13.07.2015 Views

Birth to three matters - Communities and Local Government

Birth to three matters - Communities and Local Government

Birth to three matters - Communities and Local Government

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

AcknowledgementsThe authors <strong>and</strong> publishers wish <strong>to</strong> thankthe copyright holder, Caroline Meggitt forpermission <strong>to</strong> reproduce the diagram onthe six aspects of health (Figure 1,inchapter 6).The authors’ gratitude must also go <strong>to</strong> themembers of the Manchester MetropolitanUniversity Early Years Research Team,the‘<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong> Three Matters’ Steering <strong>and</strong>Working Groups, the CCCUC Early YearsTeam <strong>and</strong> members of CISEC (the Centrefor International Studies in EarlyChildhood),<strong>and</strong> colleagues in other partsof the UK <strong>and</strong> other countries, forsuggested references, amendments <strong>and</strong>additions, especially Tina Bruce, RebeccaGoldman, Lesley Staggs, Angela Nurse <strong>and</strong>Kate Wall,whose thorough <strong>and</strong> supportivecritiques were most valuable.We should also thank the colleagues inso many fields <strong>and</strong> countries who havecarried out the exciting research aboutbabies <strong>and</strong> young children <strong>and</strong> whosework promotes the important message‘<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong> Three Matters.’ Most of all we want<strong>to</strong> thank all the babies <strong>and</strong> young childrenwho have given us endless hours ofpleasure, as we have observed <strong>and</strong>interacted with them. They have providedthe invaluable experiences that havehelped us underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> contextualisethe research.Tricia DavidKathy GoouchSacha PowellLesley AbbottAugust 20023


This review is primarily intended <strong>to</strong>supplement the pack ‘<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong> ThreeMatters:A framework <strong>to</strong> support childrenin their earliest years’ <strong>and</strong> it is about thebackground research <strong>and</strong> thinking onwhich the pack is based.THE CURRENT CONTEXTThe expansion in childcare outlined in theNational Childcare Strategy <strong>and</strong> the GreenPaper (DfEE 2001) have provided excitingchallenges <strong>and</strong> opportunities for thoseinvolved in the creation <strong>and</strong> developmen<strong>to</strong>f out-of-home experiences for youngchildren. The responsibilities that go alongwith these are enormous since, as theGreen Paper states, young children’ssubsequent development dependscritically upon their experiences in theearliest years of life.The deve l o p m e nt of a fra m ewo rk byGove rn m e nt <strong>to</strong> suppo rt pra ct i t i o n e r swo rking with children from birth <strong>to</strong> <strong>three</strong>is a miles<strong>to</strong>ne in re cognising <strong>and</strong> va l u i n gour youngest children <strong>and</strong> the co nt ri b u t i o nmade <strong>to</strong> their growth <strong>and</strong> deve l o p m e ntby the adults in their live s. It raises thes t atus of wo rk with this impo rt a nt ageg roup <strong>and</strong> marks the co m m i t m e nt of thep re s e nt gove rn m e nt <strong>to</strong> suppo rting quality<strong>and</strong> effe ct i ve pra ct i ce with children agedf rom birth <strong>to</strong> thre e.WHY BIRTH TO THREE?The age of <strong>three</strong>, traditionally the start ofearly years education in Engl<strong>and</strong>, <strong>and</strong>recognised by child development theorists<strong>and</strong> educationalists alike as a valuablestarting point for education,is now viewedby many as <strong>to</strong>o late <strong>to</strong> begin developingyoung children’s potential. Recentresearch,particularly in neuroscience,has pointed <strong>to</strong> the fact that in the processof caring for, <strong>and</strong> educating youngchildren,no time is <strong>to</strong>o soon <strong>to</strong> begin,withstudies showing that right from birth (<strong>and</strong>even before birth) children are alreadycompetent learners.Goldschmied <strong>and</strong> Jackson (1994) refer<strong>to</strong> ‘people under <strong>three</strong>’, not babies,<strong>to</strong>ddlers, or even children,but people withrights, which include being treated withdignity <strong>and</strong> respect.They also emphasisethe importance of good quality care beingbased not just on knowledge, but alsoon the ability of those adults <strong>to</strong> empathisewith them,<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> theexperiences <strong>and</strong> feelings of young childrenimaginatively, especially when they areseparated from their parents.PRINCIPLES WHICH UNDERPIN THE FRAMEWORKIn the Framework ‘<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong> Three Matters’,the principles underpinning it areidentified from a number of sourcesincluding The Early Years CurriculumGroup (1989); Bruce (1987); Lindon (2001)<strong>and</strong> from other representativeorganisations, leading child care writers<strong>and</strong> experts. These are as follows:-Parents <strong>and</strong> families are central <strong>to</strong> thewell-being of the child.6 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Relationships with other people (bothadults <strong>and</strong> children) are of centralimportance in a child’s life.Babies <strong>and</strong> young children are socialbeings, they are primed <strong>to</strong> learn <strong>and</strong>communicate.Learning is a shared process.Caring adults count more thanresources <strong>and</strong> equipment.Schedules <strong>and</strong> routines must flow withthe child.The whole day <strong>matters</strong>;babies <strong>and</strong>young children do not split up theirlives in<strong>to</strong> sections so neither shouldtheir adult carers.Children learn best when they are givenappropriate responsibility, allowed <strong>to</strong>make errors, decisions <strong>and</strong> choices, <strong>and</strong>respected as au<strong>to</strong>nomous <strong>and</strong>competent learners.Children learn most effectively when,with the support of a knowledgeable<strong>and</strong> trusted adult, they are activelyinvolved <strong>and</strong> interested.Children learn best by doing ratherthan by being <strong>to</strong>ld.Children are competent learnersfrom birth.These principles are supported by researchevidence presented in this review.However, the resulting Framework steersaway from subject headings, traditionalareas of experience <strong>and</strong> distinctcurriculum headings <strong>and</strong> takes as its focusthe child. It identifies four Aspects ofdevelopment. These are:- A Strong Child;A Skilful Communica<strong>to</strong>r; A CompetentLearner; <strong>and</strong> A Healthy Child. Eachof these Aspects celebrates the skill<strong>and</strong> competence of babies <strong>and</strong>young children <strong>and</strong> highlights theinterrelationship between growth,learning, development <strong>and</strong> theenvironment in which they are caredfor <strong>and</strong> educated.In this introduc<strong>to</strong>ry chapter readers willfind:- information about the Framework‘<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong> Three Matters’ <strong>and</strong> the Aspectswhich form its basic structure; anintroduction <strong>to</strong> some of the issuesconcerning research <strong>and</strong> theory focusedon young children’s development <strong>and</strong>learning;an outline of key researchconclusions on risk <strong>and</strong> resilience linked<strong>to</strong> experiences in the earliest years; <strong>and</strong>a summary of key ‘messages’.Th roughout the text we have addedi l l u m i n at i ve anecd o tes about individualc h i l d re n ,o b s e rved by family membe r swho are both ex pe ri e n ced early ye a r sp ra ctitioners <strong>and</strong> re s e a rc h e r s. Some ofthese anecd o tes have been used <strong>to</strong> showt h at while re s e a rch can tell us much abo u tc h i l d ren <strong>and</strong> their capabilities in genera l ,t h e re are times when a child will as<strong>to</strong> u n dclose family members by what Ga rd n e r( 1993) called their flashes of bri l l i a n ce.In other wo rd s, we hope pare nts <strong>and</strong>p ra ctitioners will not only read the rev i ewwith inte rest but also with an open <strong>and</strong>c ri t i cal mind, re f l e cting on the ways inwhich the re s e a rch info rms <strong>and</strong> re l ates<strong>to</strong> their own int i m ate kn owledge <strong>and</strong>underst<strong>and</strong>ings about young childre n .7


For those who would like more detailabout the literature search process, theAppendix explains how the team foundthe reports on different pieces of research<strong>and</strong> the kinds of information it seemedimportant <strong>to</strong> include. Following theintroduc<strong>to</strong>ry chapter, there are five mainchapters <strong>to</strong> the literature review,containing the following:-a brief introduction <strong>to</strong> influentialtheories <strong>and</strong> philosophies concernedwith early childhoodinformation about research relating<strong>to</strong> A Strong Childinformation about research concernedwith the baby <strong>and</strong> young child asA Skilful Communica<strong>to</strong>rinformation about research focusingon A Competent Learnerinformation about research onA Healthy Child<strong>and</strong> the review concludes with a furtherchapter providing some pointers aboutthe implications of the research forparents, policy makers, practitioners,trainers <strong>and</strong> researchers.The second chapter, which is anintroduction <strong>to</strong> theories <strong>and</strong> philosophiesexplains the ideas <strong>and</strong> thinking – ‘gr<strong>and</strong>’or key theories – which have helped people‘make sense’ of what they believe <strong>to</strong> bepatterns in very young children’sdevelopment <strong>and</strong> learning. Thesubsequent chapters outline research,mainly that carried out during the last tenyears, <strong>and</strong> review research reports relevant<strong>to</strong> each of the four Components of anAspect of the Framework.RESEARCH,DISCIPLINES,ASSUMPTIONSAND VALUESOwing <strong>to</strong> the broad scope of thecomponents in the pack,<strong>and</strong> the numberof different disciplines, or areas of study,which inform the field of Early ChildhoodEducation <strong>and</strong> Care (ECEC) concerningchildren from birth <strong>to</strong> <strong>three</strong>, this literaturereview covers a huge number of fields ofknowledge, ranging from law, social work,health <strong>and</strong> medicine, <strong>to</strong> developmentalpsychology, linguistics, cognitive science<strong>and</strong> neurophysiology, policy studies <strong>and</strong>sociology. Such a review could also haveincluded other areas of knowledge, suchas his<strong>to</strong>rical studies of early childhood <strong>and</strong>childrearing for example, because thesecould have provided insights in<strong>to</strong> the waysin which our attitudes <strong>to</strong> <strong>and</strong> beliefs aboutyoung children <strong>and</strong> their place in societyhave changed, <strong>and</strong>, as a result, how theways in which we actually treat childrenunder <strong>three</strong> have changed. Societychanges over time <strong>and</strong> we can learn fromanalyses of these changes. However, <strong>to</strong>include all the fields <strong>and</strong> all the researchthat could be used would have resultedin an encyclopaedia,<strong>and</strong> taken far longer<strong>to</strong> compile than the time allowed.We can also learn from observations <strong>and</strong>ex p l o rations of the ways in which peoplein other societies or co u nt ries tre at theirve ry young childre n ,w h at they be l i eve<strong>to</strong> be appro p ri ate provision for them <strong>and</strong>8 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


policies for families. So this lite rat u re rev i ewincludes some re po rts of re s e a rch fro ma b road as well as British ev i d e n ce. He rewe need <strong>to</strong> be cautious howeve r, be ca u s ed i f fe re nt co u nt ries have diffe re nt his<strong>to</strong> ri e s<strong>and</strong> what wo rks for one soc i e ty may not beri g ht for another for many diffe re nt re a s o n s( Au b rey 2001 ; Penn 2001 ). In particular weneed <strong>to</strong> be awa re of the assumptions <strong>and</strong>underst<strong>and</strong>ings about babies <strong>and</strong> yo u n gc h i l d ren which are preva l e nt in diffe re nts ocieties be cause these can affe ctre s e a rc h e r s, as well as pare nt s, p ra ct i t i o n e r s<strong>and</strong> po l i cy makers (David 1998 ; 2000 ).It is impo rt a nt <strong>to</strong> bear in mind that the keyt h e o ries <strong>and</strong> philosophies pre s e nted in therev i ew have been deve l o ped larg e ly inthe industrial <strong>and</strong> po s t - i n d u s t rial co u nt riesof the We s t / No rth (often called theMi n o ri ty Wo rld) <strong>to</strong>o, <strong>and</strong> that access<strong>to</strong> theories <strong>and</strong> kn owledge from theMa j o ri ty Wo rld co u nt ries could help usgain an even be t ter underst<strong>and</strong>ing ofyoung childre n’s enormous po te ntial <strong>and</strong>of how our assumptions may be limitingour ex pe ct at i o n s. At pre s e nt we havea c cess <strong>to</strong> far <strong>to</strong>o little info rm ation abo u tt h e o ries <strong>and</strong> ev i d e n ce from Ma j o ri ty Wo rl dco u nt ri e s, so it is exciting that re s e a rc h e r s<strong>and</strong> wri ters are beginning <strong>to</strong> make suchi n fo rm ation ava i l a b l e. As Je rome Bru n e r( 2000 : ix) state s :-‘Th e re is nothing in the wo rld <strong>to</strong> matc hchild re a ring for the depth <strong>and</strong> co m p l ex i tyof the challenges it poses both for thosed i re ct ly ca u g ht up in its daily int ri ca c i e s<strong>and</strong> for the soc i e ty <strong>to</strong> which child <strong>and</strong>ca re t a kers be l o n g. … To begin with, c h i l dre a ri n g, g i ven humans’ c u l t u ra la d a p t at i o n , is not stra i g ht - l i n eevo l u t i o n a ry ext ra po l ation of “b i o l og i ca ls pecies re p rod u ct i o n .” Cu l t u ra la d a p t at i o n , by any st<strong>and</strong>ard, is a big dealas well as a re ce nt one, pe rhaps only a halfmillion years old. Human immat u ri tyseems shaped (if a bit haphazard ly) <strong>to</strong> itsre q u i re m e nt s : not only <strong>to</strong> growing up pe rse (at be s t, rather a vapid idea) but <strong>to</strong>g rowing up Balinese or Ifaluk or Ja p a n e s e.And it is not only prolonged helplessnesst h at is special about human infancy, b u tits utter re l i a n ce on sustained <strong>and</strong>extended inte ra ction with a co m m i t te d<strong>and</strong> enculturated ca re g i ve r.’This chapter will now outline some of thekey issues involved in the study of babies<strong>and</strong> young children.IS CHILDREN’S DEVELOPMENT AND LEARNINGCOMPARTMENTALISED OR HOLISTIC?In the past, information about children’sdevelopment <strong>and</strong> learning has oftenbeen presented according <strong>to</strong> aspectssuch as physical development; emotionaldevelopment; social development;cognitive development; moraldevelopment; aesthetic development.As Greene (1999: 255) points out, thereare now‘interesting signs of attempts <strong>to</strong> breakdown the long-st<strong>and</strong>ing barriers betweenthe realms of cognition,emotion,<strong>and</strong>social development…In recent years therehas been a welcome renewal of discussionof the need for an holistic model of9


promote <strong>and</strong> enhance an individual child’scompetences <strong>and</strong> skills compared withother children who are progressing alonga more conventional pathway.‘In the early years/SEN realm interventionis purposeful <strong>and</strong> designed <strong>to</strong> effectas close a match as possible betweena young child’s identified special needs<strong>and</strong> that provision or resources whichwill meet his or her needs <strong>and</strong> bestfacilitate learning <strong>and</strong> development’(Wolfendale 2000:4).This is best achieved through givinga child access <strong>to</strong> as normal a learningsituation as possible within an inclusiveECEC context, whether that be a homeor a group setting, where perceptivepractitioners can take advantage of those‘teachable moments’ (Wilson 1998: 92)which occur naturally during the courseof a day.CONTINUITY AND DISCONTINUITY IN CHILDREN’SLIVES,DEVELOPMENT AND LEARNINGA further area of disagreement concernsthe question of the significance of theearliest years of life <strong>and</strong> whether whathappens during this period influencesthe whole life course. Certainly the Jesuitsbelieved this stage in human life <strong>to</strong> shapethe adult. Some psychologists havesuggested there are ‘sensitive periods’during babyhood, when certainexperiences are thought <strong>to</strong> have mostimpact. Others, such as Bowlby (1969;1973) claimed that there would be longlasting ill effects from experiences suchas institutionalised care during babyhood.As Bornstein (1999) argues, the idea thatearly childhood is especially important iscommon sense. This is the stage at whichthe nervous system is immature <strong>and</strong> at itsmost plastic <strong>and</strong> the prolongation of earlychildhood is thought <strong>to</strong> have adaptivesignificance.Thus it appears logical thatthe earliest years should cast an especiallylong shadow on<strong>to</strong> future life. Bornsteinstresses the complexity of fac<strong>to</strong>rsimpacting on this early development <strong>and</strong>there are a number of research studieswhich confound the view that earlychildhood is a ‘critical period’ – meaningdamage in this stage will be irreparable.For example, Rutter’s work with theRomanian Adoptees Study Team (1998)shows how children subjected early <strong>to</strong>very poor living conditions do catch upor at least are not irretrievably damaged.So Bornstein (1999: 29) concludes thatinfancy is ‘extremely important,…uniquelyformative, but not determinative’.Taking continuity <strong>to</strong> mean ‘smooth’development <strong>and</strong> learning, again thereare questions about whether childrenexperience ‘leaps’ in ability orunderst<strong>and</strong>ing – for example in chapter6 in the section about brain developmentthe view of Gopnik et al (1999) ispresented. These American cognitivescientists argue that at around 18 monthsof age there is a change in children’s brainstructure because the children’s brains‘reprogramme’ themselves, as a resul<strong>to</strong>f recognising that other people havedifferent minds/views/likes from their own.11


etween <strong>three</strong> <strong>and</strong> nine years old whichhave been enacted in the USA.In summary, the kinds of fac<strong>to</strong>rs whichappear <strong>to</strong> be linked <strong>to</strong> risk of lessfavourable development <strong>and</strong> lowerachievement in learning include:-fac<strong>to</strong>rs defined as within the child(intra-child),such as chronic illness,mental <strong>and</strong> physical disabilities. Thesemay be classed as biological (eg.genetic disorders, exposure in thewomb <strong>to</strong> the ill-effects of infectiousdiseases, drugs, alcohol); related <strong>to</strong>temperament – whether the baby is‘easy’ or ‘difficult’, babies with irregularbiological rhythms, irritable <strong>and</strong> hard<strong>to</strong> soothe babies – because these canput the parent-child relationship at risk<strong>and</strong> leave some children vulnerable<strong>to</strong> higher rates of anxiety in later life(Hirschfield et al 1992; Miller 1996)impaired parenting (for example,through severe parental mental illness,since this would probably mean thechild being exposed <strong>to</strong> a number ofinterlinked risk fac<strong>to</strong>rs, including timesof parental absence; experiences ofviolence, impaired family relationships)(Engle et al 1996; Tebes et al 2001)social disadvantage, such as familypoverty, lack of access <strong>to</strong> medical care,racism <strong>and</strong> minority status, exposure<strong>to</strong> community violence – includingchild abuse (Cairns <strong>and</strong> Dawes 1996;Davies 1999; Lowenthal 1998).It is thought that where a child ex pe ri e n ce st h ree or more risk fac<strong>to</strong> r s, both pare nts <strong>and</strong>child may be come ove rwhelmed <strong>and</strong> ri s ka c c u m u l ate s, p a rt i c u l a rly if there are nop ro te ct i ve mechanisms in place (Ga r b a ri n o1993 ; Myers <strong>and</strong> Taylor 1998 ). Ad d i t i o n a l ly,the length of time a child ex pe ri e n cesrisk fac<strong>to</strong>rs decreases the like l i h oodof be coming re s i l i e nt, p a rt i c u l a rly wherel i fe changes such as substitute ca re arei nvo lved (He n ry 1999 ; Ru t ter 2000).According <strong>to</strong> Davies (1999),the ways inwhich risk is likely <strong>to</strong> increase its impac<strong>to</strong>n a child <strong>and</strong> place that child in aposition where it is difficult <strong>to</strong> cope withthe stress involved are:-that the child has insufficient copingstrategies. The younger the child themore likely the negative effect becauseof the child’s inexperience. Farver <strong>and</strong>Frosch (1996) studied children who hadexperienced natural disasters <strong>and</strong> theyfound that those under five years oldwere more severely affected than werethe older childrenthe absence or inability of parentsor carers who can support the childin copingthe existence of a number of risk fac<strong>to</strong>rs– even a child with supportive parentsmay not be able <strong>to</strong> cope with life in anarea of poverty where violence iscommonplace. (A number of studieslink a wide range of risks <strong>to</strong> poverty <strong>and</strong>socio-economic status. These arereviewed in chapter 6.)the child has become passive as a resul<strong>to</strong>f <strong>to</strong>o much stress (for example beingsubjected <strong>to</strong> frequent abuse)13


possible. Thus stage theories are thought<strong>to</strong> impose a uniformity of both structure<strong>and</strong> direction on<strong>to</strong> changes related <strong>to</strong>the ageing process which may not beapplicable <strong>to</strong> the psychological aspectsof human beings. Although it is possible<strong>to</strong> modify sensory systems <strong>and</strong> thestructure of the brain by behaviouralexperiences (Greenough et al 1987),experience impacts more strongly onpsychological development than uponphysical development (with the exceptionof certain illnesses, accidents <strong>and</strong> severenutritional difficulties of course). As a resulta child’s psychological development doesnot simply ‘unfold’. Because theappearance of teeth generally (thoughnot always!) follows a particularpredictable sequence, it does not meanthat the changes occurring in cognitivedevelopment, or attachment, or havinga concept of oneself will be the same forevery child in the world. So the questionis – does development simply meanadditions <strong>to</strong> children’s knowledge <strong>and</strong>abilities, or are there actually qualitativechanges in the underlying structure whichmean children approach tasks or see theworld in different ways when they areolder? The idea of waiting for a child <strong>to</strong>develop, or mature, in order <strong>to</strong> be capableof learning something implies a belief ina pre-set, maturational stage theory,whereas it may be that an individual childrequires certain experiences from whichthey can learn something in order that thedesired development can then follow thelearning on which it depends. As Bee(1989) suggests following her reviewof development theories <strong>and</strong> research,the concept of stages seems initially <strong>to</strong>be a neat way of organizing what weknow, describing changes with age, butthat once one embarks on a discussionof cognitive development the conceptseems rather ‘slippery’ (Bee 1989: 12).The idea that simple explanations couldbe found <strong>and</strong> then applied <strong>to</strong> childrenin any situation has also been called in<strong>to</strong>question. Even some of the highlyrespected ‘Gr<strong>and</strong> Thinkers’ in the fieldof developmental psychology (suchas Bronfenbrenner, Bruner, Rutter, Baltes,Kagan, Donaldson) are willing <strong>to</strong> examinebasic assumptions, reflecting a‘changed consciousness <strong>and</strong> experienceaccumulated over the past decades <strong>to</strong>advance an underst<strong>and</strong>ing of childrenwhich does better justice <strong>to</strong> their complex,changing <strong>and</strong> multiple ways of being in<strong>and</strong> with their worlds.’ (Greene 1999: 265).So, while research projects are oftenlooking for patterns which might actas guides <strong>to</strong> children’s development,it is important <strong>to</strong> acknowledge the uniquepath of each child’s development <strong>and</strong>learning, <strong>to</strong>gether with the potentialinfluence of variations in culturalexpectations. In order <strong>to</strong> stress theimportance of recognising this flexibility<strong>and</strong> complexity, the Framework ‘<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong>Three Matters’ uses four broad categories<strong>to</strong> discuss diffe re nt phases of deve l o p m e nt,based on the American High/Scopeb<strong>and</strong>ings. These equate roughly <strong>to</strong>:-16 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


0 – 8 months: Heads Up, Lookers <strong>and</strong>Communica<strong>to</strong>rsDuring the first eight months youngbabies react <strong>to</strong> people <strong>and</strong> situationswith their whole bodies, neverthelessthey are competent in observing<strong>and</strong> responding <strong>to</strong> their immediateenvironment <strong>and</strong> communicatingwith those around them.8 – 18 months: Sitters, St<strong>and</strong>ers<strong>and</strong> ExplorersDuring the period from eight <strong>to</strong> 18months when babies’ exploration of theenvironment becomes more intentionaltheir increasing mobility <strong>and</strong> languagedevelopment enables them <strong>to</strong> find out<strong>and</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> more about their world.18 – 24 months: Movers, Shakers<strong>and</strong> PlayersFrom 18 <strong>to</strong> 24 months youngc h i l d ren begin <strong>to</strong> show incre a s i n gi n d e pe n d e n ce <strong>and</strong> obvious pleasurein mov i n g, co m m u n i cating <strong>and</strong>l e a rning through play.24 – 36 months: Walkers, Talkers<strong>and</strong> PretendersFrom 24 <strong>to</strong> 36 months children’scompetence at moving, talking <strong>and</strong>pretending is more <strong>and</strong> more evident<strong>and</strong> they show increasing confidencein themselves <strong>and</strong> skill in makingrelationships. However, most of theresearch literature reviewed for this textuses the ages of the children who wereinvolved, <strong>and</strong>, where that is the case,we have done likewise.Pe rhaps above all, it is re cognition fo rm e a n i n g <strong>and</strong> how young children <strong>and</strong> thosea round them share meanings in human lifewhich is gaining asce n d a n cy amongre s e a rc h e r s, so it is essential <strong>to</strong> ackn ow l e d g ethe plura l i ty <strong>and</strong> uniqueness of childre n’sex pe ri e n ces due larg e ly <strong>to</strong> co ntextual <strong>and</strong>c u l t u ral diffe re n ce s. From a ve ry early agebabies <strong>and</strong> young children search for themeaning of the be h aviours <strong>and</strong> speech ofthose around them.Th ey often seem <strong>to</strong>re cognise those be h aviours which givesomeone powe r. For ex a m p l e, when Co ra l i ewas eight months old she beamed <strong>and</strong>t ri u m p h a n t ly waved the te l evision co n t ro lwhich she had managed <strong>to</strong> get hold of for thefirst time in her life. This object had meaningin her household – <strong>and</strong> she appe a red <strong>to</strong> haverealised that whoever held it had some sor<strong>to</strong>f power (though she did not kn ow how <strong>to</strong> usethe co n t rol at that stage).Additionally, most psychologists no longerseek an answer <strong>to</strong> the question of whetherthe abilities <strong>and</strong> characteristics a childis born with (‘nature’) have a greaterimpact on their later achievements <strong>and</strong>personality than what they experience asthey grow up (‘nurture’). This thinking hasbeen superseded by the recognition thatboth are interdependent, a point which isfrequently reiterated throughout this text.DEFINING CHILDREN’S NEEDSIn attempting <strong>to</strong> establish evidence abouteffective practice in both home <strong>and</strong> groupsettings offering early childhoodeducation <strong>and</strong> care (ECEC) for babies <strong>and</strong>children aged between birth <strong>and</strong> <strong>three</strong>17


years, it is important <strong>to</strong> take accoun<strong>to</strong>f the kind of society in which childrenin Engl<strong>and</strong> are growing up – that it is amulticultural,pluralistic, democraticsociety.This means that children’sneeds will be defined in particular ways.Clearly, all children need adequate <strong>and</strong>appropriate nourishment <strong>and</strong> rest;shelter<strong>and</strong> protection from the elements <strong>and</strong>from harm; warm, responsive <strong>and</strong>affectionate relationships. Kellmer-Pringle(1980) suggested they also needopportunities <strong>to</strong> take responsibility <strong>and</strong>,of course, love, which is implied as a needby the United Nations Convention on theRights of the Child (UN 1989). However,many of the ‘needs’ that are consideredvital are so defined because of the values<strong>to</strong> which a society subscribes (Woodhead1990). Again,the call is for those of usin the field of ECEC <strong>to</strong> be aware of ourjudgements <strong>and</strong> the extent <strong>to</strong> whichwe are basing them on over simplifiedgeneralisations about children <strong>and</strong> theirupbringing. Martin Woodhead is notarguing that we should not define anyneeds, but that we should be able <strong>to</strong> saywhy we subscribe <strong>to</strong> those in whichwe believe:‘Children inherit a distinctively humannature as well as being brought up ina particular culture. Their dependency onothers <strong>to</strong> protect their interests during thelong period of human immaturity knownas childhood means that judgements mustcontinually be made by those responsiblefor them; although the length of theirdependency <strong>and</strong> the cultural articulationof what is in their best interests will varyfrom society <strong>to</strong> society <strong>and</strong> from time<strong>to</strong> time. The challenge is not <strong>to</strong> shy awayfrom developing a perspective onchildhood, but <strong>to</strong> recognize the pluralityof pathways <strong>to</strong> maturity within thatperspective. This is all the more importantat a time when the influence of childpsychology is extending well beyond thesocieties (notably North America <strong>and</strong>Europe) from which dominant theories<strong>and</strong> research data have been derived.’(Woodhead 1990:73)Research has shown how children’s needsare sometimes wrongly stereotypedbecause of ill-informed attempts atculturally defining them (see for example,Currer 1991). Dwivedi (1996) providesinsightful <strong>and</strong> useful comment onprofessional inhibitions <strong>and</strong> clumsinessabout racial <strong>and</strong> ethnic issues, culturalvalues <strong>and</strong> ‘pseudo insight’. Perhaps thebest way for a community <strong>to</strong> define whatmembers regard as young children’s needswould be <strong>to</strong> adopt the approaches of theworld famous nurseries of Reggio Emiliain Northern Italy, where parents,practitioners <strong>and</strong> politicians meet regularlyfor discussions about the place of childrenin society, what childhood is for, <strong>and</strong> howbest <strong>to</strong> provide for them.WHAT THIS REVIEW DOES AND DOES NOT COVERThe rev i ew provides re s e a rch ev i d e n cea bout babies’ <strong>and</strong> young childre n’sd eve l o p m e nt <strong>and</strong> learning be tween theages of birth <strong>and</strong> <strong>three</strong> ye a r s. Si n ce thefocus of the pack,‘Bi rth <strong>to</strong> Th ree Ma t te r s :A fra m ewo rk <strong>to</strong> suppo rt children in their18 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


e a rliest ye a r s’ 1 , is the children themselve s<strong>and</strong> effe ct i ve pra ct i ce, this is what weh ave focused on in the rev i ew of re s e a rc hl i te rat u re.The re s e a rch re po rts used arel a rg e ly, though not exc l u s i ve ly, f rom theU K , No rth Am e ri ca ,Au s t ralasia <strong>and</strong> otherEu ro pean co u nt ri e s, (in other wo rds thei n d u s t rialised <strong>and</strong> info rm ation societies ofthe Mi n o ri ty Wo rl d ).In all we have prov i d e dre fe re n ces <strong>to</strong> more than 500 re s e a rc hre po rts <strong>and</strong> books about childre n’s learn i n g<strong>and</strong> deve l o p m e nt be tween birth <strong>and</strong> thre eye a r s. Our search of the data bases yieldedthous<strong>and</strong>s of studies, m a ny of which wehad <strong>to</strong> eliminate (see Ap pendix ).Th u st h e re is a huge body of wo rk which co u l dh ave been included, but be cause of time<strong>and</strong> space co n s t ra i nts some aspe cts we red e l i be rate ly eliminated <strong>and</strong> some art i c l e sl e ft <strong>to</strong> one side be cause their messageswe re alre a dy loud <strong>and</strong> clear from otherwo rk – we apo l ogise in adva n ce <strong>to</strong> thosewhose wo rk would have been includedg i ven more time. In addition, the focus hasbeen on the children themselves <strong>and</strong>although articles about po l i cy, for ex a m p l e,discuss issues which impinge on the live sof the childre n , these have not be e ncove re d, nor some of the more spe c i a l i s e dre s e a rc h ,such as that pe rtaining <strong>to</strong>p a rticular medical co n d i t i o n s. Even someof the areas included could have be e nco n s i d e red in gre ater depth but we hopereaders will appre c i ate that each <strong>to</strong> p i ccould be – <strong>and</strong> often is – the focus of awhole book in its own ri g ht. As ex p l a i n e de a rl i e r, the sources of the lite rat u reincluded in the rev i ew we re larg e ly: -published boo k s, re fe reed pro fessional<strong>and</strong> academic journ a l s, we b s i tes <strong>and</strong> someunpublished re po rt s, which can be fo u n dby sco u ring data bases in libra ri e s.The other main source of info rm ationwas co nt a ct with colleagues in the UK<strong>and</strong> abroad who have wo rked in the fieldfor many years (see Ap pendix 1).Following this introduc<strong>to</strong>ry chapter, thesecond highlights some of the influentialresearch <strong>and</strong> theories concerned withearly childhood which have informedresearch <strong>and</strong> practice in Engl<strong>and</strong> over thelast hundred years.In the third chapter of the review, A StrongChild, we present some of the thinking<strong>and</strong> research findings related <strong>to</strong> a child’sgrowing awareness of self; recognitionof personal characteristics, preferences <strong>and</strong>capabilities; the need for recognition,acceptance <strong>and</strong> comfort, <strong>and</strong> being able<strong>to</strong> contribute <strong>to</strong> secure attachments; beingspecial <strong>to</strong> someone <strong>and</strong> exploringemotional boundaries; developing selfreliance as well as self confidence; <strong>and</strong>having a sense of belonging. The concep<strong>to</strong>f A Strong Child is one which runsthroughout the Framework ‘<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong> ThreeMatters’. We all want children <strong>to</strong> be strong,capable, confident <strong>and</strong> self-assured. Thevery early years are extremely importantfor babies <strong>and</strong> young children as theybegin on a journey of self-discovery from1The Framework in ContextIt is important that the Framework is considered within the context of the National St<strong>and</strong>ards for Under Eight’s Day Care <strong>and</strong>Childminding (DfEE 2001) <strong>to</strong>gether with the Curriculum Guidance for the Foundation Stage (DfES/QCA,2000) More detailedinformation about the nature of these links can be found on the CD ROM in the pack ‘<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong> Three Matters’ .19


a base of secure relationships with parents<strong>and</strong> key persons. The beginnings ofau<strong>to</strong>nomy can be seen in the relationshipswhich exist as babies <strong>and</strong> young childrenplay <strong>and</strong> explore in the context of a close,attentive <strong>and</strong> emotionally present adult.In order <strong>to</strong> become strong, the baby needsa nurturing environment in which theirkey person plays an essential role.By encouraging <strong>and</strong> supporting decisionmaking,empathising <strong>and</strong> providingopportunities for children,the key personhelps them grow emotionally <strong>and</strong> respond<strong>to</strong> successes <strong>and</strong> challenges. In theFramework, A Strong Child explores theways in which both the significant adults<strong>and</strong> the physical environment have animpact upon children's developing senseof themselves <strong>and</strong> their group identity,their affective world <strong>and</strong> theirrelationships with others.The fourth chapter provides researchinformation <strong>and</strong> ideas about the baby<strong>and</strong> child as A Skilful Communica<strong>to</strong>r.This includes:- being a sociable <strong>and</strong>effective communica<strong>to</strong>r; listening <strong>and</strong>responding appropriately <strong>to</strong> the languageof others;<strong>and</strong> making meaning.To become skilful communica<strong>to</strong>rs babies<strong>and</strong> children need <strong>to</strong> be <strong>to</strong>gether with akey person <strong>and</strong> with others in a warm <strong>and</strong>loving relationship. Being <strong>to</strong>gether leads <strong>to</strong>the wider development of social relationswhich include friendship, empathy, sharingemotions <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>and</strong> becominga competent language user. Early attemptsat finding a voice are rewarded in a varietyof ways, thus increasing babies’ <strong>and</strong> youngchildren’s confidence <strong>and</strong> encouragingthem <strong>to</strong> extend their range <strong>and</strong> increasetheir skills. Part of that, but by no meansall,is simply learning <strong>to</strong> make sense of thesounds around them. This is not a simpletask,as they have <strong>to</strong> begin <strong>to</strong> distinguishbetween the sounds of the language,including in<strong>to</strong>nation patterns <strong>and</strong> widerenvironmental sounds. From as early asseven months, a baby responds differently<strong>to</strong> some sounds over others <strong>and</strong> is able<strong>to</strong> distinguish sound patterns. Babies usetheir voices <strong>to</strong> make contact, let peopleknow what they need <strong>and</strong> how they feel.They are also starting <strong>to</strong> learn about‘conversation’. This requires the ability<strong>to</strong> listen <strong>and</strong> respond appropriately,underst<strong>and</strong>ing the importance of payingattention <strong>to</strong> sounds <strong>and</strong> language,interpreting non-verbal signals, imitating,repeating <strong>and</strong> mirroring others. Babies<strong>and</strong> young children also learn the ‘rules’of communication through makingmeaning with their key person <strong>and</strong> withother supportive adults <strong>and</strong> older childrenin relationships in which their earlyattempts <strong>to</strong> converse are interpreted,responded <strong>to</strong> <strong>and</strong> valued.In the chapter A Competent Learner,making connections (for example, throughthe senses) <strong>and</strong> developing the ability<strong>to</strong> compare, categorise <strong>and</strong> classify; beingimaginative <strong>and</strong> creative; <strong>and</strong> being able<strong>to</strong> use symbols <strong>to</strong> represent thoughts <strong>and</strong>language are the foci. Babies only a fewhours old gaze at patterns which resemblethe human face in preference <strong>to</strong> r<strong>and</strong>ompatterns.This shows that they are able <strong>to</strong>20 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


distinguish between things, <strong>and</strong> that theylike some things better than others. In thisway young children learn <strong>to</strong> discriminate<strong>and</strong> make connections between differen<strong>to</strong>bjects <strong>and</strong> experiences. As connectionsare made, the child makes increasing senseof the world. As babies explore the worldthrough <strong>to</strong>uch,sight, sound, taste, smell<strong>and</strong> movement, their sensory <strong>and</strong> physicalexplorations affect the patterns that arelaid down in the brain.Through repeatedexperience of people, objects <strong>and</strong>materials, young children begin <strong>to</strong> formmental images which lead them <strong>to</strong> imitate,explore <strong>and</strong> re-enact as they becomeimaginative <strong>and</strong> creative. Creativity,imagination <strong>and</strong> representation throughmark making allow children <strong>to</strong> share theirthoughts, feelings, underst<strong>and</strong>ings <strong>and</strong>identities with others, using drawings,words, movement, music, dance <strong>and</strong>imaginative play.Chapter six, A Healthy Child, brings<strong>to</strong>gether evidence about young children’smental <strong>and</strong> physical well-being, <strong>to</strong>getherwith children’s own growing knowledgeabout their own <strong>and</strong> others’ bodies <strong>and</strong>minds. Being healthy means much morethan having nutritious food <strong>and</strong> being freefrom illness. For babies <strong>and</strong> young childrenbeing special <strong>to</strong> someone <strong>and</strong> cared for isimportant for their physical,social <strong>and</strong>emotional health <strong>and</strong> well-being. Health<strong>and</strong> social well-being underpin <strong>and</strong>determine children's responses <strong>to</strong> theirenvironment, <strong>to</strong> people <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> newexperiences. Emotional well-beingincludes re l at i o n s h i p s, which are close,wa rm <strong>and</strong> suppo rt i ve ; being able <strong>to</strong>ex p ress feelings such as joy, g ri e f,f ru s t ration <strong>and</strong> fear leading <strong>to</strong> thedevelopment of coping strategies whenfaced with new, challenging or stressfuls i t u at i o n s.This chapter is there fo re closelylinked <strong>to</strong> chapter 3,since, as we shall see,emotional strength <strong>and</strong> self-esteem arei m po rt a nt ant i d o tes <strong>to</strong> some fo rms ofm e ntal illness <strong>and</strong> re l ationship pro b l e m s.Further, this is the chapter where readerswill find a précis of the current state ofinformation on early brain development,although once again the research linkswith <strong>and</strong> informs all the areas ofdevelopment <strong>and</strong> therefore all theAspects of the Framework.Meeting children’s physical needs isfundamental <strong>to</strong> their well-being. As s/heis growing <strong>and</strong> developing a child whois hungry, tired or uncomfortable willnot enjoy the company of adults <strong>and</strong>other children.A child who is physicallywell will have the energy <strong>and</strong> enthusiasm<strong>to</strong> benefit from the range of activities onoffer <strong>to</strong> her, equally she will get bored ifchoice is limited or provision inappropriate<strong>to</strong> her needs. Knowing when <strong>to</strong> ask forhelp, being protected <strong>and</strong> keeping safewith adult support, ensures that babies<strong>and</strong> young children quickly become skilfulin a range of movements involving bothlarge <strong>and</strong> fine mo<strong>to</strong>r control. In time, theymake healthy choices as they learn abouttheir bodies <strong>and</strong> what they can do.21


The final chapter brings the review <strong>to</strong> aconclusion <strong>and</strong> draws out some implicat i o n sfor practitioners <strong>and</strong> policy makers.SUMMARY OF KEY ‘MESSAGES’In this first chapter the contents of thewhole text have been introduced <strong>and</strong>issues related <strong>to</strong> the study of youngchildren’s development <strong>and</strong> learningoutlined. In particular the chapterhas presented:-links with the Framework ‘<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong> ThreeMatters’ <strong>and</strong> the importance of theexperiences <strong>and</strong> achievementsof people under <strong>three</strong>infancy as an extremely importantperiod of life but not determinativethe rationale for the Framework <strong>and</strong>the importance of providing optimalexperiences, especially sensitive,positive relationships with a smallnumber of familiar people, for childrenfrom birth <strong>to</strong> <strong>three</strong>, in order <strong>to</strong> promotedevelopment, learning <strong>and</strong> resiliencethe active, holistic nature of earlylearning <strong>and</strong> development <strong>and</strong> thenotion that environment <strong>and</strong> geneticsinteract <strong>to</strong> produce developmentkey information about thedevelopment of resilience, in particular,the crucial role played by a personor persons <strong>to</strong> whom a child ‘<strong>matters</strong>’the need for practitioners <strong>and</strong> policymakers <strong>to</strong> be informed about relevantresearch <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> recognise that whileresearch <strong>and</strong> theory can provide trends<strong>and</strong> ideas, there will be variations inearly learning <strong>and</strong> development – eachof the children they know is uniquethe need for awareness of differencesconcerning early learning <strong>and</strong>development across time <strong>and</strong> space –the impact of cultural assumptions <strong>and</strong>how this plurality can affect not onlythe ways in which children are treatedbut also the assumptions underlyingthe research <strong>and</strong> theories emanatingfrom different places at different timesthe impo rt a n ce of re cognising the ce nt ra lrole of pare nt s, the pri m a ry edu-ca re r s.22 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Chapter 2Influential research <strong>and</strong> theories concerned with early childhoodHOW THEORIES OF CHILD DEVELOPMENT CAN HELP USClearly there are problems connected with applying ‘gr<strong>and</strong>’ theories <strong>to</strong> all situations fora number of reasons. Some theories can be seen <strong>to</strong> be specific <strong>to</strong> particular societies <strong>and</strong><strong>to</strong> over generalise could be dangerous.For example, the famous Swiss researcherJean Piaget set out a theory of stagesrelated <strong>to</strong> the development of children’sthinking. One experiment demonstratedhis theory as applied <strong>to</strong> the underst<strong>and</strong>ingof conservation of volume (water usuallybeing used <strong>to</strong> fill the ‘volume’). By thishe meant that when young children arepresented with a full,tall narrow glassof water <strong>and</strong> see all the water poured in<strong>to</strong>a short wide glass, they will still argue thatthe tall narrow glass holds more water.Piaget claimed that a child needed <strong>to</strong> havereached between seven <strong>and</strong> 11 years ofage before they recognise this thinking asincorrect.This example helps practitionersthink about not only children’s thinking<strong>and</strong> how deceptive appearances of certainphenomena <strong>and</strong> events can be <strong>to</strong> youngchildren, it also makes us reflect on thekinds of questions we ask them,whetherthey are confused by the terminology –<strong>and</strong> also perhaps, whether children oftentell powerful adults what they think thoseadults want <strong>to</strong> hear! It should also make usreflect on the appropriateness of what weare dem<strong>and</strong>ing of young children, whetherthey have had enough real life experiences<strong>and</strong> h<strong>and</strong>s-on activity <strong>to</strong> be able <strong>to</strong> dealmeaningfully with the questions.Corinne Hutt, who was herself Sri Lankan,carried out a replication of Piaget’sconservation of volume experiment usingwater with children in both Engl<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong>Sri Lanka <strong>and</strong> found that the Sri Lankanchildren who fetched water regularly fortheir families (<strong>and</strong> so ‘experienced’ wateron a more frequent basis) could conservewater earlier than the English children,23


thus showing that the theory was ‘contextdependent’ (Hutt 1979).In other words,familiarity with particular activities <strong>and</strong>experiences, usually those that areembedded in the everyday life of the child,lead <strong>to</strong> developments in the abilitiesinvolved, as Margaret Donaldson (1978)pointed out. The task for early childhoodpractitioners is <strong>to</strong> make activitiesmeaningful <strong>to</strong> young children <strong>and</strong> so leadthem from those that make sense withina particular context <strong>to</strong> those which areless context-dependent (for examplebecoming able <strong>to</strong> count a small numberof objects anywhere when you are aboutfour years old, because you have hadexperiences of counting drinks or seatsneeded for a little group, or other objects,in the familiar world of your home,childminder’s or nursery).Some researchers (see for exampleDahlberg et al 1999; Singer 1992) arguethat one cannot generalise aboutchildren’s learning <strong>and</strong> development,that it is dangerous <strong>to</strong> impose ideas fromone culture on<strong>to</strong> others, because it isimportant <strong>to</strong> take account of the contextdependentnature of children’sunderst<strong>and</strong>ings. After all,it is important<strong>to</strong> note, as we argued in chapter 1,thattheories, as well as research,happen inparticular places at particular times <strong>and</strong>both are therefore subject <strong>to</strong> culturalassumptions <strong>and</strong> values. However, theimpact of postmodern 1 ideas cansometimes mean that, unintentionally,a flight from theorising follows – ‘throwingthe baby out with the bathwater’ – whichcan also be dangerous, because when weexplore the reasons underpinning practiceor policy we become aware of our ownassumptions, ideas or theories, <strong>and</strong> we canenhance them by debating the ‘gr<strong>and</strong>theories’. By reflecting on ‘gr<strong>and</strong> theories’in the light of our own practicalexperience, we can decide how applicableeach of them is <strong>to</strong> the children we workwith <strong>and</strong> in our own work contexts. Butwe also need <strong>to</strong> see the extent <strong>to</strong> whichtheories are supported by researchevidence, at least where they can beresearched empirically. We can thenaccept, adapt or reject the ‘gr<strong>and</strong> theory’in question <strong>and</strong> be more aware of our owntheories – how we ‘make sense’ of children– <strong>and</strong> how our theories affect our work.So some researchers (for example, Rabanet al 2002) propose that we should usea range of theories, while recognising theirlimitations, so that we can move the fieldof Early Childhood Education <strong>and</strong> Care(ECEC) forward, <strong>and</strong> include ideas <strong>and</strong>research findings from other relevantfields.This chapter outlines the majorMinority World child developmenttheories, which have influenced the fieldof ECEC <strong>and</strong> readers are urged <strong>to</strong> use thereferences <strong>to</strong> pursue further debate.1Philip Gammage (1999:163) argues that the ‘core of postmodernism is essentially this:a time when rules, cus<strong>to</strong>ms, beliefs, values <strong>and</strong>ideas are subjected <strong>to</strong> reanalysis, when a greater awareness of their context-dependent, subjective nature is revealed’.24 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


