoalso show callous-unemotional traits,tend to show a more severe, more aggressive,<strong>and</strong> more stable pattern <strong>of</strong> conductproblems (Frick & Dickens, 2006). Further,these children with callous-unemotionaltraits tend to be more thrill <strong>and</strong>adventure seeking, they are less sensitiveto the effects <strong>of</strong> punishment compared tothe effects <strong>of</strong> rewards, <strong>and</strong> they are lessreactive to emotionally distressing stimulithan other children with childhoodonsetCD (Frick & White, 2008). All <strong>of</strong>these characteristics are consistent with atemperamental style associated with lowemotional reactivity that can (1) placea child at risk for missing some <strong>of</strong> theearly precursors to empathetic concernthat involve emotional arousal evokedby the misfortune <strong>and</strong> distress <strong>of</strong> others;(2) lead a child to be relatively insensitiveto the prohibitions <strong>and</strong> sanctions<strong>of</strong> parents <strong>and</strong> other socializing agents;<strong>and</strong> (3) create an interpersonal style inwhich the child becomes so focused onthe potential rewards <strong>and</strong> gains involvedin using aggression to solve interpersonalconflicts that he or she ignores the potentiallyharmful effects <strong>of</strong> this behavior onhim- or herself <strong>and</strong> others (Frick & Morris,2004).In contrast to those youth with callous<strong>and</strong> unemotional traits, children withinthe childhood-onset group who do notshow these traits tend to show the oppositeextreme <strong>of</strong> emotional reactivity. Theytend to be highly reactive to emotional <strong>and</strong>threatening stimuli <strong>and</strong> to respond morestrongly to provocations in social situations(Frick, 2006; Frick & Morris, 2004). Also,their aggressive <strong>and</strong> antisocial behavior ismore strongly associated with dysfunctionalparenting practices <strong>and</strong> with deficitsin verbal intelligence than the group thatis high on callous-unemotional traits (Frick& White, 2008). These findings suggestthat children with childhood-onset antisocialbehavior, but who do not show highrates <strong>of</strong> callous-unemotional traits, mayhave problems more specifically associatedwith poor behavioral <strong>and</strong> emotionalregulation, characterized by very impulsivebehavior <strong>and</strong> high levels <strong>of</strong> emotionalreactivity. Such poor emotional regulationcan result from a number <strong>of</strong> interactingcausal factors, such as inadequate socializationfrom families, deficits in their verbalintelligence that make it difficult for themto delay gratification <strong>and</strong> anticipate consequences,or temperamental problems inresponse inhibition such as those discussedin the previous section on ADHD. The


404CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivityproblems in emotional regulation can leadto very impulsive <strong>and</strong> unplanned aggressiveacts for which the child may be remorsefulafterward but that he or she still hasdifficulty controlling. It can also lead to achild being susceptible to becoming angry(i.e., emotionally aroused) due to perceivedprovocations from peers, leading to violent<strong>and</strong> aggressive acts within the context <strong>of</strong>high emotional arousal.This developmental framework has anumber <strong>of</strong> implications for the assessmentprocess for children with severe conductproblems (McMahon & Frick, 2005). Itsuggests that, not only do interventionsfor children with severe conduct problemsneed to be comprehensive in targeting alarge number <strong>of</strong> diverse causal influences,but these interventions also need to be tailoredto the unique needs <strong>of</strong> specific subgroups<strong>of</strong> children with conduct problems(see Frick, 2006 for a more a extendeddiscussion <strong>of</strong> this issue). In order to implementsuch a comprehensive <strong>and</strong> individualizedapproach to treatment, there needsto be a comprehensive assessment <strong>of</strong> thechild that identifies the most appropriatetargets <strong>of</strong> treatment, given the individualchild’s specific developmental history.Implications for <strong>Assessment</strong>This body <strong>of</strong> psychological researchforms the basis for designing an appropriateassessment for children with conductproblems. In Table 17.5, we summarizethe critical areas <strong>of</strong> research <strong>and</strong> their relevanceto the assessment process. In Box17.5, we provide a case study <strong>of</strong> a comprehensiveevaluation <strong>of</strong> a child with severeconduct problems. As is evident fromTable 17.5, assessment <strong>of</strong> conduct problemsshares several important characteristicswith the assessment <strong>of</strong> ADHD. Thecomplex <strong>and</strong> pervasive nature <strong>of</strong> conductproblems requires a comprehensive evaluationthat assesses many aspects <strong>of</strong> thechild’s functioning <strong>and</strong> psychosocial environment.Further, conduct problems <strong>and</strong>other relevant aspects <strong>of</strong> a child’s psychosocialfunctioning should be assessed usingmultiple informants <strong>and</strong> multiple assessmenttechniques.Table 17.5 Key Research Findings <strong>and</strong> Their Implications for the <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong>renwith Conduct ProblemsResearch Findings1. Core symptoms <strong>and</strong> subtypes: Conduct disordersrepresent a heterogeneous categorywith widely varying levels <strong>of</strong> impairment<strong>and</strong> many important subtypes2. Common comorbidities: Conduct disordersare <strong>of</strong>ten accompanied by several comorbidtypes <strong>of</strong> problems that influence thecourse <strong>and</strong> treatment <strong>of</strong> conduct disorders3. Correlates with potential causal roles:Conduct disorders develop through acomplex interaction <strong>of</strong> numerous factorswithin the child <strong>and</strong> his or her psychosocialenvironmentImplications for <strong>Assessment</strong>1a. Assess a wide range <strong>of</strong> conduct problems1b. Assess the level <strong>of</strong> impairment associated withthe disorder2a. Assess for the presence <strong>of</strong> ADHD2b. Assess for the presence <strong>of</strong> anxiety <strong>and</strong>depression (including suicidal ideation)2c. Assess for substance use <strong>and</strong> abuse3a. Assess important aspects <strong>of</strong> a child’s oradolescent’s family environment3b. Assess child’s intellectual level, academicachievement, learning style, <strong>and</strong> socialproblem solving(Continues)


CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivity405Table 17.5 (Continued)Research Findings4. Multiple developmental pathways: Conductdisorders develop through multiple differentpathways each involving distinctdevelopmental mechanismsImplications for <strong>Assessment</strong>3c. Assess child’s peer interactions, social status,<strong>and</strong> associations with a deviant peer group3d. Assess critical aspects <strong>of</strong> child’s social ecology(e.g., economic disadvantage, witnessing <strong>of</strong>violence)4a. Assess the developmental sequence <strong>of</strong> onset <strong>of</strong>conduct problem behavior, especially whethersevere conduct problems onset prior to adolescence4b. Assess for callous-unemotional traitsBox 17.5Case Study: 14-Year-Old <strong>Adolescent</strong> Male with Severe Conduct ProblemsPatrick was 14 years, 1 month old when hismother requested a comprehensive psychologicalevaluation from a university-basedoutpatient psychological clinic. His motherwas concerned about Patrick’s poor gradesin school <strong>and</strong> his frequent lying. His motheralso expressed concerns about his frequentfights both at school <strong>and</strong> in his neighborhood.Because <strong>of</strong> his fighting at school, Patrick hadbeen placed in a full-time class for childrenwith behavioral problems.<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Core Features<strong>and</strong> SubtypesOn the DISC-IV, both Patrick <strong>and</strong> his motherreported the presence <strong>of</strong> a number <strong>of</strong> severeconduct problems. They both reportedrepeat instances <strong>of</strong> lying, repeat instances <strong>of</strong>stealing items from stores (shoplifting), <strong>and</strong>several school suspensions for physical fights.His mother also reported several instances<strong>of</strong> truancy, <strong>and</strong> Patrick admitted to breakinginto a neighbor’s house to steal things<strong>and</strong> using a knife in a fight. Patrick furtheradmitted to occasional use <strong>of</strong> marijuana. Theseverity <strong>of</strong> these conduct problems is supportedby parental report on the CBCL, onwhich Patrick had T-scores <strong>of</strong> 79 on both theDelinquent Behavior <strong>and</strong> Aggressive Behaviorscales. On the <strong>Personality</strong> Inventory forYouth (PIY; Lachar & Gruber, 1994), Patrickobtained a T-score <strong>of</strong> 69 on the Delinquencyscale.According to both Patrick <strong>and</strong> his mother,much <strong>of</strong> Patrick’s aggressive <strong>and</strong> antisocialbehavior occurred alone. In fact, Patrickreported problems in peer relations, as evidentby T-scores <strong>of</strong> 68 <strong>and</strong> 75 on the PIY SocialWithdrawal <strong>and</strong> Social Skills scales. Patrick’smother reported that his aggressive <strong>and</strong> antisocialbehavior is <strong>of</strong> longst<strong>and</strong>ing duration.He had averaged about two suspensions peryear since the first grade, mostly because <strong>of</strong>fighting, indicating that his aggression startedwell before adolescence.<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> ComorbiditiesBoth Patrick <strong>and</strong> his mother reported that Patrickgoes through frequent periods <strong>of</strong> depression,<strong>of</strong>ten lasting for as long as a month. Atthe time <strong>of</strong> the assessment, Patrick had beenexperiencing significant periods <strong>of</strong> sadnessfor the past 3 weeks. He had also lost interestin activities, he had not been sleeping well atnight, <strong>and</strong> he had lost his appetite resulting insignificant weight loss. His mother had also(Continues)


406CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit HyperactivityBox 17.5 (Continued)noticed a decrease in energy <strong>and</strong> a decrease inhis ability to concentrate. Patrick reported onthe DISC-IV that he had, twice in the past 2years, thought <strong>of</strong> killing himself by cutting hiswrist <strong>and</strong> on one occasion had actually startedto use a knife but was stopped by a classmate.Both Patrick <strong>and</strong> his mother reported thatthe episodes <strong>of</strong> depression seemed to coincidewith disciplinary confrontations, such asbeing suspended from school.Patrick <strong>and</strong> his mother reported someproblems <strong>of</strong> attention <strong>and</strong> concentration,but these seemed to occur during periods <strong>of</strong>depression, <strong>and</strong> therefore, did not seem to beassociated with ADHD. Also, a psychoeducationalassessment revealed that Patrick wasfunctioning in the low average range <strong>of</strong> intellectualabilities, with a particular weakness inhis verbal abilities. He scored in a range commensuratewith this intellectual level on anachievement screener.<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Correlates with PotentialCausal RolesPatrick lived alone with his mother, whoworked full-time outside <strong>of</strong> the home asa secretary. Patrick’s parents had divorcedwhen he was 6 years old <strong>and</strong> he had had minimalcontact with his father, who accordingto maternal report, was in <strong>and</strong> out <strong>of</strong> prison<strong>and</strong> had a substance abuse problem. Patrick’smother reportedly had limited social contacts.Her extended family mostly lived in adifferent region <strong>of</strong> the country <strong>and</strong> she hadnot developed a good network <strong>of</strong> friends.Patrick’s mother also reported that she hadgreat difficulty disciplining Patrick. Becausehe was so moody, she rarely tried to make himdo things. Also, whenever he did somethingwrong (e.g., getting suspended from school),she did not punish him because it would simplymake him angrier <strong>and</strong> more difficult tolive with. There was no indication that Patrickwas involved with a deviant peer group.In fact, his mother was concerned with hislack <strong>of</strong> connectedness with any peers.<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Multiple DevelopmentalPathwaysInformation from both the DISC-IV <strong>and</strong> anunstructured interview both suggested thatPatrick’s serious conduct problems have beenevident, at least since the first grade. Thus,the problems clearly had a childhood-onset.Also, a measure <strong>of</strong> callous-unemotional traits(Frick & Hare, 2001) did not suggest thatPatrick showed high rates <strong>of</strong> these traits.This would also be consistent with the highrate <strong>of</strong> depression, a weakness in his verbalintelligence, <strong>and</strong> the apparent associationbetween his conduct problems <strong>and</strong> ineffectiveparenting practices in the home, all <strong>of</strong> whichare typically more common in youth withoutcallous-unemotional traits.<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Core FeaturesThe first goal <strong>of</strong> the assessment is to carefully<strong>and</strong> thoroughly assess the number,types, <strong>and</strong> severity <strong>of</strong> the conduct problems<strong>and</strong> the level <strong>of</strong> impairment that the conductproblems are causing for the child oradolescent (e.g., school suspensions, policecontacts, peer rejection). This assessmentis important given that research has showngreat variability in these dimensions amongchildren with conduct problems, <strong>and</strong> ithas shown that these dimensions may besome <strong>of</strong> the best predictors <strong>of</strong> outcome forchildren with conduct problems (Frick &Loney, 1999). The primary methods <strong>of</strong>assessing the core symptoms are structuredinterviews, behavior rating scales, <strong>and</strong>behavioral observations (see McMahon& Frick, 2005 for a summary <strong>of</strong> specificmeasures).


CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivity407Many structured interviews <strong>and</strong> behavioralrating scales provide good coverage <strong>of</strong>the conduct problem behaviors <strong>and</strong> allowfor multi-informant assessments. However,they each <strong>of</strong>fer unique advantages inother respects. Behavior rating scales aremore time-efficient <strong>and</strong> provide some <strong>of</strong>the best norm-referenced information fordetermining the severity <strong>of</strong> conduct problems.In contrast, diagnostic interviews<strong>of</strong>ten provide important information onthe degree <strong>of</strong> impairment associated withthe conduct problems <strong>and</strong> a structuredmeans <strong>of</strong> assessing the age <strong>of</strong> onset <strong>of</strong> theproblem behaviors. As discussed in Chap.8, behavioral observations provide a thirdway <strong>of</strong> assessing conduct problem behaviors.Behavioral observations in a child’snatural setting can make a unique contributionto the assessment process by providingan assessment <strong>of</strong> a child’s behaviorthat is not filtered through the perceptions<strong>of</strong> an informant <strong>and</strong> by providing anassessment <strong>of</strong> the immediate environmentalcontext <strong>of</strong> a child’s behavior. Unfortunately,for older children <strong>and</strong> adolescents,many <strong>of</strong> the common conduct problemsare by nature covert (e.g., lying <strong>and</strong> stealing)or only occur infrequently (e.g., fighting),which makes them difficult to capturethrough some observational technique.One important advantage that manystructured interviews have over behavioralrating scales <strong>and</strong> behavioral observationsis that they provide a structuredmethod for assessing when a child firstbegan showing serious conduct problems,thereby providing an important source <strong>of</strong>information on the developmental trajectory<strong>of</strong> the child’s problem behavior. Forexample, in the DISC-IV (Shaffer, Fisher,Lucas, Dulcan, & Schwab-Stone, 2000),any question related to the presence <strong>of</strong> aconduct problem that is answered affirmativelyis followed by questions askingthe parent or child to estimate at whatage the first occurrence <strong>of</strong> the behaviortook place. Obviously, such questions canalso be integrated into an unstructuredinterview format as well.<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> ComorbiditiesCo-occurring problems can be assessedconjointly with the assessment <strong>of</strong> the conductproblems themselves, through theuse <strong>of</strong> omnibus rating scales, structuredinterviews, or multi-domain observationalsystems. Given the association betweenconduct problems <strong>and</strong> learning disabilities,a psychoeducational evaluation thatincludes a st<strong>and</strong>ardized intelligence test <strong>and</strong>academic achievement screener should alsobe a part <strong>of</strong> most evaluations <strong>of</strong> children<strong>and</strong> adolescents with conduct problems.<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Correlates withPotential Causal RolesUncovering which <strong>of</strong> the many potentialcausal factors may be operating on a childreferred for an evaluation could be crucialfor making recommendations for treatment.Of primary importance are correlateswithin the child’s family environment(Frick, 1994). In Chap. 12, we discussedsome basic issues <strong>and</strong> methods in assessingcrucial elements in a child’s family environment(see also McMahon & Frick, 2007).Research also indicates that peer rejectionis predictive <strong>of</strong> the development <strong>of</strong>conduct problems <strong>and</strong> is associated withan adolescent’s association with a deviantpeer group. Therefore, assessing a child oradolescent’s peer relationships is a criticalassessment goal. Several <strong>of</strong> the omnibusrating scales discussed in previous chaptersprovide an assessment <strong>of</strong> a child’s peerfunctioning. Also, several rating scalesfocus specifically on a child’s or adolescent’ssocial functioning (see also Cavell, Meehan,& Fiala, 2003). Social status can alsobe assessed directly through a sociometricexercise, if a child is in elementary school


408CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivity(see Chap. 9). Given that a child’s associationwith a deviant peer group is associatedwith an increase in the severity <strong>of</strong> conductproblems, some method <strong>of</strong> assessing thisaspect <strong>of</strong> a child’s social functioning (e.g.,Elliott, Huizinga, & Ageton, 1985) shouldalso be included in a comprehensive assessment.Another important class <strong>of</strong> potentiallyimportant correlates to severe conductproblems involves specific cognitive deficits<strong>and</strong> learning styles. Specifically, deficits inintelligence, especially verbal intelligence,have been associated with conduct problems,necessitating a st<strong>and</strong>ard intellectualevaluation as part <strong>of</strong> most assessment batteriesfor children with conduct problems.In addition, as discussed previously, manychildren with conduct problems, especiallythose who also show a callous <strong>and</strong> unemotionalinterpersonal style, show heightenedsensitivity to rewards compared topunishments. There are computerizedtasks developed to assess this learningstyle in young children; however, the clinicalutility <strong>of</strong> such information <strong>and</strong> somemajor limitations in the development <strong>of</strong>these tasks make their usefulness in manyclinical assessments somewhat limitedat the present time (see Frick & Loney,2000 for a review). There are also laboratorymeasures, typically involving a childbeing provided a hypothetical vignette <strong>of</strong>a social situation <strong>and</strong> asked to state how heor she would respond if the situation wasreal, that assess several deficits in socialcognition that have been associated withconduct problems, such as a hostileattributional bias. Examples include theIntention-Cue Detection Task, the Problem-SolvingMeasure for Conflict, <strong>and</strong> theWALLY Game (Conduct Problems PreventionResearch Group, 1999; Dodge &Coie, 1987; Webster-Stratton & Lindsay,1999). The recently developed Social-Cognitive<strong>Assessment</strong> Pr<strong>of</strong>ile (SCAP; Hughes,Meehan, & Cavell, 2004) shows promise asa brief (15–20 min), clinically useful interviewwith elementary school-age children,designed to assess social-cognitive deficitsassociated with conduct problems. Whilethese measures are also not without somelimitations in their clinical usefulness (Frick& Loney, 2000), interventions for the deficitsthat are assessed by these measuresare part <strong>of</strong> many treatment programs forconduct problems (Lochman et al., 2008).Therefore, the information they providecan be useful in treatment planning.Finally, a child’s social ecology is <strong>of</strong>tencrucial for underst<strong>and</strong>ing the development<strong>of</strong> conduct problems in many cases.Therefore, it is important to assess suchvariables as the economic situation <strong>of</strong> thefamily, the level <strong>of</strong> social <strong>and</strong> communitysupport provided to the child <strong>and</strong> his orher family, <strong>and</strong> other aspects <strong>of</strong> a child’ssocial climate (e.g., neighborhood, quality<strong>of</strong> school <strong>and</strong>, degree <strong>of</strong> exposure toviolence) (McMahon & Frick, 2007). Forexample, the Neighborhood Questionnaire(Greenberg, Lengua, Coie, Pinderhughes,& the Conduct Problems PreventionResearch Group, 1999) is a brief parentreportmeasure used to assess the parent’sperception <strong>of</strong> the family’s neighborhoodin terms <strong>of</strong> safety, violence, drug traffic,satisfaction, <strong>and</strong> stability.<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> ImportantDevelopmental PathwaysA key area <strong>of</strong> research for guiding theassessment process is the research documentingvarious potential developmentalpathways to conduct problems. As reviewedpreviously, children with conduct problemscan fall into childhood-onset or adolescentonsetpathways, depending on when indevelopment their level <strong>of</strong> severe antisocial<strong>and</strong> aggressive behavior started. Also,within the childhood-onset group, thereseem to be important differences betweenthose who do <strong>and</strong> do not show high levels<strong>of</strong> callous-unemotional traits. Knowledge


CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivity409<strong>of</strong> the characteristics <strong>of</strong> children in thesedifferent pathways, <strong>and</strong> the different causalmechanisms involved, can serve as a guidefor structuring <strong>and</strong> conducting the assessment(McMahon & Frick, 2005). Further,interventions can be tailored to the uniqueneeds <strong>of</strong> youth in these different pathways(Frick, 2006).Specifically, knowledge <strong>of</strong> the developmentalpathways can provide a set <strong>of</strong>working hypotheses concerning the nature<strong>of</strong> the conduct problems, the most likelycomorbid conditions, <strong>and</strong> the most likelyrisk factors (McMahon & Frick, 2005). Forexample, for a youth whose conduct problemsappear with the onset <strong>of</strong> adolescence,one would hypothesize based on the availableliterature that he or she is less likelyto be aggressive, to have intellectual deficits,to have temperamental vulnerabilities,<strong>and</strong> to have comorbid ADHD. However,the youth’s association with a deviant peergroup <strong>and</strong> factors that may contribute tothis deviant peer group affiliation (e.g., lack<strong>of</strong> parental monitoring <strong>and</strong> supervision)would be especially important to assess foryouth in this pathway.In contrast, for a youth whose seriousconduct problems began prior to adolescence,one would expect more cognitive<strong>and</strong> temperamental vulnerabilities, comorbidADHD, <strong>and</strong> more serious problemsin family functioning. For those youthsin this childhood-onset group who donot show CU traits, the cognitive deficitswould more likely be verbal deficits<strong>and</strong> the temperamental vulnerabilitieswould more likely be problems regulatingemotions, leading to higher levels <strong>of</strong>anxiety, depression, <strong>and</strong> aggression involvinganger. In contrast, for a youth withchildhood-onset conduct problems whoshows high levels <strong>of</strong> callous-unemotionaltraits, the cognitive deficits are morelikely to involve a lack <strong>of</strong> sensitivity topunishment <strong>and</strong> the temperamental vulnerabilitiesare more likely to involve a preferencefor dangerous <strong>and</strong> novel activities<strong>and</strong> a failure to experience many types <strong>of</strong>prosocial emotions (e.g., guilt <strong>and</strong> empathy).Further, assessing the level <strong>and</strong> severity<strong>of</strong> aggressive behavior, especially thepresence <strong>of</strong> instrumental aggression, wouldbe critical for children <strong>and</strong> adolescents inthis group (Marsee & Frick, 2007).As most clinicians recognize, people donot <strong>of</strong>ten fall neatly into the prototypesthat are suggested by research. Therefore,these descriptions are meant to serve ashypotheses around which to organize anassessment based on the available research.They also highlight several specific importantpieces <strong>of</strong> information that are neededwhen assessing children <strong>and</strong> adolescentswith conduct problems. One <strong>of</strong> the mostcritical pieces <strong>of</strong> information in guidingassessment, <strong>and</strong> perhaps ultimately intervention,is determining the age at whichvarious conduct problems began. Thisinformation provides some indication as towhether or not the youth may be on thechildhood-onset pathway. As noted previously,unstructured <strong>and</strong> structured interviewsare <strong>of</strong>ten the most common methodsfor obtaining this information.Unfortunately, there has been littleconsistency in the literature concerning themost appropriate operational definition <strong>of</strong>childhood- vs. adolescent-onset. For example,the DSM-IV-TR makes the distinctionbetween children who begin showing severeconduct problems before age 10 (i.e., childhood-onset)<strong>and</strong> those who do not showsevere conduct problems before age 10 (i.e.,adolescent-onset) in its definition <strong>of</strong> ConductDisorder. However, other researchstudies have used age 11 (Robins 1966) orage 14 (Patterson & Yoerger, 1993; Tibbetts& Piquero, 1999) to define the start <strong>of</strong> adolescentonset. Thus, onset <strong>of</strong> severe conductproblems before age 10 seems to be clearlyconsidered childhood-onset <strong>and</strong> onset afterage 13 clearly adolescent-onset. However,how to classify children whose conductproblems onset between the ages <strong>of</strong> 11 <strong>and</strong>13 is less clear <strong>and</strong> probably dependent on


410CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivitythe level <strong>of</strong> physical, cognitive, <strong>and</strong> socialmaturity <strong>of</strong> the child.In addition to the difficulty in determiningthe most appropriate way to dividechildren based on their age <strong>of</strong> onset, thereis also concern about how accurate theparent or youth is in reporting the timing<strong>of</strong> specific behaviors. There are threefindings from research that can help ininterpreting such reports. First, the longerthe time frame involved in the retrospectivereport (e.g., a parent <strong>of</strong> a 17-year-oldreporting on preschool behavior vs. a parent<strong>of</strong> a 6-year-old reporting on preschoolbehavior), the less accurate the report islikely to be (Green, Loeber, & Lahey,1991). Second, although a parental report<strong>of</strong> the exact age <strong>of</strong> onset may not be veryreliable over time, typical variations inyears are usually small (Green et al., 1991).As a result, these reports should be viewedas rough estimates <strong>of</strong> the timing <strong>of</strong> onset<strong>and</strong> not as exact dating procedures. Third,there is evidence that combining informants(e.g., such as a parent or youth) orcombining sources <strong>of</strong> information (e.g.,self-report <strong>and</strong> record <strong>of</strong> police contact),<strong>and</strong> taking the earliest reported age <strong>of</strong>onset from any source, provides an estimatethat shows somewhat greater validitythan any single source <strong>of</strong> information alone(Lahey et al., 1999).If the youth’s history <strong>of</strong> conduct problemsis consistent with the childhoodonsetpathway, then additional assessmentto examine the extent to which callousunemotionaltraits may also be present isimportant. There have been several reviews<strong>and</strong> critiques <strong>of</strong> the available methodsfor assessing these traits (Sharp & Kline,2008; Vincent, 2006). The two most commonlyused methods are the PsychopathyChecklist: Youth Version (PCL-YV; Forth,Kosson, & Hare, 2003) <strong>and</strong> the AntisocialProcess Screening Device (APSD; Frick &Hare, 2001). The PCL-YV is a cliniciancompleted checklist for adolescents ages12 to 18 years. It is completed based on a60 to 90 min semi-structured interview <strong>and</strong>review <strong>of</strong> all available collateral information(e.g., psychosocial histories, institutionalrecords). The APSD includes parent <strong>and</strong>teacher ratings scales (Frick & Hare, 2001)<strong>and</strong> a self-report questionnaire (Munoz &Frick, 2007). Although there is research tosupport the usefulness <strong>of</strong> both the PCL-YV <strong>and</strong> APSD (see Frick & Dickens, 2006;Frick & White, 2008), both measuresinclude only limited items specificallyrelated to callous-unemotional traits (4 <strong>and</strong>6 items, respectively). A scale that providesa more comprehensive assessment <strong>of</strong> thesetraits, the Inventory <strong>of</strong> Callous-Unemotionaltraits, has been developed <strong>and</strong> hasshown some initial promise in a large(n = 1443) community sample <strong>of</strong> young adolescentsin Germany (Essau, Sasagawa, &Frick, 2006) <strong>and</strong> moderate size (n = 248)sample <strong>of</strong> detained juvenile <strong>of</strong>fenders inthe United States (Kimonis et al., 2008). InTable 17.6, a summary <strong>of</strong> the items assessingCU traits from these three measuresare provided.ConclusionsIn this chapter we discuss two specificapplications <strong>of</strong> the assessment procedures<strong>and</strong> techniques reviewed in previous chapters.We discuss the assessment <strong>of</strong> tworelated types <strong>of</strong> childhood psychopathology:Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder<strong>and</strong> conduct problems. To continueour basic premise that clinical assessmentsshould be guided by basic psychologicalresearch, we provide a brief overview<strong>of</strong> some <strong>of</strong> the more important researchfindings with particular relevance to theassessment process. For both domains,assessments should be structured aroundour current knowledge <strong>of</strong> the core features<strong>of</strong> each domain. In addition, the mostfrequent co-occurring problems associatedwith both types <strong>of</strong> psychopathology


CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivity411Table 17.6 Items Assessing Callous-Unemotional Traits from Three Commonly UsedMeasuresPsychopathy Checklist-YouthVersion(Forth et al., 2003)Antisocial ProcessScreening Device(Frick & Hare, 2001)Inventory <strong>of</strong> Callous-UnemotionalTraits (Essau et al., 2006)Lacks guilt <strong>and</strong> remorse Feels bad or guilty (I) Feels bad or guilty (I)Shallow affectCallous use <strong>of</strong> othersFails to acceptresponsibilityDoes not show emotionConcerned about thefeelings <strong>of</strong> others (I)Concerned about school work (I)Keeps promises (I)Keeps the same friends (I)Does not feel remorseful when doingsomething wrongEasily admits to being wrong (I)Tries not to hurt others’ feelings (I)Feelings <strong>of</strong> others are unimportantDoesn’t care who he/she hurts to getwhat he/she wantsConcerned about feelings <strong>of</strong> others (I)Apologizes to persons he/she hurts (I)Does not care if he/she gets in troubleSeems very cold <strong>and</strong> uncaring to othersWorks hard on everything (I)Always tries best (I)Does not care about doing things wellCares about how well he/she does atschool or work (I)Does things to make others feel good (I)Tries not to hurt others’ feelings (I)Does not like to put the time into doingthings wellWhat he/she thinks is right <strong>and</strong> wrongis different from what others thinkDoes not show emotionsExpresses feelings openly (I)Hides feelings from othersIt is easy to tell how he/she is feeling (I)Very expressive <strong>and</strong> emotional (I)Does not care about being on timeNote: (I) designates items that are inversely scored.should be routinely assessed, because thesecomorbidities <strong>of</strong>ten have important prognostic<strong>and</strong> treatment implications.For ADHD, a difficult part <strong>of</strong> theassessment is ruling out other medical orpsychological disorders that could solelyaccount for the ADHD symptoms. Also,in assessing ADHD, one must be knowledgeable<strong>of</strong> educational laws related tolegally m<strong>and</strong>ated services for children withADHD so that one can work with educatorsin designing a treatment plan for thechild or adolescent with ADHD.For conduct problems, the myriad <strong>of</strong>potential causal factors should be assessedto determine which ones may be operating


412CHAPTER 17 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Attention Deficit Hyperactivityfor a given child <strong>and</strong> which ones should,therefore, be a focus <strong>of</strong> intervention. Also,different causal factors may be involvedin the various subgroups <strong>of</strong> children withconduct problems. Underst<strong>and</strong>ing the differentpathways through which children<strong>and</strong> adolescents develop serious conductproblems can be critical for designingassessments <strong>and</strong> interpreting the informationprovided by the evaluation.Chapter Summary1. Externalizing behaviors are the mostcommon reason for referral to childmental health clinics.2. Based on research on ADHD suggeststhat:(a) <strong>Assessment</strong>s should include a multiinformant<strong>and</strong> multi-source assessment<strong>of</strong> the core ADHD behaviors:inattention-disorganization <strong>and</strong>impulsivity-hyperactivity; assessment<strong>of</strong> these core features must be placedwithin a developmental perspective.(b) <strong>Assessment</strong>s should screen for thepresence <strong>of</strong> the most common cooccurringproblems that may accompanyADHD: conduct problems/aggression, emotional disturbance,low self-esteem, problematic socialrelationships, learning difficulties,<strong>and</strong> family conflict.(c) <strong>Assessment</strong>s should rule out alternativecauses for the core symptoms:medical/ neurological disorders,mental h<strong>and</strong>icaps, learning disorders,<strong>and</strong> adjustment reactions toenvironmental stressors.(d) Because ADHD <strong>of</strong>ten has a majorimpact on a child’s or adolescent’sschool functioning, the assessmentshould be conducted in collaborationwith school personnel <strong>and</strong> witha knowledge <strong>of</strong> local educationalstatutes relevant to services for studentswith ADHD.3. Based on research on severe conductproblems in children <strong>and</strong> adolescents:(a) <strong>Assessment</strong>s should provide a multisource<strong>and</strong> multi-method assessment<strong>of</strong> conduct problems includingdetermining the types <strong>and</strong> severity<strong>of</strong> conduct problems <strong>and</strong> the age atwhich they began.(b) <strong>Assessment</strong>s should screen for themost common co-occurring problemsthat <strong>of</strong>ten accompany conductproblems: ADHD, emotionaldisturbance, substance abuse, <strong>and</strong>learning disabilities.(c) <strong>Assessment</strong>s should assess knowncorrelates to conduct problems thatcould play a role in causing or maintainingthe problem behavior, <strong>and</strong>therefore, should be a major focus<strong>of</strong> intervention: family functioning,cognitive deficits, social ecology,peer relations, <strong>and</strong> associations witha deviant peer group.(d) The age at which the serious conductproblems began <strong>and</strong> the presence<strong>of</strong> callous-unemotional traitsshould be assessed because <strong>of</strong> theirimportance in designating uniquepathways to the development <strong>of</strong>conduct problems.


C h a p t e r 1 8<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression<strong>and</strong> AnxietyChapter Questionsl What are the core symptoms <strong>of</strong> childhooddepression <strong>and</strong> anxiety?l Which informant(s) <strong>and</strong> assessmentstrategies are most useful for the diagnosis<strong>of</strong> depression <strong>and</strong> anxiety?l What developmental factors need to beconsidered in assessment <strong>of</strong> child/adolescentdepression <strong>and</strong> anxiety?l What factors contribute to better orworse prognosis for childhood depression<strong>and</strong> anxiety?Internalizing DisordersIt is commonplace to consider symptoms<strong>of</strong> depression <strong>and</strong> anxiety under theterm internalizing problems, a tendencyreflected in the structure <strong>of</strong> many commonrating scales (e.g., Achenbach & Rescorla,2001; Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004).More specifically, rating scales <strong>of</strong>ten havean internalizing symptom composite thatconsists <strong>of</strong> depression, anxiety, <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>ten,somatic complaints. Historically, childrenwith internalizing difficulties have beendescribed in a number <strong>of</strong> ways, includingas having a personality problem syndromewith difficulties <strong>of</strong> anxiety, withdrawal, <strong>and</strong>feelings <strong>of</strong> inferiority (Peterson, 1961);anxious-fearful (Behar & Stringfield, 1974);inhibited (Miller, 1967); anxious-immature(Conners, 1970); <strong>and</strong> overcontrolled (Edelbrock,1979). <strong>Child</strong>ren solely with internalizingproblems are easily distinguishedfrom children solely with externalizingproblems, but this does not imply that suchdifficulties are easily diagnosed <strong>and</strong> treated.In fact, it can be quite the contrary.Internalizing disorders are among themost difficult to diagnose because <strong>of</strong> thenature <strong>of</strong> the symptomatology. The child’ssymptoms take more <strong>of</strong> a toll on the child’s413P.J. Frick et al., <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Behavior,DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-0641-0_18, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010


414 chapter 18 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression <strong>and</strong> anxietysubjective mental state than on significantothers or on the child’s adjustmentto school or other settings. Furthermore,children with internalizing problems havebeen found to be better adapted than childrenwith externalizing difficulties, achievinghigher reading <strong>and</strong> intelligence testscores, <strong>and</strong> performing better in school(Cohen et al., 1985). The following quotefrom Reynolds (1990) eloquently summarizesthe central assessment challenge thatis presented by the most common internalizingsyndromes <strong>of</strong> depression <strong>and</strong> anxiety.“Internalizing disorders, including depressivedisorders, anxiety disorders, obsessivecompulsive disorder, somatic disorders, <strong>and</strong>suicidal behaviors, among others, are associatedwith overcontrolled behaviors. Phenomenologically,internalizing disordersare characterized by covert, inner-directedsymptomatology. The covert nature <strong>of</strong> thesedisturbances presents challenges to pr<strong>of</strong>essionals,particularly with regard to assessment<strong>and</strong> treatment. As noted in this article,internalizing disorders as a function <strong>of</strong> theirinsidious nature, do not readily come tothe attention <strong>of</strong> psychologists. Because <strong>of</strong>this, pr<strong>of</strong>essionals need to be vigilant to thepotential existence <strong>of</strong> internalizing disordersin children <strong>and</strong> adolescents” (p. 137).Within the broad category <strong>of</strong> internalizingproblems, depression <strong>and</strong> anxietysyndromes can be reliably differentiated,although it has been theorized that theremay be a third disorder that is symptomatically<strong>and</strong> etiologically overlapping withboth these disorders (see Klein et al.,2005). Still, research has supported a tw<strong>of</strong>actormodel <strong>of</strong> internalizing problems, oneindicative <strong>of</strong> depression <strong>and</strong> one that can beconceptualized as anxiety (Ollendick et al.,2003). Klein et al. (2005) provide a clearreview <strong>of</strong> the ways in which the symptoms<strong>of</strong> either problem can be mutually influential,<strong>and</strong> while it is beyond the scope <strong>of</strong> thischapter to consider the evidence for a commondisorder for anxiety <strong>and</strong> depression,it is important for the reader to be awarethat although this chapter will discussthese syndromes as if they differ in manyrespects (including etiology <strong>and</strong> course),there is substantial comorbidity betweenthe two (Klein et al.).The present discussion is influencedprimarily by the structure <strong>of</strong> the DSM-IV,which focuses on anxiety <strong>and</strong> depressiondisorders as separate entities, althoughcomorbidity is recognized in a little understooddiagnosis <strong>of</strong> Mixed Anxiety-DepressionDisorder (American PsychiatricAssociation, 2000). Ultimately, it will beup to the clinician to determine, throughthe evidence gathered, if interventionstargeting both anxiety <strong>and</strong> depression arewarranted. The important reminder is thatboth areas <strong>of</strong> concern be assessed as eitherprimary or secondary influences on thechild’s functioning.<strong>Child</strong>hood DepressionDiagnostic CriteriaIn order to meet DSM-IV-TR diagnosticcriteria for Major Depressive Disorder, atleast five <strong>of</strong> nine symptoms must be presentfor at least a two-week period, <strong>and</strong> one<strong>of</strong> the symptoms must be either depressedmood or loss <strong>of</strong> interest or pleasure (AmericanPsychiatric Association, 2000).These symptoms include:1. depressed or irritable mood2. loss <strong>of</strong> interest in daily activities3. significant weight loss or failure to makeexpected weight gains4. frequent insomnia or hypersomnia5. motor agitation or retardation6. frequent fatigue7. feelings <strong>of</strong> worthlessness or guilt8. impaired concentration9. suicidal ideation or suicide attempt.


chapter 18 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression <strong>and</strong> anxiety415For many years, the need for diagnosticcriteria for childhood depression was notclear, as researchers debated whether ornot the disorder could exist in childhood.There is increasing evidence that children<strong>and</strong> adolescents display much <strong>of</strong> the samesymptomatology as adults, including cognitivedistortion (Haley et al., 1985). Unfortunately,there is also increasing evidencethat depression is quite persistent <strong>and</strong>impairing in childhood. In fact, the existence<strong>of</strong> childhood depression is no longera point <strong>of</strong> contention (Kovacs, 1989).Although there are some developmentalinfluences on the manifestation <strong>of</strong> depression(see below), most evidence points tothe continuity <strong>of</strong> depression from childhoodto adulthood (Klein et al., 2005).Despite some <strong>of</strong> the previous controversyregarding the existence <strong>of</strong> childhooddepression, clinicians are fairly reliable intheir diagnosis <strong>of</strong> the condition. Whenusing structured diagnostic interviewssuch as those described in Chap. 11, theinter-rater reliability <strong>of</strong> the diagnosisranges from about .75 to .90 (Ambrosini,2000; Christ, 1990). Emerging agreementregarding the existence <strong>of</strong> the depressionsyndrome in childhood, the widespreadapplication <strong>of</strong> the DSM criteria, <strong>and</strong> evidence<strong>of</strong> inter-rater reliability have allcontributed to forming a consensus thatchildhood depression is an important publichealth problem that warrants significantresearch attention.Regardless <strong>of</strong> the accuracy <strong>of</strong> diagnosis,as Reynolds (1990) observes, the needto assess for the presence <strong>of</strong> depressionis not always obvious. Consequently, thepsychologist must develop some strategies<strong>and</strong>/or cues that trigger a search fordepression. Strategies for assessing fordepression can be gleaned from an underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>of</strong> the nature <strong>of</strong> the syndrome,including its risk factors <strong>and</strong> course. Somemethods for identifying the need to assessfor the presence <strong>of</strong> depression are discussednext.Characteristics <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong>hoodDepressionAlthough prevalence rates are <strong>of</strong>tendebated, research has generally concludedthat less than 3% <strong>of</strong> preadolescent childrenexperience depression but that rates<strong>of</strong> depression increase substantially in adolescenceto around 15%, although a largerproportion <strong>of</strong> youth suffer from symptoms<strong>of</strong> depression at some point (see Hankinet al., 2008 for review). A prevalence ratethis large warrants vigilance for the existence<strong>of</strong> depression in referral populations.Also, a higher rate <strong>of</strong> depression may beexpected in adolescence because <strong>of</strong> theattendant stressors associated with thisage range (Cooper, 1990). <strong>Adolescent</strong>s arepresented with increasing developmentaldem<strong>and</strong>s <strong>and</strong> stressors for which theymay <strong>of</strong>ten be ill-prepared (Petersen et al.,1993).Several studies have suggested that theexpression <strong>of</strong> depression differs somewhatby age. While children <strong>and</strong> adolescentsboth report dysphoria as a central symptom<strong>of</strong> depression, younger children morefrequently exhibit sleep disturbances.Less than half <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescentsreport suicidal attempts, increasedappetite, hypersomnia, hallucinations, ordelusions (Christ, 1990). <strong>Adolescent</strong>s aremore likely than young children to experiencefeelings <strong>of</strong> hopelessness as part <strong>of</strong>depression (Weiss & Garber, 2003). Onestudy found that a community sample <strong>of</strong>adolescents reported significantly moresomatic complaints on the <strong>Child</strong> DepressionInventory (CDI) than preadolescents(Cooper, 1990). Further analyses revealed,however, that this finding was primarilydue to gender differences: adolescent girlsacknowledged more somatic complaintsthan preadolescents (boys <strong>and</strong> girls) <strong>and</strong>adolescent boys. However, this findingdoes suggest that excessive somatic complaintsshould trigger further assessmentfor depression.


416 chapter 18 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression <strong>and</strong> anxietyDepression is also distinctive as a syndromebecause <strong>of</strong> the crucial hereditaryfactor that has been identified. Heritabilityestimates vary widely, but the proportion <strong>of</strong>youth with depression who have a positivefamily history <strong>of</strong> depression is somewherearound one third (Rice, Harold, & Thapar,2002). In addition, family history <strong>of</strong> depressionis a significant predictor <strong>of</strong> recurrence<strong>of</strong> depression for children <strong>and</strong> adolescents(Klein et al., 2005). Therefore, to say theleast, knowledge <strong>of</strong> family history is <strong>of</strong>tenhelpful for conceptualizing cases. Familyresemblance data may affect assessment <strong>and</strong>intervention planning for a child who hasseveral symptoms <strong>of</strong> depression but doesnot meet diagnostic criteria. If this child’smother has a history <strong>of</strong> depression, moreassessment <strong>and</strong> intervention would be warrantedthan if no family resemblance waspresent. This information indicates that thepsychologist should routinely <strong>and</strong> carefullygather family history data regarding a history<strong>of</strong> depression for the child suspected <strong>of</strong>having an internalizing disorder.The duration <strong>of</strong> a depressive disordercan be substantial. In an interesting study,105 children who entered a special schoolfor children who lost a parent due to death,divorce, or separation were divided intodepressed <strong>and</strong> non-depressed groups basedon a CDI cut score <strong>of</strong> 13 or greater indicatingdepression. The elevated group wasfound to, for the most part, exceed the cutscore again four years later (Mattison et al.,1990). Mattison et al. found the depressedgroup to show considerable impairmentover the four-year duration <strong>of</strong> the study.They concluded that:“They further showed significantly pooreracademic performance, received significantlymore counseling, <strong>and</strong> more <strong>of</strong>tenseparated from the school under negativecircumstances. The most pathological scoresoverall were demonstrated by the children inthe original depressed group who separatedfrom the school during the 4 years undernegative circumstances” (p. 169).This study did not consider actual diagnoses<strong>of</strong> depression, but the results point topotentially prolonged difficulties for youthwith higher levels <strong>of</strong> depressive symptoms.Research has shown that episodes <strong>of</strong> majordepression tend to average around sevento eight months in length for children <strong>and</strong>adolescents (Birmaher, Arbelaez, & Brent,2002).The chronicity <strong>of</strong> depressive symptomatologyhas even been documented infirst-graders. In a study <strong>of</strong> 677 first-gradechildren from the Baltimore, Maryl<strong>and</strong>public schools, CDI scores tended to bemost stable for children with scores inthe upper quartile <strong>of</strong> the scale. Interestingly,all <strong>of</strong> the children who scored in theupper quartile in the fall <strong>of</strong> the academicyear remained in the upper quartile whenretested in the spring. <strong>Child</strong>ren who scoredbelow the upper quartile showed considerablymore fluctuation from the fall to thespring (Edelsohn et al., 1992). Furthermore,Luby et al. (2002) have found supportfor a depression syndrome/diagnosisin preschool-aged children, if the durationrequirement from the DSM-IV is reducedAs discussed elsewhere in this text, thedevelopmental onset <strong>and</strong> course are criticalareas to assess for underst<strong>and</strong>ing the manifestation<strong>of</strong> psychological problems, theassociated features, <strong>and</strong> the potential outcomes.This issue is illustrated quite wellfor youth depression in a study by Hammen,Brennan, Keenan-Miller, <strong>and</strong> Herr(2008). They considered the outcomes atage 20 for individuals with an early onset<strong>of</strong> depression (i.e., by age 15) versus a lateonset (i.e., after age 15), with an additionalconsideration <strong>of</strong> whether or not the depressivesymptoms were recurrent by age 20. Ingeneral, the early onset, recurrent group hadthe worst outcomes in many psychological<strong>and</strong> social domains <strong>of</strong> functioning. In particular,these individuals:l Were more likely than individuals tohave had comorbid anxiety by age 20


chapter 18 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression <strong>and</strong> anxiety417l Were more likely to have comorbid eatingdisorders <strong>and</strong> externalizing problemsl Were more likely than individuals withrecurrent depression to report not havinga close friend, not being involved ina romantic relationship, <strong>and</strong> report havingproblems in family relationshipsl Were less likely to have a history <strong>of</strong>close friendships <strong>and</strong> good family relationships.These findings suggest that early onset<strong>of</strong> depression, <strong>and</strong> particularly recurrentdepression during adolescence, have a significantimpact on many areas <strong>of</strong> functioning.Thus, knowledge <strong>of</strong> the developmentalmanifestations <strong>of</strong> depression, recognition <strong>of</strong>heterogeneity <strong>of</strong> depressive presentations(Klein et al., 2005), <strong>and</strong> comprehensivenessin the assessment <strong>of</strong> depressive symptoms<strong>and</strong> associated features are essentialfor providing appropriate <strong>and</strong> meaningfulrecommendations.ComorbidityWhen a child is diagnosed with depression,there is a high probability (i.e., the majority<strong>of</strong> cases; Klein et al., 2005) that the childwill also meet criteria for another disorder,especially an anxiety problem (Angold,Costello, & Erkanli, 1999). One studyfound that every child who was diagnosedwith depression over a four-year periodwas at some point also diagnosed with ananxiety disorder (Christ, 1990). This overlapis striking, but it does not indicate thatdepression <strong>and</strong> anxiety comprise the samesyndrome. In addition to being reliably differentiatedwith diagnostic measures, thetwo problems differ in family resemblance<strong>and</strong> course (Stavrakaki & Ellis, 1989).Also <strong>of</strong> interest is the comorbidity <strong>of</strong>depression <strong>and</strong> disruptive behavior disorders.Angold et al. (1999) found a greaterthan chance probability that youth diagnosedwith depression also meet criteriafor ADHD, ODD, <strong>and</strong> CD. The pattern<strong>of</strong> comorbidity also appears somewhat differentbased on developmental level. Morespecifically, younger children with depressionare likely to have comorbid anxietydisorders, whereas adolescents with depressionare more likely to have comorbid substanceabuse problems or eating disorders(see Hankin et al., 2008).The comorbidity <strong>of</strong> youth depressionalso extends into the realm <strong>of</strong> academicfunctioning. Hammen <strong>and</strong> colleaguesfound youth depression was associatedwith greater academic problems <strong>and</strong> poorerschool attendance (Hammen et al., 1999).As would be expected, depression is associatedwith greater social/peer problems(e.g., Klein, Lewinsohn, & Seeley, 1997;Marcotte, Fortin, Potvin, & Papillon,2002) <strong>and</strong> a greater number <strong>of</strong> stressful lifeevents, including interpersonal stressorsthat may have a particularly negative impactfor young girls (Hankin, Mermelstein, &Roesch, 2007). Although a strong case canbe made for depression being either thecause or the effect in these relations, theimportant issue in assessment is to approachreferrals for depression comprehensively soas to determine all <strong>of</strong> the relevant targets<strong>of</strong> intervention. The research on childhooddepression clearly indicates a need to assessfor symptomatology <strong>and</strong> difficulties in avariety <strong>of</strong> domains, as well as to assess indicators<strong>of</strong> impairment related to depressivesymptoms (e.g., truancy).Furthermore, some research suggeststhat one should <strong>of</strong>ten screen for depressionwhen a medical illness is present. Inone study, depression <strong>and</strong> anxiety had a41% higher prevalence rate in individualswith medical illness. Depression that is secondaryto medical illness may have differentoutcomes, thus also requiring differingapproaches to intervention (Cassen, 1990).Issues <strong>of</strong> comorbidity can also be confusedwith the notions <strong>of</strong> primary <strong>and</strong> secondaryconditions. The primacy <strong>of</strong> the conditioncan be especially important for internalizing


418 chapter 18 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression <strong>and</strong> anxietydisorders because it may be important todifferentially intervene based on knowledge<strong>of</strong> primacy. A child with both depression<strong>and</strong> anxiety may require intervention fordepression first, for example, if the child ispresenting risk factors for suicidal behavior.Relatedly, a child suffering from anxiety <strong>and</strong>depression may require intervention for anxietysymptoms initially if compulsive h<strong>and</strong>washingbehavior has resulted in impairedfunctioning in school, at home, <strong>and</strong> amongpeers. Determining primacy <strong>of</strong> conditionis fraught with problems (Winokur, 1990),<strong>and</strong> the implications <strong>of</strong> doing so are not yetentirely clear at this stage <strong>of</strong> research on thecomorbidity <strong>of</strong> internalizing disorders inchildren. Some evidence suggests that anxietyor externalizing problems <strong>of</strong>ten precededepression rather than the other way around(see Hankin et al., 2007). The clinicianshould attempt to distinguish the primacy<strong>of</strong> anxiety <strong>and</strong> depression for the purpose <strong>of</strong>developing recommendations for targets <strong>of</strong>intervention. Some characteristics that maybe useful in such work include symptomatology,course <strong>of</strong> symptoms <strong>and</strong> condition,family resemblance <strong>and</strong> background, testresults, <strong>and</strong> response to prior treatment/interventions (Winokur, 1990).Specialized Measures <strong>of</strong>DepressionAside from a general approach (i.e., likelythrough clinical interview) for gatheringevidence on symptomatology, family history,stressors, comorbidity, <strong>and</strong> developmentalcourse in the assessment <strong>of</strong>depression, the clinician also has the abilityto select specific tools that assess depressivesymptoms in an in-depth, efficientmanner. Several examples <strong>of</strong> such tools arereviewed in this section.<strong>Child</strong>ren’s Depression Inventory (CDI;Kovacs, 1992)The <strong>Child</strong>ren’s Depression Inventory(CDI; Kovacs, 1991) is a 27-item depressionself-report scale for ages 7 through17. A derivative <strong>of</strong> the Beck DepressionInventory (Semrud-Clikeman, 1990), theCDI enjoys a long history <strong>of</strong> clinical use,particularly in research investigations.Scale ContentThe 27 items <strong>of</strong> the CDI assess a widerange <strong>of</strong> depressive symptomatology, much<strong>of</strong> which is included in popular diagnosticsystems such as the DSM. Items assess sadness,cognitive symptoms, social problems,somatic complaints, <strong>and</strong> acting-out behaviors.In addition to a total score, there arefive subscales available: Negative Mood,Interpersonal Problems, Ineffectiveness,Anhedonia, <strong>and</strong> Negative Self-esteem.Administration <strong>and</strong> ScoringContributing to the popularity <strong>of</strong> the CDIis its ease <strong>of</strong> administration <strong>and</strong> scoring.The scale can be completed in minutesby a competent reader. Each item is composed<strong>of</strong> three stems from which the childmust choose one. The three stems typicallyrepresent differing levels <strong>of</strong> severity<strong>of</strong> depressive symptomatology. Items areassigned scores <strong>of</strong> 2, 1, or 0, where a higherraw score reflects more severe symptomatology.Linear T-scores are available for the CDItotal <strong>and</strong> subscale scores, with the authorusing a T-score <strong>of</strong> 65 or higher as indicative<strong>of</strong> clinical significance (Kovacs, 1992). Separatenorms are available for 7–12-year-oldboys, 7–12-year-old girls, 13–17-year-oldboys, <strong>and</strong> 13–17-year-old girlsNormingThe CDI possesses essentially local normsbased on a sample <strong>of</strong> 1,266 Florida schoolchildren in grades 2 through 8 (Kovacs,1992). Seventy-seven percent <strong>of</strong> childrenin this normative sample were white,whereas the remaining 23% were AfricanAmerican, Hispanic, or Native American(specific percentages for each group werenot reported). The normative samplegives little evidence <strong>of</strong> national representation;hence, the degree to which CDInorms can be recommended as a national


chapter 18 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression <strong>and</strong> anxiety419or some other st<strong>and</strong>ard is not known.However, it is apparent that at least someminority groups were underrepresented.ReliabilityThe reliability <strong>of</strong> the overall CDI score isgood with internal consistency coefficientstypically in the .80s. The coefficients forthe scales are modest at best, ranging from.59 (Interpersonal Problems) to .68 (NegativeSelf-esteem). Test-retest coefficientsfor the total score are adequate based ondata reported from a number <strong>of</strong> studiesemploying a range <strong>of</strong> time frames (i.e., 1week to 1 year; see Kovacs, 1992).ValidityInternal evidence <strong>of</strong> validity has beenassessed via numerous factor-analyticinvestigations. Kovacs (1991) suggestsusing a five-factor solution for conceptualizingsubscales. The presence <strong>of</strong> more thanone factor, however, does not necessarilysupport the validity <strong>of</strong> the CDI. The factorstudies do suggest dominance by a largefirst factor (Cooper, 1990).Criterion-related studies are generallysupportive <strong>of</strong> the CDI as a measure <strong>of</strong>internalizing symptomatology. The CDIcorrelates significantly with other measures<strong>of</strong> anxiety <strong>and</strong> depression (see Myers& Winters, 2002 for review).Some <strong>of</strong> the validity evidence associatedwith the CDI has been described as“mixed.” Although, for example, the CDIhas shown some validity for differentiatingbetween nonclinical children <strong>and</strong> samples<strong>of</strong> children with depression, several studieshave shown that the CDI cannot accuratelydistinguish between samples withdepression <strong>and</strong> other psychiatric groups(Myers & Winters; Silverman & Rabian,1999).Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesThe CDI continues to be a widely usedmeasure. Among the CDI’s strengths are:1. A long research history that has contributedto considerable trust by thepsychological re-search community2. Ease <strong>of</strong> administration <strong>and</strong> scoring3. Relatively low cost4. Evidence <strong>of</strong> concurrent validity withmeasures <strong>of</strong> internalizing symptomsThe CDI, however, has some noteworthyweaknesses that caution against overinterpretation.1. The wisdom <strong>of</strong> <strong>of</strong>fering such rigid cutscores for screening or diagnostic purposesin the manual is questionable, asthese suggestions are <strong>of</strong>ten applied rigidlyby the user (Kamphaus, 2001).2. The scale scores <strong>of</strong> the CDI have fairlylow internal consistency for clinicalpurposes.3. The norm-referenced scores from theCDI should be interpreted cautiouslygiven their lack <strong>of</strong> representativeness.In fact, a crucial flaw such as this suggeststhat the CDI may be more usefulfor research purposes than for clinicalassessment <strong>and</strong> diagnostic decisions.The CDI, in fact, does have animpressive history <strong>of</strong> research utilityfor which norm-referenced scores are<strong>of</strong>ten <strong>of</strong> little interest.4. The ability <strong>of</strong> the CDI to assist withdifferential diagnosis is questionable(Silverman & Rubian, 1999).Reynolds <strong>Adolescent</strong> Depression Scale,2nd Edition (RADS-2; Reynolds, 2002)The Reynolds <strong>Adolescent</strong> DepressionScale, 2nd edition (RADS-2; Reynolds) isdesigned to assess symptomatology associatedwith depression via self-report in adolescentsages 11–20. The RADS-2 is closelybased on its predecessor (Reynolds, 1986),although it may be used with a slightlywider age range than was recommendedfor the original version. The RADS-2 isnot designed provide a diagnosis <strong>of</strong> a specificdepressive disorder. However, it isdesigned for use as a screening measure orfor research.


