13.07.2015 Views

Birth to three matters - Communities and Local Government

Birth to three matters - Communities and Local Government

Birth to three matters - Communities and Local Government

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

elationships with close <strong>and</strong> protectiveadults. Babies who do not haveopportunities <strong>to</strong> <strong>to</strong>uch,gaze, coo <strong>and</strong>babble with responsive adults give upon conversation-like exchanges. Thereis evidence of this from the work ofGoldschmeid in state institutions forgroups of ab<strong>and</strong>oned children in Trieste,Italy in 1954. These children had excellentphysical care but no personal care. Thebabies who persistently failed <strong>to</strong> getattention from an adult becamewithdrawn, passive <strong>and</strong> despairing. Theys<strong>to</strong>pped looking for friendship in adultsor with other children…Sometimes thisis not recognised for what it is, but ismistaken for contentment.‘As long ago as the 1970s, research bySnow (1977), Snow <strong>and</strong> Ferguson (1977),<strong>and</strong> others, was demonstrating thatalthough babies who are <strong>three</strong> months olddo not contribute much <strong>to</strong> a conversationthrough vocalising, they already havegeneral ‘rules’ about communicating.For example, they will gaze at the familiaradult <strong>and</strong> sustain an interaction in apattern which matches exactly that oftwo adults in conversation.In their boo k , The Social Ba by, Mu rray<strong>and</strong> An d rews (2000), not only prov i d es u perb pho<strong>to</strong>g raphic ev i d e n ce ofbabies co m m u n i cating from birt h ,t h ey state,‘Babies are attracted <strong>to</strong> other people frombirth <strong>and</strong> they quickly prefer the peoplewho have become familiar. But the babyd oe s n’t simply wa nt <strong>to</strong> be near her family<strong>and</strong> their friends – she wa nts <strong>to</strong> share herex pe ri e n ce with other people <strong>and</strong> inte ra ctwith them!’ ( Mu rray <strong>and</strong> An d rews 2000: 47 ).Further, babies become quiet whentheir mother or other familiar personapproaches, apparently primed forinteraction. By the age of <strong>three</strong> months,a baby who has begun vocalising <strong>and</strong>who gets a response (being talked <strong>to</strong> <strong>and</strong>other types of positive attention) willincrease the amount of vocalisation. Snowalso points out that mothers will usuallysimplify their speech, restricting thecontent, using repetition,simplifyinggrammar <strong>and</strong> emphasising in<strong>to</strong>nation.Babies <strong>and</strong> children are able <strong>to</strong> givefeedback clues which tell the adultsaround them when the simplified speechcan be modified a little <strong>to</strong> become morelike the type we would use <strong>to</strong> anexperienced language user.Bruner (1983) cites the work of Piagetin remarking that babies may spend theirtime doing only a small range of activities(such as banging, reaching, looking) butthey are systematic in their repetitionsof these activities. He adds that we shouldnot be surprised therefore, that they arealso systematic when it comes <strong>to</strong>language, as the work of Weir (1962)(on spontaneous speech in the cot)<strong>and</strong> Bowerman’s (1978) on children’sspontaneous errors, show.When babies <strong>and</strong> children interact witheach other, they have a different ‘voice’67

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!