THE ISSUE OF THEORIES AS CULTURALCONSTRUCTIONSAs a result of recognition that the childdevelopment theories espoused in theMinority World may be affected by thecontext in which they were developed –because the children <strong>and</strong> the researcherswere influenced by the assumptions,values, expectations <strong>and</strong> practices ofa particular time <strong>and</strong> place, theories arenow seen as ‘cultural constructions’.Nevertheless, as Bruce maintains,‘Theories help us <strong>to</strong> predict <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>anticipate how children might behave <strong>and</strong>react. They help us structure what weobserve.Theories help us <strong>to</strong> make senseof what we see…When we analyse play,we find ourselves linking what we havefound with what other people (theorists)have found. We may find our observationsfit with theories. We may find that theydo not. This will help us think deeply…’(Bruce 2001: 19).This chapter merely introduces someof the key ‘gr<strong>and</strong>’ theories <strong>and</strong> theresearchers who have proposed them.It is not possible <strong>to</strong> provide a long critiqueof each theory, simply <strong>to</strong> point up wherefurther information can be found forreaders who feel tantalised by thesedeliberately brief overviews. The theoriesincluded are:- psychoanalytic; learning;cognitive developmental; ecologicalsystems; information processing; <strong>and</strong>language development theory.BIOLOGY AND MATURATIONBiological theories of human developmentusually suggest that there is a setsequence or pattern of change throughmaturation which is geneticallyprogrammed. The main pioneer theorist ofthis view was Gesell (Gesell <strong>and</strong> Armatruda1947). According <strong>to</strong> this type of theory, anindividual’s temperament would also beinherited <strong>and</strong> persist throughout their life.In the 19 70 s, Hi n d e’s Bi o l og i cal Bases ofHuman Deve l o p m e n t a rgued that in ord e r<strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> human deve l o p m e nt weshould use the same approaches as thoseused <strong>to</strong> study animals – so what is kn ow nas human ethology was bo rn . St u d i e sco n d u cted in the 1970 s, such as Huttet al (1989) Pl ay, Le a rning <strong>and</strong> Ex p l o ra t i o n,a d o p ted the approach of non-part i c i p a n<strong>to</strong> b s e rvation in at tempts <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> thep rocesses affe cting young childre n’s live s<strong>and</strong> learn i n g.The idea of the impo rt a n ceof bonding for a new bo rn baby, or animal,<strong>and</strong> at t a c h m e nt <strong>to</strong> a particular nurt u ri n gf i g u re (usually the baby’s mother) linkse t h o l ogy with Bow l by’s lite rat u re onat t a c h m e nt (see late r ). Other aspe cts ofe t h o l og i cal theory include pro po s i t i o n sa bout sensitive (or, m o re ri g i d ly, c ri t i ca lpe ri od s ); a l t ruism – helping others forno appare nt gain <strong>to</strong> oneself, e s pe c i a l lyin re l ation <strong>to</strong> surv i val <strong>and</strong> well being (fo rex a m p l e, is a human more like ly <strong>to</strong> helpa person who shares their genetic make - u pthan one who does not?); d o m i n a n ce <strong>and</strong>s u b m i s s i o n .The lat ter aspe cts link25


e t h o l og i cal theory with soc i o - b i o l ogy (seefor ex a m p l e, Daw kins 1976 ).Murray Thomas (1996) proposes abio-electrical ‘model’ of child developmentwhich,he claims, explains children’sphysical development well,in terms of thebody’s physical structure but thisapproach fails <strong>to</strong> cover children’s emotions<strong>and</strong> thoughts in any satisfac<strong>to</strong>ry way.The experience of young children bornwith immense physical problems, suchas cerebral palsy or Down’s syndrome,who have overcome many of theirapparent difficulties <strong>to</strong> go on <strong>to</strong> leadrich <strong>and</strong> exciting lives also refutes thisargument (see for example Gr<strong>and</strong>in 1996).To some extent, recent gene <strong>and</strong> brainresearch, <strong>to</strong>gether with some thinkingfrom neuroscience may be revivingbiological theories but critics are awarethat there may be dangers in believinghuman development <strong>and</strong> learning <strong>to</strong> befixed, limited or immutable.Biological explanations of childhoodIn exploring the evolution of childhood,Bogin (1998) argues that there are soundevolutionary reasons for prolongedimmaturity, which include:- feeding(a child can be fed by any other memberof the group once weaned); the relativelylow cost of feeding a child compared witha fully grown adult; the fact that nurturingcan be done by other members of thespecies, so freeing the mother – childrenretain an infantile appearance, sostimulating nurturing responses;developmental ‘plasticity’ is possible –they can adapt <strong>to</strong> different or changingenvironments more readily. Bogin claimsthat these fac<strong>to</strong>rs contribute <strong>to</strong> thesuperior survival rate of human offspringcompared with other animals.FREUD AND THE PSYCHOANALYTIC TRADITIONFre u d’s psyc h o a n a lytic theory is impo rt a nt<strong>to</strong> the field of early childhood be cause hewas one of the first <strong>to</strong> draw at te ntion<strong>to</strong> the ways in which babies’ <strong>and</strong> yo u n gc h i l d re n’s ‘inner live s’ <strong>and</strong> ex pe ri e n ce scould be shaping their deve l o p m e nt <strong>and</strong>l e a rn i n g. He pro posed diffe re nt levelsof consciousness in the mind, with theu n co n s c i o u s being a re ceptacle <strong>to</strong> whichhumans ‘s e n d’ kn owledge about aspe ctsof their lives of which they either do notneed or wa nt <strong>to</strong> remain co n s c i o u s. He alsos u g g e s ted that there are pre d e te rm i n e dstages in human ‘p s yc h o s ex u a ld eve l o p m e nt’ <strong>and</strong> that the mouth, a n u s<strong>and</strong> genitals are sensitive parts of the body– e rogenous zo n e s – which are succe s s i ve lyi nvo lve d. Freud argued that childre n’sc h i l d h ood <strong>and</strong> adult re l ationships witho t h e r s, <strong>and</strong> feelings about themselve sd e pend upon their ex pe ri e n ces duri n geach of these psyc h o s exual stages. Fre u d’s‘m od e l’ of child deve l o p m e nt was deri ve dm o re from his re f l e ctions on his adultp at i e nt s’ re co l l e ctions of childhoodex pe ri e n ces <strong>and</strong> was not based upo no b s e rvations of childre n . His daughte r,Anna Fre u d, <strong>to</strong>ok his wo rk on in this are a ,26 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


p a rt i c u l a rly in re l ation <strong>to</strong> psyc h o a n a ly t i ct re at m e nt for children with emotionalp ro b l e m s. She adapted the ideas ofp s yc h o a n a lysis <strong>to</strong> provide underst<strong>and</strong>ingsa bout all childre n . Although psyc h o a n a lys i shas never appe a red <strong>to</strong> have gre at lyi n f l u e n ced Ea rly Ch i l d h ood Ed u cation <strong>and</strong>Ca re (ECEC) in Bri t a i n , Freud <strong>and</strong> hiscolleagues must be ackn owledged fo rd rawing at te ntion <strong>to</strong> the impo rt a n ce ofthis phase in life <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> the fact that babiesdo have an ‘inner life’ which shapes theirl ater inte ra ctions <strong>and</strong> we l l - be i n g.Erikson (1963; 1968) built upon Freud’stheory <strong>to</strong> propose theoretical perspectiveson healthy personalities <strong>and</strong> how theydevelop. He argued that growing up isa process of achieving a unified self thatremains the same over time <strong>and</strong> that oneaccepts.This self should ‘fit’ with theexpectations of one’s society <strong>and</strong> culture<strong>and</strong> share some characteristics with othersin that group.This was called having anego identity. People who have attained anego identity ‘know who they are’ <strong>and</strong> arecomfortable within the group culture theyshare.The effects of disability on theformation of a strong ego identity need<strong>to</strong> be taken in<strong>to</strong> account. Positive thinkingon the part of those who care for, <strong>and</strong>work with, very young children as theydevelop a sense of self are crucial.‘Recognising disability as a positiveidentity is not easy in our society. As theygrow up, disabled children will receivenegative messages about being disabled<strong>and</strong> need a positive internal model ofdisabled identity <strong>to</strong> counteract negativestereotypes.’ (Marchant 2001: 221).Erikson accepted Freud’s stages <strong>and</strong> againbuilt on these, suggesting the process ofphysical <strong>and</strong> psychological developmentbegins in the womb <strong>and</strong> follows inner lawsof development, so Erikson ‘encapsulatedthe biological thinking <strong>and</strong> theprescriptiveness behind these stagetheories’ (Greene 1999:252). However,he also re cognised that Fre u d’s theory didnot take account of the variations in thepatterning of behaviour attributable <strong>to</strong>different cultures <strong>and</strong> he proposed thata number of psychosocial crises beseteach individual in adjusting <strong>to</strong> a part i c u l a rsocial environment. In the earliest yearsof life these include struggles between:trust v. mistrust; <strong>and</strong> au<strong>to</strong>nomy v. shame<strong>and</strong> doubt. Eri k s o n’s theory has part i c u l a rlyi n f l u e n ced thinking about the deve l o p m e n<strong>to</strong>f self-esteem <strong>and</strong> the self-concept, whichwe explore further in chapter 3.Following in this tradition, Winnicott (1964;1971) <strong>and</strong> Bowlby (1951; 1953; 1969; 1973;1980) were central figures in the Britishpsychoanalytic movement. In particular,Bowlby’s attachment theory wasinterpreted as indicating negative effectswould result for babies whose motherswent out <strong>to</strong> work,<strong>and</strong> since it becameknown shortly after the Second World War,when women were being encouraged <strong>to</strong>return <strong>to</strong> being home-makers instead ofworkers, it was regarded as being used27


politically. Bowlby had concluded fromseeing the grief <strong>and</strong> despair of youngchildren separated for long periods of timefrom the adult/s <strong>to</strong> whom they hadbecome attached (usually their mothers),that attachment is vitally necessary <strong>to</strong> thedeveloping infant <strong>and</strong> that maternaldeprivation would have damaging longterm consequences. (See chapter 3 formore about research on attachment).Later, in 1972, Rutter’s reassessment of thebasis for maternal deprivation theorywas welcomed. Rutter pointed out howthe ideas had been taken <strong>to</strong> extremes<strong>and</strong> his work through the last 30 yearshas continued <strong>to</strong> draw attention <strong>to</strong> theneed <strong>to</strong> treat young children sensitively<strong>and</strong> for re s e a rch <strong>to</strong> be used appro p ri ate ly(see further references <strong>to</strong> Rutter inchapters 1 <strong>and</strong> 3).What Rutter stressed in particular aboutBowlby’s work,however, was that he hadbeen right <strong>to</strong> draw attention <strong>to</strong> the poorquality of much substitute care at the timeof his report <strong>to</strong> the World HealthOrganisation in 1951 <strong>and</strong> it is a messagewe should dwell on as we increaseprovision for babies <strong>and</strong> young children.In the same way that Bettelheim (1987)wrote about ‘good enough parenting’those in the field of ECEC need <strong>to</strong> debate‘good enough edu-caring’. One of the aimsof this literature review is <strong>to</strong> help informthat very issue <strong>and</strong> encourage the debate.LEARNING THEORIESConditioning <strong>and</strong> Behaviourist theoryWhile the theories pre s e nted so far havebegun from the notion that ‘n a t u re’ ( w h ata child is bo rn with) fo rms the basis fo rl ater deve l o p m e nt, Le a rning Th e o ries arebased on the idea that it is ‘n u rt u re’, o rex pe ri e n ce, t h at is the most influentialin its impact on human deve l o p m e nt. Ea rlyre s e a rch ca rried out in Russia by Pav l ov,whose dogs salivated at the sound of a be l la fter a few ex po s u res <strong>to</strong> the bell ringing asfood was pre s e nte d, began a theore t i ca lpe r s pe ct i ve kn own as co n d i t i o n i n g.In the West, the most famous theorist inthe Behaviourist tradition is Skinner (1972),who built on the early ideas of Watson inthe USA, <strong>to</strong> formulate the theory ofoperant conditioning.This theory proposesthat there are two types of reinforcement.The first is called positive reinforcement,<strong>and</strong> this means a reward is given fordesired behaviour, while the second,negative reinforcement, means eithersomething is s<strong>to</strong>pped or somethingunpleasant, or punishment, happens whenthe desired behaviour is not shown. So forexample, when an individual child allowsanother child <strong>to</strong> take <strong>and</strong> play with atreasured <strong>to</strong>y, s/he would be rewardedin some way (eg. praise, reassurance, beingpassed a different <strong>to</strong>y). This theory hasunderpinned many of the interventionprogrammes devised for children withspecial needs, for example, those28 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


developed by Bereiter <strong>and</strong> Engelmannin the 1960s, which still have echoes <strong>to</strong>day.As a result of such approaches takinga largely transmission model of learning(ie. the adults know something theydecide they will teach <strong>to</strong> the children <strong>and</strong>impose upon them),they do not take in<strong>to</strong>account the rich,natural experiencesavailable, such as those that arise duringplay alongside children,<strong>and</strong> using thechildren’s interests <strong>to</strong> help them learn(Marchant 2001; Wall forthcoming).Social learning theoryAl be rt Ba n d u ra (1973; 1982 ; 1992) is thekey pro po n e nt of this theore t i ca lpe r s pe ct i ve. This theory argues that muche a rly learning comes from the childa ct i ve ly i m i t a t i n g or observing m od e l l i n gby others in the family or gro u p. Soc i a ll e a rning theorists would argue with theBe h av i o u ri s t s, by saying that childre nl e a rn when they do not get a dire ctrewa rd, for ex a m p l e, s ay i n g ‘thank yo u’be cause this has been modelled by keypeople in their live s. One of the essent i a lmessages <strong>to</strong> come out of this theory fo rthe early years is that Ba n d u ra suggestsbabies <strong>and</strong> young children can learn fro mus as m od e l s of actions but also oft h i n king – if we talk out loud as we ares o lving a pro b l e m , we are modelling ourt h i n ki n g. Si n ce being able <strong>to</strong> pay closeat te ntion <strong>to</strong> the mod e l’s be h aviour <strong>and</strong>/orlanguage is impo rt a nt for this theory <strong>to</strong>a p p ly, Ba n d u ra (1992: 19) says :-Adults alter the behaviour they model <strong>to</strong>compensate for the attentional limitationsof children…parents gain their attention<strong>and</strong> give salience <strong>to</strong> the behaviour theywant <strong>to</strong> encourage by selectively imitatingthem. Parents tend <strong>to</strong> perform thereciprocated imitations in an exaggeratedanimated fashion that is well designed <strong>to</strong>sustain the child’s attentiveness at a highlevel during the mutual modellingsequences…’Further aspects of this theory that areimportant are concerned with:- children’smemory skills, the ability <strong>to</strong> convertconceptions in<strong>to</strong> appropriate actions(these are thought of as transformationalskills);<strong>and</strong> motivation – having incentivesfor wanting <strong>to</strong> imitate modelled behaviour.Ba n d u ra co m m e nts part i c u l a rly on genderroledeve l o p m e nt <strong>and</strong> his theory can beseen <strong>to</strong> be re l eva nt <strong>to</strong> the ways in whichve ry young children notice verbal labe l l i n g,the cate g o ri s ation of males <strong>and</strong> females inthe wo rld around them, <strong>and</strong> the mod e l l i n gof gendered ro l e s.Th u s, a c co rding <strong>to</strong> thist h e o ry, adults will influence childre n’so u t l ook <strong>and</strong> be h aviour through their ow nm od e l l i n g. Engaging in po s i t i ve,‘out loud’t h i n king <strong>and</strong> problem solv i n g, t h ey ca nhelp children develop these be h av i o u r s<strong>and</strong> attitudes <strong>to</strong>o, as Sa l ly Lu be c k’s (19 86 )re s e a rch demonstrated in two diffe re ntn u r s e ry settings, w h e re the children we rere s pe ct i ve ly indepe n d e nt <strong>and</strong> co m pe t i t i veor inte rd e pe n d e nt <strong>and</strong> coo pe rat i ve, inline with the values embedded in eachs e t t i n g’s pra ct i ce s.29


COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENTAL THEORIESJean Piaget’s CognitiveDevelopment TheoryPiaget thought of knowledge not as abody of facts s<strong>to</strong>red in the human mindbut as a process <strong>and</strong> he argued that theolder a child becomes the more s/he isable <strong>to</strong> produce mental images, thinkingabout things through interiorised action.So for Piaget, a child’s development wascharacterised by a constant effort <strong>to</strong> refine<strong>and</strong> exp<strong>and</strong> a ‘reper<strong>to</strong>ire of mental actions’(Murray Thomas 1996: 234).As organisms, humans were thoughtby Piaget <strong>to</strong> be also constantly adapting<strong>to</strong> their environments <strong>and</strong> he called theprocesses by which humans do thisschemas (or schemata). Athey (1990) whohas carried out much research on this areadefines a schema as: ‘a pattern ofrepeatable behaviour in<strong>to</strong> whichexperiences are assimilated <strong>and</strong> that aregradually coordinated’ (Athey 1990: 37).She has identified <strong>and</strong> named a numberof these patterns of behaviour. For a baby,grasping a <strong>to</strong>y, or a feeding bottle, can bea schema, because in babies schemas arelimited since a newborn baby will onlyhave reflexes such as sucking, crying, butthe number of schemas, particularlyintellectual schemas, will increase rapidlyduring the first year. At the same timethe schemas are becoming interlinkedin a very complex way. Piaget called theprocesses by which schemas evolveassimilation <strong>and</strong> accommodation.According <strong>to</strong> this theory, assimilation is theprocess of absorbing new knowledge <strong>and</strong>fitting it in<strong>to</strong> what one already knows.Accommodation happens when what oneknows is not easy <strong>to</strong> match <strong>to</strong> some newknowledge, so the ‘old’ knowledge ischanged in some way, so that new <strong>and</strong>old fit <strong>to</strong>gether <strong>and</strong> make sense.Piaget’s theory has been influential in thefield of ECEC <strong>and</strong> he was in constantcontact with the English nursery pioneerSusan Isaacs, who debated his theorieswith him on the basis of her observationsof young children. His work was ofteninterpreted as meaning that childrenshould be provided with an environmentrich in possible experiences, but one inwhich the adults did not join in thechildren’s play bouts. (See also chapter 5).Piaget is also well known for havingp ro posed a series of bro a d ly age-b<strong>and</strong>ed,but universal stages in children’sdevelopment.The two stages mostrelevant <strong>to</strong> this review are that frombirth <strong>to</strong> around two years of age – thes e n s o ri m o <strong>to</strong> rpe ri od ; <strong>and</strong> that from ro u g h lytwo <strong>to</strong> seven years – the preoperationalthought stage.The sensori m o <strong>to</strong>r stage, Piaget furt h e rs u bdivided in<strong>to</strong> two main sub-pe ri ods withsix sub-stages, the first being one lastinga bout four mont h s, in which babies aresaid <strong>to</strong> co n ce nt rate on their own bod i e s,re pe ating actions over <strong>and</strong> ove r, then in thes e cond sub-stage still re pe ating act i o n seven when they are in diffe re nt or less30 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


familiar settings. By about one year old,a c co rding <strong>to</strong> this theory, an infant isex pe ri m e nt i n g, t rying <strong>to</strong> discover in whatways an object or eve nt is nove l .The ve ryyoung child is thought <strong>to</strong> be gra d u a l lycoming <strong>to</strong> kn ow about pe rm a n e n ce <strong>and</strong>a bout cause <strong>and</strong> effe ct. By eighte e nm o nths <strong>to</strong> two ye a r s, an infant is said<strong>to</strong> no longer need actual objects <strong>to</strong> solvep ro b l e m s, be cause s/he can hold a ment a lre p re s e nt ation of them in her/his mind.Piaget stressed the impo rt a n ce oflanguage during the pre o pe rational pe ri od,be cause being able <strong>to</strong> use languageenables a child <strong>to</strong> co m m u n i cate moree f fe ct i ve ly with other pe o p l e, <strong>to</strong> inte rn a l i s ewo rds as thought s, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> inte rn a l i s ea ct i o n . Ch i l d ren at this stage ca ni n c re a s i n g ly imagine things they ca n n o tsee or eve nts that happened in the past.So Piaget’s theorising has been importantin the underst<strong>and</strong>ing of ‘objectpermanence’ – meaning that in the earlyphase of life a baby does not know tha<strong>to</strong>bjects – or indeed people – still existwhen they cannot be seen.This is oneof the explanations for the fact that whenonly a few months old, babies do not seemdistressed when their familiar carers orparents leave but later – by around six<strong>to</strong> eight months, they do becomedistressed. Piaget also thought that veryyoung children are unable <strong>to</strong> distinguishthemselves from the environment – soit is by sensing the difference betweenputting one’s <strong>to</strong>e or a <strong>to</strong>y in one’s mouththat a baby begins <strong>to</strong> know theboundaries of the self.Members of the group known as thePost-Piagetians have recognised the waysin which criticisms of his theories needed<strong>to</strong> be addressed, for example by exploringthe extent <strong>to</strong> which a child’s thinking isdependent on context (see for exampleDonaldson 1978).Another psyc h o l ogist who adopted Pi a g e t’sf ra m ewo rk , Ko h l be rg (1984),p ro po s e dstages in the deve l o p m e nt of childre n’sm o ral deve l o p m e nt. While much of his wo rkis co n ce rned with older childre n , his ideasa bout the ways in which children inte rn a l i s em o ral st<strong>and</strong>ards can info rm early ye a r sp ra ct i ce. He suggests that children fro mfamilies which discuss moral issues, ex p l a i nm o ral judgement s, <strong>and</strong> model be h av i o u rbased on a moral code have a firmer gra s pof moral issues. Ko h l be rg called the earl i e s tstage in his theory the p re m o ra l l eve l .Ac co rding <strong>to</strong> Ko h l be rg, in this phase a childwill accept an adult’s ve rd i ct on whethersomething is ri g ht or wro n g. Gra d u a l ly,a child learns <strong>to</strong> act in ways which willgain approva l , so their moral judgementis depe n d e nt on the judgements of theadults around them. Later in life a childwill inte rnalise official ru l e s, pe rhaps alsoreaching a po i nt in deve l o p m e nt whens/he makes moral judgements based onu n i versal ethical pri n c i p l e s.Ca rol Gilligan (1982) provides a challenge<strong>to</strong> Ko h l be rg’s theory be cause she arg u e s31


t h at there will be diffe re n ces in mora ld e c i s i o n s, d e pending upon whether ape r s o n’s moral stance is based on abstra ctp rinciples or upon at te ntion <strong>to</strong> the effe ct son pe o p l e. Gilligan argues that women <strong>and</strong>men take these diffe re nt pri n c i p l e ds t a n d po i nts <strong>to</strong> underpin their mora ld e c i s i o n s, with men using genera l ,a b s t ra ctp rinciples or ru l e s, <strong>and</strong> women tending<strong>to</strong> ca re more about the effe cts part i c u l a rdecisions or actions would have on pe o p l e.Piaget’s ideas <strong>and</strong> research have beenimportant <strong>to</strong> not only those known as thePost-Piagetians – like Donaldson, Hughes,Grieve <strong>and</strong> others – but also <strong>to</strong> morerecent work by researchers such asKarmiloff-Smith (1992) <strong>and</strong> Gopnik et al(1999),who indicate his enduringinfluence. In fact the latter group maintainthe spirit of Piaget but give a current twist<strong>to</strong> his work by demonstrating that babies‘arrive’ knowing a lot, use this <strong>to</strong> learnmore, <strong>and</strong> need other people interactingwith them <strong>to</strong> help with that learning:-‘We can see how our knowledge emergesfrom the ideas we start out with,ourability <strong>to</strong> learn,<strong>and</strong> our interactions withother people.’ (Gopnik et al 1999: 106)These contemporary researchers, whilefollowing in Piaget’s tradition ofrecognising the ways in which childrenactively construct knowledge, haveovercome the emphasis on the worldof objects by suggesting that babies focusfirst <strong>and</strong> foremost on people, <strong>and</strong> thenon the objects <strong>and</strong> materials around them(Gopnik et al 1999; Goswami 1998; M<strong>and</strong>ler1996; Murray <strong>and</strong> Andrews 2000).It is these ‘messages’ that we can take<strong>to</strong> help us underst<strong>and</strong> early development<strong>and</strong> learning – that right from birth, youngchildren are active learners, deeplyinterested in people, who are trying <strong>to</strong>‘make sense’ of the world around them.Vygotsky’s theory of child developmentAlthough the Russian psyc h o l ogist LevVyg o t s ky died in the 1930 s, his ideas didnot reach the West until about 30 ye a r sl ate r. His theory is based on the fo l l ow i n gideas:- that act i v i ty generates thought ;t h at language <strong>and</strong> thinking deve l o ps e p a rate ly but re l ate <strong>to</strong> each other ina ve ry co m p l ex way; t h at children passt h rough the same sequences in stagesof deve l o p m e nt but that ‘Hi g h e rp s yc h o l og i cal functions are not simplys u pe ri m posed as a second s<strong>to</strong> ry over thee l e m e nt a ry proce s s e s ; t h ey re p re s e nt newp s yc h o l og i cal sys te m s.’ (Vyg o t s ky 1978 :124 ). For those in the field of ECEC,Vyg o t s ky is impo rt a nt be cause his wo rkre cognises the key role played by adults<strong>and</strong> other children who kn ow more thanthe baby or young child <strong>and</strong> who suppo rtthe baby in learn i n g. In other wo rds hist h e o ry re cognised that learning is a soc i a la ct i v i ty that happens in cultura l ly re l eva ntco ntext s. Wh e re Piaget was depicted asseeing the child as a ‘lone scient i s t’ d u ri n gp l ay, Vyg o t s ky’s theory has be e nc h a ra cte rised as po rt raying the yo u n g32 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


child as learning <strong>and</strong> developing in play f u li nte ra ction with others.Building on Vygotsky’s ideas, Bruner(1984;1990) <strong>and</strong> Nelson (1986) haveprovided much valuable theorisingconcerned with human development<strong>and</strong> learning, in particular popularisingthe notion of scaffolding – the behaviour<strong>and</strong> language adults <strong>and</strong> ‘significan<strong>to</strong>thers’ provide for young children <strong>to</strong> helpthem achieve something in which theyare interested.Vygotsky <strong>and</strong> Piaget were in broadagreement that children are activelearners <strong>and</strong> that knowledge is notacquired by a process of accretion, ratherthe child ‘constructs’ knowledge,transforming both the new knowledge<strong>and</strong> existing knowledge <strong>to</strong> ‘make sense’.While Vygotsky emphasised the social <strong>and</strong>cultural aspects <strong>and</strong> the biological aspec<strong>to</strong>f learning is implicit, Piaget put moreemphasis on the biological, with the socialaspect being implicit <strong>and</strong> the culturalabsent – hence the work <strong>to</strong> redress thisby the post-Piagetians (see above).ECOLOGICAL SYSTEMS THEORYBy 1979 Bronfenbrenner had recognisedthat the contexts of children’s livesimpacted upon their development <strong>and</strong>learning <strong>and</strong> that they in turn impactedon one another. He was critical of theorieswhich did not take in<strong>to</strong> account the ‘time<strong>and</strong> place’ elements which mean thatchildren experience different childhoodsin different communities <strong>and</strong> in differenthis<strong>to</strong>rical eras.While he insisted thatyoung children are actively involved intheir own development <strong>and</strong> learning, histheory pointed up the ways in which whathappens <strong>to</strong> children <strong>and</strong> the kinds ofsocieties in which they are brought upimpacts on that development <strong>and</strong>learning. For example, a child’s home <strong>and</strong>family would be affected by interactionswith a childcare setting, a religiouscommunity, the local authority or healthservice provision,<strong>and</strong> overall by the kindof society in which the child lived. In turn,that society or nation would be influencedby world events <strong>and</strong> thinking – a goodexample of world level influence might bethe UN Convention on the Rights of theChild (UN 1989),which has influencedpolicy in many countries <strong>and</strong> its effectshave filtered through <strong>to</strong> other levels insociety. What complicates this theory formany people is the fact that each of thelevels of his theoretical model would bedynamic <strong>and</strong> changing. It was not untilalmost twenty years after his initialpublication about the theory thatBronfenbrenner (Bronfenbrenner <strong>and</strong>Morris 1998) admitted that he also needed<strong>to</strong> take account of ‘within child’ differenceswhich, in their turn, would also influencethe child’s life course. In acknowledgingthat children actively influence their owndevelopment <strong>and</strong> learning,Bronfenbrenner focuses on the ways inwhich children interpret the contexts of33


their upbringing, rather than on objectivemeasures of those environments. It is,on Bronfenbrenner’s own admission, adifficult theory <strong>to</strong> corroborate by research,because of its complexity.The theory does,however, ‘make sense’ because it attempts<strong>to</strong> take account of all the aspects ofhuman life <strong>and</strong> interaction which impac<strong>to</strong>n children’s development.INFORMATION PROCESSING THEORIESA number of theorists have tried <strong>to</strong> equatehuman learning with the functioning ofa computer. Researchers in the field of AI(artificial intelligence) make comparisonsbetween human thinking <strong>and</strong> computerswhen attempting <strong>to</strong> produce computersimulations. Perhaps the main points inthis theory that might help us include:-children become capable of makingau<strong>to</strong>matic responses rather than consciousones after gaining experience (say, inwalking, once they no longer need <strong>to</strong>concentrate on managing their balance);they become more <strong>and</strong> more capable ofdoing more than one thing at once as theyget older; the amount children knowimplicitly, or ‘intuitively’ (tacit knowledge)increases with age, so they can draw onthis tacit knowledge <strong>to</strong> assist them insolving problems; the knowledge s<strong>to</strong>redin the long term memory is more complex<strong>and</strong> refined <strong>and</strong> children gain greatercontrol over their own ability <strong>to</strong>concentrate on something as they ge<strong>to</strong>lder. Naturally, it is important <strong>to</strong> take in<strong>to</strong>account that information processingtheories focus on thinking <strong>and</strong> ignore theinterplay of cognitive with physical,social<strong>and</strong> emotional development (see MurrayThomas 1996).PERCEPTION AND UNDERSTANDING – THE ROLEOF THE SENSESHumans, like other animals, perceivethrough their senses. We assume babies<strong>and</strong> young children can do this – but whatdo we know about children’s seeing,hearing, <strong>to</strong>uching, smelling <strong>and</strong> tasting?What are newborns able <strong>to</strong> see, hear, smell,taste <strong>and</strong> feel? Gibson, one of the maintheorists in the area of perceptionsuggests that perceptual developmentdoes not seem <strong>to</strong> occur in stages butchange in perceptual ability occurs ondifferent dimensions (Gibson 1969;<strong>and</strong> seeBremner 1998). Firstly, we now know thateven very young babies are purposeful intheir perceptual activity – they seem <strong>to</strong>use some inbuilt strategies, such as theability <strong>to</strong> perceive objective propertiesof space <strong>and</strong> objects; <strong>and</strong> that by <strong>three</strong>months of age they are quite aware of thephysical properties of the world aroundthem. Secondly, babies are very good atbecoming aware of what the appearanceof objects tells them the objects can do orcan be used for; babies gradually becomeable <strong>to</strong> focus on detail <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> make finerdiscriminations; <strong>and</strong> finally, young childrenbecome more able <strong>to</strong> concentrate onthose aspects of an event or situation that34 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


are important <strong>to</strong> them,blanking out anyperipheral noises, etc. The reason whypsychologists are especially interestedin early perception is that changes areso rapid at this age. In the subsequentchapters of this review research evidenceabout these abilities is highlighted.THEORIES ABOUT LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENTThe earliest theory about languagedevelopment assumed that childrenacquire language through imitation. Whileresearch has shown that children whoimitate the actions of those around themduring their first year of life are generallythose who also learn <strong>to</strong> talk more quickly,there is also evidence that imitation alonecannot explain how children becometalkers. For example, in the Englishl a n g u a g e, young children will say ‘Wegoed <strong>to</strong> the shops’ – they are very cleverlyinventing the past tense of ‘go’ based onthe rules they have absorbed.Skinner, the Behaviourist theorist (seeabove),suggested that children learnlanguage through reinforcement. In otherwords, when a parent or carer showsenthusiasm for something a child tries<strong>to</strong> say, this should encourage the child<strong>to</strong> repeat the utterance. But again, eventhough reinforcement may help, thistheory cannot account for children’sinventions of language.Some argue that it is not just hearinglanguage around them that is important,it is the kind of language – whether it isused responsively (for example, followinga baby’s input, such as the baby makinga noise or doing something). It is also clearthat babies need <strong>to</strong> hear language <strong>to</strong>develop this themselves. This point is ofgreat importance in relation <strong>to</strong> youngchildren with impoverished languageexperience (see for example Ward 2000).The idea of motherese (Snow <strong>and</strong> Ferguson1977; Trevarthen 1995) – accentuated,tuneful, speech <strong>to</strong> babies <strong>and</strong> repeatingtheir own language (often extended) back<strong>to</strong> young children – was posited as a basichuman requirement. However, otherresearch (see Bee 1989) indicates thatwhile motherese can be used <strong>to</strong> explainhow aspects of individual children’senvironments help or hinder them fromtalking, it does not explain the underlyingcauses of language acquisition. We can atleast suggest that talking in mothereseattracts <strong>and</strong> holds babies’ attention <strong>and</strong>that it allows the infants themselves <strong>to</strong>take part in enjoyable turn takingexchanges, the beginnings ofconversations (see also chapter 5).Chomsky (1965; 1975) proposed thatbabies are born with an inbuilt LanguageAcquisition Device (LAD). He suggested thatlanguage then simply emerges as the childmatures. Slobin (1973;1985) continuedthis line of thought, proposing that justas newborns come in<strong>to</strong> the world‘programmed’ <strong>to</strong> look at interesting,35


especially moving, objects, so babiesare pre-programmed <strong>to</strong> pay attention<strong>to</strong> language. One problem with thistheory is that children seem <strong>to</strong> havegreat proficiency in acquiring whateverlanguage/s they hear around them <strong>and</strong>during their first year of life they willgradually discard from their reper <strong>to</strong>ireof voca l i s ations sounds which they do nothear in the speech of those with whomthey spend their lives – but of course thepre-programming does not need <strong>to</strong> bethought of as tied <strong>to</strong> a specific language.Like Trevarthen <strong>and</strong> others, Chomskyindicates the centrality of interactionswith familiar adults <strong>and</strong> older childrenfrom the earliest days of life. Parents<strong>and</strong> practitioners need time <strong>to</strong> enjoy‘pro<strong>to</strong>-conversations’ <strong>and</strong> as we will seelater, research has shown,treating babiesas if they underst<strong>and</strong> talk <strong>and</strong> involvingthem in conversational exchangesare experiences on which later abilitiesare founded.Piaget argued that language is an exampleof symbolic behaviour, <strong>and</strong> no differentfrom other learning. One of his colleagues,Hermine Sinclair (1971),proposed that achild’s ability <strong>to</strong> nest a set of Russian dollsuses the same cognitive process as a childneeds for underst<strong>and</strong>ing how sentencesare embedded in one another. Nelson(1985) <strong>and</strong> others, using this cognitiveprocessing explanation,think languageis an extension of the child’s existingmeaning-making capacity. This seems<strong>to</strong> fit with the fact that children willgenerally begin <strong>to</strong> engage in pretend playat about the same time as their first wordsare expressed, indicating that they areusing symbols in the form of words <strong>and</strong>also symbolic pretend objects (forexample using a block as a pretend cake).Following on from Vygotsky’s sociallearning tradition, Bruner (1983) stressedthe importance of opportunities for babies<strong>and</strong> children <strong>to</strong> interact with <strong>and</strong> observeinteractions between others. As weexplained above, this idea is supportedby research showing that mothers whobehave as if their babies <strong>and</strong> youngchildren underst<strong>and</strong> language right fromthe start, make eye contact with them<strong>and</strong> engage in dialogue, responding <strong>to</strong>their babies’ reactions (kicking, wavingarms, smiling, etc) are laying thefoundations of conversation.Karmiloff <strong>and</strong> Karmiloff-Smith (2001)argue that none of these theories aboutlanguage is, on its own,adequate inexplaining language development <strong>and</strong>learning in the first <strong>three</strong> years of life,<strong>and</strong> that we need <strong>to</strong> take account of eachof them for their ability <strong>to</strong> explain par<strong>to</strong>f the s<strong>to</strong>ry.36 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


THEORIES ABOUT EARLY CHILDHOODEDUCATION AND CAREWhile our aim in this chapter has been <strong>to</strong>present outlines of the main influentialresearch <strong>and</strong> theories about children’sdevelopment which have been adoptedby those working in the field of ECECin Britain,practitioners might also like <strong>to</strong>refer <strong>to</strong> the theories <strong>and</strong> philosophies ofthe great pioneers of the ECEC movementin this country. These include RobertOwen, Johann Heinrich Pestalozzi,Friedrich Froebel, Maria Montessori, RudolfSteiner, Margaret McMillan <strong>and</strong> SusanIsaacs (see for example: Bruce 1987; Curtis1998; David 1990; Isaacs 1930, 1933). Morerecently, ideas from the famous nurseriesof Reggio Emilia in Northern Italy havebecome widely circulated in this country.Their philosophy is based on the notionof educating young children <strong>to</strong> beindependent thinkers who use ‘thehundred languages of children’, expressingconcepts <strong>and</strong> emotions especially throughthe arts with the support of each other<strong>and</strong> sensitive adults. The nurseries ofReggio were founded as an antidote <strong>to</strong>Fascism <strong>and</strong> abuse of power. (For moreinformation see Abbott <strong>and</strong> Nutbrown2001; Edwards et al 1998).RECOGNISING AND REFLECTING ON OUROWN THEORIESEach of us holds theories about earlyc h i l d h ood. Our theories have been builtup through our own ex pe ri e n ces asc h i l d re n , our ex pe ri e n ces with childre n ,the theories of those with whom we havehad co nt a ct, <strong>and</strong> the theories we haveread about in boo k s. The pra ctitionersin Reggio Emilia discuss their wo rk <strong>and</strong>ideas <strong>and</strong> read widely, s u p po rted bype d a g og i s t a s ( i n s e rv i ce tra i n e r s ). Th eyclaim that the theories they wo rk with aretheir ow n , s h a ped by the co n s t a ntchallenges of ex pe ri e n ce <strong>and</strong> re f l e ct i o n .SUMMARY OF KEY ‘MESSAGES’This chapter has presented informationabout:-some of the theories which have beenadopted in the field of ECEC in the UK<strong>and</strong> how they can help us reflect on ourown theories <strong>and</strong> upon observations ofchildren with whom we live <strong>and</strong> workhow cultural contexts influenceresearch <strong>and</strong> theorisingthe holistic nature of early learning <strong>and</strong>developmentthe impressive meaning-makingcapabilities of babies <strong>and</strong> youngchildrenthe active nature of their development<strong>and</strong> learning37


the importance of positive interactionswith adults (<strong>and</strong> older children)the role of modelling positive attitudes<strong>and</strong> behaviourthe importance of feedback <strong>and</strong>reinforcemente a rly childhood as an impo rt a nt phaseon which later emotional deve l o p m e nt,re l ationships <strong>and</strong> self-co n cepts are built.38 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Chapter 3A Strong ChildIn the Framework ‘<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong> Three Matters’, the section entitled ‘A Strong Child’ is concernedwith the ways in which children’s growing awareness of ‘self’ can be fostered in the yearsbetween birth <strong>and</strong> <strong>three</strong>, how they begin <strong>to</strong> show personal characteristics, preferences,capabilities <strong>and</strong> self-confidence, how they contribute <strong>to</strong> attachments <strong>and</strong> exploreemotional boundaries, <strong>and</strong> have a sense of belonging. In this chapter, relevant researchfindings are provided, <strong>and</strong> their implications for practice are pointed up. Links with theFramework are made through the inclusion of the ‘Development Matters’ statements fromthe cards of the pack in short summary sections within the chapter.Earlier in this review, in chapter 1, evidenceabout resilience <strong>and</strong> the ability <strong>to</strong> copewith challenging life situations wasintroduced. In this chapter we provide areview of the literature about babies <strong>and</strong>young children being <strong>and</strong> becomingcompetent <strong>and</strong> emotionally strong <strong>and</strong>how the adults <strong>and</strong> other children aroundthem contribute <strong>to</strong> this development<strong>and</strong> learning.It is important <strong>to</strong> note the longst<strong>and</strong>ingcontribution <strong>to</strong> this area of knowledge byAnn <strong>and</strong> Alan Clarke (1976; 2000). In theirrecent publication (Clarke <strong>and</strong> Clarke2000) they review hundreds of studiessupporting their view that we need <strong>to</strong> bewary of attributing a simplistic causalimportance <strong>to</strong> early experience <strong>and</strong> laterlife achievements.They conclude:-1. theories ascribing overwhelming,disproportionate <strong>and</strong> predeterministicimportances <strong>to</strong> the early years areclearly erroneous;2. the widespread belief in thedisproportionate effects of earlyexperiences is likely <strong>to</strong> lead <strong>to</strong>underestimation of what can be donefor deprived children,hence, on the oneh<strong>and</strong>, less than adequate interventions,or, on the others <strong>to</strong>tal inaction;3. there is no suggestion that whathappens in the early years isunimportant. For most children,however, the effects of such39


experiences represent no more thana first step in an ongoing life pathwhich may be straight or winding,incremental or decremental, dependingon the two-way relationship betweenindividuals <strong>and</strong> their contexts. Thereis little indication that any one poin<strong>to</strong>f development is more critical thananother; all are important. And in theongoing or reshaping of the person’slife path,it is <strong>to</strong> continuing influencesthat significance must be ascribed.(Clarke <strong>and</strong> Clarke 2000: 105).In other words, the Clarkes are arguingthat early life experience is important,but we should not assume that support<strong>and</strong> intervention in the earliest yearsguarantees later success, nor that earlydisadvantage cannot be overcome.Two other key figures who have devotedyears <strong>to</strong> research on early childhoodmaintain that the number of fac<strong>to</strong>rsinvolved make this a highly complexresearch area.Schaffer (1998) concludesthat subsequent influences can impact<strong>to</strong> reverse the ill-effects of poorexperiences in early childhood <strong>and</strong> Rutter(1999) that the effects of numerous,apparently linked, negative experiencesare usually indirect rather than directlylinked <strong>to</strong> babyhood <strong>and</strong> early childhood.He states ‘Life transitions have <strong>to</strong> beconsidered both as end products of pastprocesses <strong>and</strong> as instiga<strong>to</strong>rs of future ones’(Rutter 1989: 46). More recent work byKagan (1998) also argues that predictingthe life path is an uncertain business.However, Konner (1991), arguing thatit is not stress that kills us but adaptation<strong>to</strong> stress that permits us <strong>to</strong> live, becauselife is uncertain,states:-our responsibility <strong>to</strong> babies <strong>and</strong> childrenis clear: it is not <strong>to</strong> eliminate stress fromchildren’s lives completely, since that isbeyond our capacity; rather, it is <strong>to</strong> helpshape responses <strong>to</strong> stress that willsomehow permit them <strong>to</strong> live. (Konner1991: 225, our italics)More than this, however, we presumablywant our children <strong>to</strong> live happy <strong>and</strong>fulfilled lives, <strong>to</strong> meet challenges with zest<strong>and</strong> enthusiasm. Paradoxically, of thechildren tracked in<strong>to</strong> adulthood by Werner(1996),almost half the resilient ‘at risk’group declared themselves happy withtheir adult lives, compared with only tenper cent of the low risk comparison group.Konner goes on <strong>to</strong> discuss societies wherechildren have grown up through timesof enormous stress, yet have made happy<strong>and</strong> successful lives for themselves. So howdoes this strength <strong>and</strong> ability <strong>to</strong> face lifewith joy come about? The common str<strong>and</strong>found by the researchers seems <strong>to</strong> point<strong>to</strong> the need for each child <strong>to</strong> have, fromearly in their lives, at least one person withwhom they have a strong <strong>and</strong> meaningfulattachment relationship, that they ‘matter’<strong>and</strong> that what they do ‘<strong>matters</strong>’ <strong>to</strong>someone. A further important ingredientmay be having a strong sense of self, a selfwho achieves goals <strong>and</strong> of whom one canbe proud.40 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


A GROWING AWARENESS OF SELF AND THE ROLEOF ATTACHMENT‘The concepts “sense of self ” <strong>and</strong>“development of self” are abstractions.Whereas language or mo<strong>to</strong>r developmentcan be tracked definitively, the evolutionof the self is not so easily quantifiable.’(Davies 1999:197)Although researchers a quarter ofa century ago argued that children donot develop a sense of self (ie. recognisethemselves as separate people with anindividual identity) until their second yearof life, more recent research (Brether<strong>to</strong>net al 1981; Odofsky 1987) is clear that thisamazing feat begins soon after birth. It isin the everyday interactions of being caredfor that babies begin <strong>to</strong> become aware ofthemselves. As the Framework ‘<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong>Three Matters’ points out in theComponent Me, Myself <strong>and</strong> I, babies arebeing made aware of the human worldoutside themselves <strong>and</strong>, importantly,aware that they themselves exist, as aresult of being <strong>to</strong>uched, talked <strong>to</strong> <strong>and</strong>gazed at (Alvarez 1992).Even in the first month of life, babies aremaking distinctions between people,objects, self <strong>and</strong> other (Stern 1985; Rosser1994). For example, the parent or carer will,at the start, be the one mainly responsiblefor the way in which feeding proceeds.As the baby becomes more able <strong>to</strong>participate <strong>and</strong> take control <strong>to</strong> someextent, the way in which a sensitive adultallows for this will influence the baby’sfeelings of efficacy.We say the adult isbehaving contingently, being responsive<strong>to</strong> the baby’s signals <strong>and</strong> so, while themain focus may be the feeding, babiestreated in this way are learning that theycan influence their own lives.A number of studies have linked thequality of a child’s sense of self <strong>to</strong> thequality of attachments. It would beimpossible for a child <strong>to</strong> develop a senseof self without feedback <strong>and</strong> recognitionfrom those with whom they spend theirlives, so they are dependent on thosepeople for a positive view of themselves.‘While infants <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ddlers are powerfullyself-motivated <strong>to</strong> learn with their wholebody <strong>and</strong> all their senses <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>communicate what they know, theydepend on the affirmation <strong>and</strong> warmth oftrusting relationships <strong>to</strong> be able <strong>to</strong> do so.’(Post <strong>and</strong> Hohmann 2000:31).Attachment theory was formulated byBowlby (1951; 1953; 1969; 1973; 1980) <strong>and</strong>supported through later development byhis colleagues (see for example Robertson<strong>and</strong> Robertson 1989). Although the waysin which attachment behaviour isexpressed is different in different cultures,it is thought by some researchers <strong>to</strong> beuniversal (LeVine <strong>and</strong> Miller 1990). Bow l byt h o u g ht of at t a c h m e nt as a dev i ce which isi ntended <strong>to</strong> pro te ct the immat u re offs p ri n gof a species <strong>to</strong> at t ra ct adults who wille n s u re their surv i va l .He theorised that earlyat t a c h m e nt be h aviours are innate <strong>and</strong> thatwhile a new bo rn can be co m fo rted bya nyo n e, babies ve ry soon diffe re nt i atebe tween their pri m a ry at t a c h m e nt figure<strong>and</strong> other pe o p l e. At t a c h m e nt is seen asa dev i ce which provides the baby with41


a sense of securi ty <strong>and</strong> pro m o te sco m m u n i cation <strong>and</strong> the ex p ression offe e l i n g s. The at t a c h m e nt re l ationship alsoa cts as a secure base for ‘ex p l o ring thewo rl d’ <strong>and</strong> the pri m a ry re l ationship/sa secure base for learning self re g u l at i o n ,or self co nt rol (at this stage, the sensitivere g u l ation of arousal <strong>and</strong> distre s s.)Mary Ainsworth (1967), used Bowlby’sattachment theory <strong>to</strong> study babies inAfrica <strong>and</strong> the USA. She found that in bothcultures children showed attachmentpatterns but that there were somedifferences. She devised an experimentknown as the ‘Strange Situation’, where astranger <strong>to</strong> the child would enter the roomsometimes when mother <strong>and</strong> baby were<strong>to</strong>gether, at other times when the motherhad briefly left the room. Ainsworthargued that the baby’s response on themother’s return was a measure of thequality of their attachment. The qualityof this first attachment was said <strong>to</strong> setthe scene for all later relationships by‘modelling’ for the baby what allrelationships will be like. Ainsworth’sStrange Situation research has beencriticised, (see for example Clarke <strong>and</strong>Clarke 2000; Kagan 1998; Harris 1989;Singer 1993), because children in differentcountries, cultures <strong>and</strong> subcultures displaydifferent approaches <strong>to</strong> attachment,different rates of ‘ambivalent attachment ‘(apparent rejection of the mother,resistance <strong>to</strong> her attempts <strong>to</strong> pick themup following the strange situation) <strong>and</strong>because the research approach itselfis thought unnatural <strong>and</strong> therefore prone<strong>to</strong> ‘error ‘. For example, apparent rejectingbehaviour at around a year of age maybe quite common in some communities<strong>and</strong> need not on its own indicatepoor attachment.While some recent research continues<strong>to</strong> focus on mother-infant attachment,disruptions <strong>to</strong> this process <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>emotional development linked <strong>to</strong>problems such as postnatal depression(Cooper <strong>and</strong> Murray 1998; Murray <strong>and</strong>Cooper 1997; Kumar 1997; Teti <strong>and</strong> Gelf<strong>and</strong>1991),other recent research suggests that‘the family’ – all the familiar relationshipsa baby has – provides the network for theirattachment relationships (Forrest 1997).This research indicates that betweenabout <strong>three</strong> <strong>and</strong> six months of age, babieswill show signs that they are makingpreferential attachments <strong>to</strong> those whoare their primary carers. Parents <strong>and</strong>experienced practitioners note that thisbehaviour is often directed at olderchildren,who seem <strong>to</strong> fascinate babies,<strong>and</strong> not simply at adults. They will:-increase social smilingshow a strong interest in face-<strong>to</strong>-face<strong>and</strong> eye contactincrease their attempts <strong>to</strong> attract theattention of these primary carers bys m i l i n g, coo i n g, b a b b l i n g, <strong>and</strong> move m e ntbe pacified by a carer’s voice,presentation of a <strong>to</strong>y, or a lookrespond differently <strong>to</strong> different carersshow they have different expectationsof different people42 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


( a d a p ted from Davies 1999 : 129 ).Th ey mayalso show a more dra m atic re a ct i o n ,suchas passionate cry i n g, when their pre fe rre dp ri m a ry ca rer picks them up (Bra ze l <strong>to</strong> n1992 ).This can be quite disco n ce rting <strong>to</strong>n ew pare nt s, s ay, co l l e cting their childre nf rom childminders or nurseri e s, s i n ce pare nt sm ay inte rp ret the crying as meaning quitethe oppo s i te – that their babies are re j e ct i n gt h e m .In fact Wat s o n’s (1994) re s e a rc hs h owed that <strong>to</strong>ddlers made more emotionald i s p l ays (cry i n g, e tc) when co l l e cted by theirmothers from a dayca re setting than whenbeing dro p ped off or at other tra n s i t i o n a lm o m e nts in their day. Watson argues thatthis finding is linked <strong>to</strong> the strength of them o t h e r- i n f a nt at t a c h m e nt <strong>and</strong> that there s e a rch also suggests that neither divo rcenor non-mate rnal dayca re need impactn e g at i ve ly on the mother-child at t a c h m e nt.Pra ctitioners need <strong>to</strong> re a s s u re pare nts thatthese displays, while initially upsetting, ca nbe the way in which babies tell their pri m a ryat t a c h m e nt figure s, the most passionate lyl oved significa nt people in their live s, t h att h ey are pleased <strong>to</strong> be with them.A number of re s e a rch studies haveex p l o red issues re l ated <strong>to</strong> the age at whichbabies <strong>and</strong> children begin at tendinga childminder’s or a group setting. In theU S A , Howes (1991) studied 96 children(of whom 47 we re girl s ), <strong>to</strong> ex p l o re thepossible influences of the age at whichthe children began at a day ca re setting,<strong>to</strong>gether with strength of mate rn a lat t a c h m e nt <strong>and</strong> their inte ra ctions withpe e r s. She found that early childca ree n ro l m e nt (as babies/be fo re <strong>three</strong> ye a r s ),when accompanied by secure at t a c h m e nt<strong>to</strong> the early years pra ctitioner re s u l tedin the children being more soc i a l lyco m pe te nt at age four ye a r s. Other re s e a rc hin Swe d e n , Fra n ce <strong>and</strong> the UK (Me l h u i s h<strong>and</strong> Moss 1991 ; Ja rousse et al 1991 )s u p po rts this finding of early ECECex pe ri e n ce <strong>and</strong> later social co m pe te n ce.Ag a i n ,be cause leaving babies <strong>and</strong> yo u n gc h i l d ren still induces feelings of guilt, yo u n gp a re nts pro b a b ly need re a s s u ra n ce byp ra ctitioners that they may be adva nt a g i n gtheir children by enrolling them in ECEC,rather than the reve r s e, a lways with thep roviso that at te ntion is paid <strong>to</strong> thed eve l o p m e nt of wa rm at t a c h m e nts withi n d i v i d u a l ,d e s i g n ated key personnel (El fe ret al 2002). Hennessy <strong>and</strong> Mehuish (1991)co n d u cted a meta-analysis of findings fro m12 longitudinal studies of early day ca reat te n d a n ce <strong>and</strong> their effe cts on the childre nat school age. While they found a numbe rof problems with co m p a ring the re s e a rc h<strong>and</strong> at times with the fact that po te nt i a l lyuseful re s e a rch data had not be e ng at h e re d, t h ey argue that where the dayca re meets the needs of the child be t te rthan they could be met at home, t h ec h i l d ren be n e f i t. Fu rt h e r, t h ey argue thatall children can benefit from ECEC prov i d e dit meets cri te ria co n ce rned with fo s te ri n goptimal social <strong>and</strong> cog n i t i ve deve l o p m e ntin childre n .In chapter 2,<strong>and</strong> later in this text, we showbabies are intensely interested in otherpeople from the moment of birth <strong>and</strong>, inthe first few months of life, they are no<strong>to</strong>nly trying <strong>to</strong> form the close relationships43


on which they will found theirunderst<strong>and</strong>ings of human interactions,they are also beginning <strong>to</strong> develop anindividual sense of self <strong>and</strong> coming <strong>to</strong>know if that individual ‘self’ has anyagency, or power, over her/his own life.Braungart-Rieker et al (2001) suggest thatbabies’ attachments at age four monthsare good predic<strong>to</strong>rs of their affectregulation <strong>and</strong> attachments at a year old.This view is further emphasised by Davies(1999: 147) who concludes:-‘In social interactions, the infant whosebehaviour succeeds in eliciting a positive<strong>and</strong> sensitive response from the parentfeels encouraged <strong>to</strong> continue the behavior.During the first year, interaction patterns,coping experiences, <strong>and</strong> resulting viewsof self are the building blocks of the child’sworking models of self <strong>and</strong> relationships.’The message for the ECEC field seems <strong>to</strong>be that babies need <strong>to</strong> form attachments<strong>to</strong> significant people, usually a parent orother relative in the first instance, <strong>and</strong> thatthese attachments provide them with a‘model’ of attachment so they go on <strong>to</strong>relate positively <strong>to</strong> other people, such asECEC practitioners. Further, children whohave had early positive experiences ofECEC benefit from their interactions withkey adults <strong>and</strong> with other children –enrolment in ECEC can be valuable.SPECIAL CHILDREN AND ATTACHMENTSParents who are informed (often, researchhas shown,insensitively) that their babyhas been born with identifiable specialneeds are often left <strong>to</strong> deal with powerfulemotions which may colour theattachment process <strong>and</strong> impact negativelyon the family (Herbert 1994; Herbert <strong>and</strong>Carpenter 1994). According <strong>to</strong> Doyle(1999),the UK still lags behind othercountries in its ability <strong>to</strong> ensure that allthe professionals involved work <strong>to</strong>gethereffectively. She argues that while therhe<strong>to</strong>ric is present in UK legislation,research,training <strong>and</strong> resourceimplications have been neglected.As we have already indicated in thisreview, positive thinking on the par<strong>to</strong>f those who care for, <strong>and</strong> work with, veryyoung children as they develop a senseof self is crucial,especially when a childhas been recognised early as havinga disability. As Marchant (2001) stresses,such children <strong>and</strong> their families are livingin a society which assumes a very negativestance <strong>to</strong>wards disability. Wilson’s (1998)work highlights the importance ofrecognising the disruptions <strong>to</strong> theattachment process which can ensuewhen a baby is born prematurely, illor with a disability. She states that ifattachment <strong>to</strong> one or two key peoplein the earliest weeks of life is crucial, thenthe situation is potentially calami<strong>to</strong>us,because of rejection,anxiety or an inability<strong>to</strong> h<strong>and</strong>le a fragile baby physically.Menzies Lyth (1995) discusses the waysin which staff in institutions (such asnurseries, children’s homes, hospitals),need <strong>to</strong> be aware that the possible lackof continuity in relationships with thenumerous professionals involved canimpact on children’s attachments, leaving44 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


them with an inadequate modelof rewarding emotional <strong>and</strong> socialrelationships, or self assurance.Difficulties in forming attachments can beparticularly crucial for very young childrenwith special emotional <strong>and</strong> social needs,when they attend a number of differentsettings in a week,designed ‘<strong>to</strong> meet theirneeds’ but each with different personnel,routines <strong>and</strong> regulations. This is notacceptable for children without difficultiesbut is common practice nationally forthose who are the most vulnerable.Continuity of care, <strong>and</strong> support <strong>to</strong> parents,is fragmented through this process.Although Bowlby’s early pronouncementshave been revisited <strong>and</strong> mellowed,attention <strong>to</strong> the processes of attachment<strong>and</strong> acceptance for children with specialneeds remains crucial. In addition <strong>to</strong> this,children with autism do not naturally seekattention <strong>and</strong> attachments, but appearaloof <strong>and</strong> indifferent. They do not appear<strong>to</strong> wish for meaningful interactions withadults or other children,as they do notperceive their world in the same way asthose other children <strong>and</strong> adults do(Trevarthen et al 1998).There are also significant differencesbetween maternal <strong>and</strong> paternalattachments relating <strong>to</strong> children withspecial needs (Carpenter 1997).Carpenter(1997:27) identified the marginalisationof fathers when ‘all help was focused onthe mother <strong>and</strong> baby’. Clearly this hasimplications for mothers, fathers <strong>and</strong>the children.In addition <strong>to</strong> recognising the needs ofbabies <strong>and</strong> parents who require particularsupport because their children have beenborn with a recognised disability, otherspecial support can be needed when thecontext indicates potential disadvantage.Studying teenage mothers <strong>and</strong> theirnurturance of their babies, Oyserman et al(1994) found that gr<strong>and</strong>parents’ support<strong>and</strong> in particular the gr<strong>and</strong>father’snurturance <strong>to</strong>wards the baby, was linked<strong>to</strong> increased baby nurturance by theyoung mother. Other recent researchstudies on attachment (Fonagy et al 1991;Fonagy et al 1994; Steele et al 1995) whichseem linked <strong>to</strong> this issue have shown thereare strong correlations between parents’perceptions of their own earlyattachments <strong>and</strong> those with their babies,<strong>and</strong> Plomin <strong>and</strong> Bergeman (1991) haveproposed that warmth <strong>and</strong> supportivenessin parents are related <strong>to</strong> genetic fac<strong>to</strong>rs.However, Siegel (1999) claims that parents’narratives of their own attachments <strong>to</strong>their parents are the best indica<strong>to</strong>rs of thelikely warmth <strong>and</strong> responsiveness of theirown attachments <strong>to</strong> newborns. He arguesthat this should be explored duringpregnancy <strong>and</strong> that those whosenarratives indicate difficult earlyattachments could be helped <strong>to</strong> overcomethese problems in order <strong>to</strong> ensure positiveattachments for their babies.Earlier researchers (Grossman et al 1980)had found that high levels of stress inpregnant mothers is linked <strong>to</strong> high levelsof foetal activity <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> higher levels ofirritability in babies after birth. Although45


having a difficult temperament inbabyhood, which might also be involved,has not been found <strong>to</strong> persist through theyears of childhood, it can persist if parents<strong>and</strong> carers respond aggressively orinsensitively or are unable <strong>to</strong> induce calm.This then indicates that unless themother’s anxiety can be reduced, it is likelyirritable or difficult babies will experiencecontinuity of negative experiences whichcould reduce their chances of becomingresilient. Similarly, Hagekull et al (1993)tracked 110 ten month old infants <strong>and</strong>their mothers by videoing them in auniversity labora<strong>to</strong>ry as part of a largerlongitudinal project. Two observations ofa ‘strange situation’ were filmed, one withmothers present, one with them absent.Both the mothers <strong>and</strong> babies had heartratemoni<strong>to</strong>rs attached. Data on maternalsensitivity at four months had beencollected by observation prior <strong>to</strong> thisexperiment. It was found that infants whodid not reference their mothers in the‘strange situation’ had experienced lesssensitivity (eg physical contact,intrusiveness, response <strong>to</strong> distress,effective comforting) <strong>and</strong> had been moreirritable at four months. Social referencingis seen as either a way <strong>to</strong> communicateemotion, or as a mechanism of socialinfluence about how <strong>to</strong> act. Where nomaternal guidance was given, infantstended <strong>to</strong> be less positive <strong>to</strong>wards the‘stranger’. Thus they speculate thatmaternal sensitivity may depend onirritability of child <strong>and</strong> that lack ofmaternal sensitivity would in turn lead <strong>to</strong>lack of infant social referencing. Moseset al (2001) also studied social referencingin 12 <strong>and</strong> 18 month old infants. Theydiscovered that such young children relyon the responses of their familiar adults <strong>to</strong>determine whether an emotional responseshould be linked <strong>to</strong> what they see.Roberts (2002) reminds us that a baby’sfirst language is body language, usingwhat they see, feel, taste <strong>and</strong> smell asmessages <strong>to</strong> them about themselves <strong>and</strong>their world. Similarly Mollie Davies (2002in press) explains the way in whichmovement <strong>and</strong> dance contribute <strong>to</strong> a veryyoung child’s self esteem. It is importantfor spoken language <strong>and</strong> body language<strong>to</strong> match for babies <strong>and</strong> young children<strong>to</strong> feel truly accepted <strong>and</strong> through this<strong>to</strong> gradually accept themselves, <strong>to</strong> learn<strong>to</strong> be the sort of people their mothersrecognise. As we point out in chapter 6,children who have experienced abuse orneglect during babyhood try <strong>to</strong> avoidfurther pain by shutting down theiremotions <strong>and</strong> this can have a negativeeffect on their physical growth as well ason their emotional health. It also meansthey try <strong>to</strong> make themselves acceptableby negating feelings of pain,anger <strong>and</strong>fear. Being accepted, even when one isangry or distressed,‘is important for all babies <strong>and</strong> youngchildren <strong>and</strong> the bedrock of confidencethat can develop as a result is crucial forthose children who have a growingawareness that they are different fromothers. This may be because of animpairment, or because they happen <strong>to</strong> bein a minori ty in some way.’ ( Ro be rts 2002: 6 ).46 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