420 chapter 18 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression <strong>and</strong> anxietyScale ContentThe RADS-2 provides a thorough sampling<strong>of</strong> depressive symptoms that areincluded in the DSM <strong>and</strong> other nosologies<strong>and</strong> takes approximately 5–10 min toadminister.Administration <strong>and</strong> ScoringThe RADS-2 uses 30 items to which theclient responds with one <strong>of</strong> four choices <strong>of</strong>frequency: almost never, hardly ever, sometimes,<strong>and</strong> most <strong>of</strong> the time. Six <strong>of</strong> theseitems are considered critical items. A TotalDepression score, as well as scores on fourscales: Dysphoric Mood, Anhedonia/NegativeAffect, Negative Self-evaluation, <strong>and</strong>Somatic Complaints. The items are placedon a single page where a template is usedto easily sum up item scores to obtain rawscores. Raw scores are then converted topercentile ranks based on the total sample<strong>and</strong> by gender <strong>and</strong> grade, <strong>and</strong> unlike theprevious version <strong>of</strong> the RADS, T-scoresare <strong>of</strong>fered. The T-scores are based on theoriginal distributions <strong>of</strong> the total score <strong>and</strong>scale scores in the norm sample (i.e., linearT scores). This change represents a significantimprovement <strong>and</strong> brings the RADS-2in line with the scores available for manyother measures <strong>of</strong> child functioning. Reynolds(2002), in line with other approachesto interpreting rating scales, suggests thata T score <strong>of</strong> 65 or higher be consideredclinically significant <strong>and</strong> worthy <strong>of</strong> closeconsideration (along with other assessmentdata) in case conceptualization.NormingThe RADS-2 was normed on 9,052 participantsfrom seven states <strong>and</strong> one Canadianprovince recruited from schools. Thesample is described as socioeconomicallydiverse, with representation reported byage, gender, <strong>and</strong> ethnicity. The match <strong>of</strong>the sample to meaningful national criteriasuch as census data is not given. Theauthor reports increased representativeness<strong>of</strong> Hispanics in the RADS-2 normsample; however, Hispanics (i.e., 4.5% <strong>of</strong>the norm sample) <strong>and</strong> African Americans(7.4% <strong>of</strong> the norm sample) both appearto be underrepresented. A clinical sampleconsisting <strong>of</strong> 297 participants was alsorecruited. This sample was mostly female(55.6%), with the vast majority <strong>of</strong> the clinicalsample being Caucasian (86.1%).ReliabilityReliability indices for the RADS-2 totalscore are good, with the internal consistencycoefficients <strong>of</strong> the total score <strong>and</strong>scales ranging from .86 to .93 in the normingsample (Reynolds, 2002). Two-weektest-retest reliability was also good, rangingfrom .77 for Somatic Complaints to .85for the Total Depression score.ValidityLike the RADS, the RADS-2 has good contentvalidity in that it systematically assessesmany <strong>of</strong> the symptoms commonly associatedwith the syndrome. To date, there are limiteddata on the construct validity <strong>of</strong> the RADS-2, although the manual reports higher correlationsbetween the RADS-2 <strong>and</strong> HamiltonDepression Rating Scale <strong>Clinical</strong> Interviewthan were enjoyed by the RADS. In addition,the RADS-2 total <strong>and</strong> scale scores showedmoderate to high correlations (i.e., rangingfrom the mid .40s to high .70s) with theMajor Depression <strong>and</strong> Dysthymic Disorderscales <strong>of</strong> the <strong>Adolescent</strong> PsychopatologyScale (Reynolds, 1998).Many studies show reasonablecorrelations between the original RADS<strong>and</strong> measures <strong>of</strong> depression <strong>and</strong> similarinternalizing constructs. Research (e.g.,Krefetz, Steer, Gulab, & Beck, 2002) hasshown good convergence between theRADS <strong>and</strong> Beck Depression Inventory(BDI), with consistently significant correlationsranging from .68 to .76.Additional research has shown that theprevious version <strong>of</strong> the RADS correlatewell with a measure <strong>of</strong> suicide risk (Kaczmarek,Hagan, & Kettler, 2006) <strong>and</strong> to bepredictive <strong>of</strong> later suicidality in hospitalizedadolescents (Huth-Bocks et al., 2007).


chapter 18 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression <strong>and</strong> anxiety421Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesThe RADS-2 has many apparent strengthsincluding:1. A thorough assessment <strong>of</strong> depressivesymptomatology rooted in contentvalidity2. Norm-referenced scores, unlike its predecessor3. Ease <strong>of</strong> administration <strong>and</strong> scoring4. Good recommendations regardinginterpretation in the manualWeaknesses may include:1. More information <strong>of</strong> the norming sampleis provided than was the case for theRADS, but it still does not appear to berepresentative <strong>of</strong> the U.S. population, particularlyin regards to ethnicity.2. A quite limited research based on theRADS-2, particularly in comparison tothe CDI.3. The response scale (i.e., “almost never,”“hardly ever,” “sometimes,” “most <strong>of</strong>the time”) may be difficult for someadolescent respondents to interpret.Reynolds <strong>Child</strong> Depression Scale(RCDS; Reynolds, 1989)The Reynolds <strong>Child</strong> Depression Scale(RCDS; Reynolds) is a downward extension<strong>of</strong> the RADS designed for ages 8 through12. It is designed with the same rationale <strong>and</strong>intended uses as the RADS.Scale ContentThe scale includes 29 items that use a Likerttypefour-choice format. The four responseoptions measure frequency <strong>of</strong> symptomatologywith the following options: almost never,sometimes, a lot <strong>of</strong> the time, <strong>and</strong> all the time.A 30th item uses a rebus response formatwith five faces depicting a range <strong>of</strong> happyto sad faces. As with the RADS, some itemsare reverse-scored in order to discourageresponse sets. The items were selected withdiagnostic criteria clearly in mind, ensuringthat the RCDS assesses a good range <strong>of</strong>depressive symptoms.Administration <strong>and</strong> ScoringAn attempt was made to make all items readableby second-graders to enhance its utilitywith this younger age range. The RCDS isalso self-contained on one sheet for convenience,<strong>and</strong> a scoring template is provided.A machine-scannable form is also availablefor group or other large-scale administration<strong>of</strong> the RCDS. Although a cut score isprovided, the author cautions against its usefor diagnostic purposes (Reynolds, 1989).Of course, cut scores are always questionable,even for screening purposes. Again,only percentile rank scores, with their attendantproblems, are <strong>of</strong>fered for the RCDS inlieu <strong>of</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard scores.NormingThe RCDS was normed with a sample <strong>of</strong>1,620 children in the Midwest <strong>and</strong> otherportions <strong>of</strong> the country. Reynolds (1989)presents data showing similar raw scores forsamples tested in Sacramento, California <strong>and</strong>Beloit, Wisconsin <strong>and</strong> concludes that geographicrepresentation is adequate for thesample. Unfortunately, SES was not specificallyindicated for the norming sample, withthe exception that some students had teachersestimate their SES. Gender, ethnicity,<strong>and</strong> grade data are reported for the sample.Like the RADS-2, however, the representation<strong>of</strong> the sample in comparison to criteriasuch as census data is not given.ReliabilityThe internal consistency <strong>of</strong> the RCDS isstrong, with coefficients at most grade levels<strong>of</strong> .90. A coefficient <strong>of</strong> .87 was obtainedeven at grade 3. Test-retest at 2- <strong>and</strong> 4-weekintervals is strong, with coefficients rangingfrom .81 to .92 (see Myers & Winters, 2002for review; Reynolds, 1989).ValidityReynolds (1989) makes a good case forcontent validity. In addition, five studies <strong>of</strong>the relationship <strong>of</strong> the RCDS to the CDI


422 chapter 18 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression <strong>and</strong> anxietyproduced significant correlations, mostly inthe .70s. The RCDS has also shown goodcorrelations with a variety <strong>of</strong> other indicators<strong>of</strong> childhood depression (see Myers& Winters, 2002). Correlations with theRCMAS (see below) are also significant,ranging from .60 to .67 in three studies.A five-factor solution is reported for theRCDS, with the first factor clearly dominant.The presence <strong>of</strong> a strong first factorargues (lays?) for an emphasis on total scoreinterpretation.Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesThe RCDS is strikingly similar to theRADS in terms <strong>of</strong> its strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses(see above). However, the RCDS,unlike the RADS-2, uses only percentilesas norm-referenced scores. We have someconcern regarding the use <strong>of</strong> norm-referencedscores with the RCDS, given thelack <strong>of</strong> information regarding the normingsample. The RCDS may be a good way toefficiently gain self reports on depressivesymptoms, but interpretation <strong>of</strong> the RCDSshould be descriptive rather than reliant onordinal scale data (i.e., percentiles).Peer Nomination Inventory forDepression (PNID)The PNID includes 20 items that assessthree aspects <strong>of</strong> depression: depression,happiness, <strong>and</strong> popularity (Lefkowitz &Tesiny, 1980). It is designed for classroombasedassessment. The PNID is quite differentfrom the other measures reviewedin this section in that it is based on peernominations. A child’s score on each itemis the number <strong>of</strong> nominations divided bythe number <strong>of</strong> students who rated the childin the classroom. The item scores are thensummed to yield a total score. Normativedata for the PNID have been collected forgrades 3 through 5. Reliability data for thePNID are adequate, but correlations withself-report measures <strong>of</strong> depression havebeen low (Merrell, 1994) to moderate(Semrud-Clikeman, 1990). Although nota strong measure <strong>of</strong> clinical depression perse, the PNID could serve as an additionaldata source in the evaluation <strong>of</strong> children bygauging a child’s popularity, social interaction,<strong>and</strong> happiness as perceived by peers.Information <strong>of</strong> this nature could assist intargeting for intervention classroom behaviorsthat improve a child’s adaptation in thisimportant setting.An <strong>Assessment</strong> Strategyfor DepressionAs we have in previous chapters, we recommenda five-stage assessment processfor the assessment <strong>of</strong> depression: screening,classification, co-morbidities, alternativecauses, <strong>and</strong> treatment considerations(see Table 18.1).In light <strong>of</strong> the prevalence <strong>of</strong> depression, itscommon comorbidity with other problems,its adverse effects on development in avariety <strong>of</strong> domains, <strong>and</strong> its less-than-flagrantsymptomatology, we recommendthat every child who is seen by a clinicianshould be screened for depression.Research findings support the need forscreening all referrals that are seen notonly by psychologists, but also by otherpr<strong>of</strong>essionals (e.g., pediatricians, teachers).The screening process can be timeefficient<strong>and</strong> may include:l Administering a brief, self-report inventoryto the child, who has adequatereading ability.l Asking parents whether or not their childexhibits symptoms given in the DSM-IVcriteria.l Querying teachers, nurses, gr<strong>and</strong>parents,or others about symptoms <strong>of</strong> depression.Any one <strong>of</strong> these screening methods takesonly a few minutes. In fact, querying both


chapter 18 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression <strong>and</strong> anxiety423Table 18.1 The <strong>Assessment</strong> Questions Related to <strong>Child</strong>hood Depression <strong>and</strong> Implications for<strong>Assessment</strong><strong>Assessment</strong>QuestionScreeningClassificationComorbiditiesAlternativeCausesTreatmentConsiderationsImplications for <strong>Assessment</strong>Administer screening measures or screen during interview. Assess forcritical symptoms (i.e., suicidality, psychosis). Determine need for furtherassessment.Assess for presence <strong>of</strong> symptoms that meet DSM-IV criteria. Determinestability <strong>and</strong> duration <strong>of</strong> symptoms.Determine onset.Determine family resemblance.Assess for presence <strong>of</strong> anxiety disorders, ADHD, LD/MR, eating disorders,or Oppositional Defiant Disorder/Conduct Disorder. Determine the influence<strong>of</strong> depression on school performance, absenteeism, etc.Assess social relationships <strong>and</strong> peer social status.Assess for presence <strong>of</strong> substance abuse, particularly in adolescents.Obtain developmental <strong>and</strong> medical histories.Rule out dysthymia <strong>and</strong> Post-traumatic Stress Disorder.Rule out medical problems that are associated with depression.Assess for presence <strong>of</strong> maladaptive cognitions. Assess for presence <strong>of</strong>chronic stressors. Assess parental depression <strong>and</strong> parenting style.Evaluate response to previous interventions (e.g., psychotropics).the child <strong>and</strong> a parent would take only a fewminutes. Screening efforts may allow the clinicianto implement intervention in order toavert considerable suffering (see Box 18.1).Classification <strong>of</strong> a child as depressedrequires meeting the DSM-IV-TR criteria,<strong>and</strong> by this we mean meeting all criteriabased on apparently valid information.Possessing the necessary symptomatologyis only a first step in meeting criteria. Thepsychologist has to be sure that malingering,response sets, or other threats to validityhave not had an effect on the report <strong>of</strong>symptoms. An adolescent, for example,may be asked if he/she has experienceddecreased appetite, fatigue, sleeplessness,agitation, <strong>and</strong> suicidal ideation. He/shemay discern that it is wise to deny all <strong>of</strong>these difficulties if he/she thinks that hemay be a c<strong>and</strong>idate for inpatient or partialhospitalization treatment. Clinicians whotreat adolescents have seen cases in whichthe youth denies suicidal ideation, althoughhe/she may have been transferred from ahospital emergency room because <strong>of</strong> a suicideattempt. An array <strong>of</strong> valid assessmentmethods is necessary in order to ensureadequate documentation <strong>of</strong> symptoms:l Structured <strong>and</strong> semi-structured interviews(e.g., K-SADS; see Chap. 11) withchildren, their parents, teachers, <strong>and</strong>other caregivers are necessary to assessfor the presence <strong>of</strong> symptomatology<strong>and</strong> their effects on the child’s functioningin different environments. Indeed,semi-structured interviews, because <strong>of</strong>their flexibility <strong>and</strong> comprehensive havebeen described as “best practice” byKlein et al. (2005; p. 427).l Self-report inventories <strong>and</strong> parent <strong>and</strong>teacher rating scales can provide furtherdocumentation <strong>of</strong> symptoms, screenfor comorbid problems, <strong>and</strong> assess for


424 chapter 18 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression <strong>and</strong> anxietyBox 18.1A Case <strong>of</strong> Individual Screening for DepressionScreening for depression is crucial because <strong>of</strong>the nature <strong>of</strong> internalizing symptomatology.Such screening is time-efficient in most settings.This case example illustrates the importance<strong>and</strong> practicality <strong>of</strong> screening.Matt was referred for an evaluation because<strong>of</strong> his parents’ concerns about his academicprogress. He is a 16-year-old junior in highschool. His parents <strong>and</strong> teacher suspect thathe has a reading disability.Matt’s mother reported a long history <strong>of</strong> academicproblems for Matt, including considerablespeech delays as a youngster. He was reportedlyenrolled in speech therapy throughout his elementaryschool years. He was <strong>of</strong>ten ridiculed byhis peers for misarticulations <strong>and</strong> stuttering. Hisspeech problems have reportedly declined somewhatin high school. He has, however, had longst<strong>and</strong>ingproblems with reading <strong>and</strong> spelling.Matt’s parents expressed some frustrationbecause they feel that Matt has been passedalong from grade to grade because he is veryquiet <strong>and</strong> never causes any problems.He continues to have trouble in most <strong>of</strong> hisclasses. He is performing adequately in math,but he complains that he has trouble takingnotes in class <strong>and</strong> comprehending them afterhe takes them. He reportedly failed in hisEnglish class during the first semester <strong>of</strong> thecurrent academic year. He is very distraughtabout these failures because <strong>of</strong> his desire to goto college following graduation <strong>and</strong> his concernsabout disappointing his parents. Mattalso reported some other recent stressors in hislife. He recently broke <strong>of</strong>f a 1-year relationshipwith a girlfriend, <strong>and</strong> he is receiving rehabilitationfor a severe back injury that he incurredin a bicycle accident six months ago. Matt alsohas two sisters who were reportedly diagnosedwith developmental disorders, one with a readingdisability <strong>and</strong> the other with ADHD.Matt gave the impression <strong>of</strong> being welladjusted<strong>and</strong> socially skilled. He was impeccablygroomed <strong>and</strong> a good conversationalist.He even asked the examiner questions abouthis interests <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>fered his full cooperationwith the assessment process. Matt describedmany successes in his life, including beingchosen as captain <strong>of</strong> the baseball team at hisschool. He apparently has an active <strong>and</strong> successfulsocial life. Aside from athletics, heis reportedly involved in a variety <strong>of</strong> otherextracurricular activities.The results <strong>of</strong> the assessment producedclear evidence <strong>of</strong> below-average achievementin reading, even though Matt’s intellectualfunctioning was slightly above average.Following reports <strong>of</strong> moderate levels <strong>of</strong>depression on the parent <strong>and</strong> self-report versions<strong>of</strong> the BASC-2, Matt was asked to completethe <strong>Child</strong> Depression Inventory (CDI).Matt acknowledged enough symptoms on theCDI to fall into the mild/moderate range <strong>of</strong>depressive symptoms. He acknowledged guiltabout past failures, uncontrollable sadness,insomnia, hopelessness, decreased appetite,increased fatigue, low self-esteem, concernsabout his appearance, lack <strong>of</strong> interest in socialactivities, <strong>and</strong> occasional thoughts <strong>of</strong> hurtinghimself, among other symptoms. The incongruencebetween the symptoms endorsed onrating scales <strong>and</strong> his presentation was striking.During the feedback session, Matt <strong>and</strong> hisparents were apprised <strong>of</strong> these findings <strong>and</strong>asked to verify their validity, which they did.He was then referred for follow-up assessment<strong>and</strong> intervention for his depression.This case illustrates how time-efficientscreening for depression can be <strong>of</strong> potentialimportance. In this scenario, depression wasnot suspected by the referral sources, theexamining psychologist, or the client.the presence <strong>of</strong> response sets or otherthreats to validity.l History- taking from the child <strong>and</strong>/orparents is necessary in order to establishadequate duration <strong>and</strong> pervasiveness <strong>of</strong>symptoms as well as to obtain valuableinformation in areas such as family psychiatrichistory (Klein et al., 2005).


chapter 18 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression <strong>and</strong> anxiety425Anxiety disorders are especially importantto consider when ruling out comorbidities<strong>of</strong> depression. Anxiety symptomatologyshould be assessed concurrently with all <strong>of</strong>the data collection necessary for classification.Omnibus rating scales that include awell-validated anxiety scale <strong>and</strong> interviewschedules that also assess for anxiety arehelpful in this regard. The full realm <strong>of</strong>anxiety problems should be routinely ruledout. Somatization problems, phobias,milder fears, obsessive-compulsive disorder,<strong>and</strong> separation anxiety disorder shouldbe distinguished.One <strong>of</strong> the alternative causes thatlooms large in cases <strong>of</strong> depression is amedical difficulty that can cause depression.We recommend that a clinicianassessing a child who displays the symptoms<strong>of</strong> depression should have access tothe results <strong>of</strong> a recent physical exam, or anexam should be scheduled if one has notbeen conducted within a few months <strong>of</strong>the psychological evaluation.The psychological evaluation must alsorule out other conditions that may appearto be a major depressive episode. In thisregard, thorough history-taking is necessaryin order to rule out transient statessuch as bereavement (e.g., depression dueto a child’s mother being called away toactive duty in the armed forces), adjustmentdisorder with depressed mood, ordysthymia.Finally, assessment methods mustbe selected that help direct interventionefforts. Rating scales may be used tobroadly sample behavior in home, school,<strong>and</strong> other settings. A broad sampling <strong>of</strong>behavior allows the clinician to considerdepressive problems <strong>and</strong> other difficultiesthat may in some way adversely affectthe child’s adaptation. If, for example, thechild also shows considerable evidence <strong>of</strong>worry about school difficulties, an adjustmentin the child’s curriculum may have apositive effect on the child’s perceived level<strong>of</strong> stress, which, in turn, could help abatedepressive symptoms.An assessment <strong>of</strong> academic achievementmay also be necessary in order to determinewhether or not a child is suffering fromacademic under-achievement associatedwith long-term depressive symptomatology.Teacher interviews <strong>and</strong> norm-referenced<strong>and</strong> curriculum-based measures maybe helpful in this regard.Self- <strong>and</strong> other informant ratings arealso critical for initial assessment <strong>and</strong> formonitoring response to interventions. Theprevious research citing long-term adaptationdifficulties for children with depressiveproblems suggests that careful monitoring<strong>of</strong> the effects <strong>of</strong> treatment is necessary, <strong>and</strong>post-treatment follow-up is warranted inorder to prevent or effectively deal withindications <strong>of</strong> relapse.A case study <strong>of</strong> an adolescent girl withdepression is provided in Box 18.2, illustratingthis approach to assessing depression.Anxiety Disorders<strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong>hoodThe term anxiety can be traced to the Latinroot angere, which means to cause distressor strangle. That is, unpleasant feelings<strong>of</strong> anxiety, including potentially intensefeelings <strong>of</strong> fear, dread, <strong>and</strong> worry, evokeemotions associated with strangulation(Stavrakaki & Ellis, 1989). In there, discussion<strong>of</strong> evidence-based assessment <strong>of</strong> anxietyin children, Silverman <strong>and</strong> Ollendick(2005) highlight Barlow’s relatively recentdefinition <strong>of</strong> anxiety as being particularlyuseful for assessment <strong>and</strong> subsequentintervention. For Barlow (2002), anxietycan be described as being dominated byconcerns about future unpredictability orloss <strong>of</strong> control which is accompanied by a“shift in attention to the focus on potentiallydangerous events or one’s own affectiveresponse to these events” (p. 104).Problems that are commonly discussedunder the general rubric <strong>of</strong> anxiety include


Box 18.2Sample Case <strong>of</strong> an <strong>Adolescent</strong> with DepressionSeveral assessment methods are applied in thecase <strong>of</strong> Corin. The report is given as it wouldbe written in a typical psychological report,with commentary regarding interpretation.Corin, a 16-year-old girl, was referred for apsychological evaluation by her mother due toacademic problems. According to Mrs. Jacobs(mother), Corin has trouble concentrating inschool <strong>and</strong> she thought that Corin’s schoolproblems may be caused by ADHD.Corin lives with her mother <strong>and</strong> youngerbrother. Corin’s parents are divorced, <strong>and</strong> shereportedly rarely sees her father.Mrs. Jacobs stated that there were no difficultiesduring her pregnancy with Corin. Duringbirth, however, Corin was in fetal distress,<strong>and</strong> Mrs. Jacobs had an emergency Cesare<strong>and</strong>elivery. Corin was born full-term <strong>and</strong>weighed 8 pounds, 7 ounces. Corin’s motherdescribed her development as normal in thatshe reached all motor <strong>and</strong> language developmentalmilestones at age-appropriate times.Corin is, reportedly, in very good health. Sheis supposed to wear contact lenses, but hermother reports that Corin rarely wears them.Corin’s parents divorced when Corin was5 years old. In addition, Corin was reportedlyvery close to her maternal aunt who diedearlier this year. Corin stated that her motheris constantly on her back <strong>and</strong> that she has todo too much work around the house. Thereis a family history <strong>of</strong> alcohol abuse on bothher mother’s <strong>and</strong> father’s sides <strong>of</strong> the family.Corin’s mother <strong>and</strong> father also both have areported history <strong>of</strong> alcohol abuse.Corin is currently in the 10th grade. Corin’smother noted that Corin did well in elementaryschool, always receiving A’s or B’s. Startingin the seventh grade, Corin’s academic performancereportedly deteriorated. Her mothernoticed that she had problems reading aloud<strong>and</strong> problems with mathematics.Corin received her first failing grade in theninth grade <strong>and</strong> was moved from algebra backto pre-algebra, which she had in eighth grade.According to her mother, her grades continuedto deteriorate, <strong>and</strong> currently she is receiving an“F” in history <strong>and</strong> English, a “C” in science, algebra,<strong>and</strong> Spanish, <strong>and</strong> an “A” in photography.Corin’s teachers feel that she is smart but thatshe does not put forth significant effort. Theyreported that she exhibits behavioral problemsin the classroom. She is reportedly constantlytalking <strong>and</strong> disrupting the class. Her algebrateacher stated that she is fond <strong>of</strong> Corin but thatshe plays around with her classmates too much.She reportedly talks back to her teachers, whichis consistent with the reports from last year’steachers. Corin’s teachers also reported that shefrequently sleeps through class.According to records kept by the schoolfor the past two years, Corin is <strong>of</strong>ten excessivelytardy <strong>and</strong> absent from class. In the fall<strong>of</strong> this year, she served two days <strong>of</strong> suspensionin study hall for being rude to a new teacher.Subsequent to this incident, a parent conferenceat school resulted in Mrs. Jacobs agreeingto have Corin evaluated in order to ruleout ADHD.Corin has reportedly been seen in counselingat a community clinic for two years. Her currentcounselor reported that Corin remains under agreat deal <strong>of</strong> stress due to her parents’ divorce,her aunt’s death, <strong>and</strong> her continuing academicvproblems. Mrs. Jacobs mentioned that Corinalso <strong>of</strong>ten has stomachaches <strong>and</strong> dizzy spells,which she attributes to stress. Corin complains <strong>of</strong>not being able to go to sleep; once asleep, she frequentlyhas nightmares about dying. She reportedthat she worries about getting cancer. She toldthe examiner that one time, she felt a bump onher leg <strong>and</strong> thought that she had cancer.Mrs. Jacobs reported that Corin drinksalcohol regularly, <strong>and</strong> that at times, she drinksquite heavily (i.e., 12–15 beers). Corin told theexaminer that she sometimes blacks out on theweekends from drinking. She also admitted t<strong>of</strong>requent use <strong>of</strong> marijuana, <strong>and</strong> she reportedlysmokes at least one pack <strong>of</strong> cigarettes a day. Afew weeks before the evaluation, Corin wasdetained by police because <strong>of</strong> skipping school<strong>and</strong> being intoxicated.In two interview sessions, Corin describedherself as being depressed. She told the examinerthat, for the past three weeks, she hasfelt angry, sad, <strong>and</strong> hopeless. Throughout theinterview Corin appeared to be agitated.Corin’s intellectual functioning was estimatedas being above her same-aged peers.She did well on tests that were timed <strong>and</strong>involved physical manipulation <strong>of</strong> materials<strong>and</strong> the results indicate that her nonverbalreasoning abilities are better developed thanher abilities in verbal reasoning.


chapter 18 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression <strong>and</strong> anxiety427Generalized Anxiety Disorder, SeparationAnxiety Disorder, Agoraphobia, SocialPhobia, Simple Phobia, School Phobia,other fears, Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder,Post-traumatic Stress Disorder, <strong>and</strong>Panic Disorder, among others.Although they share obvious features,these anxiety problems also compriseunique facets that have clear implicationsfor assessment, differential diagnosis, <strong>and</strong>treatment planning. According to theDSM-IV-TR (2000):(a) Generalized Anxiety Disorder is characterizedby excessive, unfocused worry;(b) Separation Anxiety Disorder is centeredaround significant distress whenseparated from primary caretakers orhome;(c) Agoraphobia features anxiety regardingbeing in places or situations fromwhich escape is difficult or perceivedas difficult;(d) Social Phobia is characterized by fear <strong>of</strong>situations that involve social evaluation;(e) Simple Phobias are marked by significant,intense fear <strong>of</strong> particular objectsor stimuli, whether the exposure isdirect or anticipated;(f) Obsessive Compulsive Disorder consists<strong>of</strong> persistent, recurrent thoughts<strong>and</strong> behavioral compulsions associatedwith impaired functioning;(g) Post-traumatic Stress Disorder is basedon re-experiencing <strong>of</strong> a traumatic event<strong>and</strong> increased arousal; <strong>and</strong>(h) Panic Disorder features a sudden occurrence<strong>of</strong> intense physiological <strong>and</strong> cognitivesymptoms <strong>of</strong> anxiety that tend tooccur unexpectedly.The measures <strong>of</strong> anxiety that <strong>of</strong>ten usedin child assessments typically screen aspects<strong>of</strong> all <strong>of</strong> these problem areas broadly. It isthen up to the clinician to obtain the necessaryinformation to best conceptualize thespecific anxiety problem or disorder <strong>and</strong>then make recommendations accordingly.Characteristics <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong>hoodAnxiety DisordersComparatively speaking, much less isknown about childhood anxiety disorderscompared to the knowledge base fordepression. Classification schemes <strong>and</strong> theassessment instruments themselves are primarilybased on research on adult anxietydisorders (Silverman & Ollendick, 2005).There are issues from research on children<strong>and</strong> adolescents specifically that haveimportance for assessment practice:l In many respects, anxiety disorders canbe conceptualized as exaggerations <strong>of</strong>normal responses to developmentaldem<strong>and</strong>s (e.g., separation anxiety inyoung children; anxiety in social situationsfor adolescents). It is when theanxiety response is impaired that interventionis needed (Silverman & Ollendick,2005).l Evidence regarding the stability <strong>and</strong>prognosis <strong>of</strong> anxiety disorders in childrenis quite mixed (Weems & Stickle,2005).l Research using the Anxiety DisordersInterview Schedule for <strong>Child</strong>ren (ADIS)has found strong parent-child interraterreliability for individual anxiety disorderdiagnostic categories (Lyneham, Abbott,& Rapee, 2007) but also substantialparent-child disagreement (Grills &Ollendick, 2003). These results shouldcaution the clinician to attend to reasonsfor convergence <strong>and</strong> divergence amonginformants.l The phenomenology <strong>of</strong> childhood anxietydisorders appears to be very similarto that <strong>of</strong> adults (Silverman, 1993).However, the assessment methods usedto make diagnoses <strong>of</strong> childhood anxiety


428 chapter 18 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression <strong>and</strong> anxietydisorders have <strong>of</strong>ten not been developedwith potential developmental influenceson the manifestation <strong>of</strong> anxiety in mind(Silverman & Ollendick, 2005).l While anxiety <strong>of</strong>ten is apparent in cases<strong>of</strong> depression, anxiety disorders also<strong>of</strong>ten occur in the absence <strong>of</strong> depression(Silverman, 1993). Anxiety with comorbidproblems is associated with pooreroutcomes than anxiety alone (see Saavedra& Silverman, 2002).l Separation Anxiety Disorder typicallyoccurs in children less than 11–13 years<strong>of</strong> age (Strauss, 1993; Weems, Hammond-Laurence,Silverman, & Ginsburg,1998); however, social phobiabecomes more common in adolescence(Weems et al., 1998).l Fears <strong>of</strong> animals, the darkness, <strong>and</strong>heights are more likely to occur atyounger ages than social phobias which,in turn, occur at younger ages than agoraphobia(Strauss, 1993).l There are two main types <strong>of</strong> school phobia:those that come on acutely in a childwho has functioned reasonably normallybefore onset, <strong>and</strong> those that arise in achild who has had similar problems fromthe preschool years <strong>and</strong> has never developedthe social skills that would permitnormal functioning (Berg, 1993).l The average <strong>of</strong> onset for childhoodObsessive-Compulsive Disorder (OCD)is approximately 9–12 years, with familyhistory <strong>of</strong> OCD placing a youngster atriskfor an even younger onset <strong>of</strong> OCDsymptoms (see Baldwin <strong>and</strong> Dadds,2008 for review).l The central symptoms <strong>of</strong> PosttraumaticStress Disorder (PTSD),increased arousal, psychic numbing,<strong>and</strong> re-experiencing the trauma, occurin children as well as adults who arediagnosed with the condition (Last,1993). However, the frequency <strong>and</strong>intensity <strong>of</strong> these symptoms may beimportant indicators <strong>of</strong> impairmentdue to PTSD symptoms for children(Carrion, Weems, Ray, & Reiss, 2002).Specialized Measures <strong>of</strong> AnxietyRating ScalesRevised <strong>Child</strong>ren’s ManifestAnxiety Scale (RCMAS; Reynolds &Richmond, 1985)The Revised <strong>Child</strong>ren’s Manifest AnxietyScale (RCMAS; Reynolds & Richmond)measures the expression <strong>of</strong> anxiety symptomatologywhether or not the constructis conceptualized as being a state or a trait.We review the RCMAS in relative depthbecause it is widely used <strong>and</strong> has manyunique features for a single constructrating scaleScale ContentThe RCMAS includes 37 items distributedamong four scales: PhysiologicalAnxiety (10 items), Worry/Oversensitivity (11 items), Social concerns/Concentration(7 items), <strong>and</strong> Lie(L, 9 items). The content <strong>of</strong> the subscalesappears diverse. Items from the physiologicalanxiety subscale, for example, rangefrom “difficulty making decisions” (notethat the physiological nature <strong>of</strong> this itemis not apparent) to “awakening scared fromsleep” to “having sweaty h<strong>and</strong>s.”The L scale is a rather unique feature<strong>of</strong> a single-construct measure. TheRCMAS L scale measures children’s tendencyto portray themselves in a favorablelight with items like, “I always have goodmanners.” To obtain a high score on thisscale, children would have to deny evergetting angry <strong>and</strong> liking everyone theyknow. This scale is likely to be transparentto many adolescents. Research has shownthat younger children tend to score higheron the RCMAS Lie scale than older youth(Pina, Silverman, Saavedra, & Weems,2001).


chapter 18 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression <strong>and</strong> anxiety429Administration <strong>and</strong> Scoring<strong>Child</strong>ren have simply to respond “yes” or“no” to RCMAS items that are read froma one-page response form that is accompaniedby a template for scoring. Groupadministration is feasible for older children.A high score is indicative <strong>of</strong> higheranxiety.The Total Anxiety score is a T-scoreconversion <strong>of</strong> a raw score. St<strong>and</strong>ard scoreswith a mean <strong>of</strong> 10 <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation<strong>of</strong> 3 are also provided for the subscales. Ast<strong>and</strong>ard score <strong>of</strong> 13 on the L scale shouldlead the clinician to suspect the child’sreport (Reynolds & Richmond, 1985).Norm-referenced scores are provided bygender, age, <strong>and</strong> ethnicity, giving the usera variety <strong>of</strong> interpretive options. Nongender-basednorms are not <strong>of</strong>fered.NormingThe RCMAS was normed using 4,972children age 6 through 19 years. Thesample was collected from 13 states. Age,gender, <strong>and</strong> ethnic in-formation is givenin the manual (Reynolds & Richmond,1985). The representation <strong>of</strong> Hispanicstudents is lacking, <strong>and</strong> no formal measure<strong>of</strong> SES is included. Consequently,while the sample size is large <strong>and</strong> thegeographic representation is better thanfor some other single-trait measures, therepresentativeness <strong>of</strong> the RCMAS sampleis still open to question.ReliabilityInternal consistency reliability <strong>of</strong> the compositescore is good with overall coefficientsin the low .80s. Subscale reliabilitiesare somewhat lower, particularly for thePhysiological Anxiety <strong>and</strong> Social Concerns/Concentration scales. The PhysiologicalAnxiety scale produced a coefficient <strong>of</strong> .67for the total score in the norm sample. TheSocial Concerns/Concentration estimatewas similar at .64. The Worry/Oversensitivitycoefficient was somewhat higherat .76. Clearly, the Worry/Oversensitivitysubscale is the most trustworthy <strong>and</strong> may,in some instances, be more useful for identifyingchildren with anxiety problems perse than the RCMAS total score (see Silverman& Ollendick, 2005).ValidityConsiderable validity evidence is providedin the RCMAS manual. Factor studies <strong>of</strong>the st<strong>and</strong>ardization data do not suggest thatthe RCMAS is dominated by a single factor.A review <strong>of</strong> the factor loadings suggest thatsome <strong>of</strong> the scale placements were based onminimal loadings. The item about havingdifficulty making decisions, for example,had a .26 loading on both the first <strong>and</strong> secondfactors for the norming sample, yet theitem is placed on the Physiological Anxietyfactor, not Worry/Oversensitivity. Theoreticalor logical considerations may havehelped with the item-placement process.The RCMAS demonstrates consistentrelations with other measures <strong>of</strong> anxiety. Acorrelation <strong>of</strong> .65 is reported between theRCMAS Total Anxiety score <strong>and</strong> the StateTrait Anxiety Inventory for <strong>Child</strong>ren TraitAnxiety score.Like the CDI, the ability <strong>of</strong> the RCMASto identify true cases <strong>of</strong> specific anxietydisorders is questionable (Silverman &Rabian, 1999). Similarly, in their review <strong>of</strong>the relevant research, Silverman <strong>and</strong> Ollendick(2005) point out that the RCMAS doesnot consistently distinguish children withanxiety disorders from children with otherforms <strong>of</strong> psychopathology (e.g., depression,Conduct Disorder, ADHD), although itmay distinguish children with anxiety fromchildren without any significant psychologicaldifficulties. Clinicians are, therefore,advised to use diagnostic interviews orother measures to identify disorders per se,with the RCMAS serving as one potentialindicator <strong>of</strong> problems with anxiety.Strengths <strong>and</strong> WeaknessesStrengths <strong>of</strong> the RCMAS include:1. A broad sampling <strong>of</strong> anxiety symptomatology


430 chapter 18 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression <strong>and</strong> anxiety2. Ease <strong>of</strong> administration <strong>and</strong> scoring3. A larger <strong>and</strong> more geographicallydiverse norming sample than is typicalfor single-construct measures4. A reliable Total Anxiety score5. The presence <strong>of</strong> an L scaleWeaknesses <strong>of</strong> the RCMAS include:1. A lack <strong>of</strong> information about relevant ethnic<strong>and</strong> SES representation <strong>of</strong> the normingsample2. Subscales with questionable externalevidence <strong>of</strong> validityAdditional Self-ReportMeasuresIn addition to the RCMAS, there is a vastarray <strong>of</strong> rating scales for child anxiety. Silverman& Ollendick (2005) provide a usefulreview <strong>of</strong> many such measures.Three self-report measures that haveenjoyed wide use <strong>and</strong> extensive researchare the Fear Survey Schedule for <strong>Child</strong>ren-Revised (FSSCR; Ollendick, 1978), theMultidimensional Anxiety Scale for <strong>Child</strong>ren(MASC; March et al., 1997), <strong>and</strong> theState-Trait Anxiety Inventory for <strong>Child</strong>ren(STAIC; Spielberger, 1973). The STAIChas a companion parent report version.Some <strong>of</strong> the assessment domains <strong>of</strong>fered bythese measures are provided in Table 18.2.Two relatively new self-report ratings <strong>of</strong>anxiety focus on aspects <strong>of</strong> the individual’scognitions as they contribute to, or be a sign<strong>of</strong>, marked anxiety. The Anxiety ControlQuestionnaire for <strong>Child</strong>ren (Weems, Silverman,Rapee, <strong>and</strong> Pina, 2003) contains 30items that assess the child’s perceived controlover threatening events or stimuli. The<strong>Child</strong>ren’s Automatic Thoughts (Schniering& Rapee, 2002) assesses whether ornot the respondent has experienced particularnegative thoughts about anxietyprovokingsituations. Initial research onTable 18.2 Domains Assessed by SeveralRating Scales Assessing AnxietyFSSC-R DomainsTotal fear scoreFailure/criticismFear <strong>of</strong> unknownInjury/small animalsFear <strong>of</strong> danger/deathMedical FearsMASC DomainsPhysical symptomsSocial anxietyHarm avoidanceSeparation/panicSTAIC DomainsTotal State AnxietyState-cognitiveState-somaticTotal trait anxietyTrait-cognitiveTrait-somaticthe psychometrics <strong>of</strong> both <strong>of</strong> these scaleshas had promising results (see Silverman &Ollendick, 2005), <strong>and</strong> the unique focus <strong>of</strong>these scales also appears to translate wellto cognitive-behavioral interventions forchildhood anxiety.Interviews <strong>and</strong> Clinician RatingsThe typical way for anxiety to be assessedin children is through interviews (includingsome <strong>of</strong> the diagnostic interviewsreviewed in Chap. 11) or subscales withinbroad-b<strong>and</strong> ratings (usually completed byparents, teachers, or youths themselves).The Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedulefor <strong>Child</strong>ren (Albano &Silverman, 1996)is a unique-structured interview in that itprovides an in-depth assessment <strong>of</strong> symptoms<strong>and</strong> impairment across anxiety disordercategories.


chapter 18 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression <strong>and</strong> anxiety431The Yale-Brown Obsessive CompulsiveScale for <strong>Child</strong>ren (Goodman et al., 1989)is but one example <strong>of</strong> a clinician rating <strong>of</strong>anxiety symptoms. The adult version <strong>of</strong> thisscale is well-known <strong>and</strong> extensively usedin research <strong>and</strong> practice. Such a tool maybe useful if a clinician wants to zero in onspecific areas <strong>of</strong> anxiety symptomatologyin a quick, efficient manner (the YBOCShas ten items) rather than conducting anextensive multi-domain interview.Cognitive-Behavioral<strong>Assessment</strong>Kendall <strong>and</strong> Ronan (1990), among others,emphasize the importance <strong>of</strong> a child’sinformation processing as a mediatingvariable in the development <strong>and</strong> expression<strong>of</strong> anxiety symptoms. Some <strong>of</strong> the cognitivevariables that may mediate anxiety areself-statements, irrational beliefs, currentconcerns, images, problem-solving capacities,expectancies, <strong>and</strong> attributions. Methodsfor assessing these cognitive variablesinclude (Kendall & Ronan, 1990):l Self-monitoringl Recordings <strong>of</strong> spontaneous verbalizationsl Think aloud methodsl Role playingl Imagery assessmentl Thought sampling <strong>and</strong> thought listingtechniquesl Interviewsl Endorsement measuresIn addition, numerous other observationalmethods, including parent-child interactions<strong>and</strong> social interaction scenarios havebeen used in the assessment <strong>of</strong> youth anxiety(see Silverman & Ollendick, 2005).These informal methods <strong>of</strong> assessmentvary in the degree to which they havebeen developed for clinical use. Cognitive-behavioralmethods potentially servethe clinician, even in experimental form,by providing corroborating data for testbasedfindings, <strong>and</strong> by providing insightsthat may be useful for intervention design.Still, the incremental validity obtained byincorporating such procedures into assessmentbatteries for childhood anxiety isuncertain (Silverman & Ollendick, 2005).The preceding discussion in no wayprovides an exhaustive description <strong>of</strong> theavailable methods for assessing anxiety inchildren. Instead, we have attempted tohighlight those measures that are wellknown,widely used, <strong>and</strong>/or unique in theirimplementation or focus. The reader isencouraged to remain up-to-date on theburgeoning body <strong>of</strong> research on evidencebasedassessment <strong>of</strong> anxiety (as well as otherforms <strong>of</strong> psychopathology) to determinewhich approaches possess clinical utility.An <strong>Assessment</strong> Strategyfor AnxietyWe again recommend a five-stage assessmentprocess for the assessment <strong>of</strong> anxiety:screening, classification, comorbidities,alternative causes, <strong>and</strong> treatment considerations(see Table 18.3). Screening foranxiety can be accomplished using bothinformal <strong>and</strong> formal means. A few questionsabout anxiety symptoms may be moreappropriate for many settings than the use<strong>of</strong> self-report or other more formal psychometricdevices. As noted elsewhere in thistext, many omnibus rating scales provide ascreening <strong>of</strong> anxiety, but they do not differentiateamong different types or classes<strong>of</strong> anxiety problems. The most importantscreening principle to remember is to doit, because failure to screen for anxiety mayresult in under-serving children with lessobvious problems.Classification <strong>of</strong> anxiety difficulties iscomplicated by debate regarding the


432 chapter 18 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression <strong>and</strong> anxietyTable 18.3 The <strong>Assessment</strong> Questions Related to <strong>Child</strong>hood Anxiety <strong>and</strong> Implications for<strong>Assessment</strong><strong>Assessment</strong>QuestionScreeningClassification<strong>and</strong> SubtypesComorbiditiesAlternativeCausesTreatmentConsiderationsImplications for <strong>Assessment</strong>Administer time efficient assessment instruments (i.e., rating scales)Determine need for further assessment.Assess for presence <strong>of</strong> symptoms that meet may DSM-IV criteria throughspecific rating scales or interviews. Determine stability <strong>and</strong> duration <strong>of</strong>symptoms.Determine onset.Assess for presence <strong>of</strong> depression, academic difficulties, externalizingproblems, or other anxiety problems.Determine the influence <strong>of</strong> anxiety on school performance, absenteeism,etc.Assess social relationships <strong>and</strong> peer social status.Obtain developmental <strong>and</strong> medical histories.Rule out stressful life events or other psychopathology as primaryexplanation for anxiety symptoms.Rule out medical problems that are associated with anxiety.Assess for presence <strong>of</strong> maladaptive cognitions.Assess parental depression <strong>and</strong> parenting style.Evaluate response to previous interventions (e.g., pharmacotherapy,counseling, etc.).appropriateness <strong>of</strong> existing criteria, includingthe DSM-IV. These issues notwithst<strong>and</strong>ing, itis critical for treatment planning purposes forthe clinician to determine the phenomenology<strong>of</strong> the client’s anxiety <strong>and</strong> the situations/stimuli that tend to elicit or alleviate the anxiety.Silverman <strong>and</strong> Ollendick (2005) note thatsome available measures are better able thanothers for assessing different types <strong>of</strong> anxiety.Anxiety disorders are most likely to becomorbid with other internalizing disorderssuch as other anxiety disorders or depression.Moreover, the nature <strong>of</strong> the symptomatologymay vary over time. A child may, forexample, initially be diagnosed with SAD<strong>and</strong> not have school difficulties. Schoolrefusal <strong>and</strong> subsequent academic problems,however, may appear at a later time.Medical problems should be ruled outas alternative causes because <strong>of</strong> the role thatphysical illness <strong>and</strong> medical procedures canplay in initiating or exacerbating a child’sanxiety symptoms. In fact, one <strong>of</strong> the firsthypotheses to consider when somaticsymptoms are present (e.g., headaches,stomachaches, diarrhea) is the possibilitythat the child is physically ill. Althoughit seems obvious, it should be stated that amedical evaluation should be conductedwhen somatic complaints are evident.Further assessment should be conductedon the factors that may influence treatmentoutcome. Research on this area <strong>of</strong> assessmentis limited, but an approach that incorporatesbehavioral observation, rating scales,<strong>and</strong>/or self-monitoring is recommended(Silverman & Ollendick, 2005). Maladaptivecognitions, family stressors, <strong>and</strong> traumaticevents are but some <strong>of</strong> the variables that mayplay a role in the client’s manifestation <strong>and</strong>maintenance <strong>of</strong> anxiety symptoms <strong>and</strong> thatwill undoubtedly play a role in the progress<strong>of</strong> treatment.A sample case illustrating this assessmentapproach for anxiety is provided inBox 18.3.


chapter 18 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression <strong>and</strong> anxiety433Box 18.3A Sample Case Illustrating the <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> AnxietyThomas is a 9-year-old male who was referredfor an assessment due to academic difficultiessubsequent to being transferred to a newschool. He was referred in May <strong>of</strong> the schoolyear. His grades are reportedly lower in his newschool than in the previous school. He makesfrequent self-deprecating statements <strong>and</strong>reportedly requires much attention <strong>and</strong> supervisionto complete his homework. He tendsto give up easily on academic activities, <strong>of</strong>tenrefusing to complete them. He reportedly hastrouble separating from his mother, <strong>and</strong> he<strong>of</strong>ten seeks attention in the classroom. On thepositive side, he is described as endearing toteachers, creative, <strong>and</strong> artistically talented.Thomas was reportedly born three monthspreterm <strong>and</strong> weighed approximately threepounds at birth. He was treated in neonatalintensive care for two months after delivery.His mother reported that he did not walkuntil 16 months, <strong>and</strong> he did not speak in singlewords until 24 months <strong>of</strong> age.Thomas was reportedly diagnosed at age5 with a motor delay, ADHD, <strong>and</strong> ODD. Hecontinues to receive occupational therapy forhis motor problems. His teachers have reportedlyalways indicated that he responds better ina structured classroom. His grades have usuallybeen above grade level with the exception <strong>of</strong>mathematics. He has not been retained nor hashe been served in special education. Accordingto his mother, Thomas <strong>of</strong>ten complains <strong>of</strong>headaches <strong>and</strong> stomachaches at school.Thomas’s test behavior was also remarkablein that he refused testing initially. Heclung to his mother in the waiting room.He cried <strong>and</strong> requested that his mother joinhim for the testing. During testing (with hismother in the room), he cried, tantrumed,threw objects, refused to answer questions,<strong>and</strong> responded impulsively. Several times, heremarked, “I’m an idiot.” He was rescheduledto complete testing a week later.During the second assessment session,Thomas cooperated fully. He again pleadedwith his mother to join him, but she refused.He did not display any <strong>of</strong> the previous behaviorproblems, <strong>and</strong> he did not make self-deprecatingstatements.Thomas’s intellectual functioning was inthe Low Average range as was his measuredacademic achievement in reading, math, <strong>and</strong>writing. His academic difficulties do not appearto be the result <strong>of</strong> a specific learning disability,although it is reasonable to expect that Thomasmay struggle with some academic tasks, unlesshe is given some assistance in acquiring furtheracademic skills.Based on findings from rating scales <strong>and</strong>historical information, Thomas was diagnosedwith Separation Anxiety Disorder. This diagnosiswas based on the rationale that he displayedthe minimum <strong>of</strong> three symptoms for a minimum<strong>of</strong> four weeks. His symptoms included:1. Excessive distress when separated fromfamily members2. Worry about the well-being <strong>of</strong> familymembers3. Somatic complaints when separatedAn interesting aspect <strong>of</strong> this case is that theprevious diagnoses <strong>of</strong> ADHD <strong>and</strong> ODD werenot confirmed based on detailed interviewinformation <strong>and</strong> rating scales. For example,many <strong>of</strong> Thomas’s oppositional behaviorsseemed better explained by his refusal to separatefrom his mother <strong>and</strong> his difficulty workingon tasks to their completion when he was upset.His teacher reported that Thomas only seemsinattentive <strong>and</strong> distracted when faced with difficulttasks. On new or difficult tasks, Thomasseems to adjust better when given some initialguidance <strong>and</strong> feedback on his performance.Treatment recommendations were madethat centered around ways for Thomas tocope with his anxiety, particularly regardinggoing to school <strong>and</strong> ways for his mother t<strong>of</strong>acilitate his progress <strong>and</strong> not maintain hisdesire to avoid separation through tantrumingor refusal to perform tasks. The results <strong>of</strong>the evaluation were also shared with schoolpersonnel, <strong>and</strong> a behavioral intervention planfor Thomas was developed at school.


434 chapter 18 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression <strong>and</strong> anxietyConclusionsThe assessment <strong>of</strong> child anxiety <strong>and</strong> depressionrequires a well-trained clinician. Theless obvious nature <strong>of</strong> their symptomatology<strong>and</strong> the range <strong>of</strong> disorders related toanxiety <strong>and</strong> depression make identification<strong>of</strong> these problems a pr<strong>of</strong>essional challenge.The nature <strong>of</strong> the symptomatology alsopoints to the necessity <strong>of</strong> gathering selfreportinformation – an additional challengein the assessment <strong>of</strong> young children.Many child anxiety <strong>and</strong> depression measurescombine the two sets <strong>of</strong> symptoms.As such, scales are not likely to differentiatebetween the two syndromes, necessitatingefforts at scale refinement (Silverman& Rabian, 1999).Although our underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> childdepression <strong>and</strong> anxiety has increasedexponentially <strong>of</strong> late, our underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong>these problems still lags behind the availableknowledge base for adults. Cliniciansare advised to make a concerted effort toremain abreast <strong>of</strong> changes in the emergingresearch base. We recommend thediscussion <strong>of</strong> evidence-based assessment<strong>of</strong> childhood depression by Klein et al.(2005) <strong>and</strong> the article on evidence-basedassessment <strong>of</strong> childhood by Silverman <strong>and</strong>Ollendick (2005) as useful resources forhighlighting important issues in this area<strong>of</strong> assessment.Chapter Summary1. Depression <strong>and</strong> anxiety difficulties canbe subsumed under the more global set<strong>of</strong> adjustment difficulties <strong>of</strong> childhoodcommonly referred to as internalizingdifficulties/disorders.2. Internalizing disorders are among themost difficult to diagnose because <strong>of</strong> thenature <strong>of</strong> the symptomatology.3. In order to meet the diagnostic criteriafor Major Depressive Disorder, at leastfive <strong>of</strong> nine symptoms must be presentfor at least a two-week period, <strong>and</strong> one <strong>of</strong>the symptoms must be either depressedmood or loss <strong>of</strong> interest or pleasure(American Psychiatric Association,2000). The nine symptoms include: (1)depressed or irritable mood, (2) loss <strong>of</strong>interest in daily activities, (3) significantweight loss or failure to make expectedweight gains, (4) frequent insomnia orhypersomnia, (5) motor agitation orretardation, (6) frequent fatigue, (7)feelings <strong>of</strong> worthlessness or guilt, (8)impaired concentration, (9) suicidality.However, commonly used assessmenttools vary in the degree to which theirdepression scales match these criteria(See Box 18.3.)4. Approximately 2–3% <strong>of</strong> preadolescentchildren may be suffering from depressionat any point in time, with ratesincreasing significantly in adolescence.5. Specialized measures that may be usefulfor assessing childhood depression includethe <strong>Child</strong>ren’s Depression Inventory, theReynolds <strong>Adolescent</strong> Depression Scale-2, the Reynolds <strong>Child</strong> Depression Scale,<strong>and</strong> the Peer Nomination Inventory forDepression, among many others.6. A five-step method for assessing fordepression <strong>and</strong> anxiety involves (1)screening, (2) classification, (3) comorbidities,(4) alternative causes, <strong>and</strong> (5)past treatment.7. Each <strong>of</strong> the anxiety disorders describedin the DSM-IV has a different set <strong>of</strong>core symptoms or problems. Thus, differentialdiagnosis is <strong>of</strong>ten quite importantfor optimal treatment planning.8. Some findings regarding anxiety disordersinclude: many anxiety problemscan be conceptualized as exaggerations<strong>of</strong> normal developmental processes(e.g., separation anxiety for young children);research is mixed on the degree


chapter 18 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Depression <strong>and</strong> anxiety435<strong>of</strong> parent-child agreement for anxietysymptoms on structured interviews;the phenomenology <strong>of</strong> childhood anxietydisorders is very similar to that <strong>of</strong>adults; anxiety disorders <strong>of</strong>ten occur inthe absence <strong>of</strong> depression.9. Some relatively popular self-reportmeasures <strong>of</strong> anxiety symptomatologyin children include the Revised <strong>Child</strong>ren’sManifest Anxiety Scale, the FearSurvey Schedule for <strong>Child</strong>ren-Revised,the Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for<strong>Child</strong>ren, <strong>and</strong> the State-Trait AnxietyInventory for <strong>Child</strong>ren.10. Cognitive-behavioral assessment,behavioral observations, <strong>and</strong> clinicianratings have also been espoused asimportant assessment tools for anxietythat have implications for interventionplanning <strong>and</strong> that enable progressmonitoring. Structured diagnosticinterviews are <strong>of</strong>ten useful for differentialdiagnosis <strong>and</strong> treatment planningas well.


