13.07.2015 Views

Birth to three matters - Communities and Local Government

Birth to three matters - Communities and Local Government

Birth to three matters - Communities and Local Government

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

take in<strong>to</strong> account the rich,natural,experiences available <strong>to</strong> children whoare included within a mainstream setting.During these first years they are activelytrying <strong>to</strong> make sense of the worlds inwhich they find themselves. As Donaldson(1978:111) comments after reviewingearlier research on babies ‘we mayconclude that there exists a fundamentalhuman urge <strong>to</strong> make sense of the world<strong>and</strong> bring it under deliberate control.’Gopnik et al (1999) stress how babies <strong>and</strong>young children approach the world as ifthey are scientists, actively engaged inlooking for patterns in what is going onaround them, testing hypotheses, seekingexplanations <strong>and</strong> formulating newtheories.They suggest that two year oldsin particular have a drive <strong>to</strong> find out howpeople <strong>and</strong> their familiar world ‘work’:-‘as with scientists, finding the truth is morethan a profession – it’s a passion. And, aswith scientists, that passion maysometimes make them sacrifice domestichappiness.’ (Gopnik et al 1999:38).Gopnik <strong>and</strong> her colleagues go on <strong>to</strong>suggest that babies play imitation games<strong>to</strong> try <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> other people, babble<strong>to</strong> ‘try on’ language;<strong>and</strong> play games likepeekaboo <strong>and</strong> hide <strong>and</strong> seek <strong>to</strong> find outabout objects <strong>and</strong> how people see them.The research reported by specialist ECECresearchers such as Athey (1990), Bruce<strong>and</strong> Meggitt (2002), Matthews (2002),Davies (2002), Nutbrown (1994), <strong>and</strong>Whalley et al (2000) has providedpractitioners with powerful informationthey have used <strong>to</strong> explore young children’sschemas – early patterns of behaviourindicating that a child is working at asystematic investigation (see chapter 2for further information about schemas).As Nutbrown (1994: 11) comments:‘Early patterns of behaviour seen in babiesbecome more complex <strong>and</strong> morenumerous, eventually becoming grouped<strong>to</strong>gether so that babies <strong>and</strong> youngchildren do not perform single isolatedbehaviours but coordinate their actions.Toddlers work hard, collecting a pile ofobjects in the lap of their carer, walking <strong>to</strong><strong>and</strong> fro, bringing one object at a time. Theyare working on a pattern of behaviour thathas a consistent thread running throughit…related <strong>to</strong> the consistent back-<strong>and</strong>forthmovement. The early schemas ofbabies form the basis of the patterns ofbehaviour which children show betweenthe ages of two <strong>and</strong> five years, <strong>and</strong> thesein turn become established foundationsfor learning.’EgocentricityPiaget (1962) argued that young childrenare egocentric, that they can only see theworld from their own point of view. Theyassume that everyone thinks as they do.Gopnik et al (1999) describe an ingeniousexperiment in which they showed thatc h i l d ren ro u g h ly younger than 18 mont h scould not see another’s viewpoint,by providing bowls of broccoli <strong>and</strong>biscuits. When they asked a child under18 months <strong>to</strong> give them broccoli,sayingt h ey liked it be t ter than biscuits, the infant s88 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!