However, even sensitive parents <strong>and</strong> carerscannot be responsive <strong>to</strong> every cue fromtheir babies but those who experiencesuccess when they try <strong>to</strong> re-establishcontact with a preoccupied key adult aresaid <strong>to</strong> gain a stronger sense ofself-efficacy <strong>and</strong> as a result feel more <strong>and</strong>more successful. Failure <strong>to</strong> support ababy’s efforts <strong>to</strong> interact or <strong>to</strong> succeedin explorations may result in thedevelopment of ‘learned helplessness’(Aber <strong>and</strong> Allen 1987),whereas parents<strong>and</strong> carers who are emotionally sensitive<strong>and</strong> who encourage‘explora<strong>to</strong>ry behaviours have infants whodemonstrate greater task persistence,greater pleasure in goal-directedbehaviour, <strong>and</strong> greater subsequent social<strong>and</strong> cognitive competence’ (Lyons-Ruth<strong>and</strong> Zeanah 1993: 27).attached <strong>to</strong> different emotions <strong>and</strong> refinedthrough dominant social constructions.By the time they are about two years old,children are learning the ‘scripts’ that helpthem <strong>to</strong> ex pe ri e n ce emotion appro p ri ate ly.Davies (1999) suggests that havingexperienced sensitive <strong>and</strong> supportiveacceptance, they also become able <strong>to</strong> copewith short periods of discomfort by using‘transitional objects’ (in Western culturesthese would be objects such as dummies,fabric comforters, a favourite soft <strong>to</strong>y)<strong>to</strong> regulate their own levels of distress.Practitioners need therefore <strong>to</strong> haveparents relate <strong>to</strong> them well <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> helpthem underst<strong>and</strong> their child’s <strong>and</strong> theirfamily’s particular patterns of behaviour<strong>and</strong> cus<strong>to</strong>ms, which belong <strong>to</strong> the family’sindividual or community culture.WHAT CAN WE LEARN FROM ATTACHMENTTHE IMPORTANCE OF FAMILIARITY ANDKNOWLEDGE OF CULTURAL PRACTICESKamel <strong>and</strong> Dockrell (2000) found thatmothers’ interpretation of their babies’facial expressions as indicative of differentemotional states varied according <strong>to</strong> thesituational context, whereas those ofobservers who were not familiar with thebabies did not, indicating how intimacypromotes shared underst<strong>and</strong>ings. Harris(1989) claims that babies are born with thecapacity <strong>to</strong> experience the basic emotionsof sadness, anger <strong>and</strong> joy <strong>and</strong> that theygradually learn that other people alsoexperience these emotions. Throughgrowing up in a particular culture, childrenacquire the social practices that have beenRESEARCH?In research involving slightly olderchildren, Calkins et al (1998) found thattwo year olds were highly influenced bytheir mothers’ strategies for behaviourmanagement in the areas of emotions,behaviour, <strong>and</strong> physiological regulation.This research team suggests that it isimportant <strong>to</strong> identify the origins ofparticular maternal strategies. Further,since a quarter of the participants in thestudies by the Fonagy <strong>and</strong> Steele teams(see earlier section on special children <strong>and</strong>attachments) did not follow the suggestedintergenerational pattern,it is important<strong>to</strong> recognise that while intergenerationalattachment problems may have been in47


the majority among their participants,other variables must have intervenedfor 25 per cent of those involved whoovercame this problem. Even Bowlby(1988) himself agreed that attachmentresearch had shown up flaws in histheory. He suggested that a ‘theory ofdevelopmental pathways should replacetheories that involve specific phases ofdevelopment’ Bowlby (1988: 2). Further,Belsky et al (1996) argue that data fromdifferent attachment studies show thatthe ratings of attachment are not stable,they change over time <strong>and</strong> are contextdependent, <strong>and</strong> that the small samplesizes used in some of the attachmentresearch studies are cause for caution.Attachments may also be influenced bya family’s situation at a particular time.Dunn (1993) would say we should notbe surprised by this, she noted that in herresearch even mothers changed <strong>to</strong>wardstheir children at different ages,‘Some mothers seemed <strong>to</strong> be part i c u l a rly“t u rned on” by their children as one-ye a r-olds <strong>and</strong> less affe ct i o n ate, re l at i ve <strong>to</strong> otherm o t h e r s, when they be ca m ea rg u m e nt at i ve two- or thre e - ye a r- o l d s,while other mothers delighted espe c i a l ly intheir children as they be came talkat i ve <strong>and</strong>engaging two ye a r- o l d s.’ ( Dunn 19 93 : 28 ).Howeve r, d e s p i te the ex i s te n ce of cri t i ca lchallenges <strong>to</strong> some of the at t a c h m e ntre s e a rc h , the main messages we cant a ke from this wo rk are summed upin the deve l o p m e ntal guidelines prov i d e din the Fra m ewo rk ‘Bi rth <strong>to</strong> Th ree Ma t te r s’,which state:-Young babies become aware ofthemselves as separate from others,learning also that they have influenceupon <strong>and</strong> are influenced by others.Babies develop an underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong>awareness of themselves, which isinfluenced by their family, other people<strong>and</strong> the environment.Young children learn they havesimilarities <strong>and</strong> differences that connectthem <strong>to</strong> – but distinguish them from –others.Children show their particularcharacteristics, preferences <strong>and</strong>interests <strong>and</strong> demonstrate these in allthey do.Young babies seek <strong>to</strong> be looked at,approved of <strong>and</strong> find comfort in thehuman face.Babies gain attention: positively ornegatively.Young children strive for responsesfrom others, which confirm, contribute<strong>to</strong>, or challenge their underst<strong>and</strong>ingof themselves.Children need <strong>to</strong> feel others are positive<strong>to</strong>wards them, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> experiencerealistic expectations in order <strong>to</strong>become competent, assertive <strong>and</strong>self-assured.(<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong> Three Matters – DevelopmentMatters: Me Myself & I <strong>and</strong> BeingAcknowledged & Affirmed)Further messages from the researchinclude the following:-48 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


It is preferable <strong>to</strong> have stabilityin relationships with children<strong>and</strong> practitioners.A key worker system,with a smallnumber of individually designatedpractitioners relating <strong>to</strong> particularchildren,as advised by Elfer et al (2002)is important.As Selleck <strong>and</strong> Griffin (1996:156) point ou<strong>to</strong>n the basis of reviewing children’supbringing in other cultures,‘In Italy, key relationships with a significantadult are not seen as necessary <strong>to</strong>children’s successful development ingroup day care. Group settings may beseen as attempts <strong>to</strong> counterbalance theclinging, suffocating closeness of mother<strong>and</strong> child. … Children are encouraged <strong>to</strong>respond <strong>to</strong> the environment <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> smallgroups of adults <strong>and</strong> children rather thana key adult.‘So although the above description of theItalian model appears <strong>to</strong> imply the lack ofa key worker system,it still reinforces theidea of a small,significant number of bothadults <strong>and</strong> children being <strong>to</strong>gether so thatmeaningful relationships can be formed.What is warned against is the overlynarrow <strong>and</strong> limiting relationship that canoccur if a child is not given opportunities<strong>to</strong> be with more than one person, whetherthat is a parent or a practitioner. Selleck<strong>and</strong> Griffin (1996:156) add that‘a day educa<strong>to</strong>r must also develop a strong<strong>and</strong> complementary attachment <strong>to</strong> theinfants <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ddlers in her care. She willnot be a substitute for mothers or fathersor grannies, but she must be able <strong>to</strong> forma special relationship which can nourish<strong>and</strong> protect, <strong>and</strong> is available on a regular<strong>and</strong> predictable basis during the day.’So, in addition <strong>to</strong> advocatingcomplementary attachments withpractitioners, which ensure responsive<strong>and</strong> loving attention (Goldschmeid <strong>and</strong>Jackson 1994), Selleck <strong>and</strong> Griffin (1996)agree with Roberts (2002) that effectivepractice between birth <strong>and</strong> <strong>three</strong> willprovide individual children withopportunities <strong>to</strong> develop a positiveself-concept, interdependent relationships<strong>and</strong> a personal identity. In order <strong>to</strong> do thisit is suggested that:-‘the sort of acceptance that babies <strong>and</strong>young children need from parents <strong>and</strong>other important people is not acceptancethat is dependent on their behaviour; it isacceptance without reservations <strong>and</strong>without judgements. It can be describedas ‘unconditional positive regard’…Babieslearn that they are acceptable byexperiencing, day by day, the results ofthat acceptance…when an ‘importantperson’ smiles at the baby, <strong>and</strong> when thatperson comes at the baby’s call, the sensethat he or she is acceptable is confirmed.This is not simply a passive process; all thetime the baby is learning by experiencehow <strong>to</strong> win the smiles, how <strong>to</strong> bring theperson. Every experience is a learningexperience.’ (Roberts 2002:5-6).DEVELOPING A SENSE OF SELFEarly in the second year when <strong>to</strong>ddlershide, they will often leave themselvesvisible <strong>to</strong> their adult ‘seekers’, thinking that49


ecause they cannot see the adult, theadult cannot see them. While they willrevel in the joyfulness of the adult’s‘seeking’, which is the kind of activitywhich promotes a sense of a self whois wanted <strong>and</strong> loved, they are not yet able<strong>to</strong> recognise the other’s viewpoint or theway parts of them remain unhidden. Buthuge changes are afoot, building on theexperiences of that first year, as theyprogress in<strong>to</strong> new phases in theirdevelopment – <strong>to</strong> become st<strong>and</strong>ers <strong>and</strong>explorers; movers, shakers <strong>and</strong> players;walkers, talkers <strong>and</strong> pretenders (see theFramework <strong>and</strong> chapter 1 of this text) –stimulated <strong>and</strong> rewarded by their owngrowing abilities.Ju dy Dunn (1993) po i nts out how one of themost stri king changes during the tra n s i t i o nf rom baby h ood <strong>to</strong> early childhood is ac h i l d’s growing sense of self. In d i v i d u a lc h i l d ren are be coming awa re of how othersv i ew them <strong>and</strong> Dunn (19 93 :30) suggests‘The deve l o p m e nt of this self-co n s c i o u s n e s sa l l ows new po s s i b i l i t i e s ;for ex a m p l e, t h ep a re nt-child re l ationship can be a re s o u rcefor fo s te ring a sense of self-co m pe te n ce <strong>and</strong>s e l f - wo rth – or its oppo s i te.’At the same time this growing sense of selfis fostering a sense of independence(Karmiloff-Smith 1994) <strong>and</strong> being seen asa capable person by others (being ‘<strong>to</strong>ld’ inwords <strong>and</strong> actions that one is a ‘capableself’ when attempting <strong>to</strong> be independent)promotes self-esteem. Roberts (2002)provides many pointers <strong>to</strong> raisingchildren’s self-esteem,especially insituations where they are often frustratedat being unable <strong>to</strong> achieve tasks they setthemselves. These mainly involve thechild’s familiar adults in being able <strong>to</strong>‘decentre’ (take the child’s point of view)<strong>and</strong> create a climate in which they cansucceed or at least express theirfrustrations <strong>and</strong> be unders<strong>to</strong>od. Childrenwith warm, affectionate relationships withtheir parents are more likely <strong>to</strong> have highself-esteem,according <strong>to</strong> Mortimer (2001),who adds that they are also more likely<strong>to</strong> be positive about others, <strong>to</strong> be bettersocially adjusted <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> achieveacademically. Clearly such findings haveimplications for children with emotional,social <strong>and</strong> behavioural difficulties, whowill often demonstrate low self-esteem(Wall forthcoming).At around 18 <strong>to</strong> 24 months of age, youngchildren begin <strong>to</strong> be able <strong>to</strong> recognisethemselves in mirrors <strong>and</strong> it is also aroundthis time that they begin <strong>to</strong> assert theirown wishes (see also chapter 5). Prior <strong>to</strong>this, at about 12 months of age, they willalso have begun <strong>to</strong> point <strong>to</strong> things <strong>and</strong><strong>to</strong> be able <strong>to</strong> follow someone else’s gazewhen they are pointing. This pointingactivity also involves referencing bylooking back at the person’s face <strong>to</strong> checkif they are looking at the same object <strong>and</strong>this tells us that the young child has someunderst<strong>and</strong>ing about other people’sviewpoints (Gopnik et al 1999). Even veryyoung babies (less than six months old)have been recorded using socialreferencing strategies, searching theirparents’ faces for reassurance when50 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


something surprising <strong>and</strong> strangehappens (Channel 4 Childhood 1992 –Konner 1991;see also Brewer 2001).However, as Gopnik et al (1999) point out,one of the main tasks of very earlychildhood lies in underst<strong>and</strong>ing thedifference between their own minds <strong>and</strong>those of others <strong>and</strong> because parents <strong>and</strong>carers will often try <strong>to</strong> minimise thisdifference, ‘scaffolding’ children’s earlylearning <strong>and</strong> looking for commonality,it is in interactions with other children,often older siblings (brothers <strong>and</strong> sisters),that such underst<strong>and</strong>ings about self <strong>and</strong>others develop. In fact, Selwyn (2000)points out that families where the arrivalof a new baby (or babies) goes mostsmoothly, appear <strong>to</strong> be those in whichan older sibling, still aged under <strong>three</strong>,is included when the needs of the babyare discussed, <strong>and</strong> when the parents helptheir older children underst<strong>and</strong> feelings<strong>and</strong> needs of others by explaining the newbaby’s needs. Once children talk they alsoshow their underst<strong>and</strong>ing of other minds(Dunn 1999). For example, children under<strong>three</strong> have been observed using speechin quite different ways when speaking <strong>to</strong>younger children or babies, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> adults(Karmiloff-Smith 1994). But it seems thatit is in the earlier years that thefoundations of empathy <strong>and</strong> mindreadingare laid, as Selwyn indicates, ‘childrenunder two seem <strong>to</strong> be especially sensitive<strong>to</strong> how people are talking, <strong>and</strong> familieswhere feelings <strong>and</strong> needs are recognisedare more likely <strong>to</strong> promote pro-socialbehaviour’ (Selwyn 2000:38).Later interactions between siblings arealso important. Siblings may behavedifferently <strong>to</strong> one another in families <strong>and</strong>societies with different cultural traditions<strong>and</strong> the challenge may happen in differentways, but many parents <strong>and</strong> practitionerswill have witnessed a four-year-olds’superior <strong>and</strong> withering demonstrationsof know-how <strong>to</strong> an ‘ignorant’ two-year-old.However, such encounters can actuallyhelp the two-year-old recognise that otherminds are different from their own.Interestingly this finding does not accordwith Vygotskian theory of scaffolding,since the older child is not makingallowances <strong>and</strong> providing support for theyounger child’s learning, however, it doesaccord with Bronfenbrenner’s view thateach child needs ‘a zany uncle’ (a personwho behaves in unexpected <strong>and</strong> thereforehumorous ways, thus challengingthinking). This realisation of ‘other minds’ –called ‘mindreading’ by Judy Dunn (1999)is part of the growing underst<strong>and</strong>ing ofbeing an individual. Using others’ facialexpressions as indica<strong>to</strong>rs of what is goingon in their minds is part of this key taskof mindreading. But recognition of otherminds is extremely difficult for childrenwith autism,<strong>and</strong> children with languagecomprehension difficulties will struggle <strong>to</strong>interpret literal meanings, let alone extenddevelopment <strong>to</strong> consider another’s poin<strong>to</strong>f view. Pollack et al (2000) found thatrecognition of emotion in faces variedamong children who had been abused,with neglected children finding particulardifficulty in differentiating between facial51


expressions of emotion. So children’sinterpretations of emotional states <strong>and</strong>what is in the minds of others can beimpaired for a number of reasons.A further expression of this new-foundidentity around this time is the ability<strong>to</strong> use one’s own name. Additionally mostchildren will begin <strong>to</strong> use ‘I’, ‘me’ <strong>and</strong> ‘mine’during their second year. Between two <strong>and</strong><strong>three</strong> years of age they will also begin <strong>to</strong>develop a gender identity <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> showawareness of any effects of racism in theirsociety. Siraj-Blatchford (2001) points outthat there is only an emerging literatureon racial identity, culture <strong>and</strong> agency <strong>and</strong>even that relates mainly <strong>to</strong> older childrenthan we are considering here. This analysisis suppo rted by Goin (1998).Si ra j - Bl atc h fo rdargues that the best approach for ECECstaff is <strong>to</strong> work with all children makingthem aware they all have an ethnic/racial,gendered, cultural,diverse <strong>and</strong> linguistici d e nt i ty, be cause they would then be be t te requipped <strong>to</strong> accept that others are thesame, working in a way which is intended<strong>to</strong> break down stereotypes. Siraj-Blatchfordgoes on <strong>to</strong> discuss strategies for dealingwith racism or sexism <strong>and</strong> she states:-‘A positive self-concept is necessary forhealthy development <strong>and</strong> learning <strong>and</strong>includes feelings about gender, race,ability, culture <strong>and</strong> language. Positiveself-esteem depends on whether childrenfeel others accept them <strong>and</strong> see them ascompetent <strong>and</strong> worthwhile.’Siraj-Blatchford (2001: 104).Some early research suggested that thefact that young black children choosewhite dolls indicates low self-esteeminduced by racism,however, there is nowdisagreement in the USA as <strong>to</strong> whetherexperiments in which African-Americanpre-school children chose white dollsrather than black when given the choicewere indicating negative self-images (Katz1996) or simply reflecting their recognitionfor the more valued stimuli <strong>and</strong> not lowerlevels of self-esteem than whites (Powell1985; Spencer 1985).Again, it is around the time babies becomemore mobile, being able <strong>to</strong> st<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong>walk,that they start <strong>to</strong> socialise more withother people, making rudimentary foraysin<strong>to</strong> interactions with their peers. In a veryrecent study, Belsky et al (2001) measuredattentional persistence <strong>and</strong> negativeemotionality when children were 15months old <strong>and</strong> then when the childrenwere <strong>three</strong>.The researchers measuredproblem behaviour, social competence<strong>and</strong> ‘school (ie nursery group) readiness’.They found that children whoseattentional persistence was low, (theywere not able <strong>to</strong> concentrate forreasonable periods of time for the agegroup),<strong>and</strong> who showed high levels ofnegative emotions when younger, also hadlow levels of social competence. However,having reasonable levels of concentrationmediated the effects of negativeemotionality <strong>and</strong> such children werebetter able <strong>to</strong> integrate with their peers.It is generally during the second year thatyoung children begin feigning crying(showing they are aware of its effect),are more likely <strong>to</strong> make caring gestures52 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


when someone else is upset or hurt, <strong>and</strong>are beginning <strong>to</strong> take part in pretend playwith other children or adults. Being able<strong>to</strong> agree on what is happening in fantasyplay, even for a short bout, shows theability <strong>to</strong> ‘account for their partner’sthoughts; in turn,it is plausible that theexperience of collaborating on developinga pretend narrative will foster the growthof such abilities’ (Dunn 1999: 60).Subsequently, between two <strong>and</strong> <strong>three</strong>years old, children will often use the words‘pretend’, ‘want’ <strong>and</strong> ‘feel’ according <strong>to</strong>Bartsch <strong>and</strong> Wellman (1995). But bothKarmiloff-Smith (1994) <strong>and</strong> Perner (1999)suggest it is rare for them <strong>to</strong> use the word‘think’ before they are <strong>three</strong>.Such an observation may indicate <strong>to</strong> usthe power of a child’s emotional life at thisstage <strong>and</strong> it is interesting that studies ofchildren’s narratives (Bruner 1990; Feldman1992),said <strong>to</strong> begin <strong>to</strong> proliferate betweenthe ages of two <strong>and</strong> <strong>three</strong>, are at theirmost sophisticated when concerned withemotional events, especially when theexperiences involved negativeoccurrences. In these cases their s<strong>to</strong>rieswere sequentially <strong>and</strong> causally accurate<strong>and</strong> usually <strong>to</strong>ld of fear, distress <strong>and</strong> anger(Brown 1995 cited in Dunn 1999). BothBruner <strong>and</strong> Feldman have argued thatwe use narratives <strong>to</strong> explain the actionsof others <strong>and</strong> ourselves, <strong>and</strong> that, withinany given culture, narratives are used <strong>to</strong>generate a person’s sense of self. Theimportance of narratives, the way each ofus makes sense of our lives, is also stressedby Siegel (1999). Even at this young agechildren love listening <strong>to</strong> narratives aboutthemselves as babies <strong>and</strong> from these theybegin <strong>to</strong> construct their own lifenarratives. So telling children s<strong>to</strong>ries aboutevents <strong>and</strong> achievements in their ownearlier lives contributes <strong>to</strong> their sense ofself <strong>and</strong> sense of efficacy.Chen <strong>and</strong> McCollum (2000) explored theperceptions of 13 Taiwanese mothers of12 month old children on the benefits ofparent-infant interaction in relation <strong>to</strong> thedevelopment of social competence <strong>and</strong>found that these were congruent with thetraditional cultural ideas aboutinterdependence. Tronick et al’s (1992)research demonstrated that infants <strong>and</strong><strong>to</strong>ddlers adopt the patterns of the culturesin which they are brought up <strong>and</strong> that forsome this means that the sense of self isderived from multiple relationships. Harris(1989) explains how different culturesbuild on what may be, usually, a universal,innate ability <strong>to</strong> recognise negative <strong>and</strong>positive emotional states, but this can beimpaired for different reasons. As statedabove, the ability <strong>to</strong> ‘mindread’, forexample, does not extend <strong>to</strong> autisticchildren <strong>and</strong> their difficulties in formingrelationships <strong>and</strong> restricted imaginativeplay may relate <strong>to</strong> this inability (Harris1989; Hobson 1993;Trevarthen et al 1998).Hobson stresses the way in which beingable <strong>to</strong> engage in symbolic play meansbeing able <strong>to</strong> appreciate how <strong>to</strong> conveyattitudes, thoughts <strong>and</strong> knowingly confernovel (imagined) identities on<strong>to</strong> familiarobjects. Thus the sense of self as agen<strong>to</strong>f interactions <strong>and</strong> ideas is strengthened,but when these abilities are impaired,the sense of self <strong>to</strong>o may be affected.53


What must also be acknowledged is thatunderst<strong>and</strong>ing about self <strong>and</strong> others isdependent upon social <strong>and</strong> emotionalinteractions in which cognitive processescome in<strong>to</strong> play. It is this interweaving of allaspects of development <strong>and</strong> learning, itsholistic nature, which is now recognised.As Dunn states,‘Until quite recently, cognitive <strong>and</strong>socio-emotional developments werestudied as separate domains. The mappingof children’s discovery of the mind, whichhas proved so exciting, did not includea focus on the role of social experiencein influencing the development ofunderst<strong>and</strong>ing, <strong>and</strong> little attention wasdevoted <strong>to</strong> individual differences inmindreading <strong>and</strong> their possible linkswith social relations.’ (Dunn 1999:56).In fact, it has been shown (Pa rkes et al 1996 )that there are links between emotionaldevelopment <strong>and</strong> cognition indicating thatsecurely attached children show greaterability in metacognition (the ability <strong>to</strong>reflect on <strong>and</strong> piece <strong>to</strong>gether ideas abou<strong>to</strong>ne’s own learning). Researchers arguethat mothers of children who have beensecurely attached in their infancy weremore likely <strong>to</strong> have treated their babies asindividuals with minds <strong>and</strong> that as a result,by the age of four or five years old thesechildren are better at ‘mindreading’.However, Dunn’s earlier work (Dunn1987),showed that mothers of 18 month old girlsare more likely than mothers of boys thesame age <strong>to</strong> discuss other people’s feelingswith them, so there may be a genderdifference in mindreading <strong>to</strong>o.DEVELOPING SELF ASSURANCEMurray <strong>and</strong> Andrews (2000: 167)suggest that‘the child who has benefited from securityin her relationships is likely <strong>to</strong> developa sense of self confidence <strong>and</strong> assurance,so that she will have better resources <strong>to</strong>cope with difficulties.’Summing up the key points relating <strong>to</strong>children’s development of self-assurance,confidence <strong>and</strong> independence,the Framework pack concludes:-Young babies enjoy the company ofothers, but also need <strong>to</strong> feel safe <strong>and</strong>loved when they are not the centre ofadult attention.To develop independence babies need<strong>to</strong> feel safe <strong>and</strong> secure within healthyrelationships with key people.Young children need suppo rt in order <strong>to</strong>ex p l o re what they can do on their ow n .To appreciate what they can doindependently, children needsupportive relationships, through whichthey develop self-confidence, a belief inthemselves <strong>and</strong> healthy self-esteem.(<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong> Three Matters: Development Matters– Developing Self-Assurance)Babies who will be come self-assure dc h i l d ren <strong>and</strong> adults tend also <strong>to</strong> be co m ere s i l i e nt (see chapter 1). Th ey will be thec h i l d ren <strong>and</strong> adults who have fac<strong>to</strong>rs intheir lives enabling them <strong>to</strong> co pe with anya dverse circ u m s t a n ces which may be s e tt h e m .The re s e a rch re po rted in the sect i o non re s i l i e n ce in chapter 1 indicates that the54 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


main fac<strong>to</strong>rs enabling such self-assura n ce<strong>to</strong> develop re l ate <strong>to</strong> being valued by <strong>and</strong>h aving secure at t a c h m e nts <strong>to</strong> at least ones i g n i f i ca nt pe r s o n . Both pare nts <strong>and</strong>p ra ctitioners can co nt ri b u te <strong>to</strong> deve l o p i n ga child’s self-assura n ce. As At h ey (1990: 207 )s u g g e s t s, ‘The time is ri g ht for pare nt s,g ra n d p a re nts <strong>and</strong> pro fessionals <strong>to</strong> wo rk<strong>to</strong>gether in order <strong>to</strong> increase the qualityof mind in young childre n .’We should be concerned that somechildren do not become self-assured,nor acquire social confidence <strong>and</strong>competence. Later in life it tends <strong>to</strong> bethese children who are rated by theirteachers <strong>and</strong> by their peers astroublesome <strong>and</strong> disruptive. They seekattention <strong>and</strong> emotional support insocially unacceptable ways <strong>and</strong> are unable<strong>to</strong> settle down calmly <strong>to</strong> learning inschool. Their teenage <strong>and</strong> early adult livesare said <strong>to</strong> be punctuated by problemssuch as drug <strong>and</strong> alcohol abuse, accidents,violence, adolescent pregnancy, psychiatricdisorders, family disruption <strong>and</strong>environmental risks.By reviewing a number of studies, Clarke<strong>and</strong> Clarke (2000) claim that the basis oftheir difficulties often lies in theirirritability (which the Clarkes link <strong>to</strong>temperament <strong>and</strong> their often chaotic livesat home), poor cognitive skills, <strong>and</strong> in theirlack of emotional security or strongaffectional ties <strong>to</strong> anyone. The High/Scopeearly childhood education programmeclaims <strong>to</strong> prevent numerous anti-socialbehaviours in later life (Berrueta-Clementet al 1984). Two of the key aspects of theHigh/Scope programme are the fact thatparents are involved as well as thepre-schoolers themselves, <strong>and</strong> that theprogramme aims <strong>to</strong> promote the children’s(<strong>and</strong> the parents’) sense of agency. Oneaspect of a sense of agency is the ability<strong>to</strong> regulate oneself – managing one’semotions (this does not mean repressingthem – see Goleman 1996), gaining controlof one’s actions <strong>and</strong> having strategies forcoping with heightened arousal.Self regulation was the main focus ofa study by Kochanska et al (2001). Theyassessed 108 children at 14,22, 33 <strong>and</strong>45 months of age in ‘do’ (keep doing aboring/ unpleasant task) <strong>and</strong> ‘don’t’ (don’tdo something enjoyable) tasks <strong>to</strong> find outhow compliant the children were. Theyfound that girls were generally morecompliant than boys <strong>and</strong> when childrenshowed eagerness <strong>to</strong> comply with theirmothers’ requests (committed compliance)this was found <strong>to</strong> relate <strong>to</strong> theirinternalisation of maternal rules.Ak<strong>and</strong>e’s research (1992) provides ideasfor helping parents <strong>to</strong> promote children’sviews of themselves as valuable,responsible <strong>and</strong> capable of learning <strong>and</strong>in research on mothers’ approaches <strong>to</strong>controlling young children’s socialinteraction <strong>and</strong> expressions of au<strong>to</strong>nomy,Donovan et al (2000) found that motherswho adopted either high power-assertionstrategies, using negative control,<strong>and</strong> those who had earlier demonstratedlow rates of intervention in children’scrying, had young children whosebehaviour would be more likely <strong>to</strong> escalate55


in<strong>to</strong> defiance than those who adoptedmore moderate approaches <strong>to</strong> both infantcrying <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ddler compliance. Boukydis<strong>and</strong> Lester (1998) also explored parentalresponses <strong>to</strong> their babies’ crying when40 weeks old (both full- <strong>and</strong> pre-termbabies were included). They found thatp ra ctitioners need <strong>to</strong> kn ow how <strong>to</strong> inte rp re tdifferent risk status in certain babies <strong>and</strong>h ow <strong>to</strong> help families re s pond appro p ri ate ly.As Hutchins <strong>and</strong> Sims (1999) point out,children develop self awareness <strong>and</strong>social awareness in conjunction with asense of their own agency. When theyhave parents <strong>and</strong> practitioners who allowthem <strong>to</strong> assert some power <strong>and</strong> controlover their own lives they learn <strong>to</strong> beself-regulating <strong>and</strong> au<strong>to</strong>nomous.HAVING A SENSE OF BELONGINGThe Fra m ewo rk ‘Bi rth <strong>to</strong> Th ree Ma t te r s :A Sense of Be l o n g i n g’ c i tes the New Ze a l a n dMi n i s t ry of Ed u cation (1996:54), whoa rgue that‘The feeling of belonging…contributes<strong>to</strong> inner well-being, security <strong>and</strong> identity.Children need <strong>to</strong> know that they areaccepted for who they are. They shouldknow that what they do can make adifference <strong>and</strong> that they can explore <strong>and</strong>try out new activities.’Here the literature review examines someof the research on very young children’srelationships in their families <strong>and</strong> in ECECsettings, in order <strong>to</strong> attempt <strong>to</strong> tease outthe key ingredients which assist thedevelopment of a sense of belonging.Belonging in one’s familyClearly families at the start of the newmillennium are very different from thefamilies of 20 <strong>to</strong> 30 years ago, but asJagger <strong>and</strong> Wright (1999:3) point out,‘thefamily is neither a pan-human universalnor a stable or essential entity… Families<strong>and</strong> family relations are, like the term itself,flexible, fluid <strong>and</strong> contingent.’Throughoutthis review one of the main messages fromthe research appears <strong>to</strong> be that babies <strong>and</strong>very young children have a fundamentalneed <strong>to</strong> be with familiar, loving adults <strong>and</strong>older children. Therefore, at a time whenfamilies are fragmented <strong>and</strong> isolated formany different reasons, we need <strong>to</strong>explore ways of ensuring that all babies<strong>and</strong> young children feel part of a family,however that is constituted.Some research on young children focuseson particular types of families <strong>and</strong> may,in so doing, limit its relevance. However,findings may still ‘make sense’ if parents<strong>and</strong> practitioners find they can identifywith what appear <strong>to</strong> be authentic <strong>and</strong>useful contributions <strong>to</strong> knowledge.For example, Boyd Webb’s (1984) researchstudied 24 children aged under four, eachof whom whom lived with their mother<strong>and</strong> father. These children had experiencedmultiple carer relationships <strong>and</strong> BoydWebb wanted <strong>to</strong> tease out what aspectsof the relationships helped the children<strong>to</strong> be socially competent, confident <strong>and</strong>self-assured.Three common str<strong>and</strong>s in thechildren’s home experiences s<strong>to</strong>od out.They were the parents’ use of ‘bugging <strong>and</strong>nudging’ (for example, asking a child <strong>to</strong>56 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


demonstrate achievements – ‘Go on,showGr<strong>and</strong>ad how you dance <strong>to</strong> Bob theBuilder’s music’); the use of pet names –perhaps these are an indica<strong>to</strong>r of‘snuggling in’ <strong>and</strong> of intimacy; <strong>and</strong> thirdly,respect for rituals devised by the child (forexample, s/he always likes Big Ted/an oldsock,etc in bed; we always look out of thewindow at the sky <strong>and</strong> sing a special songbefore s/he goes <strong>to</strong> bed).So although,like Boyd Webb’s research,many projects may have been carried outin families with two heterosexual parents,it is still possible in some cases <strong>to</strong> reflec<strong>to</strong>n the findings by viewing the findingsabout the adult roles as indicative of‘mothering/fathering/parenting’ rather thanas specific. It is still important however,<strong>to</strong> recognise that mothers <strong>and</strong> fathersmay behave differently because they livein a gendered society.Children’s relationships with their fatherswere the focus of a number of studies.Examining the fac<strong>to</strong>rs that influence thefather-baby relationship during the firsttwo months of life, Anderson (1996)discovered the powerful role of the baby’smother in either including or excludingthe father’s involvement in infant care.Fathers’ attitudes <strong>and</strong> the amount of timethey are actually able <strong>to</strong> spend with theirbabies are also key ingredients in theextent <strong>to</strong> which they are able <strong>to</strong> formattachments (Cox <strong>and</strong> Marg<strong>and</strong> 1992).In another study by Belsky (1996), he setup a Strange Situation with father <strong>and</strong> sonparticipants. He found that the fathers ofinfants who were securely attached <strong>to</strong>them had particular characteristics incommon. According <strong>to</strong> Belsky they tended<strong>to</strong> be more extrovert, agreeable, havehappier marriages <strong>and</strong> more positiveemotional work-home contexts thanfathers of insecurely attached babies.A meta-analysis of available studies byFox et al (1991) indicates that attachment<strong>to</strong> one parent is indicative of attachment<strong>to</strong> the other in two parent families.Na ka m u ra et al (2000) found that fathersin their study sco red lower than motherson a scale concerned with fosteringchildren’s cognitive development.Grych <strong>and</strong> Cl a rk (1999) found that fat h e r s’interactions with their babies differedd e pending on whether or not the motherswo rked full time. Fathers whose part n e r sworked part-time or who were notemployed outside the home were found<strong>to</strong> be more sensitive <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> show morepositive behaviours <strong>to</strong>wards the infants.Babies from 77 families were observed atfo u r, 12 <strong>and</strong> 13 months old by Bra u n g a rt etal’s (1999) team in order <strong>to</strong> assess mother<strong>and</strong>father-infant attachment. They foundthat boys from dual-earner families werelikely <strong>to</strong> be strongly attached <strong>to</strong> theirmothers but not <strong>to</strong> their fathers at fourmonths, compared with babies from otherfamily types, but these fathers were alsolikely <strong>to</strong> be less sensitive <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> report lessmarital affection at this time.In an intervention study, Cullen et al(2000) found that encouraging fathers<strong>to</strong> massage their babies for 15 minutesbefore bedtime for one month enhancedtheir relationships <strong>to</strong> the extent that the57


infants displayed more warmth <strong>and</strong>enjoyment during floor play interactionsby the end of the study period.Children’s attachments <strong>to</strong> their mothers<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> their fathers were explored <strong>to</strong>expose the key features of youngchildren’s development from a dyad(paired) <strong>to</strong> an individual organisation ofself. Suess et al (1992) found that girls’competence in specific areas, behaviourproblems, <strong>and</strong> ability <strong>to</strong> resolve conflictwere related <strong>to</strong> their attachment <strong>to</strong> theirmothers, but this was not found <strong>to</strong> be thecase for boys, although overallcompetence was linked <strong>to</strong> attachment <strong>to</strong>mothers for all the children. When showncar<strong>to</strong>ons <strong>and</strong> asked about the intentionsof the characters, children with insecureattachment his<strong>to</strong>ries were more likely <strong>to</strong>attribute negative intentions than positive.Further research (Kornharber <strong>and</strong> Marcos2000) indicates that from as young as twoyears of age children are able <strong>to</strong> adjust theways in which they communicate withmothers <strong>and</strong> fathers. Fathers wereobserved <strong>to</strong> produce more messagesrelated <strong>to</strong> telling children what <strong>to</strong> do <strong>and</strong>task performance than did mothers.Teenage fathers we re found <strong>to</strong> be ofte nconfused about childca re <strong>and</strong> theirp roblems we re ex a ce r b ated by financialwo rri e s, even where they showed stro n gi nte rest <strong>and</strong> invo lve m e nt in their babies( Rhein et al 1997 ). In these conditions thef athers we re like ly <strong>to</strong> be come disinte re s te d<strong>and</strong> subsequent ly disengaged. Yet inanother study (Cu t rona et al 1998 )longitudinal data we re co l l e cted abo u ta d o l e s ce nt pare nt h ood <strong>and</strong> where bo t hp a rtners we re in an affe ct i o n ate, i nt i m ate<strong>and</strong> suppo rt i ve re l ationship six weeks afte rd e l i ve ry, the fathers of their babies we rem o re like ly <strong>to</strong> remain invo lve d. Fu rt h e r, w h e nthe re l ationship remained stable during theb a by’s first <strong>and</strong> second ye a r s, the infant s’d oc<strong>to</strong>rs we re less like ly <strong>to</strong> re po rt injuri e s,or inve s t i g ations of abuse <strong>and</strong> neglect.Howeve r, the team found that fathers we rem o re like ly <strong>to</strong> stay invo lved when them o t h e r s’ l i ves we re free of stressful eve nt s,i n d i cating a delicate web of inte rl i n ke dsensitivities <strong>and</strong> po te ntial stre s s e s.Home observations by Volling <strong>and</strong> Be l s ky( 1992) have demonstrated how them o t h e r- f i r s t bo rn <strong>and</strong> fat h e r- f i r s t bo rnat t a c h m e nts can have an enduring effe c<strong>to</strong>n the quality of (subsequent) siblingre l at i o n s h i p s. Within the family, c h i l d re nover two years of age are more like ly<strong>to</strong> show dra m atic decreases in theirat t a c h m e nt securi ty on the birth ofa brother or siste r, co m p a red with yo u n g e rf i r s t bo rns (Teti et al 1996 ). This re s e a rc hteam also found that the levels of stre s sdue <strong>to</strong> other fac<strong>to</strong> r s, such as mari t a ld i s h a rm o ny or mate rnal psyc h i at rici l l n e s s, we re also implicated in thec h i l d re n’s re a ct i o n s.We indicated earlier that Herbert <strong>and</strong>Carpenter (Herbert 1994; Herbert <strong>and</strong>Carpenter 1994; Carpenter 1997) havealerted the field <strong>to</strong> the ways in whichfathers of children born with identifiablespecial needs are often excluded fromthe circle of concern. There are also58 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


implications for siblings, which aresummarised in Carpenter (1997). Like thechildren described earlier whose familiesinvolved them in discussions about a newbaby, the brothers <strong>and</strong> sisters of babieswith special needs benefit from suchinvolvement. They may feel isolated, guiltyor resentful,despite the existence ofpositive benefits attached <strong>to</strong> the birth ofthe new baby. Other implications for thesesiblings include possible changes ofcaregivers, because parents may needextra time with the new baby, or <strong>to</strong> attendhospital appointments, for example.Dunn’s early research (1984) on siblingspoints <strong>to</strong> other studies where initialambivalence <strong>to</strong>wards a new baby brotheror sister is common. Dunn goes on <strong>to</strong>provide evidence for the mix of positive<strong>and</strong> negative encounters siblingsexperience with each other during theseearly years – quarrelling <strong>and</strong> fighting,l ov i n g, ca ring for <strong>and</strong> co m fo rt i n g. We kn owf rom other co nte m po ra ry cultures where itis still common practice for children asyoung as <strong>three</strong> <strong>to</strong> be placed in the chargeof a younger baby, that these children,<strong>to</strong>gether with children we kn ow <strong>and</strong> Du n n’sresearch participants, that even in theirthird year they are already able <strong>to</strong> adaptthe way they behave <strong>and</strong> talk, for example,using terms of endearment <strong>to</strong>wards thebaby that parents, gr<strong>and</strong>parents <strong>and</strong> othersignificant adults have modelled.Dunn’s research also cites examples ofthe ‘babies’ teasing or comforting theolder child, evidence of their ability <strong>to</strong>empathise, ‘What is particularly notableis that some of the secondborn children,as young as 14, 15 or 16 months, attempted<strong>to</strong> comfort their older siblings.’ (Dunn1984:23). Dunn reports that sisters <strong>and</strong>brothers fight with each other much morefrequently than with other childrenoutside the home – the incidence for boysbeing roughly equal <strong>to</strong> that with peers butfor girls far higher with brothers <strong>and</strong>sisters. According <strong>to</strong> Dunn,one should bewary of assuming <strong>to</strong>o much about rivalry<strong>and</strong> jealousy from such conflicts, <strong>and</strong>parents need not blame themselves.She states, ‘Siblings don’t choose <strong>to</strong> spendtheir early lives <strong>to</strong>gether – they are forced<strong>to</strong> live <strong>to</strong>gether. We shouldn’t be surprisedif in some cases they find it very difficult<strong>to</strong> get along…’(Dunn 1984: 106). Later sheargues,‘It is because they underst<strong>and</strong> theirsiblings so well <strong>and</strong> because they feel sos t ro n g ly about them, t h at their re l at i o n s h i pis so significant <strong>and</strong> so revealing.’(Dunn 1984: 144).Belonging in an Early ChildhoodEducation <strong>and</strong> Care settingBelonging may be defined differentlyin different cultures <strong>and</strong> communities.There may be traditions which have beendeveloped over time <strong>to</strong> bind communities<strong>to</strong>gether <strong>and</strong> they may mean thatindividuals are not as important as thewhole collective. Rosenthal (2000)suggests parents <strong>and</strong> practitioners inindividualistic societies will have differentexpectations from those in collectivisticsocieties <strong>and</strong> that the dominant culturalconstructions of early childhood impact59


on the practices used <strong>to</strong> socialise babies<strong>and</strong> young children. While exploringapproaches <strong>to</strong> early literacy in a crossculturalstudy, David et al (2000) foundthat in France babies are regarded ascitizens from birth,they ‘belong’ <strong>to</strong> acommunity <strong>and</strong> that parental <strong>and</strong> societalexpectations – of both children <strong>and</strong> of thestate – are reflected in ECEC provision <strong>and</strong>practices. Similarly, Moss (1990) reviewingpolicy in Sweden, found that children areregarded as the responsibility of the wholesociety, not the private or soleresponsibility of their parents. However,Gautier (1996), reviewing family policy,argued that the UK <strong>and</strong> USA havetraditionally had laissez faire policieswhich have endorsed the idea that unlessparents are defined as inadequate in someway, the state does not intervene in howthey treat their children,but nor doesit provide much support. Recently<strong>Government</strong> policies are promoting thedevelopment of ECEC services, includingprovision for children from birth <strong>to</strong> <strong>three</strong>years <strong>and</strong> clearly it is important thatchildren’s well being is at the heart ofthese developments.Elfer et al (2002) discuss the difficultiesstaff in group settings face when theyattempt <strong>to</strong> achieve close relationships withbabies <strong>and</strong> young children. There areissues relating <strong>to</strong> shifts, holidays, childrenin different age b<strong>and</strong>s being afforded new<strong>and</strong> different experiences, not <strong>to</strong> mentionstaff changes due <strong>to</strong> promotion or familycircumstances, for example. WhileDahlberg et al (1999) argue that oneshould not create ‘false closeness’, they alsoadd there should be a concept of intensityof relationships, meaning a complexnetwork connecting not only people –the adults <strong>and</strong> other children in the setting– but also the environment itself <strong>and</strong> theshared activities. Perhaps what needs <strong>to</strong>be avoided is possessiveness rather thanintimate, loving relationships which aremutually enjoyable for the adults <strong>and</strong>children involved, because babies <strong>and</strong>young children seem <strong>to</strong> thrive in ECECsettings where they experience suchrelationships <strong>and</strong> where there is continuityof care giving (Rutter 1995).Attendance at an ECEC setting also affordsbabies <strong>and</strong> young children opportunities<strong>to</strong> be with other young children <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>make friends. Young children’s friendshipsmay be subject <strong>to</strong> family mobility butwhere family location is stable, childrenhave friendships for much longer thanused <strong>to</strong> be thought possible. Dunn (1993)cites a number of studies including herown which indicate that children agedonly four had friendships as long st<strong>and</strong>ingas two years, so these friendships hadbegun when the children were about twoyears old. Further, according <strong>to</strong> Dunn,young children spend more time talkingwith,arguing (<strong>and</strong> making up) with theirfriends than they do with children who arenot their friends, but they are unable<strong>to</strong> articulate what is special about theirrelationship. Th ey will usually just say thatt h ey play <strong>to</strong>g e t h e r. Friends are clearly ve ryi m po rt a nt, for as Howes (1987) showe d,c h i l d ren who made transitions in dayca re60 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


with friends fared be t ter than those wholost their friends at that time. Dunn (1993)found that children whose friends changedd ayca re settings with them re m e m be re dt h at it was the pre s e n ce of those fri e n d st h at made them feel happy there.In addition, Dunn looked at the possiblelinks between children’s attachments withfamily members – principally theirmothers – <strong>and</strong> those with their friends.She found, surprisingly, that there was noevidence of an association betweenchildren’s expressions of emotion withtheir mothers (either positive or negative)<strong>and</strong> their interactions with their friends.However, she also found that children fromfamilies where there were frequent familyarguments <strong>and</strong> outbursts of anger wereunable <strong>to</strong> reason with their friends whendisputes arose. Most importantly, childrenwho enjoyed high levels of involvementwith their mothers were more likely <strong>to</strong>compromise <strong>and</strong> be concilia<strong>to</strong>ry withfriends, <strong>to</strong> enter elaborate shared fantasyplay bouts <strong>and</strong> conversations.One could surmise that the early closerelationships <strong>and</strong> the mothers’ modellinghelped these children learn how <strong>to</strong> get onwith other children, <strong>to</strong> solve disputeswithout being unreasonable, <strong>and</strong> thatsimilar interactions with <strong>and</strong> modelling bya practitioner can have the same effect.In fact, although her detailed qualitativeresearch was with children in two preschoolgroups, Sally Lubeck (1986) foundthat children in a setting where the <strong>three</strong>members of a staff team modelledpositive, supportive, cooperative behaviourwith each other the children showed moreof these behaviours than in a settingwhere staff did not behave in this way.The findings from re s e a rch ex a m i n i n gc h i l d re n’s adjustment <strong>to</strong> ECEC settings aremixed. Deater-Deckard et al (1996) claimthat children’s behavioural adjustment ism o re closely linked <strong>to</strong> home env i ro n m e ntfac<strong>to</strong>rs than the quality rating awardedtheir day care setting. However, whenHowes et al (1994) explored the waysin which a child’s re l ationship witha pra ctitioner affe cted their re l at i o n s h i pwith their pe e r s, t h ey found that yo u n gchildren who felt secure with theireducarer displayed complex play withpeers <strong>and</strong> were gregarious. Those whowere dependent on the adult were,however, withdrawn from their peers<strong>and</strong> hostile <strong>and</strong> aggressive <strong>to</strong>wardsthem. Where practitioners encouraged<strong>and</strong> modelled soc i a l i s at i o n ,the childre nseemed <strong>to</strong> demonstrate greateracceptance of each other. The sameresearch team (Howes et al 1994a)had also found that children’s socialcompetence with peers <strong>and</strong> theirrelationship with their educa<strong>to</strong>r at fouryears of age, were both related <strong>to</strong> theirrelationship with their first educarer.In Sweden Broberg et al (1997) found thatthe children’s cognitive abilities at ageeight could have been predicted from thequality of the out-of-home care (in centresor with childminders) they attended.Theirresearch involved 87 children agedbetween 16 <strong>and</strong> 19 months at the start ofthe project, whom they tracked through at61