C h a p t e r 1 9<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Autism SpectrumDisordersChapter Questionsl How are autism-spectrum disordersdefined by the DSM-IV?l How is autism differentiated from mentalretardation?l What types <strong>of</strong> scales or strategies aremost useful for the assessment <strong>of</strong> childrenwith these syndromes?l What is the recommended approach forthe assessment <strong>of</strong> autism spectrum disorders?Definitional IssuesAutism is believed to affect 1 in 500 children(Bertr<strong>and</strong> et al., 2001), yet it is <strong>of</strong>tennot recognized until after the child is 3years <strong>of</strong> age (Filipek et al., 2000). Hence,early screening is important for devisingthe most appropriate intervention strategy.In addition, because <strong>of</strong> the difficulty in recognizingautism or related problems, newassessment methods for infants <strong>and</strong> toddlersare needed. However, a discussion <strong>of</strong>the development <strong>of</strong> such tools is beyondthe scope <strong>of</strong> this chapter. Importantly, thearray <strong>of</strong> assessment strategies available forautism spectrum assessment referrals hasgreatly improved <strong>and</strong> more easily translateto treatment recommendations. This chapterwill give an overview <strong>of</strong> the measures<strong>and</strong> noteworthy issues in the assessment <strong>of</strong>childhood autism.DSM-IV-TR CriteriaIn the DSM-IV-TR, Autistic Disorder ispart <strong>of</strong> a class <strong>of</strong> problems known as PervasiveDevelop-mental Disorders (PDD;American Psychiatric Association, 2000).437P.J. Frick et al., <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Behavior,DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-0641-0_19, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010


438 chapter 19 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> autism spectrumHowever, the term “Autism SpectrumDisorders” (ASD) is now generally usedin lieu <strong>of</strong> PDD (see Ozon<strong>of</strong>f, Goodlin-Jones, & Solomon, 2005). Other disordersthat are included under the rubric <strong>of</strong> ASDinclude <strong>Child</strong>hood Disintegrative Disorder,Asperger’s Disorder (<strong>of</strong>ten referred toas “Asperger’s Syndrome”), Rett’s Disorder,<strong>and</strong> PDD Not Otherwise Specified (AmericanPsychiatric Association, 2000).The prototypical ASD is, <strong>of</strong> course,Autistic Disorder, a condition that has longbeen <strong>of</strong> interest to psychologists. AutisticDisorder will be the primary focus <strong>of</strong> thischapter, with some discussion <strong>of</strong> other ASDissues, particularly regarding the assessment<strong>of</strong> Asperger’s Disorder. The DSM-IV-TR describes Autistic Disorder as “thepresence <strong>of</strong> markedly abnormal or impaireddevelopment in social interaction <strong>and</strong> communications<strong>and</strong> a markedly restricted repertoire<strong>of</strong> activity <strong>and</strong> interests.”Autistic Disorder is also known by otherterms such as autism or early infantile autism.Autistic Disorder is known to be frequentlycomorbid with mental retardation (AmericanPsychiatric Association, 2000). Withco-morbidity estimates ranging as high as75%, the differential diagnosis <strong>of</strong> autism<strong>and</strong> mental retardation can be challenging.The DSM-IV emphasizes three classes<strong>of</strong> symptoms that are central to the disorder:social interaction problems, communicationproblems, <strong>and</strong> repetitive<strong>and</strong> stereotyped behaviors. The onset<strong>of</strong> these symptoms must occur by age 3.The DSM criteria require that a total <strong>of</strong>six symptoms with a minimum <strong>of</strong> one fromeach <strong>of</strong> the three classes <strong>of</strong> problems bepresent in order to make a diagnosis. Furthermore,at least two <strong>of</strong> the symptomsfrom the social interaction class <strong>of</strong> problemsmust be present.The social interaction symptoms include:1. Failure to engage in appropriate nonverbalbehaviors such as appropriate eyecontact, facial expressions, body postures,<strong>and</strong> social gestures2. Poorly developed peer relationships3. Failure to share pleasurable interests,activities, or achievements with others4. Lack <strong>of</strong> social reciprocityCommunication symptoms include:1. Delayed development <strong>of</strong> spoken language2. When speech is present, inability tobegin or maintain a conversation3. Stereotyped or repetitive use <strong>of</strong> language4. Lack <strong>of</strong> imaginative playProblems with repetitive or stereotypedplay include:1. Preoccupation with one or more behaviorpatterns2. Preoccupation with nonfunctional routinesor rituals3. Inappropriate motor movements4. Preoccupation with specific objectsCharacteristics <strong>of</strong> AutismAutism Spectrum Disorders are <strong>of</strong>tendescribed as etiologically diverse disorders.Evidence points to genetic (e.g.,Bailey et al., 1995) <strong>and</strong> neurological (e.g.,Courchesne et al., 2001) underpinnings <strong>of</strong>ASDs; however, specific etiological agentshave not been identified. Much remains tobe learned about risk factors that may beetiologically involved in autism. The lack<strong>of</strong> scientific clarity implies that cliniciansshould not place undue importance on etiologicalagents when conceptualizing cases<strong>of</strong> autism or when providing feedback toothers. Instead, the focus <strong>of</strong> assessmentshould be on symptomatology, developmentalhistory (e.g., language development),strengths, <strong>and</strong> impairments.Differentiation <strong>of</strong> autism from otherASDs <strong>and</strong> from Mental Retardation can be


chapter 19 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> autism spectrum439quite difficult. One study found that childrenwith language disorder could be more easilydifferentiated from children with autism, butautism <strong>and</strong> PDD-NOS were more difficultto differentiate (Mayes, Volkmar, Hooks, &Cicchetti, 1993). Similarly, differentiation <strong>of</strong>high functioning children with autism (i.e.,those with average or higher intelligence)from children with Asperger’s Syndromecan be quite difficult in that such youth withautism may not appear to have communicationdifficulties (Howlin, 2003).Some <strong>of</strong> the assessment tools discussedin this chapter may provide useful informationin making distinctions among ASDs aswell as related problems. However, thereremains a relative lack <strong>of</strong> research on thisissue <strong>and</strong> no measure to date has clearlyshown the ability to differentiate withinASDs in meaningful ways (Ozon<strong>of</strong>f et al.,2005). Therefore, it is incumbent on theclinician to use a variety <strong>of</strong> methods to gaina comprehensive picture <strong>of</strong> the client’s difficulties<strong>and</strong> history such that the most appropriateinterventions can then be sought.concern, particularly for high-functioningindividuals with autism, is in the internalizingproblem domain (e.g., Kim, Szatmari,Bryson, Streiner, & Wilson, 2000). However,the validity <strong>of</strong> commonly used measures<strong>of</strong> depression <strong>and</strong> anxiety – such asthose discussed in the preceding chapter– for individuals with ASDs is unknown(Ozon<strong>of</strong>f et al., 2005).Regular, comprehensive assessmentacross domains <strong>of</strong> functioning will aid intracking treatment gains as well as otherimportant areas <strong>of</strong> difficulty apart from theprimary problems associated with ASDs.For example, many children with ASDsmay also present with significant externalizingbehavioral problems that occur whenexpectations are not met, routines change,or due to frustration from communicationdifficulties. These behaviors may themselvesbe a primary focus <strong>of</strong> treatment. Inaddition, sleep difficulties that may accompanyASDs may warrant specific interventions(Ozon<strong>of</strong>f et al., 2005).ComorbidityThe most frequent comorbid problemfor autism is mental retardation, with themajority <strong>of</strong> children with autism also meetingcriteria for mental retardation (AmericanPsychiatric Association, 2000). On theother h<strong>and</strong>, the majority <strong>of</strong> individuals withmental retardation do not meet criteria forautism or another ASD. Needless to say,the inclusion <strong>of</strong> cognitive functioning inassessments <strong>of</strong> ASDs is critical for caseconceptualization <strong>and</strong> treatment planning.The same, then, can be said for the inclusion<strong>of</strong> a measure <strong>of</strong> adaptive functioningin the assessment battery.As noted by Ozon<strong>of</strong>f et al. (2005), newdifficulties may emerge over the course <strong>of</strong>development for youth with ASDs suchthat initial diagnosis as well as treatmentplanning <strong>and</strong> progress monitoring arecomplicated. A common comorbid area <strong>of</strong>Specialized Measures <strong>of</strong>AutismInfant Behavioral SummarizedEvaluation (IBSE)The IBSE (Adrien, Barthelemy, Perrot, &Roux, 1992) is a unique rating scale in thatit is appropriate for infants <strong>and</strong> preschoolers.One study evaluated some psychometricproperties <strong>of</strong> the scale for a sample <strong>of</strong>39 children with autism, 33 with MentalRetardation, <strong>and</strong> 17 with other h<strong>and</strong>icaps(Adrien et al., 1992). Good inter-raterreliability was found. A factor analysis <strong>of</strong>31 items found two factors, with the firstfactor accounting for most <strong>of</strong> the items.Adrien et al. also concluded that first factorscores were capable <strong>of</strong> differentiatingthe group <strong>of</strong> children with autism groupfrom the other two groups. The IBSE was


440 chapter 19 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> autism spectrumalso able to differentiate among preschoolerswith autism <strong>and</strong> other children withdevelopmental delays by age 26 months(Desombre et al., 2006).Autism DiagnosticInterview-Revised (ADI-R)The ADI-R is a semi-structured diagnosticinterview designed to be used with a child’sprimary caregiver <strong>and</strong> can be used for anyonewith a mental age <strong>of</strong> 2 years or higher(Lord, Rutter, & Le Couteur, 1994). TheADI-R assesses behaviors relevant to thedifferential diagnosis <strong>of</strong> pervasive developmentaldisorders in individuals from 18months to adulthood. The ADI focuses onthree domains <strong>of</strong> behavior that are largelyconsistent with the DSM-IV criteria: reciprocalsocial interaction; communication,<strong>and</strong> repetitive, restricted, or stereotypedbehavior. Scores on the ADI-R havebeen found to differentiate children withautism from children with other forms <strong>of</strong>delay (Mildenberger, Sitter, Noterdaeme,& Amorosa, 2001). Another study againfound the ADI-R to reliably differentiatebetween samples <strong>of</strong> children withautism <strong>and</strong> children with mental retardation(Lord, Storoschuk, Rutter, & Pickles,1993). This investigation, however, identifieddifficulties when trying to differentiatethese samples among children with mentalages <strong>of</strong> less than 18 months, reflecting thecontinuing dilemma <strong>of</strong> diagnostic accuracywith low-functioning children.One study evaluated inter-rater reliability<strong>of</strong> the ADI-R across seven examinersfor one case (Cicchetti, Lord, Koenig,Klin, & Volkmar, 2008) <strong>and</strong> found perfectagreement for 74% <strong>of</strong> the items <strong>and</strong> “poor”agreement on less than 10% <strong>of</strong> the items.A large scale study <strong>of</strong> the ADI-R foundsupport for a two-factor model (i.e., socialcommunication <strong>and</strong> stereotyped behaviors)as well as a three-factor model (i.e., socialcommunication, peer relationships/play,<strong>and</strong> stereotyped behaviors; Frazier, Youngstrom,Kubu, Sinclair, & Rezai, 2008).The strength <strong>of</strong> the ADI-R is its strongresearch base, including the validity evidence<strong>of</strong> the current version <strong>and</strong> its predecessorwhich goes back to two decades (LeCouteur et al., 1989). However, the ADI-Ris quite lengthy, taking anywhere from 90min to 3 h to administer <strong>and</strong> score (Ozon<strong>of</strong>fet al., 2005). Therefore, it may be lesspractical than other tools in many clinicalsettings.Parent Interview for Autism(PIA)The PIA (see Stone, Coonrod, Pozdol, &Turner, 2003) is a newer alternative to theADI-R. It is unique in its focus on trackingchanges in symptoms over time, withparents rating the frequency <strong>of</strong> symptomson a five-point scale. Research has showngood internal consistency <strong>and</strong> differentialvalidity for the PIA. In addition, changesin PIA ratings, particularly in the areas<strong>of</strong> social <strong>and</strong> communicative functioning,were related to changes reflected in otherratings <strong>of</strong> the child symptomatology (Stoneet al., 2003). While generally based on thethree core dimensions <strong>of</strong> autism, the PIAhas items that were developed <strong>and</strong> rationally/theoreticallygrouped into the followingscales (Stone & Hogan, 1993):l Social Relating Affective ResponsesMotor Imitationl Peer Interactions Object Playl Imaginative Play Language Underst<strong>and</strong>ingl Nonverbal Communicationl Motoric Behaviorsl Sensory Responses Need for SamenessWhile the PIA has shown some promise,research is limited, <strong>and</strong> the validity <strong>of</strong> the12 subscales is not documented.


chapter 19 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> autism spectrum441<strong>Child</strong>hood Autism Rating Scale(CARS)The CARS (Schopler, Reichler, & Renner,1988) is a rating form that is typicallycompleted by an observer <strong>of</strong> the behavior<strong>of</strong> a child who has reached a developmentalage <strong>of</strong> approximately 2 years or higher.In some cases, an observation period <strong>of</strong>only about 30 min is used (Sevin, Matson,Coe, & Fee, 1991). Items are rated on a7-point continuum for the 15 subscales.The CARS is composed <strong>of</strong> nine portions.It includes items such as a puzzle-like task<strong>and</strong> a set <strong>of</strong> toy objects designed to assessmake-believe play activity.The CARS produces a moderately highcorrelation <strong>of</strong>.67 with the ABC (Eaves &Milner, 1993). Although this correlation issignificant, the two instruments did classifychildren at differing rates. The CARS correctlyidentified 98% <strong>of</strong> the children withautism, whereas the ABC did so for 88% <strong>of</strong>the sample (Eaves & Milner, 1993).In an investigation by Sevin et al.(1991), 24 children were assessed withthe CARS. The inter-rater reliability <strong>of</strong>the CARS for these subjects was foundto be highly variable. Coefficients wereas low as 0.10 (Activity Level) <strong>and</strong> 0.14(Intellectual Response), <strong>and</strong> as high as0.85 (Relating to People). The inter-raterreliability coefficient for the total scorewas 0.68.In spite <strong>of</strong> such low to modest reliabilitycoefficients, the CARS results showedfairly good concordance with clinici<strong>and</strong>iagnoses <strong>of</strong> autism (Sevin et al., 1991).The CARS classified 19 <strong>of</strong> the 24 subjectswith autism correctly. Only two <strong>of</strong>the subjects were incorrectly classifiedwith autism, although this study provideda weak test <strong>of</strong> differential validity because<strong>of</strong> the small sample <strong>of</strong> subjects withoutautism. Subsequent research has shownthat the total score on the CARS correlateshighly with the ADI-R but that italso may overidentify autism among childrenwith mental retardation (Saemundsen,Magnusson, Smari, & Sigurdardottir,2003).The CARS is a well-known, widely usedassessment tool for autism; however, Ozon<strong>of</strong>fet al. (2005) caution that the content isbased on conceptualizations <strong>of</strong> autism datingprior to DSM-IV.Autism Diagnostic ObservationSchedule (ADOS)The ADOS (Lord, Rutter, DiLavore, &Risi, 2002) is designed to assess symptoms<strong>of</strong> ASDs in a wide rang <strong>of</strong> individuals basedon age <strong>and</strong> language skills. The ADOSconsists <strong>of</strong> four modules, with the moduleselected based on the child’s current expressivelanguage <strong>and</strong> developmental level (i.e.,Module 1 for no speech; Module 2 forphrase speech; Module 3 for verbally fluentchildren <strong>and</strong> young adolescents; Module 4for verbally fluent adolescents <strong>and</strong> adults;Lord et al., 2002). Module 2 is describedin Box 19.1.The ADOS emphasizes the assessment<strong>of</strong> reciprocal social interaction <strong>and</strong>communication rather than restricted orrepetitive behavior, <strong>and</strong> it is structured inthat the environment for the observationis structured in a st<strong>and</strong>ardized way, <strong>and</strong>the examiner presents predefined tasks tothe child in this structured setting. In thisway, the examiner serves as both the stimulusfor interaction in try to elicit particularresponses from the subject <strong>and</strong> as therecorder <strong>of</strong> behavior.A study on the development <strong>of</strong> the currentversion <strong>of</strong> the ADOS revealed stronginterrater reliability, internal consistency,<strong>and</strong> (Lord et al., 2000). The differentialvalidity <strong>of</strong> the ADOS for distinguishingchildren with autism vs. other delays wasalso good. Another study found substantialagreement between the ADOS <strong>and</strong>ADI-R in the classification <strong>of</strong> children ashaving autism or not in children younger


442 chapter 19 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> autism spectrumBox 19.1A Closer View <strong>of</strong> the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS)Together with the ADI-R, the ADOS has beenreferred to as the current “gold st<strong>and</strong>ard” forthe diagnosis <strong>of</strong> ASDs (Ozon<strong>of</strong>f et al., 2005,p. 524). The ADI-R, <strong>of</strong> course, allows the clinicianto gain a detailed history <strong>of</strong> the child’sdevelopment <strong>and</strong> symptomatology in a semistructuredinterview format. This approachis not particularly unique among assessmentpractices. However, the structured observationformat <strong>of</strong> the ADOS is unique to childassessment in that there are no well-establishedanalogous procedures for assessment <strong>of</strong>internalizing or externalizing problems.To further illustrate the content <strong>of</strong> the ADOS,Module 2, designed for children who speak inphrases, is described below. Module 2 consists <strong>of</strong>14 tasks as follows (see Lord et al., 2002):1. Construction task: Designed to assess howthe child communicates a need for moreobjects to complete the task. For example,some children with ASDs may grab theexaminer’s h<strong>and</strong> to reach for more objectsrather than making a verbal request2. Response to name: Evaluates how readily thechild responds to his/her name as said bythe examiner or a parent or other stimulidirected to the child (e.g., phrases, touching).<strong>Child</strong>ren with ASDs may require moredirect stimuli or prompting to response3. Make-believe play: Assesses whether child’s playincludes imaginative use <strong>of</strong> objects beyondtheir usual purpose <strong>and</strong> the extent to whichdolls are used to depict social interaction4. Joint interactive play: Assesses the extent towhich the child initiates interaction with theexaminer during joint play above <strong>and</strong> beyondresponding to the examiner’s statements orrequests5. Conversation: Evaluates extent to which thechild responds to the examiner’s statementsor questions in a way that leads to furtherback-<strong>and</strong>-forth conversation6. Response to joint attention: The examinershifts his/her gaze <strong>and</strong> observes the extentto which the child follows that shift (withor without pointing). <strong>Child</strong>ren with ASDsmay have difficulty engaging injoint attention in this manner, particularlywithout other prompts such as pointing7. Demonstration task: Designed to assesshow well the child demonstrates commonactivities (e.g., brushing teeth, gettingdressed) without the use <strong>of</strong> objects.8. Description <strong>of</strong> a picture: For this task, theexaminer attempts to elicit spontaneouslanguage from the child <strong>and</strong> to observewhat kinds <strong>of</strong> stimuli (from pictures) are<strong>of</strong> interest to the child9. Telling a story from a book: In addition toassessing the child’s spontaneous language<strong>and</strong> interest in a story, this task, inrequiring the child to tell a story from apicture book, evaluates whether the childcan provide continuity in the story bysequencing events in the story in a sensiblemanner10. Free play: This task is designed to assesswhether or not the child seeks interactionor involvement from the examiner duringfree play. In addition, the extent to which thechild maintains attention to one task for anappropriate interval is observed. Repetitivebehaviors – a symptom <strong>of</strong> ASDs – may also beevident during this task (Lord et al., 2002)11. Birthday party: This task evaluates thechild’s ability to participate in a common-scriptedevent (i.e., birthday partfor a doll). <strong>Child</strong>ren with ASDs may notengage in the make-believe <strong>of</strong> imaginingthe doll as a child or may have troubleparticipating, including with prompting12. Snack: Assesses how readily the childcommunicates a preference from a choice<strong>of</strong> snacks. Other social interaction skills(e.g., gaze, facial expression) are alsoobserved during this task.13. Anticipation <strong>of</strong> a routine with objects: Thistask is designed to assess how well a childanticipates a routine (e.g., the response <strong>of</strong>a balloon when it is deflated or let go) <strong>and</strong>whether or not the child participates inthe routine(Continues)


chapter 19 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> autism spectrum443Box 19.1 (Continued)14. Bubble play: This task allows the examinerto observe the child’s enjoyment <strong>of</strong> aplay activity, social interaction skills, <strong>and</strong>motor skills in another play contextOn an achievement test, Corin’s performanceon all areas <strong>of</strong> achievementwere in the average range, indicating thather academic achievement is consistentwith what would be expected for her age.Several measures were given to assessCorin’s behavioral <strong>and</strong> emotional functioning.Rating scales were completedby Mrs. Jacobs, two <strong>of</strong> Corin’s teachers,<strong>and</strong> Corin. Corin’s mother <strong>and</strong> a teachercompleted the Achenbach <strong>Child</strong> BehaviorChecklist (CBCL) <strong>and</strong> Teacher RatingForm (TRF), respectively. This teacherrated Corin as having significant attentionproblems, delinquent behaviors, <strong>and</strong>somatic complaints. Mrs. Jacobs did notindicate any significant problems on theCBCL. Despite the reports <strong>of</strong> attentionproblems, Corin’s teachers stated that theydo not believe that Corin has ADHD.Corin completed several self-reportmeasures that were used to determine herself-perceptions. She was given the Piers-Harris Self-Concept Scale (PHCSCS),<strong>and</strong> the YSR. On the PHCSCS, Corinrated herself high on anxiety, depression,somatization, <strong>and</strong> (external) locus <strong>of</strong> control.On the YSR, Corin’s responses weresimilar to the other self-report measures.Corin also endorsed items that indicateproblems with impulse control <strong>and</strong> compliance.On the self-report measures, Corinreported some positive qualities for herself.She described herself as being goodlooking <strong>and</strong> has having good peer relationships.She reported that all <strong>of</strong> her friendslike her because she is funny. Corin alsoindicated that her peer relationships <strong>and</strong>social life at school are the areas in her lifethat are not currently stressful for her.As a follow-up measure, Corin wasgiven a structured psychiatric interviewon which Corin endorsed all <strong>of</strong> the itemsin the area <strong>of</strong> major depression with theexception <strong>of</strong> suicidal ideation. Corindescribed herself as being anxious, <strong>and</strong>she told the examiner that she <strong>of</strong>ten hasstomachaches <strong>and</strong> dizzy spells that sheassociates with stress. Also, accordingthe structured interview, Corin meetsthe DSM criteria for alcohol abuse. Shestated that she frequently drinks largequantities <strong>of</strong> alcohol, <strong>and</strong> occasionally,she has blackouts. Corin also admittedto the examiner that she smokes marijuanaquite a bit.From the information gathered a diagnosis<strong>of</strong> ADHD does not seem appropriate.Corin’s acting-out behaviors are more thanlikely caused by the stress <strong>and</strong> depressionthat she currently feels. Current diagnosticimpressions are major depression, singleepisode, moderate, <strong>and</strong> alcohol abuse.Recommendations for Corin include:1. Consultation with a psychiatrist to helpdetermine the most appropriate treatmentstrategies for Corin’s depression.2. It is also recommended that Corin continuereceiving counseling services.3. Corin is referred to an addictionscounselor who works with adolescentswho are abusing alcohol.4. Corin may benefit from group counselingfor her alcohol abuse. The bestgroup would be one made up <strong>of</strong> otherteenagers.5. The Jacobs family should seek familycounseling to improve the home situation,particularly in regards to communication<strong>and</strong> parenting monitoring.Mrs. Jacobs may also benefit from(Continues)


444 chapter 19 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> autism spectrumBox 19.1 (Continued)individual counseling to help alleviatesome <strong>of</strong> the stress in her life.6. Corin’s teachers reportedly enjoy havingCorin in their classroom <strong>and</strong> seemeager to do what they can to help her.The school psychologist can devisebehavior modification plans to beimplemented by Corin’s teachers toincrease Corin’s appropriate classroombehaviors. Corin should take part inthe development <strong>and</strong> implementation<strong>of</strong> the plans.7. Communication between the school<strong>and</strong> home is necessary. Weekly reports<strong>of</strong> Corin’s behaviors should be provided.Mrs. Jacobs is encouraged to work withCorin’s teachers to devise solutions toany problems that may occur.8. After six months to a year <strong>of</strong> intervention,Corin needs to be reevaluated tosee if there are still issues <strong>of</strong> attentionproblems in the classroom.9. Corin would benefit from tutoring inalgebra.than age 8. However, the agreement waslower for older individuals (de Bildt et al.,2004). Still, these authors suggested thatan assessment battery that includes boththe ADOS <strong>and</strong> ADI-R is suitable, <strong>and</strong> perhapsthe best available, for the assessment<strong>of</strong> autism.Gilliam Autism Rating Scale –2nd Edition (GARS-2)The GARS-2 (Gilliam, 2008) is a normreferencedrating scale that can be completedin approximately 5–10 min byparents. The GARS-2 includes 42 items,three scales (i.e., Stereotyped Behaviors,Communication, <strong>and</strong> Social Interaction)<strong>and</strong> a total score (Autism Index).norm-referenced scores are available for theGARS-2, <strong>and</strong> initial research indicates goodinternal consistency <strong>and</strong> differential validity(Gilliam, 2008).The previous version <strong>of</strong> the GARS (Gilliam,1995) has been the subject <strong>of</strong> muchmore research. A consistent finding fromthis research is that many youth otherwiseindicated as having autism scored loweron the GARS than on other measures <strong>of</strong>autism symptoms (Mazefsky & Oswald,2006; Sikora, Hall, Hartley, Gerrard-Morris, & Cagle, 2008; South et al., 2002).This set <strong>of</strong> findings may indicate some seriousdrawbacks <strong>of</strong> the GARS, although it isunclear whether or not this issue applies tothe current version <strong>of</strong> the GARS. Becausethe GARS-2 is geared toward screeningfor symptoms <strong>of</strong> autism, the clinician maywant to use a low threshold <strong>of</strong> reporting <strong>of</strong>symptoms on the GARS-2 to initiate furtherfollow-up.Functional AnalysisIn the context <strong>of</strong> the assessment <strong>of</strong> ASDs,functional analysis helps the clinici<strong>and</strong>etermine “what the child is trying to communicatethrough the behavior” (Ozon<strong>of</strong>fet al., 2005, p. 534), which takes on addedimportance in light <strong>of</strong> the behavioralproblems <strong>and</strong> communication difficultiesthat present with ASDs. It is difficultto imagine a comprehensive assessment<strong>of</strong> ASDs that does not include some form<strong>of</strong> behavioral observation <strong>and</strong> functionalanalysis as a means to determine targets<strong>of</strong> intervention. Direct observation, coupledwith functional analysis, allows theclinician to also confirm or disconfirmthe notions <strong>of</strong> parents, teachers, or othercaretakers as to the communicative intent


chapter 19 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> autism spectrum445<strong>of</strong> the child’s behavior <strong>and</strong> on appropriateways to reduce inappropriate communicativebehaviors (e.g., using another’s h<strong>and</strong>to obtain a desired object; biting oneselffrom frustration). Functional analysis,then, is conducted squarely with behavioralinterventions in mind. It is not a toolto differentially diagnose ASDs from otherproblems.An <strong>Assessment</strong> Strategy forAutismASDs, including autism specifically, arerecognized as having heterogeneous etiologies<strong>and</strong> highly variable symptomatologyamong those affected (Campbell et al.,1991). Such within-syndrome variabilitysuggests that children who are eithersuspected <strong>of</strong> or diagnosed with autism orother ASDs should have access to multidisciplinaryassessment procedures. Theseverity <strong>of</strong> autistic symptoms, as demonstratedby the overlap with mental retardation,further emphasizes the need fora range <strong>of</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>essionals to be involved inassessing the affected child. Such interdisciplinarywork, however, can be challengingif there are no adequate structuresfor encouraging interdisciplinary practice.Consequently, the psychologist may haveto make a concerted effort to go beyondthe typical practice regimen <strong>and</strong> communicatemore frequently with colleagueswho practice related specialties.Marcus, Lansing, <strong>and</strong> Schopler (1993)described one <strong>of</strong> the better methodologiesfor fostering multidisciplinary assessment<strong>of</strong> autism. They described the State<strong>of</strong> North Carolina TEACCH evaluationsystem as follows:This process includes a developmental/psychoeducationalassessment <strong>of</strong> the child, psychologicalassessment, detailed parent interviewingincluding assessment <strong>of</strong> adaptivefunctioning, <strong>and</strong> a medical screening. Thisintegrated <strong>and</strong> multiperspective approachgenerates comprehensive data on diagnosis,intellectual <strong>and</strong> adaptive functioning, motor,language, <strong>and</strong> social functioning, behaviorproblems, medical factors, family functioning<strong>and</strong> school or community factors. The data aresufficiently detailed <strong>and</strong> objective to be organizedinto dimensions or axes that both overlapwith <strong>and</strong> extend beyond the DSM-III-R system(p. 350).In situations in which interdisciplinaryassessment <strong>and</strong> intervention teams are notas readily available, it is still incumbenton the clinician to utilize methods thatassess ASDs comprehensively <strong>and</strong> specifically,rather than rely on subjective clinicalopinion or measures (e.g., IQ tests) thatdo not differentially diagnose ASDs fromother developmental delays. Having saidthat, the incremental validity <strong>and</strong> clinicalutility <strong>of</strong> specific ASD measures arelargely unknown (Ozon<strong>of</strong>f et al., 2005). Aswith other areas <strong>of</strong> child assessment, moreresearch is needed as to the most sound,parsimonious battery that would addressreferrals for ASDs <strong>and</strong> lend itself to usefulrecommendations.Nevertheless, the severity <strong>and</strong> frankness<strong>of</strong> the symptomatology <strong>of</strong> ASDsrequire us to emphasize different aspects<strong>of</strong> our five-stage paradigm: screening,classification, comorbidities, alternativecauses, <strong>and</strong> treatment considerations (seeTable 19.1).The screening process for ASDs isaimed primarily at the population <strong>of</strong>children with developmental delays asopposed to the general population. Weadvise that clinicians consider the possibility<strong>of</strong> ASDs in all samples <strong>of</strong> youngchildren with developmental disabilities.Because the nature <strong>of</strong> ASDs usually doesnot allow the child to serve as a source<strong>of</strong> screening information, parent <strong>and</strong>teacher ratings are likely to be the mostfruitful. The time-efficiency <strong>of</strong> such ratings


446 chapter 19 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> autism spectrumTable 19.1 <strong>Assessment</strong> Questions Related to <strong>Child</strong>hood Autism Spectrum Disorders <strong>and</strong>Implications for <strong>Assessment</strong>CharacteristicScreeningClassificationComorbiditiesAlternative causesTreatmentconsiderationsImplications for <strong>Assessment</strong>Administer screening measures; target young children with signs <strong>of</strong> developmentaldelayDetermine need for further assessmentAssess for presence <strong>of</strong> symptoms that meet DSM-IV criteriaDetermine stability <strong>and</strong> duration <strong>of</strong> symptomsDetermine age <strong>of</strong> onsetAssess for presence <strong>of</strong> mental retardationDetermine the influence <strong>of</strong> autism on school performance, adaptive behavior,development, etc.Assess social relationships <strong>and</strong> peer social statusObtain developmental <strong>and</strong> medical historiesDifferential diagnosis/rule out <strong>of</strong> Rhett’s Disorder, Asperger’s Syndrome,language disorders, mental retardation, <strong>and</strong> hearing impairmentAssess adaptive behavior assets <strong>and</strong> deficitsIdentify behavior problems that require interventionAssess caregiver stress <strong>and</strong> caregiver ability to provide adaptive behaviorinstruction <strong>and</strong> behavioral problem managementEvaluate response to previous interventionsargues for their routine use with thesepopulations.Classification <strong>of</strong> the child as havingASDs requires meeting the DSM-IV-TRcriteria outlined earlier. The classificationprocess will depend heavily on caretakerinformation such as detailed histories/interviews,<strong>and</strong> possibly child behavior ratings.Furthermore, medical, language, <strong>and</strong> otherassessment data will need to be integrated inorder to make differential diagnoses.The most likely comorbid conditionfor autism is mental retardation. An indepthassessment <strong>of</strong> intelligence <strong>and</strong> adaptivebehavior development is, therefore,required in every comprehensive study<strong>of</strong> a child suspected <strong>of</strong> autism or a relateddisorder. Sensory impairments must alsobe ruled out by other clinicians.The process <strong>of</strong> ruling out alternativecauses is a lengthy one requiringfull participation by medical <strong>and</strong> otherpr<strong>of</strong>essionals in the assessment process.Neurological problems, in particular,should be ruled out as primary etiologiesfor the child’s behavior. As noted above,the differentiation within types <strong>of</strong> ASDsis also <strong>of</strong>ten quite difficult (see Box 19.2for a specific discussion <strong>of</strong> the assessment<strong>of</strong> Asperger’s Syndrome).A central aspect <strong>of</strong> psychological interventionfor children with ASDs is skilldevelopment in a variety <strong>of</strong> domains. Astructured behavioral observation (e.g.,ADOS, functional analysis) <strong>and</strong> a comprehensivemeasure <strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior aretypically very useful for defining neededskills <strong>and</strong> evaluating the effectiveness <strong>of</strong>intervention.In Box 19.1, we provide a case example<strong>of</strong> this assessment approach for a child withautism.


chapter 19 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> autism spectrum447Box 19.2Issues in the <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> Asperger’s DisorderThe most straightforward distinction betweenAsperger’s Disorder <strong>and</strong> autism is the lack <strong>of</strong>language difficulties seen in the former (AmericanPsychiatric Association, 2000). However,this difference is hard to pinpoint for youth whohave average- to above-average intellectualfunctioning (<strong>and</strong> thus, typically demonstrateno impairments in verbal communication).A careful, detailed history <strong>of</strong> developmentaldelays is critical in differentiating ASDs fromother forms <strong>of</strong> delays <strong>and</strong> for differentiatingamong ASDs. For example, past <strong>and</strong> presentimpairments in social skills <strong>and</strong> the presence <strong>of</strong>stereotypical behaviors in the absence <strong>of</strong> a history<strong>of</strong> language delays would be more indicative<strong>of</strong> Asperger’s Disorder than autism. Therehas been an increased availability <strong>of</strong> specificscreening tools for Asperger’s Disorder <strong>and</strong>other ASDs. The Gilliam Asperger’s DisorderScale (GADS; Gilliam, 2001) is a 32-itemscreener that is meant to be completed by aparent, teacher, or clinician. Items load onto four scales: Social Interaction, CognitivePatterns, Restricted Behavioral Patterns, <strong>and</strong>Pragmatic Skills. The Asperger SyndromeDiagnostic Scale (ASDS; Myles, Bock, &Simpson, 2001) consists <strong>of</strong> 50 items to be completedby a caretaker, teacher, or clinician withfive scales: Cognitive, Language, Social, Maladaptive,<strong>and</strong> Sensorimotor. Another exampleis the Krug Asperger’s Disorder Index (KADI;Krug & Arick, 2003) which consists <strong>of</strong> 32items <strong>and</strong> yields a total score. Research on thedifferential validity is limited.An exception is Campbell’s (2005) studythat compared these rating scales <strong>and</strong> twoothers that had been used in research. Campbellcited concerns regarding each <strong>of</strong> thesemeasures. For the ASDS, Campbell notedthat the st<strong>and</strong>ardization sample was weak,particularly due to the lack <strong>of</strong> there being noindependent diagnosis <strong>of</strong> Asperger’s Disorderin that sample. The GADS was describedas having a stronger st<strong>and</strong>ardization sample,although the Asperger’s Disorder diagnosesin this sample were unconfirmed. In addition,internal consistency was less-than-desirable(Campbell, 2005). The KADI was describedas the best <strong>of</strong> these measures overall in terms<strong>of</strong> reliability (i.e., internal consistency, stability,<strong>and</strong> interrater agreement) as well as incontent validity. However, the st<strong>and</strong>ardization<strong>of</strong> the KADI also lacked informationon independent diagnoses. Additionally, forall three <strong>of</strong> these scales, the cognitive functioning<strong>of</strong> individuals in the st<strong>and</strong>ardizationsample with autism was not provided, whichis important in determining how well a measure<strong>of</strong> Aspeger’s Disorder helps distinguishbetween children with Asperger’s Disordervs. high functioning children with autism(Campbell, 2005). Therefore, at present,such measures should be used as one potentialcomponent <strong>of</strong> assessments for ASDs butin no way replace detailed histories obtainedthrough interviews <strong>and</strong> behavioral observationsin determining the child’s specificimpairments.A Sample Case <strong>of</strong> Autism ina <strong>Child</strong> with NeurologicalImpairmentJulie was a 5-year, 1-month-old girl whowas referred by her parents.Referral InformationJulie was referred for a psychologicalevaluation by her parents. Original referralquestions included concern about delayedlanguage development, short attention span,difficulty in her kindergarten class, <strong>and</strong> apossible learning disability. Subsequently,


448 chapter 19 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> autism spectrumhowever, Julie had two seizures <strong>and</strong> wasdiagnosed as having tuberous sclerosis <strong>and</strong>seizure disorder. On learning that tuberoussclerosis <strong>of</strong>ten affects a child’s intelligence,her parents requested a thorough evaluationto determine Julie’s current level <strong>of</strong>functioning.Background InformationJulie is a 5-year-old girl who lives withboth parents. Her mother reported thatshe had no serious difficulties when shewas pregnant with Julie. Julie was reportedlyborn full-term <strong>and</strong> weighed 7 pounds,0 ounces. Both Julie <strong>and</strong> her mother werereportedly in good condition at the time <strong>of</strong>birth. Julie did, however, have an open soreon her back that was surrounded by a purplemark. The doctor remarked at the timethat this type <strong>of</strong> wound was very unusual.According to her mother, Julie has hadtwo seizures. The first was reportedlyabout 6 weeks prior to this evaluation. Hermother describes these seizures as lastingabout a minute <strong>and</strong> not involving severeconvulsions. Julie was seen by a neurologist.According to Julie’s medical reports,she was somewhat difficult to underst<strong>and</strong><strong>and</strong> hyperactive during that medical examination.Her doctor had difficulty obtainingher cooperation. Her physical conditionappeared to be normal, except for a largescar on her right hip <strong>and</strong> skin lesions on herface <strong>and</strong> extremities. Based on Julie’s physicalcondition, recent seizures, EEG results,<strong>and</strong> CAT scan results, she was diagnosedas having seizure disorder <strong>and</strong> tuberoussclerosis, a genetic disorder associated withtumors in the brain <strong>and</strong> internal organs <strong>and</strong>skin lesions on the face <strong>and</strong> body. Her doctorprescribed Tegretol for seizure control.According to her mother, Julie has alsohad frequent ear infections, beginning at8 months <strong>of</strong> age <strong>and</strong> continuing until 3months ago. At that time, she had tubesplaced in her ears. She has reportedly hadno difficulties since then. She had a hearingexamination 2 months ago that indicated aslight hearing loss in the right ear. Julie hasalso had frequent colds, according to hermother. Julie’s vision is reportedly normal.Julie’s mother described Julie’s developmentas inconsistent. She seemed to learngross motor skills at a normal rate, buther fine motor <strong>and</strong> language developmentappeared to be delayed. She reportedlyspoke her first words at 9 months, but by 18months, she could still speak only four words.Her mother states that Julie did not speak insentences until she was 2½ years old. Herspeech articulation is reportedly clear, buther language is still delayed. She was reportedlyin speech therapy until recently.Her mother stated that Julie is sweet, hasa good sense <strong>of</strong> humor, <strong>and</strong> enjoys singing.Julie, however, can also be stubborn <strong>and</strong>sometimes does not seem to listen to or obeyher mother. Her mother noted that Julielikes to play by herself rather than with otherchildren. She also reported that Julie canbe easily overstimulated in that she seemsuncomfortable in crowded or noisy places, isvery energetic <strong>and</strong> active, has a short attentionspan, <strong>and</strong> is impulsive (e.g., not waitingfor her turn, grabbing objects at stores withoutpermission). She added that Julie seemsto require a great deal <strong>of</strong> parental attention.Julie has reportedly been in a preschoolprogram since she was 2 years old. Hermother stated that Julie’s teachers have beenconcerned because Julie did not communicatein school, had difficulty doing ageappropriatework, required much teacherattention, <strong>and</strong> seemed to lack self-esteem<strong>and</strong> confidence. Julie is now enrolled in akindergarten class. Her current teacherreported that Julie is a sweet child <strong>and</strong> seemsto want to please others. Julie reportedlymemorizes well <strong>and</strong> knows many songs.Julie’s teacher also stated, however, thatJulie shows many behaviors that make itdifficult to teach her. She reportedly spendsmost <strong>of</strong> the day by herself, talking only toherself or inanimate objects. She seems torepeat, over <strong>and</strong> over, phrases that she hasheard adults say. According to her teacher,


chapter 19 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> autism spectrum449Julie does not interact with the other children.When called on in class, she seemsembarrassed <strong>and</strong> then blurts out anyanswer. She reportedly has a very shortattention span <strong>and</strong> also sometimes leavesthe room without teacher permission. Herteacher stated that she is teased by theother children because <strong>of</strong> her behavior,which includes chewing on her mat <strong>and</strong> herclothes. According to her teacher, Julie hasnot learned many academic skills. She seemsto have learned a few basic concepts, such asshapes. She can also make a capital “A,” butit is frequently upside down. She reportedlywrites <strong>and</strong> works with other objects upsidedown frequently <strong>and</strong> does not seem to recognizethat she is doing so. She reportedlycannot complete worksheets, as she justdraws lines up <strong>and</strong> down the page or colorsthe whole page one color. According to herteacher, Julie sometimes does better withone-on-one assistance, but at other times,she will not accept help from her teacher.Julie was briefly observed at school duringlunch. She was sitting at the end <strong>of</strong> thetable by herself <strong>and</strong> did not interact withthe other children. Although she had talkedwith the observer previously, she showedlittle response to her at this time. Whenasked if she remembered the observer, shesaid that she did, but she still did not interactwith her. She just continued eating.Behavioral ObservationsJulie was tested over a 2-day period. Threeexaminers participated in her evaluation.She was brought to the first testing sessionby her mother. She seemed very uncomfortablemeeting strangers <strong>and</strong> clung toher mother. Her mother carried her to thetesting room <strong>and</strong> stayed for a while untilJulie was more comfortable. Julie did notbecome relaxed with the examiners forsome time. Adequate rapport for testingpurposes was finally established.During testing, Julie was very active <strong>and</strong>distractible. The examiners had difficultykeeping her attention on the test materials.She required much direction <strong>and</strong> attentionfrom the examiners to stay on task. She frequentlywanted to play with the other toys.At one point, she sat on the floor, facing thewall. When asked what she was doing, shesaid she was in time-out. On further questioning,she responded that she was put therebecause she did not pay attention. Later,she again sat on the floor, this time repeating“Stephanie, it’s time to go to school.”The examiner had difficulty redirecting herfrom this behavior. This type <strong>of</strong> behaviorseemed to occur most when Julie was beingasked to perform a task that was difficult forher. After this last time, Julie could not beenticed to pay any more attention to thetest materials. She got up <strong>and</strong> left the roomwithout the examiner’s permission.Julie was more cooperative during thesecond testing session. She seemed muchmore comfortable with the examiners <strong>and</strong>accompanied them readily to the testingroom. She was, however, still very distractible<strong>and</strong> active, requiring much examinerattention for her to stay on task.Test Results <strong>and</strong> InterpretationsIntellectual FunctioningJulie’s intellectual functioning was far, towell below, that <strong>of</strong> her same-aged peers.This level <strong>of</strong> functioning is consistent withher teacher’s <strong>and</strong> mother’s reports <strong>of</strong> herability <strong>and</strong> with her current school performance.Her performance across subtestswas consistent, showing no relativestrengths or weaknesses.Visual Motor IntegrationOn a test <strong>of</strong> visual motor integration, Julie’sperformance was at or above that <strong>of</strong> 8%<strong>of</strong> children <strong>of</strong> her age, suggesting that hervisual motor integration <strong>and</strong> fine motorskills are well below average. Her mother<strong>and</strong> teacher also reported difficulty with finemotor tasks, such as drawing <strong>and</strong> writing.


450 chapter 19 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> autism spectrumAcademic AchievementOn a st<strong>and</strong>ardized test <strong>of</strong> academic achievement,only two subtests were able to beadministered. Her performance on thesubtests administered was well to far belowaverage. On these subtests <strong>and</strong> informalassessment, Julie showed the ability to singthe alphabet <strong>and</strong> count objects with oneto-onecorrespondence.Adaptive BehaviorThe Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2 is a measure <strong>of</strong> Julie’sability to take care <strong>of</strong> herself, get alongwith others, <strong>and</strong> live in the communityappropriately for her age. It includes fourdomains: Communication, Daily LivingSkills, Socialization, <strong>and</strong> Motor Skills. Onthe Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2, Julie’s mother indicatedthat Julie’s overall ability in these domainsis slightly below average for her age. Most<strong>of</strong> her scores were consistent with heroverall Adaptive Composite score. Herscore in the Socialization domain wasslightly higher than the others. This score,however, is not consistent with teacher <strong>and</strong>parent reports or behavioral observations.Julie’s teacher completed the Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2,Classroom Edition. Her teacher indicatedthat Julie’s adaptive behavior is variable.Julie’s score on the Daily Living Skillsdomain from her teacher’s report is similarto that given by her mother <strong>and</strong> isin the Below-Average range. However,her other scores are in the Well Below-Average range, more consistent with herintelligence test scores. Her Socializationdomain score is inconsistent with that <strong>of</strong>her mother’s rating but more consistentwith other assessment findings. It indicatesfar below- average socialization skills forJulie at school relative to same-aged peers.Overall, the report <strong>of</strong> Julie’s mother <strong>and</strong>teacher on the Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2 indicates thatJulie’s adaptive behavior is below what wouldbe expected for her age but is somewhat aboveher intellectual ability as assessed in this evaluation.However, it does not appear that a diagnosis<strong>of</strong> Mental Retardation is appropriate atthis point, as Julie’s adaptive functioning is notfar enough below what would be expected forher age to warrant such a diagnosis.Rating ScalesJulie’s mother <strong>and</strong> father completed theAchenbach CBCL, whereas her teachercompleted the TRF. This rating scaleassesses behavioral <strong>and</strong> emotional problems<strong>of</strong> children. Although the ratersreported similar behavioral patterns, onlyJulie’s father indicated that Julie has anyserious problems. He rated Julie as havingproblems in the areas <strong>of</strong> social withdrawal<strong>and</strong> hyperactivity. Julie’s mother<strong>and</strong> teacher also rated her high in theseareas, but not significantly so. These findingsare consistent with teacher <strong>and</strong> parentreport <strong>and</strong> behavioral observations inthat Julie has some incidents <strong>of</strong> apparentoveractivity in the classroom <strong>and</strong> consistentlyseems uncomfortable interactingwith others. Based on Julie’s current emotional,behavioral, social, intellectual, <strong>and</strong>adaptive functioning, as well as her history<strong>of</strong> communicative delays, a diagnosis <strong>of</strong>Pervasive Developmental Disorder, NotOtherwise Specified seems appropriate.Summary <strong>and</strong> ConclusionsJulie is a 5-year-old girl referred by her parentsto determine her current level <strong>of</strong> functioning.She has been diagnosed as havingseizure disorder <strong>and</strong> tuberous sclerosis.Her mother reported that her language<strong>and</strong> fine motor development were delayed.Her teacher reported that she made littleprogress in adjusting to kindergarten orlearning basic academic skills. Parent <strong>and</strong>teacher reports indicate that she is active<strong>and</strong> inattentive <strong>and</strong> requires much adultattention <strong>and</strong> supervision. These behaviorswere also observed in the testing situation,in which examiners had much difficultygetting Julie to cooperate <strong>and</strong> attend tothe tasks. She was very active <strong>and</strong> distractible.Parent <strong>and</strong> teacher reports, as well asobservation during testing, also suggest


chapter 19 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> autism spectrum451that Julie displays several unusual behaviors,such as talking to herself or inanimateobjects, repeating the same phrases ormotions, panting heavily at times, chewingon clothes, preferring to stay by herself,<strong>and</strong> not interacting with other children.Most <strong>of</strong> Julie’s test results, includingintellectual, achievement, visual motor, <strong>and</strong>some adaptive behavior areas, are in theWell Below-Average range. Other adaptivebehavior scores, however, were onlyslightly below average for her age. Informationfrom rating scales indicates that Julietends to be socially withdrawn, hyperactive,distractible, slow to learn or adapt to newsituations, <strong>and</strong> sometimes fussy too.Taking all <strong>of</strong> the assessment informationtogether, the diagnosis <strong>of</strong> PervasiveDevelopmental Disorder–Not OtherwiseSpecified seems appropriate. At this time,a diagnosis <strong>of</strong> mental retardation does notseem applicable. Although her intelligencetest scores are within the mild mentallyretarded range, her adaptive behaviorscores lie outside the mental retardationrange. Both <strong>of</strong> these areas <strong>of</strong> functioningmust be significantly below average to warranta diagnosis <strong>of</strong> Mental Retardation.RecommendationsJulie should be evaluated again at the end <strong>of</strong>this academic year to assess her progress. Atthis time the following recommendations aremade for Julie:1. Julie would benefit from a highly structurededucational environment whereshe can get the attention <strong>and</strong> supervision,she needs, to learn. Her parentsshould consult school personnel in orderto determine the most appropriate placementfor her2. Julie will need much individual instruction,gradually moving to small-groupinstruction3. Julie’s classroom assignments shouldbe within the range appropriate for herintellectual <strong>and</strong> academic levels4. Julie should be allowed to learn <strong>and</strong>work at her own pace5. Instructional tasks should be organizedinto short, structured units6. Julie will need to be taught at a veryconcrete <strong>and</strong> practical level, usingmany manipulatives <strong>and</strong> teaching aids7. Skills <strong>and</strong> concepts will need to berepeated <strong>and</strong> reviewed <strong>of</strong>ten for Julieto master them8. Julie may be helped by being tutored bysomeone who has been trained in tutoringchildren with her set <strong>of</strong> difficulties9. Julie’s parents <strong>and</strong> teachers shouldcontinue to search for activities thatshe does well <strong>and</strong> enjoys <strong>and</strong> encourageher in these activities10. Julie should be provided withincreased opportunities to learn <strong>and</strong>practice age-appropriate self-care<strong>and</strong> socialization skills11. Julie’s parents <strong>and</strong> teachers should continueto use behavioral managementstrategies, such as positive reinforcement<strong>and</strong> time-out, to help her learnappropriate behavior. Her parents maywish to attend sessions with a mentalhealth specialist to become more facilewith these techniques12. Inappropriate behavior should beignored as much as possible, whileappropriate behavior is rewarded withpraise <strong>and</strong> other appropriate reinforcers13. Communication between school <strong>and</strong>home should be maintained. A homeschoolnote, in which Julie is rewardedat home <strong>and</strong> for appropriate behavior atschool, may be helpful14. Julie should be allowed as much as possibleto be around other children herown age, so that she can learn socialization<strong>and</strong> develop behavioral skills byobservation <strong>and</strong> interaction with them15. Julie’s parents should closely monitorher physical functioning <strong>and</strong> symptoms<strong>and</strong> maintain regular contact with herphysicians regarding her seizures16. Julie should be evaluated in approximately1 year to determine her level


452 chapter 19 <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> autism spectrum<strong>of</strong> functioning, particularly after theabove strategies have been implemented.ConclusionsAutism <strong>and</strong> its related disorders are amongthe most difficult <strong>of</strong> the developmentaldisorders to reliably classify, yet classificationis central to research efforts aimed atprevention <strong>and</strong> intervention. Considerableprogress has been made in defining thethree core dimensions <strong>of</strong> impairment thatseparate this condition from other developmentaldisabilities <strong>and</strong> in developingassessment tools geared toward evaluatingthese dimensions. Many <strong>of</strong> these toolsappear to be useful in devising interventionstrategies for individuals with ASDs.Many <strong>of</strong> the available instruments alsodifferentiate ASDs from other developmentaldisabilities, but this differentiatingis particularly difficult for very youngchildren <strong>and</strong> those with more severe developmentaldelays. While new methods <strong>of</strong>assessment have made strides in terms <strong>of</strong>differential validity, considerably moreprogress is needed. The most promisingmethod for assessing <strong>and</strong> classifying autismat this time is a thorough <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardinterview <strong>of</strong> the child’s primary caregiveralong with either a structured observationthat systematically evaluates symptoms<strong>of</strong> ASDs <strong>and</strong>/or a functional analysisthat evaluates the communicative intent<strong>of</strong> a child’s behavioral problems. Complementingthe diagnostic process is the use<strong>of</strong> intelligence <strong>and</strong> adaptive behavior scalesuch as the Vinel<strong>and</strong>-2.In summary, current recommendations(e.g., de Bildt et al., 2004; Ozon<strong>of</strong>fet al., 2005) call for a combination <strong>of</strong> adetailed developmental history (e.g., ADI-R), behavioral observation (e.g., ADOS),assessment <strong>of</strong> cognitive functioning, <strong>and</strong>assessment <strong>of</strong> adaptive functioning.Chapter Summary1. Autistic Disorder is part <strong>of</strong> a class <strong>of</strong>problems known as Pervasive DevelopmentalDisorders (PDD) or AutismSpectrum Disorders (ASDs)2. The DSM emphasizes three classes<strong>of</strong> symptoms that are central to thedisorder: social interaction problems,communication problems, <strong>and</strong> repetitive<strong>and</strong> stereotyped behaviors3. Much remains to be learned aboutrisk factors that may be etiologicallyinvolved in autism4. Differentiation within ASDs <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong> autismfrom Pervasive Developmental Disorder– Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS)<strong>and</strong> Mental Retardation is difficult5. The most frequent comorbid problemfor autism is mental retardation6. The Autism Diagnostic Interview –Revised (ADI-R) is a semi-structureddiagnostic interview that is designed tobe used with a child’s primary caregiver.It provides for a detailed history <strong>of</strong> thedomains <strong>of</strong> functioning relevant to ASDs7. The Autism Diagnostic ObservationSchedule (ADOS) is designed for theassessment <strong>of</strong> autism through structuredobservation in a classroom or clinical setting.Four modules are available basedon the individual’s expressive communicationskills <strong>and</strong> developmental level8. <strong>Assessment</strong> strategies such as functionalanalysis can be useful in determiningthe communicative intent <strong>of</strong> problembehaviors in children with ASDs <strong>and</strong> maydirectly translate to intervention plans9. There has been an increase in ratingscales aimed at screening for Asperger’sDisorder. However, more research onthe psychometrics <strong>and</strong> utility <strong>of</strong> thesemeasures is needed10. The within-syndrome variability associatedwith autism suggests that childrenwho are either suspected <strong>of</strong>, or diagnosedwith, the disorder should have access tomultidisciplinary assessment procedures


R e f e r e n c e sAAMR. (1992). Mental retardation: Definition, classification<strong>and</strong> systems <strong>of</strong> supports (9th ed.). Annapolis, MD:Author.Abad, J., Forns, M., Amador, J. A., & Martorell, B.(2000). Youth self-report reliability <strong>and</strong> validity in anadolescent sample. Psicothema, 12(1), 49–54.Abidin, R. R. (1986). Parenting stress index manual (2nded.). Charlottesville, VA: Pediatric Psychology Press.Abidin, R. R. (1995). Parenting Stress Index: pr<strong>of</strong>essionalmanual (3rd ed.). Odessa, FL: Psychological<strong>Assessment</strong>.Abidin, R., Flens, J.R., & Austin, W.G. (1995). The ParentingStress Index. In R.P. Archer (Ed.), Forensic uses<strong>of</strong> clinical assessment instruments (pp. 297-328). Mahwah,NJ: Erlbaum.Abik<strong>of</strong>f, H., & Klein, R. G. (1992). Attention-deficithyperactivity <strong>and</strong> conduct disorder: Comorbidity <strong>and</strong>implications for treatment. Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong><strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 60, 881–892.Ablow, J. C., Measelle, J. R., Kraemer, H. C., Harrington,R., Luby, J., Smider, N., et al. (1999). TheMacArthur three-city outcome study: Evaluatingmulti-informant measures <strong>of</strong> young children’s symptomatology.Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong>& <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 38, 1580–1590.Abramowitz, A. J., & O’Leary, S. G. (1991). Behavioralinterventions for the classroom: Implications forstudents with ADHD. School Psychology Review, 20,220–234.Achenbach, T. M. (1982). <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>and</strong> taxonomy<strong>of</strong> children’s behavior disorders. In B. B. Lahey & A.E. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in clinical child psychology(Vol. 5, pp. 1–38). New York: Plenum.Achenbach, T. M. (1986). <strong>Child</strong> behavior checklist-directobservation form (rev. ed.). Burlington: University <strong>of</strong>Vermont.Achenbach, T. M. (1991a). Integrative guide for the 1991CBCL/4–18, YSR, <strong>and</strong> TRF pr<strong>of</strong>iles. Burlington: University<strong>of</strong> Vermont, Department <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry.Achenbach, T. M. (1991b) Manual for the child behaviorchecklist/4–18 <strong>and</strong> 1991 pr<strong>of</strong>ile. Burlington: University<strong>of</strong> Vermont, Department <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry.Achenbach, T. M. (1991c). Manual for the teacher’s reportform <strong>and</strong> 1991 pr<strong>of</strong>ile. Burlington: University <strong>of</strong> Vermont,Department <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry.Achenbach, T. M. (1991d). Manual for the youth selfreport& 1991 pr<strong>of</strong>ile. Burlington: University <strong>of</strong> Vermont,Department <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry.Achenbach, T. M. (1993). Empirically based taxonomy:How to use syndromes <strong>and</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ile types derived from theCBCL/4–18, TRT & YSR. Burlington: University <strong>of</strong>Vermont, Department <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry.Achenbach, T. M. (1995). Empirically based assessment& taxonomy: Applications to clinical research. Psychological<strong>Assessment</strong>, 7, 261–274.Achenbach, T. M. (1997). Manual for the young adult selfreport<strong>and</strong> young adult behavior checklist. Burlington:University <strong>of</strong> Vermont, Department <strong>of</strong> PsychiatryAchenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2000). Manual forthe ASEBA preschool forms <strong>and</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>iles. Burlington, VT:Research Center for <strong>Child</strong>ren, Youth, <strong>and</strong> Families.Achenbach, T. M. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA schoolageforms <strong>and</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>iles. Burlington, VT: Research Centerfor <strong>Child</strong>ren, Youth, <strong>and</strong> Families.Achenbach, T. M., Conners, C. K., Quay, H. C., Verhulst,F. C., & Howell, C. T. (1989). Replication <strong>of</strong> empiricallyderived syndromes as a basis for taxonomy <strong>of</strong>child/adolescent psychopathology. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal<strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 17, 299–323.Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. (1978). The classification<strong>of</strong> child psychopathology: A review <strong>and</strong> analysis <strong>of</strong>empirical efforts. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 1275–1301.Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. (1983). Manual forthe child behavior checklist <strong>and</strong> revised child behavior pr<strong>of</strong>ile.Burlington: University <strong>of</strong> Vermont, Department<strong>of</strong> Psychiatry.Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. (1986). Manual forthe teacher’s report form <strong>and</strong> teacher- version <strong>of</strong> the child453P.J. Frick et al., <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Behavior,DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-0641-0_5, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009


454 referencesbehavior pr<strong>of</strong>ile. Burlington: University <strong>of</strong> Vermont,Department <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry.Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. (1991). Manual forthe youth self report <strong>and</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ile. Burlington: University<strong>of</strong> Vermont Department <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry.Achenbach, T. M., & Howell, C. T. (1993). Are Americanchildren’s problems getting worse? A 13-yearcomparison. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong><strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 32, 1145–1154.Achenbach, T. M., Howell, C. T., McConaughy, S. H.,& Stanger, C. (1995a). Six-year predictors <strong>of</strong> problemsin a national sample <strong>of</strong> children & youth: I. Crossinformantsyndromes. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy<strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 34, 336–347.Achenbach, T. M., Howell, C. T., McConaughy, S. H.,& Stanger, C. (1995b). Six-year predictors <strong>of</strong> problemsin a national sample <strong>of</strong> children & youth: II.Signs <strong>of</strong> disturbance. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy<strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 34, 488–498.Achenbach, T. M., Howell, C. T., McConaughy, S. H.,& Stanger, C. (1998). Six-year predictors <strong>of</strong> problemsin a national sample: IV. Young adult signs <strong>of</strong> disturbance.Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong><strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 37, 718–727.Achenbach, T. M., Howell, C. T., Quay, H. C., &Conners, C. K. (1991). National survey <strong>of</strong> problems<strong>and</strong> competencies among four-to sixteen-year-olds:Parents’ reports for normative <strong>and</strong> clinical samples.Monographs <strong>of</strong> the Society for Research in <strong>Child</strong> Development,56(3), 1–131.Achenbach, T. M., & McConaughy, S. H. (1987).Empirically based assessment <strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong> adolescentpsychopathology: Practical applications. Newbury Park,CA: Sage.Achenbach, T. M., & McConaughy, S. H. (1996). Relationsbetween DSM-1V <strong>and</strong> empirically based assessment.School Psychology Review, 25, 330–342.Achenbach, T. M., & McConaughy, S. H. (1997).Empirically based assessment <strong>of</strong> child & adolescent psychopathology(2nd ed.). Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage.Achenbach, T. M., McConaughy, S. H., & Howell, C.T. (1987). <strong>Child</strong>/adolescent behavioral <strong>and</strong> emotionalproblems: Implications <strong>of</strong> cross-informant correlationsfor situational specificity. Psychological Bulletin,101, 213–232.Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2000). Manual forthe ASEBA preschool forms & pr<strong>of</strong>iles. Burlington: University<strong>of</strong> Vermont, Department <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry.Adrien, J. L., Barthelemy, C., Perrot, A., & Roux, S.(1992). Validity <strong>and</strong> reliability <strong>of</strong> the infant behavioralsummarized evaluation (IBSE): A rating scalefor the assessment <strong>of</strong> young children with autism <strong>and</strong>developmental disorders. Journal <strong>of</strong> Autism <strong>and</strong> DevelopmentalDisorders, 22, 371–394.AERA, APA, & NCME. (1999). St<strong>and</strong>ards for educational<strong>and</strong> psychological testing. Washington, DC:AERA.Agras, W. S., Chapin, H. N., & Oliveau, D. C. (1972).The natural history <strong>of</strong> phobias: Course <strong>and</strong> prognosis.Archives <strong>of</strong> General Psychiatry, 26, 315–317.Albano, A. M., & Silverman, W. K. (1996). Anxiety disordersinterview schedule for DSM-IV: Clinician manual.New York: The Psychological Corporation.Albert-Corush, J., Firestone, P., & Goodman, J. T.(1986). Attention <strong>and</strong> impulsivity characteristics <strong>of</strong>the biological <strong>and</strong> adoptive parents <strong>of</strong> hyperactive <strong>and</strong>normal children. American Journal <strong>of</strong> Orthopsychiatry,56, 413–423.Alderman, E., & Kennedy, C. (1995). The right to privacy.New York: Alfred A. Knopf.Allen, J. C., & Hollifield, J. (1990). Using the Rorschachwith children <strong>and</strong> adolescents: The Exner ComprehensiveSystem. In C. R. Reynolds & R. W. Kamphaus(Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong> educationalassessment <strong>of</strong> children: <strong>Personality</strong>, behavior, <strong>and</strong> context(pp. 168–185). New York: Guilford.Allen, J. C., & Hollifield, J. (2003). Using the Rorschachwith children <strong>and</strong> adolescents: The Exner ComprehensiveSystem. In C. R. Reynolds & R. W. Kamphaus(Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong> educationalassessment <strong>of</strong> children: <strong>Personality</strong>, behavior, <strong>and</strong> context– 2nd edition (pp. 182–197). New York: Guilford.Allen, M. J., & Yen, W. M. (1979). Introduction to measurementtheory. Long Grove, IL: Wavel<strong>and</strong>.Altepeter, T. S., & Breen, M. J. (1989). The home situationsquestionnaire (HSQ) <strong>and</strong> the school situationsquestionnaire (SSQ): Normative data <strong>and</strong> an evaluation<strong>of</strong> psychometric properties. Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychoeducational<strong>Assessment</strong>, 7, 312–322.Aman, M. G., Werry, J. S., Fitzpatrick, J., Lowe, M., &Waters, J. (1983). Factor structure <strong>and</strong> norms for therevised behavior problem checklist on New Zeal<strong>and</strong>children. Australian <strong>and</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong> Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry,17, 354–360.Aman, M. G. (1994). Instruments for assessing treatmenteffects in developmentally delayed populations.<strong>Assessment</strong> in Rehabilitation <strong>and</strong> Exceptionality,1, 1–19.Amanat, E., & Butler, C. (1984). Oppressive behaviorsin the families <strong>of</strong> depressed children. Family Therapy,11, 65–77.Amato, P. R., & Keith, B. (1991). Parental divorce <strong>and</strong>the well-being <strong>of</strong> children: A meta-analysis. PsychologicalBulletin, 110, 26–46.Ambrosini, P. J. (2000). Historical development <strong>and</strong>present status <strong>of</strong> the schedule for affective disorders<strong>and</strong> schizophrenia for school-age children(K-SADS). Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong><strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 39, 39–48.American Educational Research Association. AmericanPsychological Association, <strong>and</strong> National Councilon Measurement in Education. (1999). St<strong>and</strong>ards foreducational <strong>and</strong> psychological testing. Washington, DC:Author.


eferenes455American Psychiatric Association. (1968). The diagnostic<strong>and</strong> statistical manual <strong>of</strong> mental disorders (2nd ed.).Washington, DC: Author.American Psychiatric Association. (1980). The diagnostic<strong>and</strong> statistical manual <strong>of</strong> mental disorders (3rd ed.).Washington, DC: Author.American Psychiatric Association. (1987). 7 he diagnostic<strong>and</strong> statistical manual <strong>of</strong> mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.).Washington, DC: Author.American Psychiatric Association. (1994). The diagnostic<strong>and</strong> statistical manual <strong>of</strong> mental disorders (4th ed.).Washington, DC: Author.American Psychiatric Association. (2000). The diagnostic<strong>and</strong> statistical manual <strong>of</strong> mental disorders (4th ed.,text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.American Psychological Association. (1985). St<strong>and</strong>ardsfor educational <strong>and</strong> psychological testing. Washington,DC: Author.American Psychological Association. (1992). Ethicalprinciples <strong>of</strong> psychologists <strong>and</strong> code <strong>of</strong> conduct.American Psychologist, 47, 1612–1628.American Psychological Association. (1994). Guidelinesfor child custody evaluations in divorce proceedings.American Psychologist, 49, 677–680.Ames, L. B., Metraux, R., Rodell, J. L., & Walker, R.(1974). <strong>Child</strong> Rorschach responses (rev. ed.). New York:Bruner/Mazel.Anastasi, A. (1992). Introductory remarks. In K. F. Geisinger(Ed.), Psychological testing <strong>of</strong> Hispanics (pp. 1–7).Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Anastasi, A. (1988). Psychological testing (16 th ed). NewYork: Macmillan.Anastasi, A., & Urbina, S. (1998). Psychological testing(7th ed.). New York: Prentice-Hall.Anderson, J. C., Williams, S., McGee, R., & Silva, P. A.(1987). DSM-III disorders in preadolescent children.Archives <strong>of</strong> General Psychiatry, 44, 69–76.Angold, A., & Costello, J. (2000). The child <strong>and</strong> adolescentpsychiatric assessment (CAPA). Journal <strong>of</strong> theAmerican Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry,39, 49–58.Angold, A., Costello, E. J., & Erkanli, A. (1999).Comorbidity. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry& Allied Disciplines, 40, 57–87.Angold, A., Erkanli, A., Egger, H. I., & Costello, F. J.(2000). Stimulant treatment for children: A communityperspective. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong><strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 39, 975–983.Applegate, B., Lahey, B. B., Hart, E. L., Waldman, I.,Biederman, J., Hynd, G. W., et al. (1997). The age <strong>of</strong>onset criterion for DSM-W attention-deficit hyperactivitydisorder: A report <strong>of</strong> the DSM-IV field trials.Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong>Psychiatry, 36, 1211–1221.Archer, R. P. (1992). MMPI-A: Assessing adolescent psychopathology.Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Archer, R. P. (1997). MMPI-A: Assessing adolescent psychopathology(2nd ed.). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.Archer, R. P. (2005). Implications <strong>of</strong> MMPI/MMPI-Afindings for underst<strong>and</strong>ing adolescent development<strong>and</strong> psychopathology. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>,85, 257–270.Archer, R. P., Belevich, J. K. S., & Elkins, D. E. (1994).Item-level <strong>and</strong> scale-level factor structures <strong>of</strong> theMMPI-A. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>, 62, 332–345.Archer, R. P., & Elkins, D. E. (1999). Identification <strong>of</strong>r<strong>and</strong>om responding on the MMPI-A. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong><strong>Assessment</strong>, 73, 407–421.Archer, R. P., H<strong>and</strong>el, R. W., & Lynch, K. D. (2001).The effectiveness <strong>of</strong> MMPI-A items in discriminatingbetween normative <strong>and</strong> clinical samples. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong><strong>Assessment</strong>, 77, 420–435.Archer, R. P., & Krishnamurthy, R. (1994). A structuralsummary approach for the MMPI-A: Development<strong>and</strong> empirical correlates. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>,63, 554–573.Archer, R., Krishnamurthy, R., & Stredny, R. (2007).The Minnesota Multiphasic <strong>Personality</strong> Inventory-<strong>Adolescent</strong>.In S. R. Smith (Ed.), The clinicalassessment <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents: A practitioner’sh<strong>and</strong>book (pp. 237–266). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence ErlbaumAssociates.Archer, R. P., & Krishnamurthy, R. (1997). MMPI-A<strong>and</strong> Rorschach indices related to depression <strong>and</strong> conductdisorder: An evaluation <strong>of</strong> the incremental validityhypothesis. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>, 69,517–533.Archer, R. P., & Newsom, C. R. (2000). Psychological testusage with adolescent clients: Survey update. <strong>Assessment</strong>,7, 227–235.Archer, R. P., & Slesinger, D. (1999). MMPI-A patternsrelated to the endorsement <strong>of</strong> suicidal ideation. <strong>Assessment</strong>,6(1), 51–59.Arita, A. A., & Baer, R. A. (1998). Validity <strong>of</strong> selectedMMPI-A content scales. Psychological <strong>Assessment</strong>, 10,59–63.Arney, F., Rogers, H., Baghurst, P., Sawyer, M., & Prior,M. (2008). The reliability <strong>and</strong> validity <strong>of</strong> the parentingscale for Australian mothers <strong>of</strong> pre-school agedchildren. Australian Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychology, 60, 44–52.Arnold, D. S., O’Leary, S. G., Wolff, L. S., & Acker, M.M. (1993). The Parenting Scale: A measure <strong>of</strong> dysfunctionalparenting in discipline situations. Psychological<strong>Assessment</strong>, 5, 137–144.Aronen, E. T., Teerikangas, O. M., & Kurkela, S. A. (1999).The continuity <strong>of</strong> psychiatric symptoms from adolescenceinto young adulthood. Nordic Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry,53(5), 333–338.Arseneault, L., Kim-Cohen, J., Taylor, A., Caspi, A., &M<strong>of</strong>fit, T. (2005). Psychometric evaluation <strong>of</strong> 5- <strong>and</strong>7-year-old children’s self-reports <strong>of</strong> conduct problems.Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 33(5), 537–550.


456 referencesAsher, S. R., & Hymel, S. (1981). <strong>Child</strong>ren’s social competencein peer relations: Sociometric <strong>and</strong> behavioralassessment. In J. D. Wine & M. D. Smye (Eds.), Socialcompetence (pp. 125–157). New York: Guilford Press.Asher, S. R., Singleton, L. C., Tinsley, B. R., & Hymel,S. (1979). A reliable sociometric measure for preschoolchildren. Developmental Psychology, 15, 443–444.Assing, R. (1998, August). BASC parent rating scale(Spanish translation): Validity study. Paper presentedat the annual meeting <strong>of</strong> the American PsychologicalAssociation, San Francisco.Aten, J. D., Madson, M. B., & Kruse, S. J. (2008). Thesupervision genogram: A tool for preparing supervisors-in-training.Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice,Training, 45, 111–116.Atkins, M. S., Pelham, W. E., & Licht, M. (1985). Acomparison <strong>of</strong> objective classroom measures <strong>and</strong>teacher ratings <strong>of</strong> attention deficit disorder. Journal <strong>of</strong>Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 13, 155–167.Atkinson, L. (1990). St<strong>and</strong>ard errors <strong>of</strong> prediction forthe Vinel<strong>and</strong> adaptive behavior scales. Journal <strong>of</strong> SchoolPsychology, 28, 255–259.August, G. J., McDonald, A. W., Realmuto, G. M., &Skare, S. S. (1996). Hyperactive <strong>and</strong> aggressive pathways:Effect <strong>of</strong> demographic, family, <strong>and</strong> child characteristicson children’s adaptive functioning. Journal <strong>of</strong><strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 25, 341–351.Baer, R. A., Ballenger, J., & Kroll, L. S. (1998). Detection<strong>of</strong> underreporting on the MMPI-A in clinical<strong>and</strong> community samples. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>,71, 98–113.Bailey, A., LeCouteur, A., Gottesman, I., Bolton, P.,Simon<strong>of</strong>f, E., Yuzda, E., et al. (1995). Autism as astrongly genetic disorder: Evidence from a Britishtwin study. Psychological Medicine, 25, 63–77.Baldwin, J. A., & Bell, Y. R. (1985). The African-AmericanSelf-Consciousness Scale: An Africentric personalityquestionnaire. Western Journal <strong>of</strong> Black Studies,9, 61–68.Baldwin, J. S., & Dadds, M. R. (2008). Anxiety disorders.In M. Hersen (Series Ed.) & D. Reitman (Vol.Ed.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> Psychological <strong>Assessment</strong>, Case Conceptualization,<strong>and</strong> Treatment (pp. 231–263). Hoboken,NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Ball, J. D., Archer, R. P., Gordon, R. A., & French, J. (1991).Rorschach depression indices with children <strong>and</strong> adolescents:Concurrent validity findings. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong><strong>Assessment</strong>, 57, 465–476.Bao-Yu, W., & Lin-Yan, S. (2004). Family environment <strong>and</strong>the development <strong>of</strong> behavior in children with ADHD.Chinese Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 12, 145–146.Barker, P. (1990). <strong>Clinical</strong> interviews with children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.New York: Norton.Barkley, R. A. (1981). Hyperactive children: A h<strong>and</strong>bookfor diagnosis <strong>and</strong> treatment. New York: GuilfordPress.Barkley, R. A. (1988). Attention-deficit disorder withhyperactivity. In E. J. Mash & L. G. Terdal (Eds.), Behavioralassessment <strong>of</strong> childhood disorders (2nd ed., pp. 69–104).New York: Guilford Press.Barkley, R. A. (1990). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder:A h<strong>and</strong>book to diagnosis <strong>and</strong> treatment (2nd ed.).New York: Guilford Press.Barkley, R. A. (1991). The ecological validity <strong>of</strong> laboratory<strong>and</strong> analogue assessment methods <strong>of</strong> ADHD.Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 19, 149–178.Barkley, R. A. (1997a). Attention deficit hyperactivitydisorder. In E. J. Mash & L. G. Terdel (Eds.), <strong>Assessment</strong><strong>of</strong> childhood disorders (3rd ed., pp. 71–129). NewYork: Guilford Press.Barkley, R. A. (1997b). ADHD <strong>and</strong> the nature <strong>of</strong> selfcontrol.New York: Guilford Press.Barkley, R. A., & Edelbrock, C. (1987). Assessing situationalvariation in children’s problem behaviors: Thehome <strong>and</strong> school situations questionnaires. In R. J.Prinz (Ed.), Advances in behavioral assessment <strong>of</strong> children<strong>and</strong> families (Vol. 3, pp. 157–176). New York:JAI.Barkley, R. A., Fischer, M., Smallish, L., Fletcher, K.(2002). The persistence <strong>of</strong> attention deficit hyperactivitydisorder into adulthood as a function <strong>of</strong> reportingsource <strong>and</strong> definition <strong>of</strong> disorder. Journal <strong>of</strong> AbnormalPsychology, 111, 279–289.Barkley, R. A., Guevremont, D. G., Anastopoulos, A.D., & Fletcher, K. F. (1993). Driving related risks <strong>and</strong>outcomes <strong>of</strong> attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder inadolescents <strong>and</strong> young adults. Pediatrics, 92, 212–218.Barlow, D. H. (2002). Anxiety <strong>and</strong> its disorders: The nature<strong>and</strong> treatment <strong>of</strong> anxiety <strong>and</strong> panic (2nd ed.). NewYork:Guilford.Baron-Cohen, S., Allen, J., & Gillberg, C. (1992). Canautism he detected at 18 months? The needle, thehaystack, <strong>and</strong> the CHAT. British Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry,161, 839–843.Barringer, M. S. (2000). <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> ADHD-hyperactivesub-type using visual continuous performance,psychophysiological, <strong>and</strong> behavior measures. A dissertationsubmitted to the Office <strong>of</strong> Graduate Studies <strong>of</strong>Texas A & M University.Barrios, B. A. (1988). Fears <strong>and</strong> anxiety. In E. J. Mash & L.G. Terdal (Eds.), Behavioral assessment <strong>of</strong> childhood disorders(2nd ed., pp. 196–264). New York: Guilford Press.Barrios, B. A. (1993). Direct observation. In F. H. Ollendick& M. Hersen (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong> adolescentassessment (pp. 140–164). Boston, MA: Allyn& Bacon.Barrios, B. A., & Hartmann, D. P. (1988). Fears <strong>and</strong>anxiety. In E. J. Mash & L. G. Terdal (Eds.), Behavioralassessment <strong>of</strong> child-hood disorders (pp. 167–221).New York: Guilford Press.Barry, C. T., Frick, P. J., & Grafeman, S. J. (in press).<strong>Child</strong> versus parent report <strong>of</strong> parenting practices:Implications for the conceptualization <strong>of</strong> childbehavioral <strong>and</strong> emotional problems. <strong>Assessment</strong>.Barry, T., Dunlap, S., Cotten, S., Lochman, J., & Wells,K. (2005). The influence <strong>of</strong> maternal stress <strong>and</strong> dis-


eferenes457tress on disruptive behavior problems in boys. Journal<strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> & <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry,44, 265–273.Bauer, D. H. (1976). An exploratory study <strong>of</strong> developmentalchanges in children’s fear. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology<strong>and</strong> Psychiatry, 17, 69–74.Baumrind, D. (1971). Harmonious parents <strong>and</strong> their preschoolchildren. Developmental Psychology, 4, 99–102.Beck, S. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometricproperties <strong>of</strong> the Beck depression inventory:Twenty-five years <strong>of</strong> evaluation. <strong>Clinical</strong> PsychologyReview, 8, 77–100.Befera, M. S., & Barkley, R. A. (1985). Hyperactive <strong>and</strong>normal girls <strong>and</strong> boys: Mother-child interactions,parent psychiatric status, <strong>and</strong> child psychopathology.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry, 26,439–452.Behar, L. B., & Stringfield, S. A. (1974). A behavior ratingscale for the preschool child. <strong>Child</strong> Development,10, 601–610.Bell, N. L., & Nangle, R. J. (1999). Interpretive issueswith the Roberts apperception test for children:Limitations <strong>of</strong> the st<strong>and</strong>ardization group. Psychologyin the Schools, 36, 277–283.Bellak, L., & Bellak, S. (1949a). A manual for the children’sapperception test. Larchmont, NY: CPS.Bellak, L., & Bellak, S. S. (1949b). The children’s apperceptiontest. New York: CPS.Belter, R. W., & Piotrowski, C. (1999). Current status<strong>of</strong> master’s-level training in psychological assessment.Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychological Practice, 5, 1–5.Belardinelli, C., Hatch, J. P., Olvera, R. L., Fonseca, M.,Caetano, S. C., Nicoletti, M., et al. (2008). Familyenvironment patterns in families with bipolar children.Journal <strong>of</strong> Affective Disorders, 107, 299–305.Bender, W. N. (1997). Medical interventions <strong>and</strong> schoolmonitoring. In W. N. Bender (Ed.), Underst<strong>and</strong>ingADHD: A practical guide for teachers <strong>and</strong> parents (pp.107–122). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill.Benson, J. (1998). Developing a strong program <strong>of</strong> constructvalidation: A test anxiety example. EducationalMeasurement: Issues <strong>and</strong> Practice, 17, 10–22.Berg, I. (1993). Aspects <strong>of</strong> school phobia. In C. G. Last(Ed.), Anxiety across the lifespan: A developmental perspective(pp. 78–93). New York: Springer.Bergman, L. R., & Magnusson, D. (1997). A personorientedapproach in research on developmental psychopathology.Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 9,291–319.Bertr<strong>and</strong>, J., Mars, A., Boyle, C., Bove, F., Yeargin-Allsopp, M., & Decoufle, P. (2001) Prevalence <strong>of</strong>autism in the United States population: The bricktownship New Jersey investigation. Pediatrics, 108,1155–1161.Berument, S. K., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Pickles, A., &Bailey, A. (1999). Autism screening questionnaire:Diagnostic validity. British Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry, 175,441–451.Beutler, L. E., & Malik, M. (Eds.). (2002). Rethinkingthe DSM: Psychological perspectives. Washington, DC:American Psychological Association.Bickman, L. (2002). The death <strong>of</strong> treatment as usual: Anexcellent first step on a long road. <strong>Clinical</strong> PsychologyScience <strong>and</strong> Practice, 9, 195–198.Biederman, J., Gao, H., Rogers, A., & Spencer, T. (2006).Comparison <strong>of</strong> parent <strong>and</strong> teacher reports <strong>of</strong> attentiondeficit/hyperactivitydisorder symptoms from twoplacebo-controlled studies <strong>of</strong> atomoxetine in children.Biological Psychiatry, 60, 1106–1110.Biederman, J., Keenan, K., & Faraone, S. V. (1990).Parent-based diagnosis <strong>of</strong> attention deficit disorderpredicts a diagnosis based on teacher report. Journal <strong>of</strong>the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry,29, 698–701.Bigras, M., LaFrenier, P. J., & Dumas, J. E. (1996). Discriminantvalidity <strong>of</strong> the parent-child scales <strong>of</strong> theparenting stress index. Early Education <strong>and</strong> Development,7, 167–178.Bird, H. R., Gould, M. S., & Stagheeza, B. (1992). Aggregatingdata from multiple informants in child psychiatryepidemiological research. Journal <strong>of</strong> the AmericanAcademy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 31, 78–85.Bird, H. R., Gould, M. S., & Staghezza, B. M. (1993)Patterns <strong>of</strong> diagnostic comorbidity in a communitysample <strong>of</strong> children aged 9 through 16 years. Journal <strong>of</strong>the American Academy. <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry,32, 361–368.Bird, H. R., Yager, T. J., Staghezza, B., Gould, M. S.,Canino, G., & Rubio-Stipec, M. A. (1990). Impairmentin the epidemiological measurement <strong>of</strong> childhoodpsychopathology in the community. Journal <strong>of</strong>the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry,29, 796–803.Birmaher, B., Arbelaez, C., & Brent, D. (2002). Course<strong>and</strong> outcome <strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong> adolescent major depressivedisorder. <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Clinics <strong>of</strong> North America,11, 619–638.Black, K. (1994). A critical review <strong>of</strong> the MMPI-A.<strong>Child</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> News, 4(1), 9–12.Blader, J. C. (2004). Symptom, family, <strong>and</strong> service predictors<strong>of</strong> children’s psychiatric rehospitalization withinone year <strong>of</strong> discharge. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy<strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> & <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 43, 440–451.Blair, R. J. R. (1999). Responsiveness to distress cues inthe child with psychopathic tendencies. <strong>Personality</strong><strong>and</strong> Individual Differences, 27, 135–145.Blashfield, R. K. (1984). The classification <strong>of</strong> psychopathology:Neo-Kraepelinian <strong>and</strong> quantitative approaches.New York: Plenum.Block, J. H. (1965). The child rearing practices report.Berkeley: Institute <strong>of</strong> Human Development, University<strong>of</strong> California-Berkeley.Block, J. H., Block, J., & Morrison, A. (1981). Parentalagreement-disagreement on child-rearing orientations<strong>and</strong> gender-related personality correlates in children.<strong>Child</strong> Development, 52, 965–974.


458 referencesBloomquist, M. L., August, G. J., Cohen, C., Doyle,A., & Ever-hart, K. (1997). Social problem solvingin hyperactive-aggressive children: How <strong>and</strong> whatthey think in conditions <strong>of</strong> automatic <strong>and</strong> controlledprocessing. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 26,172–180.Blum, G. S. (1950). The blacky pictures: Manual <strong>of</strong> instructions.New York: The Psychological Corporation.Bölte, S., & Poustka, F. (2002). The relation betweengeneral cognitive level <strong>and</strong> adaptive behavior domainsin individuals with autism with <strong>and</strong> without co-morbidmental retardation. <strong>Child</strong> Psychiatry & HumanDevelopment, 33, 165–172.Borgen, F. H., & Barnett, D. C. (1987). Applying clusteranalysis in counseling psychology research. Journal <strong>of</strong>Counseling Psychology, 34, 456–468.Bornstein, M. R., Bellack, A. S., & Hersen, M. (1977).Social-skills training for unassertive children: A multibaselineanalysis. Journal <strong>of</strong> Applied Behavioral Analysis,10, 183–195.Bower, E. M. (1982). Severe emotional disturbance:Public policy <strong>and</strong> research. Psychology in the Schools,19, 55–60.Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment <strong>and</strong> loss: I. Attachment.New York: Basic Books.Boyd, C. P., Gullone, E., Needleman, G. L., & Burt, T.(1997). The family environment scale: Reliability <strong>and</strong>normative data for an adolescent sample. Family Process,36, 369–373.Braaten, E. (2007). The child clinician’s report writingh<strong>and</strong>book. New York: Guilford.Bracken, B. A., Keith, L. K., & Walker, K. C. (1994).<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> preschool behavior <strong>and</strong> social-emotionalfunctioning: A review <strong>of</strong> thirteen third-partyinstruments. <strong>Assessment</strong> in Rehabilitation <strong>and</strong> Exceptionality,1, 331–346.Bracken, B. A., Keith, L. K., & Walker, K. C. (1998).<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> preschool behavior <strong>and</strong> social-emotionalfunctioning: A review <strong>of</strong> thirteen third-party instruments.Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychoeducational <strong>Assessment</strong>, 16, 153–169.Br<strong>and</strong>t, H. M., & Giebink, J. W. (1968). Concreteness<strong>and</strong> congruence in psychologists’ reports to teachers.Psychology in the Schools, 5, 87–89.Briggs-Gowan, M. J., Carter, A. S., & Schwab-Stone,M. (1996). Discrepancies among mother, child, <strong>and</strong>teacher reports: Examining the contributions <strong>of</strong>maternal depression <strong>and</strong> anxiety. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal<strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 24, 749–765.Brody, G., Stoneman, Z., & Burke, M. (1987). Familysystem <strong>and</strong> individual child correlates <strong>of</strong> sibling behavior.American Journal <strong>of</strong> Orthopsychiatry, 57, 561–569.Brody, G., Stoneman, Z., & Burke, M. (1988). <strong>Child</strong>temperament <strong>and</strong> parental perceptions <strong>of</strong> individualchild adjustment: An intrafamilial analysis. AmericanJournal <strong>of</strong> Orthopsychiatry, 58, 532–542.Brown, R. A. (1999). Assessing attitudes <strong>and</strong> behaviors<strong>of</strong> high-risk adolescents: An evaluation <strong>of</strong> the selfreportmethod. Adolescence, 34, 25–32.Brown, T. E. (2001). Brown attention-deficit disorder scalesfor children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. San Antonio, TX: PsychologicalCorporation.Brubaker, R. G. & Szakowski, A. (2000). Parentingpractices <strong>and</strong> behavior problems among deaf children.<strong>Child</strong> & Family Behavior Therapy, 22, 13–28.Bruininks, R. J., Hill, B. K., Weatherman, R. F., &Woodcock, R. N. (1986). Inventory for client <strong>and</strong> agencyplanning. Allen, TX: DLM Teaching Resources.Bruininks, R. J., Woodcock, R. W., Weatherman, R. F.,& Hill, B. K. (1986). Scales <strong>of</strong> independent behavior.Allen, TX: DLM Teaching Resources.Bruininks, R. H., Woodcock, R. W., Weatherman, R.E., & Hill, B. K. (1996). Scales <strong>of</strong> independent behavior– revised. Itasca, IL: Riverside.Budd, K. S., Heilman, N. E., & Kane, D. (2000). Psychosocialcorrelates <strong>of</strong> child abuse potential in multiplydisadvantaged adolescent mothers. <strong>Child</strong> Abuse &Neglect, 24, 611–625.Burns, G. L., & Patterson, D. R. (1990). Conductproblem behaviors in a stratified r<strong>and</strong>om sample <strong>of</strong>children <strong>and</strong> adolescents: New st<strong>and</strong>ardization dataon the Eyberg child behavior inventory. Psychological<strong>Assessment</strong>: A Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology,2, 391–397.Burns, R. C., & Kaufman, S. H. (1970). Kinetic familydrawings (KF-D): An introduction to underst<strong>and</strong>ingchildren through kinetic drawings. New York: Bruner/Mazel.Buss, A. H. (1995). <strong>Personality</strong>: Temperament, socialbehavior, <strong>and</strong> the self. Boston: Allyn <strong>and</strong> Bacon.Bussing, R., Schumann, E., Belin, T. R., Widawski, M.,& Perwien, A. R. (1998). Diagnostic utility <strong>of</strong> twocommonly used ADHD screening measures amongspecial education students. Journal <strong>of</strong> the AmericanAcademy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 37, 74–82.Butcher, J. N., Cabiyo, J., Lucio, E., & Garrido, M.(2007). Assessing Hispanic clients using the MMPI-2<strong>and</strong> MMPI-A. Washington, DC: American PsychologicalAssociation.Butcher, J. N., Williams, C. L., Graham, J. R., Archer, R. P.,Tellegen, A., Ben-Porath, Y. S., et al. (1992). MIJPI-A,Minnesota multiphasic personality inventory-adolescent:Manual for administration scoring, <strong>and</strong> interpretation.Minneapolis: University <strong>of</strong> Minnesota Press.Cai, X., Kaiser, A., & Hancock, T. (2004). Parent <strong>and</strong>teacher agreement on child behavior checklist itemsin a sample <strong>of</strong> preschoolers from low-income <strong>and</strong>predominantly African American families. Journal <strong>of</strong><strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychology, 33, 303–312.Campbell, J. (2005). Diagnostic assessment <strong>of</strong> Asperger’sdisorder: A review <strong>of</strong> five third-party rating scales.Journal <strong>of</strong> Autism <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disorders, 35,25–35.Campbell, M., Kafantaris, V., Malone, R. P., & Kowalik,S. C. (1991). Diagnostic <strong>and</strong> assessment issuesrelated to pharmacotherapy for children <strong>and</strong> adolescentswith autism. Special issue: Current perspectives


eferenes459in the diagnosis, assessment, <strong>and</strong> treatment <strong>of</strong> child<strong>and</strong> adolescent disorders. Behavior Modification, 15,326–354.Campbell, S. B. (1986). Developmental issues in childhoodanxiety. In R. Gittelman (Ed.), Anxiety disorders<strong>of</strong> childhood (pp. 24–57). New York: Guilford Press.Campbell, S. B. (1990). Behavior problems in preschoolchildren. New York: Guilford Press.Canino, G., & Bravo, M. (1999). The translation <strong>and</strong>adaptation <strong>of</strong> diagnostic instruments for cross-culturaluse. In D. Shaffer <strong>and</strong> C. P. Lucas (Eds.), Diagnosticassessment in child <strong>and</strong> adolescent psychopathology (pp.285–298). New York: Guilford Press.Cantwell, D. P. (1996). Classification <strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong> adolescentpsychopathology. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology <strong>and</strong>Psychiatry, 37, 3–12.Cantwell, D. P., Lewinsohn, P. M., Rohds, P., & Seeley,J. R. (1997). Correspondence between adolescentreport <strong>and</strong> parent report <strong>of</strong> psychiatric diagnostic data.Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong>Psychiatry, 36, 610–619.Capaldi, D. M. (1992). Co-occurrence <strong>of</strong> conduct problems<strong>and</strong> depressive symptoms in early adolescent boys: II. A2-year follow-up at Grade 8. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology,4, 125–144.Cardell, C. P., & Parmer, R. S. (1988). Teacher perception<strong>of</strong> temperament characteristics <strong>of</strong> childrenclassified as learning disabled. Journal <strong>of</strong> Learning Disabilities,21, 497–502.Carlson, C. L., Lahey, B. B., & Neeper, R. (1984). Peerassessment <strong>of</strong> the social behavior <strong>of</strong> accepted, rejected,<strong>and</strong> neglected children. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology,12, 189–198.Caron, C., & Rutter, M. (1991). Comorbidity in childpsychopathology: Concepts, issues, <strong>and</strong> researchstrategies. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry,32, 1063–1080.Carrion, V. G., Weems, C. F., Ray, R., & Reiss, A. L.(2002). Toward an empirical definition <strong>of</strong> pediatricPTSD: The phenomenology <strong>of</strong> PTSD symptoms inyouth. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> & <strong>Adolescent</strong>Psychiatry, 41, 166–173.Carroll, A., Houghton, S., Taylor, M., West, J., & List-Kerz, M. L. (2006). Responses to interpersonal <strong>and</strong>physically provoking situations: The utility <strong>and</strong> application<strong>of</strong> an observation schedule for school-aged studentswith <strong>and</strong> without attention deficit/hyperactivitydisorder. Educational Psychology, 26, 483–498.Carter, C. L., & Dacey, C. M. (1996). Validity <strong>of</strong> theBeck’s depression inventory, MMPI, <strong>and</strong> Rorschachin assessing adolescent depression. Journal <strong>of</strong> Adolescence,19, 223–231.Casat, C., Norton, H., & Boyle-Whitesel, M. (1999).Identification <strong>of</strong> elementary school children at riskfor disruptive behavioral disturbance: Validation <strong>of</strong> acombined screening method. Journal <strong>of</strong> the AmericanAcademy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> & <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 38, 1246–1253.Cassen, E. H. (1990). Depression <strong>and</strong> anxiety secondaryto medical illness. In G. Winokur (Ed.), The psychiatricclinics <strong>of</strong> North America: Anxiety <strong>and</strong> depression assecondary phenomena (pp. 597–612). Philadelphia, PA:W. B. Saunders.Castellanos, F. X., Lee, P. P., Sharp, W., Jeffries, N. O.,Greenstein, D. K., Clasen, L. S., et al. (2002). Developmentaltrajectories <strong>of</strong> brain volume abnormalitiesin children <strong>and</strong> adolescents with attention deficithyperactivity disorder. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American MedicalAssociation, 288, 1740–1748.Cavell, T. A., Meehan, B. T., & Fiala, S. E. (2003).Assessing social competence in children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.In C. R. Reynolds <strong>and</strong> R. W. Kamphaus (Eds.),H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong> educational assessment <strong>of</strong>children (2nd ed., pp. 433–454). New York: Guilford.Chambers, W. J., Puig-Antich, J., Hirsch, M., Paez, P.,Ambrosini, P. J., Tabrizi, M. A., et al. (1985). Theassessment <strong>of</strong> affective disorders in children <strong>and</strong>adolescents by semi-structured interview: Test-retestreliability <strong>of</strong> the schedule for affective disorders <strong>and</strong>schizophrenia for school-aged children, present episode.Archives <strong>of</strong> General Psychiatry, 42, 696–702.Ch<strong>and</strong>ler, L. A. (1990). The projective hypothesis <strong>and</strong>the development <strong>of</strong> projective techniques for children.In C. R. Reynolds & R. W. Kamphaus (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book<strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong> educational assessment <strong>of</strong> children:<strong>Personality</strong>, behavior, & context (pp. 55–69). NewYork: Guilford Press.Chapieski, I., Friedman, A., & Lachar, D. (2000). Psychologicalfunctioning in children <strong>and</strong> adolescentswith Sturge-Weber syndrome. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Neurology,15(10), 660–665.Chilcoat, H. D., & Breslau, N. (1997). Does psychiatrichistory bias mothers’ reports? An application <strong>of</strong> a newanalytic approach. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong><strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 36, 971–979.Christ, M. G. (1990). A four-year longitudinal study <strong>of</strong>depression in boys: Phenomenology <strong>and</strong> comorbidity <strong>of</strong>conduct disorder. Doctoral dissertation: University <strong>of</strong>Georgia, Athens.Christensen, A., Margolin, G., & Sullaway, M. (1992).Inter-parental agreement on child behavior problems.Psychological <strong>Assessment</strong>, 4, 419–425.Cicchetti, D., Lord, C., Koenig, K., Klin, A., & Volkmar,F. (2008). Reliability <strong>of</strong> the ADI-R: Multiple examinersevaluate a single case. Journal <strong>of</strong> Autism <strong>and</strong> DevelopmentalDisorders, 38, 764–770.Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (1998). The development <strong>of</strong>depression in children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. American Psychologist,53, 221–241.Cicchetti, D. & Walker, E. F. (2001). Stress <strong>and</strong> development:Biological <strong>and</strong> psychological consequences.Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 13, 413–418.Claiborn, C. D. (1995). Review <strong>of</strong> the Minnesota multiphasicpersonality inventory – <strong>Adolescent</strong>. In: J. C.Conoley & J. Impara (Eds.), Twelfth mental measurementsyearbook. Lincoln, NE: Burns Institute.


460 referencesClerkin, S. M., Marks, D. J., Policaro, K., & Halperin, J.M. (2007). Psychometric properties <strong>of</strong> the Alabamaparenting questionnaire-preschool revision. Journal <strong>of</strong><strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychology, 36, 19–28.Cohen, N. J., Gotlieb, H., Kershner, J., & Wehrspann,W. (1985). Concurrent validity <strong>of</strong> the internalizing<strong>and</strong> externalizing pr<strong>of</strong>ile patterns <strong>of</strong> the Achenbachchild behavior check-list. Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong><strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 53, 724–728.Cohen, P., & Kasen, S. (1999). The context <strong>of</strong> assessment:Culture, race, <strong>and</strong> socioeconomic status asinfluences on the assessment <strong>of</strong> children. In D. Shaffer,C. P. Lucas, & J. E. Richters (Eds.), Diagnosticassessment in child <strong>and</strong> adolescent psychopathology (pp.299–318). New York: Guilford Press.Coie, J.D. & Dodge, K.A. (1983). Cotinuities <strong>and</strong>changes in children’s social status: A five yearlongitudinal study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 29,261–282.Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Coppotelli, H. (1982).Dimensions <strong>and</strong> types <strong>of</strong> social status: A cross-ageperspective. Developmental Psychology, 18, 557–570.Coie, J.D. & Kupersmidt, J.B. (1983). A behavioral analysis<strong>of</strong> emerging social status in boys’ groups. <strong>Child</strong>Development, 54, 1400–1416.Collett, B., Ohan, J., & Myers, K. (2003). Ten-yearreview <strong>of</strong> rating scales. VI: Scales assessing externalizingbehaviors. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong>& <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 42, 1143–1170.Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (1999).Initial impact <strong>of</strong> the fast track prevention trial forconduct problems: I. The high-risk sample. Journal <strong>of</strong>Consulting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 67, 631–647.Connell, J. P. (1985). A new multidimensional measure <strong>of</strong>children’s perception <strong>of</strong> control. <strong>Child</strong> Development, S6,1018–1041.Conners, C. K. (1970). Symptom patterns in hyperkinetic,neurotic, <strong>and</strong> normal children. <strong>Child</strong> Development,41, 667–682.Conners, C. K. (1995). Conners continuous performancetest. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.Conners, C. K. (1997). Conners rating scales -revised.Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.Conners, C. K. (2008a). Conners (3rd ed.). Toronto:Multi-Health Systems.Conners, C. K. (2008b). Conners comprehensive behaviorrating scales. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.Conners, C. K. (1999). Conners Rating Scales-Revised.In M. E. Maruish (Ed.), The use <strong>of</strong> psychological testingfor treatment planning <strong>and</strong> outcomes assessment (2nded., pp. 467–495). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence ErlbaumAssociates, Inc.Conoley, J. C., & Conoley, C. W. (1991). Collaborationfor child adjustment: Issues for school <strong>and</strong> clinicbasedchild psychologists. Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong><strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, S9, 821–829.Constantino, G., & Malgady, R. G. (1983). Verbal fluency<strong>of</strong> Hispanic, Black, & White children on TAT<strong>and</strong> TEMAS, a new thematic apperception test. HispanicJournal <strong>of</strong> Behavioral Sciences, 5, 199–206.Constantino, G., Malgady, R. G., Rogler, L. H. (1988).Technical manual: TEMAS thematic apperception test.Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.Cooper, D. K. (1990). Developmental trends in children’sdepression inventory responses across age <strong>and</strong> gender.Master’s thesis, University <strong>of</strong> Georgia, Athens.Corbett, J. A., & Trimble, M. R. (1983). Epilepsy <strong>and</strong>anticonvulsant medication. In M. Rutter (Ed.),Developmental neuropsychiatry (pp. 112–132). NewYork: Guilford Press.Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO personalityinventory manual. New York: Psychological <strong>Assessment</strong>Resources.Costello, A. J. (1983). The NIMH diagnostic interviewschedule for children. Pittsburgh, PA: University <strong>of</strong>Pittsburgh.Costa, N. M., & Weems, C. W. (2005). Maternal <strong>and</strong>child anxiety: Do attachment beliefs or children’sperceptions <strong>of</strong> maternal control mediate their association?Social Development, 14, 574–590Costello, A. J., Edelbrock, C. S., Dulcan, M. D., Kalas,R., & Marie, S. H. (1984). Report <strong>of</strong> the NIMH diagnosticinterview schedule for children (DISC). Washington,DC: National Institute <strong>of</strong> Mental Health.Costello, E. J. (1989). <strong>Child</strong> psychiatric disorders <strong>and</strong>their correlates: A primary care pediatric sample.Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong>Psychiatry, 28, 851–888.Costello, E. J., Edelbrock, C. S., & Costello, A. J. (1985).Validity <strong>of</strong> the NIMH diagnostic interview schedulefor children: A comparison between psychiatric <strong>and</strong>pediatric referrals. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology,13, 579–595.Costello, E. J., Egger, H., & Angold, A. (2005). 10-yearresearch update review: The epidemiology <strong>of</strong> child<strong>and</strong> adolescent psychiatric disorders: I. Methods <strong>and</strong>public health burden. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy<strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 44, 972–986.Courchesne, E., Karns, C. M., Davis, B. S., Ziccardi,R., Carper, R. A., Tigue, Z. D., et al. (2001). Unusualbrain growth patterns in early life in patients withautistic disorder: An MRI study. Neurology, 57, 245–254.Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social informationprocessing mechanisms in reactive <strong>and</strong> proactiveaggression. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 67, 993–1002.Crijnen, A. A. M., Achenbach, T. M., & Verhulst, F. C.(1997). Comparisons <strong>of</strong> problems reported by parents<strong>of</strong> children in 12 cultures: Total problems, externalizing,<strong>and</strong> internalizing. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy<strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 36, 1269–1277.Crijnen, A. A. M., Achenbach, T. M., & Verhulst, F. C.(1999). Problems reported by parents <strong>of</strong> children inmultiple cultures: The child behavior checklist syndromeconstructs. American Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry, 156,569–574.


eferenes461Crnic, K. A., & Reid, M. (1989). Mental retardation.In E. J. Mash & R. A. Barkley (Eds.), Treatment <strong>of</strong>childhood disorders (pp. 247–346). New York: GuilfordPress.Crooks, C. V. & Wolfe, D. A. (2004). <strong>Child</strong> abuse<strong>and</strong> neglect. In E. J. Mash & R. A. Barkleyl (Eds.),<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> childhood disorders (4th ed., pp. 639–684). New York: Guilford Press.Cuellar, L., Harris, I. C., & Jasso, R. (1980). An acculturationscale for Mexican American normal <strong>and</strong>clinical population. Hispanic Journal <strong>of</strong> Behavioral Science,2, 199–217.Cumella, E. J., Wall, A. D., & Kerr-Almcida, N. (1999).MMPI-A in the inpatient assessment <strong>of</strong> adolescentswith eating disorders. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>,73, 31–44.Current Population Survey. (2001). Washington, DC:U.S. Census Bureau.Cummings, E. M., Keller, P. S., & Davies, P. T. (2005).Towards a family process model <strong>of</strong> maternal <strong>and</strong> paternaldepressive symptoms: Exploring multiple relationswith child <strong>and</strong> family functioning. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology<strong>and</strong> Psychiatry, 46, 479–489.Cummings, J. A. (1986). Projective drawings. In H. M.Knoll (Ed.), The assessment <strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong> adolescent personality(pp. 199–244). New York: Guilford Press.Cunningham, C. E. & Boyle, M. H. (2002). Preschoolersat risk for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder<strong>and</strong> oppositional defiant disorder: Family, parenting,<strong>and</strong> behavioral correlates. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong>Psychology, 30, 555–569.Curry, J. F., & Craighead, W. E. (1993). Depression. InT. H. Ollendick & M. Hersen (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong>child <strong>and</strong> adolescent assessment (pp. 251–268) Boston,MA: Allyn <strong>and</strong> Bacon.Cytryn, L., & McKnew, D. H. (1974). Factors influencingthe changing clinical expression <strong>of</strong> the depressiveprocess in children. American Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry,131, 879–881.Dadds, M. R., Barrett, P. M., Rapee, R. M., & Ryan, S.(1996). Family process <strong>and</strong> child anxiety <strong>and</strong> aggression:An observational analysis. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal<strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 24, 715–734.Dadds, M. R., Maujean, A., & Fraser, J. A. (2003). Parenting<strong>and</strong> conduct problems in children: Australi<strong>and</strong>ata <strong>and</strong> psychometric properties <strong>of</strong> the Alabamaparenting questionnaire. Australian Psychologist, 38,238–241.Dadds, M. R., & S<strong>and</strong>ers, M. R. (1992). Family interaction<strong>and</strong> child psychopathology: A comparison <strong>of</strong>two observation strategies. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> FamilyStudies, 1, 371–391.Dahlstrom, W. G. (1993). Small samples, large consequences.American Psychologist, 48, 393–399.Dalby, J. T. (1985). Taxonomic separation <strong>of</strong> attentiondeficit disorders. Contemporary Educational Psychology,10, 228–234.Dalton, J. E. (1996). Juvenile male sex <strong>of</strong>fenders: Meanscores <strong>and</strong> the BASC self-report <strong>of</strong> personality. PsychologicalReports, 79, 634.Dana, R. H. (1993). Multicultural assessment perspectivesfor pr<strong>of</strong>essional psychology. Boston, MA: Allyn <strong>and</strong>Bacon.Dana, R. H. (2000). H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> cross-cultural <strong>and</strong> multiculturalpersonality assessment. Mahwah, NJ: LawrenceErlbaum.Danforth, J. S., & DuPaul, G. J. (1996). Interraterreliability <strong>of</strong> teacher rating scales for children withattention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychopathology<strong>and</strong> Behavioral <strong>Assessment</strong>, 18, 227–238.Daniel, M. H. (1992, August). Item-level covariancestructure analysis for constructing personality/behavioralscales. Paper presented at the annual meeting <strong>of</strong> theAmerican Psychological Association, Washington,DC.Daniel, M. H. (1993, April). Diagnostic specificity <strong>of</strong> parents’vs. teachers’ behavior ratings. Paper presented atthe Annual Meeting <strong>of</strong> the National Association <strong>of</strong>School Psychologists, Washington, DC.Daniel, M. H. (1993, August). Diagnostic specificity <strong>of</strong>parents’ <strong>and</strong> teachers’ behavior ratings. Paper presentedat the annual meeting <strong>of</strong> the American PsychologicalAssociation, Washington, DC.Darling, N., & Steinberg, L. (1993). Parenting style ascontext: An integrative model. Psychological Bulletin,113, 487–496.David, C. F., & Kistner, J. A. (2000). Do positive selfperceptionshave a “dark side”? Examination <strong>of</strong> thelink between perceptual bias <strong>and</strong> aggression. Journal<strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 28, 327–337.David-Ferdon, C., & Kaslow, N. J. (2008). Evidencebasedpsychosocial treatments for child <strong>and</strong> adolescentdepression.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong>Psychology, 37, 62–104.de Bildt, A., Sytema, S., Ketelaars, C., Kraijer, D., Mulder,E., Volkmar, F., et al. (2004). Interrelationshipsbetween the ADOS, ADI-R, <strong>and</strong> DSM-IV-TR classificationin children <strong>and</strong> adolescents with mentalretardation. Journal <strong>of</strong> Autism <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disorders,34, 129–137.De Groot, A., Koot, H. M., & Verhulst, F. C. (1996).Cross-cultural generalizability <strong>of</strong> the youth self-report<strong>and</strong> teacher’s report form cross-informant syndromes.Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 24, 651–664.De Los Reyes, A. & Kazdin, A. E. (2005). Informantdiscrepancies in the assessment <strong>of</strong> childhood psychopathology:A critical review, theoretical framework,<strong>and</strong> recommendations for further study. PsychologicalBulletin, 131, 483–509.Deater-Deckard, K., Reiss, D., & Hetherington, F.M. (1997). Dimensions <strong>and</strong> disorders <strong>of</strong> adolescentadjustment: A quantitative genetic analysis <strong>of</strong> unselectedsamples <strong>and</strong> selected extremes. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong>Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry, 38, 515–525.


462 referencesDeBaryshe, B. D., Patterson, G. R., & Capaldi, D. M.(1993). A performance model for academic achievementin early adolescent boys. Developmental Psychology,29, 795–804.Department <strong>of</strong> Health <strong>and</strong> Human Services. (2000).St<strong>and</strong>ards for privacy <strong>of</strong> individually identifiable healthinformation. Federal Register, 65(250), 82462–82829.Departments <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry <strong>and</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychiatry, TheInstitute <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry, & The Maudsley HospitalLondon (1987). Psychiatric examination: Notes oneliciting <strong>and</strong> recording clinical in-formation psychiatricparents. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Desombre, H., Malvy, J., Roux, S., de Villard, R., Sauvage,D., Dalery, J., et al. (2006). Autism <strong>and</strong> developmentaldelay: A comparative clinical study in veryyoung children using IBSE scale. European <strong>Child</strong> &<strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 15, 343–351.DeStefano, L. (1998). Review <strong>of</strong> the <strong>Personality</strong> Inventoryfor Youth. Thirteenth Mental Measurements Yearbook.Lincoln, NE: Buros Institute.DeStefano, L., & Thompson, D. S. (1990). Adaptivebehavior: The construct <strong>and</strong> its measurement. In C.R. Reynolds & R. W. Kamphaus (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book<strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong> educational assessment <strong>of</strong> children:<strong>Personality</strong>, behavior, <strong>and</strong> context (pp. 445–469). NewYork: Guilford Press.Dishion, T. J., Patterson, G. R., & Skinner, M. S. (1989).A process model for the role <strong>of</strong> peers in adolescent socialadjustment. Paper presented at the biennial meeting<strong>of</strong> the Society for Research in <strong>Child</strong> Development,Kansas City, MO.Dishion, T. J., Capaldi, D., Spracklen, K. M., & Li, F.(1995). Peer ecology <strong>of</strong> male adolescent drug use.Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 7, 803–824.DiSibio, M. (1993). Conjoint effects <strong>of</strong> intelligence <strong>and</strong>adaptive behavior on achievement in a nonreferredsample. Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychoeducational <strong>Assessment</strong>, 11,304–313.Dodge, K. A. (1983). Behavioral antecedents <strong>of</strong> peersocial crams. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 54, 1386–1399.Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social-informationprocessing factors in reactive <strong>and</strong> proactive aggression inchildren’s peer groups. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology,53, 1146–1158.Dodge, K. A., Coie, J., Brakkc, N. (1982). Behavior patterns<strong>of</strong> socially rejected <strong>and</strong> neglected adolescents:The roles <strong>of</strong> social approach <strong>and</strong> aggression. Journal <strong>of</strong>Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 10, 389–410.Dodge, K. A., McClaskey, C. L., & Feldman, E. (1985).Situational approach to the assessment <strong>of</strong> social competencein children. Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong>Psychology, 53, 344–353.Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (2003). A biopsychosocialmodel <strong>of</strong> the development <strong>of</strong> chronic conduct problemsin adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 39, 349–371.Doleys, D. M. (1977). Behavioral treatments for nocturnalenuresis in children: A review <strong>of</strong> the recent literature.Psychological Bulletin, 84, 30–54.Doll, B. (1998). Review <strong>of</strong> the <strong>Child</strong> Behavior Checklist.Thirteenth Mental Measurements Yearbook. Lincoln,NE: Buros Institute.Doll, E. A. (1940). A social basis <strong>of</strong> mental diagnosis.Journal <strong>of</strong> Applied Psychology, 24, 160–169.Doll, E. A. (1953). Measurement <strong>of</strong> social competence: Amanual for the Vinel<strong>and</strong> Social Maturity Scale. CirclePines, MN: American Guidance Services.Dopke, C. A., Lundahl, B. W., Dunsterville, E., & Lovejoy,M. C. (2003). Interpretations <strong>of</strong> child compliancein individuals at high- <strong>and</strong> low-risk for child physicalabuse. <strong>Child</strong> Abuse & Neglect, 27, 285–302.Downey, G., & Coyne, J. C. (1990). <strong>Child</strong>ren <strong>of</strong>depressed parents: An integrative review. PsychologicalBulletin, 108, 50–76.Doyle, A., Ostr<strong>and</strong>er, R., Skare, S., Crosby, R. D., &August, C. (1997). Convergent <strong>and</strong> criterion-relatedvalidity <strong>of</strong> the behavior assessment system for children-parentrating scales. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong>Psychology, 26, 276–284.Dresden, J. (1994). Gender, temperament, <strong>and</strong> mathematicsachievement. Doctoral dissertation, University <strong>of</strong>Georgia, Athens.Drotar, D., Stein, R. E. K., & Perrin, E. C. (1995).Methodological issues in using the child behaviorchecklist <strong>and</strong> its related instruments in clinical childpsychology research. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology,24, 184–192.DuBois, P. H. (1970). A history <strong>of</strong> psychological testing.Boston, MA: Allyn <strong>and</strong> Bacon.Duhig, A., Renk, K., Epstein, M., & Phares, V. (2000).Interparental agreement on internalizing, externalizing,<strong>and</strong> total behavior problems: A meta-analysis.<strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology: Science <strong>and</strong> Practice, 7, 435–453.Durbin, C., Hayden, E., Klein, D., & Olino, T. (2007).Stability <strong>of</strong> laboratory-assessed temperamental emotionalitytraits from ages 3 to 7. Emotion, 7, 388–399.Dunbar, J. L. (1999). Differential item performance bygender on the externalizing scales <strong>of</strong> the behaviorassessment system for children. Dissertation AbstractsInternational Section A: Humanities <strong>and</strong> Social Sciences,60(6-A), 1902.DuPaul, G. J., Anastopolous, A. D., Shelton, T. L.,Guevremont, I. C., & Metevia, L. (1992). Multimethodassessment <strong>of</strong> attention-deficit hyperactivitydisorder: The diagnostic utility <strong>of</strong> clinic-based tests.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 21, 394 402.DuPaul, G. J., Power, T. J., Anastopolous, A. D., & Reid,R. (1998). ADHD-IV rating scale: Checklists, norms,<strong>and</strong> clinical interpretation. New York: Guilford.DuPaul, G. J., Rapport, M. D., & Perrillo, L. M. (1991).Teacher ratings <strong>of</strong> academic skills: The development<strong>of</strong> the academic performance rating scale. School PsychologyReview, 20, 284–300.DuPaul, G.J. & Stoner, G. (1994). ADHD in theschools: <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>and</strong> intervention strategies.New York: Guilford.


eferenes463Eaves, L. J., Silberg, J. L., Hewitt, J. K., Rutter, M.,Meyer, J. M., Neale, M. C., et al. (1993). Analyzingtwin resemblance in multisymptom data: Geneticapplications <strong>of</strong> a latent class model for symptoms <strong>of</strong>conduct disorder in juvenile boys. Behavior Genetics,23, 5–19.Eaves, R. C., & Milner, B. (1993). The criterion-relatedvalidity <strong>of</strong> the childhood autism rating scale <strong>and</strong> theautism behavior checklist. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong>Psychology, 21, 481–491.Edelbrock, C. (1979). Empirical classification <strong>of</strong> children’sbehavior disorders: Progress based on parent <strong>and</strong>teacher ratings. School Psychology Digest, 8, 355–369.Edelbrock, C. (1985). Age differences in the reliability<strong>of</strong> the psychiatric interview <strong>of</strong> the child. <strong>Child</strong> Development,56, 265–275.Edelbrock, C., & Costello, A. J. (1988). Convergencebetween statistically derived behavior problem syndromes<strong>and</strong> child psychiatric diagnoses. Journal <strong>of</strong>Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 16, 219–231.Edelbrock, C., Costello, A. J., Dulcan, M. K., Conover,N. C., & Kalas, R. (1986). Parent child agreement onchild psychiatric symptoms assessed via structuredinterview. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry,27, 181–190.Edelbrock, C., Costello, A. J., Dulcan, M. K., Kalas, R., &Conover, N. C. (1985). Age differences in the reliability<strong>of</strong> the psychiatric interview <strong>of</strong> the child. <strong>Child</strong> Development,56, 265–275.Edelbrock, C., Crnic, K., & Bohnert, A. (1999). Interviewingas communication: An alternative way <strong>of</strong>administering the diagnostic interview schedule forchildren. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 27,447–453.Edelsohn, G., Ialongo, N., Werthamer-Larrson, L.Crockett, L., & Kellam, S. (1992). Self-reporteddepressive symptoms in first-grade children: Developmentallytransient phenomena? Journal <strong>of</strong> theAmerican Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry,31, 282–290.Education for All H<strong>and</strong>icapped <strong>Child</strong>ren’s Act <strong>of</strong> 1975,P.L. 94–142, 20 U.S.C. Section 1401 (1975).Eiraldi, R. B., Power, T. J., Karustis, J. L., & Goldstein, S.G. (2000). Assessing ADHD <strong>and</strong> comorbid disordersin children: The <strong>Child</strong> Behaviour Checklist <strong>and</strong> theDevereaux Scales <strong>of</strong> Mental Disorders. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong><strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 29, 3–16.Eisenstadt, T. H., Eyberg, S., McNeil, C. B., Newcomb,K., & Funderbunk, B. (1993). Parent-child interactiontherapy with behavior problem children: Relativeeffectiveness <strong>of</strong> two stages <strong>and</strong> overall treatment outcome.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 22, 42–51.Elgar, F. J., Waschbusch, D. A., Dadds, M. R., & Sivaldason,N. (2007). Development <strong>and</strong> validation <strong>of</strong> ashort form <strong>of</strong> the Alabama parenting questionnaire.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> Family Studies, 16, 243–259.Elliott, S., & Bretzing, B. (1980). Using <strong>and</strong> updatinglocal norms. Psychology in the Schools, 17, 196–201.Elliot, D. S., Huizinga, D., & Ageton, S. S. (1985).Explaining delinquency <strong>and</strong> drug use. Newbury Park,CA: Sage.Emery, R. E. (1982). Interparental conflict <strong>and</strong> the children<strong>of</strong> discord <strong>and</strong> divorce. Psychological Bulletin, 92,310–330.Endicott, J., & Spitzer, R. L. (1978). A diagnostic interview:The schedule for affective disorders <strong>and</strong> schizophrenia.Archives <strong>of</strong> General Psychiatry, 35, 57–63.Erickson, M. T. (1998). Etiological factors. In T. H.Ollendick & M. Hersen (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong>Psychopathology (3rd ed., pp. 37–62). New York: PlenumPress.Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity, youth, <strong>and</strong> crisis. New York:Norton.Ernst, M., Cookus, B. A., & Moravec, B. C. (2000). Pictorialinstrument for children <strong>and</strong> adolescents (PICA-III-R). Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong><strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 39, 94–99.Essau, C. A., Sasagawa, S., & Frick, P. J. (2006). Psychometricproperties <strong>of</strong> the Alabama parenting questionnaire.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> Family Studies, 15,597–616.Essau, C. A., Sasagawa, S., & Frick, P. J. (2006). Callousunemotionaltraits in a community sample <strong>of</strong> adolescents.<strong>Assessment</strong>, 13, 454–469.Eyberg, S. M., Nelson, M. M., Duke, M., & Boggs, S.R. (2005). Manual for the dyadic parent-child interactioncoding system (3rd ed.). Available on-line at www.PCIT.org.Exner, J. E. (1974). The Rorschach: A comprehensive system,I. New York: Wiley.Exner, J. E. (1978). The Rorschach: A comprehensive system,II: Recent research <strong>and</strong> advanced interpretation.New York: Wiley.Exner, J. E. (1983, June). The depression index. 1983Alumni Newsletter (p. 7). Bayville, NY: RorschachWorkshops.Exner, J. E. (1990, April). The depression index. 1990Alumni Newsletter (pp. 12–13). Asheville, NC: RorschachWorkshops.Exner, J. E., & Martin, L. S. (1983). The Rorschach: Ahistory <strong>and</strong> description <strong>of</strong> the comprehensive system.School Psychology Review, 12, 407–413.Exner, J. E., Jr., & Weiner, I. B. (1982). The Rorschach: Acomprehensive system, Vol. 3: <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong>adolescents. New York: Wiley.Exner, J. E., & Weiner, I. B. (1994). The Rorschach: Acomprehensive system, Vol. 3: <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong>adolescents (2nd ed.). New York: John Wiley <strong>and</strong>Sons.Exner, J. E., Thomas, E. A., & Mason, B. (1985). <strong>Child</strong>ren’sRorschachs: Description <strong>and</strong> prediction. Journal<strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>, 49, 13–20.Eyberg, S. M., & Robinson, E. A. (1982). Conductproblem behavior: St<strong>and</strong>ardization <strong>of</strong> a behavioralrating scale with adolescents. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong>Psychology, 12, 347–354.