least 36 months of attendance at a daycaresetting <strong>and</strong> on in<strong>to</strong> primary school.‘Quality’ is a value-laden concept whichmakes comparisons, particularly thoseacross national or cultural boundaries,difficult <strong>to</strong> interpret. It is possible that thedifferences in these findings may reflectthe contexts in which the studies werecarried out, but they may also indicate thatsome children need extra support frompractitioners in becoming a member ofthe setting’s community – in ‘belonging ‘there <strong>and</strong> perhaps some individualpractitioners <strong>and</strong> some national systemstake greater pains <strong>to</strong> ensure inclusion.Sadly, McGuire (1991) found that nurserystaff often failed <strong>to</strong> give the additionalattention a withdrawn child needed <strong>to</strong>help them become a member of thegroup, or <strong>to</strong> be engaged in play activities –since during her observations thesechildren were found <strong>to</strong> spend more timethan others apparently unoccupied.Further research which can providepointers <strong>to</strong> children’s sense of belongingin an ECEC setting are those concernedwith young children’s conflicts. Thefindings from these obser vational studiessuggest that such episodes help youngchildren <strong>to</strong> learn the cognitive skill of‘st<strong>and</strong>ing in someone else’s shoes’, <strong>to</strong> thinkin terms of multiple attributes <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>recognise the consequences of theiractions (Sims et al 1997; Singer 2001).The ways in which adults manage suchepisodes can help children developeffective conflict resolution strategies.Sometimes, allowing children space <strong>and</strong>time, in a supportive <strong>and</strong> safe atmosphere,means they resolve their own conflictswithout adult interference (Singer 2001).Children who learn <strong>to</strong> resolve conflictssuccessfully develop interpersonal skills,becoming more popular <strong>and</strong> interactingwith their peers more effectively.Further messages for practitioners canbe found in the detailed qualitativeresearch by Anning (1999).This researchfocused on parents’ <strong>and</strong> educa<strong>to</strong>rs’interpretations of children’s meaningmaking in a group setting. While thechildren in her study were already <strong>three</strong>years old, Anning argues that they couldbe seen <strong>to</strong> be developing a sense of selfas members of a home community <strong>and</strong>of a pre-school community.‘What was impressive was the range <strong>and</strong>fluidity of the meaning making systemsthey were using. It was also quite clearthat their particular passions <strong>and</strong>preoccupations shaped theirrepresentations <strong>and</strong> communications athome. In the nursery the adults weredriven by a national climate of pre-schoolsettings having <strong>to</strong> provide a ’foundationfor schooling’…imagine how childrenstruggle <strong>to</strong> make sense of the continuities<strong>and</strong> discontinuities between jointinvolvement episodes at home <strong>and</strong> in preschoolsettings, we can speculate that forsome children there may be someoverlap…But for many, the discontinuitiesmust be daunting.’ (Anning 1999:16-17)62 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Attending <strong>to</strong> external dem<strong>and</strong>s (such asSt<strong>and</strong>ards <strong>and</strong> Inspections) can bedaunting for practitioners so they needreassurance that the children in their care<strong>and</strong> their families are their first priorities.The key ‘Development Matters’ points fromthe Framework for the development of asense of belonging are as follows:-‘Snuggling in’ gives young babiesphysical, psychological <strong>and</strong> emotionalcomfort.To sustain healthy emotionalattachments, babies need familiar,trusting, safe <strong>and</strong> secure relationships.Young children's developing attitudes<strong>and</strong> beliefs are shaped by the valueplaced on individual differences <strong>and</strong>similarities.The development of a strong sense ofidentity both individually <strong>and</strong> withina group helps children feel they belong.Finally, as Dunn concludes,‘relationships change in nature as childrengrow up. New dimensions of intimacy, selfdisclosure,<strong>and</strong> shared intimacy becomeapparent over the early years, reflectingchildren’s growing social underst<strong>and</strong>ing.These developments bring new sources<strong>and</strong> new patterns of individual differencesin close relationships as children develop.’(Dunn 1993:114).Initially those close relationships will bewith parents <strong>and</strong> siblings, but practitioners<strong>and</strong> other children attending an ECECsetting will be drawn in<strong>to</strong> the child’sgrowing circle of significant relationships<strong>and</strong> it is through the interactions involvedthat a young child begins <strong>to</strong> know whothey are <strong>and</strong> what they can achieve.SUMMARY OF KEY MESSAGESResearch concerned with babies’ <strong>and</strong>young children’s sense of self, selfassurance,<strong>and</strong> sense of belongingsuggests that:-babies seem <strong>to</strong> come in<strong>to</strong> the wo rl dp rimed for at t a c h m e nt <strong>to</strong> wa rm ,f a m i l i a rca re r s, who will usually be mothers,f at h e r s, g ra n d p a re nt s, older brothers <strong>and</strong>s i s ters <strong>and</strong> key adults in ECEC settingsthese attachments form the basis forsubsequent relationships, <strong>and</strong> for senseof self/self-assurance – <strong>and</strong> thosechildren who have had experience ofwarm attachments <strong>and</strong> positiveresponses become socially adept,self assured, independent <strong>and</strong> interdependent,higher achievers in theirlater ECEC <strong>and</strong> school settings63


all areas of learning <strong>and</strong> developmentare intricately intertwined, youngchildren develop <strong>and</strong> learn holistically<strong>and</strong> their emotional <strong>and</strong> socialdevelopment seems <strong>to</strong> form thebedrock of other areaschildren’s competence <strong>and</strong> ability<strong>to</strong> resolve conflict in ECEC settingsseems <strong>to</strong> be related <strong>to</strong> their primaryattachments, especially theirattachments <strong>to</strong> their mothersparents who express negativenarratives concerning their own earlyrelationships with their parents needsupport <strong>to</strong> overcome perpetuating suchpatterns <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> form joyful,mutuallyloving relationships with their babies<strong>and</strong> small children.64 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Chapter 4A Skilful Communica<strong>to</strong>rThis chapter provides research information <strong>and</strong> ideas about the baby <strong>and</strong> child as A SkilfulCommunica<strong>to</strong>r, the focus of the second Aspect of the Framework ‘<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong> Three Matters’.This aspect includes:- being a sociable <strong>and</strong> effective communica<strong>to</strong>r; listening <strong>and</strong>responding appropriately <strong>to</strong> the language of others;<strong>and</strong> making meaning. To becomeskilful communica<strong>to</strong>rs babies <strong>and</strong> children need <strong>to</strong> be <strong>to</strong>gether with the people who areimportant <strong>to</strong> them,with whom they have warm <strong>and</strong> loving relationship. Being <strong>to</strong>getherleads <strong>to</strong> the wider development of social relations so that children become adept atforming friendships, being able <strong>to</strong> empathise, share emotions <strong>and</strong> experiences. It isthrough these interactions <strong>and</strong> upon these foundations that they become competentlanguage users. When early attempts at finding a voice are rewarded in a variety of waysbabies become more confident <strong>to</strong> extend their range of vocalisations <strong>and</strong> increase theirlanguage skills. Babies use their voices <strong>to</strong> make contact, let people know what they need<strong>and</strong> how they feel. They are also starting <strong>to</strong> learn about ‘conversation’, which requires theability <strong>to</strong> listen <strong>and</strong> respond appropriately, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> learn the ‘rules’ of communicationthrough making meaning with the familiar people in their lives. One of the main‘messages’ of this aspect of the Framework is that, above all,those around them need <strong>to</strong>value, interpret <strong>and</strong> respond <strong>to</strong> babies’ <strong>and</strong> young children’s early attempts <strong>to</strong> converse.BEING TOGETHERThis section of the literature review isdevoted <strong>to</strong> some of the research availableabout babies <strong>and</strong> their ability <strong>to</strong>communicate with the significant peoplein their lives. As Jerome Bruner points out:‘Infants are, in a word, tuned <strong>to</strong> enter theworld of human action…It is obvious thatan enormous amount of the activityof the child during the first year <strong>and</strong>a half of life is extraordinarily social <strong>and</strong>communicative…Even in the openingweeks of life the infant has the capacity<strong>to</strong> imitate facial <strong>and</strong> manual gestures;theyrespond with distress if their mothers aremasked during feeding; <strong>and</strong>, they show65


a sensitivity <strong>to</strong> expression in the motherby turn taking in vocalisation when theirlevel of arousal is moderate <strong>and</strong> bysimultaneous expression when it is high.’(Bruner 1983:27).Indeed, Karmiloff <strong>and</strong> Karmiloff-Smithclaim,in their exciting <strong>and</strong> informative tex<strong>to</strong>n early language development, thatduring its last months in the uterus thefoetus develops a growing sensitivity <strong>to</strong>the unique qualities of its mother’s voice<strong>and</strong> rhythms of its native language(Karmiloff <strong>and</strong> Karmiloff-Smith 2001).For ex a m p l e, re s e a rchers have establishedt h at babies as young as four days <strong>and</strong> eve ne a rlier can distinguish the languages po ken by their mother <strong>and</strong> family fro manother language, although they do notd i s c ri m i n ate be tween unkn own languages.For ex a m p l e, French re s e a rchers (seeKa rmiloff <strong>and</strong> Ka rm i l o f f - Smith 2001 : 17 )used a sucking technique <strong>to</strong> ex p l o re t h i s.Th ey found that babies suck harder whenh e a ring French than when hearing Ru s s i a nbut their rate of sucking showed nod i f fe re n ce be tween Russian <strong>and</strong> En g l i s h .Goldschmeid <strong>and</strong> Selleck (1996: 11) arg u et h at ‘co n s t a n cy in re l ationships with“m o t h e r s” is nece s s a ry for babies <strong>to</strong> learn<strong>to</strong> co m m u n i cate’. By mothers they mean‘a person who takes on re s po n s i b i l i ty fo rc h i l d re n’s lives <strong>and</strong> for whom providing childca re is a significa nt part of her or his wo rki n gl i fe’ ( Goldschmeid <strong>and</strong> Selleck 19 96 :6).Above all,babies appear <strong>to</strong> comein<strong>to</strong> the world with a drive <strong>to</strong> be nearfamiliar people:-‘Even at a few weeks old the infant’s loveaffair with people is shown by differentreactions <strong>to</strong> persons, as opposed <strong>to</strong>interesting objects. Moving objects maybe watched <strong>and</strong> reached for, but people,especially a carer, are responded <strong>to</strong> withsmiles, lip movements <strong>and</strong> arm waving…Getting in<strong>to</strong> relationships with peopleprobably begins in the earliest hours oflife:many newborns will imitate adult face<strong>and</strong> h<strong>and</strong> move m e nt s’. (Wh i tehead 1996 :4).Murray <strong>and</strong> Trevarthen (1985) showedthat from two months of age, around thetime they also engage in social smiling,infants are sensitive <strong>to</strong> ‘social contingency’(the mother’s responsiveness <strong>to</strong> theinfant’s signals), especially <strong>to</strong> the timingof their mothers’ emotional attunementin their two-way exchanges. These attunedexchanges indicate the developmen<strong>to</strong>f primary intersubjectivity – the rudimentsof turn taking, sensitive timing <strong>and</strong>responsiveness <strong>to</strong> the other’s behaviour,especially facial expressions.Intersubjectivity is thought <strong>to</strong> be thefoundation of early social interaction.Such early, playful interactions are called‘pro<strong>to</strong>conversations’ <strong>and</strong> they graduallyoffer the young child opportunities foranticipating <strong>and</strong> predicting <strong>and</strong> they formthe basis for social <strong>and</strong> cognitive advancesthat occur during the first year (Trevarthen<strong>and</strong> Aitken 2001).However, as Goldschmeid <strong>and</strong> Selleck(1996:11) stress:-‘Young babies are only sociable, loving <strong>and</strong>curious about each other if they havealready developed trusting, secure66 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


elationships with close <strong>and</strong> protectiveadults. Babies who do not haveopportunities <strong>to</strong> <strong>to</strong>uch,gaze, coo <strong>and</strong>babble with responsive adults give upon conversation-like exchanges. Thereis evidence of this from the work ofGoldschmeid in state institutions forgroups of ab<strong>and</strong>oned children in Trieste,Italy in 1954. These children had excellentphysical care but no personal care. Thebabies who persistently failed <strong>to</strong> getattention from an adult becamewithdrawn, passive <strong>and</strong> despairing. Theys<strong>to</strong>pped looking for friendship in adultsor with other children…Sometimes thisis not recognised for what it is, but ismistaken for contentment.‘As long ago as the 1970s, research bySnow (1977), Snow <strong>and</strong> Ferguson (1977),<strong>and</strong> others, was demonstrating thatalthough babies who are <strong>three</strong> months olddo not contribute much <strong>to</strong> a conversationthrough vocalising, they already havegeneral ‘rules’ about communicating.For example, they will gaze at the familiaradult <strong>and</strong> sustain an interaction in apattern which matches exactly that oftwo adults in conversation.In their boo k , The Social Ba by, Mu rray<strong>and</strong> An d rews (2000), not only prov i d es u perb pho<strong>to</strong>g raphic ev i d e n ce ofbabies co m m u n i cating from birt h ,t h ey state,‘Babies are attracted <strong>to</strong> other people frombirth <strong>and</strong> they quickly prefer the peoplewho have become familiar. But the babyd oe s n’t simply wa nt <strong>to</strong> be near her family<strong>and</strong> their friends – she wa nts <strong>to</strong> share herex pe ri e n ce with other people <strong>and</strong> inte ra ctwith them!’ ( Mu rray <strong>and</strong> An d rews 2000: 47 ).Further, babies become quiet whentheir mother or other familiar personapproaches, apparently primed forinteraction. By the age of <strong>three</strong> months,a baby who has begun vocalising <strong>and</strong>who gets a response (being talked <strong>to</strong> <strong>and</strong>other types of positive attention) willincrease the amount of vocalisation. Snowalso points out that mothers will usuallysimplify their speech, restricting thecontent, using repetition,simplifyinggrammar <strong>and</strong> emphasising in<strong>to</strong>nation.Babies <strong>and</strong> children are able <strong>to</strong> givefeedback clues which tell the adultsaround them when the simplified speechcan be modified a little <strong>to</strong> become morelike the type we would use <strong>to</strong> anexperienced language user.Bruner (1983) cites the work of Piagetin remarking that babies may spend theirtime doing only a small range of activities(such as banging, reaching, looking) butthey are systematic in their repetitionsof these activities. He adds that we shouldnot be surprised therefore, that they arealso systematic when it comes <strong>to</strong>language, as the work of Weir (1962)(on spontaneous speech in the cot)<strong>and</strong> Bowerman’s (1978) on children’sspontaneous errors, show.When babies <strong>and</strong> children interact witheach other, they have a different ‘voice’67


according <strong>to</strong> Goldschmeid <strong>and</strong> Selleck(1996:17). ‘Their conversations flow <strong>to</strong> <strong>and</strong>fro through complex <strong>and</strong> personal activity.Children communicate firstly throughintense gazing, vigorous sucking <strong>and</strong>explora<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>to</strong>uching <strong>and</strong> vocalisations.’They cite the research observations ofWhaley <strong>and</strong> Rubenstein (1994),who notedthat <strong>to</strong>ddlers will develop rituals, usingobjects as props, developing intimacy <strong>and</strong>communicating non-verbally. In onerelationship two children would use theirfeeding bottles in a ritual that involvedthem in alternately drinking <strong>and</strong> thenplacing the bottles on their feet. This ritualof imitating with similar objects not onlypromoted their friendship, it was a way ofexcluding other <strong>to</strong>ddlers. As Rouse Selleck(1995:cited in the Framework ‘<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong>Three Matters – Finding a Voice’) asserts:‘Although most infants do not learn <strong>to</strong> talkuntil their second year, their voices arethere for us <strong>to</strong> hear from birth.’The first <strong>three</strong> years co nt ri b u te substant i a l ly<strong>to</strong> children being highly pro f i c i e nt inlanguage by the time they are six. Al t h o u g hlanguage is learnt on seve ral diffe re nt leve l sat the same time (Ka rmiloff <strong>and</strong> Ka rm i l o f f -Smith 2001) – they do not simply learnvoca b u l a ry then move on <strong>to</strong> gra m m a r, e tc –t h ey must get <strong>to</strong> grips with the sounds oflanguage (phonology); wo rds (voca b u l a ry );the way wo rds are ord e red (gra m m a r );the way sente n ces are put <strong>to</strong>gether <strong>to</strong>mean something (disco u r s e ); as wellas h ow <strong>to</strong> use language (Ta bors 1997 ).As Wh i tehead (2002) stre s s e s, a child’s firstwo rd is based on ex pe ri e n ce of liste n i n g,o b s e rving <strong>and</strong> ex pe ri m e nting with sounds<strong>and</strong> making highly select i ve imitiations offamiliar pe o p l e.Treva rthen (2000) analysed the musica l i tyof rhyming games <strong>and</strong> he found that theyfo l l ow the same pat te rn s, length ofu t te ra n ces by the adult be fo re the infantm a kes a co nt ri b u t i o n ,as in re l a xe d,m u t u a l ly enjoyable baby-adult inte ra ct i o n s.It seems that no other animals engage ingames such as pe e ka boo, Ride a coc k -h o r s e, <strong>and</strong> other inte ra ctions that depe n don rhyming <strong>and</strong>/or re petition in the sameway (Bruner 1983 ),be cause all depend onthe use <strong>and</strong> exchange of language orlanguage as an ‘a nt i c i p a<strong>to</strong> ry dev i ce’( e g. Round <strong>and</strong> round the gard e n ).It is interesting that ways of talking <strong>to</strong>babies is not a taught skill but is aculturally learned behaviour <strong>and</strong> one that,even in this technological society,continues <strong>to</strong> dominate early interactions(Brice Heath 1983;Schieffelin <strong>and</strong> Ochs1986). The most self conscious parent isdriven by the very appearance <strong>and</strong> natureof babies <strong>to</strong> converse <strong>and</strong> communicatein ways that have been used in families forgenerations (Gopnik et al 1999). Bruner’ssuggestion,that mothers <strong>and</strong> carers ‘createframeworks of interaction in jointlyconstructed little worlds’ (Bruner 1987: 114)is a reminder that even the way we think(cognition) is ‘encultured’ (Hil<strong>to</strong>n 1996) <strong>and</strong>that babies learn how <strong>to</strong> behave in aparticular social setting in collusion with68 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


adults <strong>and</strong> siblings around them. Theworld of the family is the first culturalcontact a baby encounters <strong>and</strong> the familymembers <strong>and</strong> carers who surround herhelp her <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> develop therole she needs <strong>to</strong> play <strong>to</strong> become part ofthe drama of family life (Bruner 1983;1987;Dunn 1988;Gopnik et al 2001).The Framework ‘<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong> Three Matters’sums up the key development points forthe component Being Together as follows:-Young babies are sociable from birth,using a variety of ways <strong>to</strong> gainattention.Babies make social contact according<strong>to</strong> their physical possibilities.Young children form friendships <strong>and</strong>can be caring <strong>to</strong>wards each other.Children learn social skills <strong>and</strong> enjoybeing <strong>and</strong> talking with adults <strong>and</strong>other children.The implications of the re s e a rch findingswhich lead <strong>to</strong> the above state m e nts abo u tbabies <strong>and</strong> young children as ski l f u lco m m u n i ca<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> the impo rt a n ce ofbeing <strong>to</strong>gether are pri m a ri ly that they needre l a xe d, p l ayful <strong>and</strong> loving co nve r s at i o n sri g ht from birt h . Ad d i t i o n a l ly, p a re nts needre a s s u ra n ce that talking with their babieswill fo s ter their language deve l o p m e nt sot h at by the time they are in their third ye a rt h ey will be ex pe rt at taking turns <strong>and</strong>s ocial inte ra ctions which include talk. Fro mas early as po s s i b l e, it is impo rt a nt <strong>to</strong>o b s e rve, n o te <strong>and</strong> discuss with pare nts anya p p a re nt co n s t ra i nts on a child’s ability <strong>to</strong>d evelop speech due <strong>to</strong> impairm e nts in thep hys i cal or nervous sys te m ,so that helpcan be sought.Some of the other key messages forpractice are that babies need <strong>and</strong>/or seem<strong>to</strong> enjoy:-responsive <strong>and</strong> encouraginginteractionsturn-taking patterns of interactionmotherese, rhyming games, singing <strong>and</strong>word playnot <strong>to</strong>o much background noise (eg.from television).FINDING A VOICEThe early sounds that babies make areoften thought <strong>to</strong> be merely a mimic of theconstantly repeated sounds that adultsmake <strong>to</strong> them. Imitation, though,is ahighly sophisticated skill <strong>and</strong> certainlyVygotsky maintained that it is more thana simple mechanical activity, as we pointedout in chapter 2. He argued that ‘<strong>to</strong>imitate, it is necessary <strong>to</strong> possess themeans of stepping from something oneknows <strong>to</strong> something new’ (Vygotsky 1986:187). But as Meltzoff (1988) claims that thisability <strong>to</strong> imitate, demonstrated in hisprotruding <strong>to</strong>ngue experiments withbabies who respond similarly, can evenbe seen in newborns, we can assume thatimitation forms one aspect of languageacquisition. It is not, however, the wholes<strong>to</strong>ry, because babies <strong>and</strong> young childrendo not simply mimic what they hear. Theyactually create language, <strong>and</strong> practitioners69


<strong>and</strong> parents will be familiar with veryclever instances such as the rule-boundaddition of ‘-ed’ <strong>to</strong> form a past participle,instead of the form for an irregular verb –for example, ‘I goed’ instead of ‘I went’.Chomsky’s (1964) contribution <strong>to</strong> thisdebate, with the idea that infants are born‘prewired’ for language was essential (seechapter 2). But Bruner <strong>and</strong> otherresearchers have argued that languageis embedded in the social <strong>and</strong> emotionalinteractions of babies’ lives <strong>and</strong> that thelaws governing their acquistion oflanguage are the same as those governingother aspects of learning.So babies are born in<strong>to</strong> families,communities <strong>and</strong> cultures as attentive,curious learners <strong>and</strong> their brains arealready fine tuned <strong>to</strong> attend <strong>to</strong> soundsaround them <strong>and</strong> process them as par<strong>to</strong>f their developing underst<strong>and</strong>ing of theworlds in which they find themselves.And, because traditionally most carersof young babies have been mothers,the nature of the cooing <strong>and</strong> babbling thatgoes on between adult <strong>and</strong> child has beencalled ‘motherese’. Cooing usually starts ataround the <strong>three</strong> month stage (Karmiloff-Smith 1994) <strong>and</strong> the adult’s (usually it isthe mother) response can act as a ‘reward’encouraging these early attempts atsound making <strong>and</strong> interaction. Babbling,repeating the same sound over <strong>and</strong> overas practice, begins <strong>to</strong> proliferate in babies’behaviour, often when they are alone, onlya few months later. All babies babble, eventhose with severe hearing losses, <strong>and</strong> evenwhen they are getting no reinforcemen<strong>to</strong>ther than the sound of their own efforts.In an early study by the Newsons ofchildren <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ys, mothers were describedas babies’ first <strong>to</strong>y, their first plaything asmothers interact with their children inplayful spoken <strong>and</strong> physical ways (Newson<strong>and</strong> Newson 1979). When both playing<strong>and</strong> offering spoken interchanges duringdaily routines, nappy changes, washing,etc, Stern says that mothers intuitivelytreat their babies ‘as the people they areabout <strong>to</strong> become by working in their zoneof proximal development’ (Stern 1985: 43)<strong>and</strong> the study by Gordon Wells in Bris<strong>to</strong>ldescribed the language between children<strong>and</strong> their mothers in home contexts asbeing crucial <strong>to</strong> the child’s constructionof their ’internal model of the world’(Wells 1987:35). He claims that babieshave an ’inherent sociability’ that isessential for language development;thatis they show an interest in faces <strong>and</strong>human sounds from the outset <strong>and</strong>mothers, or regular care givers, are able<strong>to</strong> tune in<strong>to</strong> this interest <strong>and</strong> join withbabies in what seems <strong>to</strong> be ’conversation’.Gopnik <strong>and</strong> her colleagues describe thesewordless conversations involving cooing<strong>and</strong> ’motherese’ type talk as responses <strong>to</strong>the baby ’flirting’, almost as if the adult<strong>and</strong> child are engaged in an ’intricatedance’ (Gopnik et al 1999:30). Indeed, theintimacy of these ’conversations’ is almostirresistible <strong>to</strong> most adults who invariablywarm <strong>to</strong> babies’ responses <strong>to</strong> theirattentions. The success of such70 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


interactions <strong>and</strong> interchanges dependson the sensitivity of the adult <strong>to</strong> the tunes<strong>and</strong> rhythms of the baby as they begin <strong>to</strong>laugh <strong>and</strong> ‘talk’ in turn.Kuhl et al (1992) tell us that‘At the beginning of life, human infantsexhibit a similar pattern of phoneticperception regardless of the languageenvironment in which they are born. Theydiscern differences between the phoneticunits of many different languages,including languages they have neverheard, indicating that the perception ofhuman speech is strongly influenced byinnate fac<strong>to</strong>rs.’ (Kuhl et al 1992:606)These researchers go on <strong>to</strong> discuss the factthat adults are limited in the sounds theyare able <strong>to</strong> perceive, whereas babies areopen <strong>and</strong> show a perception <strong>to</strong> speechsounds that is universal. Kuhl et al’s (1992)research indicated that by six months ofage, well before they acquire language<strong>and</strong> can underst<strong>and</strong> word meanings,babies’ perception of the sounds usedin the languages they hear around themhave influenced what they are sensitive<strong>to</strong>/perceive. They tested 32 American <strong>and</strong>32 Swedish babies <strong>and</strong> found a ’magneteffect’ <strong>to</strong> home language. The researchersinterpret this as meaning that the speechbabies hear in their first six months formsa basis for learning language, words <strong>and</strong>their meanings <strong>and</strong> that the ability <strong>to</strong>perceive (as important) <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> mimicsounds in babble, closes down <strong>to</strong>e n compass only familiar speech pat te rn s<strong>and</strong> sounds. It is also the case that by12 months old, babies babble only in thelanguage sounds they have been hearingaround them. Kuhl et al (1992) point outthat this evidence also means that thereseem <strong>to</strong> be phonetic pro<strong>to</strong>types (modelsof language <strong>and</strong> how it sounds, evenbefore language is unders<strong>to</strong>od or spoken)that are ‘fundamental perceptual-cognitivebuilding blocks rather than by-products oflanguage acquisition’ (Kuhl et al 1992: 608).This highlights the links between languageexperience <strong>and</strong> cognitive development.It also alerts us <strong>to</strong> the fact that beingspoken <strong>to</strong>, sung <strong>to</strong> <strong>and</strong> hearing languagepatterns clearly, without backgroundinterference such as television <strong>to</strong> interferewith hearing the sounds, right from birthis very important.In a study researching the effects ofovercrowded homes, Evans et al (1999)found parents living in such conditions areless responsive than those not living incrowded conditions but having the samesocio-economic status. In crowded homesparents would use less complex language<strong>to</strong> their children. The researchers suggestthis finding may provide some explanationfor the delays in cognition found amongchildren from overcrowded homes.Markus et al (2000) carried out alongitudinal study of 21 babies <strong>and</strong> theirparents. They found that language at18 months was related <strong>to</strong> differences inearlier infant-caregiver joint attentionepisodes – the frequency, quality,71


esponsiveness <strong>and</strong> duration of suchepisodes. They also report a link betweenthis finding at 18 months <strong>and</strong> thechildren’s scores on both the MacArthurCommunicative Development Inven<strong>to</strong>ries<strong>and</strong> Bayley Scales of Infant Developmentat 21 <strong>and</strong> 24 months of age. In otherwords, the more babies experience sharedtalk <strong>and</strong> activity, the more articulate theyare as young talkers.In another project with babies agedbetween two <strong>and</strong> six months, Kokkinaki<strong>and</strong> Kugiumutzakis (2000) explored theinteractions of 15 babies in Crete with theirmothers <strong>and</strong> with their fathers. They foundno differences in the infants’s vocalimitations of either parent in terms offrequency, pauses, <strong>to</strong>tal duration of theinteractions, but there were differences inthe nature of some of the sounds imitated.In interactions with their fathers, both girls<strong>and</strong> boys of around two years old tend <strong>to</strong>use more directives, whereas with theirmothers they use more expressions abouttheir reactions <strong>to</strong> objects <strong>and</strong> events(Marcos 1995),indicating the ways inwhich, by the age of two, children will uselanguage differently with different people<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> achieve various effects.Marian Whitehead (1996) explains howfirst words are often only meaningful <strong>to</strong>those familiar with the particular child’slife <strong>and</strong> likes or dislikes, <strong>and</strong> within thecontexts in which they are used. Alonein the car with Coralie, then 12 months,her gr<strong>and</strong>mother could not underst<strong>and</strong>her repeated utterances of ‘Medic, medic’until Coralie pointed <strong>to</strong> the car’s cassetteplayer <strong>and</strong>, with much joint laughter,‘SillyNana’ turned on appropriate music.Whitehead’s (1996: 12) explanationscontinue with a helpful outline of the‘golden age of grammar’ <strong>and</strong> how childrenbetween two <strong>and</strong> four years will ‘attempt<strong>to</strong> tidy up irregular plurals’, regulariseirregular past tenses, <strong>and</strong> create newwords. Further, she points out that‘By the age of four the phys i cal mat u ri ty ofthe nervous sys tem <strong>and</strong> the finer muscleco nt rol over the mouth, t h ro at <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> n g u e,<strong>and</strong> even the pre s e n ce of te e t h ,m a ke theyoung child’s pro n u n c i ation of languagesve ry much closer <strong>to</strong> the adult fo rms <strong>and</strong>easier <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>.’ Wh i tehead (1996: 13 ).Further research which once again drawsattention <strong>to</strong> the holistic <strong>and</strong> interwovennature of early development <strong>and</strong> learning,by Hirose <strong>and</strong> Barnard (1997),providesevidence of a link between baby boys’cognitive development (measured on theBayley MDI scale) <strong>and</strong> the amount of jointverbal interactions they experienced withtheir mothers. This finding was part of astudy examining the potential differencesbetween depressed <strong>and</strong> non-depressedmothers <strong>and</strong> their babies. The group ofdepressed mothers talked more with girlthan with boy babies, whereas in thecontrol group there were no suchdifferences. Murray et al (1993) also foundrelationships between the quality ofmaternal communication with their babies72 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


in the first 18 months. Depressed motherswere less focused on their babies’experience <strong>and</strong> less likely <strong>to</strong> encourageor even acknowledge the child’s agency(growing personhood <strong>and</strong> attempts <strong>to</strong> beindependent, develop a self-concept – seechapters 3 <strong>and</strong> 6) than non-depressedmothers. Again,the quality of the mother’sinteractions was found <strong>to</strong> impact oncognitive development. Work by Molfeseet al (1996) found associations betweenperinatal risk (see chapter 6 section onpoverty) <strong>and</strong> language development,which have implications for the use ofmaterials for testing very young children’sprogress. They argue that professionalsshould check scores on a variety ofindividual sections of assessment scalesrather than the final summed predic<strong>to</strong>rsonly. What is particularly important aboutthis issue is the fact that in general, boys’language development is slower than tha<strong>to</strong>f girls (Hutt 1972). This is said <strong>to</strong> relate <strong>to</strong>the relative immaturity of the male centralnervous system at birth <strong>and</strong> it hasconsequences for both girls <strong>and</strong> boys.Firstly, boys may be assessed as beingless able, when in fact they may beconcentrating on other modes ofexploration <strong>and</strong> discovery; it may alsomean that adults provide girls withexplanations for events <strong>and</strong> phenomena,instead of encouraging them <strong>to</strong> explorefirst h<strong>and</strong> through play.The component of the Framework headedFinding a Voice provides the following keydevelopmental guidelines:-Young babies communicate in a varietyof ways including crying, gurgling,babbling <strong>and</strong> squealing.Babies enjoy experimenting, exploring<strong>and</strong> using sounds <strong>and</strong> words <strong>to</strong>represent objects around them.Young children use single word <strong>and</strong>two word utterances <strong>to</strong> convey simple<strong>and</strong> more complex messages.Children use language as a powerfulmeans of widening contacts, sharingfeelings, experiences <strong>and</strong> thoughts.BILINGUALISMAccording <strong>to</strong> Siraj-Blatchford <strong>and</strong> Clarke’s(2000:30) review of research onbilingualism, there are many advantages<strong>to</strong> being exposed <strong>to</strong> more than onelanguage from birth. These include:-‘self-esteem, positive identity <strong>and</strong> attitudes<strong>to</strong>wards language learning, cognitiveflexibility, increased problem-solving<strong>and</strong> a greater metalinguistic awareness.’Further, these researchers tell us thatwhere children are ‘sequentially’ bilingual(learn their second language later, sayat a nursery) it is important they haveopportunities <strong>to</strong> carry on developing theirhome language because it is the strengthof this that provides the basis for theadditional language.In her highly informative chapter on‘Young bilinguals’ Whitehead (1996)synthesises research findings withprofessional <strong>and</strong> personal experience <strong>to</strong>provide information about bilingualism73


with pointers for practice. She states thatlanguage mixing in the early years is notan indica<strong>to</strong>r of ‘muddle <strong>and</strong> inadequacy’but of children making choices about how<strong>to</strong> express themselves most fluently. Shecontinues: ‘It is absolutely essential thatearly years carers <strong>and</strong> educa<strong>to</strong>rs respectthe languages of the young children forwhom they are responsible. This must godeeper than vague goodwill or <strong>to</strong>lerance.’(Whitehead 1996:21)It is also important for babies <strong>and</strong> children<strong>to</strong> feel their home languages are valued<strong>and</strong> reflected in their ECEC settings, <strong>and</strong>that their ties with family members arepromoted. Linda Thompson (1999) foundthat none of the bilingual childreninvolved in her study in the North East ofEngl<strong>and</strong>, were using Mirpuri-Panjabi,theirfirst language, at nursery by the time theyhad completed their first term. This is apity because we know that young children(<strong>and</strong> many adults) think in their firstlanguage <strong>and</strong> they need <strong>to</strong> use this in theearly years as they explore <strong>and</strong> learn aboutthe people <strong>and</strong> world around them mosteffectively. Clarke (1999) <strong>and</strong> Parke <strong>and</strong>Drury (2001) emphasise the need fornurseries <strong>to</strong> provide some languageteaching (that is, <strong>to</strong> plan some interactionswith young bilingual children <strong>and</strong>intervene in play <strong>to</strong> effect these) ratherthan simply relying on the ad hocinteractions the children may have in theadditional language.SIGNING AS FIRST LANGUAGEBabies born with a severe hearing loss willuse the part of their brain used forlanguage in hearing babies <strong>and</strong> adults <strong>to</strong>extend the part generally used for vision(Karmiloff-Smith 1994). Similarly,a congenitally blind baby will devote agreater part of the brain <strong>to</strong> <strong>to</strong>uch, the partfor sight being reduced. In this way eachof them develops a more sophisticatedability in the senses which are strong.Karmiloff-Smith (1994:50) adds‘In both cases…it is language thatis being processed. That is the commondenomina<strong>to</strong>r. The fact that in onecase sensory input from the ears isprocessed <strong>and</strong> in the other sensoryinput from the eyes attests <strong>to</strong> the brain’sextraordinary plasticity.’At around 12 months, when hearingbabies’ babble begins <strong>to</strong> sound likesentences, deaf babies will s<strong>to</strong>p babbling.In homes where parents <strong>and</strong> carers sign,they do so in a signing type of ‘motherese’,making more exaggerated actions for thebaby <strong>to</strong> decifer <strong>and</strong> learn. Deaf childrenwhose parents use signing as their firstlanguage at home, go through all thesame stages as hearing children usingspoken language. In this way ‘deaf childrenacquire their native language as naturallyas any hearing child learns <strong>to</strong> speak.Likewise, hearing children of one deafparent <strong>and</strong> one hearing parent grow upbilingual,in much the same way <strong>and</strong> withthe same ease as children learning twonative spoken languages’ (Karmiloff <strong>and</strong>74 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Karmiloff-Smith 2001: 183). Further,research has shown how a child learningsign language begins <strong>to</strong> move from singlesigns <strong>to</strong> combinations at about the sametime hearing children begin <strong>to</strong> usecombinations of words (Barrett 1999).Although a co nt roversial view, Ho rn by et al( 1997) argue that subsequent deve l o p m e ntshould include ex po s u re <strong>to</strong> sign language.The impo rt a n ce of this <strong>to</strong> developing thel a n g u a g e, rather than spe e c h ,of a seve re ly orp ro fo u n d ly deaf child has been establishedin the last <strong>three</strong> deca d e s.These re s e a rc h e r ssuggest that failure <strong>to</strong> int rod u ce sign aspart of a <strong>to</strong>tal communication approachcan co m p romise the future ability of a childwith a hearing impairm e nt <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>the deeper meaning of language, <strong>to</strong> re a d<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> wri te co m pe te nt ly.Coupe <strong>and</strong> Goldbart (1988) <strong>and</strong> Karmiloff<strong>and</strong> Karmiloff-Smith (2001) provide usefulaccounts of other types of youngchildren’s language delays, which familiesmay encounter.What we must recognise is the amazingpotential of young children <strong>to</strong> overcomesensory impairments <strong>and</strong>, withappropriate scaffolding by parents,gr<strong>and</strong>parents <strong>and</strong> practitioners, <strong>to</strong> makesense of their worlds.Sean was still only two when he recounted<strong>to</strong> his gr<strong>and</strong>father that he had been makingcakes with his gr<strong>and</strong>mother. The detail of hisaccount indicated not only his ability <strong>to</strong>remember the sequence <strong>and</strong> process of thebaking, but <strong>to</strong> enjoy a joke by signing thatthey had used flour not flower.LISTENING AND RESPONDINGIf, as Brit<strong>to</strong>n (1970) claimed, languagelearning begins with listening, thenchildren who are surrounded by talk <strong>and</strong>engaged in talk interchanges are in anideal position <strong>to</strong> develop as languageusers. The utterances they make, however,may not necessarily be matched in time<strong>and</strong> context <strong>to</strong> what has been heard.Indeed, as children develop <strong>and</strong> overhearlanguage used in different contexts theyare s<strong>to</strong>ring words <strong>to</strong> fit with experiences<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> use in their own time. Children aremost likely <strong>to</strong> learn words when ‘they arenot pressed, or otherwise expected <strong>to</strong>reply, as in a conversation’ (Bloom et al1996:3171). Difficulties arise when childrenare directed <strong>to</strong> answer questions orrespond at a particular time when oftenthe making <strong>and</strong> shaping of a responsethen restricts the language used. Vygotskyclaims that development <strong>and</strong> instructionhave different ‘rhythms’ although they maybe closely connected (Vygotsky 1978) <strong>and</strong>from their research Bloom <strong>and</strong> colleaguesdiscovered that the ‘language a childbrings <strong>to</strong> a conversation is languagealready learned from what has been heard<strong>and</strong> overheard in a multitude of actions<strong>and</strong> interactions that have come before’(Bloom et al 1996: 3171).The implicationof these findings for adults is that theyneed <strong>to</strong> be good listeners <strong>to</strong>o –75


‘Whether it involves children, babies oradults, interpersonal communication is atwo way process…Listening <strong>to</strong> childrenshows our respect for them <strong>and</strong> buildstheir self esteem.’ Petrie (1997:25).In Ruth Weir’s seminal study of ‘crib talk’she gathered evidence of sound play inchildren’s pre-sleep monologues ( Weir1962) <strong>and</strong> in Meek’s work replicating thiswith her students she discovered that veryyoung children were experimenting within<strong>to</strong>nation in such pre-sleep talk,when‘variations of <strong>to</strong>ne, pitch <strong>and</strong> stress weregreatly exaggerated as they practised <strong>and</strong>repeated segments of language…pushingthe <strong>to</strong>nal range ever farther as they did so’(Meek 1985: 45). Of course suchmonologues can only occur if language isfirst modelled <strong>and</strong> heard, <strong>and</strong> thengathered <strong>and</strong> s<strong>to</strong>red, <strong>to</strong> be played with<strong>and</strong> rehearsed in safe spaces. Fen<strong>to</strong>n <strong>and</strong>Martello’s close examination of bath timetalk offers similar explora<strong>to</strong>ryopportunities when,as well as functionalbath time business, talk occurs, so <strong>to</strong>o doesplay, playful interachange, teasing <strong>and</strong>joking between siblings <strong>and</strong>/or adults(Fen<strong>to</strong>n <strong>and</strong> Martello 1996).Ste rn maintained that by the time thatc h i l d ren reach the age of about ninem o nths the beginning of jokes <strong>and</strong> te a s i n goccur <strong>and</strong> from 15 months onwa rds play<strong>and</strong> language be come possible (Ste rn1985 ; see also Dunn 1988 ).Ch i l d ren havebegun by this age <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> some ofthe social inte ra ctions that are oc c u rri n ga round them <strong>and</strong>, as they be co m e‘wo rd s m i t h s’ ( Meek 1985) so then do theybegin also <strong>to</strong> take pleasure in nonsense,using <strong>and</strong> subve rting sounds <strong>to</strong> make fun,u pend co nve ntion <strong>and</strong> provide humour fo rt h e m s e lves <strong>and</strong> willing adult audience s<strong>and</strong> part i c i p a<strong>to</strong>rs (Grainger <strong>and</strong> Goo u c h19 99 ). In discussion of Michael Ro s e n’swo rk with children <strong>and</strong> poe t ry, Meek vo i ce sRo s e n’s claim that ‘c h i l d re n ,d i s cove ri n glanguage <strong>and</strong> playing with it, meet itsp hys i cal nat u re be fo re its sense’ ( Me e k2000 :203). Ce rt a i n ly the rhythm <strong>and</strong> rhy m et h at ve ry young children delight in wo u l dbe te s t i m o ny <strong>to</strong> this, <strong>and</strong> nursery rhy m e s,s kipping rhymes <strong>and</strong> playg round songs<strong>and</strong> games are still ve ry much in ev i d e n ce,<strong>and</strong> often co n s i s te nt in co nte nt ove rg e n e rat i o n s. ( Opies 1959 ; Grugeon 2001 ).When cousins Richard (4) <strong>and</strong> Ceris (5) wereurged by their beloved gr<strong>and</strong>mother <strong>to</strong> pu<strong>to</strong>n some ‘woollies’ against the cold, theirdelight in this newfound <strong>and</strong> enjoyablesounding word (their parents called themsweaters <strong>and</strong> cardigans) was displayed in ajoint romp with much laughter <strong>and</strong> fallingabout, accompanied by repetitions of‘Woolly, woolly, woolly’. Davies (2002) writesof similar examples of <strong>to</strong>ddlers using dancelikemovements accompanying the relishing<strong>and</strong> rhythms of newfound words.The rich range of sounds, l a n g u a g ep at te rn s, d ramas <strong>and</strong> tunes of family lifefo rm the fodder on which the curiositiesof babies <strong>and</strong> young children feed as theybegin their language journ eys. An d,if vision develops in a changing wo rldof colours <strong>and</strong> text u res (Greenfield 2000),76 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


then language grows in a safe <strong>and</strong> familiars p a ce of wo rd s, co nve r s at i o n s, rhymes<strong>and</strong> songs.The key developmental notes includedin the Framework ‘<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong> Three Matters’for Listening & Responding are:-Long before young babies cancommunicate verbally, they listen <strong>to</strong>,distinguish <strong>and</strong> respond <strong>to</strong> in<strong>to</strong>nationsin adults’ voices.In a familiar context, with a key person,babies can underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> respond<strong>to</strong> the different things said <strong>to</strong> them.Young children are able <strong>to</strong> respond<strong>to</strong> simple requests <strong>and</strong> grasp meaningfrom context.Children learn new words very rapidly<strong>and</strong> are able <strong>to</strong> use them in talkingabout <strong>matters</strong> which interest them.So parents <strong>and</strong> practitioners need time<strong>to</strong> listen <strong>to</strong> <strong>and</strong> enjoy the sounds <strong>and</strong>language of babies <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ddlers whenthey are secure <strong>and</strong> comfortable, perhapsin their cots/beds, or playing in a familiarsetting. Throughout these early years,adults need <strong>to</strong> engage in rhythmic songs,dances <strong>and</strong> finger plays, share picturebooks, especially those with repetitive,lyrical words. They also need <strong>to</strong> show they<strong>to</strong>o relish particular words <strong>and</strong> phrases,sometimes using h<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> armmovements, as well as facial expressions<strong>and</strong> expressive in<strong>to</strong>nation, <strong>to</strong> help childrenunderst<strong>and</strong> meanings.‘Children are enthusiastic <strong>to</strong> struggle <strong>to</strong>make meaning of adults’ communications<strong>and</strong> they need <strong>to</strong> encounter adults whoare equally enthusiastic <strong>to</strong> make meaningof their communication.’ Elfer et al (2002draft version: 15).MAKING MEANING‘Social realities are not bricks that we tripover or bruise ourselves on when we kickat them,but the meanings that we achieveby the sharing of human cognitions.’(Bruner 1986:122)The close physical <strong>and</strong> emotional bondthat exists between mother <strong>and</strong> child inthe earliest days of a baby’s life enable heroften <strong>to</strong> interpret the nature of the baby’scry, its purpose <strong>and</strong> need (see chapter 3).As the relationship develops <strong>and</strong> feeding<strong>and</strong> sleeping routines are negotiated <strong>and</strong>patterned, each learns something aboutthe roles <strong>and</strong> rituals involved in family life<strong>and</strong> the baby is initiated in<strong>to</strong> family culture(Bruner 1986). Indeed, from birth, Sterntells us that babies often occupy a statecalled ‘alert inactivity’ when they are quietbut taking in events around them (Stern1985:39). The first recorded smile of a babyis always an event of celebration <strong>and</strong> joy<strong>and</strong> although it is claimed that the firstsocial smiling emerges by two monthsof age (Rochat et al 1999) smiles <strong>and</strong>open-mouthed responses have beenanecdotally recorded before that age.However, the smile is a signpost,at whatever age, that the baby is now able77


<strong>to</strong> engage in a new way as ‘when infantsstart <strong>to</strong> smile in response <strong>to</strong> socialstimulation, there is an apparent changein the way they relate <strong>to</strong> the world’ <strong>and</strong>the nature of the adult response becomessensitive <strong>to</strong> the appearance of smilesas ‘on the basis of the expression ofreciprocal pleasure, interactions are finetuned’(Rochat et al 1999: 950).Thus,smiling seems <strong>to</strong> signal the beginningof identifiable <strong>and</strong> intentional meaningmaking. The second physical developmentthat appears <strong>to</strong> change the position of thebaby from being a recipient or secondpartner in meaning making in<strong>to</strong> an activeconstruc<strong>to</strong>r <strong>and</strong> communica<strong>to</strong>r ofmeaning is gesture. Reaching <strong>to</strong>wardsfaces or moving objects occurs in the earlymonths before a baby can position itselfby ‘crawling or cruising’ in order <strong>to</strong> exploreor capture objects or people or discoveralternative perspectives (Stern 1985).In fact it is possible <strong>to</strong> claim from the workof Stern <strong>and</strong> others that pointing mayoriginate from reaching <strong>and</strong> so acommunicative gesture is created.The development of language, a symbolicmethod of referring <strong>and</strong> the way in whichwe are distinct from other animals, is amiles<strong>to</strong>ne in life as ‘the child learns how<strong>to</strong> mean’ within the family context (Bruner1986: 113) <strong>and</strong> the process of‘enculturation’ really begins. Given theopportunities, children under <strong>three</strong> willalso use other ways of expressingthemselves through movement <strong>and</strong>dance, singing <strong>and</strong> other expressive arts(Davies 2002; Duffy 1998; Edwards et al1998;<strong>and</strong> see chapter 5). But it is mainlythrough language that a young child isenabled <strong>to</strong> begin <strong>to</strong> construct the s<strong>to</strong>ry,or narrative, of her/his own life (Stern 1985)<strong>and</strong> such narratives, constructions <strong>and</strong>reconstructions help children <strong>to</strong> developtheir sense of self, their sense of the socialworlds around them <strong>and</strong> their place withinthese worlds (Bruner 1986). As well as this,we learn how <strong>to</strong> ‘behave linguistically’(Halliday 1978) from the audi<strong>to</strong>ryprocessing that occurs before birth <strong>and</strong>continues through life.Thus, learning how <strong>to</strong> mean <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> makemeaning draws on this specialisedknowledge, developed in the womb <strong>and</strong>progressing as the child ‘processes thesounds, rhythms <strong>and</strong> basic building blocksof the words <strong>and</strong> grammar of his mother<strong>to</strong>ngue’ (Karmiloff & Karmiloff-Smith 2001:vii). In this respect Karmiloff <strong>and</strong> Karmiloff-Smith stress the difference betweenspeech <strong>and</strong> language. Babies are activelyprocessing sounds they hear a long timebefore they underst<strong>and</strong> words orgrammar. They are becoming sensitive <strong>to</strong>key aspects of the language/s they heararound them. As Karmiloff <strong>and</strong> Karmiloff-Smith (2001) explain, research has shownthat the capacity for speech perception ofa foetus, newborn, or very young infant isimpressive. But we should not confusespeech perception with language.Sometimes speech discriminationcapacities in babies have led theorists<strong>to</strong> conclude that humans are born with78 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


‘language’. However, there is much more<strong>to</strong> language learning than the ability <strong>to</strong>appropriately segment a stream of speech.Traditionally, from the work of Bruner(1986) <strong>and</strong> Wells (1986) <strong>and</strong> others,we have come <strong>to</strong> see parents, carersor adults as leaders <strong>and</strong> scaffolders oflanguage use <strong>and</strong> conversation. However,the research of Bloom <strong>and</strong> colleagues,when observing <strong>and</strong> recording childrenfrom nine months onwards, indicates thatchildren are often the initia<strong>to</strong>rs, leaders<strong>and</strong> direc<strong>to</strong>rs of talk events <strong>and</strong>conversations, determining <strong>and</strong> shapingthe content, length <strong>and</strong> nature of the talk.They say that ‘both child <strong>and</strong> mothercontributed <strong>to</strong> the architecture of the earlyconversations between them, but in factit was the children who were “in charge”’.(Bloom et al 1996: 3171; our italics). Thoseof us who have either lived or worked withbabies <strong>and</strong> young children would supportthis view from experience of often beingled in<strong>to</strong> conversations, or indeed corners,by young developing conversationalists,often in public spaces! And asopportunities for satisfying curiosity arise,the questions of young children,as developing philosophers, are oftenunanswerable, for example ‘Where wereyou when the sun was built?’ (Coles 1996).The child as ‘direc<strong>to</strong>r’ of play orconversation is only possible if a sensitiverelationship is evident between the child<strong>and</strong> adult. Singer claims that the childlearns <strong>to</strong> ‘read’ her/his mother’s face <strong>and</strong>that equally ‘the mother, as the moreexperienced other, attributes certainmeanings <strong>to</strong> the infant’s diffusemovements. She mirrors <strong>and</strong> imitates herchild. She interprets her child’s movementsas communication cues that suggest somejoint course of action’ (Singer 2001:2). Thisreciprocity, or mirrored language, actions,gestures or expressions, develops ably in<strong>to</strong>shared underst<strong>and</strong>ings as games, play,teasing <strong>and</strong> word play become part ofdaily interaction.Play of course soon becomes thecontextual space where meaning is made<strong>and</strong> negotiated as children develop waysof interacting with <strong>to</strong>ys, space <strong>and</strong> ‘others’<strong>to</strong> construct <strong>and</strong> reconstruct worlds.As children attempt <strong>to</strong> make sense of theirlives through play, s<strong>to</strong>ries are <strong>to</strong>ld,narratives are constructed (see chapter 3).Rosen claims that ‘we can locate narrativein human actions <strong>and</strong> the events whichsurround them <strong>and</strong> in our capacity <strong>to</strong>perceive the world as consisting of actions<strong>and</strong> events sequentially ordered (Rosen1988: 13) <strong>and</strong> as children recreate the rolesof those around them such narratives areconstructed in often the simplest forms,using basic resources <strong>and</strong> barely indicativelanguage <strong>and</strong> gesture. Meek offers theview that ‘in their own versions of s<strong>to</strong>rieschildren explore intellectually the natureof their own situation – childhood…<strong>and</strong>as they learn <strong>to</strong> become both the teller<strong>and</strong> the <strong>to</strong>ld, they are also learning <strong>to</strong>dialogue with their futures’ (Meek 1998:118). These lessons are best learned fromthe s<strong>to</strong>ries heard, shared, discussed <strong>and</strong>loved in the company of parents, adults<strong>and</strong> others who are prepared <strong>to</strong> suspend79


eality <strong>and</strong> join with children in riskyjourneys of the imagination, <strong>to</strong> l<strong>and</strong> alwayssafely, like Max in Sendak‘s classic tale,Where the Wild Things Are, back home,where his supper is waiting <strong>and</strong> still hot!It is also important <strong>to</strong> be aware that ‘inplay (children) can say all they know in anyway they like’ (Meek 1985:49) <strong>and</strong> oftenthis means that the boundaries ofconvention <strong>and</strong> appropriateness are oftenchallenged <strong>and</strong> extended, in terms of bothlanguage <strong>and</strong> physical behaviour. Theboundaries in play contexts are, by thevery nature of play, broad <strong>and</strong> fluid,although rules may exist, linguistic orphysical, <strong>and</strong> are defined <strong>and</strong> negotiatedby the players, <strong>and</strong> often renegotiatedduring the drama of play. The very earlycommunications between adults <strong>and</strong>babies that often include turn taking <strong>and</strong>the beginnings of conversation may beimportant in defining appropriateness<strong>and</strong>, later, the development of ‘earlymorality’ as very often ‘as a result of face<strong>to</strong>-faceturn-taking behaviour with caregivers, infants learn rules for reciprocity,for give <strong>and</strong> take, <strong>to</strong>gether with the strongmotive for using these rules: “<strong>to</strong>gether”is so pleasurable’ (Singer 2001: 4). Singermaintains that such behaviour is ‘basedon a strong motivation <strong>to</strong> share <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>connect, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> construct shared rules’(Singer 2001: 4).It is important <strong>to</strong> remember, however,as we discuss in chapter 5, that this kindof involvement in joint play, which requiressharing imagination <strong>and</strong> communicatingfreely, is extremely limited by the cognitiveprocesses of children with autism (Hobson1993; Trevarthen et al 1998).The role of siblings in supporting babies<strong>and</strong> young children in developing <strong>and</strong>shaping meaning is significant (Dunn1984; 1993). In her research (although witholder children, still important in thiscontext), Gregory describes the potentialfor ‘synergy’ between siblings as they play<strong>to</strong>gether with younger children imitating,repeating, echoing, listening, challenging,etc; but older children are also learningthrough ‘practising consciously what theyknow <strong>and</strong> through translating officialmeanings in<strong>to</strong> personal sense <strong>and</strong> viceversa for the younger child’ (Gregory 2001:313).The relationships here are describedas generally equal <strong>and</strong> the mutuality of thelearning opportunities is celebrated. This‘personal sense’ develops in families,communities <strong>and</strong> cultures <strong>and</strong> it isthrough developing such knowledge <strong>and</strong>knowledge of language, from ‘exposure <strong>to</strong>cues <strong>and</strong> models’ from a range of ‘moreknowledgeable others’ that young childrenlearn how <strong>to</strong> mean,learn how <strong>to</strong> makesense of the world (Bruner 1986).MAKING MEANING AND PRINT LITER ACY WITHUNDER THREESDuring the years before they are five or six,children in Engl<strong>and</strong> are gathering <strong>and</strong>making sense of information about theliteracy-dependent society in which theylive (David et al 2000). Campbell (1999)provides a powerful,in-depth account80 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


of his gr<strong>and</strong>daughter Alice’s very earlyexperiences with print literacy. Alice’smutual enjoyment of books with herparents <strong>and</strong> gr<strong>and</strong>parents displays auniqueness about the path a child takes<strong>to</strong> becoming an effective language user<strong>and</strong> sharer in lite rate aspe cts of eve ryd aylife. During these years, the mostimportant aspects of engaging youngchildren in activities involving literacyneed <strong>to</strong> be aimed at enabling them <strong>to</strong>acquire the ‘big picture’, <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>h ow lite ra cy wo rk s, h ow books wo rk ,e tc.<strong>and</strong> not <strong>to</strong> put them in<strong>to</strong> meaninglesssituations where they are expected <strong>to</strong>decode symbols, other than their ownmark-making when they wish <strong>to</strong> conveymessages (David et al 2000),(see chapter5 ). As Ma c ro ry (2001) arg u e s, p ra ct i t i o n e r sneed <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> about individualvariation in language acquisition <strong>and</strong> nots i m p ly the genera l i s ations from larg e - s ca l eresearch. By observing young children atplay, practitioners can listen <strong>and</strong> noteindividual differences. Further, Macroryemphasises that young children need <strong>to</strong>play rather than <strong>to</strong> be made <strong>to</strong> engage in<strong>to</strong>o great or <strong>to</strong>o early a focus on formall i te ra cy act i v i ty, adding that fo rmal lite ra cyinstruction may be <strong>to</strong> the detriment oftheir development of spoken language.Apart from wasting the children’s time ifthey do not underst<strong>and</strong> what is going on,the language adults use <strong>to</strong> discuss lite ra cyis sometimes beyond the experience ofsuch young children <strong>and</strong> can simply leavethem feeling inadequate. Adults willfrequently focus on their own questions<strong>and</strong> meanings rather than recognising thesense in a child’s response. This sense inresponding is well illustrated by Coralie, agedtwo, looking at a book with her mother. Nearthe picture of an apple a large ‘a’ had beenprinted. She asked about the ‘a’ <strong>and</strong> so hermother tried <strong>to</strong> explain. As she ended theexplanation Coralie’s mother asked, ‘So ifapple begins with ‘a’ (sounded),what doesorange begin with?’ To which Coralie replied‘Peeling.’ A brilliant, logical answer <strong>to</strong> a‘question <strong>to</strong>o far’.Prog rammes devised by re s e a rchers suchas Wade <strong>and</strong> Moore (1993;1996; 2000)<strong>and</strong> Hannon et al (1991) focus on theloving, sharing of books as sources ofi n s p i ration <strong>and</strong> enjoy m e nt from ve ry earlyin life. Their studies have highlighted thecrucial role of the adults – parents <strong>and</strong>ca rers – in developing childre n’s abilitiescoincidentally while enjoying books<strong>to</strong>gether. Wade <strong>and</strong> Moore followedup the participants in their Bookstartprogramme, in which parents weresupplied with <strong>and</strong> encouraged <strong>to</strong> enjoybooks with their ve ry young childre n ,t h ey found that the children invo lvedin the pro j e ct had sco res in both Ba s e l i n eAs s e s s m e nts <strong>and</strong> Key Stage 1 assessment swhich were superior <strong>to</strong> those of theirmatched controls who had not beenenrolled in the Bookstart scheme. Thec h i l d ren outpe rfo rmed their peers in no<strong>to</strong> n ly English but also in Mat h e m atics <strong>and</strong>Science. Wade <strong>and</strong> Moore suggest thatsharing books at home in the early yearsmay contribute <strong>to</strong> children’s ability <strong>to</strong>81


attend <strong>and</strong> concentrate. The evaluationof the PEEP (Peers Early EducationPartnership) programme, another projectwhich encourages parents <strong>to</strong> share bookswith their children from a very early age,is already showing differences in thereading abilities of the children at school,following their own <strong>and</strong> their parents’involvement in the project (Evangelou<strong>and</strong> Sylva 2002).Nowadays young children ‘make sense’of their print dependent society throughmeaning making involving a range ofmedia (Westbrook 1999). Sam (one ofpremature twin boys aged 15 months) wasenthralled <strong>to</strong> be dressed in his older brother’s‘h<strong>and</strong>-me-down’ Postman Pat pyjamasinstead of a baby’s sleep suit. For the nextfew days he re pe a te d ly brought Postman Pa tbooks from among their collection <strong>to</strong> hismother, <strong>to</strong> delight in <strong>and</strong> share the pictures<strong>and</strong> have her tell the s<strong>to</strong>ries (abridgedversions of the text where the print proved<strong>to</strong>o long),<strong>and</strong> he wanted <strong>to</strong> watch theirPostman Pat video.SUMMARY OF KEY ‘MESSAGES’Human beings communicate in numerousways. Language is said <strong>to</strong> form only a smallpercentage of interaction. Children arekeen observers of all forms ofcommunication, both in order <strong>to</strong> makesense of what is going on <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> be able<strong>to</strong> participate. Practitioners need <strong>to</strong> reflec<strong>to</strong>n their observations of the children’scommunications <strong>and</strong> their own ability <strong>to</strong>make these possible for all children, <strong>and</strong>particularly those who might be silencedbecause they are not being encouraged <strong>to</strong>use their first language or because theyhave a hearing loss.The Framework Component MakingMeaning provides the following essentialpoints about how babies <strong>and</strong> childrenstrive <strong>to</strong> convey <strong>and</strong> interpret meaningsas skilful communica<strong>to</strong>rs.From the very beginning of life, youngbabies convey messages about whatthey want <strong>and</strong> need, as well as howthey feel.Babies learn that their voice <strong>and</strong> actionshave effects on others <strong>and</strong> they strive <strong>to</strong>share meanings.Young children use actions <strong>and</strong> words<strong>to</strong> make <strong>and</strong> justify choices <strong>and</strong>influence the behaviour <strong>and</strong> responsesof others.As vocabulary increases, children makesense of the world through bargaining,negotiating, questioning, describing<strong>and</strong> labelling.Thus it is during these first <strong>three</strong> years thatyoung children begin <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> thatlanguage is not just about being able <strong>to</strong>use words but how <strong>and</strong> when <strong>to</strong> use them,because they have also come <strong>to</strong> know a lotabout the culture in which their family lifeis embedded <strong>and</strong> in which each of themlives. According <strong>to</strong> Bruner (1983) youngchildren underst<strong>and</strong> culturally appropriaterequests, invitations, <strong>and</strong> linguistic forms –learning how <strong>to</strong> get things done <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> beaccepted in that particular culture.82 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


For example, Eliot (just two years old) wasable <strong>to</strong> respond <strong>to</strong> his gr<strong>and</strong>father’s directivenot <strong>to</strong> kill a small spider with ‘I was onlygoing <strong>to</strong> give it a cuddle’, thus not onlyproviding his gr<strong>and</strong>father with anacceptable response, but attempting <strong>to</strong>ingratiate himself <strong>to</strong>o.Similarly, at just <strong>three</strong>, Ché <strong>and</strong> Cymion,who live in Australia, already unders<strong>to</strong>odhow <strong>to</strong> reason <strong>and</strong> negotiate inconversation,they also show, in thissequence, incredible insight <strong>and</strong>mindreading abilities, because theirmother does not like cats:-Ché: you like cats?Mum: sometimes…Cymion: sometimes you not like cats?Ché: just tie a long rope cat can walklong way.Cymion: close gate… cat cat no go throughgate cat cat safe.Mum: yes, but what about the pool? Whatif it fell in the p ool <strong>and</strong> can’t swim?Cymion: me jump in, cat go down <strong>and</strong> down<strong>and</strong> down water, me get cat, catalright. Daddy get me, me alright.Me go down <strong>and</strong> down <strong>and</strong> downme alrightChé: Cymi alright? Daddy get Cymi?Mum: yes, Cymi was fine, he was safe. Butwhat about the cat?Ché: me get arm b<strong>and</strong>s for cat, catarmb<strong>and</strong>s.Mum:well, we couldn’t have a cat hereMum:well yes, but I don’t think cats wearbecause of the road, could we?arm b<strong>and</strong>s…Cymion: cat die, cat get run ove r, cars go ve ryfast…cat…Cats no know, cats, umcars go very, very very fast. Cats dieChé: mummy, mummy get long rope umum tie cat up…Can’t can’t go road.Cymion: dogs can swim?Mum: yes, I think dogs can swim…Ché: you like dogs?Mum:well yes, but cats like <strong>to</strong> walkaround, it’s not fair <strong>to</strong> tie him up,is it?83


The part of babies’ brains engaged informing connections in response <strong>to</strong>sounds (the audi<strong>to</strong>ry cortex) is moreactive than it will be at any other timethroughout life (Karmiloff <strong>and</strong> Karmiloff-Smith 2001). By the end of that first yearthey are able <strong>to</strong> make gestures indicatingtheir needs, take turns in conversations,recognise <strong>and</strong> use the language soundst h ey hear around them <strong>and</strong> assoc i ate facialexpressions with talk (Makin et al 1995).Dunn (1988) argues that this links withchildren’s grasp, between 12 <strong>and</strong> 36months, of what is <strong>and</strong> is not culturallyacceptable behaviour <strong>and</strong> speech but asthe research reported in this chaptershows, experiences of hearing language<strong>and</strong> being involved in interactions as ifone is a person who underst<strong>and</strong>s <strong>and</strong> canrespond during the first year of life is thecrucial foundation of the process.84 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Chapter 5A Competent LearnerA Competent Learner, one of the Aspects of the Framework ‘<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong> Three Matters’ isconcerned with babies’ <strong>and</strong> young children’s ability <strong>to</strong> make connections (for example,through the senses) <strong>and</strong> develop the ability <strong>to</strong> compare, categorise <strong>and</strong> classify; <strong>to</strong> beimaginative <strong>and</strong> creative;<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> be able <strong>to</strong> use symbols <strong>to</strong> represent thoughts <strong>and</strong>language. Babies only a few hours old gaze at patterns which resemble the human facein preference <strong>to</strong> r<strong>and</strong>om patterns. This shows that they are able <strong>to</strong> distinguish betweenthings, <strong>and</strong> that they appear <strong>to</strong> come in<strong>to</strong> the world ‘programmed’ <strong>to</strong> have preferences forhuman faces <strong>and</strong> human beings <strong>and</strong> movements. They search out patterns. In this wayyoung children learn <strong>to</strong> discriminate <strong>and</strong> make connections between different objects <strong>and</strong>experiences. As connections are made, the child makes increasing sense of the world.As babies explore the world through <strong>to</strong>uch,sight, sound, taste, smell <strong>and</strong> movement, theirsensory <strong>and</strong> physical explorations affect the patterns that are laid down in the brain.Through repeated experience of people, objects <strong>and</strong> materials, young children begin<strong>to</strong> form mental images which lead them <strong>to</strong> imitate, explore <strong>and</strong> re-enact as they becomeimaginative <strong>and</strong> creative. Creativity, imagination <strong>and</strong> representation through mark makingallows children <strong>to</strong> share their thoughts, feelings, underst<strong>and</strong>ings <strong>and</strong> identities with others,using drawings, words, movement, music, dance <strong>and</strong> imaginative play.MAKING CONNECTIONS‘Over the past 20 years or so a growingbody of data on early perceptualcompetence has forced a change in theprevalent image of the young infant…the image of the competent infant hasemerged, with even newborns creditedwith objective awareness of theirsurroundings.’ (Bremner 1998:239).As we have already stated, when babiesfirst enter the world the things that attractthem most are the human voices, faces –the pattern of two eyes, stripes, edges <strong>and</strong>movement. In particular, between the ages85


of <strong>three</strong> <strong>and</strong> six months, babies showa growing interest in the world aroundthem as they play <strong>and</strong> above all they areinterested in other people. Their eyesighthas become more coordinated, so thebaby is able <strong>to</strong> focus on people <strong>and</strong>objects that are nearer or further awaythan the earlier short distance whichoriented them <strong>to</strong> being in focus whenlooking in<strong>to</strong> the face of an adult carryingthem. Now a baby will be seen <strong>to</strong> scanthe surroundings <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> use fingers <strong>and</strong>mouth <strong>to</strong> explore objects. Stern (1974)suggested that at this age babies enjoybeing drawn in<strong>to</strong> play by their familiaradults <strong>and</strong> that the adults come <strong>to</strong> knowjust the right level of arousal for the baby –under-stimulation or over-stimulation willboth result in non-involvement orcurtailing of interest by the baby – soadults who are responsive adjust theiractions in response <strong>to</strong> babies’ cues.Babies’ preferred games at this stage arerepetitious <strong>and</strong> ritualised, such as ticklinga baby’s chest before putting on a cleannappy, the beginnings of intimate familyor baby-carer rituals. Winnicott (1971)believed that because the adult adjuststhe play according <strong>to</strong> the baby’s cues,this helps the baby feel <strong>to</strong> some extentin control of the interaction.As Trevarthen et al (1998: 185) suggest:‘Dyadic, emotional <strong>and</strong> dynamic patternsof communication are shown by infancyre s e a rch <strong>to</strong> fo rm the fo u n d ations ofp s yc h o l og i cal <strong>and</strong> cog n i t i ve deve l o p m e nt,social adaptation <strong>and</strong> personalityintegration.’They continue by stressing the adverseimpact of a lack of, or disruption <strong>to</strong>, the‘fundamental vitality’ of thesecommunications, adding that theexpressions <strong>and</strong> responses of a child withautism are often difficult <strong>to</strong> ‘read’ becausethey tend <strong>to</strong> be idiosyncratic orimperceptible. As Kate Wall (forthcoming)maintains, there are also implications forany child who is experiencing difficultiesin communication skills.It is in early enco u nte r s, ex p l o ring thro u g hm ove m e nt <strong>and</strong> the senses, t h at cog n i t i ved eve l o p m e nt prog re s s e s. In fact Pa po u s e k’sresearch over 30 years ago (reported inPa pousek <strong>and</strong> Pa pousek 1987) showed thatvery young babies enjoyed learning <strong>and</strong>co ntinued <strong>to</strong> engage in ex pe ri m e nts whenthe milk which had been their reward nolonger satisfied them,the activity had itsown intrinsic reward.In chapter 2 the theories of Piaget <strong>and</strong>Vygotsky were outlined very briefly <strong>and</strong> inparticular the way in which Piaget’s ideashad been interpreted in ECEC practice washighlighted. In the field of ECEC they havehad a major impact <strong>and</strong> the messagestaken from their theories have largelybeen that the Piagetian model of children’sthinking involved the idea of the ‘child aslone scientist’ <strong>and</strong> stages in cognitivedevelopment. Margaret Donaldson (1978)<strong>and</strong> the Post-Piagetians (for example,Hughes, McGarrigle <strong>and</strong> others)demonstrated how Piaget’s theory hadbeen misunders<strong>to</strong>od <strong>and</strong> misapplied, <strong>and</strong>that their revisions ensured that thecontext in which children developed their86 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


underst<strong>and</strong>ings was crucial. They showedhow young children’s underst<strong>and</strong>ing iscontingent – related <strong>to</strong> what theyexperience <strong>and</strong> know – <strong>and</strong> that it is up <strong>to</strong>the adults who are educating them <strong>to</strong> helpthem (usually later in life) underst<strong>and</strong> how<strong>to</strong> apply those underst<strong>and</strong>ings <strong>to</strong> thinkingabout other unfamiliar contexts. Researchby Tizard <strong>and</strong> Hughes (1984) <strong>and</strong> Gelman<strong>and</strong> Gallistel (1978) was seminal in drawingattention <strong>to</strong> the ways in which childrenfound tasks incomprehensible in nursery<strong>and</strong> school which they could have solvedin their homes because the tasks wouldhave been meaningful <strong>and</strong> relevant.Among others, Gopnik et al (1999) havefurther shown that babies <strong>and</strong> youngchildren arrive in ECEC settings alreadyknowing a great deal <strong>and</strong> they use thisknowledge <strong>to</strong> learn more, but they needother people <strong>to</strong> help them. Parents <strong>and</strong>practitioners can provide that help – <strong>and</strong>sometimes other children do <strong>to</strong>o – inindirect as well as direct ways. Forexample, by hanging a mobile over a co<strong>to</strong>r giving a baby a rattle which they haveenjoyed <strong>to</strong>gether previously, <strong>to</strong> exploresound, feel <strong>and</strong> taste, independently.Meanwhile Vygotsky’s theory has beenseen in terms of an emphasis on learningas social,the ‘young child as apprentice’model,<strong>and</strong> his notion of the ‘zone ofproximal development’ – meaning adultsresponsible for a child’s learning wouldneed <strong>to</strong> observe <strong>and</strong> assess both what thechild knows <strong>and</strong> can do <strong>and</strong> what thatchild is striving <strong>to</strong> learn. The adult is then<strong>to</strong> plan how <strong>to</strong> help that child engage inappropriate activity which will bothinterest the child <strong>and</strong> help the childachieve the next step in the process oflearning relevant <strong>to</strong> the <strong>to</strong>pic in h<strong>and</strong>.Another important idea which links withVygotsky’s work is that of ‘scaffolding’.Bruner (1977) suggested that when anadult or more knowledgeable other childensures manageable steps, support <strong>and</strong>encouragement for a child trying <strong>to</strong> door <strong>to</strong> learn something new, they arescaffolding the learning.Meadows (1993) provides an overview <strong>and</strong>critique of Piaget’s <strong>and</strong> Vygotsky’s theories<strong>and</strong> research on children’s thinking inwhich she points out the flaws but alsohighlights the tremendous achievementsof these two researchers. As we pointedout in chapter 1 however, it is important<strong>to</strong> remember that both were from‘Northern’/minority countries <strong>and</strong> eventheorising is subject <strong>to</strong> culturalassumptions. Perhaps one of the mostimportant ideas that should be taken fromtheir research <strong>and</strong> theories is the key pointthat babies <strong>and</strong> young children are activelearners. They are not simply vesselswaiting <strong>to</strong> be filled up with knowledge.This response <strong>to</strong> learning has formed thebasis for many of the interventionprogrammes devised for children withspecial needs, particularly in the USA, forexample, the Direct Instructionprogramme developed by Bereiter <strong>and</strong>Engelmann in the 1960s, which still hasechoes <strong>to</strong>day. Such approaches do not87


take in<strong>to</strong> account the rich,natural,experiences available <strong>to</strong> children whoare included within a mainstream setting.During these first years they are activelytrying <strong>to</strong> make sense of the worlds inwhich they find themselves. As Donaldson(1978:111) comments after reviewingearlier research on babies ‘we mayconclude that there exists a fundamentalhuman urge <strong>to</strong> make sense of the world<strong>and</strong> bring it under deliberate control.’Gopnik et al (1999) stress how babies <strong>and</strong>young children approach the world as ifthey are scientists, actively engaged inlooking for patterns in what is going onaround them, testing hypotheses, seekingexplanations <strong>and</strong> formulating newtheories.They suggest that two year oldsin particular have a drive <strong>to</strong> find out howpeople <strong>and</strong> their familiar world ‘work’:-‘as with scientists, finding the truth is morethan a profession – it’s a passion. And, aswith scientists, that passion maysometimes make them sacrifice domestichappiness.’ (Gopnik et al 1999:38).Gopnik <strong>and</strong> her colleagues go on <strong>to</strong>suggest that babies play imitation games<strong>to</strong> try <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> other people, babble<strong>to</strong> ‘try on’ language;<strong>and</strong> play games likepeekaboo <strong>and</strong> hide <strong>and</strong> seek <strong>to</strong> find outabout objects <strong>and</strong> how people see them.The research reported by specialist ECECresearchers such as Athey (1990), Bruce<strong>and</strong> Meggitt (2002), Matthews (2002),Davies (2002), Nutbrown (1994), <strong>and</strong>Whalley et al (2000) has providedpractitioners with powerful informationthey have used <strong>to</strong> explore young children’sschemas – early patterns of behaviourindicating that a child is working at asystematic investigation (see chapter 2for further information about schemas).As Nutbrown (1994: 11) comments:‘Early patterns of behaviour seen in babiesbecome more complex <strong>and</strong> morenumerous, eventually becoming grouped<strong>to</strong>gether so that babies <strong>and</strong> youngchildren do not perform single isolatedbehaviours but coordinate their actions.Toddlers work hard, collecting a pile ofobjects in the lap of their carer, walking <strong>to</strong><strong>and</strong> fro, bringing one object at a time. Theyare working on a pattern of behaviour thathas a consistent thread running throughit…related <strong>to</strong> the consistent back-<strong>and</strong>forthmovement. The early schemas ofbabies form the basis of the patterns ofbehaviour which children show betweenthe ages of two <strong>and</strong> five years, <strong>and</strong> thesein turn become established foundationsfor learning.’EgocentricityPiaget (1962) argued that young childrenare egocentric, that they can only see theworld from their own point of view. Theyassume that everyone thinks as they do.Gopnik et al (1999) describe an ingeniousexperiment in which they showed thatc h i l d ren ro u g h ly younger than 18 mont h scould not see another’s viewpoint,by providing bowls of broccoli <strong>and</strong>biscuits. When they asked a child under18 months <strong>to</strong> give them broccoli,sayingt h ey liked it be t ter than biscuits, the infant s88 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


still gave them biscuits, because that iswhat the children themselves preferred.The children older than 18 mont h sh oweve r, g ave them broc coli despite theirown expressed preference for biscuits.Gopnik <strong>and</strong> her colleagues suggest thatat about this age a child’s brain is actuallyreprogramming itself <strong>to</strong> recognise thisnew underst<strong>and</strong>ing about other minds.So although babies seem <strong>to</strong> be able <strong>to</strong>empathise by crying when other babiescry, it may be that they are emotionallytuned in <strong>to</strong> other people – both adults <strong>and</strong>children – but do not yet underst<strong>and</strong> otherviewpoints. Despite this, Bruner (1983) haspointed out that in observations of twobabies, begun when they were <strong>three</strong> <strong>and</strong>five months old respectively, they beganpointing (see chapter 4) at just over ninemonths <strong>and</strong> at 13 months, <strong>and</strong> pointingindicates an underst<strong>and</strong>ing of otherminds, <strong>and</strong> of trying <strong>to</strong> share what is ‘inone’s own mind’. To Bruner, their behavioureven before they began pointing, signalledthat babies are ‘Naïve Realists’, they believethere is a world of objects <strong>and</strong> that othersexperience the same world they do.Making connections about the world,about what things are <strong>and</strong> theirsignificance in one’s culture, is important,but being able <strong>to</strong> appreciate that others’minds may ‘hold different perceptions <strong>and</strong>thoughts from one’s own’ is a major step.Memory <strong>and</strong> cognitive developmentAlthough some recent experiments showthat even young babies can rememberwhat happened in the past <strong>and</strong> canpredict (see for example Wynn’s 1992experiment on babies counting <strong>to</strong>y duckswhich ‘disappear’), their memory abilitiesclearly develop rapidly between eightmonths <strong>and</strong> a year (see also chapter 6).This is displayed by their fear of strangersduring this phase, where earlier theywould have smiled.Memory plays an important role inunderst<strong>and</strong>ing the world <strong>and</strong> in beingable <strong>to</strong> think. When a <strong>to</strong>y is hidden undera cloth a baby under about eight monthswill not search for it. Gopnik et al (1999)argue that babies live in a ‘magic’ worldwhere things can just vanish. Later, thebaby will search for the hidden <strong>to</strong>y,realising that it still exists somewhere. Thisphenomenon is called object permanence.Similarly, when a baby recognises thatpeople do not simply vanish,this is knownas person permanence. It is thoughttherefore that during the last part of theirfirst year babies are able <strong>to</strong> internallyrepresent – or think about – people orobjects that are familiar <strong>to</strong> them. Asmemory develops during the second <strong>and</strong>third year of life, children become able <strong>to</strong>engage in the construction of narrativesabout themselves, their families <strong>and</strong> thingsthat interest them. These narratives helpthem develop their sense of self <strong>and</strong> selfregulation(see chapter 3), relate <strong>to</strong> pastexperiences <strong>and</strong> project in<strong>to</strong> the future.These activities indicate a child’s level ofthinking as we will see in the next section.89


Language <strong>and</strong> thoughtVygotsky (1978:24) suggested that ‘themost significant moment in the courseof intellectual development, which givesbirth <strong>to</strong> the purely human forms ofpractical <strong>and</strong> abstract intelligence, occurswhen speech <strong>and</strong> practical activity, twopreviously completely independent linesof development, converge.’ Vygotskybelieved that although language maybegin in social interactions, its use as amethod of reflection means it is a <strong>to</strong>olof complex thinking. According <strong>to</strong> Nelson(1999),Bakhtin’s idea of multi-voicednessmay be useful in underst<strong>and</strong>ing youngchildren – for multi-voicedness means thata parent’s or carer’s ‘voice’ may ‘infect’ themind, so influencing the child without thisbeing realised. Nelson adds that narrativehas now been recognised as linkinghuman thought <strong>and</strong> language, <strong>and</strong> thatwhat dramatists do is <strong>to</strong> emphasise thepsychological processes involved in theconstruction of a s<strong>to</strong>ry-line or plot. Even<strong>three</strong> year olds need <strong>to</strong> be able <strong>to</strong> weavetheir underst<strong>and</strong>ing of action,intentions,motives, emotions, other minds, <strong>and</strong> so on,in order <strong>to</strong> engage in ‘s<strong>to</strong>rying’ (dramaticplay) (Nelson 1999). (See chapters 2 <strong>and</strong> 6for the emphasis on narrative by leadfigures such as Bruner, Feldman <strong>and</strong> Siegelrespectively). The research indicates thatlanguage development facilitatescognitive development, but equally,cognitive development fosters languagedevelopment.Jean M<strong>and</strong>ler has ex p l o red the fact thatbabies do not wait until the onset oflanguage <strong>to</strong> start thinki n g. She state s:‘The more I delve in<strong>to</strong> cognition in thefirst year of life the more it be co m e sa p p a re nt that many of the most basicfo u n d ations on which adult co n cepts re s ta re laid down during this pe ri od.’ Ma n d l e r( 1996 : 365 ). M<strong>and</strong>ler suggests that babiescan fo rm cate g o ri e s, be ca u s e, for ex a m p l e,babies re s pond <strong>to</strong> dolls in a diffe re nt wayf rom how they re s pond <strong>to</strong> humans, t h eycan pick up the pro pe rty of motion.M<strong>and</strong>ler concludes by stating that‘language is mapped on<strong>to</strong> a meanings ys te m’ ( M<strong>and</strong>ler 1996 : 380 ). Ac co rding<strong>to</strong> Bru ce this means that ‘it is developinga meaning sys tem that will lead a <strong>to</strong>d d l e ri n<strong>to</strong> incre a s i n g ly more co m p l ex laye ri n g sin play.’ ( Bru ce 2001 :43).Talk with other familiar people is anessential component of cognitivedevelopment. Researchers of privatespeech tentatively suggest (becausemuch of their research is small scale) thatpretend play with a caring adult duringthe second year of life may form a basis forthe private speech that is used <strong>to</strong> solveproblems <strong>and</strong> for self-regulation in youngchildren (Smolucha 1992).Practitioners should be aler ted therefore<strong>to</strong> the fact that, as we pointed out inchapter 3,depressed mothers were found<strong>to</strong> talk less frequently with their babies ifthey were boys <strong>and</strong> that this was found<strong>to</strong> correlate with the infants’ delayedcognitive development compared withgirls from both groups <strong>and</strong> boys whosemothers were not depressed (Hirose <strong>and</strong>Barnard 1997).90 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


The importance of social interactions<strong>and</strong> cognitive development formaking connectionsSocial inte ra ctions with their mothers<strong>and</strong> re l i a n ce on them during emotionalchallenges at six <strong>to</strong> nine months have be e nl i n ked with cog n i t i ve <strong>and</strong> language skillsat age two years (Robinson <strong>and</strong> Aceve d o2001 ).Ch i l d ren who had shown lowre l i a n ce on their mothers when distre s s e d<strong>and</strong> whose mothers had low psyc h o l og i ca lre s o u rce s, had less well deve l o pe dlanguage <strong>and</strong> cog n i t i ve skills than thosewhose re s ponses displayed high emotionalre a ct i v i ty <strong>and</strong> who relied heav i ly on theirmothers for suppo rt <strong>and</strong> social re fe re n c i n g.Fu rt h e r, Mu rray <strong>and</strong> Coo per (1997) discussthe growing body of ev i d e n ce indicat i n gt h at po s t n atal depression is implicated ina range of adverse outcomes for babies,e s pe c i a l ly males, i m p a cting on theircog n i t i ve <strong>and</strong> emotional deve l o p m e nt.Emphasis on the impo rt a n ce of early,s u p po rt i ve inte ra ctions be tween babies<strong>and</strong> the adults ‘m o t h e ring ‘ them <strong>and</strong> thei m p a ct on cog n i t i ve abilities, is also fo u n din Wi j h n ro k s’s (1998) re s e a rc h .Wh e remothers we re able <strong>to</strong> maintain theat te ntion of their babies in inte ra ct i o n s,whether the babies we re pre - te rm orf u l l - te rm ,the be t ter the outcome for theb a b i e s’ cog n i t i ve deve l o p m e nt (Smithet al 1996 ).Bo rn s tein (1998: 301) re po rted on re s e a rc hco n ce rned with the way ‘Me ntal growth inthe human child consists of the incre a s i n gcoo rd i n ation of mind <strong>and</strong> re a l i ty.’ To do thishe loo ked at ‘h a b i t u at i o n’ – the way inwhich babies are at first inte re s te d, t h e nbe come bo red with, <strong>and</strong> lose inte rest in,o b j e cts they are shown seve ral times.An impo rt a nt finding from this re s e a rc hre l ated <strong>to</strong> babies who had been ex po s e d<strong>to</strong> cocaine in ute ro. At first it was assumedt h at the problems such children had one nte ring school we re the result of longte rm damage from the dru g s. Howeve r,Bo rn s te i n’s wo rk indicates that, while thec h i l d ren may well be ex pe riencing difficultl i ves through co ntinuing ri s ky be h aviour intheir pare nt s, the dru g - ex posed babieswe re not nece s s a ri ly disadva ntaged in theiri n fo rm ation processing abilities per se, t h eywe re suffe ring from an arousal re g u l at i o np ro b l e m ,o ften crying in re s ponse <strong>to</strong> nove ls t i m u l ation where the dru g - f ree babieswe re inte re s ted <strong>and</strong> po s i t i ve.Teasing of <strong>to</strong>ddlers by fathers is moreprevalent than by mothers, according<strong>to</strong> research by Labrell (1994). Suchinteractions introduce novelty <strong>and</strong>ambiguity which challenge <strong>and</strong> oftendelight young children. However,interactions resulting in arousal meanthat the adults involved need <strong>to</strong> besensitive <strong>to</strong> levels with which theirchildren are comfortable.Equally, as children interact more withsiblings <strong>and</strong> peers, they will be insituations where conflicts may arise.Several researchers (for example Gopniket al 1999; Light 1983) discuss how conflictcan act as a spur <strong>to</strong> cognitivedevelopment. This idea was also putforward by Piaget (1932),particularly inrelation <strong>to</strong> moral development.91


In re s e a rch in French day ca re ce nt re s,Sy lvie Rayna (2001) found that ve ry yo u n gc h i l d ren (less than two years old) we red i s p l aying the notion of a ‘co m m u n i ty ofl e a rn e r s’. In one ex a m p l e, a seven mont hold baby had tried <strong>to</strong> co py the action ofan older child who had inserted a strawi n<strong>to</strong> a tube. Rayna discusses hero b s e rvations as examples of the cog n i t i vedimension of <strong>to</strong>g e t h e rn e s s, u s u a l ly note dbe tween mothers <strong>and</strong> infant s, but in thiscase noted among children themselve s.She links her wo rk <strong>to</strong> Pi a g e t i a nco n s t ru ct i v i s m (the child as act i veco n s t ru c<strong>to</strong>r of kn owledge thro u g hex pe ri e n ce <strong>and</strong> not as an ‘e m p ty ve s s e l’ ).BEING IMAGINATIVEPlay <strong>and</strong> cognitive developmentIn 1972, Bruner’s article ‘The nature <strong>and</strong>uses of immaturity’ was published. In thisarticle he argued that the young ofanimals play <strong>to</strong> learn <strong>and</strong> that the capacityfor learning is related <strong>to</strong> the length ofimmaturity. Bruner added that playinvolves flexibility of thought. Corinne Hutt(1966) building on the work of Piagetsuggested that when children explore, it isas if they are asking the question ‘Whatdoes this do?’<strong>and</strong> when they havediscovered some of the properties ofwhatever they are interested in,they playas if they are asking the question ‘Whatcan I do with this?’ Research with schoolage children (Bruner et al 1976) suggeststhat when they have had opportunities <strong>to</strong>play (explore <strong>and</strong> experiment freely) withmaterials, they are better able <strong>to</strong> solvetasks using those materials later. Althoughplay with babies may be initiated by afamiliar adult, once they have had someexperience, babies will begin playingspontaneously – this spontaneity is anessential characteristic of play. The otherkey aspect is that it is the baby, or child,who is in control <strong>and</strong> any adult involvedneeds <strong>to</strong> follow that lead. Stern’s (1977)research showed that parents who behaveas if their interactions with their babies area ‘dance’ in which the baby takes the leadare those who most effectively foster theirchildren’s development <strong>and</strong> learning.In the very early years young children areplaying <strong>to</strong> find out about the materials<strong>and</strong> people – the world – they find aroundthem.Young babies will repeat the sameaction – for example, throwing a <strong>to</strong>y downon the floor when in a high chair, so anadult or another, older child will retrieveit – or enjoying a hiding game when theyare beginning <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> objectpermanence (coming <strong>to</strong> know that evenwhen out of sight, an object or a personstill exists).Goldschmeid’s (1987) video Infants at Workshowed the play <strong>and</strong> social interactionsbetween very young babies, as they sharethe contents of a treasure basket, withcaring adults nearby <strong>to</strong> give reassurance.Bruce (2001:46) stresses that‘Ch i l d ren at play are able <strong>to</strong> stay flex i b l e,re s pond <strong>to</strong> eve nts <strong>and</strong> changing situat i o n s,be sensitive <strong>to</strong> pe o p l e, <strong>to</strong> adapt, think ontheir fe e t, <strong>and</strong> keep alte ring what they do in92 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


a fast-moving sce n e. When the process ofp l ay is ri c h ,it can lead children in<strong>to</strong> cre at i n grich prod u cts in their s<strong>to</strong> ri e s, p a i nt i n g s,d a n ce s, music maki n g, d raw i n g s, s c u l p t u re s<strong>and</strong> co n s t ru ct i o n s, or in the solving ofs c i e ntific <strong>and</strong> mat h e m at i cal pro b l e m s.’Some learning in young children occursthrough imitation.They will try <strong>to</strong> do whattheir carer does, sometimes after a timelapse, which indicates how memory isassisting that learning, but usually that‘imitation’ is actually a re-construction –the child’s own version of what s/he hasobserved <strong>and</strong> noted, constructed <strong>and</strong>transformed – in just the same way apainter like Picasso would transformexperiences, ideas, feelings <strong>and</strong>perceptions in<strong>to</strong> a work of art. Bruce (2001)provides a detailed overview of learningthrough play in the years before six,commenting that during the <strong>to</strong>ddler yearschildren begin <strong>to</strong> rehearse roles, pretend<strong>and</strong> create play props, as their ability <strong>to</strong>imagine accelerates rapidly, along withlanguage <strong>and</strong> play with symbols.Meanings are made, not mere ly dispatc h e d<strong>and</strong> co n s u m e d, as Meek arg u e s,‘The most strenuous period of imaginativeactivity is that time in childhood when weplay with the boundaries of our view ofthe world:sense <strong>and</strong> nonsense, the real<strong>and</strong> the fictive, the actual <strong>and</strong> the possible,all within the cultural domain we inhabit.’(Meek 1985: 53)Observing children when they play infamiliar surroundings is not onlyenjoyable, it is essential, because it isduring play that children are relaxedenough <strong>to</strong> ‘perform’ in ways whichdemonstrate the amazing extent of whatthey know <strong>and</strong> can do (see Lindon 2001).BEING CREATIVERobinson, <strong>and</strong> others, claim that‘creativity is in crisis’ (Robinson 2001: 195)as instruction <strong>and</strong> information giving<strong>and</strong> gathering overwhelm the timeavailable for nurturing <strong>and</strong> valuingdifferent intelligences <strong>and</strong> ways of being<strong>and</strong> belonging.And yet the curi o s i ty of ve ry yo u n gc h i l d ren <strong>and</strong> their ability <strong>to</strong> take risks ind i s cove ry, p rovide a firm basis on whichc re at i v i ty can be deve l o pe d. By about eightm o nths of age, when children can movere a s o n a b ly indepe n d e nt ly, away from thefamiliar l<strong>and</strong>sca pes of the adult faces whoe nte rtain <strong>and</strong> co m m u n i cate with them,c h i l d ren will range around their homel a n d s ca pe, seek out <strong>and</strong> ex p l o re object sco n s t a nt ly. Anything reachable can bet u rned in<strong>to</strong> a play t h i n g. Ma ny pare nts arefamiliar with the idea that, in this, t h ec h i l d’s re s e a rc h ,p l a ces such as ki tc h e nc u p bo a rds be come play sites (Pi e rce 2000).Such ex p l o ration satisfies curi o s i ty but it isalso helping the child develop pe rce p t u a l<strong>and</strong> spatial awa re n e s s. Ag a i n ,the re s t ri cte da b i l i ty <strong>to</strong> move <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> ex p l o re of their ow na c co rd, ex pe ri e n ced by children withce re b ral palsy or those not motivated <strong>to</strong>m ove, l i ke some children with Dow n’ss y n d ro m e, means that these children willnot develop pe rceptual <strong>and</strong> spat i a lawa reness as effe ct i ve ly as other childre n .93


In Kate Pa h l’s longitudinal re s e a rch study ofyoung boys’ meaning, making activities intheir homes, she found that children oftendrew on the ‘cultural capital’ available <strong>to</strong>them at home, including games played,te l evisual text s, Su pe rm a ri o, the home site :spaces <strong>and</strong> artefacts as well as s<strong>to</strong>riesheard, etc, (Pahl 2001:120) in order <strong>to</strong>enrich their meaning, constructions <strong>and</strong>transformations. Commercially produced<strong>to</strong>ys are seldom used in isolation forc re at i ve purposes <strong>and</strong> sometimes their usecan be counter productive, particularly ifdirected by adults. In Pridham, Becker <strong>and</strong>Brown’s research project, investigating theoptimum conditions for childre n’s foc u s e dexploration of <strong>to</strong>ys, they found that acaregiver’s ‘attention-directing behaviourhad a negative effect on infant explorationof <strong>to</strong>ys. The more a mother directed <strong>and</strong>consequently, refocused her infant’sattention, the less focused exploration of<strong>to</strong>ys the infant did’ (Pridham et al 2000:1445). More often,play, s<strong>to</strong>rying <strong>and</strong>creative acts appeared <strong>to</strong> take place inthe co-constructed worlds of adults <strong>and</strong>children <strong>and</strong> Pierce discovered throughher observations the significance of ‘dyadicinterplay between the occupations of themother <strong>and</strong> those of the infant <strong>and</strong><strong>to</strong>ddler’. She found <strong>to</strong>o that the mothersin her study ‘supported <strong>and</strong> shaped infant<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ddler play in the home throughtheir management of home space <strong>and</strong> itsplay objects’ (Pierce 2000:297). It is, asMeek suggests, the affective nature of play,the exploration of alternate worlds, the‘rituals of s<strong>to</strong>ry play’ that serve as culturalreference points in the development ofwhat children see as real <strong>and</strong> not real(Meek 1985: 49). (Note again here theinvolvement of shared, co-constructedrituals <strong>and</strong> narratives, shaped by thechildren with adult support <strong>and</strong>encouragement, reiterating the ‘messages’in the section on language <strong>and</strong> thoughtearlier in this chapter.)It is Ma l a g u z z i ,in discussing the philosophyof Reggio Em i l i a , who descri bes the nat u reof children <strong>and</strong> creativity from his work<strong>and</strong> observations:‘They have the privilege of not beingexcessively attached <strong>to</strong> their own ideas,which they construct <strong>and</strong> reinventcontinuously. They are apt <strong>to</strong> explore,make discoveries, change their points ofview <strong>and</strong> fall in love with forms <strong>and</strong>meanings that transform themselves.’(Edwards et al 1998:75)Ma l a g u z z i’s belief is that ‘c re at i v i ty re q u i re st h at the s c h ool of kn ow i n gfinds co n n e ct i o n swith the s c h ool of ex p re s s i n g, o pening thed oors <strong>to</strong> the hundred languages of childre n’( Edwa rds et al 1998 :77).Through the use of language <strong>and</strong>literature, <strong>and</strong> playful encounters withboth,parents <strong>and</strong> carers are able <strong>to</strong> showyoung children the nature of what ispossible. Robinson claims that ‘creativity isa process of seeing new possibilities’ <strong>and</strong>that ‘intuition <strong>and</strong> a feel for the materials<strong>and</strong> processes involved’ is critical(Robinson 2001: 137). The emphasis in thediscussion of creativity in young children ison process rather than production <strong>and</strong> the94 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


explora<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>and</strong> affective nature ofchildren in determining their play spaces<strong>and</strong> contexts is, by definition,creative. If,as Robinson claims, ‘creative processesare rooted in imaginative thought,in envisaging new possibilities…withoutcomes in the public world’ (Robinson2001: 115) then the child ranging throughkitchen cupboards <strong>and</strong> using saucepans<strong>and</strong> kitchen utensils as percussioninstruments is engaging in creative acts.‘Where there are high qualityopportunities for babies <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ddlers <strong>to</strong>create <strong>and</strong> imagine…the key person isattentive <strong>to</strong> a child’s creative explorations,providing assistance in a way that doesnot disrupt the child’s flow of thinking <strong>and</strong>through their unobtrusive support givesthe child the emotional security <strong>to</strong>experiment.’ (Manning-Mor<strong>to</strong>n <strong>and</strong> Thorp2001:section 7: 3).Although,most frequently, creativity isseen in childhood as taking part in acooperative <strong>and</strong> co-constructed world(Meek 1985, Dyson 2001),the very youngchild can be regarded as being creativewhen engaging in a monologue whilepushing a <strong>to</strong>y train,or while simplydigging s<strong>and</strong>. Similarly, Weir’s pre-sleepmonologues, the noise production of asingle child exploring clanking resourcesor a song or rhyme constructed playingwith sounds or words are examples ofchildren’s creativity. Anna Craft maintainsthat ‘creativity is dispositional <strong>and</strong> not amatter of ability…choosing a creative pathin any given situation is less a matter ofability <strong>to</strong> do so <strong>and</strong> more about “mind set”or attitude’ (Craft 2000: 107) <strong>and</strong>, withoutthe constraints perceived or collected aschildren grow <strong>and</strong> mingle in criticalgroups, young children in emotionally safecontexts are able <strong>to</strong> follow an explora<strong>to</strong>ry<strong>and</strong> risk taking mind set.Of course, as Chambers reminds us, it is ‘inliterature that we find the best expressionof the human imagination <strong>and</strong> the mostuseful means by which we come <strong>to</strong> gripswith ideas about ourselves <strong>and</strong> what weare’ (Chambers 1993: 16) <strong>and</strong> childrenneed, not only <strong>to</strong> construct <strong>and</strong>reconstruct their lived lives in playfulcontexts, but also <strong>to</strong> find their livesmirrored or referenced in the textsencountered in their early years, althoughthere are still very few s<strong>to</strong>ry books whichinclude young children with disabilities ascentral, powerful characters.There has been much celebration ofMaurice Sendak’s (1970) s<strong>to</strong>ry of Where theWild Things Are (Meek 1998) simply for thisreason;children are able <strong>to</strong> find their ownrelationships <strong>and</strong> temperament in Max aswell as their own sense of safety <strong>and</strong> wellbeing in the delicious closure <strong>to</strong> the tale,back in his very own room. Such texts arereassuring but also risky <strong>and</strong> challenging<strong>and</strong> they dare <strong>to</strong> play with safe contexts<strong>and</strong> upend them, exactly as children dothrough play <strong>and</strong> role play.Robinson believes that ‘the relationshipbetween knowing <strong>and</strong> feeling is at theheart of the creative process’ (2001:137)which brings <strong>to</strong>gether the investigative,information gathering explorations of95


young children <strong>and</strong> the affective nature ofthe world of babies <strong>and</strong> young children insafe, shared contexts. What artists do is <strong>to</strong>take what we know or see, feel or hear, <strong>and</strong>actively experiment (Gopnik et al 1999),transforming that knowledge throughtheir creations – <strong>and</strong> babies <strong>and</strong> childrendo this <strong>to</strong>o. As Bruce (2001:4) suggests:-The imagination makes images in themind. Creativity is the process by whichchildren turn these images in<strong>to</strong> creations.They try out ideas, feelings <strong>and</strong>relationships in their role play or pretendplay or find things <strong>to</strong> be used as playprops…In the context of play, creativityis more of a process than a product.’REPRESENTING THROUGH MARK MAKING‘Children want <strong>to</strong> write… They mark upwalls, pavements newspapers withcrayons, chalk, pens or pencils…Anythingthat makes a mark. The child’s marks say,“I am”.‘ (Graves 1983:3)Babies’ fingers <strong>and</strong> h<strong>and</strong>s, as we have seen,are used <strong>to</strong> feel, <strong>to</strong> <strong>to</strong>uch, <strong>to</strong> explore,<strong>to</strong> point, <strong>to</strong> reach <strong>and</strong> then <strong>to</strong> wave orgesture. In their movements, cruising <strong>and</strong>explorations, materials with which <strong>to</strong> makemarks are often accidentally found, tested<strong>and</strong> enjoyed. For example, as soon as solidfood <strong>and</strong> drink is introduced ,babies canbe seen <strong>to</strong> pour drinks on<strong>to</strong> surfaces <strong>and</strong>trail their fingers through it <strong>and</strong> dip fingersin<strong>to</strong> food, not only <strong>to</strong> use their fingers as<strong>to</strong>ols for feeding but also <strong>to</strong> tracepathways <strong>and</strong> investigate trails <strong>and</strong> tracks.Physical traces of babies <strong>and</strong> youngchildren are often <strong>to</strong> be found, before theuse of pens, pencils, crayon <strong>and</strong> paint, onsurfaces, walls <strong>and</strong> fabrics <strong>and</strong> before thesymbolic nature of mark making that weknow as literacy is discovered. What ishappening, however, is that very youngchildren soon discover ‘internationality’,that is the desire <strong>to</strong> make a mark, <strong>to</strong>‘signify’ <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> produce an effect. Thepleasurable effect of such tactile events isalso felt by adults, as they <strong>to</strong>o trail fingersin s<strong>and</strong>, tracing patterns or shapes orindeed doodle or idle with pens or pencils,letting minds w<strong>and</strong>er <strong>and</strong> allowing oftennon representational shapes <strong>to</strong> develop.Making marks, ‘leaving my mark’, has oftenbeen described as a basic human desire<strong>and</strong> function,leaving signposts orsignifiers of our existence.Parents <strong>and</strong> carers are often quick <strong>to</strong> seizeon such marks made by infants <strong>and</strong>interpret them as having significancealthough,while such marks are clearlyimportant, imposing representationalsignificance at this stage would beincorrect. However, in western societies,children are growing <strong>and</strong> learning in printrich <strong>and</strong> print significant cultures <strong>and</strong> veryquickly young children becomeencultured in<strong>to</strong> sign making activities.Also, as children begin <strong>to</strong> develop anability <strong>to</strong> conceptualise the abstract natureof language, the next stage, Vygotsky’s‘second order symbolism’ (1978) begins <strong>to</strong>take shape. As Kress explains, ‘alphabeticcultures dem<strong>and</strong> that children change tackfrom their route which relates voice withimage, which is plausible <strong>to</strong> them, <strong>to</strong> theroute of the alphabet, which relates sound96 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


with image (the image of a letter) whichis not plausible <strong>to</strong> them’ (Kress 2000: 69).Indeed, at this stage, during their first yearsof life, young children are busy makingsense of their world, interpreting sounds,language, in<strong>to</strong>nation <strong>and</strong> voice <strong>and</strong>establishing strong bonds <strong>and</strong>relationships with family <strong>and</strong> carers. It is inthis context that the representation of thisworld becomes possible with all thecultural <strong>to</strong>ols available <strong>to</strong> them (Pahl 1999;Kress 2000). That is, children will happilyrepresent their world with <strong>to</strong>ys, artefacts<strong>and</strong> available materials, in a multi modalsense as well as with pen <strong>and</strong> paper, stillthrough play, as they placehold, or fix intime sounds, objects, events or people(Barrs 1988; Pahl 1999). At this stage, Kressargues, ‘drawing the world <strong>and</strong> writing theworld are much the same thing for a child;both are recordings, transcriptions,translations, ‘spellings’ of aspects of theworld of the child’. (Kress 2000: 69).Beanie (then two) wanted <strong>to</strong> play at being ina café. Her Mum <strong>and</strong> Nan were <strong>to</strong>ld <strong>to</strong> becus<strong>to</strong>mers but when Mum was asked whatshe would like by the waitress (Beanie),sheasked for a menu. For a moment Beaniethought, then decided she would go <strong>to</strong>another room <strong>to</strong> fetch a book. Nan gave hera piece of paper folded instead. Without anyprompting, she went <strong>to</strong> a table where therewere pens <strong>and</strong> began <strong>to</strong> mark across thepaper, each line a different set of shapes orloops. She had created a menu whichbecame part of the shared ritual of café playlasting many months <strong>and</strong> becoming moreelaborate with each enactment.‘Children need <strong>to</strong> represent theirexperiences, their feelings, <strong>and</strong> ideas ifthey are <strong>to</strong> preserve them <strong>and</strong> share themwith others. When we represent we makean object or symbol st<strong>and</strong> for somethingelse’ Duffy (1998:9).Be fo re any ev i d e n ce of alphabetic pri nte m e rg e s, c h i l d ren engage in mark maki n g<strong>to</strong> re p re s e nt immediate oc c u rre n ce s, fo rexample ‘two <strong>to</strong> <strong>three</strong> year olds mayre p re s e nt the move m e nts of clouds <strong>and</strong>wate r; the wind blowing the wa s h i n g ;washing one’s h<strong>and</strong>s; combing one’s hair;bubbles rising <strong>to</strong> the surf a ce of wate r;dinosaurs bleeding <strong>to</strong> deat h ; the act i o n sof wa l king along, t ripping over <strong>and</strong> fallingover in<strong>to</strong> a dustbin; or even simply the ac<strong>to</strong>f sitting dow n .’ ( Mat t h ews 1998 : 94 ). Su c h‘a ction re p re s e nt at i o n s ‘, claims Mat t h ews,a re fo rmed from observat i o n s, t h e ni nte rp re ted <strong>and</strong> re p re s e nte d, o ften inways that appear not <strong>to</strong> bere p re s e nt ational or meaningful <strong>to</strong> adults.It is inte resting that when children areengaged in such mark making act i v i t i e s,s peech almost always accompanies thep roce s s, establishing Vyg o t s ky’s notiont h at ‘inner spe e c h’ be comes the dire c<strong>to</strong> rof language <strong>and</strong> act i o n ,‘b ri n g i n gawa reness <strong>to</strong> speech‘ (Vyg o t s ky 1986 :183 ). So, c i rcular marks on the page maybe the wheel ‘going round <strong>and</strong> ro u n d’ ora firewo rk shape, <strong>and</strong> such drawing willo ften be accompanied by onoma<strong>to</strong>e p i cs o u n d s, s h rieks <strong>and</strong> ex p l o s i o n s.97