464 referencesEyberg, S. M., Bessmer, J., Newcomb, K., Edwards, D.,& Robin-son, E. (1994). Dyadic parent-child interactioncoding system 11: A manual. Unpublished manuscript,University <strong>of</strong> Florida.Eyberg, S. M., & Robinson, E. A. (1983). Dyadic parent-childinteraction coding system: A manual. PsychologicalDocuments, 13, 24 (Ms. No., 2582).Eyberg, S. M., & Ross, A. W. (1978). <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> childbehavior problems: The validation <strong>of</strong> a new inventory.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 7, 113–116.Eyde, L. D., Robertson, G. J., Krug, S. E., Morel<strong>and</strong>, K.L., Robertson, A. G., Shewan, C. M., et al. (1993).Responsible test use: Case studies for assessing humanbehavior. Washington, DC: American PsychologicalAssociation.Eysenck, H. J. (1965). Review <strong>of</strong> the Wittenborn PsychiatricRating Scales. In O. K. Buros (Ed.), MentalMeasurements Yearbook (5th ed.). Highl<strong>and</strong> Park, NJ:Gryphon Press.Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., Keenan, K., & Tsuang, M.T. (1991). Separation <strong>of</strong> DSM-III attention deficitdisorder <strong>and</strong> conduct disorder: Evidence from a familygenetic study <strong>of</strong> American child psychiatric patients.Psychological Medicine, 21, 109–121.Federal Register. (March 12, 1999). Assistance to statesfor the education <strong>of</strong> children with disabilities <strong>and</strong> theEarly Intervention Program for infants <strong>and</strong> toddlerswith disabilities; Final Regulations.Feighner, J. P., Robins, E., Guze, S. B., Woodruff, R. A.,Winokur, G., & Munoz, R. (1972). Diagnostic criteriafor use in psychiatric research. Archives <strong>of</strong> GeneralPsychiatry, 26, 57–63.Feinberg, T. L., Crouse-Novak, M. A., Paulauskas, S.L., & Finkelstein, R. (1984). Depressive disordersin childhood: I. A longitudinal prospective study <strong>of</strong>characteristics <strong>and</strong> recovery. Archives <strong>of</strong> General Psychiatry,41, 229–237.Feinfield, K. A. & Baker, B. L. (2004). Empirical supportfor a treatment program for families <strong>of</strong> young childrenwith externalizing problems. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong>& <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychology, 33, 182–195.Ferdin<strong>and</strong>, R. (2008). Validity <strong>of</strong> the CBCL/YSRDSM-IV scales anxiety problems <strong>and</strong> affective problems.Journal <strong>of</strong> Anxiety Disorders, 22, 126–134.Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, J. (1995). Predictivevalidity <strong>of</strong> categorically <strong>and</strong> dimensionally scoredmeasures <strong>of</strong> disruptive childhood behaviors. Journal<strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychology, 32,396–407.Fergusson, D. M., Swain, N. R., & Horwood, L. J.(2002). Deviant peer affiliations, crime <strong>and</strong> substanceuse: A fixed effects regression analysis. Journal <strong>of</strong>Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 30, 419–430.Figueroa, R. A. (1990). <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> linguistic minoritygroup children. In C. R. Reynolds & R. W. Kamphaus(Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong> educationalassessment <strong>of</strong> children: Intelligence <strong>and</strong> achievement (pp.671–696). New York: Guilford Press.Filipek, P. A., Accardo, P. J., Ashwal, S., Baranek, G. T.,Cook, E. H., Jr., Dawson, G., et al. (2000). Practiceparameter: Screening <strong>and</strong> diagnosis <strong>of</strong> autism. Neurology,55, 468–479.Finch, A. J., & Belter, R. W. (1993). Projective techniques.In T. H. Ollendick & M. Hersen (Eds.),H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong> adolescent assessment (pp. 229–238). Boston, LH : Allyn <strong>and</strong> Bacon.Finch, A. J., Lipovsky, J. A., & Casat, C. D. (1989).Anxiety <strong>and</strong> depression in children <strong>and</strong> adolescents:Negative affectivity or separate constructs? In P. C.Kendall & D. Watson (Eds.), Anxiety <strong>and</strong> depression:Distinctive <strong>and</strong> overlapping features (pp. 171–202).New York: Academic Press.First, M. B., Spitzer, R. I., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B.W. (1995). Structure clinical interview for Axis I DSM-IV Disorders – Patient edition. New York: New YorkState Psychiatric Institute.Fischer, M., Barkley, R. A., Fletcher, K., & Smallish, L.(1993). The adolescent outcome <strong>of</strong> hyperactive childrendiagnosed by research criteria: V. Predictors <strong>of</strong>outcome. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong><strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 32, 324–332.Fisher, L., & Blair, R. J. R. (1998). Cognitive impairment<strong>and</strong> its relationship to psychopathic tendenciesin children with emotional <strong>and</strong> behavioral difficulties.Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 26, 511–519.Fisher, P., Wicks, J., Shaffer, D., Piacentini, J., & Laplun,J. (1992). NIMH diagnostic interview schedule for children:Users manual. New York: New York State PsychiatricInstitute.Flanagan, D. P., Alfonso, V. C., Primavera, L. H., Povall,L., & Higgins, D. (1996). Convergent validity <strong>of</strong> theBASC <strong>and</strong> SSRS: Implications for social skills assessment.Psychology in the Schools, 33, 13–23.Flanagan, R. (1995). A review <strong>of</strong> the behavior assessmentsystem for children (BASC): <strong>Assessment</strong> consistentwith the requirements <strong>of</strong> the individuals withdisabilities education act (IDEA). Journal <strong>of</strong> SchoolPsychology, 33, 177–186.Flanagan, R., & DiGiuseppe, R. (1999). Critical review<strong>of</strong> the TEMAS: A step within the development <strong>of</strong>thematic apperception instruments. Psychology in theSchools, 36, 21–30.Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Morris, R. D., & Lyon, G.R. (2005). Evidence-based assessment <strong>of</strong> learning disabilitiesin children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong><strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychology, 34, 506–522.Foley, D. L., Rutter, M., Agnold, A., Pickles, A., Maes,H. M., Silberg, J. L., et al. (2005). Making sense <strong>of</strong>informant disagreement for overanxious disorder.Journal <strong>of</strong> Anxiety Disorders, 19, 193–210.Fombonne, F. (1992). Diagnostic assessment in a sample<strong>of</strong> autistic <strong>and</strong> developmentally impaired adolescents.Special issue: Classification <strong>and</strong> diagnosis. Journal <strong>of</strong>Autism & Developmental Disorders, 22, 563–581.Forbey, J. D., H<strong>and</strong>el, R. W., & Ben-Porath, Y. S.(2000). A real-data simulation <strong>of</strong> computerized adap-


eferenes465tive administration <strong>of</strong> the MMPI-A. Computers inHuman Behavior, 16, 83–96.Foreh<strong>and</strong>, R., Long, N., & Hedrick, M. (1987). Familycharacteristics <strong>of</strong> adolescents who display overt <strong>and</strong>covert behavior problems. Journal <strong>of</strong> Behavior Therapy<strong>and</strong> Experimental Psychiatry, 18, 325–328.Forness, S. R., & Kritzer, J. (1992). A new proposed definition<strong>and</strong> terminology to replace “serious emotionaldisturbance” in the individuals with disabilities act. SchoolPsychology Review, 21, 12–20.Forth, A. E., Kosson, D. S., & Hare, R. D. (2003). Harepsychopathy checklist: Youth version. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.Fowler, P. (1982). Factor structure <strong>of</strong> the family environmentscale: Effects <strong>of</strong> social desirability. Journal <strong>of</strong><strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 38, 285–292.Francis, D. J., Fletcher, J. M., Sniebing, K. K., Davidson, K. C.,& Thompson, N. M. (1991). Analysis <strong>of</strong> change: Modelingindividual growth. Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology,59, 27–37.Franco, J. N. (1983). An acculturation scale for Mexican–Americanchildren. Journal <strong>of</strong> General Psychology,108, 175–181.Frazier, T. W., Youngstrom, E. A., Kubu, C. S., Sinclair, L.,& Rezai, A. (2008). Exploratory <strong>and</strong> confirmatory factoranalysis <strong>of</strong> the autism diagnostic interview-revised. Journal<strong>of</strong> Autism <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disorders, 38, 474–480.Freeman, B. J., Ritvo, E. R., Yokota, A., & Rirvo, A.(1986). A scale for rating symptoms <strong>of</strong> patients withthe syndrome <strong>of</strong> autism in real life settings. Journal <strong>of</strong>the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychiatry, 25, 130–136.French, J. L., & Hale, R. L. (1990). A history <strong>of</strong> thedevelopment <strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong> educational testing.In C. R. Reynolds & R. W. Kamphaus (Eds.),H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong> educational assessment <strong>of</strong>children: Intelligence <strong>and</strong> achievement (pp. 3–28). NewYork: Guilford Press.Freud, S. (1936). The Problem <strong>of</strong> anxiety. New York: W.W. Norton & Co.Frick, P. J. (1993). <strong>Child</strong>hood conduct problems in familycontext. School Psychology Review, 22, 376–385.Frick, P. J. (1994). Family dysfunction <strong>and</strong> the disruptivebehavior disorders: A review <strong>of</strong> recent empirical findings.In T. H. Ollendick & R. J. Prinz (Eds.), Advancesin clinical child psychology (Vol. 16, pp. 213–226). NewYork: Plenum.Frick, P. J. (1998). Conduct disorders <strong>and</strong> severe antisocialbehavior. New York: Plenum.Frick, P. J. (2000). Laboratory <strong>and</strong> performance-basedmeasures <strong>of</strong> childhood disorders. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong><strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 29, 475–478.Frick, P. J. (2004). Integrating research on temperament<strong>and</strong> childhood psychopathology. Its pitfalls <strong>and</strong> promise.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychology,33, 2–7.Frick, P. J. (2006). Developmental pathways to conductdisorder. <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatric Clinics <strong>of</strong> NorthAmerica, 15, 311–331.Frick, P. J., Bodin, S. D., & Barry, C. T. (2000). Psychopathictraits <strong>and</strong> conduct problems in community <strong>and</strong>clinic-referred samples <strong>of</strong> children: Further development<strong>of</strong> the Psychopathy Screening Device. <strong>Assessment</strong>,12, 382–393.Frick, P. J., Christian, R. C. & Wootton, J. M. (1999).Age trends in the association between parenting practices<strong>and</strong> conduct problems. Behavior Modification, 23,106–128.Frick, P. J., & Dickens, C. (2006). Current perspectiveson conduct disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports, 8,59–72.Frick, P. J. & Hare, R. D. (2001). The antisocial processscreening device. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.Frick, P. J., & Jackson, Y. K. (1993). Family functioning<strong>and</strong> childhood antisocial behavior. Yet another reinterpretation.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 22,410–419.Frick, P. J., Kamphaus, R. W., Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R.,Christ, M. A. G., Hart, E. L., et al. (1991). Academicunderachievement <strong>and</strong> the disruptive behavior disorders.Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 59,289–294.Frick, P. J., Kimonis, E. R., D<strong>and</strong>reaux, D. M., & Farell,J. M. (2003). The 4 year stability <strong>of</strong> psychopathic traitsin non-referred youth. Behavioral Sciences & the Law,21(6), 713–736.Frick, P. J. & Kimonis, E. (2008). Externalizing disorders. InJ. E. Maddux & B. A. Winstead (Eds.), Psychopathology:Contemporary issues, theory, <strong>and</strong> research (pp. 349–374).Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Frick, P. J., & Lahey, B. B. (1991). The nature <strong>and</strong> characteristics<strong>of</strong> attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.School Psychology Review, 20, 163–173.Frick, P. J., Lahey, B. B., Applegate, B., Kerdyck, L.,Ollendick, T., Ilynd, G. W., et al. (1994). DSM-IVfield trials for the disruptive behavior disorders. Journal<strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong>Psychiatry, 33, 529–539.Frick, P. J., Lahey, B. B., Christ, M. G., Loeber, R., & Green,S. (1991). History <strong>of</strong> childhood behavior problems in biologicalrelatives <strong>of</strong> boys with attention-deficit-hyperactivitydisorder <strong>and</strong> conduct disorder. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong>Psychology, 20, 445–451.Frick, P. J., Lahey, B. B., Hartdagen, S., & Hynd, C.W. (1989). Conduct problems in boys: Relations tomaternal personality, marital satisfaction, <strong>and</strong> socioeconomicstatus. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology,18, 114–120.Frick, P. J., Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Stouthamer-Loeber,M., Christ, M. G., & Hanson, K. (1992). Familial riskfactors to oppositional defiant disorder <strong>and</strong> conductdisorder: Parental psychopathology <strong>and</strong> maternal parenting.Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology,60, 49–55.Frick, P. J., Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Tannenbaum, L. E.,Van Horn, Y., Christ, M. A. C., et al. (1993). Oppositionaldefiant disorder <strong>and</strong> conduct disorder: A meta-


466 referencesanalytic review <strong>of</strong> factor analyses <strong>and</strong> cross-validation ina clinic sample. <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology Review, 13, 319–340.Frick, P. J. & Loney, B. R. (1999). Outcomes <strong>of</strong> children<strong>and</strong> adolescents with conduct disorder <strong>and</strong> oppositionaldefiant disorder. In H. C. Quay & A. Hogan (Eds.),H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> disruptive behavior disorders (pp. 507–524).New York: Plenum Press.Frick, P. J. & Loney, B. R. (2000). The use <strong>of</strong> laboratory<strong>and</strong> performance-based measures in the assessment <strong>of</strong>children <strong>and</strong> adolescents with conduct disorders. Journal<strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 29, 540–554.Frick, P. J. & Loney, B. R. (2002). Underst<strong>and</strong>ing theassociation between parent <strong>and</strong> child antisocial behavior.In R. J. McMahon & R. Dcv. Peters (Eds.), Theeffects <strong>of</strong> parental dysfunction on children (pp. 105–126).New York: Plenum Press.Frick, P. J., & Marsee, M. A. (2006). Psychopathic traits<strong>and</strong> developmental pathways to antisocial behavior inyouth. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> psychopathictraits (pp. 355–374). New York: Guilford Press.Frick, P. J., & McMahon, R. J. (2008). <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> adolescentconduct problems. In J. Hunsley & E. J. Mash(Eds.), A guide to assessments that work (pp. 41–66). NewYork: Oxford University.Frick, P. J., & Morris, A. S. (2004). Temperament <strong>and</strong>developmental pathways to conduct problems. Journal<strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychology, 33, 54–68.Frick, P. J., & O’Brien, B. S. (1994). Conduct disorders.In R. T. Ammerman & M. Hersen (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book<strong>of</strong> child behaviour therapy in the psychiatric setting (pp.199–216). New York: Wiley.Frick, P. J., & Silverthorn, P. (2001). Psychopathologyin children. In H. E. Adams & P. B. Sutker (Eds.),Comprehensive h<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> psychopathology (3rd ed., pp.881–920). New York: Kluwer-Plenum.Frick, P. J., Silverthorn, P., & Evans, C. S. (1994).<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> childhood anxiety using structuredinterviews: Patterns <strong>of</strong> agreement among informants<strong>and</strong> association with maternal anxiety. Psychological<strong>Assessment</strong>, 6, 372–379.Frick, P. J. & White, S. F. (2008). The importance <strong>of</strong>callous-unemotional traits for the development <strong>of</strong>aggressive <strong>and</strong> antisocial behavior. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong>Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry, 49, 359–375.Friedl<strong>and</strong>er, S., Weiss, D. S., & Traylor, J. (1986). Assessingthe influence <strong>of</strong> maternal depression on the validity <strong>of</strong> thechild behavior checklist. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology,14, 123–133.Friedman, S. L., & Waches, T. D. (1999). Measuringenvironment across the life span: Emerging methods <strong>and</strong>concepts. Washington, DC: American PsychologicalAssociation.Friman, P. C., H<strong>and</strong>werk, M. L., Smith, G. L., Larzelere,R. E., Lucas, C. P., & Shaffer, D. M. (2000).External validity <strong>of</strong> conduct <strong>and</strong> oppositional defiantdisorders determined by the NIMH diagnostic interviewschedule for children. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong>Psychology, 28, 277–286.Fuchs, D. (1987). Examiner familiarity effects on testperformance: Implications for training <strong>and</strong> practice.Topics in Early <strong>Child</strong>hood Special Education, 102,90–104.Fuchs, D., & Fuchs, L. S. (1986). Test procedure bias: Ameta-analysis <strong>of</strong> examiner familiarity effects. Review<strong>of</strong> Educational Research, 56, 243–262.Funderbunk, B. W., & Eyberg, S. M. (1989). Psychometriccharacteristics <strong>of</strong> the Sutter-Eyberg student behaviorinventory: A school behavior rating scale for use withpreschool children. Behavioral <strong>Assessment</strong>, 11, 297–313.Gadow, K. D., & Sprafkin, J. (1997). Early <strong>Child</strong>hoodInventory-4: Screening manual. Stony Brook, NY:Checkmate Plus.Gadow, K. D., & Sprafkin, J. (1998). <strong>Adolescent</strong> SymptomsInventory-4 norms manual. Stony Brook, NY:Checkmate Plus.Gadow, K. D., & Sprafkin, J. (2002). <strong>Child</strong> Symptom Inventory-4screening <strong>and</strong> norms manual. Stony Brook, NY:Checkmate Plus.Gadow, K. D., Sprafkin, J., Salisbury, H., Schneider, J.,& Loney, J. (2004). Further validity evidence for theteacher version <strong>of</strong> the child symptom inventory-4.School Psychology Quarterly, 19, 50–71.Gallucci, N. T. (1989). <strong>Personality</strong> assessment withchildren <strong>of</strong> superior intelligence: Divergence versuspsychopathology. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>, 53,749–760.Gallucci, N. T. (1997). On the identification <strong>of</strong> patterns<strong>of</strong> substance abuse with the MMPI-A scales. Psychological<strong>Assessment</strong>, 9(3), 224–232.Galton, F. (1884). Measurement <strong>of</strong> character. FortnightlyReview, 42, 179–185. Reprinted in L. D. Goodstein &R. I. Lanyon (Eds.), Readings in <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>,New York: Wiley.Ganmer, A. B., Graham, J. R., & Archer, R. A. (1992).Usefulness <strong>of</strong> the MAC scale in differentiating adolescentsin normal, psychiatric, <strong>and</strong> substance abusesettings. Psychological <strong>Assessment</strong>, 4, 133–137.Garb, H. N., Wood, J. M., & Nezworski, M. T. (2000).Projective techniques <strong>and</strong> the detection <strong>of</strong> child sexualabuse. <strong>Child</strong> Maltreatment, 5, 161–168.Garber, J. (1984). Classification <strong>of</strong> childhood psychopathology:A developmental perspective. <strong>Child</strong> Development,55, 30–48.Garcia, M., & Lega, L. I. (1979). Development <strong>of</strong> aCuban ethnic identity questionnaire. Hispanic Journal<strong>of</strong> Behavioral Sciences, 1, 247–261.Geisinger, K. F. (1992). Fairness <strong>and</strong> selected psychometricissues in the psychological testing <strong>of</strong> Hispanics.In K. F. Geisinger (Ed.), Psychological testing <strong>of</strong>Hispanics (pp. 17–42). Washington, DC: AmericanPsychological Association.Gelf<strong>and</strong>, D. M., & Hartmann, D. P. (1984). <strong>Child</strong>Behavior analysis <strong>and</strong> therapy (2nd ed.). New York:Pergamon Press.Geller, B., Fox, L. W., & Fletcher, M. (1993). Effect<strong>of</strong> tricyclic antidepressants on switching to mania


eferenes467<strong>and</strong> on the onset <strong>of</strong> bipolarity in depressed 6- to12-year-olds. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong><strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 32, 43–50.Gelman, C. F. (1998). Characteristics <strong>of</strong> a prenatallycocaine-exposed clinical population at school age.Dissertation Abstracts International Section B: The Sciences<strong>and</strong> Engineering, 58(11-B), 6234.Ghiselli, E. E., Campbell, J. P., & Zedeck, S. (1981) Measurementtheory for the behavioral sciences. San Francisco:W. H. Freeman.Gibbs, J. T. & Huang, L. N. (1989). <strong>Child</strong>ren <strong>of</strong> color: Psychologicalinterventions with minority youth. San Francisco:Jossey-Bass, Inc.Gilliam, J. E. (1995). Gilliam autism rating scale. Austin,TX: Pro-Ed.Gilliam, J. E. (2001). Gilliam Asperger’s Disorder scale.Austin, Texas: Pro-Ed.Gilliam, J. E. (2008). Gilliam autism rating scale, 2nd edition.Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Gittelman, R., Mannuzza, S., Shenker, R., & Bonegura,N. (1985). Hyperactive boys almost grown up:I. Psychiatric status. Archives <strong>of</strong> General Psychiatry, 42,937–947.Gittelman-Klein, R. (1986). Questioning the clinicalusefulness <strong>of</strong> projective psychological tests forchildren. Developmental <strong>and</strong> Behavioral Pediatrics, 7,378–382.Glueck, S., & Glueck, E. (1968). Delinquents <strong>and</strong> nondelinquentsin perspective. Cambridge, MA: HarvardUniversity Press.Glutting, J. J., & Kaplan, D. (1990). Stanford-Binet IntelligenceScale-Fourth Edition: Making the case forreasonable interpretations. In C. R. Reynolds & R. W.Kamphaus (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong> educationalassessment <strong>of</strong> children: <strong>Personality</strong>, behavior, <strong>and</strong> context(pp. 277–295). New York: Guilford Press.Goldberg, L. R. (1992). The development <strong>of</strong> markers forthe big-five factor structure. Psychological <strong>Assessment</strong>,4, 26–42.Goldsmith, H. H., & Rieser-Danner, L. A. (1990).Assessing early temperament. In C. R. Reynolds <strong>and</strong>R. W. Kamphaus (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong>educational assessment <strong>of</strong> children: <strong>Personality</strong>, behavior,<strong>and</strong> context (pp. 245–278). New York: The GuilfordPress.Goodman, S. H. & Gotlib, I. H. (1999). Risk for psychopathologyin the children <strong>of</strong> depressed mothers: Adevelopmental model for underst<strong>and</strong>ing mechanisms<strong>of</strong> transmission. Psychological Review, 106, 458–490.Goodman, W. K., Price, L. H., Rasmussen, S. A.,Mazure, C., Fleischmann, R. L., Hill, C. L., et al.(1989). The Yale–Brown obsessive–compulsive scale:I. development, use, <strong>and</strong> reliability. Archives <strong>of</strong> GeneralPsychiatry, 46, 1006–1011.Gordon, M. (1983). The Gordon diagnostic system. DeWitt,NY: Gordon Systems.Gorsuch, R. L. (1988). Factor analysis (3rd ed.). Hillsdale,NJ: Erlbaum.Gottesman, I. I., McGuffin, P., & Farmer, A. E. (1987).<strong>Clinical</strong> genetics as clues to the “real” genetics <strong>of</strong>schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 13, 23–47.Gottlieb, G. (1991). Experiential canalization <strong>of</strong> behavioraldevelopment: Theory. Developmental Psychology,27(1), 4–13.Green, K., Vosk, B., Foreh<strong>and</strong>, R., & Beck, S. (1981).An examination <strong>of</strong> differences among sociometricallyidentified accepted, rejected, <strong>and</strong> neglected children.<strong>Child</strong> Study Journal, 11, 117–124.Green, M. (1992). Pediatric diagnosis: Interpretation <strong>of</strong>symptoms <strong>and</strong> signs in infants, children, <strong>and</strong> adolescents(5th ed.). Philadelphia: Saunders.Green, S. M., Loeber, R., & Lahey, B. B. (1991). Stability<strong>of</strong> mothers’ recall <strong>of</strong> the age <strong>of</strong> onset <strong>of</strong> their child’sattention <strong>and</strong> hyperactivity problems. Journal <strong>of</strong> theAmerican Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry,30, 135–137.Greenberg, M. T., Lengua, L. J., Coie, J., Pinderhughes,E., & the Conduct Problems Prevention ResearchGroup. (1999). Predicting developmental outcomesat school entry using a multiple-risk model: FourAmerican communities. Developmental Psychology, 35,403–417.Greenspan, S. (2006). Functional concepts in mentalretardation: Finding the natural essence <strong>of</strong> an artificialcategory. Exceptionality, 14, 205–224.Greenspoon, P. J., & Sakl<strong>of</strong>ske, D. H. (1997). Validity<strong>and</strong> reliability <strong>of</strong> the multidimensional students’ lifesatisfaction scale with Canadian children. Journal <strong>of</strong>Psychoeducational <strong>Assessment</strong>, 15, 138–155.Gresham, F. M., & Little, S. G. (1993). Peer-referencedassessment strategies. In T. H. Ollendick & M. Hersen(Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong> adolescent assessment (pp.165–179). Boston, MA: Allyn <strong>and</strong> Bacon.Gresham, R. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1990). Social skillsrating system. Circle Pines, MN: American GuidanceService.Grieten, H., De Haene, L., & Uyttebroek, K. (2007).Cross-cultural validation <strong>of</strong> the child abuse potentialinventory in Belgium (Fl<strong>and</strong>ers): Relations with demographiccharacteristics <strong>and</strong> parenting problems. Journal<strong>of</strong> Family Violence, 22, 223–229.Grills, A. E., & Ollendick, T. H. (2003). Multiple informantagreement <strong>and</strong> the Anxiety disorders interviewschedule for parents <strong>and</strong> children. Journal <strong>of</strong> theAmerican Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> & <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 42,30–40.Gutterman, E. M., O’Brien, J. D., & Young, J. G. (1987).Structured diagnostic interviews for children <strong>and</strong> adolescents:Current status <strong>and</strong> future directions. Journal<strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry,26, 621–630.Haak, R. A. (1990). Using the sentence completion toassess emotional disturbance. In C. R. Reynolds & R.W. Kamphaus (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong>educational assessment <strong>of</strong> children: <strong>Personality</strong>, behavior,<strong>and</strong> context (pp. 147–167). New York: Guilford Press.


468 referencesHaak, R. A. (2003). Using the sentence completion toassess emotional disturbance. In C. R. Reynolds & R.W. Kamphaus (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong>educational assessment <strong>of</strong> children: <strong>Personality</strong>, behavior,<strong>and</strong> context-2nd edition (pp. 159–181). New York:Guilford.Hammen, C., Rudolph, K., Weisz, J., Rao, U., & Burge,D. (1999). The context <strong>of</strong> depression in clinic-referredyouth: Neglected areas in treatment. Journal <strong>of</strong> theAmerican Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> & <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 38,64–71.Hampson, S., Andrews, J., Barckley, M., & Peterson, M.(2007). Trait stability <strong>and</strong> continuity in childhood:Relating sociability <strong>and</strong> hostility to the five-factormodel <strong>of</strong> personality. Journal <strong>of</strong> Research in <strong>Personality</strong>,41, 507–523.H<strong>and</strong>, C., Archer, R., H<strong>and</strong>el, R., & Forbey, J. (2007).The classification accuracy <strong>of</strong> the Minnesota multiphasicpersonality inventory-adolescent: Effects<strong>of</strong> modifying the normative sample. <strong>Assessment</strong>, 14,80–85.Hankin, B. L., Grant, K. E., Cheeley, C., Wetter,E., Farahm<strong>and</strong>, F. K., & Westerholm, R. I. (2008).Depressive disorders. In M. Hersen (Series Ed.) & D.Reitman (Vol. Ed.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> psychological assessment,case conceptualization, <strong>and</strong> treatment (pp. 199–230). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Hankin, B. L., Mermelstein, R., & Roesch, L. (2007).Sex differences in adolescent depression: Stress exposure<strong>and</strong> reactivity. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 78, 279–295.Harrison, P. L., & Oakl<strong>and</strong>, T. (2003). Adaptive Behavior<strong>Assessment</strong> System (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: ThePsychological Corporation.Hayes, S. C. (2005). Diagnosing intellectual disabilityin a forensic sample: Gender <strong>and</strong> age effects on therelationship between cognitive <strong>and</strong> adaptive functioning.Journal <strong>of</strong> Intellectual & Developmental Disability,30, 97–103.Hayes, S. C., Nelson, R. O., & Jarrett, R. B. (1987). Thetreatment utility <strong>of</strong> assessment: A functional approachto evaluating assessment quality. American Psychologist,42, 963–974.Hays, S., & McCallum, R. (2005). A comparison <strong>of</strong> thepencil-<strong>and</strong>-paper <strong>and</strong> computer-administered Minnesotamultiphasic personality inventory-adolescent. Psychologyin the Schools, 42, 605–613.Haynes, S.N., & O’Brien, W.H. (1988). The Gordianknot <strong>of</strong> DSM-III use: Integrating principles <strong>of</strong> behaviorclassification <strong>and</strong> complex causal models. Behavioral<strong>Assessment</strong>, 10, 95–105.Hawes, D. J. & Dadds, M. R. (2006). Assessing parentingpractices through parent-report <strong>and</strong> direct observationduring parenting-training. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong>Family Studies, 15, 555–568.Holaday, M. (2000). Rorschach protocols from children<strong>and</strong> adolescents diagnosed with posttraumaticstress disorder. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>, 75,143–157.Haley, G. M. T., Fine, S., Marriage, K., Moretti, M. M.,& Freeman, R. J. (1985). Cognitive bias <strong>and</strong> depressionin psychiatrically disturbed children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 53,535–537.Hall, C. W., & Haws, H. D. (1989). Depressive symptomatologyin learning-disabled <strong>and</strong> nonlearning-disabledstudents. Psychology in the Schools, 26, 359–364.Hamilton, C., Fuchs, D., Fuchs, L. S., & Roberts, H.(2000). Rates <strong>of</strong> classroom participation <strong>and</strong> thevalidity <strong>of</strong> sociometry. School Psychology Review, 29,251–266.Hammen, C., Brennan, P. A., Keenan-Miller, D., & Herr,N. R. (2008). Early onset recurrent subtype <strong>of</strong> adolescentdepression: <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>and</strong> psychosocial correlates.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry, 49, 443–440.Hammen, C., Gordon, G., Burge, D., Adrian, C.,Jaenicke, C., & Hiroto, G. (1987). Maternal affectivedisorders, illness, <strong>and</strong> stress: Risk for children’spsychopathology. American Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry, 144,736–741.Hammer, E. F. (1958). The clinical application <strong>of</strong> projectivedrawings. Springfield, IL: Thomas.H<strong>and</strong>finger, A. S. (2000). The effects <strong>of</strong> behavior problemson the development <strong>of</strong> adaptive skills in children. A dissertationsubmitted to the graduate faculty <strong>of</strong> TheUniversity <strong>of</strong> Georgia.H<strong>and</strong>werk, M. L., Friman, P. C., & Larzelere, R. (2000).Comparing the DISC <strong>and</strong> the youth self-report. Journal<strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong>Psychiatry, 39, 807–808.H<strong>and</strong>werk, M. L., Larzelere, R. E., Soper, S. H., & Friman,P. C. (1999). Parent <strong>and</strong> child discrepancies inreporting severity <strong>of</strong> problem behaviors in three out<strong>of</strong>-homesettings. Psychological <strong>Assessment</strong>, 11, 14–23.Harrington, R. G., & Follett, G. M. (1984). The readability<strong>of</strong> child personality assessment instruments.Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychoeducational <strong>Assessment</strong>, 2, 37–48.Harrington, R. G., Fudge, H., Rutter, M., Pickles, A., &Hill, J. (1991). Adult outcomes <strong>of</strong> childhood <strong>and</strong> adolescentdepression: II. Links with antisocial disorders.Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong>Psychiatry, 30, 434–439.Harris, F. C., & Lahey, B. B. (1982a). Recording systembias in direct observational methodology: A review<strong>and</strong> critical analysis <strong>of</strong> factors causing inaccurate codingbehavior. <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology Review, 2, 539–556.Harris, F. C., & Lahey, B. B. (1982b). Subject reactivityin direct observational assessment: A review <strong>and</strong> criticalanalysis. <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology Review, 2, 523–538.Harrison, P. L. (1990). <strong>Assessment</strong> with the Vinel<strong>and</strong>Adaptive Behavior Scales. In C. R. Reynolds & R. W.Kamphaus (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong> educationalassessment <strong>of</strong> children: <strong>Personality</strong>, behavior, <strong>and</strong>context. New York: Guilford Press.Harrison, P. L., & Oakl<strong>and</strong>, T. (2000). Adaptive Behavior<strong>Assessment</strong> System manual. San Antonio, TX: The PsychologicalCorporation.


eferenes469Hart, D. H. (1972). The Hart sentence completion test forchildren. Salt Lake City: Educational Support Systems.Hart, D. H. (1986).The sentence completion techniques.In H. M. Kn<strong>of</strong>f (Ed.), The assessment <strong>of</strong> child<strong>and</strong> adolescent personality (pp. 245–272). New York:Guilford Press.Hart, D. H., Kehle, T. J., & Davies, M. V. (1983). Effectiveness<strong>of</strong> sentence completion techniques: A review<strong>of</strong> the hart sentence completion test for children.School Psychology Review, 12, 428–434.Hart, E. L., & Lahey, B. B. (1999). General child behaviorrating scales. In D. Shaffer <strong>and</strong> C. P. Lucas (Eds.),Diagnostic assessment in child <strong>and</strong> adolescent psychopathology(pp. 65–87). New York: Guilford Press.Hartley, M. M. M. (1999). The relationship among disruptivebehaviors, attention, <strong>and</strong> academic achievementin a clinic referral sample. Dissertation AbstractsInternational Section A: Humanities <strong>and</strong> Social Sciences,60(2-A), 0333.Hartmann, D. P., Roper, B. L., & Bradford, D. C. (1979).Some relationships between behavioral <strong>and</strong> traditionalassessment. Journal <strong>of</strong> Behavioral <strong>Assessment</strong>, 1, 3–21.Harvey, V. S. (1989, March). Eschew obfuscation: Support<strong>of</strong> clear writing. Communique, p. 12.Hasegawa, C. (1989). The unmentioned minority. In C.J. Maker & L. W. Schiever (Eds.), Defensive programsfor cultural <strong>and</strong> ethnic minorities (Vol. 2). Austin, TX:Pro-Ed.Haskett, M. E., Ahern, L. S., Ward, C. S., & Allaire, J.C. (2006). Factor structure <strong>and</strong> validity <strong>of</strong> the parentingstress index-short form. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong><strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychology, 35, 302–312.Haskett, M. E., Scott, S. S. & Fann, K. D. (1995).<strong>Child</strong> abuse potential inventory <strong>and</strong> parentingbehavior: Relationships with high-risk correlates.<strong>Child</strong> Abuse & Neglect, 19, 1483–1495.Hathaway, S. R., & McKinley, J. C. (1940). A multiphasicpersonality schedule (Minnesota): I. Construction<strong>of</strong> the schedule. Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychology, 10, 249–254.Hathaway, S. R., & McKinley, J. C. (1942). MinnesotaMultiphasic <strong>Personality</strong> Inventory. Minneapolis: University<strong>of</strong> Minnesota Press.Hathaway, S. R., & McKinley, J. C. (1989). MMPI-2:Minnesota Multiphasic <strong>Personality</strong> Inventory – SecondEdition. Minneapolis: University <strong>of</strong> Minnesota Press.Hayvren, M., & Hymnel, S. (1984). Ethical issues insociometric testing: Impact <strong>of</strong> sociometric measureson interaction behavior. Developmental Psychology, 20,849–884.Haz, A. M., & Ramirez, V. (1998). Preliminary validation<strong>of</strong> the child abuse potential inventory in Chile.<strong>Child</strong> Abuse & Neglect, 22, 869–879.Heber, R. (1959). A manual on terminology <strong>and</strong> classificationin mental retardation (2nd ed.). (Monographsupplement). American Journal <strong>of</strong> Mental Deficiency.Heber, R., & Helzer, J. E. (1988). Schizophrenia: Epidemiology.In R. Michels, J. O. Cavenar, A. M. Cooper,S. B. Guze, L. L. Judd, G. L. Merman, & A. J. Solnit(Eds.), Psychiatry. New York: Lippincott.Helms, J. E. (1986). Exp<strong>and</strong>ing racial identity theory tocover counseling process. Journal <strong>of</strong> Counseling Psychology,33, 62–64.Helzer, J.E. & Pryzbeck, T.T. (1988). The co-occurrence<strong>of</strong> alcoholism with other psychiatric disorders in thegeneral population <strong>and</strong> its impact on treatment. Journal<strong>of</strong> Studies on Alcohol, 49, 2129–224.Helzer, J.E. & Robins, L.N. (1988). The DiagnosticInterview Schedule: Its development, evolution, <strong>and</strong>use. Social Psychiatry <strong>and</strong> Psychiatric Epidemiology,23, 6–16.Hembree-Kigin, T. L., & McNeil, C. B. (1995). Parentchildinteraction therapy. New York: Plenum Press.Hendren, R. L., DeBacker, I., & P<strong>and</strong>ina, G. J. (2000).Review <strong>of</strong> neuroimaging studies <strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong> adolescentpsychiatric disorders from the past 10 years.Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong>Psychiatry, 39, 815–828.Henning-Stout, M. (1998). Assessing the behavior <strong>of</strong>girls: What we see <strong>and</strong> what we miss. Journal <strong>of</strong> SchoolPsychology, 36, 433–455.Hicks, M. M., Rogers, R., & Cashel, M. (2000). Predictions<strong>of</strong> violent <strong>and</strong> total infractions amonginstitutionalized male juvenile <strong>of</strong>fenders. Journal <strong>of</strong>the American Academy <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry <strong>and</strong> the Law, 28,183–190.Hill, A. B., Kemp-Wheeler, S. M. & Jones, S. A. (1986).What does the Beck depression inventory measurein students? <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Individual Differences, 7,39–47.Hiller, J. B., Rosenthal, R., Bornstein, R. F., Berry, D.T. R., & Brunell-Neulieb, S. (1999). A comparativemeta-analysis <strong>of</strong> Rorschach <strong>and</strong> MMPI validity. Psychological<strong>Assessment</strong>, 11, 278–296.Hinshaw, S. P. (1987). On the distinction between attentionaldeficits/hyperactivity <strong>and</strong> conduct problems/aggression in child psychopathology. PsychologicalBulletin, 101, 443–463.Hinshaw, S. P. (1991). Stimulant medications <strong>and</strong> theirtreatment <strong>of</strong> aggression in children with attention deficits.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 20, 301–312.Hinshaw, S. P. (1992). Externalizing behavior problems<strong>and</strong> academic underachievement in childhood <strong>and</strong>adolescence: Causal relationships <strong>and</strong> underlyingmechanism. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 127–155.Hodges, K. (1994). <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> adolescent functional assessmentscale. Ypsilanti, MI: Eastern Michigan University.Hodges, K., Cool, J., & McKnew, D. (1989). Testretestreliability <strong>of</strong> a clinical research interview forchildren: The child assessment schedule (CAS). Psychological<strong>Assessment</strong>, 1, 317–322.Hodges, K., Doucette-Gates, A., & Liao, Q. (1999).The relationship between the child <strong>and</strong> adolescentfunctional assessment scale (CAFAS) <strong>and</strong> indicators<strong>of</strong> functioning. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> Family Studies, 8,109–122.


470 referencesHodges, K., & Kim, C. (2000). Psychometric study <strong>of</strong>the child <strong>and</strong> adolescent functional assessment scale:Prediction <strong>of</strong> con-tact with the law <strong>and</strong> poor schoolattendance. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 28,287–297.Hodges, K., & Saunders, W. (1989). Internal consistency<strong>of</strong> a diagnostic interview for children: The childassessment schedule. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology,17, 691–701.Hodges, K., & Wong, M. M. (1996). Psychometriccharacteristics <strong>of</strong> a multidimensional measure toassess impairment: The child <strong>and</strong> adolescent functionalassessment scale. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> FamilyStudies, 5, 445–467.Hodges, K., & Zeman, J. (1993). Interviewing. In T. H.Ollendick & M. Hersen (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong>adolescent assessment (pp. 65–81). Boston, MA: Allyn<strong>and</strong> Bacon.Hojnoski, R. L., Morrison, R., Brown, M., & Matthews, W.J. (2006). Projective test use among school psychologists:A survey <strong>and</strong> critique. Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychoeducational <strong>Assessment</strong>,24, 145–159.Holaday, M., Smith, D. A., & Sherry, A. (2000). Sentencecompletion tests: A survey <strong>of</strong> the literature <strong>and</strong>results <strong>of</strong> a survey <strong>of</strong> members <strong>of</strong> the Society for <strong>Personality</strong><strong>Assessment</strong>. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>,74, 371–383.Holden, E. W., & Banez, G. A. (1996). <strong>Child</strong> abusepotential <strong>and</strong> parenting stress within maltreatingfamilies. Journal <strong>of</strong> Family Violence, 11, 1–12.Holtzman, W. H., & Swartz, J. D. (2003). Use <strong>of</strong> theHoltzman Inkblot Technique (HIT) with children. InC. R. Reynolds & R. W. Kamphaus (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book<strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong> educational assessment in children:<strong>Personality</strong>, behavior, <strong>and</strong> context (2nd ed., pp. 198–2218). New York: Guilford.Howlin, P. (2003). Outcome in high-functioningadults with autism with <strong>and</strong> without early languagedelays: Implications for the differentiation betweenautism <strong>and</strong> Asperger syndrome. Journal <strong>of</strong> Autism<strong>and</strong> Developmental Disorders, 33, 3–14.Hoza, B., Gerders, A. C., Mrug, S., Hinshaw, S. P.,Bukowski, W. M., Gold, J. A., et al. (2005). Peer–assessed outcomes in the multimodal treatmentstudy <strong>of</strong> children with attention deficit hyperactivitydisorder. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychology,34, 74–86.Huberty, C. J., DiStefano, C. & Kamphaus, R. W.(1997). Behavioral clustering <strong>of</strong> school children. MultivariateBehavioral Research, 32, 105–143.Huber, C. J., Kamphaus, R. W., & DiStefano, C. (1997).Behavioral clustering <strong>of</strong> school children. MultivariateBehavioral Research, 32, 105–134.Hudziak, J. J., Heath, A. C., Madden, P. F., Reich, W.,Bucholz, K. K., Slutske, W., et al. (1998). Latent class<strong>and</strong> factor analysis <strong>of</strong> DSM-IV ADHD: A twin study<strong>of</strong> female adolescents. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy<strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 37, 848–857.Hudziak, J. J., Wadsworth, B. A., Heath, A. C., &Achenbach, T. M. (1999). Latent class analysis <strong>of</strong>child behavior checklist attention problems. Journal <strong>of</strong>the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry,38, 985–991.Huesmann, L. R., Eron, L. D., Lefkowitz, M. M., &Walder, L. O. (1984). Stability <strong>of</strong> aggression overtime <strong>and</strong> generations. Developmental Psychology, 20,1120–1134.Hughes, J. (1990). <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> social skills: Sociometric<strong>and</strong> behavioral approaches. In C. R. Reynolds &R. W. Kamphaus (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong>educational assessment <strong>of</strong> children: <strong>Personality</strong>, behavior,<strong>and</strong> context (pp. 423–444). New York: Guilford Press.Hughes, T. L., Gacono, C. B., & Owen, P. F. (2007).Current status <strong>of</strong> Rorschach assessment: Implicationsfor the school psychologist. Psychology in the Schools,44, 281–291.Hughes, J. N., Meehan, B. T., & Cavell, T. A. (2004).Development <strong>and</strong> validation <strong>of</strong> a gender-balancedmeasure <strong>of</strong> aggression-relevant social cognition.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychology, 33,292–302.Hunsley, J. & Bailey, J. M. (2001). Whither the Rorschach?An analysis <strong>of</strong> the evidence. Psychological<strong>Assessment</strong>, 13, 472–485.Hunsley, J. & DiGiulio, G. (2001). Norms, norming, <strong>and</strong>clinical assessment. <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology: Science <strong>and</strong> Practice,8, 378–382.Hutcheson, J. J. & Black, M. M. (1996). Psychometricproperties <strong>of</strong> the Parenting Stress Index in a sample<strong>of</strong> low-income African-American mother <strong>of</strong> infants<strong>and</strong> toddlers. Early Education <strong>and</strong> Development, 7,381–400.Huth-Bocks, A., Kerr, D., Ivey, A., Kramer, A., & King,C. (2007). <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> psychiatrically hospitalizedsuicidal adolescents: Self-report instruments as predictors<strong>of</strong> suicidal thoughts <strong>and</strong> behavior. Journal <strong>of</strong>the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> & <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry,46, 387–395.Hutt, M. L. (1980). The Michigan picture test – revised.New York: Grune & Stratton.Hynd, G. W., Hern, K. L., Novey, E. S., Eliopulos, D.,Marshall, R., Gonzales, J. J., et al. (1993). Attentiondeficithyperactivity disorder <strong>and</strong> asymmetry <strong>of</strong> thecaudate nucleus. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Neurology, 8, 339–347.Hynd, G. W., & Willis, W. G. (1988). Pediatric neuropsychology.Boston, MA: Allyn <strong>and</strong> Bacon.Inclan, J. E., & Herron, D. G. (1985). Puerto Ricanadolescents. In G. T. Gibbs & L. N. Huang (Eds.),<strong>Child</strong>ren <strong>of</strong> color (pp. 251–277). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Ivanova, M. Y., Achenbach, T. M., Rescorla, L. A.,Dumenci, L., Almqvist, F., Bilenberg, N., et al.(2007). The generalizability <strong>of</strong> the youth self-reportsyndrome structure in 23 societies. Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting<strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 75, 729–738.


eferenes471Jackson, M. F., Barth, J. M., Powell, N., Lochman, J.E. (2006). Classroom contextual effects <strong>of</strong> race onchildren’s peer nominations. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 77,1325–1337.James, E. M., Reynolds, C. R., & Dunbar, J. (1994).Self-report instruments. In T. H. Ollendick & N. J.King, et al. (Eds.), International h<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> phobic<strong>and</strong> anxiety disorders in children <strong>and</strong> adolescents: Issuesin clinical child psychology (pp. 317–329). New York:Plenum Press.Janosz, M., Le Blanc, M., Boulerice, B., & Tremblay,R. E. (2000). Predicting different types <strong>of</strong> schooldropouts: A typological approach with two longitudinalsamples. Journal <strong>of</strong> Educational Psychology, 92,171–190.Janus, M. D., de Groot, C., & Toepfer, S. M. (1998). TheMMPI-A <strong>and</strong> 13-year-old inpatients: How young istoo young? <strong>Assessment</strong>, 5, 321–332.Jensen, P. S. (2003). Comorbidity <strong>and</strong> child psychopathology:Recommendations for the next decade. Journal<strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 31, 293–300.Jensen, P. S., Kettle, L., Roper, M. T., Sloan, M. T., Dulcan,M. K., Hoven, C., et al. (1999). Are stimulantsoverprescribed? Treatment <strong>of</strong> ADHD in four U.S.communities. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong><strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 38, 797–804.Jensen, P. S., Rubio-Stipec, M., Canino, G., Bird, H. R.,Dulcan, M. K., Schwab-Stone, M. E., et al. (1999).Parent <strong>and</strong> child contributions to diagnosis <strong>of</strong> mentaldisorder: Are both informants always necessary?Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> & <strong>Adolescent</strong>Psychiatry, 38, 1569–1579.Jensen, P. S., Watanabe, H. E., & Richters, J. E. (1999).Who’s up first? Testing for order effects in structuredinterviews using a counterbalanced experimental design.Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 27, 439–446.Jersild, A. T., & Holmes, R. (1935). <strong>Child</strong>ren’s fears(<strong>Child</strong> Development Monograph No. 20). New York:Columbia University Press.Jesness, C. F. (1988). Jesness inventory <strong>of</strong> adolescent personality.North Tonaw<strong>and</strong>a, NY: Multi-Health Systems.Jiang, X. L. & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2005). Stability <strong>of</strong>continuous measures <strong>of</strong> sociometric status: A metaanalysis.Developmental Review, 25, 1–25.Johnson, S. B. (1988). Diabetes mellitus in childhood. InD. Routh (Ed.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> pediatric psychology (pp.9–31). New York: Guilford Press.Johnston, C. (1996). Parent charecteristics <strong>and</strong> parentchildinteractions in families <strong>of</strong> nonproblem children<strong>and</strong> ADHD children with higher <strong>and</strong> lower levels <strong>of</strong>oppositional-defiant behaviour. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal<strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 24, 85–104.Johnston, C. & Mash, E. J. (2001). Families <strong>of</strong> childrenwith attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Review<strong>and</strong> recommendations for future research. <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong><strong>and</strong> Family Review, 4, 183–207.Johnston, C., Pelham, W. E., & Murphy, H. A. (1985).Peer relationships in ADHD <strong>and</strong> normal children: Adevelopmental analysis <strong>of</strong> peer <strong>and</strong> teacher ratings.Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 13, 89–100.Joiner, T. E., Schmidt, K. L., & Barnett, J. (1996). Size,detail, <strong>and</strong> line heaviness in children’s drawings ascorrelates <strong>of</strong> emotional distress: (More) negative evidence.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>, 67, 127–141.Joint Committee on Testing Practices (2004). Code <strong>of</strong>Fair Testing Practices in Education. Washington, DC:American Psychological Association.Jonsen, A. R., Siegler, M., & Winslade, W. J. (1992).<strong>Clinical</strong> ethics (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.Jung, C. J. (1910). The association method. AmericanJournal <strong>of</strong> Psychology, 21, 219–235. Reprinted in L. D.Goodstein & R. I. Lanyon (Eds.), Readings in personalityassessment. New York: Wiley.Kachigan, S. K. (1982). Multivariate statistical analysis: Aconceptual introduction. New York: Radus Press.Kaczmarek, T., Hagan, M., & Kettler, R. (2006). Screeningfor suicide among juvenile delinquents: Reliability<strong>and</strong> validity evidence for the suicide screening inventory(SSI). International Journal <strong>of</strong> Offender Therapy <strong>and</strong> ComparativeCriminology, 50, 204–217.Kagan, J., & Snidman, N. (1991). Temperamental factorsin human development. American Psychologist, 46,856–862.Kamphaus, R. W. (1987). Conceptual <strong>and</strong> psychometricissues in the assessment <strong>of</strong> adaptive behavior. Journal<strong>of</strong> Special Education, 21, 27–36.Kamphaus, R. W. (1998). Intelligence test interpretation:Acting in the absence <strong>of</strong> evidence. In A. Prifitera & D.Sakl<strong>of</strong>ske (Eds.), WISC-III <strong>Clinical</strong> Use <strong>and</strong> interpretation:Scientist-practitioner perspectives (pp. 39–57). SanDiego: Academic Press.Kamphaus, R. W. (2001a). <strong>Clinical</strong> assessment <strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong>adolescent intelligence. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn& Bacon.Kamphaus, R. W. (2001b). <strong>Clinical</strong> assessment <strong>of</strong> children’sintelligence (2nd ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn <strong>and</strong> Bacon.Kamphaus, R. W., & Frick, P. J. (1996). <strong>Clinical</strong> assessment<strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong> adolescent personality <strong>and</strong> behavior.Boston: Allyn <strong>and</strong> Bacon.Kamphaus, R.W., & Campbell, J.C. (2006). Psychodiagnosticassessment <strong>of</strong> children: dimensional <strong>and</strong> categoricalmethods. New York, NY: Wiley.Kamphaus, R. W., Huberty, C. J., DiStefano, C., &Petoskey, M. D. (1997). A typology <strong>of</strong> teacher ratedchild behavior for a national U. S. sample. Journal <strong>of</strong>Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 25, 453–463.Kamphaus, R. W., Jimenez, M. E., Pineda, D. A.,Rowe, E. W., Fleckenstein, L., Restrepo, M. A., etal. (2000). Analisis transcultural de un instrumento dedimensiones multiples en el diagnostico del deficit deatencidn. Revista de Neuropsicologia, Neuropsygiatria yNeurociencias, 2, 51–63.Kamphaus, R. W., Morgan, A. W., Cox, M. R., & Powell,R. M. (1995). <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> intelligence in thepsychodiagnostic process. In D. H. Sakl<strong>of</strong>ske & M.Zeidner (Eds.), International h<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> personality


472 references<strong>and</strong> intelligence (pp. 525–544). New York: PlenumPress.Kamphaus, R. W., Petoskey, M. D., Cody, A. H., Rowe,E. W., Huberty, C. J., & Reynolds, C. R. (1999a). Atypology <strong>of</strong> parent-rated child behavior for a nationalU. S. sample. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry<strong>and</strong> Allied Disciplines, 40, 1–10.Kamphaus, R. W., Petoskey, M. D., Cody, A. H., Rowe,E. W., Huberty, C. J., & Reynolds, C. R. (1999b). Atypology <strong>of</strong> parent-rated child behavior for a nationalU.S. sample. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry<strong>and</strong> Allied Disciplines, 40, 607–616.Kamphaus, R. W., Petoskey, M. D., & Rowe, E. W.(2000). Current trends in psychological testing <strong>of</strong>children. Pr<strong>of</strong>essional Psychology: Research <strong>and</strong> Practice,31, 155–164.Kamphaus, R. W., & Reynolds, C. R. (1998). Behavior<strong>Assessment</strong> System for <strong>Child</strong>ren (BASC) ADHD Monitor.Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Kamphaus, R. W., Reynolds, C. R., & Imperato-McCammon, C. (1998). Roles <strong>of</strong> diagnosis <strong>and</strong> classificationin the schools. In T. B. Gutkin & C. R.Reynolds (Eds.), The H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> School Psychology (3rded., pp. 292–316). New York: Wiley.K<strong>and</strong>el, D. B., & Davies, M. (1988). Adult sequelae <strong>of</strong>adolescent depressive symptoms. Archives <strong>of</strong> GeneralPsychiatry, 43, 255–262.Karver, M. S. (2006). Determinants <strong>of</strong> multiple informantagreement on child <strong>and</strong> adolescent behavior.Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 34, 251–261.Karver, M. S., H<strong>and</strong>elsman, J. B., Fields, S., & Bickman,L. (2006). Meta-analysis <strong>of</strong> therapeutic relationshipvariables in youth <strong>and</strong> family therapy: The evidencefor different relationship variables in the child <strong>and</strong>adolescent treatment outcome literature. <strong>Clinical</strong> PsychologyReview, 26, 50–65.Kashani, J. H., Orvaschel, H., Burk, J. P., & Reid, J. C.(1985). Informant variance: The issue <strong>of</strong> parent-childdisagreement. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong><strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 24, 437–441.Kashner, T. M., Rush, A. J., Surís, A., Biggs, M. M.,Gajewski, V. L.Hooker, D. J., et al. (2003). Impact <strong>of</strong>structured clinical interviews on physicians’ practicesin community mental health settings. Psychiatric Services,54, 712–718.Kaufman, A. S. (1994). Intelligent testing with the WISC-III. New York: WileyKazdin, A. E. (1981). Behavioral observation. In M.Hersen & A. S. Bellack (Eds.), Behavioral assessment:A practical h<strong>and</strong>book (pp. 59–100). New York: PergamonPress.Kazdin, A. E. (1988). <strong>Child</strong>hood depression. In E. J.Mash & L. G. Terdal (Eds.), Behavioral assessment <strong>of</strong>childhood disorders (2nd ed., pp. 157–195). New York:Guilford Press.Kazdin, A. E. (1989). <strong>Child</strong>hood depression. In E. J.Mash & R. A. Barkley (Eds.), Treatment <strong>of</strong> childhooddisorders (pp. 135–166). New York: Guilford Press.Keenan, K. (2000). Emotional dysregulation as a riskfactor for child psychopathology. <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology:Science <strong>and</strong> Practice, 7, 418–434.Keiley, M. K., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G.S. (2000). A cross-domain growth analysis: Externalizing<strong>and</strong> internalizing behaviors during 8 years<strong>of</strong> childhood. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology,28(2), 161–179.Keith, T. Z. (1990). Confirmatory <strong>and</strong> hierarchical confirmatoryanalysis <strong>of</strong> the differential ability scales.Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychoeducational <strong>Assessment</strong>, 8, 391–405.Keller, H. R. (1986). Behavioral observation approachesto personality assessment. In H. M. Kn<strong>of</strong>f (Ed.), Theassessment <strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong> adolescent personality (pp. 353–390). New York: Guilford Press.Kendall, P. C., & Ronan, K. R. (1990). <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong>children’s anxieties, fears, <strong>and</strong> phobias: Cognitivebehavioralmodels <strong>and</strong> methods. In C. R. Reynolds &R. W. Kamphaus (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong>educational assessment <strong>of</strong> children: <strong>Personality</strong>, behavior,<strong>and</strong> context (pp. 223–244). New York: Guilford Press.Kendall, P. C., & Clarkin, J. F. (1992). Introduction tospecial section: Comorbidity <strong>and</strong> treatment implications.Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 60,833–834.Keogh, B. K. (1994). A matrix <strong>of</strong> decision points in themeasurement <strong>of</strong> learning disabilities. In G. R. Lyon(Ed.), Frames <strong>of</strong> reference for the assessment <strong>of</strong> learning disabilities:New views on measurement issues (pp. 15–26).Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Kerr, D., Lunkenheimer, E., & Olson, S. (2007). <strong>Assessment</strong><strong>of</strong> child problem behaviors by multiple informants:A longitudinal study from preschool to schoolentry. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry, 48,967–975.Kim, J. A., Szatmari, P., Bryson, S. E., Streiner, D. L.,& Wilson, F. J. (2000). The prevalence <strong>of</strong> anxiety<strong>and</strong> mood problems among children with autism <strong>and</strong>Asperger syndrome. Autism, 4, 117–132.Kimonis, E. R., Frick, P. J., Skeem, J. L., Marsee, M.A., Cruise, K., Muñoz, L. C., & Aucoin, K. J. (2008).Assessing callous-unemotional traits in adolescent<strong>of</strong>fenders: Validation <strong>of</strong> the inventory <strong>of</strong> callousunemotionaltraits. Journal <strong>of</strong> the International Association<strong>of</strong> Psychiatry <strong>and</strong> Law. Special Issue: ‘Psychopathy<strong>and</strong> risk taxation’ in adolescent <strong>of</strong>fenders , 31, 241–251.Klein, D. N., Dougherty, L. R., & Olino, T. M. (2005).Toward guidelines for evidence-based assessment<strong>of</strong> depression in children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. Journal <strong>of</strong><strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychology, 34, 412–432.Klein, D. N., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Seeley, J. R. (1997).Psychosocial characteristics <strong>of</strong> adolescents with a pasthistory <strong>of</strong> dysthymic disorder: Comparison with adolescentswith past histories <strong>of</strong> major depressive <strong>and</strong>non-affective disorders, <strong>and</strong> never mentally ill controls.Journal <strong>of</strong> Affective Disorders, 42, 127–135.Kleinmmntz, B. (1967). <strong>Personality</strong> measurement: Anintroduction. Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press.


eferenes473Kline, R. B. (1989). Is the Fourth Edition Stanford-Bineta four-factor test? Confirmatory analysis <strong>of</strong> alternativemodels for ages 2 through 23. Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychoeducational<strong>Assessment</strong>, 7, 913.Kline, R. B. (1994). Test review: New objective ratingscales for child assessment, I. Parent- <strong>and</strong> teacherinformantinventories <strong>of</strong> the behavior assessmentsystem for children, the child behavior checklist, <strong>and</strong>the teacher report form. Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychoeducational<strong>Assessment</strong>, 12, 289–306.Kline, R. B. (1995). New objective rating scales for childassessment, II. Self-report scales for children <strong>and</strong> adolescents:Self-report <strong>of</strong> personality <strong>of</strong> the behaviorassessment system for children, the youth self-report,<strong>and</strong> the personality inventory for youth. Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychoeducational<strong>Assessment</strong>, 13, 169–193.Knight, L. W. (1997). The BASC parent rating scales:Patterns <strong>of</strong> behavior by gender, race, <strong>and</strong> socioeconomicstatus. Dissertation Abstracts International SectionA: Humanities <strong>and</strong> Social Sciences, 58(1-A), 0084.Kn<strong>of</strong>f, H. M. (1983). Justifying projective/personalityassessment in school psychology: A response toBatsch <strong>and</strong> Peterson. School Psychology Review, 12,446–451.Kochanska, G. (1993). Toward a synthesis <strong>of</strong> parentalsocialization <strong>and</strong> child temperament in early development<strong>of</strong> con-science. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 64, 325–347.Kochanska, G., Kuczynski, L., & Radke-Yarrow, N.I. (1989). Correspondence between mothers’ selfreported<strong>and</strong> observed child-rearing practices. <strong>Child</strong>Development, 60, 56–63.Konold, T. R., Walthall, J. C., & Pianta, R. C. (2004).The behavior <strong>of</strong> child ratings: Measurement structure<strong>of</strong> the child behavior checklist across time, informants,<strong>and</strong> child gender. Behavioral Disorders, 29, 372–383.Koocher, G. P. (1993). Ethical issues in the psychologicalassessment <strong>of</strong> children. In T. H. Ollendick & M.Hersen (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong> adolescent assessment(pp. 51–61). Boston, MA: Allyn <strong>and</strong> Bacon.Koppitz, E. M. (1968). Psychological evaluation <strong>of</strong> children’shuman figure drawings. New York: Grune atStratton.Koppitz, E. M. (1983). Projective drawings with children,<strong>and</strong> adolescents. School Psychology Review, 12,421–427.Kovacs, M. (1989). Affective disorders in children <strong>and</strong>adolescents. American Psychologist, 44(2), 209–215.Kovacs, M. (1992). The <strong>Child</strong>ren’s Depression Inventorymanual. New York: Multi-Health Systems.Kovacs, M. (2001). Gender <strong>and</strong> the course <strong>of</strong> majordepressive disorder through adolescence in clinicallyreferredyoungsters. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong><strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 40, 1079–1085.Kovacs, M., Paulauskas, S., Gatsonis, C., & Richards,C. (1988). Depressive disorders in childhood: III. Alongitudinal study <strong>of</strong> comorbidity with <strong>and</strong> risk forconduct disorders. Journal <strong>of</strong> Affective Disorders, 15,205–217.Krefetz, D., Steer, R., Gulab, N., & Beck, A. (2002).Convergent validity <strong>of</strong> the Beck depression inventory-IIwith the Reynolds adolescent depression scalein psychiatric inpatients. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>,78, 451–460.Kroon, N., Goudena, P. P., & Rispens, J. (1998). Thematicapperception tests for child <strong>and</strong> adolescentassessment: A practitioner’s consumer guide. Journal<strong>of</strong> Psychoeducational <strong>Assessment</strong>, 16, 99–117.Krug, D. A., & Arick, J. R. (2003). Krug Asperger’s disorderindex. Austin, Texas: Pro-Ed.Kuehne, C., Kehle, T. J., & McMahon, W. (1987).Differences between children with attention deficitdisorder, children with specific learning disabilities,<strong>and</strong> normal children. Journal <strong>of</strong> School Psychology, 25,161–166.Kuramoto, H., Kanbayaski, Y., Nakata, Y., Fukui, T.,Mukai, T., & Negishi, Y. (1999). St<strong>and</strong>ardization <strong>of</strong>the Japanese version <strong>of</strong> the youth self report (YSR).Japanese Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 40,329–344.Lachar, D. (1982). <strong>Personality</strong> inventory for childrenrevised(PICR). Los Angeles: Western PsychologicalServices.Lachar, D. (1990). Objective assessment <strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong>adolescent personality: The <strong>Personality</strong> Inventoryfor <strong>Child</strong>ren. In C. R. Reynolds & R. W. Kantphaus(Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong> educationalassessment <strong>of</strong> children: <strong>Personality</strong>, behavior, <strong>and</strong>context (pp. 298–323). New York: Guilford Press.Lachar, D. (1993). Symptom checklists <strong>and</strong> personalityinventories. In T. R. Kratochwill & R. J. Morris(Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> psychotherapy for children <strong>and</strong> adolescents(pp. 38–57). Boston, MA: Allyn <strong>and</strong> Bacon.Lachar, D. (1999). <strong>Personality</strong> inventory for <strong>Child</strong>ren,Second Edition (PTC-2), <strong>Personality</strong> Inventory forYouth (PM, <strong>and</strong> Student Behavior Survey (SBS). InM. Maiuish (Ed.), The use <strong>of</strong> psychological testing fortreatment planning <strong>and</strong> outcomes assessment (2nd ed.).Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Lachar, D., & Gruber, C. P. (1992). <strong>Personality</strong> Inventoryfor Youth (P1Y). Los Angeles: Western PsychologicalServices.Lachar, D., & Gruber, C. P. (1993). Development <strong>of</strong> thepersonality inventory for youth: A self-report companionto the personality inventory for children. Journal<strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>, 61, 81–98.Lachar, D., & Gruber, C. P. (1994). The <strong>Personality</strong>Inventory for Youth. Los Angeles: Western PsychologicalServices.Lachar, D., & Gruber, C. P. (1997). <strong>Personality</strong> Inventoryfor Youth (PIP) manual: Administration <strong>and</strong> interpretationguide. Los Angeles: Western PsychologicalServices.Lachar, D. & Gruber, C. (2001). <strong>Personality</strong> Inventoryfor <strong>Child</strong>ren, second edition (PIC-2): St<strong>and</strong>ard form <strong>and</strong>behavioral summary manual. Los Angeles: WesternPsychological Services.