Eliot (at two <strong>and</strong> a half) was at homechalking on a blackboard <strong>and</strong> narrating atale of a donkey who ran away from ahorrible, cruel person – he traced swirls as he<strong>to</strong>ld of the donkey’s w<strong>and</strong>erings <strong>and</strong> finallybrought his drawing <strong>to</strong> the centre of theboard when the donkey found shelter <strong>and</strong>happiness with a little boy <strong>and</strong> girl. The staffat his nursery, when <strong>to</strong>ld of this s<strong>to</strong>ryingexplained that they had read a tale <strong>to</strong> thechildren about a donkey who had a cruelowner <strong>and</strong> who ran away.As well as signifying objects <strong>and</strong> eventsthrough making marks <strong>to</strong> symbolise them<strong>and</strong> fix them in time <strong>and</strong> space, childrenare also often encouraged at this stage <strong>to</strong>be involved in writing acts in differentgenres that are socially <strong>and</strong> culturallysignificant, for example signing birthdaycards, mimicking shopping lists <strong>and</strong>writing notes. Their name, <strong>and</strong> the lettersfrom their name, become the focus at thisstage for much of the writing as well as arange of other circular <strong>and</strong> stroke-likeshapes that young children perceive <strong>to</strong> bethe nature of adult writing. It is interestingthat very young children have beenobserved <strong>to</strong> make marks which reflect thewritten language of their culture at thisstage with, for example, children from nonwestern cultures making marks from right<strong>to</strong> left, or <strong>to</strong>p <strong>to</strong> bot<strong>to</strong>m,using dots,pictures <strong>and</strong> other symbols, some of theirown invention, <strong>to</strong> represent <strong>and</strong> makemeaning. They are drawing on all availablecultural capital <strong>to</strong> create such scripts <strong>and</strong>’the decisions which children make inreading <strong>and</strong> writing…are not onlyorganised but are laced with bothpersonal <strong>and</strong> social organisation. Thisinterplay between personal <strong>and</strong> socialorganisation in the evolution of literacy isuniversal’ (Harste et al 1984: 107). Thus,individual children can be seen <strong>to</strong>represent, in their mark making, whicheversociety, community <strong>and</strong> culture <strong>to</strong> whichthey belong.It is important <strong>to</strong> acknowledge thecrucial part that the context, a writingenvironment which is accepting,emotionally positive <strong>and</strong> in which thereare appropriate resources, plays inchildren’s developing underst<strong>and</strong>ingsof the codes, symbols <strong>and</strong> signs thatdetermine effective print communication.Such contexts, determined <strong>and</strong> definedby parents <strong>and</strong> carers, as well asaccompanying interactions, form the basisof the affective nature of the experience.Because a child must ’disengage himselffrom the sensory aspect of speech <strong>and</strong>replace words by images of worlds…which lack the musical, expressive,in<strong>to</strong>national qualities of oral speech’(Vygotsky 1986: 181) then whether or notthere is any sense of emotionalengagement by <strong>and</strong> with adults will effectwhether or not children wish <strong>to</strong> repeat theprocess or event.For many adults <strong>and</strong> caregivers, suchpleasurable interactions are natural.Indeed, when Campbell recorded a day98 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


in the literacy life of his <strong>three</strong> year oldgr<strong>and</strong>daughter, he found that she wasnaturally, intentionally, playfully <strong>and</strong>cooperatively engaged in oral <strong>and</strong> writtenliteracy events throughout the day.Further, Alice orchestrated these activitieswith everyday rituals <strong>and</strong> routines, such asmeal times, <strong>and</strong> the events were almostexclusively initiated <strong>and</strong> led by the child(Campbell 1998). However, with parents<strong>and</strong> practitioners currently feelingpressurised in<strong>to</strong> engaging young childrenin functional literacy at a very young age,it is important <strong>to</strong> remember that at thisage children will attend most voraciously<strong>to</strong> what interests them. The experiences,memories <strong>and</strong> interaction in which youngchildren are engaged are constantly beingrevisited, refined <strong>and</strong> consolidated withboth concrete <strong>and</strong> abstract connectionsbeing made between objects, events,people <strong>and</strong> interactions. It is in thiseveryday, social context in which youngchildren’s development occurs that literacycan also emerge <strong>and</strong> grow. It is this idea of’literacy <strong>and</strong> literate outcomes asprocesses <strong>to</strong> be experienced, <strong>to</strong> be placedin relation <strong>to</strong> other literacy events <strong>and</strong>practices rather than seen as unchangingobjects of study or unquestioningreverence’ (Bearne 1995: 4). Also, becausethe beliefs that we hold as adults, parents<strong>and</strong> carers, affect what child behaviours wevalue <strong>and</strong> encourage (Harste et al 1984),then it seems important <strong>to</strong> develop <strong>and</strong>give status <strong>to</strong> opportunities <strong>and</strong> resourcesin safe <strong>and</strong> meaningful contexts forchildren <strong>to</strong> explore, imitate <strong>and</strong>reconstruct the sign systems which willinfluence <strong>and</strong> shape their lives, within theirown <strong>and</strong> others’ cultures. Indeed, asMatthews claims, ‘what we represent, theforms this representation may take, <strong>and</strong>how far societies support the growth ofrepresentational <strong>and</strong> expressive thought,are pedagogical, social <strong>and</strong> political issues‘(Matthews 1998:105).What children ‘represent’ will beinfluenced by the material nature of theirsurroundings (including the voices <strong>and</strong>exchanges they witness) <strong>and</strong> thesignificance given <strong>to</strong> such materials byfamily <strong>and</strong> community. Bruner takes theview that ‘human mental activity dependsfor its full expression upon being linked<strong>to</strong> a cultural <strong>to</strong>ol kit’ (Bruner 1986:15) <strong>and</strong>Kress reminds us that ‘as children aredrawn in<strong>to</strong> culture,“what is <strong>to</strong> h<strong>and</strong>”,becomes more <strong>and</strong> more that which theculture values <strong>and</strong> therefore makes readilyavailable’ (Kress 1997: 13). Allowingchildren the freedom <strong>to</strong> explore, express,denote <strong>and</strong> communicate through markmaking in a supported environment thatvalues talk <strong>and</strong> interaction,will ensure thatearly mark making feeds in<strong>to</strong> developingliterate practices (David et al 2000).Developing effective oral <strong>and</strong> written skillsof communication <strong>and</strong> engagingaffectively with such practices are bothcrucial <strong>to</strong> young learners as ‘language has<strong>to</strong> interpret the whole of our experience…with the child carving out for himself a99


oute that reflects the particularcircumstances of his own individualhis<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>and</strong> experience’ (Halliday 1978:21).As children ex pe ri m e nt, t a ke risks <strong>and</strong> makem a rks that ‘st<strong>and</strong> for things in the wo rl d’( Kress 2000: 6 ), within a lite ra cy ri c he nv i ro n m e nt, including news p a pe r s, ce re a lp a c ke t s, s i g n s, n o t i ce s, a dve rt i s e m e nt s,p ri nted tee shirts as well as screen lite ra cy,with adults who ca re <strong>and</strong> inte ra ct in as u p po rting <strong>and</strong> challenging manner, t h e nt h ey will emerge as lite ra cy userst h e m s e lve s. Howeve r, this is not a tidyp rocess – such as underw riting or co py i n gm ay seem <strong>to</strong> be – <strong>and</strong> it is nece s s a ry <strong>to</strong> ‘l i vewith the litter of lite ra cy’ ( Ha r s te et al 1984 :140) as those who have doc u m e nted theirown <strong>and</strong> other childre n’s lite ra cyd eve l o p m e nt will co n f i rm (eg Bi s s ex 1980 ,Pahl 1999) with signs, n o t i ce s, s c raps ofp a per establishing ident i ty, re l ationships <strong>and</strong>o f fe ring pe r s u a s i o n ,as well as maps,co n s t ru ctions <strong>and</strong> arte f a cts appe a ring atw i l l . Howeve r, the lasting effe cts of thiste m po ra ry state are that young childre nd evelop as makers <strong>and</strong> users of the symbo l i ccode in meaningful ways, l e a rning of thepower <strong>and</strong> influence that co m m u n i cat i o na cts invo lving writing can wield.LINKS WITH THE FRAMEWORKThe Development Matters points forthis Aspect, A Competent Learner, inthe Framework <strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong> Three Matters areas follows:-Making ConnectionsYoung babies use movement <strong>and</strong>sensory exploration <strong>to</strong> connect withtheir immediate environment.As they become more mobile, babiesconnect with <strong>to</strong>ys, objects <strong>and</strong> a widergroup of people.Young children learn through repeatingpatterns of play (sometimes calledSCHEMA).(CD-ROM).Children begin <strong>to</strong> connect objects <strong>and</strong>ideas: a pair of socks, a big teddy,armb<strong>and</strong>s for swimming.Being ImaginativeYoung babies enjoy <strong>and</strong> learn byimitating their key person.As they become mobile, babies usetheir whole bodies <strong>to</strong> recreate anexperience.Young children re-enact familiar sceneswith the help of people, props <strong>and</strong>resources.Children engage in concentrated playin which they extend their language<strong>and</strong> try out ideas, feelings, relationships<strong>and</strong> movements.100 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Being CreativeYoung babies explore their immediateenvironment of people, objects <strong>and</strong>feelings through all their senses.Babies quickly make sense of <strong>and</strong>respond <strong>to</strong> what they see, hear, feel,<strong>to</strong>uch <strong>and</strong> smell.As young children become moremobile, they express themselvesthrough physical action <strong>and</strong> sound.As children become more skilful inusing language <strong>and</strong> other forms ofcommunication,dance, music, 2D <strong>and</strong>3D art, they talk about, <strong>and</strong> share inother ways, the things they paint, draw<strong>and</strong> play with.Representing Through Mark MakingYoung babies discover mark makingby chance, noticing for instance thatfingers trailed through spilt juice canextend it or that a h<strong>and</strong> imprint remainsin wet s<strong>and</strong>.Babies imitate <strong>and</strong> improvise actionsthey have observed – a scarf is made <strong>to</strong>st<strong>and</strong> for a blanket or a skipping ropeh<strong>and</strong>le for a microphone.As young children explore <strong>to</strong>ols <strong>and</strong>materials they make marks <strong>to</strong> whichthey give meaning.Children begin <strong>to</strong> recognise somemarks <strong>and</strong> realise these meansomething <strong>to</strong> others.Parents <strong>and</strong> practitioners can observebabies <strong>and</strong> young children engaging inthe behaviour outlined in the lists above<strong>and</strong> reflect on the research informationprovided in the different sections of thechapter, which demonstrates the amazingcompetence, scientific exploration <strong>and</strong>‘sense making’ capacity of these ver yyoung people.SUMMARY OF KEY ‘MESSAGES’This chapter has reviewed research aboutchildren’s incredible competence inlearning – from birth (begun even beforethen,in the womb). The most important‘messages’ from this research are:-Babies seem <strong>to</strong> be tuned <strong>to</strong> learn from,with <strong>and</strong> about, firstly the people <strong>and</strong>the cultural environment around them,followed by the material environment –they come in<strong>to</strong> the world primed <strong>to</strong> becurious, competent learners.Play, in which the baby or child takesthe lead <strong>and</strong> makes choices, is a processwhich fosters cognitive development.Children ‘make sense’ of <strong>and</strong> ‘transform’knowledge, experiences <strong>and</strong> eventsthrough imaginative <strong>and</strong> creativeactivity.Language <strong>and</strong> thought aredevelopmentally linked, they eachdepend on <strong>and</strong> also promote thedevelopment of the other.101


Children’s developing memories <strong>and</strong>use of narrative help them make senseof their lives.They want <strong>to</strong> share <strong>and</strong> express theirideas playfully through the ‘hundredlanguages of children’ (for example,dancing, singing, talking, ‘s<strong>to</strong>rying’,music making, painting, makingpatterns, building, model-making,‘animating’ puppets <strong>and</strong> other <strong>to</strong>ys,dressing up, gardening, looking afteranimals, drawing, mark making – <strong>to</strong> listbut a few possibilities).Once again,the research points <strong>to</strong> thecentrality of positive relationships withparents <strong>and</strong> other key people in youngchildren’s lives.102 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Chapter 6A Healthy ChildA Healthy Child, the focus of the fourth Aspect of the Framework ‘<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong> Three Matters’,brings <strong>to</strong>gether evidence about young children’s mental <strong>and</strong> physical well-being. As westated in the introduc<strong>to</strong>ry chapter, being healthy means much more than havingnutritious food <strong>and</strong> being free from illness. For babies <strong>and</strong> young children being special<strong>to</strong> someone <strong>and</strong> cared for is important for their physical, social <strong>and</strong> emotional health <strong>and</strong>well-being. Health <strong>and</strong> social well-being underpin <strong>and</strong> determine children's responses<strong>to</strong> their environment, <strong>to</strong> people <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> new experiences. Emotional well-being includesrelationships, which are close, warm <strong>and</strong> supportive; being able <strong>to</strong> express feelings suchas joy, grief, frustration <strong>and</strong> fear leading <strong>to</strong> the development of coping strategies whenfaced with new, challenging or stressful situations. As the research in chapter 3 <strong>and</strong> thatpresented in this chapter shows, early emotional health <strong>and</strong> freedom from stress bodeswell for both mind <strong>and</strong> brain.Meeting children’s physical needs isfundamental <strong>to</strong> their well-being. As theyare growing <strong>and</strong> developing children whoare hungry, tired, ill or uncomfortable willnot enjoy the company of adults <strong>and</strong> otherchildren. However, young children willsoon become bored if choice is limited orprovision inappropriate <strong>to</strong> their needs.Responsible adults need <strong>to</strong> plan <strong>and</strong> offercaring support <strong>to</strong> keep young childrensafe <strong>and</strong> protected in a variety of ways <strong>and</strong>gradually they will know when <strong>to</strong> ask forhelp. It is through their own explorations<strong>and</strong> struggles <strong>to</strong> be physicallyindependent that they quickly becomeskilful in a range of movements involvingboth large <strong>and</strong> fine mo<strong>to</strong>r control. In time,young children learn about their bodies<strong>and</strong> what they can do <strong>and</strong> are able <strong>to</strong>make their own healthy choices.Work by researchers over the last twentyfive years has changed much of thethinking about babies <strong>and</strong> young children<strong>and</strong> their capabilities. Stern (1977) <strong>and</strong>Kagan (1984) showed how newbornbabies already have some abilities, or they103


ecome evident during the first month oflife, <strong>and</strong> they then build on these. Sight,smell, hearing, <strong>to</strong>uch,the ability <strong>to</strong> feelpain <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> respond <strong>to</strong> changes in bodyposition are all present when a baby isborn. This means that the human body<strong>and</strong> brain are amazingly primed <strong>to</strong> ‘takeon’ <strong>and</strong> try <strong>to</strong> make sense of the uniqueworld in<strong>to</strong> which each individual comes.This chapter outlines information <strong>and</strong>issues concerned with healthy bodies,brains <strong>and</strong> minds, how they grow <strong>and</strong>develop <strong>and</strong> what they appear <strong>to</strong> need.In particular, the chapter provides a reviewof what is now known about the brain inthe early years. It also raises some of theissues that relate <strong>to</strong> health <strong>and</strong> researchevidence concerning early intervention<strong>to</strong> ameliorate the effects of disadvantageon health.So while there may be some specialconditions or circumstances <strong>to</strong> whichattention should be paid, <strong>and</strong> any relevantreligious rules relating <strong>to</strong> diet for exampleneed <strong>to</strong> be acknowledged <strong>and</strong> followed inECEC settings, practitioners need above all<strong>to</strong> recognise parents’ <strong>and</strong> gr<strong>and</strong>parents’fund of knowledge, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> respect this,while ensuring they address all children’shealth <strong>and</strong> safety needs.Meggitt (2001:1) discusses health inholistic terms, <strong>and</strong> suggests that beinghealthy constitutes a number of differentphysical,mental <strong>and</strong> social,emotional,environmental <strong>and</strong> spiritual states (seediagram below). These are being fit, notbeing ill, being able <strong>to</strong> adapt, living <strong>to</strong> anold age, <strong>and</strong> being happy. Her Six aspectsof health,irrespective of age, are presentedin Figure 1 on the following page.WHAT DOES ‘HEALTHY’ MEAN?Our underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the term ‘healthy’is dependent upon particular social <strong>and</strong>cultural constructions. What constitutesbeing ‘healthy’ in the UK may well differfrom its definitions in other socialcontexts. Within the UK,different ethnicgroups are said <strong>to</strong> have varied healthneeds. However, Currer’s (1991) researchraised issues about the effects ofinstitutional racism through stereotyping<strong>and</strong> Black (1989) contributed theunderst<strong>and</strong>ing that the attitudes <strong>and</strong>expectations (in relation <strong>to</strong> health) ofchildren of British minority groups aresimilar <strong>to</strong> those of their schoolmates.104 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


FIGURE 1:SIX ASPECTS OF HEALTH (MEGGITT 2001:2)Environmental healthEnv i ro n m e ntal healthre fers <strong>to</strong> the genera lhealth of the soc i e ty inwhich we live. In areas offamine – where the firstp ri o ri ty for health is <strong>to</strong>obtain enough food –people may be denieda c cess <strong>to</strong> health. Pove rty<strong>and</strong> ove rc rowded livingconditions are alln e g at i ve aspe cts ofe nv i ro n m e ntal health.Physical healthThis is the easiest aspec<strong>to</strong>f health <strong>to</strong> measure.It involves the physicalfunctioning of the body<strong>and</strong> includes thegrowth <strong>and</strong> physicaldevelopment of thebaby <strong>and</strong> child.Mental healthMental health involvesour ability <strong>to</strong> organiseour thoughts logically,<strong>and</strong> is closely linked<strong>to</strong> emotional <strong>and</strong>social health.The six aspects of healthSocial healthSocial health involvesthe way we relate <strong>to</strong>other people <strong>and</strong> formrelationships.Emotional healthEmotional healthinvolves how we expressemotions such as joy,grief, frustration,hurt<strong>and</strong> fear. This ability <strong>to</strong>express our ownemotions <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> react <strong>to</strong>other people’s emotionsleads on <strong>to</strong> copingstrategies for anxiety<strong>and</strong> stress.Spiritual healthSpiritual healthinvolves personal,moral codes ofconduct – as wellas religious beliefs<strong>and</strong> practices.Figure 1: Reproduced with permission from Meggitt (2001:2)105


Meggitt (2001:4) also recommends thebasic needs of babies <strong>and</strong> children forachieving <strong>and</strong> maintaining healthy growth<strong>and</strong> development as being,‘protectionfrom infection <strong>and</strong> injury; shelter, warmth,clothing; access <strong>to</strong> health care;intellectualstimulation; praise <strong>and</strong> recognition ofachievements; social contacts; security;cleanliness; fresh air <strong>and</strong> sunlight; love<strong>and</strong> consistent <strong>and</strong> continuous affection;sleep, rest <strong>and</strong> activity; <strong>and</strong> food’From her own review of the researchliterature, Meggitt lists the aspects ofchildren’s lives that affect their healthas being‘genetic fac<strong>to</strong>rs, pre-conceptual fac<strong>to</strong>rs,antenatal fac<strong>to</strong>rs, perinatal fac<strong>to</strong>rs,nutrition,infectious diseases, housing,income, accidents, emotional <strong>and</strong> socialfac<strong>to</strong>rs, lifestyle – social <strong>and</strong> personalhabits, environmental fac<strong>to</strong>rs, <strong>and</strong>government policy’ (Meggitt 2001: 9-10).The articles of the United NationsConvention on the Rights of the Child (UN1989) also stress many health <strong>and</strong> safetyissues <strong>and</strong> the view of protection fromharm in the Convention has a widerinterpretation than that referring <strong>to</strong> childabuse as defined in the UK (Home Office,DoH, DES, Welsh Office 1991). TheConvention,like the Children Act 1989,recognises children’s own views <strong>and</strong>wishes as important – in other words theirpersonhood is acknowledged..Mayall (1996) re po rts on a study of (5 <strong>to</strong> 9ye a r-old) childre n’s own management ofo rd i n a ry health mainte n a n ce <strong>and</strong> note st h at children have tended <strong>to</strong> be theo b j e cts of health ca re ; their opinions havef re q u e nt ly been ‘d ow n g ra d e d’ by healthp ro fe s s i o n a l s.Yet childre n , she assert s,also ‘a c q u i re health-re l ated kn ow l e d g et h rough info rmal learning at home…t h ey use their kn owledge <strong>to</strong> pro m o te theirown we l l - be i n g, in the co ntext of <strong>and</strong> ini nte ra ction with social <strong>and</strong> phys i ca lfe at u res of their env i ro n m e nt’ ( Mayall 1996 :30 ). Ch i l d ren are act i ve ly part i c i p ating inthe mainte n a n ce of healthy life s tyles (theirown <strong>and</strong> others). Pri d m o re <strong>and</strong> Ste p h e n s’(2000) rev i ew of the child-<strong>to</strong> - c h i l da p p roach <strong>to</strong> health education (albeit withre fe re n ce <strong>to</strong> older children) also showsh ow this appro a c h , which is in ope rationin more than eighty co u nt ries wo rl dw i d e,a c kn owledges children as act i vep a rt i c i p a nts in health promotion <strong>and</strong>m a i nte n a n ce of healthy life s ty l e s.While the children on whom this reviewis focused are younger than those citedabove, Post & Hohmann (2000:203)provide guidelines which are relevant<strong>to</strong> children aged under <strong>three</strong>.‘Infants <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ddlers in group care haveno choice about being in childcare. Eachpart of the day, however, presentsopportunities for choices <strong>and</strong> decisionsthey can make…what <strong>to</strong> hold, look at, orwhether, how <strong>and</strong> how long <strong>to</strong> participatein an activity…Making these choices <strong>and</strong>decisions on a daily basis <strong>and</strong> being able<strong>to</strong> change their mind from one day <strong>to</strong> thenext tends <strong>to</strong> give children a sense ofcontrol over their day.’106 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


It is only by being given oppo rtunities <strong>to</strong>m a ke choices like those suggested abovet h at children begin <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> theco n s e q u e n ces of their decisions <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>be come co m pe te nt at ste e ring their ow nl i fe co u r s e s. It may be that one of the keyf a c<strong>to</strong>rs leading <strong>to</strong> the later life succe s s e s(including gre ater freedom from ment a lillness) of the Hi g h / S co pe pre s c h oo lp rog ramme (Schwe i n h a rt et al 1993) is theemphasis on the ‘Pl a n ,d o, rev i ew’ ro u t i n ewhich places the re s po n s i b i l i ty for howt h ey spend their time on the childre n .The ‘<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong> Three Matters’ Frameworkincludes the following developmentalpointers relating <strong>to</strong> Healthy Choices, forpractitioners:-From birth, young babies showpreferences for people <strong>and</strong> for whatthey see, hear <strong>and</strong> taste.Babies discover more about what theylike <strong>and</strong> dislike.As young children become moremobile <strong>and</strong> their boundaries widen,they make choices that can involvereal risk.Children become more aware thatchoices have consequences.Cl e a rly, w h e re babies <strong>and</strong> ve ry yo u n gc h i l d ren are co n ce rned it is pri m a ri ly theirmothers who take re s po n s i b i l i ty fo rp ro te cting <strong>and</strong> promoting their child’sh e a l t h . A range of fac<strong>to</strong>rs co nt ri b u te <strong>to</strong>health <strong>and</strong> we l l - be i n g. For ex a m p l e, s oc i o -e conomic inequalities (which are moref u l ly discussed in the section of this tex<strong>to</strong>n pove rty) have been shown <strong>to</strong> lead <strong>to</strong>d i f fe re n ces in childre n’s health. Mo re n o( 1993 :23) re po rts that low birt h - we i g ht isalmost always ‘the result of pre n atal soc i o -e co n o m i c, l i fe s tyle or be h av i o u ral fac<strong>to</strong> r son the part of the mother <strong>and</strong> family’.Bra d s h aw (2001) rev i ewed social <strong>and</strong>b i o l og i cal fac<strong>to</strong>rs in the deaths of childre nunder <strong>three</strong> in Engl<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> Wales fro m1981 <strong>to</strong> 1997 <strong>and</strong> found thema p p rox i m ate ly halved over this pe ri od.Howeve r, Bra d s h aw’s (2001) textco n ce rning re s e a rch on pove rty <strong>and</strong>c h i l d h ood re po rts that although deat hrates we re decreasing be tween 1987 <strong>and</strong>1991 , gender diffe re n ces re m a i ns i g n i f i ca nt, with the death rate for boysaged be tween one <strong>and</strong> 14 years 30 pe rce nt higher than for girl s.Wachs (2000) rev i ews the ev i d e n ce ofre s e a rch in<strong>to</strong> the links be tween nutri t i o n a ldeficits <strong>and</strong> be h av i o u ral deve l o p m e nt.He re po rts that seve re po s t n at a lm a l n u t ri t i o n , both mild <strong>and</strong> seve revitamin <strong>and</strong> mineral deficiencies, a n dc h ronic under- n u t rition have been show n<strong>to</strong> lead, for ex a m p l e, <strong>to</strong> problems such asg e n e ral <strong>and</strong> specific cog n i t i ve deficits;l ower neonatal re a ct i v i ty; a p at hy,i rri t a b i l i ty, l ower act i v i ty levels <strong>and</strong> higheri n h i b i t i o n ; <strong>and</strong> increased risk of poo re rcog n i t i ve <strong>and</strong> academic pe rfo rm a n cere s pe ct i ve ly. Zeanah (1993: 353) discussesthe inte rp l ay be tween env i ro n m e nt <strong>and</strong>genetics in infantile onset obe s i ty <strong>and</strong>i n fo rms us that ‘e nv i ro n m e ntal risk fac<strong>to</strong> r ssuch as low soc i o - e conomic status arekn own <strong>to</strong> be strong pre d i c<strong>to</strong>rs ofs u b s e q u e nt obe s i ty’.107


The National Heart Forum (2002: 4)expresses grave concern about ‘currenttrends <strong>and</strong> inequalities in diet, physicalactivity, obesity <strong>and</strong> smoking’ which are soserious as <strong>to</strong> be leading <strong>to</strong> ‘an epidemic ofcoronary heart disease, as well as stroke,cancer <strong>and</strong> diabetes in 40-50 years’ time’unless remedial action is taken now. Thismeans educating children <strong>to</strong> adopt ahealthy lifestyle from a very early age, aswell as taking action at national,local <strong>and</strong>family levels on the recommendations ofthe British Medical Association (BMA 1999)concerned with poverty, diet, exercise,smoking, drugs, <strong>and</strong> other measuresincluding improved access <strong>to</strong> ECEC.Protecting young children againstinfectious diseases through immunisationstargets one area that is responsible forinfant <strong>and</strong> childhood mortality (Kristensenet al 2002). Just under ten years agoinfectious diseases accounted for four percent of postnatal deaths in the UK in 1990,<strong>and</strong> for seven per cent of deaths ofchildren aged between one <strong>and</strong> four years.However, controversy about the ‘MMR’(measles, mumps, rubella) triple vaccinehas given rise <strong>to</strong> concern about childrenwho are not being immunised in 2002.While Christie (2002) claims experts havefound no links between the MMR vaccine<strong>and</strong> autism,there have been reports in thepopular <strong>and</strong> scientific press (See TheSunday Times 23 June 2002, <strong>and</strong> the BMJ,May 2002) that contradict one another onthe subject, <strong>and</strong> the debate continues.Travis (2002) reports the results of a surveyof 1003 parents in the UK which revealedthat <strong>three</strong> in every four parents surveyedwanted the government <strong>to</strong> provide ‘freeseparate measles, mumps <strong>and</strong> rubellavaccinations for their children amid risingconcerns about the combined MMRinjection’. (Guardian online 2002).(See Barr<strong>and</strong> Limb, 1997 for a review of medical,scientific <strong>and</strong> ethical issues relating <strong>to</strong>the MMR vaccines).It is not possible <strong>to</strong> cover all the healthissues concerning young children in thistext <strong>and</strong> readers are directed <strong>to</strong> the reviewby the British Medical Association (BMA1999) <strong>and</strong> their recommendations.However, in this chapter we have tried <strong>to</strong>provide an introduction <strong>to</strong> a number ofkey aspects of health in babies <strong>and</strong> youngchildren. Firstly, we explore researchevidence on brain development.YOUNG BRAINSIn the last twe nty- f i ve ye a r s, te c h n o l og i ca la dva n ces have enabled scient i f i cre s e a rchers <strong>to</strong> make new discove ries abo u tthe deve l o p m e nt of the human bra i n : i t sfo rm at i o n ,g rowth <strong>and</strong> cog n i t i ve act i v i ty,for example (see Nelson <strong>and</strong> Bl oom 1997 ).Howeve r, a number of re s e a rchers havea rgued that the ev i d e n ce should bere g a rded with some scepticism be ca u s esome of it is old, some from studies ofdiseased brains <strong>and</strong> some from studiesof rat s, rather than humans (Au b rey 2002;Bl a ke m o re 2002; Bruer 19 99 ).Meanwhile, Catherwood (1999) outlinedthese technological advances in thecontext of their relevance <strong>to</strong>108 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


developmental psychology <strong>and</strong> earlyeducation, <strong>and</strong> concluded that newresearch has begun <strong>to</strong> indicate thatPiaget’s characterisations of infantdevelopment ‘vastly underestimate infantcognitive development’ (Catherwood1999:28). Mark Johnson (1999), a cognitiveneuroscientist at Birkbeck College inLondon,has claimed it is curious thatPiaget, despite his biological approach <strong>to</strong>human cognitive development <strong>and</strong> beliefin an activity-dependent nature ofdevelopment, was typical in neglectingbrain functions, perhaps because at thetime of Piaget’s writing there was littleinformation about the brain <strong>and</strong> certainlynot the imaging <strong>and</strong> computer equipmentavailable <strong>to</strong>day. In spite of thesophistication of <strong>to</strong>day’s equipment,Greenfield (2002) claims that thenon-invasive techniques are still notrefined enough <strong>to</strong> examine connec<strong>to</strong>rsin the brain.Meade (2001) reviews the evidenceavailable about brain research in relation<strong>to</strong> early childhood, <strong>and</strong> helpfullysummarises the advantages <strong>and</strong>disadvantages for researchers <strong>and</strong>practitioners of neuro-imagingprocedures. Neuro-imaging technologies,such as PET scans (Positron EmissionTomography) not only have allowedneuroscientists <strong>to</strong> study brain activity,but have also led <strong>to</strong> new or revisedperspectives about early childhooddevelopment:It is now known that the nature versusnurture debate is not productive in thequest <strong>to</strong> better underst<strong>and</strong> the ways inwhich biological (genetic) <strong>and</strong>environmental fac<strong>to</strong>rs impact uponeach individual child’s development(see Chapter 1). Barnet <strong>and</strong> Barnet(1998) describe development as‘a lifelong dialogue between inheritedtendencies <strong>and</strong> our life his<strong>to</strong>r y’ <strong>and</strong> thisview is central <strong>to</strong> their discussions ofchildren’s intellectual,moral <strong>and</strong>emotional development fromneurological <strong>and</strong> psychologicalperspectives. Shonkoff <strong>and</strong> Phillips(2000) also concur that neuroscientificevidence has led <strong>to</strong> a clearerunderst<strong>and</strong>ing of the ways in whichgenetic <strong>and</strong> environmental influencesoperate dynnamically <strong>to</strong>gether over thecourse of development <strong>and</strong> that theyinfluence each other. Johnson <strong>and</strong>Mareschal (2001) provide evidence fromresearch in<strong>to</strong> infants’ perceptualdevelopment (vision <strong>and</strong> attention,action <strong>and</strong> space, social cognition, <strong>and</strong>speech perception) which has used,among other technologies, neuroimaging<strong>to</strong> help reveal the ways inwhich nature <strong>and</strong> nurture interact.There are conflicting views about theextent <strong>to</strong> which environmentalinfluences <strong>and</strong> stimulation shape earlybrain development <strong>and</strong> subsequentlyimpact upon one’s later physical <strong>and</strong>emotional well-being. There areopposing views about whether missed(or neglected) opportunities duringearly brain development can beregained later in life. Shore (1997), forexample, drew <strong>to</strong>gether insights on109


early development gathered largely from a1996 conference in the USA of leadingbrain scientists <strong>and</strong> experts in childdevelopment <strong>and</strong> early education. Theystated that early experiences have adecisive impact upon how the brainis ‘wired’ <strong>and</strong> that the timing of suchexperiences is crucial. Furthermore,Shore warns that research has indicatedthat the emotional neglect orab<strong>and</strong>onment of children very earlyin life can often lead <strong>to</strong> the impairmen<strong>to</strong>f brain-mediated functions such asempathy, attachment, <strong>and</strong> affectregulation (See Gunnar 1996). Theclaims that the first <strong>three</strong> years of lifeare critical <strong>to</strong> brain development lednot only <strong>to</strong> a proliferation of articlesaimed at parents, carers <strong>and</strong> educa<strong>to</strong>rs,but also <strong>to</strong> a critical response fromBruer (1999). In his review ofneurological <strong>and</strong> psychologicalevidence, Bruer refuted the view thatwindows of opportunity for braindevelopment close down after the first<strong>three</strong> years of life (with the exceptionof vision). Instead, he argues,neuroscientific findings havesometimes been stretched <strong>to</strong> formtenuous claims about early braindevelopment <strong>to</strong> fit the aims of researchor policy. While acknowledging thatthere are critical periods in braindevelopment, he reports that the brain’splasticity allows lifelong learning; forexample, he claims that,‘the limiting fac<strong>to</strong>r in vocabularygrowth, <strong>and</strong> presumably for some ofthe other things Verbal IQ measures,is exposure <strong>to</strong> new words, facts <strong>and</strong>experiences. The brain can benefit fromthis exposure at almost any time – earlychildhood, childhood, adolescence,adulthood <strong>and</strong> senescence.’(Bruer 1999:177)Further, as stated in chapter 1, Romanianchildren who were adopted after a periodof serious early deprivation made up theirphysical <strong>and</strong> psychological losses.There are, however, aspects of theevidence on brain development aboutwhich Bruer <strong>and</strong> Shore agree. Namely, thatit is during the first <strong>three</strong> years of life thatthe human brain makes trillions of new(synaptic) connections, <strong>and</strong> thatenvironmental influences are known <strong>to</strong>impact upon these connections. Bruer(1999: 264) noted that neuroscience hasled <strong>to</strong> the discovery that ‘humans <strong>and</strong>other animals experience a rapid burst inbrain connectivity – an exuberant burst ofsynapse formation – early in development’.Furthermore, environmental influences onthe brain’s early formation <strong>and</strong> ‘wiring’ canbe both positive <strong>and</strong> negative <strong>and</strong> cantake place even before a child is born.Nelson (1999) also argues thatneuroscientific work has shown that neuralplasticity <strong>and</strong> the subtle ‘dance’ that occursbetween the brain <strong>and</strong> the environmentmay lead <strong>to</strong> reconceptualisation of ideasabout intervention, competence <strong>and</strong>resilience. He stresses that it is important<strong>to</strong> dispense with nature versus nurturearguments in f avour of a new approach <strong>to</strong>u n d e r s t a n d i n gh ow ex pe ri e n ce can mod i f ythe bra i n , <strong>and</strong> how kn owledge deri ve d110 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


f rom such an approach can be t ter ident i f yi nte rve ntion proce d u re s, for ex a m p l e.Thus the implications for children withspecial needs are that interventionprogrammes should be fine tuned <strong>to</strong>ensure maximum stimulation for thoseaspects, sensory, physical or intellectual,<strong>to</strong> enable the synapses <strong>to</strong> form/connect.Babies’ brains develop at an as<strong>to</strong>nishingpace in the early years. Brains aregenetically wired at birth, but the complexcircuitry that permits mature thoughtprocesses <strong>to</strong> occur only begins <strong>to</strong> developin early childhood, <strong>and</strong> connectionscontinue <strong>to</strong> be made throughout life, <strong>and</strong>are shaped by experience.The human brain begins its lifelong,developmental journey in the womb; bythe end of the first month the humanfoetus already has a primitive brain which,by nine months’ gestation has formedvirtually all of the neurons that it is everlikely <strong>to</strong> have (Greenfield 1997).It is now known that a baby’s brain is notfully developed at birth, although it arrivesin the world with most, if not all,of itsneurons in place <strong>and</strong> with someconnections between them (synapses)that have permitted basic foetalmovement in the womb, <strong>and</strong> subsequentlyallow vital <strong>and</strong> reflex functions <strong>to</strong> occurneonatally. While a newborn,or neonate’s,brain still has a long way <strong>to</strong> go on itsdevelopmental journey, it is not ‘empty’or inactive. At birth,the process of wiringup synaptic connections (calledsynap<strong>to</strong>genesis) continues <strong>and</strong>accelerates. Webb et al (2001) reviewin neurological detail the process ofpostnatal neuroana<strong>to</strong>mical change <strong>and</strong>its implications for behaviour, <strong>and</strong> theyemphasise that brain development ischaracterised by two main periods, thefirst beginning at conception, <strong>and</strong> thesecond during gestation (the lattercontinuing for up <strong>to</strong> two decades).They observe that many researchers(such as Goldman-Racik 1987; Katz <strong>and</strong>Shatz 1997 cited in Webb et al 2001) haveproposed that early brain developmentinvolves a huge overproduction ofsynaptic connections; some of theseconnections will become redundant <strong>and</strong>are subsequently ‘pruned’ away.Connections that have been repeatedlyused tend <strong>to</strong> be retained, those that havenot been used often are shed. Pruning inthe brain,much as in the garden, not onlyeliminates circuits that are surplus <strong>to</strong>requirements, but also allows theremaining circuits <strong>to</strong> grow bigger <strong>and</strong>stronger. The metaphor of plant growthwas used by Diamond <strong>and</strong> Hopkins (1998)in their synthesis of early brain research<strong>and</strong> information on children’s playfulactivity, Magic Trees of the Mind. A detailedexplanation of the processes ofsynap<strong>to</strong>genesis, synaptic pruning canbe found in Bruer (1997). He also discussesthe ‘windows of opportunity’ otherwiseknown as ‘critical periods’ in braindevelopment. Greenough, Black <strong>and</strong>Wallace (1987) have described t h e s epe ri od s, s aying that ‘it is as if evo l u t i o nhas resulted in neural systems t h at ex pe ct<strong>to</strong> find ce rtain kinds of stimuli in the111


environment in order <strong>to</strong> fine-tune theirperformance (reported in Bruer, 1987:7).The ‘Brainwonders’ website defines criticalperiods as developmental phases that aredependent upon environmental input; itadds that there are differing criticalperiods for ‘the presence of certainnutrients…for certain types of sensorystimuli (such as vision <strong>and</strong> speech sounds),<strong>and</strong> for certain emotional <strong>and</strong> cognitiveexperiences (attachment, languageexposure)’. However, it is also made clearthat ‘there are mental skills, such asreading, vocabulary size, <strong>and</strong> the ability<strong>to</strong> see colour, which do not appear <strong>to</strong> passthrough tight critical periods in theirdevelopment.’ However, yet again therehave been disagreements about thepossible existence of such periods inhuman development. Blakemore (2002: 28)writes that ‘Most neuroscientists nowbelieve that critical periods are not rigid<strong>and</strong> inflexible. Rather, most interpret themas ‘sensitive’ periods.’ Those who believedin the existence of critical periods set inmotion ‘a plethora of videos <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ysclaiming <strong>to</strong> ‘boost’ your baby’s brain,<strong>and</strong>implying beneficial long term effects…such claims currently have scantneuroscience backing, <strong>and</strong> should betreated with caution.’ (Johnson 2002:42).But this does not mean we should treatbabies as if they have no brain nor youngchildren as if they cannot benefit fromeducation (in its broadest sense – notformal schooling) until they are admitted<strong>to</strong> school, as one Times edi<strong>to</strong>rial claimednot long ago, suggesting that the purposeof nursery provision is simply <strong>to</strong> preparechildren,so that ‘the reception class canbegin the proper process of education.’(The Times 1995:17).Importantly, Johnson (2002: 40-41 – ouritalics) draws attention <strong>to</strong> the fact that‘babies brains have sometimes beenviewed as being passively shaped by theirenvironment…because over the first fewmonths they are unable <strong>to</strong> walk or evenreach accurately. Appearances can bedeceiving however. We have seen, forinstance, how, from birth, infants arecapable of directing their eyes <strong>to</strong>wardthings that interest them, such as faces.’This comment sums up the traditionalEnglish view reflected in the saying‘children should be seen <strong>and</strong> not heard’<strong>and</strong> the fact that adults, including youngparents, in Engl<strong>and</strong> who have publicconversations with babies are often lookedat askance. Perhaps one outcome of the‘<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong> Three Matters’ project would bethe promotion of joyful, publicconversation between babies <strong>and</strong> youngchildren <strong>and</strong> their carers in shops, streets,everywhere, <strong>to</strong> push home the messagethat children have brains that are trying <strong>to</strong>make sense of the world from the momen<strong>to</strong>f birth. It is in sharing these ordinaryencounters that brain development ispromoted as well as other aspects such asemotional attachment <strong>and</strong> self-esteem,aswe have already discussed.Neuroscience has started <strong>to</strong> map out theways in which young brains make theconnections that are the key <strong>to</strong> eachindividual child’s mind.112 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


It has also provided useful insights in<strong>to</strong> theways in which environmental influencescan impact upon the brain. Eliot (1999)describes the medical research in<strong>to</strong>environmental influences on the prenatalbrain. She reports that poor nutrition,substances <strong>and</strong> chemicals, alcohol,cigarettes, illegal drugs (includingmarijuana) <strong>and</strong> maternal emotion <strong>and</strong>stress can all have detrimental influencesupon the developing brain. Caffeine,sweeteners <strong>and</strong> MSG were also tested butno ill effects were found. Research byDrewett et al (2001) with malnourishedEthiopian children also found that earlymalnutrition does not have a permanentadverse effect on brain development.By contrast, neuroscience does not offerkeys <strong>to</strong> raising a super-intelligent child;indeed, Bruer (1997) argues thatneuroscience has been incorrectly applied<strong>to</strong> devise cognitive developmentprogrammes that claim <strong>to</strong> boost a youngchild’s intelligence. Discussing criticalperiods in brain development, he statesthat, ‘experience-expectant brain plasticitydoes not depend on specific experiencesin specific environments, <strong>and</strong> for thisreason,does not provide much guidancein choosing <strong>to</strong>ys, preschools, or earlychild-care policies. The experienceschildren need <strong>to</strong> develop fundamentalsensory-mo<strong>to</strong>r <strong>and</strong> language skills occur inany normal environment.’ (Bruer 1997:8).What is evident from neuroscience is that‘normal’ brain development in earlychildhood is dependent uponenvironmental input <strong>and</strong>, for parents <strong>and</strong>carers, this means warm <strong>and</strong> loving,appropriate interaction with children whoare living in a safe context in which theyare nourished <strong>and</strong> nurtured <strong>and</strong> allowedopportunities <strong>to</strong> explore. Gopnik et al(1999) summarise evidence from researchin philosophy, psychology, neuroscience,linguistics <strong>and</strong> other disciplines <strong>to</strong> providean account of how babies <strong>and</strong> youngchildren learn about the world aroundthem,about people <strong>and</strong> relationships, <strong>and</strong>about language, linking their discussion <strong>to</strong>what is known about brain development.They assert that, ‘One thing that sciencetells us is that nature has designed us <strong>to</strong>teach babies, as much as it has designedbabies <strong>to</strong> learn…The scientific researchsays that we should do just what we dowhen we are with our babies – talk, play,m a ke funny face s, p ay at te nt i o n .We justneed time <strong>to</strong> do it.’ ( Gopnik et al 1999 : 202 ).So it would appear that babies <strong>and</strong> youngchildren need <strong>to</strong> play <strong>and</strong> interact withtheir parents <strong>and</strong> other significant peoplein their lives, because it is in theseenjoyable everyday exchanges <strong>and</strong>conversations that their brains develop –are ‘redesigned’ even – as a result oflearning. However, in a society whereparents (<strong>and</strong> other family members) mayhave less time <strong>to</strong> spend in the home, theirchildren could be losing out on the qualitytime that can be spent doing just thosevery simple activities on which their braindevelopment can thrive. Added <strong>to</strong> this arethe pressures of poverty <strong>and</strong> socioeconomicdisadvantage, which may giverise <strong>to</strong> depression in parents. Population113


studies (Richman 1978; Richman et al 1982)found that as many as 20-40% of Britishmothers were suffering from depression(although not all sought medical help),<strong>and</strong> that depressed emotional statesadversely impact upon parent-childrelationships because mothers are lesslikely <strong>to</strong> stimulate their babies <strong>and</strong>children.(See Goleman 1996; Kendall2002 for a summary of evidence on theeffects of maternal mental health ondevelopment in the first twelve monthsof life.) The research seems <strong>to</strong> indicatethat we need <strong>to</strong> ensure parents havesupport when they need it, that theyhave time <strong>to</strong> enjoy being with theirchildren, <strong>and</strong> that they feel assured thatwhen they are at work their babies <strong>and</strong>children are still enjoying interactionswith other key adults <strong>and</strong> children.Some of the main messages for parents,early years practitioners, policy makers<strong>and</strong> researchers about the changes inthinking about early brain development<strong>and</strong> function can be found in Shore’ssummary of the differences between ‘oldthinking’ <strong>and</strong> ‘new thinking’ about thebrain,as shown in Figure 2 on thefollowing page.114 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


FIGURE 2:SHORE’S (1997:18) CHART OF ‘OLD’ AND ‘NEW’THINKING ABOUT THE BRAINOld thinkingHow a brain develops depends onthe genes you are born with.The experiences you have beforethe age of <strong>three</strong> have a limitedimpact on later development.A secure relationship with a primarycaregiver creates a favourablecontext for early development <strong>and</strong>learning.Brain development is linear: thebrain’s capacity <strong>to</strong> learn <strong>and</strong> changegrows steadily as an infantprogresses <strong>to</strong>ward adulthood.A <strong>to</strong>ddler’s brain is much less activethan the brain of a college student.New thinkingHow a brain develops hingeson a complex interplay betweenthe genes you’re born with <strong>and</strong> theexperiences you have.Early experiences have a decisiveimpact on the architecture of thebrain,<strong>and</strong> on the nature <strong>and</strong> exten<strong>to</strong>f adult capacities.Early interactions don’t just createa context; they directly affect the waythe brain is ‘wired’.Brain development is non-linear: thereare prime times for acquiring differentkinds of knowledge <strong>and</strong> skills.By the time children reach the ageof <strong>three</strong>, their brains are twice asactive as those of adults. Activitylevels drop during adolescence.Fi n a l ly, one area of debate about bra i nd eve l o p m e nt co n ce rns the question ofm a l e / female diffe re n ces <strong>and</strong> theiri m p l i cat i o n s. As long ago as 1972 Co ri n n eHu t t, in her book Males <strong>and</strong> Fe m a l e s,discussed the action of adre n a l i n ,w h i c hshe suggested as act i vated by the pre s e n ceof male Y chro m o s o m e s, in the ve ry earlystages of the growth of a foe t u s. Some ofthe main effe cts of this we re thought <strong>to</strong> be(in general) higher levels of aggression <strong>and</strong>easier arousal <strong>to</strong> aggression <strong>and</strong> otherri s ky act i v i ty in males co m p a red withfe m a l e s, <strong>and</strong> more generalised brainsin fe m a l e s, with stronger co n n e ct i o n sbe tween the two hemisphere s. Nat u ra l lythese are issues that need <strong>to</strong> be addre s s e d<strong>and</strong> debate d, be cause whether or nota soc i e ty ove rl ays these re po rte dp ro pensities <strong>to</strong> maximise or minimiset h e m , has implications for the way thes oc i e ty functions <strong>and</strong> raises its yo u n g.115