474 referencesLachar, D., Harper, R. A., Green, B. A., Morgan, S.T., & Wheeler, A. C. (1996, August). The <strong>Personality</strong>Inventory fir Youth: Contribution to diagnosis. Posterpresented at the 104th Annual Convention <strong>of</strong> theAmerican Psychological Association, Toronto, Canada.Lachar, D., Morgan, S. T., Espades, A., & Schomer, O.(2000, August). Effect <strong>of</strong> defensiveness on two self-reportchild adjustment inventories. Poster presented at the108th Annual Convention <strong>of</strong> the American PsychologicalAssociation, Washington, DC.Lachar, D., Wingenfeld, S. A., Kline, R. B., & Gruber,C. P. (2000). Student behavior survey. Los Angeles,CA: Western Psychological Services.LaCombe, J. A., Kline, R. B., Lachar, D., Butkus, M., & Hillman, S. B.(1991). Case history correlates <strong>of</strong> a personality inventoryfor children (PIC) pr<strong>of</strong>ile typology. Psychological <strong>Assessment</strong>,3, 678–687.Lahey, B. B., Applegate, B., Waldman, I. D., L<strong>of</strong>t, J.D., Hankin, B. L., & Rick, J. (2004). The structure<strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong> adolescent psychopathology: Generatingnew hypotheses. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal Psychology, 113,358–385.Lahey, B. B., Applegate, B., Barkley, R. A., Garfinkel, B.,McBurnett, K., Kerdyck, L., et al. (1994). DSM-IVfield trials for oppositional defiant disorder <strong>and</strong> conductdisorder in children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. American Journal <strong>of</strong>Psychiatry, 151, 1163–1171.Lahey, B. B., Applegate, B., McBurnett, K., Biederman,J., Greenhill, L., Hynd, G. W., et al. (1994). DSM-IVfield trials for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorderin children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. American Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry,151, 1673–1685.Lahey, B. B., Carlson, C. L. & Frick, P. J. (1997). Attention-deficitdisorders: A review <strong>of</strong> research relevant todiagnostic classification. In T. A. Widiger, A. J. Frances,H. A. Pincus, R. Ross, M. B. First, & W. Davis (Eds.),DSM-IV sourcebook (Vol. 3, pp. 163–188). Washington,DC: American Psychiatric Association.Lahey, B.B., Goodman, S.H., Waldman, I.D., Bird, H.,Canino, G., Jenson, P., Regier, D., Leaf, P.J., Gordon,R., & Applegate, B. (1999). Relation <strong>of</strong> age <strong>of</strong> onsetto the type <strong>of</strong> severity <strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong> adolescent conductproblems. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 27,247–260.Lahey, B. B., Hart, E. L., Pliszka, S., Applegate, B.,& McBurnett, K. (1993). Neurophysiological correlates<strong>of</strong> conduct disorder: A rationale <strong>and</strong> a review<strong>of</strong> research. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 22,141–153.Lahey, B. B., & Loeber, R. (1994). Framework for adevelopmental model <strong>of</strong> oppositional defiant disorder<strong>and</strong> conduct disorder. In D. K. Routh (Ed.), Disruptivebehavior disorders in childhood (pp. 139–180). NewYork: Plenum.Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Quay, H. C., Applegate, B.,Shaffer, D., Waldman, I., et al. (1998). Validity <strong>of</strong>DSM-IV subtypes <strong>of</strong> conduct disorder based on age<strong>of</strong> onset. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong><strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 37, 435–442.Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Quay, H. C., Frick, P. J., &Grimm, J. (1992). Oppositional defiant disorder <strong>and</strong>conduct disorders: Issues to be resolved for DSM-IV.Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong>Psychiatry, 31, 539–546.Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Stouthamer-Loeber, M.,Christ, M. A. G., Green, S. M., Russo, M. F., et al.(1990). Comparison <strong>of</strong> DSM-III <strong>and</strong> DSM-III-Rdiagnoses for prepubertal children: Changes in prevalence<strong>and</strong> validity. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong><strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 29, 620–626.Lahey, B. B., Pelham, W. E., Loney, J., Lee, S. S., &Willcutt, E. (2005). Instability <strong>of</strong> the DSM-IV subtypes<strong>of</strong> ADHD from preschool through elementaryschool. Archives <strong>of</strong> General Psychiatry, 62, 896–902.Lahey, B. B., Pelham, W. E., Schaughency, E. A.,Atkins, M. S., Murphy, A., Hynd, G. W., et al. (1988).Dimensions <strong>and</strong> types <strong>of</strong> attention deficit disorder.Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong>Psychiatry, 27, 330–335.Lahey, B. B., & Piacentini, J. C. (1985). An evaluation <strong>of</strong>the Quay-Peterson revised behavior problem checklist.Journal <strong>of</strong> School Psychology, 23, 285–289.Lambert, M., Whipple, J., Hawkins, E., Vermeersch, D.,Nielsen, S., & Smart, D. (2003). Is it time for cliniciansto routinely track patient outcome? A meta-analysis.<strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology: Science <strong>and</strong> Practice, 10, 288–301.Lambert, N. M. (1988). <strong>Adolescent</strong> outcomes for hyperactivechildren: Perspectives on general <strong>and</strong> specificpatterns <strong>of</strong> child-hood risk for adolescent educational,social <strong>and</strong> mental health problems. American Psychologist,43, 786–799.Lambert, N., Nihira, K., & Lel<strong>and</strong>, H. (1993). AAMRadaptive behavior scale-school (2nd ed.)(ABS-S:2).Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Lanyou, R. I. (1995). Review <strong>of</strong> the Minnesota Multiphasic<strong>Personality</strong> Inventory – <strong>Adolescent</strong>. ‘TwelfthMental Measurements Yearbook. Lincoln, NE: BurosInstitute.Last, C. G. (1993). Introduction. In C. G. Last (Ed.),Anxiety across the lifespan: A developmental perspective(pp. 1–6). New York: Springer.Last, C. G., Hersen, M., Kazdin, A. E., Francis, G., &Grubb, H. J. (1987). Psychiatric illness in the mothers<strong>of</strong> anxious children. American Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry,144, 1580–1583.Le Couteur, A., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Rios, P., Robertson,S., Holdgrafer, M., et al. (1989). Autism diagnosticinterview: A st<strong>and</strong>ardized investigator-basedinstrument. Journal <strong>of</strong> Autism <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disorders,19, 363–387.Lefkowitz, M. M., & Tesiny, E. P. (1980). <strong>Assessment</strong><strong>of</strong> child-hood depression. Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong><strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 48, 43–50.Lemery, K., Essex, M., & Smider, N. (2002). Revealingthe relation between temperament <strong>and</strong> behavior


eferenes475problem symptoms by eliminating measurement confounding:Expert ratings <strong>and</strong> factor analyses. <strong>Child</strong>Development, 73, 867–882.Lett, N. J. (1997). Differential validity <strong>of</strong> the BASC studentobservation system <strong>and</strong> the BASC teacher ratingscale. Canadian Journal <strong>of</strong> School Psychology, 13, 1–14.Lett, N. J., & Kamphaus, R. W. (1997). Differentialvalidity <strong>of</strong> the BASC student observation system<strong>and</strong> the BASC teacher rating scales. Canadian Journal<strong>of</strong> School Psychology, 13, 1–14.Lewczyk, C. M., Garl<strong>and</strong>, A. F., Hurlburt, M. S., Gearity,J., & Hough, R. L. (2003). Comparing DISC-IV<strong>and</strong> clinician diagnoses among youths receiving publicmental health services. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy<strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> & <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 42, 349–356.Lichtenberger, E. O., Mather, N., Kaufman, N. L., &Kaufman, A. S. (2004). Essentials <strong>of</strong> assessment reportwriting. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Lilienfeld, S. O. (2003). Comorbidity between <strong>and</strong>within childhood externalizing <strong>and</strong> internalizing disorders:Reflections <strong>and</strong> directions. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal<strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 31, 285–291.Lochman, J. E., Wells, K. C., & Lenhart, L. A. (2008).Coping power: <strong>Child</strong> group program. New York:Oxford.Locke, H. J., & Wallace, K. M. (1959). Short maritaladjustment<strong>and</strong> prediction tests: Their reliability <strong>and</strong>validity. Marriage <strong>and</strong> Family Living, 21, 251–255.Loeber, R. (1991). Antisocial behavior: More enduringthan changeable? Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong><strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 30, 393–397.Loeber, R., Burke, J. D., Lahey, B. B., Winters, A., &Zen, M. (2000). Oppositional defiant <strong>and</strong> conductdisorder: A review <strong>of</strong> the past 10 years, Part I. Journal<strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry,39, 1468–1484.Loeber, R., Green, S. M., & Lahey, B. B. (1990). Mentalhealth pr<strong>of</strong>essionals’ perception <strong>of</strong> the utility <strong>of</strong>children, mothers, <strong>and</strong> teachers as informants onchildhood psychopathology. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong>Psychology, 19, 136–143.Loeber, R., Green, S. M., Lahey, B. B., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1991). Differences <strong>and</strong> similaritiesbetween children, mothers, <strong>and</strong> teachers as informantson disruptive child behavior. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong>Psychology, 19, 75–95.Loeber, R., Green, S. M., Lahey, B. B., Christ, M. A. G.,& Frick, P. J. (1992). Developmental sequences in theage <strong>of</strong> onset <strong>of</strong> disruptive child behaviors. Journal <strong>of</strong><strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> Family Studies, 1, 21–41.Loeber, R., Green, S. M., Lahey, B. B., Christ, M. A. G.,& Frick, P. J. (1992). Developmental sequences in theage <strong>of</strong> onset <strong>of</strong> disruptive child behaviors. Journal <strong>of</strong><strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> Family Studies, 1, 21–41.Loeber, R., & Keenan, K. (1994). Interaction betweenconduct disorder <strong>and</strong> its comorbid conditions: Effects<strong>of</strong> age <strong>and</strong> gender. <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology Review, 14,497–523.Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1986). Familyfactors as correlates <strong>and</strong> predictors <strong>of</strong> juvenileconduct problems <strong>and</strong> delinquency. In M. Tonry& N. Morris (Eds.), Crime <strong>and</strong> justice (Vol. 7, pp.29–149). Chicago: University <strong>of</strong> Chicago Press.Loney, B. R., & Frick, P. J. (2003). Structured diagnosticinterviewing. In C. R. Reynolds & R. W. Kamphaus(Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> educational assessment <strong>of</strong> children(2nd ed., pp. 235–247). New York: Guilford.Loney, B. R., Frick, P. J., Ellis, M. L., & McCoy, M.G. (1998). Intelligence, psychopathy, <strong>and</strong> antisocialbehavior. Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychopathology <strong>and</strong> Behavioral<strong>Assessment</strong>, 20, 231–247.Longman, Inc. (1984). Longman dictionary <strong>of</strong> psychology<strong>and</strong> psychiatry. New York: Author.Lopez, S. R. (1999). Teaching culturally informed psychologicalassessment. In R. H. Dana (Ed.), H<strong>and</strong>book<strong>of</strong> cross-cultural/multicultural personality assessment (pp.669–687). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (1999).Autism diagnostic observation schedule-WPS (ADOS-WPS). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Jr., Leventhal,B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autismdiagnostic observation schedule-generic: A st<strong>and</strong>ardmeasure <strong>of</strong> social <strong>and</strong> communication deficits associatedwith the spectrum <strong>of</strong> autism. Journal <strong>of</strong> Autism <strong>and</strong>Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autismdiagnostic interview-revised: A revised version <strong>of</strong> adiagnostic inter-view for caregivers <strong>of</strong> individuals withpossible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal <strong>of</strong>Autism <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685.Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (2002).Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule manual. LosAngeles: Western Psychological Services.Lord, C., Storoschuk, S., Rutter, M., & Pickles, A.(1993). Using the ADI-R to diagnose autism inpreschool children. Infant Mental Health Journal, 14,234–252.Lorr, M. (1965). Review <strong>of</strong> the Wittenborn PsychiatricRating Scales. In O. K. Buros (Ed.), Mental measurementsyearbook (5th ed.). Highl<strong>and</strong> Park, NJ: GryphonPress.Lou, H. C., Henriksen, L., Bruhn, P., Borner, H., &Nielsen, J. B. (1989). Striatal dysfunction in attentiondeficit <strong>and</strong> hyperkinetic disorder. Archives <strong>of</strong> Neurology,46, 48–52.Love, A. S., & McIntosh, D. E. (March, 1994). Differentialdiagnosis <strong>of</strong> attention deficit hyperactivity disordered<strong>and</strong> emotionally/behaviorally disordered children using theTemperament <strong>Assessment</strong> Battery for children. Paper presentedat the National Association <strong>of</strong> School Psychologyannual meeting, Seattle, WA.Lowman, M. G., Schwanz, K. A., & Kamphaus, R.W. (1996). WISC III third factor: Critical measurementissues. Canadian Journal <strong>of</strong> School Psychology, 12,15–22.


476 referencesLynam, D. R., Caspi, A., M<strong>of</strong>fitt, T. E., Raine, A., Loeber,R. & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2005). <strong>Adolescent</strong>psychopathy <strong>and</strong> the big five: Results from two samples.Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal Psychology, 33, 431–443.Luby, J. L., Heffelfinger, A., Mrakotsky, C., Hessler, M.J., Brown, K. M., & Hildebr<strong>and</strong>, T. (2002). Preschoolmajor depressive disorder: Preliminary validation fordevelopmentally modified DSM-IV criteria. Journal <strong>of</strong>the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> & <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry,41, 928–937.Lucia, V. C. & Breslau, N. (2006). Family cohesion <strong>and</strong>children’s behavior problems: A longitudinal investigation.Psychiatry Research, 141, 141–149.Luckasson, R., Borthwick-Duffy, S., Buntinx, W. H. E.,Coulter, D. L., Craig, E. M., Reeve, A., et al. (2002).Mental retardation: Definition, classification, <strong>and</strong> systems<strong>of</strong> supports (10th ed.). Washington, DC: AmericanAssociation on Mental Retardation.Lyneham, H. J., Abbott, M. J., & Rapee, R. M. (2007).Interrater reliability <strong>of</strong> the anxiety disorders interviewschedule for DSM-IV: <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> parent version.Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> & <strong>Adolescent</strong>Psychiatry, 46, 731–736.Lynskey, M. T., & Fergusson, D. M. (1995). <strong>Child</strong>hoodconduct problems, attention deficit behaviors, <strong>and</strong>adolescent alcohol, tobacco, <strong>and</strong> illicit drug use. Journal<strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 23, 281–302.Lytton, H. (1990). <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> parent effects in boys’ conductdisorder: A reinterpretation. Developmental Psychology,26, 683–697.Maccoby, E. E., & Martini, A. (1983). Socialization inthe context <strong>of</strong> the family: Parent-child interaction. InP. H. Mussen & E. M. Hetherington (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book<strong>of</strong> child psychology: IV; Socialization, personality,<strong>and</strong> social development (pp. 1–101). New York: Wiley.Machover, K. (1949). <strong>Personality</strong> projection in the drawing<strong>of</strong> the human figure. Springfield, IL: Charles C.Thomas.Madsen, C. J. (1990, September). Attention deficit disorders<strong>and</strong> application <strong>of</strong> the EFL4 <strong>and</strong> Section 504. Paperpresented to the Seventh Annual Pacific NorthwestInstitute on Special Education <strong>and</strong> Law.Malgady, R. G. & Costantino, G. (1998). Symptomseverity in bilingual Hispanics as a function <strong>of</strong> clinicianethnicity <strong>and</strong> language <strong>of</strong> interview. Psychological<strong>Assessment</strong>, 10, 120–127.Mann, B. J., & MacKenzie, E. P. (1996). Pathwaysamong marital functioning, parental behaviors, <strong>and</strong>child behavior problems in school-age boys. Journal <strong>of</strong><strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 25, 183–191.Mannuzza, S., Gittelman-Klein, R., Konig, P. H., &Giampino, T. L. (1989). Hyperactive boys almostgrown up: IV. Criminality <strong>and</strong> its relationship topsychiatric status. Archives <strong>of</strong> General Psychiatry, 46,1073–1079.March, J. S., Parker, J. D. A., Sullivan, K., Stallings, P., &Conners, K. (1997). The Multidimensional AnxietyScale for <strong>Child</strong>ren (MASC): Factor, structure, reliability,<strong>and</strong> validity. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong><strong>Child</strong> & <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 36, 554–565.Marchant, G. J., & Ridenour, T. A. (1998). [Review <strong>of</strong>the <strong>Personality</strong> Inventory for Youth]. Thirteenth MentalMeasurements Yearbook (pp. 757–758). Lincoln,NE: Burns Institute.Marcotte, D., Fortin, L., Potivin, P., & Papillon, M.(2002). Gender differences in depressive symptomsduring adolescence: Role <strong>of</strong> gender-typed characteristics,self-esteem, body image, stressful life events, <strong>and</strong>pubertal status. Journal <strong>of</strong> Emotional <strong>and</strong> BehavioralDisorders, 10, 29–42.Marcus, L. M., Lansing, M. D., & Schopler, E. (1993).<strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> the autistic <strong>and</strong> pervasive developmentallydisordered child. In D. J. Willis & J. L. Culbertson(Eds.), Testing young children (pp. 319–344).Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Maths, G. (1992). Issues in the measurement <strong>of</strong> acculturationamong Hispanics. In K. F. Geisinger (Ed.),Psychological testing <strong>of</strong> Hispanics (pp. 235–252). Washington,DC: American Psychological Association.Marin, G. (1992). Issues in the measurement <strong>of</strong> acculturationamong Hispanics. In K. F. Geisinger (Ed.),Psychological testing <strong>of</strong> Hispanics (pp. 235–252). Washington,DC: American Psychological Association.Marin, G., Sabogal, F., Marin, V. B., Otero-Sabogal, R.,& Perez-Stable, E. J. (1987). Development <strong>of</strong> a shortacculturation scale for Hispanics. Hispanic Journal <strong>of</strong>Behavioral Sciences, 9, 183–205.Marin, G., Sabogal, F., VanOss Marin, B., Otero-Sabogal, R.,& Perez-Stable, E. J. (1987). Development <strong>of</strong> a short acculturationscale for Hispanics. Hispanic Journal <strong>of</strong> BehavioralScience, 9, 183–205.Marsee, M. A., & Frick, P. J. (2007). Exploring the cognitive<strong>and</strong> emotional correlates to proactive <strong>and</strong> reactiveaggression in a sample <strong>of</strong> detained girls. Journal <strong>of</strong>Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 35, 969–981.Martin, R. P. (1983). The ethical issues in the use <strong>and</strong>interpretation <strong>of</strong> the Draw-a-Person Test <strong>and</strong> othersimilar projective procedures. The School Psychologist,38(6), 8.Martin, R. P. (1988). <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> personality <strong>and</strong> behaviorproblems: Infancy through adolescence. New York:Guilford Press.Martin, R. P. (1989). Activity level, distractibility, <strong>and</strong>persistence: Critical characteristics in early schooling.In G. A. Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates, & M. K. Rothbart(Eds.), Temperament in childhood (pp. 451–462) London:Wiley.Martin, R. P., & Bridger, R. (1999). Temperament <strong>Assessment</strong>Battery for <strong>Child</strong>ren – Revised (TABC-R). Athens,GA: Authors.Martin, R. P., & Holbrook, J. (1985). Relationship<strong>of</strong> temperament characteristics to the academicachievement <strong>of</strong> first grade children. Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychoeducational<strong>Assessment</strong>, 3, 131–140.Martin, R. P., Nagle, R., & Paget, K. (1984). Relationshipsbetween temperament <strong>and</strong> classroom behavior,


eferenes477teacher attitudes <strong>and</strong> academic achievement. Journal<strong>of</strong> Psychoeducational <strong>Assessment</strong>, 1, 377–386.Martin, R. P., Wisenbaker, J., Matthews-Morgan, J.,Holbrook, J., Hooper, S., & Spalding, J. (1986). Stability<strong>of</strong> teacher temperament ratings over 6 <strong>and</strong> 12months. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 14,167–179.Martinez, R., Norman, R. D., & Delaney, H. D. (1984).A children’s Hispanic background scale. HispanicJournal <strong>of</strong> Behavioral Sciences, 6, 103–112.Mash, E. J., & Dozois, D. J. A. (2003). <strong>Child</strong> psychopathology:A developmental-systems perspective. In E.J. Mash & R. A. Barkley (Eds.), <strong>Child</strong> psychopathology(2nd ed., pp. 3–71). New York: Guilford.Mash, E., & Hunsley, J. (2005). Evidence-based assessment<strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong> adolescent disorders: Issues <strong>and</strong>challenges. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychology,34, 362–379.Mash, E. J., & Terdal, L. G. (1988). Behavioral assessment<strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong> family disturbance. In E. J. Mash& L. G. Terdal (Eds.), Behavioral assessment <strong>of</strong> childhooddisorders (2nd ed., pp. 3–68). New York: GuilfordPress.Massetti, G. M., Lahey, B. B., Pelham, W. E., Loney,J., Ehrhardt, A., Lee, S. S., et al. (2008). Academicachievement over 8years among children who metmodified criteria for attention-deficit/hyperactivitydisorder at 4–6 years <strong>of</strong> age. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong>Psychology, 36, 399–410.Masuda, M., Matsumoto, G. H., & Meredith, G. M.(1970). Ethnic identity in three generations <strong>of</strong> JapaneseAmericans. Journal <strong>of</strong> Social Psychology, 81, 199–207.Matazow, G. S., & Kamphaus, R. N. T. (in press). Behaviorassessment system for children (BASC): Towardaccurate diagnosis <strong>and</strong> effective treatment. In J. J. W.Andrews, H. Janzen, & D. Sakl<strong>of</strong>ske (Eds.), Ability,achievement, <strong>and</strong> behavior assessment: A practical h<strong>and</strong>book.San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Mattison, R. E., H<strong>and</strong>ford, H. A., Kales, H. C., Goodman,A. L., & McLaughlin, R. E. (1990). Four-yearpredictive value <strong>of</strong> the <strong>Child</strong>ren’s Depression Inventory.Psychological <strong>Assessment</strong>, 2, 169–174.Mayes, L., Volkmar, F. R., Hooks, M., & Cicchetti, D.V. (1993). Differentiating pervasive developmentaldisorder not otherwise specified from autism <strong>and</strong> languagedisorders. Journal <strong>of</strong> Autism & DevelopmentalDisorders, 23, 79–90.Mayfield, J. W. & Reynolds, C. R. (1998). Are ethnicdifferences in diagnosis <strong>of</strong> childhood psychopathologyan artifact <strong>of</strong> psychometric methods? An experimentalevaluation <strong>of</strong> Harrington’s hypothesis usingparent-reported symptomatology. Journal <strong>of</strong> SchoolPsychology, 36, 313–334.Mayeux, L., Bellmore, A. D., & Cillessen, A. H. N.(2007). Predicting changes in adjustment usingrepeated measures <strong>of</strong> sociometric status. The Journal<strong>of</strong> Genetic Psychology, 168, 401–424.Mayeux, L., Underwood, M. K., & Risser, S. D. (2007).Perspectives on the ethics <strong>of</strong> sociometric research withchildren. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 53, 53–78.Mazefsky, C., & Oswald, D. (2006). The discriminativeability <strong>and</strong> diagnostic utility <strong>of</strong> the ADOS-G,ADI-R, <strong>and</strong> GARS for children in a clinical setting.Autism, 10, 533–549.McArthur, D. S., & Roberts, G. E. (1982). Roberts ApperceptionTest for children. Los Angeles: Western PsychologicalServices.McCarney, S. B. (2004). The attention deficit disordersevaluation scale, home <strong>and</strong> school versions: Technicalmanual. Columbia, MO: Hawthorne.McCarthy, L., & Archer, R. P. (1998). Factor structure<strong>of</strong> the MMPI-A content scales: Item-level <strong>and</strong> scalelevelfindings. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>, 71,84–97.McConaughy, S. H. (2005). Direct observational assessmentduring test sessions. School Psychology Review,34, 490–506.McConaughy, S. H., & Achenbach, T. M. (1989).Empirically based assessment <strong>of</strong> serious emotionaldisturbance. Journal <strong>of</strong> School Psychology, 27, 91–117.McConaughy, S. H., & Achenbach, T. M. (2004). Manualfor the Test Observation Form for ages 2–18. Burlington,VT: Research Center for <strong>Child</strong>ren, Youth, <strong>and</strong>Families.McConaughy, S. H., Achenbach, T. M., & Gent, C. L.(1988). Multiaxial empirically based assessment: Parent,teacher, observational, cognitive, <strong>and</strong> personalitycorrelates <strong>of</strong> child behavior pr<strong>of</strong>ile types for 6- to11-year-old boys. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology,16, 485–509.McConaughy, S. H., Stanger, C., & Achenbach, T. M.(1992). Three year course <strong>of</strong> behavioral emotionalproblems in a national sample <strong>of</strong> 4- to 16-year-olds:I. Agreement among informants. Journal <strong>of</strong> the AmericanAcademy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 31,932–940.McConell, S. R., & Odom, S. L. (1986). Sociometrics:Peer-referenced measures <strong>and</strong> the assessment <strong>of</strong> socialcompetence. In P. S. Strain, M. J. Guralnick, & H. M.Walker (Eds.), <strong>Child</strong>ren’s social behavior: Development,assessment, <strong>and</strong> modification (pp. 215–286). New York:Academic Press.McCrae, R. R., Stone, S. V., Fagan, P. J., Costa, P. T.(1998). Identifying causes <strong>and</strong> disagreement betweenself-reports <strong>and</strong> spouse ratings <strong>of</strong> personality. Journal<strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong>, 66, 285–312.McDonald, K. L., Putallaz, M., Grimes, C. L.,Kupersmidt, J. B., & Coie, J. D. (2007). Girl talk:Gossip, friendship, <strong>and</strong> sociometric status. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 53, 381–411.McGoldrick, M., Gerson, R., & Shellenberger, S.(2008). Genograms: <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>and</strong> intervention (2nded.). New York: W. W. Norton.McGrew, K. S., Bruininks, R. H., & Thurlow, M. L.(1992). Relationship between measures <strong>of</strong> adaptive


478 referencesfunctioning <strong>and</strong> community adjustment for adultswith mental retardation. Exceptional <strong>Child</strong>ren, 58,517–529.McMahon, R. J., & Frick, P. J. (2005). Evidence-basedassessment <strong>of</strong> conduct problems in children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychology,34, 477–505.McMahon, R. J. & Frick, P. J. (2007). Conduct <strong>and</strong>oppositional disorders. In E. J. Mash & R. A. Barkley(Eds)., <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> childhood disorders (4th ed.,pp. 132–183). New York: Guilford.McMahon, R. J., & Peters, R. D. V. (2002). The effects<strong>of</strong> parental dysfunction on children. New York: PlenumPress.McNally, S., Eisenberg, N., & Harris, J. D. (1991).Consistency <strong>and</strong> change in maternal child-rearingpractices <strong>and</strong> values: A longitudinal study. <strong>Child</strong>Development, 62, 190–198.McNamara, J. R., Hollmann, C., & Riegel, T. (1994).A preliminary study <strong>of</strong> the usefulness <strong>of</strong> theBehavior <strong>Assessment</strong> System for <strong>Child</strong>ren in theevaluation <strong>of</strong> mental health needs in a Head Startpopulation. Psychological Reports, 75, 1195–1201.McNeil, C. B., Eyberg, S., Eisenstadt, T. H., Newcomb,K., & Funderbunk, B. (1991). Parent-child InteractionTherapy with behavior problem children: Generalization<strong>of</strong> treatment effects to the school setting.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 20, 140–151.McReynolds, P., Ballachey, E.L., & Ferguson, J.T.Development <strong>and</strong> evaluation <strong>of</strong> a behavioral scale forappraising the adjustment <strong>of</strong> hospitalized patients.American Psychologists, 1952, 7, 340Measelle, J. R., Ablow, J. C., Cowan, P. A., & Cowan,C. P. (1998). Assessing young children’s views <strong>of</strong> theiracademic, social, <strong>and</strong> emotional lives: An evaluation<strong>of</strong> the self-perception scales <strong>of</strong> the Berkeley PuppetInterview. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 69, 1556–1576.Meehl, P. E. (1973). Psychodiagnosis. Selected papers. Minneapolis:University <strong>of</strong> Minnesota Press.Meehl, P. E. & Hathaway, S. R. (1946). The K factoras a suppressor variable in the Minnesota Multiphasic<strong>Personality</strong> Inventory. Journal <strong>of</strong> Applied Psychology,30, 525–564.Mendoza, R. H. (1989). An empirical scale to measuretype <strong>and</strong> degree <strong>of</strong> acculturation in Mexican-Americanadolescents <strong>and</strong> adults. Journal <strong>of</strong> Cross CulturalPsychology, 20, 372–385.Merenda, P. F. (1996). BASC: Behavior <strong>Assessment</strong>System for <strong>Child</strong>ren. Measurement <strong>and</strong> Evaluation inCounseling <strong>and</strong> Development, 28, 229–232.Merrell, K. W., & Popinga, M. R. (1994). The alliance <strong>of</strong>adaptive behavior <strong>and</strong> social competence: An examination<strong>of</strong> relationships between the Scales <strong>of</strong> IndependentBehavior <strong>and</strong> the Social Skills Rating System.Research in Developmental Disabilities, 15, 39–47.Merydith, S. P. (2000). Aggression Intervention SkillTraining: Moral reasoning <strong>and</strong> moral emotions.NASP Communique, 28, 6–8.Merydith, S. P., & Joyce, F. K. (1998, August). Temporalstability <strong>and</strong> convergent validity <strong>of</strong> the BASC Parent <strong>and</strong>Teacher Rating Scales. Paper presented at the AnnualMeeting <strong>of</strong> the American Psychological Association,San Francisco.Meyer, G. J., Erdberg, P., & Shaffer, T. W. (2007). Towardinternational normative reference data for the ComprehensiveSystem. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>,89(Suppl. 1), S201–S216.Mikami, A. Y. & Hinshaw, S. P. (2003). Buffers <strong>of</strong> peerrejection among girls with <strong>and</strong> without ADHD: Therole <strong>of</strong> popularity with adults <strong>and</strong> goal-directed solitaryplay. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 31, 381–397.Mildenberger, K., Sitter, S., Noterdaeme, M., & Amorosa,H. (2001). The use <strong>of</strong> the ADI–R as a diagnostictool in the differential diagnosis <strong>of</strong> children withinfantile autism <strong>and</strong> children with receptive languagedisorder. European <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 10,248–255.Milich, R. (1984). Cross-sectional <strong>and</strong> longitudinalobservations <strong>of</strong> activity level <strong>and</strong> sustained attentionin a normative sample. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology,12, 261–276.Milich, R., Loney, J., & L<strong>and</strong>au, S. (1982). The independentdimensions <strong>of</strong> hyperactivity <strong>and</strong> aggression:A validation with play-room observations. Journal <strong>of</strong>Abnormal Psychology, 91, 183–198.Miller, D. C. (1994, Winter). Behavior assessment systemfor children (BASC): Test critique. Texas Association<strong>of</strong> School Psychologists Newsletter, 23–29.Miller, L. C. (1967). Louisville Behavior Checklist formales 6–12 years <strong>of</strong> age. Psychological Reports, 21,885–896.Milliones, J. (1980). Construction <strong>of</strong> a Black consciousnessmeasure: Psychotherapeutic implications. Psychotherapy:Theory, Research <strong>and</strong> Practice, 17(2), 175–182.Milner, J. S. (1986). The <strong>Child</strong> Abuse Potential inventory:Manual – Second Edition. De Kalb, IL: Psytec.Milner, J. S., & Crouch, J. L. (1997). Impact <strong>and</strong> detection <strong>of</strong>response distortions on parenting measures used to assessrisk for child physical abuse. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>,69, 633–650.Milner, J. S., & Wimberley, R. C. (1980). Prediction <strong>and</strong>explanation <strong>of</strong> child abuse. Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychology, 36,875–884.Mintz, N. L., & Schwartz, D. T. (1964). Urban ecology<strong>and</strong> psychosis: Community factors in the incidence <strong>of</strong>schizophrenia <strong>and</strong> manic-depression among Italiansin Greater Boston. Inter-national Journal <strong>of</strong> Social Psychiatry,10, 101–117.Mischel, W. (1968). <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> assessment. New York:Wiley.M<strong>of</strong>fitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited <strong>and</strong> lifecoursepersistent antisocial behavior: A developmentaltaxonomy. Psychological Reports, 100, 674–701.M<strong>of</strong>fitt, T. E. (2003). Life-course persistent <strong>and</strong> adolescence-limitedantisocial behavior: A 10-year researchreview <strong>and</strong> research agenda. In B. B. Lahey, T. E. M<strong>of</strong>-


eferenes479fitt, & A. Caspi (Eds.), Causes <strong>of</strong> conduct disorder <strong>and</strong>juvenile delinquency (pp. 49–75). New York: Guilford.Monica, R. F., Poole, C., Son, L., & Murry, C. C. (1997).Effects <strong>of</strong> war trauma on Cambodian refugee adolescents’functional health <strong>and</strong> mental health stains.Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> & <strong>Adolescent</strong>Psychiatry, 36, 1098–1106.Monuteaux, M. C., Faraone, S. V., Gross, L. M., & Biederman,J. (2007). Predictors, clinical characteristics,<strong>and</strong> outcome <strong>of</strong> conduct disorder in girls with attentiondeficit hyperactivity disorder: A longitudinalstudy. Psychological Medicine, 37, 1731–1741.Moore, S. G., & Updegraff, R. (1964). Sociometric status<strong>of</strong> pre-school children as related to age, sex, nurturance-giving,<strong>and</strong> dependency. <strong>Child</strong> Development,35, 519–524.Moos, R. H., & Moos, B. S. (1986). Family EnvironmentScale Manual – Second Edition. Palo Alto, CA:Consulting Psychologists Press.Morgan, A. B. W. (1998). <strong>Child</strong>hood social withdrawal:The role <strong>of</strong> social skills in the relation <strong>of</strong> withdrawal<strong>and</strong> anxiety. Dissertation Abstracts International SectionB: The Sciences <strong>and</strong> Engineering, 59(6-B), 3067.Morgan, C. J., & Cauce, A. M. (1999). PredictingDSM-III-R disorders from the Youth Self-Report:Analysis <strong>of</strong> data from a field study. Journal <strong>of</strong> theAmerican Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry,38, 1237–1245.Morgenthaler, W. (1954). Hermann Rorschach,deceased, at his 70th birthday. Schweizerische Zeitschriftfuer Psychologie <strong>and</strong> ihre Anwendungen, 13, 315.Morris, R. J., & Kratochwill, T. R. (1983). Treatingchildren’s fears <strong>and</strong> phobias: A behavioral approach.New York: Pergamon Press. Murray, H. A. (1943).Thematic apperception test. Cambridge, MA: HarvardUniversity Press.Morsbach, S. K., & Prinz, R. J. (2006). Underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>and</strong> improving the validity <strong>of</strong> self-report <strong>of</strong> parenting.<strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> Family Review, 9, 1–21.Munoz, L. C. & Frick, P. J. (2007). The reliability, stability,<strong>and</strong> predictive utility <strong>of</strong> the self-report version<strong>of</strong> the Antisocial Process Screening Device. Sc<strong>and</strong>inavianJournal <strong>of</strong> Psychology, 48, 299–312.Mussman, M. C. (1964). Teacher’s evaluations <strong>of</strong> psychologicalreports. Journal <strong>of</strong> School Psychology, 3, 35–37.Myers, K., & Winters, N. C. (2002). Ten-year review <strong>of</strong>rating scales: II. Scales for internalizing disorders. Journal<strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> & <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry,41, 634–659.Myles, B. S., Bock, S. J., & Simpson, R. L. (2001).Asperger syndrome diagnostic scale. Los Angeles, CA:Western Psychological Services.Nadeau, K. G. (1995). Attention-deficit disorder in adults:Research, diagnosis, <strong>and</strong> treatment. New York: Brunner& Mazel.Naglieri, J. A., LeBuffe, P. A., & Pfeiffer, S. I. (1994).Devereux scales <strong>of</strong> mental disorders. New York: The PsychologicalCorporation.Nail, J. M., & Evans, J. G. (1997). The emotional adjustment<strong>of</strong> gifted adolescents: A view <strong>of</strong> global functioning.Roeper Review, 20(1), 18–21.Neeper, R., & Lahey, B. B. (1986). The <strong>Child</strong>ren’sBehavior Rating Scale: A factor analytic developmentalstudy. School Psychology Review, 15, 277–288.Neeper, R., Lahey, B. B., & Frick, P. J. (1990). Comprehensivebehavior rating scale for children. New York:The Psychological Corporation.Negy, C., Lachar, D., Gruber, C., & Garza, N. (2001).The <strong>Personality</strong> Inventory for Youth: Validity <strong>and</strong>comparability <strong>of</strong> English <strong>and</strong> Spanish versions forregular education <strong>and</strong> juvenile justice samples. Journal<strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>, 76, 250–263.Nelson, B., Martin, R. P., Hodge, S., Havill, V., & Kamphaus,R. (1999). Modeling the prediction <strong>of</strong> elementaryschool adjustment from preschool temperament.<strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Individual Differences, 26, 687–700.Neuman, R. J., Todd, R. D., Beath, A. C., Reich, W.,Hudziak, J. J., Bucholz, K. K., et al. (1999). Evaluation<strong>of</strong> ADHD typology in three contrasting samples: Alatent class approach. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy<strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 38(1), 25–33.Newman, J., & Matsopoulos, A. (1994, March). Temperamentprecursors <strong>of</strong> early reading ability. Paper presentedat the annual meeting <strong>of</strong> the National Association <strong>of</strong>School Psychologists, Seattle, WA.Nezu, A. M., & Nezu, C. M. (1993). Identifying <strong>and</strong>selecting target problems for clinical interventions:A problem-solving model. Psychological <strong>Assessment</strong>, 5,254–263.Nigg, J. T. (2006). What causes ADHD? Underst<strong>and</strong>ingwhat goes wrong <strong>and</strong> why. New York: Guilford.Nihira, K., Foster, R., Shellhaus, M., Lel<strong>and</strong>, H., Lambert,N., & Windmiller, M. (1981). AAMR adaptivebehavior scale, school edition. Monterey, CA: Publisher’sTest Service.Nihira, K., Lel<strong>and</strong>, H., & Lambert, N. (1993). AAMRadaptive behavior scales-residential <strong>and</strong> community(2nd. ed.) (AAMR-RC: 2). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Nitko, A. (1983). Educational tests <strong>and</strong> measurement: Anintroduction. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Nixon, R. D. V., Sweeney, L., Erickson, D. B., & Touyz,S. W. (2003). Parent-child interaction therapy: A comparison<strong>of</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard <strong>and</strong> abbreviated treatments for oppositionaldefiant preschoolers. Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong><strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 71, 251–260Nurnberger, J. I., Jr., Blehar, A. S. C., Kaufmann, C.A., York-Cooler, C., Simpson, S. G., Harkavy-Friedman, J., et al. (1994). Diagnostic interviewfor genetic studies: Rationale, unique features, <strong>and</strong>training. Archives <strong>of</strong> General Psychiatry, 51, 849–859.O’Brien, B. S., & Frick, P. J. (1996). Reward dominance:Associations with anxiety, conduct problems, <strong>and</strong> psychopathyin children. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology,24, 223–240.Oehler-Stinnett, J., & Stinnett, T. A. (1995). Teacherrating scales for attention deficit hyperactivity: A


480 referencescomparative review. Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychoeducational <strong>Assessment</strong>,Monograph Series, Special Issue on <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong>Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorders. Cordova,TN: Psychoeducational Corporation.Oesterheld, J. R., & Haber, J. (1997). Acceptability <strong>of</strong>the Conners Parent Rating Scale <strong>and</strong> <strong>Child</strong> BehaviorChecklist to Dakotan/Lakotan parents. Journal <strong>of</strong> theAmerican Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry,36, 55–64.Offord, D. R., Boyle, M. H., Fleming, J. E., Blum, H.M., & Grant, N. I. R. (1989). Ontario child healthstudy: Summary <strong>of</strong> selected results. Canadian Journal<strong>of</strong> Psychiatry, 34, 483–491.Okiishi, J., Lambert, M., Eggett, D., Nielsen, L., Dayton,D., & Vermeersch, D. (2006). An analysis <strong>of</strong> therapisttreatment effects: Toward providing feedback to individualtherapists on their clients’ psychotherapy outcome.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 62, 1157–1172.Ollendick, T. H. (1978). The Fear Survey Schedule for<strong>Child</strong>ren-Revised. Unpublished rating scale: IndianaState University.Ollendick, T. H., & Hersen, M. (1993). H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong>child <strong>and</strong> adolescent assessment. Boston, MA: Allyn <strong>and</strong>Bacon.Ollendick, T. H., & King, N. J. (1994). Diagnosis,assessment, <strong>and</strong> treatment <strong>of</strong> internalizing problemsin children: The role <strong>of</strong> longitudinal data.. Journal <strong>of</strong>Consulting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 62, 918–927.Ollendick, T. H., Seligman, L. D., Goza, A. B., Byrd,D. A., & Singh, K. (2003). Anxiety <strong>and</strong> depression inchildren <strong>and</strong> adolescents: A factor-analytic examination<strong>of</strong> the tripartite model. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> FamilyStudies, 12, 157–170.Olmedo, E. L., Martinez, J. L., Jr., & Martinez, S. R.(1978). Measure <strong>of</strong> acculturation for Chicano adolescents.Psychological Reports, 42, 159–170.Ondersma, S. J. (2007). Introduction to the special sectionon substance abuse <strong>and</strong> child maltreatment. <strong>Child</strong>Maltreatment, 12, 111–113.Ondersma, S. J., Chaffin, M. J., Mullins, S. M., & LeBreton,J. M. (2005). A brief form <strong>of</strong> the <strong>Child</strong> AbusePotential Inventory: Development <strong>and</strong> validation.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychology, 34,301–311.Orvaschel, H., Ambrosini, P., & Rabinovich, H. (1993).Diagnostic issues in child assessment. In T. H. Ollendick& M. Hersen (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong> adolescentassessment (pp. 26–40). Boston, MA: Allyn <strong>and</strong>Bacon.Orvaschel, H., Welsh-Allis, G., & Ye, W. (1988). Psychopathologyin children <strong>of</strong> parents with recurrentdepression. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology,16(1), 17–28.Os<strong>of</strong>sky, J. D., Wewers, S., Hann, D. M., & Fick, A. C.(1993). Chronic community violence: What is happeningto our children? Psychiatry, 56, 36–45.Ostr<strong>and</strong>er, R., Weinfurt, K. P., Yarnold, P. R., & August,G. J. (1998). Diagnosing attention-deficit disorderswith the Behavioral <strong>Assessment</strong> System for <strong>Child</strong>ren<strong>and</strong> the <strong>Child</strong> Behavior Checklist: Test <strong>and</strong> constructvalidity analyses using optimal discriminant classificationtrees. Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology,66, 660–672.Ostrov, J. M. & Keating, C. F. (2004). Gender differencesin preschool aggression during free play <strong>and</strong>structured interactions: An observational study. SocialDevelopment, 13, 255–277.Ownby, R. L., & Wallbrown, F. (1986). Improvingreport writing in school psychology. In T. R. Kratochwill(Ed.), Advances in school psychology (Vol. 5, pp.7–49). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Ozon<strong>of</strong>f, S., Goodlin-Jones, B., & Solomon, M. (2005).Evidence-based assessment <strong>of</strong> autism spectrum disordersin children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong><strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychology, 34, 523–540.Paik<strong>of</strong>f, R. L., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1991). Do parent-childrelationships change during puberty? Psychological Bulletin,110, 47–66.Papadopoulos, L., Bor, R., & Stanion, P. (1997). Genogramsin counselling practice: A review (part 1).Counseling Psychology Quarterly, 10, 17–28.Pardini, D. A. (2008). Novel insights into longst<strong>and</strong>ingtheories <strong>of</strong> bidirectional parent-child influences:Introduction to the special section. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal<strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 36, 627–632.Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1987). Peer relations <strong>and</strong>later personal adjustment: Are low-accepted childrenat risk? Psychological Bulletin, 102, 357–389.Patterson, G. R. (1982). Coercive family process. Eugene, OR:Castalia.Patterson, G. R., & Dishion, T. J. (1985). Contributions<strong>of</strong> families <strong>and</strong> peers to delinquency. Criminology, 23,63–79.Patterson, G. R., & Yoerger, K. (1993). Developmentalmodels for delinquent behavior. In S. Hodgins (Ed.),Mental Disorder <strong>and</strong> Crime. (pp. 140–172). NewburyPark: Sage.Payne, A. F. (1928). Sentence completions. New York: NewYork Guidance Clinic.Pearson, D. A., & Aman, M. G. (1994). Ratings <strong>of</strong>hyperactivity <strong>and</strong> developmental indices: Shouldclinicians correct for developmental level? Journal <strong>of</strong>Autism <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disorders, 24, 395–411.Pearson, D. A., & Lachar, D. (1994). Using behavioralquestionnaires to identify adaptive deficits in elementaryschool children. Journal <strong>of</strong> School Psychology, 32,33–52.Pearson, K. (1901). On lines <strong>and</strong> planes <strong>of</strong> closest fitto systems <strong>of</strong> points in space. Philosophical Magazine(Series 6), 2, 559–572.Peeples, F., & Loeber, R. (1994). Do individual factors<strong>and</strong> neighborhood context explain ethnic differences injuvenile delinquency? Journal <strong>of</strong> Quantitative Criminology,10, 141–157.Pelham, W. E., Fabiano, G. A., & Massetti, G. M.(2005). Evidence-based assessment <strong>of</strong> Attention-


eferenes481Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in children <strong>and</strong>adolescents. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong>Psychology, 34, 477–505.Pelham, W. E., Gnabry, E. M., Greenslade, K. E., &Milich, R. (1992). Teacher ratings <strong>of</strong> DSM-III-Rsymptoms for the disruptive behavior disorders. Journal<strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> ’ <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong>Psychiatry, 31, 210–218.Perry, M. A. (1990). The interview in developmentalassessment. In J. H. Johnson & J. Goldman (Eds.),Developmental assessment in clinical child psychology: Ah<strong>and</strong>book (pp. 58–77). New York: Pergamon Press.Petersen, A. C., Compas, B. E., Brooks-Gunn, J., Stemmler,M., Ey, S., & Grant, K. E. (1993). Depression inadolescence. American Psychologist, 48, 155–168.Petersen, N. S., Kolen, M. J., & Hoover, H. D. (1989)Scaling, norming, <strong>and</strong> equating. In R. L. Linn (Ed.),Educational measurement (3rd ed., pp. 221–262). NewYork: Macmillan.Peterson, D. R. (1961). Behavior problems <strong>of</strong> middle childhood.Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting Psychology, 25, 205–209.Pfiffner, L. J. & McBurnett, K. (2006). Family correlates<strong>of</strong> comorbid anxiety disorders in children with attentiondeficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal<strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 34, 725–736.Phares, E. J. (1984). <strong>Clinical</strong> psychology: Concepts, methods,<strong>and</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ession (rev.). Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press.Piacentini, J. (1993). Checklists <strong>and</strong> rating scales. In T.H. Ollendick & M. Hersen (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> child<strong>and</strong> adolescent assessment (pp. 82–97). Boston, MA:Allyn <strong>and</strong> Bacon.Piacentini, J. C., Cohen, P., & Cohen, J. (1992). Combiningdiscrepant diagnostic information from multiplesources: Are complex algorithms better thansimple ones? Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 20,51–63.Piacentini, J., Robper, M., Jensen, P., Lucas, C., Fisher,P., Bird, H., et al. (1999). Informant-based determinants<strong>of</strong> symptom attenuation in structured childpsychiatric interviews. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology,27, 417–428.Piacentini, J., Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Schwab-Stone, M.,Davies, A. T., & Gioia, P. (1993). The DiagnosticInterview Schedule for <strong>Child</strong>ren – Revised Version(DISC-R): III. Concurrent criterion validity. Journal<strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry,32, 648–656.Pinderhughes, E. E., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., Pettit,G. S., & Zelli, A. (2000). Discipline responses:Influences <strong>of</strong> parents’ socioeconomic status, ethnicity,beliefs about parenting, stress, <strong>and</strong> cognitiveemotionalprocesses. Journal <strong>of</strong> Family Psychology, 14,280–400.Piotrowski, C. (1999). Use <strong>of</strong> tests <strong>and</strong> measures inmarital <strong>and</strong> family research. Psychological Reports, 84,1251–1252.Pisecco, S., Lachar, D., Gruber, C. P., Gallen, R. T.,Kline, R. B., & Huzinec, C. (1999). Development<strong>and</strong> validation <strong>of</strong> disruptive behavior scales for theStudent Behavior Survey (SBS). Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychoeducational<strong>Assessment</strong>, 17, 314–331.Pope, K. S., Butcher, J. N., & Seelen, J. (2000). TheMMPI, MMPI-2 <strong>and</strong> MMPI-A in court: A practicalguide for expert witnesses <strong>and</strong> attorneys (2nd ed.). Washington,DC: American Psychological Association.Porter, B., & O’Leary, K. D. (1980). Marital discord <strong>and</strong>child-hood behavior problems. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal<strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 8, 287–295.Power, T., Doherty, B., Panichelli-Mindel, S., Karustis,J., Eiraldi, R., Anastopoulos, A., et al. (1998). Thepredictive validity <strong>of</strong> parent <strong>and</strong> teacher reports <strong>of</strong>ADHD symptoms. Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychopathology <strong>and</strong>Behavioral <strong>Assessment</strong>, 20, 57–81.Poznanski, E. O., Cook, S. C., & Carroll, J. B. (1979).A depression rating scale for children. Pediatrics, 64,442–450.Prifitera, A., Weiss, L. G., & Sakl<strong>of</strong>ske, D. H. (1998).The WISC-III in context. In A. Prifitera & D. Sakl<strong>of</strong>ske(Eds.), WISC-III: <strong>Clinical</strong> use <strong>and</strong> interpretation(pp. 1–38). San Diego: Academic Press.Pressman, L. J., Loo, S. K., Carpenter, E. M., Asarnow,J. R., Lynn, D., McCracken, J. T., et al. (2006). Relationship<strong>of</strong> family environment <strong>and</strong> parental psychiatricdiagnosis to impairment in ADHD. Journal <strong>of</strong> theAmerican Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry,45, 346–354.Prinstein, M. J. (2007). <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> adolescents’ preference-<strong>and</strong> reputation-based peer status using sociometricexperts. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 53, 243–261.Prinstein, M. J., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2003). Forms<strong>and</strong> functions <strong>of</strong> adolescent peer aggression associatedwith high levels <strong>of</strong> peer status. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,49, 310–342.Prinzie, P., Onghena, P., & Hellinckx, W. (2007).Reexamining the Parenting Scale: Reliability, factorstructure, concurrent validity <strong>of</strong> a scale for assessingthe discipline practices <strong>of</strong> mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers <strong>of</strong>elementary-school-aged children. European Journal<strong>of</strong> Psychological <strong>Assessment</strong>, 23, 24–31.Puig-Antich, J. (1982). Major depression <strong>and</strong> conductdisorder in prepuberty. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy<strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 21, 118–128.Puig-Antich, J., Blau, S., Marx, N., Greenhill, L. L., &Chambers, W. (1978). Prepubertal major depressivedisorders. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong><strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 17, 695–707.Puig-Antich, J., & Chambers, W. (1978). The schedulefor affective disorders <strong>and</strong> schizophrenia for school-agedchildren (Kiddie-SADS). New York: New York StatePsychiatric Institute.Quay, H. C. (1986). Classification. In H. C. Quay & J. S.Werry (Eds.), Psychopathological disorders <strong>of</strong> childhood(3rd ed., pp. 1–34). New York: Wiley.Quay, H. C., & Love, C. T. (1977). The effect <strong>of</strong> a juvenilediversion program on rearrests. Criminal Justice<strong>and</strong> Behavior, 4, 377–396.