WHAT IS THE DIFFERENCE BETWEEN BRAINAND MIND?Asting<strong>to</strong>n’s (1999) fundamental interest isin the development of a theory of mind<strong>and</strong>, in explaining the research thatdevelopmental psychologists under<strong>to</strong>ok inthis area in the late 1980s <strong>and</strong> early 1990s,she begins by considering how the mindcan be explained <strong>and</strong> defined. Asting<strong>to</strong>nstates that the answer lies in ‘everyday,commonsense psychology’ or ‘beliefdesirepsychology’ which refers <strong>to</strong> theways in which a person explains <strong>and</strong>predicts another’s actions by thinkingabout his or her ‘beliefs, desires emotions<strong>and</strong> intentions’ (Asting<strong>to</strong>n 1999:2).Asting<strong>to</strong>n adds that thoughts <strong>and</strong> feelings(or states of mind) originate in the brainbut she differentiates between the two,saying that the mind <strong>and</strong> the brain are no<strong>to</strong>ne <strong>and</strong> the same. (Asting<strong>to</strong>n 1999:3) Shealso discusses philosophical ideas aboutthe mind’s existence, as does Greenfield(1997) who differentiates between mind<strong>and</strong> brain <strong>and</strong> reveals that the ‘seeminglyindividual <strong>and</strong> unchanged mind iscompletely at the mercy of the physicalbrain’ (Greenfield 1997:84). In herconcluding thoughts, she adds that themind may be, ‘the evolving personalaspect of the physical brain’ <strong>and</strong>‘consciousness brings the mind alive’(Greenfield 1997:149).Gopnik et al (1999: 175) devoteconsiderable attention <strong>to</strong> the mind/brainsynthesis <strong>and</strong> state that, ‘studying babies’minds…is studying babies’ brains <strong>and</strong> is,so far, the most productive method ofstudying their brains’, but they devoteindividual chapters <strong>to</strong> ‘Children’s Minds’<strong>and</strong> ‘Children’s Brains’. They also draw ananalogy between the human mind <strong>and</strong>a computer, <strong>and</strong> say that little is knownabout how we feel conscious experiences,but it is known that,‘babies begin by translating informationfrom the world in<strong>to</strong> rich, complex, abstract,coherent representations. Thoserepresentations allow babies <strong>to</strong> interprettheir experience in particular ways <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>make predictions about new events.Babies are born with powerfulprogrammes already booted up <strong>and</strong>ready <strong>to</strong> run’ (Gopnik et al 1999: 142)Scientists are themselves in differentminds about whether there is a differencebetween brain <strong>and</strong> mind. Maybe if we usea computer analogy we can, for now, thinkof the brain as the hardware <strong>and</strong> the mindas the software.THE BRAIN, ATTUNEMENT AND AUTISMAttachments or emotional bonds formedbetween children <strong>and</strong> other people havebeen shown <strong>to</strong> be partly ‘environmentexpectant’(genetically programmed) <strong>and</strong>‘environment-dependent’ (requiringexternal stimuli) (see chapter 3). This does,of course, have implications for a varietyof issues relating <strong>to</strong> the feeling <strong>and</strong>expression of emotions. The part of thebrain responsible for emotional <strong>and</strong> socialresponses is located in the amygdala,which, if damaged, leads <strong>to</strong> profoundemotional changes in a person. Eliot (1999:293) reports that ‘amygdala damage or116 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


dysfunction is one of the leadinghypotheses <strong>to</strong> explain autism’. Trevarthenet al (1998:82) concur. They state:-‘In nearly every case of autism, whenappropriate techniques are available, someabnormality in the brain can be found.’Whereas other children learn <strong>to</strong> imitatefrom birth onwards, <strong>and</strong> through thisbegin <strong>to</strong> develop a sense of empathy,autistic children are far less likely <strong>to</strong> imitateothers (Eliot 1999).Goleman (1996) reports that Sternbelieved healthy attachments betweeninfants <strong>and</strong> their mothers were based ona more active contribution <strong>to</strong> emotionalrelationships than pure imitation,<strong>and</strong> are‘enviroment-dependent’. He defined thisactive participation in exchanges as‘attunement’ <strong>and</strong> ‘prolonged absence ofattunement takes a tremendous emotional<strong>to</strong>ll on the child’. Children with autism,however, also have a biological deficiencythat makes attunement difficult orimpossible. Baron-Cohen et al (1985)devised a series of tasks <strong>to</strong> test autisticchildren <strong>and</strong> found that the children no<strong>to</strong>nly were unable <strong>to</strong> recognise the mentalstate of surprise in another person, butthey also were unable <strong>to</strong> attribute a falsebelief <strong>to</strong> another. The compelling evidencesuggested that the autistic childrenappeared not <strong>to</strong> develop a theory of mindin the ways non-autistic children do.Asting<strong>to</strong>n (1999) explains that,‘Like <strong>three</strong>-year-olds, autistic children donot attribute false belief <strong>to</strong> others, cannotdeceive others, do not remember theirown false beliefs, <strong>and</strong> cannot distinguishbetween appearance <strong>and</strong> reality. However,they are not like <strong>three</strong>-year-olds in otherways…Autistic children do not pretend,<strong>and</strong> they find pretence <strong>and</strong> imaginationdifficult <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> in experimentaltasks. They do not underst<strong>and</strong> thedistinction between thoughts <strong>and</strong> things,between real <strong>and</strong> mental entities’(Asting<strong>to</strong>n 1999: 148).B RAIN DEV E LO P M E N T, M E M O RY AND T H EI M P O RTANCE OF NARRATIVES TO MENTAL HEALT HThe dire c<strong>to</strong>r of the Am e ri can Ce nter fo rHuman Deve l o p m e nt, Daniel Siegel (1999),whose wo rk as a paediat rician <strong>and</strong>p s yc h i at rist focuses on individual, f a m i ly<strong>and</strong> co m m u n i ty deve l o p m e nt in the are aof human re l ationships <strong>and</strong> their linkswith biolog i cal proce s s e s, explains howm e m o ry develops in early life, with firstlythe implicit memory being roo ted inm ove m e nt / be h av i o u r, e m o t i o n s, a n dpe rce p t i o n s. Be tween 12 <strong>and</strong> 24 monthsof age, the part of the brain – theh i p pocampus – which provides for thesecond form of memory, maturess o m ew h at. Then explicit memory d eve l o p s,enabling re call <strong>and</strong> a sense of the self thatincludes kn owing about one’s past. Si e g e lclaims that narrat i ve, which he sees asessential <strong>to</strong> healthy emotionald eve l o p m e nt, d e pends on both ty pes ofm e m o ry. In co n s t ru cting au<strong>to</strong> b i og ra p h i ca ln a rrat i ves – s<strong>to</strong> ries about ourselves whichhelp us make sense of our lives – we usea u <strong>to</strong> n oetic co n s c i o u s n e s s, the ability <strong>to</strong> ‘t i m et rave l’ in our minds, which Siegel suggestsbe comes available <strong>to</strong> children in their third117


year of life, when the part of the bra i nkn own as the orbi<strong>to</strong> f ro ntal co rtex be co m e scapable of mediation of this proce s s.Narratives are thought <strong>to</strong> rely on memorythat is consciously accessible, but they arealso influenced by the memories s<strong>to</strong>redimplicitly. Perhaps one reason why s<strong>to</strong>rytelling is so attractive <strong>to</strong> human beings istheir ability <strong>to</strong> draw on these implicitmemories which have been hidden fromus <strong>and</strong> they are often emotionally charged.Narratives can have the effect of helpingorganise the mind, but they can also shapeself-regulation because in developingthem we seek coherence. Narrativesrequire the involvement of both halves ofthe brain,the right – said <strong>to</strong> deal withimagery <strong>and</strong> the left with ‘making sense’/logic. So in narratives the left hemisphere,which seeks <strong>to</strong> make sense of cause <strong>and</strong>effect, interprets <strong>and</strong> shapes the imagesconjured by the right hemisphere.According <strong>to</strong> Siegel (1999) the righthemisphere grows more rapidly <strong>and</strong> ismore active than the left in the earliestyears of life. But by age <strong>three</strong> the twohemispheres have developed sufficiently<strong>to</strong> allow the transfer of information acrossthe brain <strong>and</strong> by age four children are wellable <strong>to</strong> use words <strong>to</strong> tell others about theirinner feelings <strong>and</strong> inclinations. Siegelclaims that narratives are a fundamentalaspect of integration,our ability <strong>to</strong> create acoherent internal interpersonal, family <strong>and</strong>community experience. Narratives are alsoimportant because they help us makesense of other minds – after all, that isessentially what narratives/s<strong>to</strong>ries areabout. Effective interpersonal relationships<strong>and</strong> secure attachments depend onemotional attunement, sharing in theconstruction of narratives, memory talk<strong>and</strong> dialogue involving reflection <strong>and</strong>collaboration <strong>to</strong> repair disruptedinteractions. Bilateral integration, theprocess whereby both hemispheresof the brain engage in informationprocessing <strong>to</strong> ensure adaptive,coordinated functioning, promotescoherent narratives, which,according<strong>to</strong> Siegel are a mark of mental health.SUMMING UP THE IMPLICATIONS OF BRAINRESEARCH FOR ECEC PROFESSIONALSIn summing up her explorations of brainresearch, Meade (2001) pondered on therole of play for brain development. Shesuggests that play is important becauseAll types of development arepractised…it affords appropriateexperience for different regionsof the brain.Play seems <strong>to</strong> have a relationship withthe blooming of synapses.Play of the kind where children’sinterest <strong>and</strong> motivation are optimalseems <strong>to</strong> have a relationship with thesculpting of the brain … these occurwhen children have caregivers who areattuned; activities where the childrendisplay most interest may optimisesynapse stabilisation…because thereis likely <strong>to</strong> be repetition; selectionprocesses as <strong>to</strong> play <strong>to</strong>pics…willactivate the prefrontal cortex <strong>and</strong> limbicsystem <strong>and</strong> therefore conscious118 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


memory; synapses associated withexperiences not chosen will begin <strong>to</strong>wither away…when play is limited,fewer modalities are active <strong>and</strong>emotions such as motivation adverselyaffect brain function – so child-centred,play-based programmes are important.Children usually display high levels ofmotivation in play.Play seems <strong>to</strong> help lay down implicitmemories of skills, dispositions <strong>and</strong>schemas.Play in a complex environment affordschildren lots of opportunities <strong>to</strong> satisfynovel preferences. (Adapted fromMeade 2001: 22-24).Additionally, Meade draws attention<strong>to</strong> children’s need, among other things,for opportunities which allow them <strong>to</strong>develop theories about themselves <strong>and</strong>other people. She advocates educa<strong>to</strong>rsshould be warm, responsive <strong>and</strong> capableof fostering young children’s braindevelopment through appropriateplay activities.Further key messages from the researchare that young brains are exceptionally‘plastic’ so they are shaped by experience<strong>and</strong> the plasticity allows for catching upif development <strong>and</strong> learning arehampered in anyway. However, they arealso incredibly active <strong>and</strong> thirsty forinteractions <strong>and</strong> activities which will fosterfurther learning <strong>and</strong> brain development.EMOTIONAL WELL-BEINGBeing special <strong>to</strong> someoneIn chapters 1 <strong>and</strong> 3 we outlined re s e a rc hon the importance of ‘being special <strong>to</strong>someone/mattering’. Here we look furtherat these aspects of early development <strong>and</strong>learning, through some of the studieswhich inform us about emotionalwell-being.In a rigorous research study in the USA,theNICHD Early Child Care Research Network(1996) observed 576 babies in fivedifferent types of ECEC settings, makingthe first observation when the baby waswithin two weeks of being six months old.The aim of the study was <strong>to</strong> tease out thecharacteristics of settings contributing <strong>to</strong>‘positive’ care-giving – defined in terms ofsensitivity, warmth, <strong>and</strong> being cognitivelystimulating. They identified the structuralaspects of the settings (eg. ratio of adults<strong>to</strong> children,safe environment) as well asqualitative characteristics (eg. stimulation,staff attitudes <strong>and</strong> approaches). The teamfound that the main fac<strong>to</strong>rs associatedwith positive caregiving were:- low adultchildratios – the closer the ratio is <strong>to</strong> oneadult per child, the higher the probabilityof the adult being sensitive <strong>to</strong> individualchildren <strong>and</strong> thus fostering attachment(this fac<strong>to</strong>r is consistent with findingsabout ratios for <strong>to</strong>ddlers by Clarke-Stewartet al 1994); practitioners having nonauthoritarianbeliefs about child-rearing –such workers tended <strong>to</strong> have a highernumber of positive interactions withchildren; the physical environment being119


clean, safe <strong>and</strong> uncluttered <strong>and</strong> havingappropriate materials for play <strong>and</strong>interaction for the age-group. What isspecial about this research is its inclusion,for the first time in such a study, ofinformal care by other family members(fathers, gr<strong>and</strong>parents, nannies). Theresearchers state that it was the inclusionof this form of care which increased thelevels of their results related <strong>to</strong> sensitivity<strong>and</strong> stimulation.Another American in-depth,observationalstudy (Galinsky et al 1994),which focusedsolely on children in home-based care(with a childminder or member of theirown family), issued a warning that in theUSA, children from low income familieswere more likely <strong>to</strong> be in care which gavecause for concern. They found that parentschoose providers who are similar <strong>to</strong>themselves in income <strong>and</strong> ethnicity. Theseresearchers reiterated results like those ofthe NICHD study outlined above – thatcarers need <strong>to</strong> be ‘committed, caring, open<strong>to</strong> learning, <strong>and</strong> purposeful about theimportant work of being a family childcare provider.’ (Galinsky et al 1994: 96). Asa result they recommended that publicbodies should not push reluctant workersin<strong>to</strong> becoming child carers; that thereshould be investments in advocacy <strong>and</strong>education about ECEC; that government<strong>and</strong> businesses should fund child caretraining initiatives <strong>and</strong> help families payfor childcare; that family child careproviders should be entitled <strong>to</strong> access <strong>to</strong>resources enabling them <strong>to</strong> providestimulating learning experiences; thereshould be associations <strong>to</strong> offer social <strong>and</strong>technical support <strong>to</strong> family carers atnational <strong>and</strong> local levels; that anyregula<strong>to</strong>ry system should be providerfriendly<strong>and</strong> inspection visits r<strong>and</strong>om <strong>and</strong>rotating. One important finding from thisextensive study was the fact that thechildren’s mothers, when interviewed infocus groups, expressed their mostessential requirements of a practitioner <strong>to</strong>be exactly those listed by the NICHD team– warmth, attention <strong>to</strong> the child,cleanliness <strong>and</strong> communication (with boththe child <strong>and</strong> themselves). Sadly, this studyfound only 50 per cent of the children <strong>to</strong>be securely attached <strong>to</strong> their child careproviders but they were no more likely <strong>to</strong>be securely attached <strong>to</strong> a carer who was arelative than a non-relative. This figure of50 per cent tallies with that found byHowes <strong>and</strong> Hamil<strong>to</strong>n’s (1993) study ofchildren in group settings. Galinsky et al(1994) concluded that the best way ofensuring children become attached <strong>to</strong>their carers (seen as an essentialpre-requisite of effective provision; seealso the section on resilience in chapter 1<strong>and</strong> discussion in chapter 3), is by drawingin<strong>to</strong> ECEC only those workers who want <strong>to</strong>learn more about being a care-giver, areintentional in their approaches <strong>and</strong> aboveall,are committed.In a longitudinal study of 354 parents <strong>and</strong>their firstborn infants, Laucht et al (1994)examined the relationship betweenchildren’s development <strong>and</strong> parentalpsychopathology (mental illness). Theycompared the social-emotional <strong>and</strong>120 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


cognitive development of the children attwo years of age, comparing them with acontrol group, <strong>and</strong> found that the childrenof the mentally ill parents displayed morebehavioural problems <strong>and</strong> poorerlanguage development. There weredifferences, however, depending on whichparent was ill. The effect of mothers’mental illness impacted on bothsocio-emotional <strong>and</strong> cognitivedevelopment, whereas the impact offathers’ mental illness appeared <strong>to</strong> beconfined <strong>to</strong> cognitive functioning only.In addition <strong>to</strong> the physical healthproblems associated with low birth weight(see further discussion later in thischapter), Weiss et al (2001) suggest thatthe tactile experiences of low birthweightbabies, how they are <strong>to</strong>uched <strong>and</strong>h<strong>and</strong>led, whether they are being accepted– or not – by their parents, may havelonger term effects on their socialadaptation <strong>and</strong> may be linked <strong>to</strong>emotional <strong>and</strong> behavioural problems attwo years of age.Nancy Boyd Webb (1984) investigatedrelationships of young children who hadexperienced multiple-care in their earliestyears. At the time of her research theywere aged between four <strong>and</strong> six years, butsome had had more than twenty carers <strong>to</strong>cover while their parents worked longhours outside home (all the children in herstudy had both parents living at home).Those children who were coping well <strong>and</strong>thriving were found <strong>to</strong> have <strong>three</strong> aspectsof their lives in common. These were:-bugging <strong>and</strong> nudging – parents’ positive‘nagging’ encouraging them <strong>to</strong> displaysome special ability – for example ‘Singthat song for Gr<strong>and</strong>ma’; pet names, whichseemed <strong>to</strong> act as a binding function – <strong>and</strong>of course, use of full names <strong>to</strong> indicatedisapproval showed parental expectation<strong>and</strong> ‘investment’; finally, idiosyncraticbehaviour, meaning shared rituals that thechild or family members have evolved,such as the reading of certain favourites<strong>to</strong>ries or food, promote intimacy. Suchbehaviours are kinds of narratives thatfamilies <strong>and</strong> children co-construct, so onesees echoes of Siegel’s work in the verypractical research applications of BoydWebb’s research.In the Framework ‘<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong> Three Matters’the development points for EmotionalWell-Being are as follows:-Young babies are primed <strong>to</strong> becomesocial beings. They crave closeattachments with a special personwithin their day care setting.Warm, mutual, affirmative relationshipsgive babies the courage <strong>to</strong> expresstheir feelings.When young children have a closerelationship with a warm <strong>and</strong>responsive adult, they explore froma safe place <strong>to</strong> which they can return.As children learn <strong>to</strong> do things forthemselves they gain confidence,knowing that the adult is close by,ready <strong>to</strong> support <strong>and</strong> help if needed.However, poverty <strong>and</strong> other fac<strong>to</strong>rs whichimpinge on family life, creating stress forparents, can also impact on children.121


The effects of poverty on mental healthLinks with low soc i o - e conomic family stat u s<strong>and</strong> poor living conditions we re fre q u e nt lyfound among children needing tre at m e ntfor mental health problems (Goodman<strong>and</strong> Scott 1997 ). Ch i l d ren with be h av i o u rp roblems have also been found <strong>to</strong> bem o re like ly <strong>to</strong> come from disadva nt a g e df a m i l i e s, e s pe c i a l ly those re s i d e nt inn e i g h bo u rh oods where the majori ty offamilies share their low soc i o - e co n o m i cs t atus (Kalff et al 2001 ). A clear diffe re n cein the preva l e n ce of mental disorders inc h i l d ren was found be tween diffe re nts oc i o - e conomic gro u p s. Ch i l d ren in thel owest soc i o - e conomic group we re fivetimes more like ly than others <strong>to</strong> suffer fro ma co n d u ct disorder (Bra d s h aw 2001 :202).The effects of parental separation<strong>and</strong> divorceResearch which claims ill-effects ofparental separation <strong>and</strong> divorce (see forexample, Fürstenberg <strong>and</strong> Cherlin 1991)is countered by qualitative research fromSweden (Berg 2001) which argues thathow the family h<strong>and</strong>les the split, <strong>and</strong> howa society perceives family break-up areguiding fac<strong>to</strong>rs in whether the childrenwill be negatively affected in the longterm. In other words, Berg claims thatchildren can cope well with separation <strong>and</strong>divorce if the social context is supportive<strong>and</strong> accepting rather than negative <strong>and</strong>condemna<strong>to</strong>ry. Clearly there are otherfac<strong>to</strong>rs which will influence children’s longterm well-being that are often present indivorce situations, such as the incidence ofpoverty among one-parent families <strong>and</strong>the absence of positive male role models.The study by Cochran et al (1990) foundthat where the involvement of otheradult male relatives <strong>and</strong> friends wasencouraged, children in general,butparticularly boys, showed positive gainsin relation <strong>to</strong> social <strong>and</strong> academic abilities.(However, it must be noted that thechildren in this study were mostly over<strong>three</strong> years of age.)Emotional abuse <strong>and</strong> its effectsEmotional abuse, as well as beingrecognised in its own right, is usuallyinvolved when children are subjected <strong>to</strong>any other form of abuse, be it physicalabuse, sexual abuse, or neglect. Indeed,the scars of emotional abuse will be thosethat often remain after the other abusehas ended. However, an examination ofthe statistics for referral show thatemotional abuse appears less prevalentthan other forms of abuse. This may bebecause it is difficult <strong>to</strong> pinpoint, althoughin babies failure <strong>to</strong> thrive may be anindica<strong>to</strong>r in some cases, associated witha lack of sensitivity <strong>to</strong> a particular baby’sneeds embedded within a range ofcomplex interactions between physicalproblems, family relationships <strong>and</strong>inappropriate interventions (Underdown2000). However, Herbert (1999: 97-98)stresses that‘it is no<strong>to</strong>riously difficult (not <strong>to</strong> saydangerous) <strong>to</strong> infer causal relationshipsfrom retrospective data…Correlations donot necessarily imply causation…Maternal122 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


ejection – when it is primary – is quitelikely <strong>to</strong> be expressed in rough,hostilefeeding patterns which lead <strong>to</strong> phobicavoidance (food aversion) on the part ofthe child.’A baby who is abused may also show signsof ‘hyperalertness, hyperarousal, <strong>and</strong>numbing’ according <strong>to</strong> Davies (1999: 118).As discussed earlier in the section on braindevelopment, the neural pathways whichcontrol these aspects of self-regulation willbe strengthened through high levels ofuse. Further, Perry (1994) suggests that asa result of constantly expecting danger,the constraint <strong>and</strong> emotional impairmentinvolved will also have a negative effec<strong>to</strong>n a child’s cognitive development.In young children a failure <strong>to</strong> gain weightcommensurate with height (thus fallinglow on a centile scale) might be a sign ofa child’s distress. As with most of the signs<strong>and</strong> symp<strong>to</strong>ms of child abuse, carefulinterpretation tempered with experienceis essential (David 1993; 1993a).PHYSICAL WELL-BEINGGrowing bodies:gaining control <strong>and</strong>acquiring physical skillsBa by It’s Yo u, by Ka rm i l o f f - Smith (1994)is a text <strong>and</strong> pho<strong>to</strong>g raphic acco u nt ofc h i l d re n’s deve l o p m e nt during the firstt h ree years in En g l a n d, p rod u ced <strong>to</strong>a c co m p a ny a te l evision series of the samen a m e. Th e re is not the space in this rev i ew<strong>to</strong> cover phys i cal deve l o p m e nt in the samed e t a i l , so as well as dire cting readers <strong>to</strong>t h at book <strong>and</strong> others, this chapter signalsup the significa nt po i nts in phys i cal grow t hin these earliest years <strong>and</strong> indicate sex pe ri e n ces which, a c co rding <strong>to</strong> re s e a rc h ,seem <strong>to</strong> enhance or co n s t rain healthybod i ly deve l o p m e nt. In part i c u l a r, t h ea d e p t n e s s, capabilities <strong>and</strong> dete rm i n at i o na s s oc i ated with babies <strong>and</strong> young childre nas they seek <strong>to</strong> satisfy their curi o s i ty <strong>and</strong>t a ke part in family life, will be stre s s e d.In the first few hours after birt h , a d re n a l i nl evels are higher than those of someones u f fe ring a heart attack – this is a nat u ra l‘s a fe ty measure’ <strong>to</strong> help the baby co pe withthe trauma of being bo rn <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> ensure allthe baby’s bod i ly org a n s, n ow funct i o n i n gi n d e pe n d e nt ly for the first time, do soe f fe ct i ve ly. Fo l l owing this bout of higha ro u s a l , the baby will sleep, <strong>and</strong> sleepbe comes the main pre oc c u p at i o n ,fo l l owe dby fe e d i n g, for the early weeks of life.Babies are bo rn with re f l exe s, p hys i ca la ctions which occur au<strong>to</strong> m at i ca l ly, most ofwhich will fo rm the basis for later phys i ca la b i l i t i e s. He a ring is almost as sensitive ast h at of an adult <strong>and</strong> babies are usuallys oothed by the familiar sounds they heardwhile in the womb (mother’s vo i ce,m o t h e r’s heart be at, familiar music, e tc ).Their vision is less acute howeve r, l i m i te d<strong>to</strong> about a metre away <strong>and</strong> with a distanceof 20 <strong>to</strong> 25 cm their best foc u s. New bo rnbabies are at t ra cted <strong>to</strong> moving objects <strong>and</strong>t h ey seem <strong>to</strong> arri ve with a simple ‘m od e l’of the human face, <strong>to</strong> which they are alsoat t ra cted (Davies 19 99 ; Ka rm i l o f f - Sm i t h1994 ). Th ey also be come ve ry adept atre cognising facial ex p ressions <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ne of123


vo i ce, i n d i ca<strong>to</strong>rs of emotion, d u ring theirfirst year of life (Nelson 1987 ). Ta s te <strong>and</strong>smell do be come more refined but smell isa c u te enough at birth <strong>to</strong> mean that a babycan identify his mother’s milk when thre ed ays old (Ho f fer 1975) <strong>and</strong> pare nt s’ n at u ra lodours be come pre fe rred <strong>to</strong> others. Du ri n gthese earliest weeks babies are inte n s e lyi nte re s ted in what or who is around thembut they cannot re m ove their at te nt i o nwithout the help of an adult – once loc ke do n<strong>to</strong> a face or an object, t h ey remain fixe du pon that. By <strong>three</strong> or four months old,h oweve r, t h ey begin <strong>to</strong> be able <strong>to</strong> havem o re co nt ro l ,their brains have deve l o pe d,co n n e cted up enough <strong>to</strong> enable them <strong>to</strong>c h oose what <strong>to</strong> look at – or not. This shiftf rom re f l ex - l i ke be h aviour <strong>to</strong> vo l u nt a rya ctions is linked <strong>to</strong> the deve l o p m e nt ofthe area of the brain called the ce re b ra lco rtex . Having ex pe ri e n ces is essential<strong>to</strong> brain deve l o p m e nt – <strong>and</strong> bra i nd eve l o p m e nt is essential <strong>to</strong> taki n ga dva ntage of those ex pe ri e n ce s.‘Between brain <strong>and</strong> behaviour, the baby isprogressively building up what are called‘mental representations’. This complexprocess gradually turns a brain in<strong>to</strong> anindividual mind.’ (Karmiloff-Smith1994:48).Human babies are helpless for a very longperiod compared with other animals, butit is this that allows for the extensiveplasticity of the brain,equipping them <strong>to</strong>reorganise, adapt <strong>and</strong> learn from others.Babies’ arm <strong>and</strong> leg movements, whenthey are lying on their backs, althoughseemingly r<strong>and</strong>om,are a way of practising,strengthening muscles. The first physicalmiles<strong>to</strong>ne is probably being able <strong>to</strong> holdup <strong>and</strong> move one’s own head. This may befollowed by rolling, sitting alone, crawling,st<strong>and</strong>ing, <strong>and</strong> finally walking, after aperiod of mobility made possible byholding on <strong>to</strong> people <strong>and</strong> furniture. Witheach new ability, the baby sees its worldin a new <strong>and</strong> different way. Again,theseexperiences are important not just forgaining physical control <strong>and</strong> developingcoordination, they actually affect brain‘wiring’ <strong>and</strong> this again in turn influenceswhat the baby can do physically. By beingallowed <strong>to</strong> explore through movement,babies ‘map’ their spatial surroundings,making mental images of a place, feelingsecure when they know the location oftheir parent or familiar carer within that‘map’. So it is important <strong>to</strong> ensure thatchildren who have movement difficultiesare enabled <strong>to</strong> explore through whatevermeans are available <strong>to</strong> them. Further it isimportant <strong>to</strong> note that infants who havea restricted ability <strong>to</strong> move independently,either physically due <strong>to</strong>, for example,cerebral palsy or motivationally throughsomething like Down’s syndrome, maydevelop perceptual difficulties which latereffect educational progress, particularlyin reading or maths ( Wilson 1998). Babies<strong>and</strong> young children need <strong>to</strong> move no<strong>to</strong>nly <strong>to</strong> gain knowledge about theirenvironment <strong>and</strong> become oriented, butalso <strong>to</strong> gain knowledge about theirown abilities <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> enjoy the feeling ofindependence. Further, movement itselfhas its own rewards (Davies 2002). Parents<strong>and</strong> carers can help children gain a sense124 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


of control through safe explorations, asSelleck <strong>and</strong> Griffin point out:-‘Physical care <strong>and</strong> loving attention isrequired in different ways as a <strong>to</strong>ddlerbecomes mobile…explora<strong>to</strong>ry behaviour…takes the child away as she crawls, walks<strong>and</strong> inspects the world around her. Theeduca<strong>to</strong>r is required not only <strong>to</strong> protectthe child through closeness, but also <strong>to</strong>let go <strong>to</strong> encourage growing au<strong>to</strong>nomy.’Selleck & Griffin (1996:157).Naturally nutrition as well as activity isimportant here because the baby isgrowing bone, muscle <strong>and</strong> other tissue.However, activity <strong>and</strong> its relationship <strong>to</strong>well-being is the focus of work by GoddardBlythe (2001) <strong>and</strong> Nuttall (1999). WendyNuttall argues from the point of view ofthe Alex<strong>and</strong>er Technique, that placingemphasis on long periods of sittingrelatively still without freedom ofmovement had had a noticeable effec<strong>to</strong>n children’s posture. She adds that the‘slumped <strong>and</strong> hunched posture’ of thechildren is noticeable <strong>and</strong> that they ‘comeup’ when engaged <strong>and</strong> collapse downagain when not engaged or whenchastised. Her observations were mainlywith slightly older children in classroomsbut she found a high level of physicalstress which should alarm early yearspractitioners <strong>and</strong> which has messagesfor those who work with the childrenin younger age groups.In addition <strong>to</strong> this, re s e a rchers haveex p l o red whether being mobile influence scog n i t i ve deve l o p m e nt, or underst<strong>and</strong>ingof the wo rl d, <strong>and</strong> it seems that being able<strong>to</strong> look at the wo rld from diffe re nt anglesd oes improve other aspe cts ofd eve l o p m e nt (Ka rm i l o f f - Smith 1994 ).Howeve r, re s e a rchers at the Un i ve r s i ty ofDublin (Ga rrett et al 2002) re ce nt ly found ina study of over 170 childre n ,t h at a gro u pwhose pare nts or ca rers placed them inb a by- wa l kers from around six months oldwe re slower <strong>to</strong> be come indepe n d e nt lymobile than babies who we re not put in<strong>to</strong>wa l kers – they we re also said <strong>to</strong> have morea c c i d e nt s. While their re s e a rch has be e nchallenged in the same edition of the BMJ,the re s e a rch team members are co nt i n u i n gtheir study <strong>to</strong> find out if ex pe ri e n ce in awa l ker enhances or detra cts from prog re s sin other areas of deve l o p m e nt <strong>and</strong> learn i n g.Research has shown, surprisingly, thateven very young babies can translate fromone way of perceiving (say sight <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>uchor taste) <strong>to</strong> another. Meltzoff <strong>and</strong> Bor<strong>to</strong>n(1979) gave <strong>three</strong> week old babiesdifferently shaped dummies <strong>and</strong> theyfound that after they had sucked on thedummies without being allowed <strong>to</strong> seethem, the infants spent longer looking atthe type of dummy they had sucked ratherthan the other. So having opportunities<strong>to</strong> explore the world through the differentsenses also seems <strong>to</strong> be important.Using h<strong>and</strong>s <strong>and</strong> mouth <strong>to</strong> explore theworld is also important. Two importantabilities usually become part of a youngchild’s physical reper<strong>to</strong>ire at around 12m o nths old. Being able <strong>to</strong> bring two object s<strong>to</strong>gether so that they meet one another isthe first – in fact ‘clapping’ is an example125


of this <strong>and</strong> clapping games <strong>and</strong> songs mayhelp in its development. Picking things up<strong>and</strong> h<strong>and</strong>ling objects, acquiring the ability<strong>to</strong> use the pincer movement which isexclusive <strong>to</strong> humans (Karmiloff-Smith1994), a baby is practising so that by theage of two years, s/he will be proficient inh<strong>and</strong> use in whatever ways are expectedin the familiar family or community group,although feeding oneself may take a littlelonger (Karmiloff-Smith 1994). Using ‘<strong>to</strong>ols’,whether that be a chair <strong>to</strong> st<strong>and</strong> on <strong>to</strong> getat a desired object, or brushing one’s hair,very quickly become part of a child’saccomplishments. This shows that thechild is physically able <strong>to</strong> use the objectsbut also that they are able <strong>to</strong> memorisewhat they want <strong>to</strong> do, remain focused,plan how <strong>to</strong> use the <strong>to</strong>ol <strong>and</strong> so on,whichmeans that different areas of development<strong>and</strong> learning are being used <strong>to</strong>gether.What also helps is the fact that the childno longer has <strong>to</strong> focus on the physical taskinvolved, for most children this has nowbecome unconscious <strong>and</strong> other aspects ofinteractions or their environment can beexplored without having <strong>to</strong> concentrateon posture or balance. So, for practitionersone clear message is:-‘The most important fac <strong>to</strong>rs for healthydevelopment are that you shouldrecognise the skills a child has developed<strong>and</strong> provide plenty of opportunities <strong>to</strong>practise them.’Bruce & Meggitt (2002: 139).Issues related <strong>to</strong> physical developmentBre a s t feeding is cited by Pro fessor He l e nRo be rts (2002; 2001a) as the most effe ct i veway <strong>to</strong> bo l s ter the health of babies. Bo n at iet al (2000) re po rt on the pro te ct i o na f fo rded by bre a s t feeding but add thatb re a s t feeding is linked <strong>to</strong> income (see alsoBMA 1999 ). Th ey note the co m p l exities ofpove rty <strong>and</strong> health, e d u cation <strong>and</strong> soc i a ls e rv i ce s, claiming that the well being ofpoor mothers <strong>and</strong> children wo rl dw i d ewould be improved by increases inb re a s t fe e d i n g. A study ca rried out inAu s t ralia by Ro s s i ter (1998) urges cultura ls e n s i t i v i ty, p a rt i c u l a rly with re f u g e ef a m i l i e s, whose traditions on bre a s t fe e d i n gm ay differ from those of the dominantc u l t u re. Black (1989) also re m i n d sp ra ctitioners of the sensitivity needed here,as in some cultures bre a s t feeding is notbegun until the baby is a few days old, s omothers who wish <strong>to</strong> fo l l ow this tra d i t i o nshould not be assumed <strong>to</strong> be re j e ct i n gb reast fe e d i n g. Howeve r, Ro be rts (2000a)a rgues that English soc i e ty is still notco n d u c i ve <strong>to</strong> bre a s t fe e d i n g, citing eve nG Ps’ s u rg e ries as failing <strong>to</strong> prov i d ea p p ro p ri ate ly for this po s s i b i l i ty. As thisrev i ew indicates in the section on pove rty,mothers from disadva ntaged inco m eg roups are less like ly <strong>to</strong> bre a s t feed thanthose from more affluent groups in soc i e ty,d e s p i te the claims of medical re s e a rc hi n d i cating adva ntages for their babies.Roberts adds that many measures duringthe last 100 years have improved children’ssurvival rates – for example, immunisation,begun in 1940,has had a dramatic impac<strong>to</strong>n death through infectious diseases; lowbirth weight, associated with many laterproblems, has declined; <strong>and</strong> deaths in thefirst year of life for both boys (who are at126 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


greater risk) <strong>and</strong> girls halved between1971 <strong>and</strong> 1991. But even in relation <strong>to</strong>these statistics there are socio-economicclass differences, as we report later.Improvements in medical skills also meanan increase in the survival rates of childrenborn with special needs compared with afew decades ago. While their survival rateshould be met with joy, the extra cost ofsupport <strong>and</strong> other implications for theirfamilies, siblings, children themselves <strong>and</strong>their communities (both local <strong>and</strong>national) must also be acknowledged.As we pointed out in chapter 1,childrenwho inherit medical conditions ordisabilities can demonstrate resilience <strong>and</strong>develop strengths which they use <strong>to</strong>inform society <strong>and</strong> support youngerchildren in similar situations (Mason 1992).Yet for some children, a kaleidoscope ofproblems in their families, their homeenvironments <strong>and</strong> related <strong>to</strong> their ownbirth weight, appears <strong>to</strong> link with theirdisplaying developmental delays <strong>and</strong>behaviour problems (Feldman et al 2000).In other areas of childre n’s lives dire ct lyre l ated <strong>to</strong> health, such as nutri t i o n ,Gra h a m’s (1984) re s e a rch showed thatfood is assoc i ated with affe ction <strong>and</strong> love,<strong>and</strong> that both children <strong>and</strong> adults usefood as measures of co m m i t m e nt. If thefoods used <strong>to</strong> this end are not thosewhich provide the best diet, it can be hardfor those invo lved <strong>to</strong> change this pat te rnof eat i n g. Neve rt h e l e s s, the Jo s e p hRow nt ree funded study Food Pro j e cts <strong>and</strong>How they Wo rk ( Mc Glone et al 1999) hasi n d i cated that access <strong>to</strong> cheap, n u t ri t i o n a lfood helps <strong>to</strong>wa rds improving purc h a s i n g<strong>and</strong> eating pat te rn s. The Gove rn m e nt’sp rovision of vouchers should help in thisre s pe ct. This is espe c i a l ly impo rt a ntbe cause re ce nt surveys of the diets ofc h i l d ren aged be tween birth <strong>and</strong> five haves h own the inadequacy of babies’ a n dyoung childre n’s fruit int a ke, <strong>and</strong> thatthose in low income groups we re 50 pe rce nt less like ly <strong>to</strong> eat fruit <strong>and</strong> ve g e t a b l e sthan those in the higher income gro u p s(BMA 1999 ). Fu rt h e r, vitamin D co nt ri b u te s<strong>to</strong> building strong teeth <strong>and</strong> bones <strong>and</strong>its lack is implicated in ri c ke t s. Howeve r,re s e a rch by Lawson <strong>and</strong> Thomas (1999)i n d i cates that there are co n ce rns abo u tAsian (Ba n g l a d e s h i , Indian or Pa kistani ino rigin) childre n , in their study two ye a ro l d s, whose levels of vitamin D we rebe l ow the level thought nece s s a ry <strong>to</strong>p reve nt such phys i cal pro b l e m s.The British Medical Association (BMA 1999)report on a 16 year study by the MedicalResearch Council. They state that nutritionhas a proven effect on cognitivedevelopment <strong>and</strong> that this effect wasparticularly pronounced in boys.In addition they review a meta-analysis ofresearch on the effects of eating breakfas<strong>to</strong>n cognitive functioning, concluding thatthe omission of breakfast seems <strong>to</strong> alterbrain function.Being born pre-termIn a study of 36 <strong>three</strong> year old children,of whom 18 had been born pre-term,Walker (1989) found the pre-term grouphad more problems, such as sleep related127


difficulties, temper tantrums <strong>and</strong> beingoverly dependent. As one might expect,Walker argues that since there was noevidence of neurological delay, thesebehaviours may be the result of parentalmanagement, rather than intra-childproblems. Parents are naturally moreanxious about babies who have been bornpre-term <strong>and</strong> who may have been kept inhospital during their first weeks of life <strong>and</strong>this may result in parents being unable <strong>to</strong>set appropriate limits on their children’sbehaviour. Similarly, Walker’s finding abouthigher levels of separation anxiety seemlikely <strong>to</strong> relate <strong>to</strong> ‘vulnerable childsyndrome’ resulting from a continuation ofparents’ early fears for their child’s life. Butcan this account for Walker’s finding thatchildren from manual socio-economicgroups display significantly morebehaviour problems than those fromnon-manual groups? Walker’s researchalso found that mothers from manualgroups had significantly poorer mentalhealth <strong>and</strong> this finding ties in with thoseof other earlier studies (eg. Richman 1978;Stevenson 1985) which found strongassociations between disturbance inchildren <strong>and</strong> mothers’ depression.However, Walker did not find that thosemothers with mental health problemswere the ones who had children withbehaviour problems. In her study themothers’ depression tended <strong>to</strong> be related<strong>to</strong> a recent event (such as bereavement)<strong>and</strong> most were not expected <strong>to</strong> be of longduration. Thus Walker’s study suggests thatparents of pre-term children need advice<strong>and</strong> support, for example from HealthVisi<strong>to</strong>rs, but that other remedies arenecessary <strong>to</strong> break the link between lowsocio-economic status <strong>and</strong> children’sbehaviour problems.Poverty <strong>and</strong> its effects on bodily healthNumerous ill effects have been reportedas linked <strong>to</strong> poverty for babies <strong>and</strong> youngchildren. Firstly, it was recognised yearsago that poverty impacts on the mother’shealth, so children are affected evenbefore birth ( Townsend <strong>and</strong> Davidson1982). Higher infant mortality rates are alsolinked <strong>to</strong> socio-economic disadvantage(Spencer 2000) <strong>and</strong> although infantmortality rates have reduced in the‘developed’/minority world, the higherrates are still found among the lessaffluent (Leon et al 1992; Pearl et al 2001).Smoking, risky levels of consumption ofalcohol <strong>and</strong> caffeine, <strong>and</strong> other (socioeconomic)fac<strong>to</strong>rs are found <strong>to</strong> be moreprevalent among poorer sections ofsociety <strong>and</strong> it is among these families thatlow birth-weight (LBW) is found mostfrequently (Spencer et al 1999). In fact,according <strong>to</strong> Spencer (2000), death amongbabies under one year old is said <strong>to</strong> be themost sensitive indica<strong>to</strong>r of the health of apopulation. In Engl<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> Wales, SuddenInfant Death Syndrome (SIDS) is <strong>three</strong> timeshigher in the lowest socio-economic groupthan in the highest (Basso et al 1997;Woodroffe et al 1993; Zimmer-Gembeck<strong>and</strong> Helford 1996). Babies with a birthweight of less than 1.5 Kg have a risk ofdeath that is 200 times higher than tha<strong>to</strong>f those weighing 3 Kg or more (Haddad128 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


et al 2000). Further, lone mothers, amongwhom the incidence of smoking is higherthan for any other group, have double therisk of having a baby with a low birthweight than more affluent mothers (Leonet al 1992) <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> experience SIDS (Blairet al 1996; Haglund <strong>and</strong> Cnattingius 1990).Barry <strong>and</strong> Kirke’s (1997) research showedt h at mothers who are be t ter educated aremore likely <strong>to</strong> take folic acid duringpregnancy <strong>to</strong> help promote healthygrowth in the womb <strong>and</strong> that higherinfant mortality rate is also linked <strong>to</strong>inherited <strong>and</strong> congenital difficulties suchas heart disease. If the baby lives in adisadvantaged neighbourhood, then thiscompounds the other fac<strong>to</strong>rs (Reading etal 1993; Wasserman et al 1998). Teenagemothers are <strong>three</strong> times more likely thanolder women <strong>to</strong> have a premature baby orstillbirth (Smith <strong>and</strong> Pell 2001). In theNetherl<strong>and</strong>s, the highest infant mortalityrate is found among ethnic minorities(Van Enk et al 1998).Diet is a further fac<strong>to</strong>r indicat i n gd i s c repancies be tween more <strong>and</strong> lessa f f l u e nt families. Fi r s t ly, b re a s t fe e d i n g,which can provide pro te ction as well asa p p ro p ri ate nutrition in the earliest mont h sof life, is less preva l e nt among families ind i s a dva ntaged circ u m s t a n ces (Ma rtin et al1987 ). Foods high in ca l o ries but with littlen u t ritional co nte nt fo rm the diets of morec h i l d ren in lower soc i o - e conomic gro u p sthan those living in families that are be t te roff <strong>and</strong> these same children are less like ly<strong>to</strong> have adequate phys i cal act i v i tya c co rding <strong>to</strong> Ki n ra et al (2000).Howeve r, another study by De Sp i e g e l a e reet al (1998) found no link be tween lowe rs oc i o - e conomic status <strong>and</strong> children be i n gu n h e a l t h i ly ove rwe i g ht (obe s e ).While it might be thought unlikely thatchildren under <strong>three</strong> would be out in thestreet alone, their chances of being injuredin a road traffic accident are much higherfor those living in disadvantaged families<strong>and</strong> areas than for the more affluent. Thesame is true for other injuries sustained bychildren (Hasselberg et al 2001; Roberts<strong>and</strong> Pless 1993; Roberts <strong>and</strong> Power 1996;Spencer et al 1993; Walsh <strong>and</strong> Jarvis 1992).Among pre-school children the accidentrate is higher among those from singleparent households, or one where themother is unwell,than for children livingwith two healthy adults (Wadsworth et al1983). Children living in temporaryaccommodation,in refuges for example,have a very high injury rate according <strong>to</strong>Webb et al (2001).Children’s health is also threatened by theconditions in which disadvantagedfamilies often live. Risk from atmosphericpollution <strong>and</strong> poor nutrition are associatedwith household disadvantage (Gordon etal 1999; Koopman et al 2001; Martin et al1987). Similarly, inferior housing is linked<strong>to</strong> children’s rates of illness (Fall et al 1997;Patel et al 1994; Polnay <strong>and</strong> Hull 1998;Thompson et al 2001). At the same time,it is reported that the number of visits <strong>to</strong>GPs’ surgeries may represent only the tipof the iceberg in this respect <strong>and</strong> are nota good measure of rates of infection <strong>and</strong>illness (Baker et al 1998). In fact this129


situation appears <strong>to</strong> show that there hasbeen little change since Sir Douglas Black’sreport (Black 1980),which stated thatfamilies from low socio-economic groupsare less likely <strong>to</strong> use preventive orscreening services than those in highersocio-economic groups.Lynch (1998),welcoming the recentAcheson Inquiry in<strong>to</strong> Health,sees thereport as an opportunity <strong>to</strong> influencepolicy <strong>to</strong> improve the health of the nationthroughout childhood <strong>and</strong> in<strong>to</strong> adult life.The key points from the Framework ‘<strong>Birth</strong><strong>to</strong> Three Matters’ <strong>to</strong> be borne in mind hereare that:-Young babies thrive when both theirnutritional <strong>and</strong> emotional needsare met.For babies <strong>and</strong> children, rest <strong>and</strong> sleepare as important as food.Young children have a biological drive<strong>to</strong> use their bodies <strong>and</strong> develop theirphysical skills.Children only gradually gain controlof their whole bodies.KEEPING SAFESelf regulation <strong>and</strong> internalising rulesIn the earliest months of life a baby isbecoming paradoxically, at one <strong>and</strong> thesame time, more alert <strong>and</strong> more settled,having bodily rhythms <strong>to</strong> which a sensitiveadult will respond, creating an effectivecontext for growth,development <strong>and</strong>learning (Davies 1999; Emde et al 1976;Stern 1977). According <strong>to</strong> Davies (1999),during the first <strong>three</strong> months of life,regulation is about those body rhythms<strong>and</strong> about regulation of arousal. Parents<strong>and</strong> carers help babies <strong>to</strong> establishpatterns of sleeping <strong>and</strong> wakefulness,as the child’s own mechanisms (suchas the circadian rhythm regulatingwakefulness during hours of daylight)begin <strong>to</strong> take effect. Parents <strong>and</strong>practitioners who are responsive <strong>and</strong>predictable, foster a baby’s awareness thatcomfort <strong>and</strong> food can be depended uponwithin a certain time frame, but it is thecarer’s responsibility <strong>to</strong> lay thegroundwork on which the later ability<strong>to</strong> cope will rest. Thumb sucking or usinga dummy independently are earlystrategies <strong>to</strong> deal with waiting or anxiety,until an adult dependably satisfies theneed causing arousal. Turning away whenaroused <strong>to</strong> the point of distress is anotherself-regula<strong>to</strong>ry mechanism. Illness or beingpremature can mean that a baby’s centralnervous system,implicated in this ability<strong>to</strong> begin <strong>to</strong> be an active participant in thisself-regulation,may be underdeveloped<strong>and</strong> self-regulation may take longer insuch cases (Minde 1993).(See chapter3 for further discussion of the importanceof sensitive, affectionate interaction <strong>and</strong>the development of attachment linked<strong>to</strong> self regulation.)Parents with ‘difficult’ babies oftenrespond <strong>to</strong> their infant’s irritability innegative ways, rather than adapting <strong>to</strong> thebaby. These parents then end up in a cycleof negative feedback – <strong>to</strong> both parent <strong>and</strong>child – which interferes with the process of130 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


ecoming securely attached (Seifer <strong>and</strong>Sameroff 1986;<strong>and</strong> see chapter 3). Adultswho repeatedly respond with empathy,affection <strong>and</strong> emotional support whenbabies <strong>and</strong> young children are distressed,help children gain positive views ofthemselves <strong>and</strong> make them feel valuedby others (Barrett <strong>and</strong> Campos 1987;Thompson 1991).Adult support during the first <strong>three</strong> yearshelps children cope with powerful feelings<strong>and</strong> fears, <strong>and</strong>, especially, with frustrationat being unable <strong>to</strong> complete self-chosentasks. As children become proficient inlanguage they can use this <strong>to</strong> reduce thestrength of their emotions according <strong>to</strong>Bruner <strong>and</strong> Lucariello (1989),sincerepresenting the world in thoughts <strong>and</strong>words begins <strong>to</strong> give them the power <strong>to</strong>think about consequences of an action,but before the age of <strong>three</strong> this ability willstill be rudimentary. Young children willalso look at adults <strong>to</strong> whom they areattached when they know that they areabout <strong>to</strong> break one of the adult’s rules(Lieberman 1993), because they can nowrecall what the adult approves <strong>and</strong>disapproves. This finding ties in with theresearch on brain development whichsuggests that it is not until 18 months ofage that a young child can appreciate tha<strong>to</strong>ther people have different ‘minds’ fromtheir own (Gopnik et al 1999).Being safe <strong>and</strong> protectedFor children under <strong>three</strong>, it is theresponsibility of the adults around them<strong>to</strong> ensure they are in safe environments.Lindon (1999) provides a useful review ofsafety issues <strong>and</strong> discusses the ways inwhich parents <strong>and</strong> practitioners caninvolve children in awareness about theirown safety <strong>and</strong> that of younger siblings orfriends. As Lindon says,‘Gi rls <strong>and</strong> boys need ca re <strong>and</strong> supe rv i s i o nt h roughout the years of childhood whent h ey have a limited underst<strong>and</strong>ingof common risks in their immediateenvironment. There are real dangers<strong>to</strong> children from household or trafficaccidents <strong>and</strong> considerable scope forresponsible adults <strong>to</strong> reduce the levelof preventable injuries <strong>and</strong> deaths…However,…it is counter-productive <strong>to</strong>focus on keeping children away fromevery risk,however slight. We are likely<strong>to</strong> create a boring environment for them,without challenge or excitement. We alsoproject an image of ourselves as peoplewho block children’s interests <strong>and</strong>curiosity.’ (Lindon 1999:5).A quarter of a century ago Ainsworth et al(1974) concluded that humans, like otherapes, have an inbuilt mechanism forresponding <strong>to</strong> a parent’s danger cry. Theylinked this response of moving closer,or clinging, <strong>to</strong> the adult (usually the mother)when she signalled imminent danger.It is not possible <strong>to</strong> cover further aspectsof safety in this text, although road <strong>and</strong>other injuries are mentioned later inreviewing research on a number of issues.The numbers of babies <strong>and</strong> youngchildren in the child abuse statisticsindicates <strong>to</strong> us the seriousness of this131


issue. In March 1998, 9 per cent of thechildren on the at risk register were agedunder one year old, 30 per cent were agedbetween one <strong>and</strong> four (BMA 1999).Generally speaking, parents who subjecttheir very young children <strong>to</strong> abuse oftendeny the child’s needs, have unrealisticexpectations of babies <strong>and</strong> youngchildren’s abilities, are more punitive <strong>and</strong>less flexible in their parenting (Caselles<strong>and</strong> Milner 2000). The majority of abusedchildren fail <strong>to</strong> develop secureattachments (Alex<strong>and</strong>er 1992; Barnett et al1999; Briere 1992;Ciccetti et al 1988;Crittendon 1992) <strong>and</strong> neglected childrenhave had little support in developingself-regulation <strong>and</strong> routines (Egel<strong>and</strong> et al1983). Babies who have been abused <strong>and</strong>thus traumatised, do not yet have thecognitive abilities <strong>to</strong> think about or thelanguage <strong>to</strong> speak about theirexperiences. Thus it is suggested they willhave developed ways of coping that meanthey become hyper-alert as well as numb<strong>to</strong> emotional involvement, because theycan no longer trust. When young childrencannot put their feelings in<strong>to</strong> words theywill act out <strong>and</strong> resort <strong>to</strong> aggressive <strong>and</strong>negative interactions. Maltreated childrenseem <strong>to</strong> view the world as a dangerousplace where they are unloved, <strong>and</strong> so theyhave difficulty forming positiverelationships. These findings are supportedby Crittenden (1992) <strong>and</strong> Zeanah (1993).Mor<strong>to</strong>n <strong>and</strong> Browne (1998) have reviewedthe literature on attachment <strong>and</strong> childmaltreatment, in relation <strong>to</strong>intergenerational experience of abusiveparenting. They <strong>to</strong>o support the view thatearly maltreatment impacts on the longterm ability <strong>to</strong> form relationships. Schoolagechildren who have been abused whenvery young, tend <strong>to</strong> be more self-centred<strong>and</strong> generally have a reduced capacity forempathy (Vondra et al 1989; 1990).Abused <strong>and</strong> maltreated children are at ahigher risk of school failure <strong>and</strong> droppingout of school later in life. They appear lessmotivated <strong>to</strong> achieve. However, there aredifferences linked <strong>to</strong> the outcomes ofdifferent types of abuse, with neglectedchildren,who are often rejected by theirpeers, having the poorest academicachievement, <strong>and</strong> rarely displaying joy,humour <strong>and</strong> emotional reactions(Eckenrode et al 1993; Haynes-Seman <strong>and</strong>Baumgarten 1998;Kurtz et al 1993).‘Listening <strong>to</strong> young children throughobserving their play <strong>and</strong> talking abouttheir drawings has a particular significancefor those working with children who havebeen abused, neglected or traumatised…play is a powerful means of developinga relationship with a child <strong>and</strong> providingopportunities <strong>to</strong> observe the child'sconcerns <strong>and</strong> preoccupations.’Pugh & Selleck (1996: 123)Important measures (Lier 1997; Lowenthal1999) <strong>to</strong> prevent abuse <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> promoteresilience in the earliest years include theavailability of alternative caregivers; localsupport networks <strong>and</strong> informal supportsystems for parents (other familymembers, friends) (Barnett et al 1999;Kotch 1997); formal support systems, suchas home visiting (Barnett et al 1999) <strong>and</strong>intensive parent education programmes132 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


(Whipple 1999);training projects for earlyyears workers (Culp et al 1991; Daro 1993).The NSPCC’s (2000) recent guide forpractitioners includes data on the nature<strong>and</strong> prevalence of abuse, family risk fac<strong>to</strong>rs<strong>and</strong> sudden infant death. It providesguidance for parents <strong>and</strong> professionals.Since a baby in the UK under one year oldis four times more likely <strong>to</strong> die at the h<strong>and</strong>of another human being (Butler 1996),knowledge about babies <strong>and</strong> children <strong>and</strong>the prevention of abuse is vital for parents<strong>and</strong> professionals.In some cases, children displayingdisruptive <strong>and</strong> apparently hyperactivebehaviour, or extremes of hyperactivity<strong>and</strong> periods of being very withdrawnin an ECEC setting, may alert staff <strong>to</strong>domestic violence. The abuse may be‘indirect’ <strong>and</strong> in families where anotherchild is the focus of that violence, theChildren Act 1989‘s grave concerncategory may be invoked.Current research (Cox forthcoming)indicates that parents of older childrenplace paedophilia as one of their greatestconcerns. Yet West (2000) argues thatsuch anxieties are fuelled by presssensationalism <strong>and</strong> that the statisticsused are misleading, since criminalstatistics do not confirm an escalation.Re s e a rch on inte ra g e n cy pra ct i ce (Tay l o r<strong>and</strong> Daniel 1999) in Scotl<strong>and</strong> reveals apossible gap be tween the re s ponses ofhealth <strong>and</strong> social ca re pro fe s s i o n a l s. Th e s ere s e a rchers state that although there arekn own <strong>to</strong> be links be tween neglect <strong>and</strong>emotional abuse <strong>and</strong> failure <strong>to</strong> thri ve,some pra ctitioners are not re cog n i s i n gthe po te ntial risks for some of the childre nwho are not thri v i n g. Ti te (1993) fo u n dt h at early years pra ctitioners in Ca n a d awould often fail <strong>to</strong> call on otherp ro fessionals in abuse cases where theybe l i eved they could suppo rt the pare nt s<strong>and</strong> help them ove rcome their difficulties.While Walsh (2002) in Au s t ralia has arg u e dt h at early years pra ctitioners are ofte nm a rg i n a l i zed by other pro fe s s i o n a l s.Howeve r, as David (1993a.) has state d,ECEC pra ct i t i o n e r s, g i ven re l eva nti n fo rm at i o n ,t raining <strong>and</strong> suppo rt are themost info rmed about babies <strong>and</strong> yo u n gc h i l d ren genera l ly <strong>and</strong>, a p a rt from thec h i l d re n’s pare nts <strong>and</strong> family, the mostkn owledgeable about individual childre nwith whom they wo rk . Often staff mayh ave a ‘gut fe e l i n g’ a bout a child be fo ret h ey be come conscious of any signs ors y m p <strong>to</strong>ms that a child is witnessing orbeing subjected <strong>to</strong> abuse. All ECECp ra ctitioners need designate d,ex pe ri e n ced colleagues with whom theycan discuss their co n ce rns in co n f i d e n ce.This is espe c i a l ly impo rt a nt fo rchildminders who may feel isolated <strong>and</strong>anxious about the child in question <strong>and</strong>a bout their re s po n s i b i l i t i e s.The developmental points for Keeping Safe,one of the components of the aspectA Healthy Child, include:-Young babies make strong <strong>and</strong>purposeful movements. They tend not<strong>to</strong> stay in the position in which theywere placed.133