482 referencesQuay, H. C., & Peterson, D. R. (1983). Interim manualfor the Revised Behavior Problem Checklist. Coral Gables,FL: Author.Rabin, A. I. (1986). Concerning projective techniques.In A. I. Rabin (Ed.), Projective techniques for adolescents<strong>and</strong> children (pp. 3–13). New York: Springer.Raine, A. (2002). Biosocial studies <strong>of</strong> antisocial <strong>and</strong> violentbehavior in children <strong>and</strong> adults: A review. Journal<strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 30, 311–326.Ramirez, M. (1984). Assessing <strong>and</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>ingbiculturalism–multiculturalism in Mexican–Americanadults. In J. L. Martinez, Jr. & R. H. Mendoza(Eds.), Chicano Psychology (pp. 77–94). Orl<strong>and</strong>o, FL:Academic Press.Rapoport, J. L., & Castellanos, F. X. (1996). Attentiondeficithyperactivity disorder. In J. M. Wiener (Ed.),Diagnosis <strong>and</strong> psychopharmacology <strong>of</strong> childhood <strong>and</strong> adolescentdisorders (2nd ed., pp. 265–292). New York: Wiley.Rapport, M. D., Chung, K. M., Shore, G., Denney, C.B., & Isaacs, P. (2000). Upgrading the science <strong>and</strong>technology <strong>of</strong> assessment <strong>and</strong> diagnosis: Laboratory<strong>and</strong> clinic-based assessment <strong>of</strong> children with ADHD.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 29, 555–568.Realmuto, G. M., August, G. J., Sieler, J. D., & Pessoa-Br<strong>and</strong>ao,L. (1997). Peer assessment <strong>of</strong> socialreputation in community samples <strong>of</strong> disruptive <strong>and</strong>nondisruptive children: Utility <strong>of</strong> the Revised ClassPlay method. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 26,67–76.Reed, M. L., & Edelbrock, C. (1983). Reliability <strong>and</strong>validity <strong>of</strong> the direct observational form <strong>of</strong> the <strong>Child</strong>Behavior Checklist. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology,11, 521–530.Reich, W. (2000). Diagnostic interview for children <strong>and</strong>adolescents (DICA). Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy<strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 39, 59–66.Reich, W., & Earls, F. (1987). Rules for making psychiatricdiagnoses in children on the basis <strong>of</strong> multiplesources <strong>of</strong> information: Preliminary strategies. Journal<strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 1S, 601–616.Reich, W., Herjanic, B., Weiner, Z., & G<strong>and</strong>hy, P. R.(1982). Development <strong>of</strong> a structured psychiatric interviewfor children: Agreement in diagnosis comparingchild <strong>and</strong> parent interviews. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong>Psychology, 10, 325–336.Reitman, D., Currier, R. O., Hupp, S. D. A., Rhode, P.C., Murphy, M. A., & O’Callahan, P. M. (2001). Psychometriccharacteristics <strong>of</strong> the parenting Scale in aHead Start population. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology,30, 514–524.Rende, R., & Weissman, M. M. (1999). <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong>family history <strong>of</strong> psychiatric disorder. In D. Shaffer, C.P. Lucas, & J. E. Richters (Eds.), Diagnostic assessmentin child <strong>and</strong> adolescent psychopathology (pp. 230–255).New York: Guilford Press.Renk, K., & Phares, V. (2004). Cross-informant ratings<strong>of</strong> social competence in children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.<strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology Review, 24, 239–254.Reynolds, C. R., & Kaiser, S. (1990). Test Bias in psychologicalassessment. In C. R. Reynolds <strong>and</strong> R. W.Kamphaus (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong> educationalassessment <strong>of</strong> children. New York: Guilford.Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (1992). Behaviorassessment system. for children (BASC). Circle Pines,MN: American Guidance Services.Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (2004). Behaviorassessment system for children, 2nd edition (BASC-2).Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services.Reynolds, C. R., Kamphaus, R. W., & Hatcher, N. M.(1999). Treatment planning <strong>and</strong> evaluation with theBASC: The Behavior <strong>Assessment</strong> System for <strong>Child</strong>ren.In M. Maruish (Ed.) (2nd ed.), The use <strong>of</strong> psychologicaltesting for treatment planning <strong>and</strong> outcome assessment (pp.563–597). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Reynolds, C. R., & Richmond, B. O. (1985). Revisedchildren’s manifest anxiety scale (RCMAS). Los Angeles:Western Psychological Services.Reynolds, W. M. (1986a). A model for screening <strong>and</strong>identification <strong>of</strong> depressed children <strong>and</strong> adolescentsin school settings. Pr<strong>of</strong>essional School Psychology, 1,117–129.Reynolds, W. M. (1986b). Reynolds <strong>Adolescent</strong> DepressionScale. Odessa, FL: Psychological <strong>Assessment</strong>Resources.Reynolds, W. M. (1989). Reynolds <strong>Child</strong> Depression Scale.Odessa, FL: Psychological <strong>Assessment</strong> Resources.Reynolds, W. M. (1990). Introduction to the nature <strong>and</strong>study <strong>of</strong> internalizing disorders in children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.School Psychology Review, 19(2), 137–141.Reynolds, W. M. (1998). <strong>Adolescent</strong> psychopathology scale.Odessa, FL: Psychological <strong>Assessment</strong> Resources.Reynolds, W. M. (2002). Reynolds <strong>Adolescent</strong> DepressionScale, 2nd edition (RADS-2). Lutz, FL: Psychological<strong>Assessment</strong> Resources.Rhoades, K. A. & O’Leary, S. G. (2007). Factor structure<strong>and</strong> validity <strong>of</strong> the Parenting Scale. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong><strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychology, 36, 137–146.Rice, F., Harold, G. T., & Thapar, A. (2002). The geneticaetiology <strong>of</strong> childhood depression: A review. Journal <strong>of</strong><strong>Child</strong> Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry, 43, 65–79.Riccio, C., & Reynolds, C. (2003). The assessment <strong>of</strong>attention via continuous performance tests. H<strong>and</strong>book<strong>of</strong> Psychological <strong>and</strong> Educational <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong><strong>Child</strong>ren: <strong>Personality</strong>, Behavior, <strong>and</strong> Context (2nd ed.,pp. 291–319). New York: Guilford Press.Richardson, R. D., & Burns, M. K. (2005). AdaptiveBehavior <strong>Assessment</strong> System (2nd edition) by Harrison,P. L, & Oakl<strong>and</strong>, T. (2002). San Antonio, TX:Psychological Corporation. <strong>Assessment</strong> for EffectiveIntervention, 30, 51–54.Richmond, B. O., & Kicklighter, R. H. (1979). <strong>Child</strong>ren’sadaptive behavior scale. Atlanta, GA: Humantics Limited.Richters, J. E. (1992). Depressed mothers as informantsabout their children A critical review <strong>of</strong> the evidence<strong>of</strong> distortion. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 485–499.


eferenes483Ricks, J. H. (1959). On telling parents about test results.Test Service Bulletin, 54, 1–4.Roberts, B. W. & DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The rankorderconsistency <strong>of</strong> personality traits from childhoodto old age: A quantitative review <strong>of</strong> longitudinal studies.Psychological Bulletin, 126, 3–25.Roberts, C., McCoy, M., Reidy, D., & Crucitti, F.(1993). A comparison <strong>of</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> assessing adaptivebehaviour in pre-school children with developmentaldisabilities. Australia <strong>and</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong> Journal<strong>of</strong> Developmental Disabilities, 18, 261–272.Roberts, G. C., Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1984). Continuity<strong>and</strong> change in parents’ child-rearing practices.<strong>Child</strong> Development, 55, 586–597.Roberts, M. A., Milich, R., & Loney, J. (1984). Structuredobservations <strong>of</strong> academic <strong>and</strong> play settings (SOAPS).Iowa City, IA: University <strong>of</strong> Iowa.Roberts, M. A., Ray, R. S., & Roberts, R. J. (1984). Aplayroom observational procedure for assessing hyperactiveboys. Journal <strong>of</strong> Pediatric Psychology, 9, 177–191.Roberts, R. E., Roberts, C. R., & Xing, Y. (2007). Rates<strong>of</strong> DSM-IV psychiatric disorders among adolescentsin a large metropolitan area. Journal <strong>of</strong> PsychiatricResearch, 41, 959–967.Robertson, D., & Hyde, J. (1982). The factorial validity<strong>of</strong> the Family Environment Scale. Educational <strong>and</strong>Psychological Measurement, 42, 1233–1241.Robins, L. N. (1966). Deviant children grown up. Baltimore,MD: Williams <strong>and</strong> Wilkins.Robins, L. N., Helzer, J. E., Croughan, J., & Ratcliff, K. S.(1981). The National Institute <strong>of</strong> Mental Health DiagnosticInterview Schedule: Its history, characteristic <strong>and</strong>validity. Archives <strong>of</strong> General Psychiatry, 38, 381–389.Robins, R. W., John, O. P., Caspi, A., M<strong>of</strong>fitt, T. E., &Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1996). Resilient, overcontrolled,<strong>and</strong> undercontrolled boys: Three replicablepersonality types. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology,70, 157–171.Robinson, E. A., & Eyberg, S. M. (1981). The DyadicParent <strong>Child</strong> Interaction Coding System: St<strong>and</strong>ardization<strong>and</strong> validation. Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong>Psychology, 49, 245–250.Rockett, J. L., Murrie, D. C., & Boccacini, M. T. (2007).Diagnostic labeling in juvenile justice settings: Dopsychopathy <strong>and</strong> conduct disorder findings influenceclinicians? Psychological Services, 4, 107–122.R<strong>of</strong>f, J. D., & Wirt, R. D. (1984). <strong>Child</strong>hood aggression<strong>and</strong> social adjustments as antecedents <strong>of</strong> delinquency.Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 12, 111–126.Roid, G. (2003). Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale:Fifth Edition. Itasca, IL: Riverside.Rorschach, H. (1921). Psychodiagnostik. Bern: Bircher.Rorschach, H. (1951). Psychodiagnostics: A diagnostictest based on perception (P. Lemkau & B. Kronenberg,Trans.). New York: Grune & Stratton.Rosenthal, G. (1996). The Behavior <strong>Assessment</strong> System for<strong>Child</strong>ren: Locus <strong>of</strong> control scale. <strong>Child</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong> News,5(5), 1–5, 12.Ross, A. O. (1981). <strong>Child</strong> behavior therapy: Principles,procedures, <strong>and</strong> empirical basis. New York: Wiley.Rotter, J. B., & Rafferty, J. E. (1950). Manual for theRotter In-complete Sentences Blank, College Form. NewYork: The Psychological Corporation.Rourke, B. P., & Strang, J. D. (1983). Subtypes <strong>of</strong> reading<strong>and</strong> arithmetical disabilities: A neuropsychologicalanalysis. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Developmental neuropsychiatry(pp. 473–488). New York: Guilford Press.Rounsaville, B. J., Alarcon, R. D., Andrews, G., Jackson,J. S., Kendell, R. E., & Kendler, K. (2002). Basicnomenclature issues for DSM-V. In D. J. Kupfer, M.B. First, & D. E. Regier (Eds.), A research agenda forDSM-V (pp. 1–30). Washington, DC: American PsychiatricAssociation.Routh, D. K., & Ernst, A. R. (1983). Somatization disorderin relatives <strong>of</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents withfunctional abdominal pain. Journal <strong>of</strong> Pediatric Psychology,9, 427–437.Rowe, E. W., Circle, E. L., Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus,R. W. (1998). Behavior <strong>Assessment</strong> System for<strong>Child</strong>ren (BASC) Monitor for ADHD: An introductionto applications in practice. The ADHD Report,6(6), 113–129.Rucker, C. N. (1967). Technical language in the school psychologists’report. Psychology in the Schools, 4, 146–150.Rudolph, K. D., Hammen, C., & Burge, D. (1994).Interpersonal functioning <strong>and</strong> depressive symptomsin childhood: Addressing the issues <strong>of</strong> specificity <strong>and</strong>comorbidity. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 22,355–373.Ruffalo, S. L., & Elliott, S. N. (1997). Teachers’ <strong>and</strong> parents’ratings <strong>of</strong> children’s social skills: A closer look atcross-informant agreements through an item analysisprotocol. School Psychology Review, 26, 489–501.Rutter, M. (1983). Behavioral studies: Questions <strong>and</strong> findingson the concept <strong>of</strong> a distinctive syndrome. In M.Rutter (Ed.), Developmental neuropsychiatry (pp. 259–280). New York: Guilford.Rutter, M. (1999). Forward. In D. Shaffer, C. P. Lucas,& J. E. Lucas (Eds.), Diagnostic assessment in child<strong>and</strong> adolescent psychopathology (pp. xii–xiv). New York:Guilford.Rutter, M., & Garmezy, N. (1983). Developmental psychopathology.In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), Manual<strong>of</strong> child psychology, IV: Social <strong>and</strong> personality development(pp. 775–912). New York: Wiley.Rutter, M., & Giller, H. (1983). Juvenile delinquency:Trends <strong>and</strong> perspectives. London: Penguin.Rutter, M., Chadwick, O., & Shaffer, D. (1983). Headinjury. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Developmental neuropsychiatry(pp. 83–111). New York: Guilford.Saavedra, L. M., & Silverman, W. K. (2002). Classification<strong>of</strong> anxiety disorders in children: What a differencetwo decades make. International Review <strong>of</strong>Psychiatry, 14, 87–100.Saemundsen, E., Magnusson, P., Smari, J., & Sigurdardottir,S. (2003). Autism Diagnostic Interview


484 references– Revised <strong>and</strong> the <strong>Child</strong>hood Autism Rating Scale:Convergence <strong>and</strong> discrepancy in diagnosing autism.Journal <strong>of</strong> Autism <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disorders, 33,319–328.S<strong>and</strong>berg, S. T., Wieselberg, M., & Shaffer, D. (1980).Hyperkinetic <strong>and</strong> conduct problem children in a primaryschool population: Some epidemiological considerations.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, Psychiatry <strong>and</strong>Applied Disciplines, 21, 293–311.S<strong>and</strong>ers, M. R., & Dadds, M. R. (1989). Depressionobservation schedule: An observers manual. Brisbane,Australia: University <strong>of</strong> Queensl<strong>and</strong>.S<strong>and</strong>ers, M. R., Dadds, M. R., Johnston, B. M., & Cash,R. (1992). <strong>Child</strong>hood depression <strong>and</strong> conduct disorder:I. Behavioral, affective, <strong>and</strong> cognitive aspects <strong>of</strong> familyproblem-solving interactions. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal Psychology,101, 495–504.S<strong>and</strong>ers, M. R., Markie-Dadds, C., Tully, L. A., & Bor,W. (2000). The Triple P-Positive Parenting Program:A comparison <strong>of</strong> enhanced, st<strong>and</strong>ard, <strong>and</strong> self-directedbehavioral family intervention for parents <strong>of</strong> childrenwith early onset conduct problems. Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting<strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 68, 624–640.S<strong>and</strong>oval, J., & Ech<strong>and</strong>ia, A. (1994). Behavior assessmentsystem for children. Journal <strong>of</strong> School Psychology,32, 419–425.S<strong>and</strong>oval, J., & Mille, M. (1979, September). Accuracyjudgments <strong>of</strong> WISC-R item difficulty for minority groups.Paper presented at the annual meeting <strong>of</strong> the AmericanPsychological Association, New York.S<strong>and</strong>strom, M. J., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2006). Likeableversus popular: Distinct implications for adolescentadjustment. International Journal <strong>of</strong> Behavioral Development,30, 305–314.Sanford, K., Bingham, C. R., & Zucker, R. A. (1999).Validity issues with the Family Environment Scale:Psychometric resolution <strong>and</strong> research applicationwith alcoholic families. Psychological <strong>Assessment</strong>, 11,315–325.Sanger, M., MacLean, W., & Van Slyke, D. (1992).Relation between maternal characteristics <strong>and</strong> childbehavior ratings: Implications for interpreting behaviorchecklists. <strong>Clinical</strong> Pediatrics, 31, 461–466.Sarason, I.G., Johnson, J.H, & Siegel, J.M. (1978).Assessing the impact <strong>of</strong> life changes: Development<strong>of</strong> the Life Experiences Survey. Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting<strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 46, 932–946.Sargent, J., Liebman, R., & Silber, M. (1985). Familytherapy for anorexia nervosa. In D. M. Garner& P. E. Garfinkel (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> psychotherapyfor anorexia nervosa <strong>and</strong> bulimia (pp. 257–279). NewYork: Guilford Press.Sattler, J. M. (1988). <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> children (3rd ed.). SanDiego, CA: Author.Scahill, L., Schwab-Stone, M., Merikangas, K. R.,Leckman, J. F., Zhang, H., & Kasl, S. (1999). Psychosocial<strong>and</strong> clinical correlates <strong>of</strong> ADHD in a communitysample <strong>of</strong> school-age children. Journal <strong>of</strong> theAmerican Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry,38, 976–984.Schachar, R., Rutter, M., & Smith, A. (1981). The characteristics<strong>of</strong> situationally <strong>and</strong> pervasively hyperactivechildren: Implications for syndrome definition. Journal<strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry, 22, 375–392.Schaughency, E. A., & Lahey, B. B. (1985). Mothers’<strong>and</strong> fathers’ perceptions <strong>of</strong> child deviance: Roles <strong>of</strong>child behavior, parental depression, <strong>and</strong> marital satisfaction.Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology,53, 718–723.Schaughency, E. A., & Rothlind, J. (1991). <strong>Assessment</strong><strong>and</strong> classification <strong>of</strong> attention-deficit hyperactivitydisorder. School Psychology Review, 20, 187–202.Schinka, J. A., Elkins, D. E., & Archer, R. P. (1998).Effects <strong>of</strong> psychopathology <strong>and</strong> demographic characteristicson MMPI-A scale scores. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong><strong>Assessment</strong>, 71, 295–305.Schniering, C. A., & Rapee, R. M. (2002). Development<strong>and</strong> validation <strong>of</strong> a measure <strong>of</strong> children’s automaticthoughts: The <strong>Child</strong>ren’s Automatic Thoughts Scale.Behaviour Research <strong>and</strong> Therapy, 40, 1091–1109.Scholte, E., Van Berckelaer-Onnes, I., & Van der Ploeg,J. (2008). Rating scale to screen symptoms <strong>of</strong> psychiatricdisorders in children. European Journal <strong>of</strong> SpecialNeeds Education, 23, 47–62.Schorre, B. E. H., & V<strong>and</strong>vik, I. H. (2004). Globalassessment <strong>of</strong> psychosocial functioning in child <strong>and</strong>adolescent psychiatry: A review <strong>of</strong> three unidimensionalscales (CGAS, GAF, GAPD). European <strong>Child</strong><strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 13, 273–286.Schwab-Stone, M., Fisher, P., Piacentini, J., Shaffer, D.,Davies, M., & Briggs, M. (1993). The Diagnostic InterviewSchedule for <strong>Child</strong>ren – Revised Version (DISC-R): H. Test-retest reliability. Journal <strong>of</strong> the AmericanAcademy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 32, 651–657.Schwanz, K. A., & Kamphaus, R. W. (1997). <strong>Assessment</strong><strong>and</strong> diagnosis <strong>of</strong> ADHD. In W. N. Bender(Ed.), Underst<strong>and</strong>ing ADHD: A practical guide forparents <strong>and</strong> teachers. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill/Prentice-Hall.Schwean, V. L., Burt, K., & Sakl<strong>of</strong>ske, D. H. (1999). Correlates<strong>of</strong> mother- <strong>and</strong> teacher-ratings <strong>of</strong> hyperactivityimpulsivity<strong>and</strong> inattention in children with AD/HD.Canadian Journal <strong>of</strong> School Psychology, 15, 43–62.Scotti, J. R., Morris, T. L., McNeil, C. B., & Hawkins,R. P. (1996). DSM–IV <strong>and</strong> disorders <strong>of</strong> childhood <strong>and</strong>adolescence: Can structural criteria be functional?Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 64,1177–1191.Semrud-Clikeman, M. (1990). <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> childhooddepression. In C. R. Reynolds & Kamphaus, R. W.(Ed.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong> educational assessment<strong>of</strong> children: <strong>Personality</strong>, behavior, <strong>and</strong> context (pp.279–297). New York: Guilford Press.Semrud-Clikeman, M. (1991). ADHD <strong>and</strong> majordepression in children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. <strong>Child</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>News, 1, 6–7.


eferenes485Sevin, J. A., Matson, J. L., Coe, D. A., & Fee, V. F. (1991).A comparison <strong>and</strong> evaluation <strong>of</strong> three commonly usedautism scales. Journal <strong>of</strong> Autism <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disorders,21, 417–432.Shaevel, B., & Archer, R. P. (1996). Effects <strong>of</strong> PI-2 <strong>and</strong>MMPI-A norms on T-Score elevations for 18-yearolds.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>, 67(1), 72–78.Shaffer, D., Fisher, P. W., & Lucas, C. P. (1999).Respondent-based interviews. In D. Shaffer, C. P.Lucas, & J. E. Richters (Eds.), Diagnostic assessmentin child <strong>and</strong> adolescent psychopathology (pp. 3–33). NewYork: Guilford.Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Lucas, C. P., Dulcan, M. K., &Schwab-Stone, M. E. (2000). NLMH diagnosticinterview schedule for children version, IV IMHDISC-IV): Description, differences from previousversions, <strong>and</strong> reliability <strong>of</strong> some common diagnoses.Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong>Psychiatry, 39, 28–38.Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Piacentini, J., Schwab-Stone, M.,& Wicks, J. (1991). NIMH diagnostic interview schedulefor children-version 2.3. New York: New York StatePsychiatric Institute.Shaffer, D., Garl<strong>and</strong>, A., Gould, M., Fisher, P., & Trautman,P. (1988). Preventing teenage suicide: A criticalreview. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong><strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 27, 675–687.Shaffer, D., Gould, M. S., Brasic, J., Ambrosini, P.,Fisher, P., Bird, H., et al. (1983). A children’s globalassessment scale (CGAS). Archives <strong>of</strong> General Psychiatry,40, 1228–1231.Shaffer, D., Schwab-Stone, M., Fisher, P., Cohen,P., Piacentini, J., Davies, M., Conners, C. K., et al.(1993). The Diagnostic Interview Schedule for <strong>Child</strong>ren-RevisedVersion (DISC-R): I. Preparation, fieldtesting, interrater reliability, <strong>and</strong> acceptability. Journal<strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry,32, 643–650.Shapiro, E. S. (1987). Behavioral assessment in school psychology.Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Shapiro, E. S., & Skinner, C. H. (1990). Principles <strong>of</strong>behavioral assessment. In C. R. Reynolds & R. W.Kamphaus (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong> educationalassessment <strong>of</strong> children: <strong>Personality</strong>, behavior, <strong>and</strong>context (pp. 343–363). New York: Guilford Press.Sharp, C. & Kine, S. (2008). The assessment <strong>of</strong> juvenilepsychopathy: Strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses <strong>of</strong> currentlyused questionnaire measures. <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong>Mental Health, 13, 85–95.Shaw, J. G., Hammer, D., & Lel<strong>and</strong>, H. (1991). Adaptivebehavior <strong>of</strong> preschool children with developmentaldelays: Parent versus teacher ratings. MentalRetardation, 29, 49–53.Shelby, M. D., Nagle, R. J., Barnett-Queen, L. L., Quattlebaum,P. D., & Wuori, D. F. (1998). Parental reports<strong>of</strong> psychosocial adjustment <strong>and</strong> social competence inchild survivors <strong>of</strong> acute lymphocytic leukemia. <strong>Child</strong>ren’sHealth Care, 27(2), 113–129.Shellenberger, S., Dent, M. M., Davis-Smith, M., Seale,J. P., Weintraut, R., & Wright, T. (2007). Culturalgenogram: A tool for teaching <strong>and</strong> practice. Families,Systems, & Health, 25, 367–381.Shelton, K. K., Frick, P. J., & Wootton, J. (1996). Theassessment <strong>of</strong> parenting practices in families <strong>of</strong> elementaryschool-aged children. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong>Psychology, 25, 317–327.Sherrill, J. T., & Kovacs, M. (2000). Interview schedulefor children <strong>and</strong> adolescents (ISCA). Journal <strong>of</strong> theAmerican Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry,39, 67–75.Shiner, R., & Caspi, A. (2003). <strong>Personality</strong> differences inchildhood <strong>and</strong> adolescence: Measurement, development,<strong>and</strong> consequences. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology <strong>and</strong>Psychiatry, 44, 2–32.Shopler, E., Reichler, R. J., & Renner, R. R. (1988).<strong>Child</strong> autism rating scale. Los Angeles: Western PsychologicalServices.Sikora, D. M., Hall, T. A., Hartley, S. L., Gerrard-Morris,A. E., & Cagle, S. (2008). Does parent report <strong>of</strong>behavior differ across ADOS-G classifications: Analysis<strong>of</strong> scores from the CBCL <strong>and</strong> GARS. Journal <strong>of</strong>Autism <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disorders, 38, 440–448.Silverman, W. K. (1993). DSM <strong>and</strong> classification <strong>of</strong>anxiety disorders in children <strong>and</strong> adults. In C. G. Last(Ed.), Anxiety across the lifespan: A developmental perspective(pp. 7–36). New York: Springer.Silverman, W. K., & Albano, A. M. (1996). AnxietyDisorders Interview Schedule for <strong>Child</strong>ren for DSM-IV:(<strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> Parent Versions). San Antonio, TX: PsychologicalCorporation/Graywind.Silverman, W. K., & Nelles, W. B. (1989). The stability <strong>of</strong>mothers’ rating <strong>of</strong> child fearfulness. Journal <strong>of</strong> AnxietyDisorders, 3, 1–5.Silverman, W. K., & Ollendick, T. H. (2005). Evidencebasedassessment <strong>of</strong> anxiety <strong>and</strong> its disorders in children<strong>and</strong> adolescents. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong><strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychology, 34, 380–411.Silverman, W. K., & Rabian, B. (1999). Rating scalesfor anxiety <strong>and</strong> mood disorders. In D. Shaffer, C. P.Lucas, & J. E. Richters (Eds.), Diagnostic assessmentin child <strong>and</strong> adolescent psychopathology (pp. 127–166).New York: Guilford.Silverman, W. K., Saavedra, L. M., & Pina, A. A. (2001).Test–retest reliability <strong>of</strong> anxiety symptoms <strong>and</strong> diagnosesusing the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedulefor DSM-IV: <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> Parent Versions (ADIS forDSM-IV: C/P). Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong><strong>Child</strong> & <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 40, 937–944.Silverthorn, P., & Frick, P. J. (1999). Developmentalpathways to antisocial behavior: The delayed-onsetpathway in girls. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 11,101–126.Singleton, L. C., & Asher, S. R. (1979). Peer preferences<strong>and</strong> social interaction among third-grade children inan integrated school district. Journal <strong>of</strong> EducationalPsychology, 69, 330–336.


486 referencesSiskind, G. (1967). Fifteen years later: A replication <strong>of</strong>a semantic study <strong>of</strong> concepts <strong>of</strong> clinical psychologists<strong>and</strong> psychiatrists. Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychology, 65, 3–7.Skinner, B. F. (1963). Operant behavior. American Psychologist,18, 503–515.Smith, B. H., Barkley, R. A., & Shapiro, C. J. (2007). Attention-deficit/hyperactivitydisorder. In E. J. Mash & R. A.Barkley (Eds.), <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> childhood disorders (4th ed.).New York: Guilford.Smith, S. R. (2007). Making sense <strong>of</strong> multiple informantsin child <strong>and</strong> adolescent psychopathology: Aguide for clinicians. Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychoeducational <strong>Assessment</strong>,25, 139–149.Smith, S. R., Baity, M. R., Knowles, E. S., & Hilsenroth,M. J. (2001). <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> disordered thinking inchildren <strong>and</strong> adolescents: The Rorschach Perceptual-Thinking Index. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>, 77,447–463.Smolla, N., Valla, J., Bergeron, L., Berthiaume, C., &St-Georges, M. (2004). Development <strong>and</strong> reliability<strong>of</strong> a pictorial mental disorders screen for youngadolescents. The Canadian Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry, 49,828–837.Snyder, J. (1991). Discipline as a mediator <strong>of</strong> the impact<strong>of</strong> maternal stress <strong>and</strong> mood on child conduct problems.Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 3, 263–276.Snyder, M., Tanke, E. D., & Berscheid, F. (1977). Socialperception <strong>and</strong> interpersonal behavior on the self-fulfillingnature <strong>of</strong> social stereotypes. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong><strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 35, 656–666.Solis, M. L., & Abidin, R. R. (1991). The Spanish version<strong>of</strong> the Parenting Stress Index: A psychometric study.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 20, 372–378.Solomon, I. L., & Starr, B. D. (1968). School apperceptionmethod: SAM. New York: Springer.Sorneson, E., & Johnson, E. (1996). Subtypes <strong>of</strong> incarcerateddelinquents constructed via cluster analysis.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 37, 293–303.Sour<strong>and</strong>er, A., Helstelae, L., & Helenius, H. (1999).Parent-adolescent agreement on emotional <strong>and</strong>behavioral problems. Social Psychiatry <strong>and</strong> PsychiatricEpidemiology, 34, 657–663.South, M., Williams, B. J., McMahon, W. M., Owley,T., Filipek, P. A., Shern<strong>of</strong>f, E., et al. (2002). Utility<strong>of</strong> the Gilliam Autism Rating Scale in research <strong>and</strong>clinical populations. Journal <strong>of</strong> Autism <strong>and</strong> DevelopmentalDisorders, 32, 593–599.Southam-Gerow, M. A., & Chorpita, B. F. (2007). Anxietyin children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. In E. J. Mash & R.A. Barkley (Eds.), <strong>Assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> childhood disorders (4thed., pp. 347–397). New York: Guilford.Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment:New scales for assessing the quality <strong>of</strong> marriage <strong>and</strong>similar dyads. Journal <strong>of</strong> Marriage <strong>and</strong> the Family, 38,15–28.Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984).Vinel<strong>and</strong> adaptive behavior scales. Circle Pines, MN:American Guidance Services.Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Balla, D. A. (2005).Vinel<strong>and</strong> adaptive behavior scales (2nd ed.). CirclePines, MN: American Guidance Services.Speece, D. L., & Cooper, D. H. (1990). Ontogeny <strong>of</strong>school failure: Classification <strong>of</strong> first grade children.American Educational Research Journal, 27, 119–140.Spielberger, C. D. (1973). Manual for the State-TraitAnxiety Inventory for children. Palo Alto, CA: ConsultingPsychologists Press.Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E.(1970). STAI manual for the State-Trait Inventory. PaloAlto, CA: Consulting Psychologist Press.Spitzer, R., & Cantwell, D. (1980). The DSM-III classification<strong>of</strong> psychiatric disorders <strong>of</strong> infancy, childhood,<strong>and</strong> adolescence. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong><strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 19, 356–370.Spitzer, R. L., Davies, M., & Barkley, R. A. (1990). TheDSM-III-R field trials <strong>of</strong> disruptive behavior disorders.Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong>Psychiatry, 29, 690–697.Spivack, G., & Spotts, J. (1966). Devereux child behaviorrating scale. Devon, PA: The Devereux Foundation.Spivack, G., Spotts, J., & Haines, P. E. (1967). Devereuxadolescent behavior rating scale. Devon, PA: TheDevereux Foundation.Stanger, C., Dumenci, L., Kamon, J., & Burstein, M.(2004). Parenting <strong>and</strong> children’s externalizing problemsin substance-abusing families. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong><strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychology, 33, 590–600.Strakaki, C., & Ellis, J. (1989). The relationship <strong>of</strong>anxiety to depression in children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.In C. Stavrakaki (Ed.), The psychiatric clinics <strong>of</strong> NorthAmerica: Affective disorders <strong>and</strong> anxiety in the child <strong>and</strong>adolescent (pp. 777–789). Philadelphia, PA: W. B.Saunders.Steele, R. G., Nesbitt-Daly, J. S., Daniel, R. C., & Foreh<strong>and</strong>,R. (2005). Factor structure <strong>of</strong> the ParentingScale in a low-income African-American sample.Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> Family Studies, 14, 535–549.Stein, L. A. R., & Graham, J. R. (1999). Detecting fakegoodMMPI-A pr<strong>of</strong>iles in a correctional facility. Psychological<strong>Assessment</strong>, 11(3), 386–395.Stein, L., & Graham, J. (2005). Ability <strong>of</strong> substanceabusers to escape detection on the Minnesota Multiphasic<strong>Personality</strong> Inventory-<strong>Adolescent</strong> (MMPI-A) in a juvenile correctional facility. <strong>Assessment</strong>, 12,28–39.Steinberg, L., Lamborn, S., Dornbusch, S., & Darling,N. (1992). Impact <strong>of</strong> parenting practices on adolescentachievement: Authoritative parenting, schoolinvolvement, <strong>and</strong> encouragement to succeed. <strong>Child</strong>Development, 63, 1266–1281.Steinhausen, H. C., & Metzke, C. W. (1998). Youthself-report <strong>of</strong> behavioral <strong>and</strong> emotional problems in aSwiss epidemiological study. Journal <strong>of</strong> Youth <strong>and</strong> Adolescence,27, 429–441.Stone, W. L., Coonrod, E. E., Pozdol, S. L., & Turner, L.M. (2003). The Parent Interview for Autism – clinical


eferenes487version (PIA-CV): A measure <strong>of</strong> behavioral changefor young children with autism. Autism, 7, 9–30.Stone, W. L., & Hogan, K. L. (1993). A structuredparent interview for identifying young children withautism. Journal <strong>of</strong> Autism <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disorders,23(4), 639–652.Stoolmiller, M., Eddy, J. M., & Reid, J. B. (2000). Detecting<strong>and</strong> describing preventive intervention effects ina universal school-based r<strong>and</strong>omized trial targetingdelinquent <strong>and</strong> violent behavior. Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting<strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 68, 296–306Stowers-Wright, L. E. (2000). A behavioral cluster analysis<strong>of</strong> male juvenile defenders. Unpublished doctoral dissertation:The University <strong>of</strong> Georgia.Strauss, A. A., & Lehtinen, L. F. (1947). Psychopathology<strong>and</strong> education <strong>of</strong> the brain-injured child. New York:Grune & Stratton.Strauss, C. C. (1990). Overanxious disorder in childhood.In M. Hersen & C. G. Last (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book<strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong> adult psychopathology (pp. 237–246). NewYork: Pergamon Press.Strauss, C. C. (1993). Developmental differences inexpression <strong>of</strong> anxiety disorders in children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.In C. G. Last (Ed.), Anxiety across the lifespan:A developmental perspective (pp. 63–77). New York:Springer.Strauss, C. C., Lahey, B. B., Frick, P. J., Frame, C. L.& Hynd, G. W. (1988). Peer social status <strong>of</strong> childrenwith anxiety disorders. Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong>Psychology, 56, 137–141.Stredny, R. V. & Ball, J. D. (2005). The utility <strong>of</strong> the RorschachCoping Deficit Index as a measure <strong>of</strong> depression<strong>and</strong> social skills deficits in children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.<strong>Assessment</strong>, 12, 295–302.Sundberg, N. D. (1961). The practice <strong>of</strong> psychologicaltesting in clinical settings in the United States. AmericanPsychologist, 16, 79–83. Reprinted in L. D. Goodstein& R. I. Lanyon (Eds.), Readings in <strong>Personality</strong><strong>Assessment</strong>. New York: Wiley.Sundre, D. L. (1998). [Review <strong>of</strong> the Personnel ReactionBlank]. Thirteenth Mental Measurements Yearbook.Lincoln, NE: Buros Institute.Sutter, J., & Eyberg, S. M. (1984). Sutter-Eyberg studentbehavior inventory. Gainesville, FL: University <strong>of</strong>Florida.Suveg, C., Zeman, J., Flannery-Schroeder, E., & Cassano,M. (2005). Emotion socialization in families <strong>of</strong> childrenwith an anxiety disorder. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology,33, 145–155.Swensen, C. H. (1968). Empirical evaluations <strong>of</strong> humanfigure drawings: 1957–1966. Psychological Bulletin, 70,20–44. Reprinted in L. D. Goodstein & R. I. Lanyon(Eds.), Readings in <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>Assessment</strong>. New York:Wiley.Szapocznik, J., & Kurtines, W. (1980). Acculturation,biculturalism <strong>and</strong> adjustment among Cuban Americans.In A. M. Padilla (Ed.), Acculturation: Theory,models <strong>and</strong> some new findings (pp. 139–159). AmericanAssociation for the Advancement <strong>of</strong> ScienceSelected Symposium 39. Boulder, CO: Westview.Szapocznik, J., Scopetta, M. A., & Aranalde, M. A.(1978). Theory <strong>and</strong> measurement <strong>of</strong> acculturation.Interamerican Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychology, 12, 113–130.Szatmari, P., Saigal, S., Rosenbaum, P., & Campbell,D. (1993). Psychopathology <strong>and</strong> adaptive functioningamong extremely low birth weight children ateight years <strong>of</strong> age. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology,5, 345–357.Tallent, N. (1988). Psychological report writing (3rd ed.).Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Tallent, N. (1993). Psychological report writing (4th ed.).Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Tanaka, J. S., & Westerman, M. A. (1988). Commondimensions in the assessment <strong>of</strong> competence inschool-aged girls. Journal <strong>of</strong> Educational Psychology,80, 579–584.Tannock, R. (2000). Attention deficit disorders withanxiety disorders. In T. Brown (Ed.), Attention deficitdisorders <strong>and</strong> comorbidities in children, adolescents <strong>and</strong>adults (pp. 125–170). Washington, DC: AmericanPsychiatric PressTapscott, M., Frick, P. J., Wootton, J. & Kruh, I. (1996).The intergenerational link to antisocial behavior:Effects <strong>of</strong> paternal contact. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> FamilyStudies, 5, 229–240.Teglasi, H. (1983). Report <strong>of</strong> a psychological assessmentin a school setting. Psychology in the Schools, 20,466–479.Tellegen, A., & Ben-Porath, Y. S. (1992). The new uniformT-scores for the A[MPI-2: Rationale, derivation,<strong>and</strong> appraisal. Psychological <strong>Assessment</strong>, 4, 145–155.Teodori, J. B. (1993). Neurological assessment. In T. H.Ollendick & M. Hersen (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong>adolescent assessment (pp. 41–50). Boston, MA: Allyn<strong>and</strong> Bacon.Teufel-Shone, N., Staten, L. K., Irwin, S., Rawiel, U.,Bravo, A. B., & Waykayuta, S. (2005). Family cohesion<strong>and</strong> conflict in an American Indian community.American Journal <strong>of</strong> Health Behavior, 29, 413–422.Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament <strong>and</strong> development.New York: Bruner & Mazel.Thompson, C. E. (1949). Manual for thematic apperceptiontest: Thompson modification. Cambridge, MA:Harvard University Press.Thompson, L. L., Riggs, P. D., Mikulich, S. K., & Crowley,T. J. (1996). Contribution <strong>of</strong> ADHD symptomsto substance problems <strong>and</strong> delinquency in conductdisorderedadolescents. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology,24, 325–348.Thurber, S., & Hollingsworth, D. K. (1992). Validity<strong>of</strong> the Achenbach <strong>and</strong> Edelbrock Youth Self-Reportwith hospitalized adolescents. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong>Psychology, 21, 249–254.Tibbetts, S. G., & Piquero, A. R. (1999). The influence<strong>of</strong> gender, low birth weight, <strong>and</strong> disadvantaged environmentin predicting early onset <strong>of</strong> <strong>of</strong>fending: A test


488 references<strong>of</strong> M<strong>of</strong>fitt’s interactional hypothesis. Criminology, 37,843–877.Tillman, R., Geller, B., Craney, J. L., Bolh<strong>of</strong>ner, K.,Williams, M., Zimerman, B., Frazier, J., et al. (2003).Temperament <strong>and</strong> character factors in a prepubertal<strong>and</strong> early adolescent bipolar disorder phenotypecompared to attention deficit hyperactive <strong>and</strong> normalcontrols. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychopharmacology,13, 531–543.Tripp, G., Schaughency, E., & Clarke, B. (2006).Parent <strong>and</strong> teacher rating scales in the evaluation<strong>of</strong> attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Contributionto diagnosis <strong>and</strong> differential diagnosisin clinically referred children. Journal <strong>of</strong> Developmental& Behavioral Pediatrics, 27, 209–218.Trites, R. L., Bouin, A. G. A., & Laprade, K. (1982).Factor analysis <strong>of</strong> the Conners teacher rating scalebased on a large normative sample. Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting<strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 50, 615–623.Tsoubris, K. T. (1998). Relationship between prenatalcocaine exposure <strong>and</strong> behavioral patterns among preschoolchildren with disabilities. Dissertation AbstractsInternational Section B: The Sciences <strong>and</strong> Engineering,59(5-B), 2441.Tuma, J. M., & Elbert, J. C. (1990). Critical issues <strong>and</strong>current practice in personality assessment <strong>of</strong> children.In C. R. Reynolds & R. W. Kamphaus (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book<strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong> educational assessment <strong>of</strong> children:<strong>Personality</strong>, behavior, <strong>and</strong> context (pp. 3–29). NewYork: Guilford.Tupes, E. C., & Christal, R. E. (1961). Recurrent personalityfactors based on trait ratings. Technical ReportASD-TR-61–97. Lackl<strong>and</strong> Air Force Base, TX: U.S.Air Force.Vaillancourt, T., & Hymel, S. (2006). Aggression <strong>and</strong>social status: The moderating roles <strong>of</strong> sex <strong>and</strong> peervaluedcharacteristics. Aggressive Behavior, 32, 396–408.Valdez, G. M., & McNamara, J. R. (1994). Matching toprevent adoption disruption. <strong>Child</strong> & <strong>Adolescent</strong> SocialWork Journal, 11, 391–403.Valla, J. P., Bergeron, L., Bidaut-Russell, M., St., M., &Gaudet, N. (1997). Reliability <strong>of</strong> the Dominic-R: Ayoung child mental health questionnaire combiningvisual <strong>and</strong> auditory stimuli. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology<strong>and</strong> Psychiatry, 38, 717–724.Valla, J., Bergeron, L., & Smolla, N. (2000). The Dominic-R:A pictorial interview for 6- to 11-year-oldchildren. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong><strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 39, 85–93.Vance, F. I. (1965). I was an imaginary playmate.American Psychologist, 20, 990.Van Hasselt, V. B., Hersen, M., Bellack, A. S., Rosenblum,N. D., & Lamparski, D. (1979). Tripartiteassessment <strong>of</strong> the effects <strong>of</strong> systematic desensitizationin a multi-phobic child: An experimental analysis.Journal <strong>of</strong> Behavior Therapy <strong>and</strong> Experimental Psychiatry,10, 51–55.Van Horn, M. L., Atkins-Burnett, S., Karlin, E., Ramey,S. L., & Snyder, S. (2007). Parent ratings <strong>of</strong> children’ssocial skills: Longitudinal psychometric analyses <strong>of</strong>the Social Skills Rating System. School PsychologyQuarterly, 22, 162–199.Vasay, M. W., & Lonigan, C. J. (2000). Consideringthe clinical utility <strong>of</strong> performance-based measures <strong>of</strong>childhood anxiety. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology,29, 493–508.Vaughn, M. L., Riccio, C. A., Hynd, G. W., & Hall, J.(1997). Diagnosing ADHD subtypes: Discriminantvalidity <strong>of</strong> the Behavior <strong>Assessment</strong> System for <strong>Child</strong>ren(BASC) <strong>and</strong> the Achenbach paren <strong>and</strong> teacherrating scales. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 26,349–357.Verhulst, F. C., Koot, H. M., & Van der Ende, J. (1994).Differential predictive value <strong>of</strong> parents’ <strong>and</strong> teachers’reports <strong>of</strong> children’s problem behaviors: A longitudinalstudy. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 22,531–546.Victor, J. B. (1994). The five-factor model applied toindividual differences in school behavior. In C. F.Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.),The developing structure <strong>of</strong> temperament <strong>and</strong> personalityfrom infancy to adulthood (pp. 355–366). Hillsdale, NJ:Erlbaum.Vincent, G. M. (2006). Psychopathy <strong>and</strong> violence riskassessment in youth. <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> PsychiatricClinics <strong>of</strong> North America, 15, 407–428.Voelker, S. L., Shore, D. L., Brown-More, C., & Hill, L.C. (1990). Validity <strong>of</strong> self-report <strong>of</strong> adaptive behaviorskills by adults with mental retardation. Mental Retardation,28, 305–309.Waldman, I. D. & Gizer, I. R. (2006). The genetics <strong>of</strong>attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. <strong>Clinical</strong> PsychologyReview, 26, 396–432.Walker, C. E., Milling, L. S., & Bonner, B. L. (1988).Incontinence disorders: Enuresis <strong>and</strong> encopresis. InD. K. Routh (Ed.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> pediatric psychology(pp. 363–398). New York: Guilford Press.Walker, H. M., & McConnell, S. R. (1988). The Walker-McConnell scale <strong>of</strong> f social competence <strong>and</strong> school adjustment.Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Walker, J. L., Lahey, B. B., IIynd, G. W., & Frame, C. L.(1987). Comparison <strong>of</strong> specific patterns <strong>of</strong> antisocialbehavior in children with conduct disorder with or withoutcoexisting hyper-activity. Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong><strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 55, 666–678.Walker, J. L., Lahey, B. B., Russo, M. F., Frick, P. J., Christ,M. A. G., McBurnett, K., et al. (1991). Anxiety, inhibition,<strong>and</strong> conduct disorder in children: I. Relations tosocial impairment. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong><strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 30, 187–191.Walsh, C., MacMillan, H. L., & Jamieson, E. (2003).The relationship between parental substance abuse<strong>and</strong> child maltreatment: Findings from the OntarioHealth Supplement. <strong>Child</strong> Abuse <strong>and</strong> Neglect, 27,1409–1425.


eferenes489Wang, J. J. (1992, April). An analytical approach to generatingnorms for skewed normative distributions. Paperpresented at the Annual Meeting <strong>of</strong> the AmericanEducational Research Association, San Francisco.Ward, J. H. (1963). Hierarchical grouping to optimizean objective function. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American StatisticalAssociation, 58, 236–244.Waschbusch, D. A. (2002). A meta-analytic examination<strong>of</strong> comorbid hyperactive-impulsive-attentionproblems <strong>and</strong> conduct problems. Psychological Bulletin,128, 118–150.Wasielewski, S. M., Gridley, B. J., & Hall, J. (1998,April). Construct validity <strong>of</strong> the Behavior <strong>Assessment</strong>System for Correlations with the <strong>Personality</strong> Inventoryfor <strong>Child</strong>ren-Revised with a clinical sample. Paper presentedat the Annual Meeting <strong>of</strong> the National Association<strong>of</strong> School Psychologists, Orl<strong>and</strong>o.Watkins, C. E., Campbell, V. L., Nieberding, R., &Hallmark, R. (1995). Contemporary practice <strong>of</strong> psychologicalassessment by clinical psychologists. Pr<strong>of</strong>essionalPsychology: Research <strong>and</strong> Practice, 26, 54–60.Webster-Stratton, C., & Lindsay, D. W. (1999). Socialcompetence <strong>and</strong> conduct problems in young children:Issues in assessment. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong>Psychology, 28, 25–43.Wechsler, D. (1974). Wechsler intelligence scale for children- revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.Wechsler, R. D. (1991). Wechsler intelligence scale for children(3rd ed.). New York: Psychological Corporation.Weems, C. F., Hammond-Laurence, K., Silverman, W.K., & Ginsburg, G. S. (1998). Testing the utility <strong>of</strong> theanxiety sensitivity construct in children <strong>and</strong> adolescentsreferred for anxiety disorders. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong><strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 27, 69–77.Weems, C. F., Silverman, W. K., Rapee, R. R., & Pina, A.A. (2003). The role <strong>of</strong> control in childhood anxiety disorders.Cognitive Therapy <strong>and</strong> Research, 27, 557–568.Weems, C. F., & Stickle, T. R. (2005). Anxiety disordersin childhood: Casting a nomological net. <strong>Clinical</strong><strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> Family Psychology Review, 8, 107–134.Weiner, I. B. (1986). Assessing children <strong>and</strong> adolescentswith the Rorschach. In H. M. Kn<strong>of</strong>f (Ed.), The adjustment<strong>of</strong> child <strong>and</strong> adolescent personality (pp. 141–172).New York: Guilford Press.Weiner, I. B. (2001). Advancing the science <strong>of</strong> psychologicalassessment: The Rorschach Inkblot Method asexemplar. Psychological <strong>Assessment</strong>. 13, 423–432.Weiss, B., & Garber, G. (2003). Developmental differencesin the phenomenology <strong>of</strong> depression. Development<strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 15, 403–430.Weiss, B., & Weisz, J. R. (1988). Factor structure <strong>of</strong> selfreporteddepression: Clinic-referred children versus adolescents.Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal Psychology, 97, 492–495.Weiss, G., Hechtman, L., & Perlman, T. (1978). Hyperactivesas young adults: School, employer, <strong>and</strong> selfrating scales obtained during ten-year follow-upevaluation. American Journal <strong>of</strong> Orthopsychiatry, 48,438–445.Weissman, M. M., Warner, V., & Fendrich, M. (1990).Applying impairment criteria to children’s psychiatricdiagnosis. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong><strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry, 29, 789–795.Weissman, M. M., Warner, V., Wickramaratne, P., &Prus<strong>of</strong>f, B. A. (1988). Early-onset major depressionin parents <strong>and</strong> their children. Journal <strong>of</strong> Affective Disorders,15, 269–277.Weissman, M. M., Wickramaratne, P., Adams, P., Wolk,S., Verdeli, H., & Olfson, M. (2000). Brief screeningfor family psychiatric history: The family historyscreen. Archives <strong>of</strong> General Psychiatry, 57, 675–682.Weller, E. B., Weller, R. A., Fristad, M. A., Rooney,M. T., & Schecter, J. (2000). <strong>Child</strong>ren’s interviewfor psychiatric syndromes (ChIPS). Journal <strong>of</strong> theAmerican Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Adolescent</strong> Psychiatry,39, 76–84.Wender, P. H. (1995). Attention deficit disorder in adults.London: Oxford University.West, M. O., & Prinz, R. J. (1987). Parental alcoholism<strong>and</strong> childhood psychopathology. PsychologicalBulletin, 102, 204–218.Whalen, C. K., & Henker, B. (1998). Attention-deficithyperactivity disorder. In T. H. Ollendick & M.Hersen (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> child psychopathology (3rded., pp. 181–212). New York: Plenum Press.Whalen, C. K., Henker, B., & Dotemoto, S. (1980).Methylphenidate <strong>and</strong> hyperactivity: Effects on teacherbehaviors. Science, 208(4449), 1280–1282.Whalen, D. K., Henker, B., Jamner, L. D., Ishikawa, S.S., Floro, J. N., Swindle, R., et al. (2006). Toward mappingdaily challenges <strong>of</strong> living with ADHD: Maternal<strong>and</strong> child perspectives using electronic diaries. Journal <strong>of</strong>Abnormal <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 34, 115–130.Whipple, E.E. & Webster-Stratton, C. (1991). The role<strong>of</strong> parental stress in physically abusive families. <strong>Child</strong>Abuse & Neglect, 15, 2779–291.Whiteside, S. P., McCarthy, D. M. & Miller, J. D.(2007). An examination <strong>of</strong> the factor structure <strong>of</strong> theSocial Skills Rating System parent elementary form.<strong>Assessment</strong>, 14, 246–254.Whiteside-Mansell, L., Ayoub, C., McKelvey, L., Faldowski,R. A., Hart, A., & Shears, J. (2007). Parentingstress <strong>of</strong> low-income parents <strong>of</strong> toddlers <strong>and</strong>preschoolers: Psychometric properties <strong>of</strong> a short form<strong>of</strong> the Parenting Stress Index. Parenting: Science <strong>and</strong>Practice, 7, 27–56.Widaman, K. F., Stacy, A. W., & Borthwick-Duffy, S.A. (1993). Construct validity <strong>of</strong> dimensions <strong>of</strong> adaptivebehavior: A multitrait–multimethod evaluation.American Journal on Mental Retardation, 98, 219–234.Widiger, T. A., Frances, A. J., Davis, W., & First, M.(Eds.) (1994). DSM-IV sourcebook (Vol. 1). Washington,DC: American Psychiatric Association.Widiger, T. A., Frances, A. J., Pincus, H. A., Davis, W.W., & First, M. B. (1991). Toward an empirical classificationfor the DSM-IV. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal Psychology,100, 280–288.


490 referencesWidiger, T. A., Frances, A. J., Pincus, H. A., Ross, R.,First, M. B., Davis, W., et al. (1998). DSM-IV sourcebook:Vol. 4. Washington, DC: American PsychiatricAssociation.Wiener, J. (1985). Teachers’ comprehension <strong>of</strong> psychologicalreports. Psychology in the Schools, 22, 60–64.Wiender, I. B. (2001). Advancing the science <strong>of</strong> psychologicalassessment: The Rorschach Inkblot Method asexemplar. Psychological <strong>Assessment</strong>, 13, 423–432.Wiener, J., & Kohler, S. (1986). Parents’ comprehension<strong>of</strong> psychological reports. Psychology in the Schools, 23,265–270.Williams, C. L., Butcher, J. N., Ben-Porath, Y. S., &Graham, J. R. (1992). MMPI-A Content Scales: Assessingpsychopathology in adolescents. Minneapolis: University<strong>of</strong> Minnesota Press.Wingenfeld, S. A., Lachar, D., Gruber, C. P., & Kline,R. B. (1998). Development <strong>of</strong> the teacher-informantStudent Behavior Survey. Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychoeducational<strong>Assessment</strong>, 16, 226–249.Winokur, G. (1990). The concept <strong>of</strong> secondary depression<strong>and</strong> its relationship to comorbidity. In G. Winokur& R. B. Wesner (Eds.), The psychiatric clinics<strong>of</strong> North America: Anxiety <strong>and</strong> depression as secondaryphenomena (pp. 567–583). Philadelphia, PA: W. B.Saunders.Wirt, R. D., Lachar, D., Klinedinst, J. K., & Seat, P. S.(1984). Multidimensional description <strong>of</strong> child personality:A manual for the personality inventory for children.Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Wirt, R. D., Lachar, D., Klinedinst, J. K., & Seat, P. S.(1990). <strong>Personality</strong> inventory for children-1990 edition.Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Witt, J. C., Heffer, R. W., & Pfeiffer, J. (1990). Structuredrating scales: A review <strong>of</strong> self-report <strong>and</strong> informantrating processes, procedures, <strong>and</strong> issues. In C.R. Reynolds & R. W. Kamphaus (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book<strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong> educational assessment <strong>of</strong> children:<strong>Personality</strong>, behavior <strong>and</strong> context (pp. 364–394). NewYork: Guilford Press.Witt, J. C. & Jones, K. M. (1998). Review <strong>of</strong> the Behavior<strong>Assessment</strong> System for <strong>Child</strong>ren. In J. C. Impara& B. S. Plake (Eds.), The Thirteenth, Mental MeasurementsYearbook (pp. 131–133). Lincoln, NE: BurosInstitute.Wolf, T. H. (1966). Intuition <strong>and</strong> experiment: AlfredBinet’s first efforts in child psychology. Journal <strong>of</strong> History<strong>of</strong> Behavioral Sciences, 2, 233–239.Wolfe, D. A., Edwards, B., Manion, I., & Koverola, C.(1988). Early intervention <strong>of</strong> parents at risk for childabuse <strong>and</strong> neglect: A preliminary investigation. Journal<strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 56, 40–47.Woodcock, R. W., & Johnson, M. B. (1989). Woodcock-Johnson tests <strong>of</strong> achievement – revised. New York: Riverside.Woodin, M. F. (1999). Screening for the interfacebetween attention, executive functioning, <strong>and</strong> workingmemory: A cluster <strong>and</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ile analytic study. DissertationAbstracts International Section B: The Sciences<strong>and</strong> Engineering, 59(10-B), 5590.Woodworth, R. S. (1918). Personal data sheet. Chicago:Stoelting.Wootten, S. A. (1999). Attention-deficit/hyperactivitydisorder. Dissertation Abstracts International Section B:The Sciences & Engineering, 60(1-B), 0380.Wootton, J. M., Frick, P. J., Shelton, K. K., & Silverthorn,P. (1997). Ineffective parenting <strong>and</strong> childhoodconduct problems: The moderating role <strong>of</strong> callousunemotionaltraits. Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong>Psychology, 65, 301–308.Worchel, F. F., & Dupree, J. L. (1990). Projective storytellingtechniques. In C. R. Reynolds & R. W. Kamphaus(Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> psychological <strong>and</strong> educationalassessment <strong>of</strong> children: <strong>Personality</strong>, behavior, <strong>and</strong> context(pp. 70–88). New York: The Guilford Press.World Health Organization (1993). The ICD-10 classification<strong>of</strong> mental <strong>and</strong> behavioural disorders: Diagnostic criteriafor research. Geneva: World Health Organization.Wrobel, T. A., Lachar, D., Wrobel, N. H., Morgan, S.T., Gruber, C. P., & Neher, J. A. (1999). Performance<strong>of</strong> the <strong>Personality</strong> Inventory for Youth validity scales.<strong>Assessment</strong>, 6, 367–376.Wrobel, N., Lachar, D., & Wrobel, T. (2005). Selfreportproblem scales <strong>and</strong> subscales <strong>and</strong> behavioralratings provided by peers: Unique evidence <strong>of</strong> testvalidity. <strong>Assessment</strong>, 12, 255–269.Youngstrom, E., Izard, C., & Ackerman, B. (1999).Dysphoria-related bias in maternal ratings <strong>of</strong> children.Journal <strong>of</strong> Consulting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Clinical</strong> Psychology, 67,905–916.Zettergren, P. (2007). Cluster analysis in sociometricresearch: A pattern-oriented approach to identifyingtemporally stable peer status groups <strong>of</strong> girls. Journal <strong>of</strong>Early Adolescence, 27, 90–114.Zettergren, P., Bergman, L. R., & Wangby, M. (2006).Girls’ stable peer status <strong>and</strong> their adult adjustment: Alongitudinal study from age 10 to age 43. InternationalJournal <strong>of</strong> Behavioral Development, 30, 315–325.Zimmerman, I. L., & Woo-Sam, J. M. (1967). Reportingresults. In A. J. Glasser & I. L. Zimmerman (Ed.),<strong>Clinical</strong> interpretation <strong>of</strong> the Wechsler Intelligence Scalefor <strong>Child</strong>ren (WISC). New York: Grune & Stratton.Zimmerman, I. L., & Woo-Sam, J. M. (1985). <strong>Clinical</strong>applications. In B. B. Wolman (Ed.), H<strong>and</strong>book<strong>of</strong> intelligence: Theories, measurements, <strong>and</strong> applications(pp. 873–898). New York: Wiley.Zuckerman, M. (1990). Some dubious premises inresearch <strong>and</strong> theory on racial differences: Scientific,social, <strong>and</strong> ethical issues. American Psychologist, 45,1297–1303.


A u t h o r I n d e xAbad, J., 115Abbott, M.J., 428Abidin, 289, 291Abidin, R.R., 289, 291Abik<strong>of</strong>f, H., 400Ablow, J.C., 264Abramowitz, A.J., 397Achenbach, T.M., 15, 16, 18–20, 52, 61–63, 73, 89,103, 109, 110, 113–116, 126, 132, 146, 148,155, 156, 158–164, 170, 174, 178, 187, 200,201, 212, 235, 333, 334, 335, 336, 340, 346,350, 377, 386, 389, 413, 425,Acker, M.M., 280Adrien, J.L., 439Ageton, S.S., 345, 408Ahern, L.S., 288Albano, A.M., 256, 431Albert-Corush, J., 294Alderman, E., 72Allaire, J.C., 288Allen, J.C., 235Allen, M.J., 21Altepeter, T.S., 184Amador, J.A., 115Amato, P.R., 277, 291Ambrosini, P.J., 257, 415Ames, L.B., 11, 231Amorosa, H., 440Anastasi, 186, 226–228, 245Anastasi, A., 13, 21, 22, 34, 37, 39, 40, 44, 67, 76Anastopoulos, A.D., 382, 393, 394Anderson, J.C., 260Andrews, J., 22Angold, A., 256, 378, 380, 417Applegate, B., 380, 381Aranalde, M.A., 79Arbelaez, C., 416Archer, R., 122, 128Archer, R.P., 19, 121, 122, 125–128, 130, 235, 277,Arick, J.R., 446Arita, A.A., 126Arney, F., 286, 393Arnold, D.S., 280, 286Aronen, E.T., 115, 116Arseneault, L., 263Asher, S.R., 212–214, 217, 219, 223Aten, J.D., 305Austin, 291Baer, R.A., 121, 126Baghurst, P., 286Bailey, A., 438Bailey, J.M., 226, 227Baity, M.R., 236Baker, B.L., 285Baldwin, 78Baldwin, J.S, 429Ball, J.D., 229, 235, 341Balla, 316, 320–322Balla, D.A., 33, 316, 320–322Ballenger, J., 121Banez, 291Bao-Yu, W., 281Barckley, M., 22Barker, P., 89, 94, 95Barkley, R.A., 55, 184, 207, 279, 294, 378, 381–384,395, 397, 399Barlow, D.H., 427, 428Barnett, D.C., 42Barnett, J., 249Barrett, P.M., 295Barrios, B.A., 192, 198, 199, 204Barry, C.T., 116, 167Barth, J.M., 219Barthelemy, C., 439Bates, J.E., 291Bauer, D.H., 58Baumrind, D, 280Beck, A., 420Beck, S., 218Beck, S.T., 295, 296Behar, L.B., 413Belardinelli, C., 281Belevich, J.K.S., 125Bell, 78Bell, N.L., 240, 243Bellack, A.S., 196, 203Bellak, L., 11, 237491


492 author indexBellak, S.S., 11, 237Bellmore, A.D., 212Belter, R.W., 19, 229, 248Ben-Porath, Y.S., 29, 30, 120Berg, I., 429Bergman, L.R., 19, 212Berscheid, F., 51Bertr<strong>and</strong>, J., 437Beutler, L.E., 23Bickman, L., 23, 82Biederman, J., 62, 142, 294Bigras, M., 291Bingham, C.R., 282Bird, H.R., 62, 185, 344, 345Birmaher, B., 416Black, K., 130Black, M.M., 291Blader, J.C., 285Blair, R.J.R., 401Blashfield, R.K., 49Blum, G.S., 237Boccacini, M.T., 51Bock, S.J., 446Bodin, 167Boggs, S.R., 280Bohnert, A., 262Bolte, S., 317Bonner, B.L., 58Bor, R., 305Bor, W., 286Borgen, F.H., 42Bornstein, M.R., 196Borthwick-Duffy, S.A., 318, 336Bower, E.M., 18Bowlby, J., 58Boyd, C.P., 282Boyle, M.H., 384Boyle-Whitesel, M., 164Braaten, E., 358Bradford, D.C., 190Brakkc, N., 218Bravo, M., 74Breen, M.J., 184Brennan, 416Brent, D., 416Breslau, N., 282Bretzing, B., 30Briggs-Gowan, M.J., 276Brooks-Gunn, J., 346Brown, M., 226Brown, R.A., 115Brown, T.E., 393Brubaker, R.G., 285Bruininks, R.H., 320, 328, 329Bryson, S.E., 439Budd, K.S., 289Burge, D., 204Burke, J.D., 402Burns, G.L., 186Burns, M.K., 331Burns, R.C., 249Burstein, M., 285Burt, T., 282Buss, A.H., 57Butcher, J.N., 40, 41, 103, 117–123, 125, 126, 128Cabiya, J., 122Cagle, S., 443Campbell, 18Campbell, J., 446Campbell, J.P., 51Campbell, M., 443Campbell, S.B., 58, 381Campbell, V.L., 226Canino, G., 74Cantwell, 19Cantwell, D., 54Cantwell, D.P., 342, 345Capaldi, D., 402Capaldi, D.M., 358, 400Carlson, C.L., 144, 215, 379Caron, 19Carrion, V.G., 429Carroll, A., 190, 192, 193Carter, A.S., 229, 235, 276Carter, C.L., 229Cas-soano, M., 281Casat, C., 164Cashel, M., 126Caspi, A., 22Cassen, E.H., 417Castellanos, F.X., 384Cauce, A.M., 115Cavell, T.A., 407, 408Chaffin, M.J., 288Chambers, W., 258Ch<strong>and</strong>ler, L.A., 6, 7, 11Chorpita, B.F., 203, 204Christ, M.A.G., 381, 384Christ, M.G., 415, 417Christensen, A., 345Christian, R.C., 283Chung, K.M., 394Cicchetti, 306, 316, 320–322Cicchetti, D., 306, 385, 440Cicchetti, D.V., 46, 306, 316, 320–322, 439Cillessen, A.H.N., 212, 219Claiborn, C.D., 130Clarke, B., 142Clarkin, 62, 64Clerkin, S.M., 283, 284Coe, D.A., 441Cohen, J., 170, 343, 344Cohen, N.J., 16, 414


author index493Cohen, P., 76, 341, 342Coie, J.D., 214, 216–219, 221, 408Conners, C.K., 6, 104, 109, 110, 131–133, 138,139, 147–149, 170–174, 180, 181, 187,390–393, 413Conoley, J.C., 96Constantino, G., 237Cookus, B.A., 257, 263Coonrod, E.E., 440Cooper, D.K., 415, 419Coppotelli, H., 218Costa, N.M., 295Costa, P.T., 4, 62, 295Costantino, G., 76Costello, A.J., 265, 269Costello, E.J., 260, 269, 378, 417Costello, F.J., 380Courchesne, E, 438Cowan, C.P., 264Cowan, P.A., 264Coyne, J.C., 293Craighead, W.E., 49Crick, N.R., 401Crnic, K., 262Crooks, C.V., 278, 279Crouch, J.L., 288Crowley, T.J., 400Cuellar, L., 78Cumella, E.J., 126Cummings, E.M., 285Cummings, J.A., 11, 248, 249Cunningham, C.E., 384Curry, J.F., 49Cytryn, L., 49Dacey, C.M., 229, 235Dadds, M.R., 283, 285, 286, 295, 429Dahlstrom, W.G., 22Dana, R.H., 77, 78D<strong>and</strong>reaux, D.M., 283Danforth, J.S., 184Daniel, R.C., 286Darling, N., 280David-Ferdon, C., 191Davies, M., 55, 267, 399Davies, P.T., 285Davis, W.W., 16de Bildt, A., 443, 451De Groot, A., 115de Groot, C., 122De Haene, L., 289De Los Reyes, A., 142, 143, 275, 336, 345DeBacker, I., 384DeBaryshe, B.D., 356Delaney, H.D., 79DelVeccio, W.F., 60Denney, C.B., 394Desombre, H., 440DeStefano, L., 137, 138, 316–318, 329Dickens, C., 403, 410DiGiulio, G., 232DiGiuseppe, R., 237DiLavore, P.C., 193, 441Dishion, T.J., 402DiSibio, M., 317DiStefano, C., 42Dodge, K.A., 193, 196, 197, 205, 214, 218,219, 221, 291, 306, 401, 408Doleys, D.M., 58Doll, E.A., 315, 316, 321–323, 334Dopke, C.A., 288Dornbusch, S., 280Dougherty, L.R., 142, 301Downey, G., 293Dozois, D.J.A., 23DuBois, P.H., 6–8, 10, 11Duke, M., 280Dulcan, M.K., 407Dumas, J.E., 291Dumenci, L., 113, 158, 285Dunsterville, E., 288DuPaul, G.J., 184, 382, 386, 393, 394Durbin, C., 22Earls, F., 343, 345Eaves, R.C., 441Edelbrock, C., 15, 16, 20, 184, 201, 262,269, 345, 346Edelbrock, C.S., 144, 255, 269Edelsohn, G., 416Edwards, B., 289Egger, H., 378Egger, H.I., 380Eiraldi, R., 54Eisenstadt, T.H., 286, 288Elbert, J.C., 19Elgar, F.J., 283, 285Elkins, D.E., 121, 122, 125Elliot, D.S., 345, 408Elliott, S., 30Elliott, S.N., 332–334, 345Ellis, J., 417, 427Ellis, M.L., 401Emery, R.E., 292Endicott, J., 254Erdberg, P., 232Erickson, D.B., 291Erickson, M.T, 271Erikson, E.H., 60Erkanli, A., 280, 417Ernst, M., 257, 263Eron, L.D., 221Essau, C.A., 285, 410, 411Evans, C.S., 276, 345


494 author indexExner, J.E., 10, 12, 49, 126, 227, 229–236,250, 251, 346Eyberg, S., 194, 286Eyberg, S.M., 186, 280, 286–288Eyde, L.D., 67, 68Eysenck, H.J., 15Fabiano, G.A., 43, 383Fagan, P.J., 62Fann, K.D., 289Faraone, S.V., 294, 340Farmer, A.E., 295Farrell, J.M., 283Fee, V.F., 441Feighner, J.P., 254Feinfield, K.A., 285Feldman, E., 196, 197Fendrich, M., 185Ferdin<strong>and</strong>, R., 24, 160Fergusson, D.M., 19, 400–402Fiala, S.E., 407Fick, A.C., 401Fields, S., 82Figueroa, R.A., 75Filipek, P.A., 437Finch, A.J., 229, 248Firestone, P., 294First, M.B., 16, 293, 296Fischer, M., 294, 383, 384Fisher, L., 401Fisher, P., 258, 265, 267, 400, 407Flanagan, R., 237Flannery-Schroeder, E., 281Flens, 291Fletcher, J.M., 316Fletcher, K., 294, 382, 383Fletcher, M., 273Foley, D.L., 347Follett, G.M., 43Forbey, J.D., 122Foreh<strong>and</strong>, R., 218, 286, 292Forness, S.R., 18Forns, M., 115Forth, A.E., 411Fortin, L., 417Fowler, P., 282Fox, L.W., 273Frame, C.L., 216Frances, A.J., 16Francis, D.J., 316Francis, G., 6, 8, 295Franco, J.N., 78Fraser, J.A., 283Frazier, T.W., 440French, J.L., 7, 76Freud, S., 8, 11, 13Frick, P.J.,Friman, P.C., 115, 268Fuchs, D., 89, 217Fudge, H., 400Funderbunk, B., 286Gacono, C.B., 225Gadow, K.D., 146, 148, 166, 168, 169Gallucci, N.T., 126, 236Galton, F., 6, 18, 19Gao, H., 142Garb, H.N., 249Garber, G., 415Garber, J., 48Garbin, M.G., 296Garcia, M., 79Garl<strong>and</strong>, A., 400Garmezy, N., 48, 58Garrido, M., 122Geisinger, K.F., 77, 78Gelf<strong>and</strong>, D.M., 192Geller, B., 273, 293Gent, C.L., 201Gerrard-Morris, A.E., 443Gerson, R., 305Ghiselli, E.E., 51, 61Giampino, T.L., 383Gibbon, M., 296Gilliam, J.E., 443, 446Ginsburg, G.S., 429Gittelman-Klein, R., 226, 236, 383Gizer, I.R., 384Glueck, E., 294Glueck, S., 294Glutting, J.J., 41Goldberg, L.R., 4Goldsmith, H.H., 5Goldstein, 54Goodlin-Jones, B., 316, 438Goodman, J.T., 277Goodman, S.H., 293Goodman, W.K., 432Gordon, M., 393Gorsuch, R.L., 39, 296Gotlib, I.H., 293Gotlieb, H., 16Gottesman, I.I., 295Goudena, P.P., 237Gould, M., 400Gould, M.S., 62, 344, 345Graham, J., 121Graham, J.R., 121Green, K., 218Green, M., 302, 313Green, S., 384Green, S.M., 96, 142, 222, 342, 344, 381, 410Greenspan, S., 318Gresham, F.M., 214, 217, 219, 223Gresham, R.M., 332–334Grills, A.E., 428


author index495Grimes, C.L., 219Grimm, J., 399Gross, L.M., 294Grubb, H.J., 295Gruber, C., 102, 136, 147, 174, 176, 177, 276, 391,Gruber, C.P., 75, 104, 133–138, 149, 178, 180, 276,352, 391, 405Guevremont, I.C., 382, 394Gulab, N., 420Gullone, E., 282Gutterman, E.M., 258Haak, R.A., 11, 227, 244, 245Hagan, M., 420Hale, R.L., 7, 76Haley, G.M.T., 415Hall, J., 154Hall, T.A., 443Hallmark, R., 226Halperin, J.M., 283Hamilton, C., 217, 220, 420Hammen, C., 204, 293, 417Hammer, D., 321Hammer, E.F., 248Hammond-Laurence, K., 429Hampson, S., 22H<strong>and</strong>, C., 122H<strong>and</strong>el, R., 122H<strong>and</strong>el, R.W., 120, 130H<strong>and</strong>elsman, J.B., 82H<strong>and</strong>werk, M.L., 115Hankin, B.L., 415, 417, 418Hann, D.M., 401Hare, R.D., 406, 410, 411Harold, G.T., 416Harrington, R.G., 43Harris, F.C., 190Harris, I.C., 78Harrison, P.L., 317, 321, 330, 331Hart, D.H., 243, 244, 246Hart, E.L., 25, 142. 161, 401Hartdagen, S., 401Hartley, S.L., 443Hartmann, D.P., 190, 192, 204Harvey, V.S., 365Hasegawa, C., 77Haskett, M.E., 288, 289, 291Hathaway, S.R., 12, 117, 118, 121, 296Hawes, D.J., 285Hawkins, R.P., 23Hayden, E., 22Hayes, S.C., 44, 317Haynes, 19Hays, S., 120Hayvren, M., 214Haz, A.M., 289Hedrick, M., 292Heffer, 142Heilman, N.E., 289Helenius, H., 115Hellinckx, W., 286Helms, 78Helstelae, L., 115Helzer, J.E., 295, 296Hembree-Kigin, T.L., 288Hendren, R.L., 384Henker, B., 384Herr, 416Herron, D.G., 77Hersen, M., 89, 196, 199, 203, 295Hicks, M.M., 126Hill, B.K., 320, 328Hill, J., 400Hiller, J.B., 229, 233Hilsenroth, M.J., 236Hinshaw, S.P., 63, 383, 384, 400Hodges, K., 255, 258, 261Hogan, K.L., 440Hojnoski, R.L., 226Holaday, M., 236, 243, 244Holden, 291Hollifield, J., 235Hollingsworth, D.K., 115Holmes, R., 203Holtzman, W.H., 231Hooks, M., 439Hoover, H.D., 30, 37, 341Horwood, L.J., 19, 401Houghton, S., 190Howell, C.T., 61, 63, 155, 212, 340Howlin, P., 439Hoza, B., 112, 212Huang, L.N., 77Huberty, C.J., 42Hudziak, J.J., 18Huesmann, L.R., 221Hughes, J., 217, 218Hughes, J.N., 408Hughes, T.L., 225Huizinga, D., 343, 408Hunsley, J., 23, 43, 44, 185, 186, 226, 227, 254,260, 359Hutcheson, J.J., 291Huth-Bocks, A., 420Hutt, M.L., 237Hyde, J., 282Hymel, S., 214, 217, 219, 223Hynd, C.W., 401Hynd, G.W., 154, 216Inclan, J.E., 77Isaacs, P., 394Ivanova, M.Y., 16, 113Jackson, M.F., 219Jackson, Y.K., 272


496 author indexJamieson, E., 294Janus, M.D., 122Jarrett, R.B., 44Jasso, R., 78Jensen, P.S., 86, 261, 262, 380Jersild, A.T., 203Jiang, X.L., 219Johnson, 288Johnston, 288Johnston, C., 384Joiner, T.E., 249Jung, C.J., 8Kaczmarek, T., 420Kagan, J., 51, 54Kaiser, S., 73–75Kamon, J., 285Kamphaus, R.W., 4, 6, 16, 18, 22, 23, 29, 30, 32, 36,39, 40, 42, 69, 73–76, 102, 103, 105–110, 145,146, 148, 150–155, 201–203, 277, 308, 315,317, 335, 346, 374, 390, 396, 413, 419Kane, D., 289Kaplan, D., 41, 44Karustis, J., 54Karver, M.S., 82, 341, 347Kasen, S., 76Kashani, J.H., 345Kashner, T.M., 260Kaslow, N.J., 191Kaufman, A.S.,358, 396Kaufman, N.L., 358Kaufman, S.H., 35, 249Kazdin, A.E., 190, 193, 259, 275, 295, 340, 347Keating, C.F., 195, 196Keenan, K., 61, 62, 294, 340, 400, 416Keenan-Miller, 416Keith, B., 277, 291Keith, T.Z., 41Keller, H.R., 191, 197, 199Keller, P.S., 285Kendall, P.C., 62, 64, 432Kennedy, C., 72Kerr, D., 24, 164Kerr-Almcida, N., 126Kershner, J., 16Kettler, R., 420Kicklighter, 321Kim, J.A., 439Kimonis, E., 87, 89, 378, 399, 410Kimonis, E.R., 283King, N.J., 61Klein, D., 22Klein, D.N., 39, 142, 226, 236, 260, 301, 303,414–417, 423, 424, 435Klein, R.G., 400Kleinmuntz, B., 3, 7, 9, 12, 14, 15, 22Klin, A., 440Kline, R.B., 130, 149, 159, 178, 180, 391Kine, S., 410Klinedinst, J.K., 174Kn<strong>of</strong>f, H.M., 228, 245, 246, 250Knowles, E.S., 236Koenig, K., 440Kohler, S., 364Kolen, M.J., 30, 37, 341Konig, P.H., 383Konold, T.R., 340Koot, H.M., 115Koppitz, E.M., 11, 230, 245–249Kosson, D.S., 410Kovacs, M., 23, 38, 48, 255, 415, 418, 419Kratochwill, T.R., 203Krefetz, D., 420Krishnamurthy, R., 126, 128Kritzer, J., 18Kroll, L.S., 121Kroon, N., 237, 238Krug, D.A., 446Kruh, I, 294Kruse, S.J., 305Kubu, C.S., 440Kupersmidt, J.B., 216, 219Kuramoto, H., 115Kurkela, S.A., 116Kurtines, W., 79Lachar, D., 14, 75, 102, 105, 133–138, 141, 147,149, 174, 176–181, 276, 352, 391, 405LaCombe, J.A., 42LaFrenier, P.J., 291Lahey, B.B., 25, 53, 54, 56, 96, 142, 144, 161, 185,190, 215, 216, 222, 342, 344, 350, 378–381,383, 384, 386, 399, 401, 402, 410Lambert, M., 186, 187Lamborn, S., 280Lamparski, D., 203L<strong>and</strong>au, S., 207Lansing, M.D., 444Larzelere, R., 115Last, C.G., 429Le Couteur, A., 440LeBreton, J.M., 288Lee, S.S., 380Lefkowitz, M.M., 221, 222, 422Lega, L.I., 79Lel<strong>and</strong>, H., 321Lenhart, L.A., 401Lett, N.J., 203Lewczyk, C.M., 268Lewinsohn, P.M., 342, 417Li, F., 402Lichtenberger, E.O., 358, 365, 369–371, 373Liebman, R., 273Lilienfeld, S.O., 273Lin-Yan, S., 281Lindsay, D.W., 408


author index497List-Kerz, M.L., 190Little, S.G., 214, 217, 219, 223Lochman, J.E., 219, 401, 408Locke, H.J., 292Loeber, R., 96, 142, 144, 222, 261, 342, 344, 345,381, 399–402, 410Loney, B.R., 61, 206, 294, 400–402, 406, 408Loney, J., 207, 380Long, N., 292Lonigan, C.J., 203, 204Lord, C., 193, 440–442Lorr, M., 15Lovejoy, M.C., 288Luby, J.L., 416Lucas, C.P., 407Lucia, V.C., 282Lucio, E., 122Luckasson, R., 316, 336Lundahl, B.W., 288Lunkenheimer, E., 24Lushene, R.E., 296Lynam, D.R., 5Lynch, K.D., 130Lyneham, H.J., 428Lynskey, M.T., 400Lyon, G.R., 316Maccoby, E.E., 289MacKenzie, E.P., 292MacMillan, H.L., 294Madsen, C.J., 398Madson, M.B., 305Magnusson, D., 19Magnusson, P., 441Malgady, R.G., 76, 237Malik, M., 23Manion, I., 289Mann, B.J., 292Mannuzza, S., 383March, J.S., 431Marchant, G.J., 137Marcotte, D., 417Marcus, L.M., 444Margolin, G., 320Marin, 78Marin, G., 79Markie-Dadds, C., 286Marks, D.J., 283Marsee, M.A., 402, 409Martin, L.S., 230, 231Martin, R.P., 3, 5, 13, 19, 22, 30, 32, 60, 61, 76, 79,83, 84, 230, 231, 250, 289Martinez, S.R., 79Martorell, B., 115Mash, E., 23, 43, 44, 185, 186, 254, 260, 359Mash, E.J., 23, 190, 191, 198, 384, 385Mason, B., 234, 346Massetti, G.M., 43, 383, 384Masuda, 78Mather, N., 358Matson, J.L., 441Matsumoto, 78Matthews, W., 226Mattison, R.E., 416Maujean, A., 283Mayes, L., 439Mayeux, L., 212, 214Mazefsky, C., 443McArthur, D.S., 227, 230, 236–240, 242McBurnett, K., 285, 380, 401McCallum, R., 120McCarney, S.B., 393McCarthy, 334McCarthy, L., 125McClaskey, C.L., 196, 197McConaughy, S.H., 61, 63, 89, 155, 161, 200, 201,212, 235, 340McConnell, S.R., 214, 216, 223McCoy, M.G., 401McCrae, R.R., 4, 62, 295McDonald, K.L., 219McGoldrick, M., 305McGrew, K.S., 329McGuffin, P., 295McKinley, J.C., 12, 117, 296McKnew, D.H., 49McMahon, R.J., 110, 273, 279, 292, 296, 399, 404,406–409McNeil, C.B., 23, 286, 288McReynolds, 15Measelle, J.R., 264Meehan, B.T., 408Meehl, 121Meehl, P, 12, 13Mendoza, R.H., 79Meredith, 78Mermelstein, R., 417Merrell, K.W., 332, 422Metevia, L., 394Metraux, R., 11Metzke, C.W., 115Meyer, G.J., 232Mikami, A.Y., 384Mikulich, S.K., 400Mildenberger, K., 440Milich, R., 196, 207Mille, 74Miller, 330Miller, L.C., 413Milling, L.S., 58Milliones, J., 78Milner, B., 441Milner, J.S., 288, 289Mischel, W., 60M<strong>of</strong>fitt, T.E., 60, 401–403Monuteaux, M.C., 294


498 author indexMoore, S.G., 216Moos, B.S., 280–282Moos, R.H., 280–282Moravec, B.C., 257, 263Morgan, C.J., 115Morgenthaler, W., 9, 10Morris, A.S., 403Morris, R.D., 316Morris, R.J., 203Morris, T.L., 23Morrison, R., 226Morsbach, S.K., 279Mullins, S.M., 288Munoz, L.C., 410Murray, H.A., 9, 228, 230, 237Murrie, D.C., 51Myers, K., 419, 421Myles, B.S., 446Nagle, R.J., 240, 243Needleman, G.L., 282Neeper, R., 215, 350, 386Negy, C., 136Neiberding, R., 226Nelles, W.B., 61Nelson, M.M., 280Nelson, R.O., 44Nesbitt-Daly, J.S., 286Newcomb, K., 286Newsom, C.R., 19Nezu, A.M., 347–349Nezu, C.M., 347–349Nezworski, M.T., 249Nigg, J.T., 384Nitko, A., 35Nixon, R.D.V., 291Norman, R.D., 79Norton, H., 164Noterdaeme, M., 440Nurnberger, J.I., Jr., 304O’Brien, 19, 63O’Brien, J.D., 258, 401O’Leary, K.D., 292O’Leary, S.G., 280, 286, 397Oakl<strong>and</strong>, T., 321, 330, 331Odom, S.L., 214, 216, 223Offord, D.R., 59Okiishi, J., 187Olino, T.M., 22, 142, 301Ollendick, T.H., 61, 89, 142, 199, 260, 388, 414,427–433, 435Olmedo, E.L., 79Olson, S., 24Ondersma, S.J., 288, 294Onghena, P., 286Orvaschel, H., 293Os<strong>of</strong>sky, J.D., 401Ostrov, J.M., 195, 196Oswald, D., 443Owen, P.F., 225Ownby, R.L., 358Ozon<strong>of</strong>f, S., 316, 438, 439, 442–445, 451Paik<strong>of</strong>f, R.L., 346P<strong>and</strong>ina, G.J., 384Papadopoulos, L., 305Papillon, M., 417Pardini, D.A., 272Parker, J.G., 212, 213Patterson, D.R., 186Patterson, G.R., 206, 207, 272, 288, 358, 402, 409Payne, A.F., 10Pearson, K., 39Peeples, F., 401Pelham, W.E., 43, 260, 380, 383, 385, 388,393, 394, 397Perriello, L.M., 364Perrot, A., 439Perry, M.A., 94Peters, R.D.V., 273Petersen, A.C., 415Petersen, N.S., 30, 33, 37, 415Peterson, D.R., 16, 341, 413Peterson, M., 22Petit, G.S., 291Petoskey, M.D., 42Pettit, G.S., 306, 401Pfeiffer, 142Pfiffner, L.J., 285Phares, E.J., 89Phares, V., 62Piacentini, J., 144, 164, 261, 262, 267Piacentini, J.C., 170, 343, 344Pianta, R.C., 340Pickles, A., 400, 440Pina, A.A., 256, 430, 431Pincus, H.A., 16Pinderhughes, E.E., 291, 408Piotrowski, C., 19, 281, 282Piquero, A.R., 409Pisecco, S., 180Pliszka, S., 401Policaro, K., 283Pope, K.S., 128Popinga, M.R., 332Porter, B., 292Potvin, P., 417Powell, N., 219Power, T., 54Power, T.J., 393Pozdol, S.L., 440Pressman, L.J., 281, 282Prinstein, M.J., 216, 219Prinz, R.J., 278, 293Prinzie, P., 286


author index499Prior, M., 286Prus<strong>of</strong>f, B.A., 293Puig-Antich, J., 258, 354Putallaz, M., 219Quay, H.C., 49, 51, 52, 54, 377, 399Rabian, B., 419, 430, 435Rabin, A.I., 225, 227, 228Rafferty, J.E., 11, 244, 246Raine, A., 401Ramirez, M., 79Ramirez, V., 289Rapee, R.M., 295, 428, 431Rapee, R.R., 431Rapport, M.D., 386, 394Ray, R., 429Ray, R.S., 207Reed, M.L., 201Reich, W., 256, 343, 345Reichler, R.J., 441Reid, R., 393Reiss, A.L., 429Reitman, D., 286Rende, R., 304Renk, K., 62Renner, R.R., 441Rescorla, L.A., 103, 113, 114, 116, 146, 148,156, 158–160, 162, 163, 413Reynolds, C., 43Reynolds, C.R., 6, 16, 23, 43, 73, 102, 103,105–108, 110, 145, 146, 148, 150–155,201, 202, 277, 308, 335, 346, 388, 390,413, 421, 429, 430, 435Reynolds, W.M., 37, 74, 75, 414, 415, 419, 420Rezai, A., 440Rhode, P., 342Rhoades, K.A., 286Riccio, C., 43, 154Rice, F., 416Richardson, R.D., 327Richmond, 321Richmond, B.O., 388, 429, 430Richters, J.E., 261, 275, 276, 341Ricks, J.H., 372Ridenour, T.A., 137Rieser-Danner, L.A., 5Riggs, P.D., 400Risi, 193Risi, S., 441Rispens, J., 237Risser, S.D., 214Roberts, B.W., 60, 217Roberts, C., 329Roberts, C.R., 261Roberts, G.C., 207Roberts, G.E., 227, 230, 236Roberts, M.A., 207Roberts, R.E., 261Robertson, D., 282Robins, L.N., 254, 265, 296, 409Robinson, E.A., 194, 286–288Rockett, J.L., 51Rodell, J.L., 11Roesch, L., 417R<strong>of</strong>f, J.D., 401Rogers, A, 142Rogers, H., 286Rogers, R., 126Rogler, L.H., 237Roid, 201Ronan, 432Roper, B.L., 190Rorschach, H., 9–11, 20, 230Rosenblum, N.D., 203Ross, A.O., 272Ross, A.W., 186Rothlind, J., 222Rotter, J.B., 11, 244, 246Rounsaville, B.J., 19Roux, S., 439Rubian, B., 419Rudolph, K.D., 204Ruffalo, S.L., 333Rutter, M., 19, 48, 58, 193, 400–401Ryan, S., 295Saavedra, L.M, 430Saavedra, L.M., 430Saccuzzo, 44Saemundsen, E., 441S<strong>and</strong>ers, M.R., 286S<strong>and</strong>oval, J., 74S<strong>and</strong>strom, M.J., 219Sanford, K., 282Sarason, 288Sargent, J., 273Sasagawa, S., 285, 410Sattler, J.M., 89Sawyer, M., 286Scahill, L., 18Schaughency, E., 142Schaughency, E.A., 222Schinka, J.A., 122Schmidt, K.L., 249Schniering, C.A., 431Scholte, E., 160Schopler, E., 441, 444Schorre, B.E.H., 185Schwab-Stone, M., 267, 276Schwab-Stone, M.E., 407Scopetta, M.A., 79Scott, S.S., 289Scotti, J.R., 23Seat, P.S., 174Seelen, J., 128


500 author indexSeeley, J.R., 342, 417Semrud-Clikeman, M., 418, 422Sevin, J.A., 441Shaevel, B., 122Shaffer, D., 184, 256, 267, 400Shaffer, D.M., 74, 254, 259, 265, 266, 268, 386, 407Shaffer, T.W., 232Shapiro, C.J., 378Shapiro, E.S., 189, 196–199Sharp, C., 410Shaw, J.G., 321Shellenberger, S., 305Shelton, 280Shelton, K.K., 283, 285Shelton, T.L., 394Sherrill, J.T., 255Sherry, 243Shiner, R., 22Shore, G., 394Siegel, 288Sigmund Freud, 8Sigurdardottir, S., 441Sikora, D.M., 160, 443Silver, M., 273Silverman, W.K., 61, 142, 256, 260, 419,427–433, 435Silverthorn, P., 103, 168, 259, 345Simpson, R.L., 446Sinclair, L., 440Singleton, L.C., 219, 223Siskind, G., 360Sitter, S., 440Sivaldason, N., 283Skinner, B.F., 5Skinner, C.H., 189Slesinger, D., 126Smallish, L., 294, 383Smari, J., 441Smith, B.H., 343Smith, G.L., 243Smith, M., 343, 378, 382, 384, 394Smith, S.R., 236, 378, 388Smolla, N., 257, 263Snidman, N., 51, 54Snyder, 288Snyder, M., 51Solis, M.L., 291Solomon, I.L., 237,Solomon, M., 316, 438Sour<strong>and</strong>er, A., 115, 116South, M., 443Southam-Gerow, M.A., 203Spanier, G.B., 292Sparrow, 316Sparrow, S.S., 33, 316, 320–324Spencer, T., 142Spielberger, C.D., 296, 431Spitzer, R., 54Spitzer, R.I., 296Spitzer, R.L., 55, 254, 399Spracklen, K.M., 402Sprafkin, J., 146, 148, 166, 168, 169Stacy, A., 318Stacy, A.W., 336Stagheeza, B., 345Staghezza, B.M., 62Stanger, C., 235, 285Stanion, P., 305Starr, B.D., 237Stavrakaki, C., 417, 427Steele, R.G., 286Steer, R., 420Steer, R.A., 296Stein, L., 121Stein, L.A.R., 121Steinberg, L., 280Steinhausen, H.C., 115Stickle, T.R., 428Stone, S.V., 62Stone, W.L., 440Storoschuk, S., 440Stouthamer-Loeber, M., 222, 401Strauss, C.C., 63, 215–219, 429Stredny, R., 128Stredney, R.V., 229Streiner, D.L., 439Stringfield, S.A., 413Sullaway, M., 345Sundberg, N.D., 12Suveg, C., 281, 282Swain, N.R., 401Swartz, J.D., 231Sweeney, L., 291Swensen, C.H., 11Szakowski, A., 285Szapocznik, J., 79Szatmari, P., 317, 439Tallent, N., 358, 360–362, 367, 368Tanaka, J.S., 316Tanke, E., 51Tannock, R., 384Tapscott, M., 294Taylor, M., 190Teerikangas, O.M., 115Teglasi, H., 365, 369–372, 374Tellegen, A., 29, 30Teodori, J.B., 307Terdal, L.G., 190, 191, 198Tesiny, E.P., 222, 422Teufel-Shone, N., 281Thapar, A., 416Thomas, E.A., 234, 346Thompson, D.S., 316–318, 329Thompson, L.L., 400Thurber, S., 115Thurlow, M.L., 329Tibbetts, S.G., 409


author index501Tinsley, B.R., 223Toepfer, S.M., 122Toth, S.L., 306Touyz, S.W., 291Trautman, P., 400Tripp, G., 142Tsuang, M.T., 294Tully, L.A., 286Turner, L.M., 440Underwood, M.K., 214Updegraff, R., 216Urbina, S., 14, 21, 22, 25, 34, 37, 39, 40, 43, 45Uyteeborek, K., 289Vaillancourt, T., 219Valla, J., 255, 257, 263Valla, J.P., 263Van Berckelaer-Onnes, I., 160Van der Ploeg, J., 160Van Hasselt, V.B., 203Van Horn, M.L., 333, 334V<strong>and</strong>vik, I.H., 185Vasay, M.W., 203, 204Vaughn, 154Verhulst, F.C., 39, 73, 115Vincent, G.M., 410Voelker, S.L., 321Volkmar, F., 439Volkmar, F.R., 440Vosk, B., 218Walder, L.O., 221Waldman, I.D., 384Walker, E.F., 385Walker, H.M., 58Walker, J.L., 221Walker, R., 11Wall, A.D., 126Wallace, K.M., 292Wallbrown, F., 358Walsh, C., 294Walthall, J.C., 340Wangby, M., 212Ward, C.S., 288Ward, J.H., 42Warner, V., 185, 293Waschbusch, D.A., 283, 400Watanabe, H.E., 261Watkins, C.E., 226Weatherman, R.E., 320, 328Webster-Stratton, 288Webster-Stratton, C., 408Wechsler, R.D., 396Weems, C.F., 428–431Weems, C.W., 295Wehrspann, W., 16Weiner, 227Weiner, I.B., 225, 230–232, 234–236Weiner, Z., 10, 12, 49Weiss, B., 31, 415Weissman, M.M., 185, 293, 304Weisz, J.R., 31Wells, K.C., 401Welsh-Allis, G., 293West, J., 190West, M.O., 293Westerman, M.A., 316Wewers, S., 401Whalen, C.K., 199Whalen, D.K., 384Whipple, 288White, S.F., 403, 410Whiteside, 334Whiteside-Mansell, L., 288, 291Wickramaratne, P., 293Widaman, K.F., 318, 336Widiger, T.A., 16, 17, 56, 166Wiener, J., 232, 362, 364Wilcutt, E., 380Williams, C.L., 119, 122, 126Williams, J.B.W., 296Wilson, F.J., 439Wimberley, R.C., 289Wingenfeld, S.A., 178, 180, 391Winokur, G., 418Winters, A., 419Winters, N.C., 402, 419, 422Wirt, R.D., 101, 174, 401Witt, 142Wolf, T.H., 3Wolfe, D.A., 278, 279, 289Wolff, L.S., 280Wood, J.M., 249Woodcock, R.W., 320, 325, 328, 330Woodworth, R.S., 7Wootton, J., 280, 294Wootton, J.M., 283Wrobel, T., 136Wrobel, T.A., 134Xing, Y., 261Ye, W., 293Yen, W.M., 21Yoerger, K., 409Young, J.G., 258Youngstrom, E.A., 440Zedeck, S., 51Zelli, A., 291Zeman, J., 255, 258, 261, 281Zera, M., 402Zettergren, P., 212, 219Zucker, R.A., 282Zuckerman, M., 77


S u b j e c t I n d e xAAID. See American association on intellectual <strong>and</strong>developmental disabilities (AAID)Adaptive behavior assessment system-II (ABAS-II)administration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 329–330, 332content, 329interpretation, 329norming, 329reliability, 329strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses, 330–331validity, 329Adaptive behavior scalescharacteristicsadministration format, 321domains assess, 318–320informant choice, 320–321norm vs. criterion referencing, 320general recommendationsadaptive behavior assessment system-II(ABAS-II), 319–324scales <strong>of</strong> independent behavior, 324–326historyAAIDD criteria, 316definition, 316intelligence relationship, 317–318mental retardation, 316scales uses, 318social quotient, 316measuring social skillsdevelopment <strong>and</strong> revision, 332social skills rating system, 332strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses, 334omnibusrating scales component, 335referral reason, 325Vinel<strong>and</strong> adaptive behavior scales, 2nd edition,322–325Administer diagnostic interviews, 264African American measures, 78Aggression, peer-referenced assessment, 221Agoraphobia, 428Alabama parenting questionnaire (APQ)content, 283–285norms, 285reliability <strong>and</strong> validity, 285American association on intellectual <strong>and</strong>developmental disabilities (AAID), 316American psychological association (APA), 68–69Anxiety disordersassessment strategyalternative causes <strong>and</strong> treatment, 432–433classification <strong>and</strong> co-morbidity, 432–433questions <strong>and</strong> implications, 433screening process, 432–433case study, 434characteristics features, 428–429cognitive-behavioral assessment, 432definition, 428interviews <strong>and</strong> clinician ratings, 431–432specialized measuresfear survey schedule for children-revised(FSSCR), domains, 431revised children’s manifest anxiety scale(RCMAS), 429–431APQ. See Alabama parenting questionnaireAsian American measures, 78Asperger’s disorder, 446Assessor-administered exercise with unlimitedchoiceformat, 215Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)alternative causes assessmentintensity <strong>and</strong> duration, symptom, 396medication side effects, 397primary <strong>and</strong> secondary dysfunction, 397assessment implicationscombined type, 385predominantly inattentive type, 385behavior rating scalesAchenbach system for empirically basedassessment, 389behavior assessment system for children–2ndedition, 390Conners rating scales–3rd edition, 390–392callous-unemotional traits assessment, 408–409classification <strong>and</strong> subtypeswith (ADD/H) <strong>and</strong> without hyperactivity(ADD/WO), 379conceptualization, 378–379core features, 378–379DSM-IV-TR, 380–383proposed behaviors, 384comorbiditiesconduct problems/aggression, 383learning disability, 383comorbid problems assessment503


504 subject IndexAttention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)(Continued)family functioning, 396learning difficulties, 396conceptual modelbehavioral inhibition, 384environmental factors, 385neurological abnormalities, 384conduct problemsassessment implications, 404–406callous-unemotional traits, 403case study, 405–406causal factors assessment, 407–408classification <strong>and</strong> diagnosis, 398–399cognitive <strong>and</strong> learning style, 401comorbidities, 400, 407core features assessment, 406–407developmental pathways assessment,408–410ecological factors, 401onset subtype model, 402–403core behavior assessmentage <strong>and</strong> gender based comparison, 393close attention <strong>and</strong> scale selection, 388–389continuous performance test (CPT), 393diagnosis limitations, 394ecological validity, 395freedom from distractibility (FD) factor, 396multiple modalities, 393ratings scales, 389–393subtype differentiation, 389task performance, 394DISC-IV evaluation, 262special education placementindividuals with disabilities act (IDEA), 398requirement, 397–398Autism diagnostic interview-revised (ADI-R)assessment behaviors, 440strength, 440Autism diagnostic observation schedule (ADOS)assessment <strong>of</strong>, 441–442differential validity, 441modules, 441Autism spectrum disorders (ASD)<strong>and</strong> Asperger’s disorder,assessment strategyalternative cause rule out, 444co-morbidity, 444multidisciplinary assessment, 444questions <strong>and</strong> implications, 445screening process <strong>and</strong> classification, 444case studyacademic achievement, 448adaptive behavior, 448–449background information, 445–447behavioral observations, 447–448intellectual functioning analysis, 448rating scale assess, 449recommendations, 450referral information, 445visual motor integration, 448characteristics, 438–439co-morbidity, 439DSM-IV-TR criteriadefinition <strong>of</strong>, 437–438symptoms, 438functional analysis, 443specialized measuresautism diagnostic interview-revised (ADI-R), 440autism diagnostic observation schedule (ADOS),441–443childhood autism rating scale (CARS), 441Gilliam autism rating scale–2nd Edition(GARS-2), 443infant behavioral summarized evaluation(IBSE), 439–440parent interview for autism (PIA), 440BASC-2. See Behavior assessment system forchildren, 2nd edition (BASC-2)Behavioral avoidance tests (BATs), 203Behavior assessment system for children, 2nd edition(BASC-2)parent rating scaleadministration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 151content, 145interpretation, 152–153norming, 151reliability, 152strength <strong>and</strong> weakness, 153validity, 152self-report <strong>of</strong> personality (SRP)administration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 105age, 102interpretation, 110norming <strong>and</strong> reliability, 108–109scale content, 102–105strengths <strong>and</strong> weakness, 110, 112validity scales, 109teacher rating scaleadministration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 154composites <strong>and</strong> scales, 154content, 153–154interpretation, 155–156norming, 154reliability, 154–155strength <strong>and</strong> weakness, 156validity, 155Behavior problems, interpretation, 345Bias <strong>and</strong> cultural competenceconstruct validity, 74–75content validity, 73–74predictive validity, 75test, 73CAPI-second edition. See <strong>Child</strong> abuse potentialinventory (CAPI)-second editionCAT. See <strong>Child</strong>ren’s apperception test (CAT)


subject Index505CBCL-TRF. See <strong>Child</strong> behavior checklist- teacherreport form (CBCL-TRF)<strong>Child</strong> abuse <strong>and</strong> neglect assessment strategychild-rearing dem<strong>and</strong>s, parental responses,278–279general strengths <strong>and</strong> problem areas identify, 278identify needs <strong>of</strong> the child, 279parent–child relationship <strong>and</strong> abuse dynamics, 279suspected child abuse, 277<strong>Child</strong> abuse potential inventory (CAPI)-secondeditioncomposite abuse scale, 288–289norms, 289reliability <strong>and</strong> validity, 289<strong>Child</strong> behavior checklist (CBCL)parent reportadministration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 158–159content, 158interpretation, 160–161norming, 159reliability, 159–160sample case, 157strength <strong>and</strong> weakness, 161–162validity, 160teacher report form (TRF)administration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 162–163content, 162interpretation, 164norming, 163reliability, 163sample case, 165strength <strong>and</strong> weakness, 166validity, 163–164<strong>Child</strong> behavior checklist-teacher report form(CBCL-TRF), 333<strong>Child</strong> global assessment scale (CGAS), 184–185<strong>Child</strong>hood autism rating scale (CARS), 441<strong>Child</strong>hood problems, family contextaggression, social learning perspective, 272–273anxiety, an operant perspective, 272eating disorders, family systems perspective, 273<strong>Child</strong>ren’s apperception test (CAT), 11, 237<strong>Child</strong>ren’s depression inventory (CDI)administration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 418norming <strong>and</strong> reliability, 418–419scale content, 418strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses, 419validity, 419<strong>Child</strong>ren’s MMPI, 14<strong>Child</strong> symptom inventory-4 (CSI-4)parent <strong>and</strong> teacher report checklistsadministration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 167–168content, 167DSM-IV disorders, 167interpretation, 169–170norming, 168reliability, 168strength <strong>and</strong> weakness, 170validity, 168–169Classificationcommunication, 49–50dangers <strong>and</strong> limitations, 50–51documentation, 50evaluating systems, 51–52integration, medical <strong>and</strong> multivariate, 53–54levels, 48medical models, 52multivariate approaches, 52–53Commonalities across interview schedulescharacteristics, 256–257DISC-IV question format, 254Comorbidity, 62Confidence b<strong>and</strong>s, 36–37Conflict negotiation task, 204–205Conners-3. See Conners, 3rd edition(Conners-3)Conners, 3rd edition (Conners-3)parent rating scaleadministration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 170interpretation, 172norming, 171reliability, 171scale content, 170–171strength <strong>and</strong> weakness, 172validity, 160self-report (Conners-3 SR), 131–133administration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 131–132interpretation, 133reliability, 132scale content, 131strengths <strong>and</strong> weakness, 133validity, 132–133teacher rating scaleadministration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 173content, 173interpretation, 174norming, 173reliability, 173strength <strong>and</strong> weakness, 174validity, 173–174Constructs vs. categories, 18–19Construct validity bias, 74–75Content validity, 37–38Contextual factors, 306Controversy surrounding projective techniquesprojection/behavioral sampledem<strong>and</strong> characteristics, 230hypothesis, 228, 230interpretations types, 230perceptual test, 230stimulus type selection, 230psychometric/clinical technique testapproaches <strong>and</strong> limitations inherent, 228clinical impressions, 228person’s unique qualities, 227reliability <strong>and</strong> validity, 227st<strong>and</strong>ardization efforts problem, 227traditional measurement theory, 227


506 subject IndexConvergent/discriminant validity, 39Criterion-related validityconcurrent <strong>and</strong> predictive, 38–39correlation, 39Data reanalyses, 17Depressionassessment strategyalternative causes, 425classification, 423comorbidity, 423questions <strong>and</strong> implications, 423screening process, 422treatment, 423case study, 424characteristics featureschronicity, 416duration, 416early <strong>and</strong> late onset, 416gender differences, 415heritability, 416prevalence rate, 415comorbidityacademic functioning, 417anxiety disorder, 417developmental level difference, 417internalizing disorders, 417–418medical illness, 417diagnostic criteriaimportance <strong>and</strong> inter-rater reliability, 415symptoms, 414internalizing disorders, 413–414peer-referenced assessment,specialized measureschildren’s depression inventory (CDI),418–419peer nomination inventory for depression(PNID), 422Reynolds adolescent depression scale, 2ndedition (RADS-2), 419–421Reynolds child depression scale (RCDS),421–422Developmental psychopathologycomorbidities, 62–64definition, 48Erikson’s psychosocial theory, 60meta-analysis <strong>of</strong> cross-informant correlation, 63norms, 58practical implications, 64process-oriented approach., 59situational stability, 61–62stability <strong>and</strong> continuity, 60–61Diagnostic <strong>and</strong> statistical manual <strong>of</strong> mental disorders(DSM)changes, 54–55children <strong>and</strong> adolescents, 56comparison, diagnostic criteria, 55Direct observation form (DOF), 200DISC-IV. See NIMH diagnostic interview schedulefor childrenDPICS. See Dyadic parent-child interaction codingsystem (DPICS)Drawing techniques. See Projective techniquesDSM-IV-TRdefinition, 360diagnosis parameterscross-situational consistency, 381–383developmental change in, 382diagnostic threshold, 380functioning impairment, 383onset <strong>and</strong> duration, symptoms, 381subtype stability, 380symptoms, 380–381Dyadic parent–child interaction coding system(DPICS)behavioral observation, 280categories, 287content, 287norms, 287reliability <strong>and</strong> validity, 287–288ECS. See Exner’s comprehensive system (ECS)Emic vs. etic perspectives, 76–77Empirical criterion keying, 13–14Enhance reliability, 84Erikson’s psychosocial theory, 60Ethics, peer-referenced assessmentassessor-administered exercise with unlimitedchoiceformat, 215later psychological adjustment, 213teacher-administered exercise with fixed-choiceformat, 215–216time-efficient relatively, 213Etiology, 301–303Evaluationdesigningcontexts screening, 87developmental considerations, 85–86enhance reliability, 84practical considerations, 87–88psychological domains, 86–87reasons, 88scientific approach, 85non-specifics, 81–82purpose <strong>of</strong> testing, 82–83referral problems <strong>and</strong> clarification, 82–84Event recording, 196–197Evidence-based interpretationmedical record, 72psychologists view, 70–71self monitoring questions, 71–72test st<strong>and</strong>ards, 69Exner’s comprehensive system (ECS)administration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 232age trends, 234evaluation, 236


subject Index507process measured, 231–232reliability, 233–234Rorschach interpretation, 231, 233–234validity <strong>and</strong> interpretations, 234–236Fairnessacculturation assessmentAfrican American measures, 78Hispanic American measures, 78–79emic vs. etic perspectives, 76–77guidelines, children assessment, 77–78symptom severity, 76Family context assessmentfunctioningconsiderations, 277, 279issues, 276–277history <strong>and</strong> differential diagnoses, 257–258influences, causal rolechildhood problems, 272–273psychological adjustment, 272interpreting information, provided by parentanxiety, 276child’s adjustment, 275depressed mothers, 275–276parental adjustment, 275marital conflictmeta-analysis, 291–292O’Leary-Porter scale (OPS), 292parental adjustmentADHD, 294–295antisocial behavior, 294anxiety, 295depression, 293family history assessment, 295–296schizophrenia, 295substance abuse, 293–294parenting stresschild abuse potential inventory (CAPI),288–289stress index-second edition (PSI), 289–291parenting styles <strong>and</strong> practicesAlabama parenting questionnaire (APQ),283–285child development, 280dyadic parent–child interaction coding system(DPICS), 286–288family environment scale-second edition (FES),280–282parenting scale (PS), 286–288social phobia, 274youth characteristics, 271Family environment scale (FES)-scale-secondeditioncontent, 281–282family environment scale, 281norms, 282reliability <strong>and</strong> validity, 282Family interaction coding system (FICS), 206Family psychiatric history, 303–305Field trials, 17Gaussian curve, 32Genograms symbols, 305Gilliam autism rating scale–2nd Edition(GARS-2), 443Hispanic American measures, 78–79History taking, child assessmentage <strong>of</strong> onset, 300content, 306–307contextual factors, 306course/prognosis, 300–301etiology, 301–303family history illustration, 304–305family psychiatric history, 303formats, 307–309genograms usage, 305nonverbal cues observation, 313previous assessment/treatment/intervention, 306role <strong>of</strong>, 299–300sample history form, 301HIT. See Holtzman inkblot technique (HIT)Holtzman inkblot technique (HIT), 231Hyperactivity, peer-referenced assessment, 221–222Individuals with disabilities education improvementact (IDEIA), 17–18, 20, 398Infant behavioral summarized evaluation (IBSE),autism, 439–440Information integrationcase study, behavior problems, 350, 351case study, language disorder <strong>and</strong> social phobia,351, 352clinical reasoningerrors, 348problem orientation, 348, 349combining rules, multiple sourcesinformation, 343cross-informant disagreementanalysis level, 340behavior types, 339different measurement techniques, 340situational variability, 340different informants use, 340discrepancies <strong>and</strong> problemsdifferential validity, 345differing motivations, 347emotional distress, 345parental adjustment <strong>and</strong> marital conflict, 347reliability, parent report, 346structured interviews <strong>and</strong> behavior ratingscales, 346multistep strategychild’s adjustment report, 347convergent findings, sources <strong>and</strong> methods,350–353critical information determination, 351–352


508 subject IndexInformation integration (Continued)discrepancy explanation, 352strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses report, 353Informed consent, 90Inkblot techniquesExner’s comprehensive system (ECS)administration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 232evaluation, 236process measured, 231–232reliability, 233–234validity <strong>and</strong> interpretations, 234–236Holtzman inkblot technique (HIT), 231Integrating assessment information. See InformationintegrationInternal consistency coefficients, 35, 175–176Internalizing <strong>and</strong> externalizing dimensions, 15–16Interpreting assessment information. SeeInformation integrationLanguage disorder, interpretation, 349–350Mean score differences, 73Measurement issues. See <strong>Personality</strong> testsMedical models, 52Minnesota multiphasic personality inventory(MMPI)original scales, 14personality inventory for children (PIC), 14–15rating scales, 15Minnesota multiphasic personality inventoryadolescent(MMPI-A)administration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 119–120case study, MMPI-A, 129–130interpretation, 128norming, 121–122reliability, 122–125scale content, 117–119strengths <strong>and</strong> weakness, 128validity, 125–127validity scales, 121MMPI-A. See Minnesota multiphasic personalityinventory-adolescent (MMPI-A)Multiple source information, combiningrules, 341Multivariate approaches, 52–53NIMH diagnostic interview schedule for children(DISC-IV)administration, 266–267development, 265reliability, 267–268structure <strong>and</strong> content, 265–266test–retest <strong>and</strong> internal consistencyestimates, 267validity, 268–269Normal curves. See Gaussian curveNorm developmentage-based, 32clinical, 32–34gender-based, 31sampling, 30–31Norm-referenced scores, 25Objective tests, 12ObservationsADHD, 190aggressive behaviorobservation study, 195preschool children, 195–196ASEBA, 200–201assessment techniques, 191basics <strong>of</strong>, 192BAT, 203–204behavioral assessment system, childrendefinition, 202clinical assessor, 191conflict negotiation task, 204–205data collectiondodge study, social competence, 197duration recording, 197event recording, 196–197momentary recording, 198time sampling, 197whole/partial interval, 198direct observation form (DOF), 200event recording, 196–197examples <strong>of</strong>, 200–207FICS, 206–207intrapsychic variables, 191limitations, 190naturalistic observations, 194, 195overt behavior, 191peer interaction, 205–206personal digital assistants (PDA), 198–199reactivity, 190samples, 201selection <strong>of</strong> observer, 172–174settings, 191, 196, 198simple antecedent-behavior-consequence(A-B-C), 193SOAPS, 207systemsanalog, 196analysis level, 192basics <strong>of</strong>, 192naturalistic observation, 194, 195several behavioral domains, 193simple A-B-C, 193subjective <strong>and</strong> idiosyncratic definition, 192target behavior, 192, 205, 206TAB, 206test observation form (TOF), 200total problems score, 201Observer selectionself monitoringadvantages <strong>of</strong>, 198


subject Index509PDA, 198–199steps in, 198Observing nonverbal cues, 313Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD)anxiety disorder, 429O’Leary-Porter scale (OPS), 292Omnibus personality inventories. See Self-reportinventoriesOPS. See O’Leary-Porter scale (OPS)Parental adjustmentADHD, 294–295antisocial behavior, 294anxiety, 295depression, 293family history assessment, 295–296schizophrenia, 295substance abuse, 293–294Parenting scale (PS)attitudes <strong>and</strong> beliefs about discipline, 286content, 286norms, 286reliability <strong>and</strong> validity, 286Parenting stress index (PSI)-second editioncontent, 289–291norms, 291reliability <strong>and</strong> validity, 291Parent interview for autism (PIA), 440Parent rating scaleschildren evaluation, 141–142factors that influence, 142omnibus scales, 144overview <strong>of</strong>, 144–145Parent report PIC-2, 174–177Peer nomination inventory for depression(PNID), 422Peer nominations, 214–217Peer-referenced assessmentclassroom participation levels, 220ethicsassessor-administered exercise with unlimitedchoiceformat, 215later psychological adjustment, 213teacher-administered exercise with fixed-choiceformat, 215–216time-efficient relatively, 213intuitive <strong>and</strong> empirical basis, 211sociometric techniques, 211types <strong>of</strong>aggression, 221depression, 222–223hyperactivity, 221–222paired comparison technique, 223peer nominations, 214–217sociometrics, 217–221Peer relationships vs. later psychologicaladjustment, 213Percentile ranks, 29<strong>Personality</strong> assessmentdistinguishing features, 5history, 6projective techniquesassociation, 8–9for children, 11–12Rorschach’s approach, 9–10thematic apperception test (TAT), 9traits, 4<strong>Personality</strong> inventory for children-2 (PIC-2),174–177<strong>Personality</strong> inventory for children (PIC), 14<strong>Personality</strong> inventory for youth (PIY)abbreviated form, 135administration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 134–135interpretation, 136–137norming, 135reliability, 135–136scale content, 133–134strengths <strong>and</strong> weakness, 137–138validity scales, 134, 136<strong>Personality</strong> testsdefinition, 22diagnostic schedulesidentification, 22limitation, 23rating scales, 23Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), 428Predictive validity bias, 75Process-oriented approach, 59, 61Projective techniquesfor children, 11–12drawing techniquesdraw-a-person technique, 246house–tree–person (HPT), 248–249kinetic family drawing, 249Koppitz emotional indicators, 247nonverbal, 245psychometric cautions, 249–250inkblot techniquesExner’s comprehensive system (ECS),231–236Holtzman inkblot technique (HIT), 231projection/behavioral sampledem<strong>and</strong> characteristics, 230hypothesis, 230interpretation types, 230perceptual test, 230stimulus type selection, 230projective controversy, 226projective debate, 226psychometric testapproaches <strong>and</strong> limitations inherent, 228clinical impressions, 228person’s unique qualities, 228reliability <strong>and</strong> validity, 227st<strong>and</strong>ardization efforts problem, 227traditional measurement theory, 227


510 subject IndexProjective techniques (Continued)Rorschach’s approach, 9–10sentence completion techniques (SCT)administration, 244content, 243–244evaluation, 245interpretation, 244–245interpretive approaches, 246thematic apperception test (TAT), 9thematic story-telling techniquesgeneral interpretation, 237–238roberts apperception technique, 238–243word association, 8PSI-second edition. See Parenting stress index(PSI)-second editionPurpose <strong>of</strong> testing, 82–83Rapport buildingexplaining testingchild, 93–94parent, 94–96teachers, 96importance <strong>and</strong> recommendations, 89informed consent, 90–91strategies, 95RATC. See Roberts apperception test for children(RATC)Rating scales, 15Raw score, 25Readability, 43Referral reasonbackground information, 325behavioral observations, 326previous evaluation, 326test results <strong>and</strong> interpretationadaptive behavior, 323communication, 323daily living skills, 323psychoeducational assessment, 326Reliabilityaffecting variables, 35internal consistency coefficients, 35reliable specific variance, 35–36st<strong>and</strong>ard error <strong>of</strong> measurement (SEM), 36test–retest method, 34Reliable specific variance, 35–36Report writingabbreviations <strong>and</strong> acronyms, 365adaptation, audience <strong>and</strong> setting, 368difficult word reduction, 366editing, 366evidence-based assessment, 357examples usage, 366grammar <strong>and</strong> spelling check, 368heading <strong>and</strong> list usage, 367instruments description, 366length reduction, 367, 368oral communication, resultschild feedback, 375parent conferences, 373, 374teacher conferences, 374pertinent information, 365pitfallsconsumer’s view <strong>of</strong>, 362failure, address referral questions, 362faulty interpretation, 361number obsession, 361report length, 361vocabulary problems, 360problems <strong>and</strong> challengesassessment result presentation, 357improper interpretation, 358score check, 368sectionsassessment procedures, 369assessment results <strong>and</strong> interpretation, 370background information, 369behavioral observations, 370diagnostic considerations, 371information identification, 369psychometric summary, 372recommendations, 371referral questions, 369self-test, 372summary <strong>and</strong> signatures, 371word—number emphasize, 365Revised children’s manifest anxiety scale (RCMAS)administration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 430norming <strong>and</strong> reliability, 430scale content, 429–430strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses, 431validity, 430Reynolds adolescent depression scale, 2nd edition(RADS-2)administration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 420norming <strong>and</strong> reliability, 420scale content, 420strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses, 421validity, 420Reynolds child depression scale (RCDS)administration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 421norming <strong>and</strong> reliability, 421scale content, 421strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses, 422validity, 421–422Roberts apperception test for children (RATC),238–240Rorschach’s approach, 9–10Sample impairment-oriented scaleschild global assessment scale (CGAS), 184–185home situations questionnaire (HSQ), 184school situations questionnaire (SSQ), 184Scales <strong>of</strong> independent behavior-R (SIB-R)administration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 329content, 328


subject Index511interpretation, 329–331norming, 329reliability, 329strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses, 330validity, 329School apperception method, 237Scoresnormal distribution, 28, 29norm-referenced, 25percentile ranks, 29raw, 25st<strong>and</strong>ardconversion table, 26–27T-scores, 28–29uniform T-scores, 29–30Self-report inventoriesConners, 3rd edition, self-report (Conners-3 SR)administration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 131–132case sample, 129–130interpretation, 133reliability, 132scale content, 131strengths <strong>and</strong> weakness, 133validity, 132–133Minnesota multiphasic personality inventoryadolescent(MMPI-A)administration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 119–120interpretation, 128norming, 121–122reliability, 122–125scale content, 117–119strengths <strong>and</strong> weakness, 128validity, 125–126validity scales, 121omnibus personality, 101–102overview <strong>of</strong>, 103–104personality inventory for children (PIC), 101personality inventory for youth (PIY)abbreviated form, 135administration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 134–135interpretation, 136–137norming, 135reliability, 135–136scale content, 133–134strengths <strong>and</strong> weakness, 137–138validity scales, 134, 136self-report <strong>of</strong> personality (SRP), 102–112single construct personality, 138youth self-report (YSR)administration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 113–114interpretation, 116norming, 114reliability, 114scale content, 113strengths <strong>and</strong> weakness, 116validity, 114–116Self-report <strong>of</strong> personality (SRP)administration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 105age, 103interpretation, 110norming, 108reliability, 108–109scale content, 102–105strengths <strong>and</strong> weakness, 110–112validity scales, 105–107Sensitivity <strong>and</strong> specificity, 42Sentence completion techniques (SCT), 243–245administration, 244content, 243–244evaluation, 245interpretation, 244–245interpretive approaches, 246SIB-R. See Scales <strong>of</strong> independent behavior-R(SIB-R)Simple antecedent-behavior-consequence(A-B-C), 192Situational stability, 61–62Social competence, dodge study, 197Social phobia, 428Social phobia, interpretation, 351–352Social skills rating system (SSRS)administration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 329content, 330norming, 331reliability, 331validity, 329Sociometric nominations social statusdetermination, 218SSRS. See Social skills rating system (SSRS)St<strong>and</strong>ard error <strong>of</strong> measurement (SEM), 36St<strong>and</strong>ardsAmerican psychological association (APA), 68–69ethics <strong>and</strong> self-examination checklist, 68evidence-based interpretationmedical record, 72psychologists view, 70–71self monitoring questions, 71test st<strong>and</strong>ards, 69psychological assessment practice, 67use <strong>and</strong> misuse, 68Storytelling approach. See Thematic story-tellingtechniquesStructured diagnostic interviewscommonalities across interview schedulescharacteristics, 255DISC-IV question format, 254computer-administered versions, 255evaluationadvantages, 259–260disadvantages, 260–261K-SADS, 258, 259NIMH diagnostic interview schedule for children(DISC-IV)administration, 266–267development, 265reliability, 267–268


512 subject IndexStructured diagnostic interviews (Continued)structure <strong>and</strong> content, 265–266test–retest <strong>and</strong> internal consistency estimates, 267validity, 268–269organization <strong>and</strong> content, 266recommendationsadminister diagnostic interviews, 264attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, 262pictorial interviews, 263–264symptoms, 264strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses, 260variation across interview schedules, 258–259verbal instructions, 266Structured observation <strong>of</strong> academic <strong>and</strong> play settings(SOAPS), 207Student behavior survey (SBS), 178–182parent report PIC-2, 174, 176–177teacher reportadministration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 178–179content, 178interpretation, 181norms, 179reliability, 179–180sample case, 182–183strength <strong>and</strong> weakness, 181–182validity, 180–181TAT. See Thematic apperception technique (TAT)Teacher-administered exercise with fixed-choiceformat, 215–216Teacher rating scaleschildren evaluation, 143factors that influence, 144overview <strong>of</strong>, 144–145Teacher report form (TRF), 162–164Tell-me-a-story technique (TEMAS), 237TEMAS. See Tell-me-a-story technique(TEMAS)Test bias, 73Test observation form (TOF), 201–202individual st<strong>and</strong>ardised test, 201narrow b<strong>and</strong> scale, 201Test–retest <strong>and</strong> internal consistency estimates, 267Test–retest method, 34Test st<strong>and</strong>ards, 69Thematic apperception technique (TAT), 237Thematic story-telling techniquesgeneral interpretation, 237–238Roberts apperception techniqueadministration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 238–240content, 238evaluation, 243indicators, 241–242norming, 240reliability, 240stimulus cards depictions, 239validity <strong>and</strong> interpretations, 240tell-me-a-story technique (TEMAS), 237thematic apperception technique (TAT), 237Traits, 4T-scores, 26–29Uniform T-scores, 29–30Validityconstruct, 37content, 37–38convergent/discriminant, 39criterion-relatedconcurrent, 38correlations, 39predictive, 37–38factor analysiscluster, 41–42confirmatory factor, 40–41MMPI-A, 40threatsguarding against, 44readability <strong>and</strong> response set, 43utility, 44–45Variation across interview schedules, major sources,258–259Vinel<strong>and</strong> adaptive behavior scales-2. See alsoAdaptive behavior scalesadaptive behavior construct, 322administration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 323content, 322–323norming, 323reliability, 324strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses, 324–325validity, 324Whine, 194Woodworth personal data sheet, 7Word association techniques, 8–9Youth self-report (YSR), 113–116administration <strong>and</strong> scoring, 113–114interpretation, 116norming, 114reliability, 114scale content, 117strengths <strong>and</strong> weakness, 116validity, 114–116

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!