Beginning <strong>to</strong> walk, climb <strong>and</strong> run withlittle sense of danger, babies focus onwhat they want.Given opportunities <strong>to</strong> practise whatthey can do in safe surroundings, youngchildren learn some sense of danger.Children’s need for affection, attention<strong>and</strong> being special in some way makesthem particularly vulnerable in relation<strong>to</strong> keeping safe.Ch i l d ren who have not ex pe ri e n ced wa rm ,l oving re l ationships <strong>and</strong> who crave affe ct i o nare often those targeted by abusers. As wehave seen in earlier chapters, positiveemotional attachments seem <strong>to</strong> underlieso many areas of human development.DIFFICULT BEHAVIOUR AND WHAT IT MAY MEANWalker-Hall <strong>and</strong> Sylva have reviewedwhat works for families of children withbehaviour problems. They argue thatstudies of children <strong>and</strong> infants withproblematic behaviour generally suggestthese are caused by environmental fac<strong>to</strong>rs,‘of perhaps which parents are the mostprominent source. Aggressive fathers…,exposure <strong>to</strong> violence, high rates ofmaternal punishment…,mothers withpsychiatric conditions…<strong>and</strong> generalmarital discord/stressful homeenvironment…have been associated withconduct problems appearing early in achild’s life…low-quality parenting hasrepeatedly been associated withbehaviour problems…low-qualityparenting is characterised as beinginconsistent, authoritarian, lacking inwarmth, uninvolved, physically punitive,<strong>and</strong> not providing adequate care.’Walker-Hall <strong>and</strong> Sylva (2001: 162).It appears that some parents are morenegative in their interactions with theirchildren,or ignore them until thesechildren learn that it is only by whiningthat they gain attention <strong>and</strong> that althoughthis may eventually lead <strong>to</strong> beingsmacked, this is preferable <strong>to</strong> beingignored. The British Medical Association(BMA 1999) state that increasing researchattention is being paid <strong>to</strong> this subject <strong>and</strong>evidence is accumulating that smacking isnot an effective child care practice. Theyargue that it can lead <strong>to</strong> an increase inviolent <strong>and</strong> aggressive behaviour. The firstmeta-analysis of physical punishment,covering 88 studies, shows consistentlythat while smacking may ‘secure children’simmediate compliance, it also increasesthe likelihood of negative outcomes.’(BMA 1999: 128).Gardner (1994) observed that mothers ofchildren with behaviour problems initiatedactivities less frequently than othermothers <strong>and</strong> made fewer attempts <strong>to</strong>maintain shared activity once begun.These mothers used rigid instructions,or orders, <strong>to</strong> their children, whereas othermothers used around four times as manyrequests <strong>and</strong> engaged in more than <strong>three</strong>times as much ‘teaching’ (positive,supportive contingent interactions <strong>to</strong> helpchildren underst<strong>and</strong> something). Many ofthe difficult behaviours encountered withfive year olds thus appear <strong>to</strong> have theirroots in the interactions childrenexperienced at one <strong>and</strong> two years of age.134 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


As we have found over <strong>and</strong> over againthroughout this review, babies <strong>and</strong> youngchildren are primed <strong>to</strong> try <strong>to</strong> ‘make sense’of the context in which they findthemselves <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> be responsive <strong>to</strong> warm,loving adults with whom they haveformed their primary attachments.Thus this leads us <strong>to</strong> ask,as Rodd (1996)did – what is ‘normal behaviour’ forchildren aged one <strong>and</strong> two, since thisseems <strong>to</strong> trigger parents’ unfortunatereactions. Jill Rodd argues that it isimportant <strong>to</strong> be informed about children’sdevelopment during this phase, forexample, because‘Sensitive early childhood professionalsunderst<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ddlers’ needs for choice<strong>and</strong> decision making opportunities. Theyunderst<strong>and</strong> that a two year old might evensay no when she means yes <strong>and</strong> are<strong>to</strong>lerant of <strong>to</strong>ddlers’ changes of mind. Theyprovide opportunities for <strong>to</strong>ddlers <strong>to</strong>cooperate by not dem<strong>and</strong>ing compliancewith a dem<strong>and</strong> but rather by offeringsimple alternatives for how the needs ofthe situation might be met. …If childrendo not feel that they are au<strong>to</strong>nomous <strong>and</strong>independent beings, they may displaydevelopmentally immature <strong>and</strong>dependent behaviours <strong>and</strong> will find itdifficult <strong>to</strong> display initiative later’ (Rodd1996:22-23 – our italics).In the light of the research conclusionsoutlined above it is important <strong>to</strong> note thedebate about ADHD (Attention DeficitHyperactive Disorder) <strong>and</strong> the use of thedrug Ritalin (usually with school agechildren).(See for example Llana <strong>and</strong>Crismon 1999; Kong 1995;Kwasman et al1995; Marshall 2000; Tillery et al 2000).It would seem that whatever the cause,problems in the early years of childhoodmay indicate potential for difficulties (suchas antisocial behaviour, or mental illness)later in life, according <strong>to</strong> the BMA (1999).They add that effective interventions inthe form of non-stigmatising parentingprogrammes in which any parent mayenrol are proving effective. Examples ofvoluntary sec<strong>to</strong>r support for parentsinclude:- Homestart; Newpin; Pippin;Positive Parenting; Mellow Parenting.EARLY INTERVENTION STUDIES AIMED ATIMPROVING INFANT HEALTH AND WELL BEINGHow theories have influencedintervention programmesA comprehensive study of earlyintervention programmes, research <strong>and</strong>theory in the USA (Meisels <strong>and</strong> Shonkoff1990), explored different approaches <strong>to</strong>intervention, <strong>and</strong> their underpinningrationales. In this collection of papers,Gallagher (1990) explains five differentperspectives, which are based on differentassumptions, <strong>and</strong> which form the basis fordifferent programmes. Most programmesadopt approaches based on a mix of theseassumptions. The assumptions are:-Changing children will change otherfamily members (children withdevelopmental delay may have anegative impact on the family; a familythat is in trouble will respond better ifthe child’s needs are being met);135


Providing information <strong>and</strong> teachingparenting skills can change families(parents may have inaccurate orincomplete information, be frustratedat their own inability <strong>to</strong> enhance theirchild’s behaviour or learning);Personal counselling can changefamilies (people can have negativeperceptions of themselves <strong>and</strong> theirabilities);Increasing parental empowerment canchange families (better resources,changing a poor environment, reducingstress <strong>and</strong> sharing responsibility reducethe impact of an unhealthy context).(Adapted from Gallagher 1990:543).In a rev i ew of the re s e a rch ev i d e n ce one a rly inte rve ntions <strong>to</strong> enhance the ment a lhealth of children <strong>and</strong> their familiescompiled for the Me ntal He a l t hFo u n d at i o n ,Ba rnes <strong>and</strong> Fre u d e - La g eva rd i(2002) conclude that prog rammes need <strong>to</strong>be po s i t i ve, n o n - d e f i c i t, c u l t u ra l ly sensitive,s ys temic (based on an eco l og i cal mod e l )<strong>and</strong> so matched <strong>to</strong> part i c i p a nt s’ n e e d s.Such prog rammes need <strong>to</strong> be targ e ted onat - risk co m m u n i t i e s, those with high rate sof pove rty, single <strong>and</strong> adolesce ntp a re nt h ood, <strong>and</strong> invo lve both children <strong>and</strong>p a re nt s, without stigmatising individuals.Their rev i ew indicates that bo t hp ro fessionals <strong>and</strong> para - p ro fe s s i o n a l s,wo rking alongside families, should sharedecision making <strong>and</strong> that both pre- <strong>and</strong>po s t - n atal inte rve ntions which do not try<strong>to</strong> ope rate on <strong>to</strong>o many fro nts at the sametime are like ly <strong>to</strong> prove most effe ct i ve. On eof their most telling state m e nts co n ce rn sthe impo rt a n ce of ‘a reasonable sat i s f y i n gt h e ra peutic re l at i o n s h i p’ ( Ba rnes <strong>and</strong>Fre u d e - La g eva rdi 2002: 46 ), w h i c h , if note s t a b l i s h e d, cannot be co m pe n s ated fo rt h rough lengthy inte rve nt i o n s. In otherwo rd s, ra p po rt be tween wo rkers <strong>and</strong>families is essential (Se i fer et al 1991 ).Fawcett’s (2001) review of early childhoodinterventions related <strong>to</strong> special needs forthe DfES concludes, inter alia:-the need <strong>to</strong> move <strong>to</strong> a ‘joined up’ modelof delivery<strong>to</strong> build on the model of Sure Start inareas of moderate disadvantage<strong>to</strong> accept that some ‘false positives’ willbe identified, who will later prove theintervention <strong>to</strong> have been unnecessary<strong>to</strong> build the potential of parents <strong>and</strong>paraprofessionals for effective <strong>and</strong> costeffectiveidentification <strong>and</strong> interventionharness the natural involvement ofparents <strong>to</strong> build parenting skillsprovide for accreditation of priorexperience <strong>and</strong> learning <strong>to</strong>wardsqualifications for paraprofessionalsprovide training for ECEC personnelwhich bridges the health-educationdivideprovide training through a variety ofmodels, including distance <strong>and</strong> parttimeresidential coursesimprove interagency communication,underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> mutual respectring fence funding for identification<strong>and</strong> intervention for children from 0-2fund further research136 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


disseminate effective practicerespect children’s <strong>and</strong> parents’ rights.(Adapted from Fawcett 2001: 11-12).Fawcett adds that many of herre co m m e n d ations are alre a dy in proce s sbe cause of the Gove rn m e nt’s initiat i ve s<strong>to</strong>wa rds early ident i f i cation <strong>and</strong> inte rve nt i o n .The co m m u n i ty-based schemesa dvocated by Ca roline We b s te r- St rat <strong>to</strong> n ,which have the po te ntial <strong>to</strong> re s <strong>to</strong> re <strong>and</strong>benefit disadva ntaged communities bo t hp s yc h o l og i ca l ly <strong>and</strong> soc i a l ly, w h i l ere cognised as challenging, a re lauded byWa l ke r- Hall <strong>and</strong> Sy lva (2001). This iss u p po rted by the rev i ew of inte rve nt i o np rog rammes in the USA by Ra m ey <strong>and</strong>Ra m ey (1998). Th ey argue that thei nte rve ntions most like ly <strong>to</strong> succeed arei nte n s i ve, high quality <strong>and</strong> eco l og i ca l lype rva s i ve. The Ra m eys offer six pri n c i p l e sa bout effica cy. These are :-d eve l o p m e ntal timing (the youngerthe child is enrolled <strong>and</strong> the longerthe inte rve nt i o n , the gre ater thebe n e f i t s, although there is no ev i d e n ceof cri t i cal pe ri od s )programme intensity (greater intensity,for example by number of home visitsper week etc, produces greater effects)direct intervention is more successfulthan indirect (intervention professionals<strong>and</strong> paraprofessionals need <strong>to</strong> workwith the children themselves as well aswith parents, although the Rameysstress the importance of celebratingparents’ <strong>and</strong> other family members’contributions as primary carers)programme breadth <strong>and</strong> flexibility(comprehensive <strong>and</strong> multi-routeprogrammes generally have largereffects)individual differences matter (matchingthe intervention <strong>to</strong> the child’s orfamily’s need)the intervention should be ecologicallypervasive <strong>and</strong> maintained (early yearsinterventions alone may not beenough, involving whole communities,local schools, for example, tends <strong>to</strong> bemore effective). (Adapted from Ramey<strong>and</strong> Ramey 1998: 115-117)Many of the <strong>Government</strong>’s initiatives,such as Sure Start, the National ParentingInstitute, <strong>and</strong> the exp<strong>and</strong>ed role of healthvisi<strong>to</strong>rs are therefore encouragingdevelopments in the spirit of the Ramey’s(1998) conclusions.SUMMARY OF KEY ‘MESSAGES’Practitioners need <strong>to</strong> be able <strong>to</strong>:-Underst<strong>and</strong> attachment <strong>and</strong> theimportance of a child being special<strong>to</strong> at least one significant person inorder <strong>to</strong> promote resilienceUnderst<strong>and</strong> children’s dietary <strong>and</strong>physical needsProvide opportunities <strong>to</strong> explore <strong>and</strong>play in a safe <strong>and</strong> secure environment;children’s mobility <strong>and</strong> movement areimportant for their development137


Know about brain development <strong>and</strong>the importance of ‘nourishment’(a good diet – in both the form of food<strong>and</strong> of physical <strong>and</strong> psychologicalstimulation)Help parents see that intimatebehaviours such as bugging <strong>and</strong>nudging, pet names <strong>and</strong> idiosyncraticbehaviour are important <strong>and</strong> thatchildren’s development sometimesseems difficult because they are trying<strong>to</strong> become independent people witha sense of selfHave reasonable rules which fit withchildren’s rhythms <strong>and</strong> give a pattern<strong>to</strong> lifeKnow that parents, as well as children,need supportKnow about child abuse <strong>and</strong> neglect<strong>and</strong> have other colleagues <strong>to</strong> consultRecognise the additional requirementsof babies <strong>and</strong> young children withspecial needs, <strong>and</strong> plan how <strong>to</strong> ensurethese children have access, in aphilosophical,as well as practical sense,<strong>to</strong> similar experiences <strong>and</strong>opportunities <strong>to</strong> their peersHelp communities <strong>and</strong> the publicunderst<strong>and</strong> the importance of positiveinteractions <strong>and</strong> experiences in the first<strong>three</strong> years for all areas of development,including brain development, <strong>and</strong> forenjoyment in the here <strong>and</strong> now.138 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Chapter 7Conclusions <strong>and</strong> implicationsThis review of research literature has proved <strong>to</strong> be an exciting but huge task, because ofthe wide range of <strong>to</strong>pics which could have been included. Clearly there are allied researchareas, such as that on parenting <strong>and</strong> projects <strong>to</strong> increase parental empowerment, forexample, which would have been covered had there been time <strong>and</strong> space. Staff training<strong>to</strong>o could be helpfully reviewed at another time <strong>and</strong> the DfES’s current initiativesevaluating new training opportunities for the field are positive developments. Further,although multiprofessional working, like early intervention,is <strong>to</strong>uched upon,it is notpossible for these aspects <strong>to</strong> be covered comprehensively here. However, readers can turn<strong>to</strong> other relevant research <strong>and</strong> reviews by using the reference list provided with this text.What then are the key messages wewould want readers <strong>to</strong> take from thisreview of the literature concerning babies<strong>and</strong> children aged between birth <strong>and</strong><strong>three</strong> years?One of the messages from any researchreview concerns the ‘gaps’ which havebeen identified. Both this review <strong>and</strong> thatfor the British Educational ResearchAssociation’s Early Years Special InterestGroup (BERA EYSIG forthcoming), coveringcurriculum, pedagogy <strong>and</strong> training (largelyfor the 3-6 year old age group butincluding a section on babies <strong>and</strong> youngchildren) have thrown up the paucity ofresearch on the processes <strong>and</strong> practices ofECEC for children from birth <strong>to</strong> <strong>three</strong>.In particular the field needs informationabout <strong>to</strong>ddlers in educare settings.A second important area needing moreresearch is that exploring the impact ofpractitioner training on children’s <strong>and</strong>parents’ experiences of ECEC.In the USA, Kagan (1996) has argued thatfocusing on curriculum <strong>and</strong> pedagogicalissues alone will not be sufficient <strong>to</strong>address developments in early childhoodeducation <strong>and</strong> care (ECEC). She suggeststhat it is necessary <strong>to</strong>:-139


a c kn owledge cri t i cal socialt rends affe cting young children<strong>and</strong> their familiesmeet the needs of diverse communitiesfind out what families want <strong>and</strong> what isalready provided by communitiesestablish priorities in relation <strong>to</strong> servicesfor children <strong>and</strong> services for familiesaddress issues of continuity <strong>and</strong>coherenceconsider infrastructure as well as actualservice provisiondisseminate information about earlychildhood <strong>and</strong> ECEC more effectively.These messages could apply equally inEngl<strong>and</strong>, but what other messages arethere from this review of the literature?Since we have drawn out someconclusions <strong>and</strong> implications in each ofthe preceding chapters, we will limit ourfinal points <strong>to</strong> <strong>three</strong>, for or about each ofthe main groups involved – the parents,practitioners, policy makers, trainers,researchers, <strong>and</strong>, last but certainly notleast, the children. These messages are:-PARENTSFamilies are a context for developmentfor parents <strong>to</strong>o <strong>and</strong> perhaps all parentsneed support in relating their ownexperiences of being parented <strong>to</strong> howthey feel about their baby or youngchild. By dealing with any unresolveddissatisfaction with their ownchildhoods through narrative, <strong>and</strong> bylearning about how children develop,preferably before their first child isborn,they can face the challenging taskahead with the joy that will promotepositive relationships.All parents need respect <strong>and</strong>recognition as the primary edu-carersfor their children. Support offeredshould be provided by warm,approachable professionals, whorecognise that they may have differentvalues from those of the parents. Someparents need extra support eitherbecause their circumstances are socioeconomicallydisadvantaged or becausetheir children have special needs. Whenparents <strong>and</strong> professionals are trulypartners they <strong>and</strong> their children achievemuch (Whalley et al 2000).Parents need time <strong>to</strong> be with theirbabies <strong>and</strong> young children, <strong>to</strong> helpthem learn <strong>and</strong> develop, <strong>and</strong> sufficientfinances <strong>to</strong> enjoy them. Many who arein paid employment find their liveshighly pressured time-wise <strong>and</strong> thosewho are not are generally pressured bystress about finances.The issue ofemployer involvement in local EarlyYears Development <strong>and</strong> ChildcarePartnerships <strong>and</strong> work <strong>to</strong> improve thework-life balance needs more publicity<strong>and</strong> debate, because of its long-termimplications.(These statements are based on theresearch reviewed in this text <strong>and</strong> on otherwork, for example see Ferri <strong>and</strong> Smith140 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


1996; Pugh et al 1994; Smith <strong>and</strong> Pugh1996; Smith 1996;Utting 1995; White <strong>and</strong>Woollett 1992).PRACTITIONERSMost of all, we conclude thatpractitioners need commitment.In order <strong>to</strong> be effective in theirrelationships with babies <strong>and</strong> youngchildren they need <strong>to</strong> be loving <strong>and</strong>knowledgeable, <strong>and</strong> because they workin positions of great responsibility, theyneed <strong>to</strong> be professional,accountable<strong>and</strong> trained in ways which keep theminformed about research <strong>and</strong> able <strong>to</strong>reflect on that research in the light oftheir own observations of children.As one practitioner in an Americanresearch report said:-‘I was at a party recently, whensomeone asked me what I did.“I’m aninves<strong>to</strong>r,” I said. People gathered roundme asking about s<strong>to</strong>cks, mutual funds,<strong>and</strong> so on. I explained that I wasn’tdealing with any of those products.“I’m investing in America’s future, ”I <strong>to</strong>ld them – “America’s children <strong>and</strong>their families.” Kay Mayo, Family ChildCare Provider Austin, TX’ Galinsky et al(1994:96).Practitioners require knowledge ofresearch about child developmentin order <strong>to</strong> be able <strong>to</strong> articulate whycertain practices are important.In particular they need information<strong>and</strong> training about young childrenwith special needs. This means theyneed time for that ongoing training.Support <strong>to</strong> develop more effectiveinter-agency working in the interestsof young children <strong>and</strong> their families.(The main fac<strong>to</strong>rs relating <strong>to</strong> thiseffectiveness are detailed in Atkinsonet al 2002).POLICY MAKERSEvangelou <strong>and</strong> Sylva (2002) reported onthe evaluation of the PEEP (Peers EarlyEducation Partnership) based in Oxford.The project, which involves work withadults (parents, carers <strong>and</strong> pre-schoolstaff) who live <strong>and</strong> work with some 2,000children from birth <strong>to</strong> five years in an areaof low socio-economic status, set out <strong>to</strong>effect positive change in educationalachievement, especially in literacy. Theresearchers found that the project didindeed have a positive effect on children’sliteracy, numeracy <strong>and</strong> self-esteem butthat at this time it is impossible <strong>to</strong> claimthat there will be long term gains.As Brooks-Gunn (2000:12) has argued,‘If policy makers believe that offering earlychildhood intervention for two years oreven <strong>three</strong> will permanently <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>tallyreduce SES disparities in children’sachievements, they may be engagingin magical thinking’Thus, with this research in mind, <strong>to</strong>getherwith that reviewed earlier, we concludethat policy makers’ priorities must be:-141


a commitment <strong>to</strong> ensure that ECECservices for children 0-3 are firstlyappropriate <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> the advantage ofthe children <strong>and</strong> not simply <strong>to</strong> enableparents <strong>to</strong> participate in the workforce.Further, that appropriate support forparents <strong>and</strong> children is availablethroughout a child’s life, not simplyin the early yearspolicies which enable the recruitmen<strong>to</strong>f only those who will be committedpractitioners, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> enable access <strong>to</strong>training, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> conditions of servicewhich will ensure retentionfunding more research,especially thatfocusing on the experiences of youngchildren in ECEC settings <strong>and</strong> the livesof two year olds.TRAINERSNeed <strong>to</strong> provide meaningful coursesin child development.They also need <strong>to</strong> act as conduits forresearch information,ensuring linksbetween practitioners <strong>and</strong> the researchcommunity,<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> foster the development ofpractitioner research networks, <strong>to</strong>promote discussion <strong>and</strong> reflection onresearch allied <strong>to</strong> practitioners’ ownobservations.RESEARCHERSResearchers need <strong>to</strong> forge links acrossdisciplinary boundaries,<strong>and</strong> across parent-practitionerresearcherboundaries, so that thosewho are most familiar <strong>to</strong> babies <strong>and</strong>children,<strong>and</strong> with whom they willdisplay the highest levels of theirknowledge <strong>and</strong> achievements, willbe enabled <strong>to</strong> contribute <strong>to</strong> researchknowledge.They also need <strong>to</strong> develop methodsof <strong>and</strong> approaches <strong>to</strong> dissemination –some of the new journals, such asInterplay <strong>and</strong> Early Years Practitioner,<strong>and</strong> the developments made byexisting professional journals, such asNursery World <strong>and</strong> Coordinate, are fineexamples of such efforts.CHILDRENAbove all,children need loving,responsive, sensitive key personsaround them, people who recognisetheir fascination with <strong>and</strong> curiosityabout what is going on around them,their drive <strong>to</strong> explore <strong>and</strong> problemsolve through active learning, <strong>and</strong> whowill provide opportunities <strong>to</strong> play, makefriends <strong>and</strong> share experiences, <strong>and</strong> yetallow time for them <strong>to</strong> be deeplyfocused alone but near others, as well ase n s u ring all their health needs are met.They need <strong>to</strong> be respected as people intheir own right, <strong>and</strong><strong>to</strong> live in a society which is informedabout their development <strong>and</strong> learning,<strong>and</strong> which is involved <strong>and</strong> delighted intheir amazing abilities.142 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Finally, in this review of the researchabout children under <strong>three</strong> <strong>and</strong> theirdevelopment <strong>and</strong> learning, the last wordgoes <strong>to</strong> Gopnik, Meltzoff <strong>and</strong> Kuhl,whotell us that:-‘One benefit of knowing the science is akind of protective scepticism. It shouldmake us deeply suspicious of anyenterprise that offers a formula for makingbabies smarter or teaching them more,from flash cards <strong>to</strong> Mozart tapes <strong>to</strong> BetterBaby Institutes. Everything we know aboutbabies suggests that these artificialinventions are at best useless <strong>and</strong> at worstdistractions from the normal interactionbetween grown-ups <strong>and</strong> babies. Babies arealready as smart as they can be, they knowwhat they need <strong>to</strong> know, <strong>and</strong> they are veryeffective <strong>and</strong> selective in getting the kindsof information they need. They aredesigned <strong>to</strong> learn about the real wo rld thats u rrounds them, <strong>and</strong> they learn by play i n gwith the things in that wo rl d, most of all byp l aying with the people who love them.’Gopnik et al (1999:201).143


ReferencesAbbott, L. (2001) Perceptions of play –a question of priorities? In L. Abbott <strong>and</strong>C. Nutbrown (eds) Ex pe riencing Re g g i oEm i l i a . Im p l i cations for pre - s c h oo lp rov i s i o n. Buckingham: Open UniversityPress pp.8-20Abbott, L.<strong>and</strong> Gillen,J. (1999) Revelationsthrough research partnerships. Early Years20(1):43-52Abbott, L <strong>and</strong> Nutbrown, C (eds)Experiencing Reggio Emilia. Implications forpre-school provision. Buckingham: OpenUniversity PressAber, J.L.<strong>and</strong> Allen,J.P. (1987) Effects ofmaltreatment on young children’ssocioemotional development: anattachment theory perspective.Developmental Psychology 23:406-14Ainsworth,M. (1967) Infancy in Ug<strong>and</strong>a:Infant Care <strong>and</strong> the Growth of LoveBaltimore: Johns Hopkins UniversityAinsworth,M., Bell, S.M.<strong>and</strong> Stay<strong>to</strong>n, D.J.(1974) Infant-mother attachment <strong>and</strong>social development: ‘socialisation’ asproducts of reciprocal responsiveness<strong>to</strong> signals. In M.P.M. Richards (ed) TheIntegration of a Child in<strong>to</strong> a Social WorldCambridge: Cambridge University Presspp.99-136Ak<strong>and</strong>e, A. (1992) Strategies for becomingan inviting parent: suggestions forcaregivers Early Child Development<strong>and</strong> Care 80: 3-12Alex<strong>and</strong>er, P.C. (1992) Application ofattachment theory <strong>to</strong> the study of sexualabuse. Journal of Consulting <strong>and</strong> ClinicalPsychology 60: 185-195Alvarez,A. (ed) (1992) Live Company.Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy with Autistic,Borderline, Deprived <strong>and</strong> Abused Children.London: RoutledgeAnderson,A.M. (1996) Fac<strong>to</strong>rs influencingthe father-infant relationship. Journal ofFamily Nursing 2(3): 306-24Anning, A. (1999) The influence of socioculturalcontext on young children’s144 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


meaning making. Paper presented at theBritish Educational Research AssociationConference, Sussex University, SeptemberAsting<strong>to</strong>n,J. (1994) The Child’s Discovery ofthe Mind. London: Fontana Press.At h ey, C . ( 1990) Extending Thought in Yo u n gChildren London: Paul Chapman PublishingAt ki n s o n ,M.,Wi l ki n ,A., S<strong>to</strong> t t, A ., Do h e rty, P.<strong>and</strong> Kinder, K. (2002) Multi-AgencyWorking:a Detailed Study Slough:NFERAubrey, C. (2001) Questioning comparativeapproaches: context <strong>and</strong> the role ofinternational comparison in policy-making.In T. David (ed) Advances in Applied EarlyChildhood Education. Vol.1. PromotingEvidence-Based Practice in Early ChildhoodEducation London:JAI pp.273-88Aubrey, C. (2002) A bridge <strong>to</strong>o far InterplaySummer 2002:32-35Baker, D., Taylor, H., Henderson,J.<strong>and</strong> theALSPAC Study Team (1998) Inequality ininfant mortality: causes <strong>and</strong>consequencesin Engl<strong>and</strong> in the 1990s.Journal of Epidemiology <strong>and</strong> CommunityHealth 52:451-457B<strong>and</strong>ura,A. (1973) Aggression:a SocialLearning Analysis Englewood Cliffs NJ:Prentice HallB<strong>and</strong>ura,A. (1982) Self-efficacymechanisms in human agency. AmericanPsychologist 37: 122-47B<strong>and</strong>ura, A. (1992) Social cognitive theory.In R. Vasta (ed) Six Theories of ChildDevelopment: Revised Formulations <strong>and</strong>Current Issues London: Jessica KingsleyPress pp.1-60Barker, D.J.P., Forsen, T., Uutela,A.,Osmond, C. <strong>and</strong> Eriksson, J.G. (2001) Sizeat birth <strong>and</strong> resilience <strong>to</strong> effects of poorliving conditions in adult life: alongitudinal study. British Medical Journal323(7324):1273-6Barnes, J.<strong>and</strong> Freude-Lagevardi, A. (2002)From Pregnancy <strong>to</strong> Early Childhood: EarlyInterventions <strong>to</strong> Enhance the Mental Healthof Children <strong>and</strong> Families London: MentalHealth FoundationBarnet, A.<strong>and</strong> Barnet, R. (1998) TheYoungest Minds: Parenting <strong>and</strong> Genes inthe Development of Intellect <strong>and</strong> Emotion.New York: Simon <strong>and</strong> Schuster.Barnett, D.W., Pepi<strong>to</strong>n, A.E., Bell. S.H.,Gilkey, C.M., Smith,J.J., S<strong>to</strong>ne, C.M.,Nelson, K.I.<strong>and</strong> Maples, K.A. (1999)Evaluating early interventions:accountability methods for servicedelivery Journal of Special Education33(3):177-88Barnett, A. (1996) Lives in the balance: age27 Benefit-Cost Analysis of the High/ScopePerry Pre-school Program. (Monograph 11)Ypsilanti MI: High/Scope EducationalResearch FoundationBaron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A.<strong>and</strong> Frith,U.(1985)‘Does the Autistic Child Have a“Theory of Mind”?’ Cognition 21:37-45.Barr, R <strong>and</strong> Limb, K (1997)‘Mumps, Measles<strong>and</strong> Rubella (MMR) <strong>and</strong> Measles Rubella(MR) Vaccines’. Available at:145


http://www.whale.<strong>to</strong>/vaccines/dawbarns.html (accessed 21 February 2002).Barrett, K.C.<strong>and</strong> Campos, J.J. (1987)Perspectives on emotional development:II. A functionalist approach <strong>to</strong>development. In J.D. Osofsky (ed)H<strong>and</strong>book of Infant Development New York:John Wiley <strong>and</strong> Sons pp.555-78Barrett, M. (ed) (1999) The Development ofLanguage Hove: Psychology PressBarrs, M. (1988) Maps of Play. In M. Meek<strong>and</strong> C. Mills (eds) Language <strong>and</strong> Literacy inthe Primary School Lewes: Falmer Presspp.101-15Barry, J.<strong>and</strong> Kirke, P. (1997) Congenitalanomalies in the Irish Traveller CommunityIrish Medical Journal 90:233-234Bartsch,K.<strong>and</strong> Wellman,H.M. (1995)Children Talk about Mind Oxford: OxfordUniversity PressBasso, O., Olsen,J., Johansen,A.M.T. <strong>and</strong>Christensen,K. (1997) Change in socialstatus <strong>and</strong> risk of low birth weight inDenmark:a population based cohort studyBritish Medical Journal 315:1498-502Bateman,A. (1998) Child protection, risk<strong>and</strong> allegation. In C. Owen,C. Cameron <strong>and</strong>P. Moss (eds) Men as Workers in Services forYoung Children. London: University ofLondon IoE pp: 182-189Bearne, E. (1995) Greater Expectations:Reflections of Difference. In E. Bearne (ed)Greater Expectations London: Cassellpp.1-10Bearne, E. <strong>and</strong> Watson, V. (2000) WhereTexts <strong>and</strong> Children Meet London: RoutledgeBee, H. (1989) The Developing Child(5th edition) New York: Harper <strong>and</strong> RowBelsky, J. (1996) Parent, infant <strong>and</strong> socialcontextualantecedents of father-sonattachment security. DevelopmentalPsychology 32 (5):905-13Belsky, J., Campbell, S.B., Cohn, J.F. <strong>and</strong>Moore, G. (1996) Instability of infantparentattachment security. DevelopmentalPsychology 32:921-4Belsky, J., Friedman, S.R.<strong>and</strong> Hsieh,K-H.(2001) Testing a core emotion-regulationprediction: does early attentionalpersistence moderate the effect of infantnegative emotionality later on? ChildDevelopmental 72(1): 123-33Benzeval,M., Judge, K.<strong>and</strong> Whitehead, M.(1995) Tackling Inequalities in Health: anAgenda for Action London: King’s FundPublishingBERA Early Years SIG Members(forthcoming) Review of Early YearsResearch: Pedagogy, Curriculum <strong>and</strong>Adult RolesBerg, L.E. (2001) Divorce <strong>and</strong> fatheringin late modern Sweden. In T. David (ed)Advances in Applied Early ChildhoodEducation.Vol.1. Promoting Evidence-BasedPractice in Early Childhood EducationLondon: JAI pp.27-42Berrueta-Clement, J-R.,Schweinhart, L.J.,Ba rn e t t,W. S .,Ep s te i n ,A. S .<strong>and</strong> We i ka rt, D. D.(1984) Changed Lives: the Effects of the Perry146 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Pre-school Program on Youths throughAge 19. Monograph of the High/ScopeEducational Research Foundation YpsilantiMI: High/Scope PressBettelheim, B. (1987) A good enoughparent:the guide <strong>to</strong> bringing up your childLondon: Thames <strong>and</strong> HudsonBissex,G. (1980) Gyns at Wrk:a child learns<strong>to</strong> read <strong>and</strong> write Cambridge Mass: HarvardUniversity PressBlack, D. (1980) Report on the WorkingGroup on Inequalities in Health (The BlackReport) London: HMSOBlack,J. (1989) Child Health in aMulticultural Society. London: BritishMedical Journal (2nd Edition).Bl a i r, P. S ., Fl e m i n g, P. J ., Be n s l ey, D. , Smith I.,Bacon,C., Taylor Berry, J., Golding, J.<strong>and</strong>Tripp, J. (1996) Smoking <strong>and</strong> the SuddenInfant Death Syndrome: results of the1993-5 case controlled study for theconfidential inquiry in<strong>to</strong> stillbirths <strong>and</strong>deaths in infancy. British Medical Journal313: 195-8Blakemore, S-J. (2002) More questionsthan answers? In te rp l ay Summer 2002: 24 - 30Bloom,L., Marguilis, C., Tinker, E. <strong>and</strong>Fujita,N. (1996) Early Conversations<strong>and</strong> Word Learning: Contributions fromChild <strong>and</strong> Adult in Child Development67(6):3154-75BMA (1999) Growing up in Britain: Ensuringa Healthy Future for our Children – A Stud yof 0-5 Year Olds. London: British MedicalAssociationBog i n , B. ( 1998) Evo l u t i o n a ry <strong>and</strong> biolog i ca laspects of childhood. In C. Panter-Brick (ed)Biosocial Perspectives on ChildrenCambridge: Cambridge University Presspp.10-44Bonati, M., Campi, R.<strong>and</strong> Raisler, J. (2000)Breastfeeding <strong>and</strong> infant illness AmericanJournal of Public Health 90(9): 1478-9Bornstein,M.H. (1998) Stability in mentaldevelopment from early life:methods,measures, models, meanings <strong>and</strong> myths.In F. Simion <strong>and</strong> G. Butterworth (eds)The Development of Sensory, Mo<strong>to</strong>r <strong>and</strong>Cognitive Capacities in Early Infancy: fromPerception <strong>to</strong> Cognition Hove: PsychologyPress pp.301-332Bornstein,M.H. (1999) Human infancy:past, present, future. In M. Bennett (ed)Developmental Psychology. Achievements<strong>and</strong> Prospects London: Taylor & Francis/Psychology Press pp.13-35Boukydis, C.F.Z.<strong>and</strong> Lester, B.M. (1998)Infant crying, risk status <strong>and</strong> social supportin families of preterm <strong>and</strong> term infants.Early Development <strong>and</strong> Parenting 7(1): 31-9Bowerman, M. (1978) The acquisition ofword meaning:an investigation in<strong>to</strong> somecurrent conflicts. In N. Waterson <strong>and</strong>C. Snow (eds) The Development ofCommunication New York: Wiley pp.263-87Bowlby, J. (1953) Child Care <strong>and</strong> the Growthof Love Harmondsworth: PenguinBowlby, J. (1973) Attachment <strong>and</strong> Loss(Vol 2). Separation New York: Basic Books147


Bowlby, J. (1951) Maternal Care <strong>and</strong> MentalHealth. (Report <strong>to</strong> the World HealthOrganisation) Geneva:WHOBowlby, J. (1969) Attachment <strong>and</strong> Loss(Vol 1). Attachment. New York: Basic BooksBowlby, J. (1980 ) Attachment <strong>and</strong> Loss(Vol 3): Loss. New York: Basic BooksBow l by, J . ( 1988) Deve l o p m e ntal psyc h i at rycomes of age. American Journal ofPsychiatry 145:1-10Boyd Webb, N. (1984) Pre-school Childrenwith Working Parents:an Analysis ofAttachment New York: University Pressof AmericaBradshaw, J. (ed) (2001) Poverty: theOutcomes for Children London: FamilyPolicy Studies CentreBråten,S. (1996) When <strong>to</strong>ddlers providecare: infants’ companion space Childhood3(4): 449-66Braungart-Rieker, Garwood, M.M.,Powers, B.P. <strong>and</strong> Wang, X. (2001) Parentalsensitivity, infant affect, <strong>and</strong> affectregulation:predic<strong>to</strong>rs of later attachment.Child Development 72(1):252-70Braungart-Rieker, J., Courtney, S.<strong>and</strong>Garwood, M.M. (1999) Mother- <strong>and</strong> fatherinfantattachment:families in contextJournal of Family Psychology 13(4):535-53Bra ze l <strong>to</strong> n ,B. ( 1992) To u c h po i n t s :Your Ch i l d’sEmotional <strong>and</strong> Behavioural DevelopmentReading Mass: Addison WesleyBrazel<strong>to</strong>n, B. (1995) Touchpoints: YourChild’s Emotional <strong>and</strong> BehaviouralDevelopment (2nd Edition). London:Penguin Books Ltd.Bremner, G. (1998) From perception<strong>to</strong> action:the early development ofknowledge. In F. Simion <strong>and</strong> G. Butterworth(eds) The Development of Sensory, Mo<strong>to</strong>r<strong>and</strong> Cognitive Capacities in Early Infancy.From Perception <strong>to</strong> Cognition Hove:Psychology Press pp.239-56Brether<strong>to</strong>n,I., McNew, S.<strong>and</strong> Beegley-Smith,M. (1981) Early person knowledgeas expressed in gestural <strong>and</strong> verbalcommunication: when do infants acquirea ‘Theory of mind’. In M.E. Lamb <strong>and</strong>L.R.Sherrod (eds) Infant Social CognitionHillsdale NJ: Erlbaum pp.333-73Bre<strong>to</strong>n,M. (2001) Neighbourhoodresiliency. Journal of Community Practice9(12):21-36Briere, J. (1992) Child Abuse Trauma: Theory<strong>and</strong> Treatment of the Lasting EffectsNewbury Park, CA: SageBrit<strong>to</strong>n, J. (1970) Language <strong>and</strong> LearningLondon: PenguinBroberg, A.G., Wessels, H., Lamb, M.E. <strong>and</strong>Hwang, C.P. (1997) Effects of day care onthe development of cognitive abilities ineight year olds: a longitudinal studyDevelopmental Psychology 33(1): 62-9Brodsky, A.E. (1999)‘Making it’:thecomponents <strong>and</strong> process of resilienceamong urban, African-American, singlemothers. American Journal ofOrthopsychiatry 69(2):148-60148 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979) The Ecologyof Human Development Cambridge Mass:Harvard University PressBronfenbrenner, U. (1992) Ecologicalsystems theory. In R.Vasta (ed) Six Theoriesof Child Development: Revised Formulations<strong>and</strong> Current Issues London: Jessica KingsleyPress pp.187-249Bronfenbrenner, U. <strong>and</strong> Morris, P. (1998)The ecology of developmental processes.In W. Damon <strong>and</strong> R.M. Lerner (eds)The H<strong>and</strong>book of Child Psychology Vol1.Theoretical Models of Human Development.New York: John Wiley pp.993-1028Brooke, O.G., Anderson,H.R., Bl<strong>and</strong>, J.M.,Peacock, J.L.<strong>and</strong> Stewart, C. (1989) Effectson birth weight of smoking, alcohol,caffeine, socio-economic fac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong>psychological stress British Medical Journal298:795-801Brooks-Gunn, J. (2000) Do you believe inmagic? What we can expect from earlychildhood intervention programs. Paperpresented at Early Childhood InterventionPrograms:are the costs justified?Congressional Briefing. Washing<strong>to</strong>n DCBruce, T. (1987) Early Childhood EducationLondon: Hodder <strong>and</strong> S<strong>to</strong>ugh<strong>to</strong>nBruce, T. (1996) Helping Young Children <strong>to</strong>Play London: Hodder <strong>and</strong> S<strong>to</strong>ugh<strong>to</strong>nBruce, T. (2001) Learning through Play:Babies, Toddlers <strong>and</strong> the Foundation YearsLondon: Hodder <strong>and</strong> S<strong>to</strong>ugh<strong>to</strong>nBruce, T. <strong>and</strong> Meggitt, C. (2002) Child Care<strong>and</strong> Education. 3rd Edition. London:Hodder & S<strong>to</strong>ugh<strong>to</strong>n.Bruer, J. (1997) ‘Education <strong>and</strong> the Brain:A Bridge Too Far?’ Educational Researcher26(8): 4-16.Bruer, J. (1999) The Myth of the First ThreeYears. New York: The Free Press.Bruner, J.S. (1972) The nature <strong>and</strong> uses ofimmaturity. American Psychologist 27:1-23Bruner, J.S. (1977) The Process of InstructionCambridge Mass: Harvard University PressBruner, J. (1983) Child’s Talk: Learning <strong>to</strong> UseLanguage Oxford: Oxford University PressBruner, J. (1984) Vygotsky’s zone ofproximal development: the hiddenagenda. In B. Rogoff <strong>and</strong> J.V. Wertsch (eds)Children’s Learning in the Zone of ProximalDevelopment San Francisco: Jossey-Basspp.134-58Bruner, J. (1986) Actual Minds, PossibleWorlds Cambridge, Mass: HarvardUniversity PressBruner, J. (1990) Acts of MeaningCambridge Mass: Harvard University PressBruner, J. (1996) The Culture of EducationCambridge Mass: Harvard University PressBruner, J. (2000) Foreword. In J. DeLoache<strong>and</strong> A. Gottlieb (eds) A World of BabiesCambridge: Cambridge University Presspp.ix-xiiBruner, J.S., Jolly, A.<strong>and</strong> Sylva,K. (eds)(1976) Play: its Role in Development <strong>and</strong>Evolution Harmondsworth: Penguin149


Bruner, J.S.<strong>and</strong> Lucariello, J. (1989)Monologue as narrative recreation of theworld. In K. Nelson (ed) Narratives from theCrib Cambridge Mass: Harvard UniversityPress pp.73-97Burman, E. (1994) DeconstructingDevelopmental Psychology London:RoutledgeButler, A.J.P. (1996) Review of children <strong>and</strong>violence. Child Abuse Review 5(4): 297-8Ca i rn s, E. <strong>and</strong> Dawe s, A . ( 1996) Ch i l d re n :ethnic <strong>and</strong> po l i t i cal violence – a co m m e nt a ryChild Development 67(1):129-39Calkins, S.D., Smith,C.L., Gill,K.L. <strong>and</strong>Johnson,M.C. (1998) Maternal interactivestyle across contexts: relations <strong>to</strong>emotional, behavioural <strong>and</strong> physiologicalregulation during <strong>to</strong>ddlerhood SocialDevelopment 7(3):350-69Campbell, R. (1998) A <strong>three</strong> year oldlearning literacy at home. Early Years19(1):76-89Campbell,R. (1999) Literacy from Home <strong>to</strong>School. Reading with Alice S<strong>to</strong>ke-on-Trent:Trentham BooksCa rpe nte r, B. (ed) (1997) Families in Co n text :Emerging Trends in Family Support <strong>and</strong> EarlyIntervention London: David Ful<strong>to</strong>nCaselles, C.E. <strong>and</strong> Milner, J.E. (2000)Evaluations of child transgressions,disciplinary choices, <strong>and</strong> expected childcompliance in a no-cry <strong>and</strong> a crying infantcondition in physically abusive <strong>and</strong>comparison mothers. Child Abuse <strong>and</strong>Neglect 24(4): 477-91Catherwood, D. (1999) ‘New Views on theYoung Brain:offerings from developmentalpsychology <strong>to</strong> early childhood education’.Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood1(1):23—35.Chambers, A. (1993) Tell Me, Children,Reading <strong>and</strong> Talk Stroud: Thimble PressChambers, E. <strong>and</strong> Belicki, K. (1998) Usingsleep dysfunction <strong>to</strong> explore the nature ofresilience in adult survivors of childhoodabuse or trauma Child Abuse <strong>and</strong> Neglect22(8):753-8Channel 4 Television (1992) Childhood(Television series) London: Channel 4Chen,Y-J. <strong>and</strong> McCollum,J.A. (2000)Taiwanese mothers’ perceptions of therelationship between interactions withtheir infants <strong>and</strong> the development ofsocial competence. Early ChildDevelopment <strong>and</strong> Care 162:25-40Chenoweth, L. <strong>and</strong> Stehlik, D. (2001)Building resilient communities:social workpractice <strong>and</strong> rural Queensl<strong>and</strong>. AustralianSocial Work 54(2):47-54Chomsky, N. (1965) Aspects of a Theory ofSyntax MA: MIT PressChomsky, N. (1975) Reflections onLanguage NY: Pantheon BooksChristie, B. (2002) Scottish expert groupfinds no link between MMR <strong>and</strong> Autism.BJM 324: 1224Ciccetti, D., Toth, S. <strong>and</strong> Bush,M. (1988)Developmental psychopathology <strong>and</strong>incompetence in childhood: suggestions150 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


for intervention. In B.B. Lahey <strong>and</strong>A.E. Kazdin (eds) Advances in Clinical ChildPsychology New York: Plenum Presspp.1-77Clarke, P. (1999) Supporting bilinguallearners. Workshop Papers. Melbourne:Free Kindergarten AssociationClarke, A. (2001) Early adversity <strong>and</strong>adoptive solutions Adoption <strong>and</strong> Fostering25(1):24-32Clarke, A.<strong>and</strong> Clarke, A. (2000) EarlyExperience <strong>and</strong> the Life Path London:Jessica Kingsley Open BooksClarke, A.D.B <strong>and</strong> Clarke, A.M. (1996)Varied destinies: a study of unfulfilledpredictions. In B. Bernstein <strong>and</strong> J. Brannen(eds) Children, Research <strong>and</strong> Policy London:Taylor <strong>and</strong> Francis pp.47-62Clarke, A.M.<strong>and</strong> Clarke, ADB. (eds) (1976)Early Experience: Myth <strong>and</strong> EvidenceLondon: Open BooksClarke-Stewart, A., Gruber, C.P.,<strong>and</strong>Fitzgerald, L.M. (1994) Children at Home<strong>and</strong> in Day Care Hillsdale NJ: ErlbaumCochran,M., Larner, M., Riley, D.,Gunnarsson,L.<strong>and</strong> Hendreson,C.R.Jr.(1990) Extending Families. The SocialNetworks of Parents <strong>and</strong> their ChildrenCambridge: Cambridge University PressColes, M. (1996) The Magnifying Glass.In N. Hall <strong>and</strong> J. Martello (eds) Listening <strong>to</strong>Children Think: Exploring Talk in the EarlyYears Sevenoaks: Hodder <strong>and</strong> S<strong>to</strong>ugh<strong>to</strong>npp.1-17Cooper, P.J. <strong>and</strong> Murray, L. (1998)Fortnightly review: postnatal depressionBritish Medical Journal 316(7148): 1884-6Coupe, J. <strong>and</strong> Goldbart, J. (eds) (1988)Communication Before SpeechLondon: Croom HelmCox,J. (forthcoming) Childhood in Crisis?Work in progress for PhD study CanterburyChrist Church University College/University of Kent at CanterburyCox,M.J. <strong>and</strong> Marg<strong>and</strong>, N.A.(1992)Prediction of infant-father <strong>and</strong> infantmotherattachment DevelopmentalPsychology 28:474-83Craft, A. (2000) Creativity – across theprimary curriculum London: RoutledgeCri t te n d o n ,P. M . ( 1992) Ch i l d re n’s strate g i e sfor coping with adverse environments:aninterpretation of attachment theory. ChildAbuse <strong>and</strong> Neglect 16: 329-343Cullen, C., Field, T., Escalona, A.<strong>and</strong>Hartshorn, K. (2000) Father-infantinteractions are enhanced by massagetherapy. Early Child Development <strong>and</strong> Care164:41-7Cu l p, R . E. ,Li t t l e, V. , Le t t s, D. <strong>and</strong>Law re n ce, H . (1991) Maltreated children’sself-concept. American Journal ofOrthopsychiatry 61: 11-21Currer, C. (1991) Underst<strong>and</strong>ing themother’s viewpoint:the case of Pathanwomen in Britain. In S. Wyke <strong>and</strong>J. Hewison (eds) Child Health MattersMil<strong>to</strong>n Keynes: Open University Presspp.40-52151


Curtis, A.M. (1998) Curriculum for thePre-school Child: Learning <strong>to</strong> Learn.(2nd edition) London: RoutledgeCutrona,C.E., Hessling, R.M., Bacon, P.L.<strong>and</strong> Russell. D.W. (1998) Predic<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong>correlates of continuing involvement withthe baby’s father among adolescentmothers Journal of Family Psychology12(3):369-87Dahlberg, G., Moss, P. <strong>and</strong> Pence, A. (1999)Beyond Quality in Early Childhood Education<strong>and</strong> Care. Postmodern Perspectives.London: Falmer PressDale, N. (1996) Working with Families ofChildren with Special Needs: Partnership <strong>and</strong>Practice London: RoutledgeDalli,C. (2000) Starting child care. Keynoteaddress, Symposium Curriculum, Policies<strong>and</strong> Practices in Early ChildhoodEducation, Malta, 28 Nov-3 Dec 2000Daniel, B. <strong>and</strong> Baldwin, N. (2001)Assessment practice in cases of childneglect: a developmental project. Practice13(4):21-38Daro, D. (1993) Child maltreatmentresearch:implications for program design.In D. Cicchetti <strong>and</strong> S. Toth (eds) Child Abuse,Child Development <strong>and</strong> Social PolicyNorwood NJ: Ablex pp.331-67David, T. (1990) Under Five – Undereducated?Mil<strong>to</strong>n Keynes:Open University PressDavid, T. (1993) Child Protection <strong>and</strong> EarlyYears Teachers Buckingham: OpenUniversity PressDavid, T. (1993a) Families <strong>and</strong> childprotection: the role of the early childhoodeduca<strong>to</strong>r. International Journal of EarlyChildhood 25(2): 8-12David, T. (ed) (1998) Researching EarlyChildhood: European Perspectives London:PCP/SageDavid, T., Raban, B., Ure, C., Goouch, K.,Jago, M., Barrière, I., Lambirth, A.(2000) Making Sense of Early Literacy: aPractitioner’s Perspective S<strong>to</strong>ke-on-Trent:Trentham BooksDavies, D. (1999) Child Development.A Practitioner’s Guide. New York: TheGuildford PressDavies, M. (2002 in press) Young ChildrenLearning through Dance <strong>and</strong> MovementLondon: Paul Chapman/SageDawkins, R. (1976) The Selfish Gene Oxford:Oxford University PressDfEE (2001) Schools Building on Success(Green Paper) London: DfEEDeater-Deckard, K., Pinker<strong>to</strong>n,R.<strong>and</strong>Scarr, S. (1996) Child care quality <strong>and</strong>children’s behavioural adjustment:a fouryear longitudinal study Journal of ChildPsychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry <strong>and</strong> AlliedDisciplines 37(8):937-48De Haan, D. <strong>and</strong> Singer, E. (2001) Youngchildren’s language of <strong>to</strong>getherness.International Journal of Early YearsEducation 9(2): 117-24Deloache, J.S. <strong>and</strong> Brown, A.L. (1987) Theearly emergence of planning skills in152 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


children. In J. Bruner <strong>and</strong> H. Haste (eds)Making Sense London: Methuen pp. 108-30DeSpiegelaere, M., Dramaix, M.<strong>and</strong>Hennart, P. (1998) Socio-economic status<strong>and</strong> changes in body mass from 3-5 yearsArchives of Disease in Childhood 78:477-478DfES (2001) The National Childcare Strategy(Green Paper) London:DfESDiamond, M. <strong>and</strong> Hopkins, J. (1998) MagicTrees of the Mind. How <strong>to</strong> Nurture YourChild’s Intelligence, Creativity <strong>and</strong> HealthyEmotions from <strong>Birth</strong> through Adolescence.New York: Dut<strong>to</strong>n/PenguinDiaz, R.M.<strong>and</strong> Berk, L.E. (eds) (1991)Private Speech: From Social Interaction <strong>to</strong>Self-regulation. Hillsdale NJ: ErlbaumDonaldson,M. (1978) Children’s MindsGlasgow: Fontana/CollinsDo n ova n , W. L ., Le av i t t, L . A .<strong>and</strong> Wa l s h , R . O.(2000) Maternal illusory control predictssocialization strategies <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ddlercompliance. Developmental Psychology36(3): 402-11Doyle, B. (1997) Transdisciplinaryapproaches <strong>to</strong> working with families.In B. Carpenter (ed) Families in Context:Emerging Trends in Family Support <strong>and</strong>Early Intervention London: David Ful<strong>to</strong>npp.150-63Drewett, R. (1991) The promotion ofbreastfeeding. In S. Wyke <strong>and</strong> J. Hewison(eds) Child Health Matters Mil<strong>to</strong>n Keynes:Open University Press pp.107-16Drewett, R., Wolke, D., Asefa, M., Kaba,M.<strong>and</strong> Tessema, F. (2001) Malnutrition <strong>and</strong>mental development:is there a sensitiveperiod? A nested case-control studyJournal of Child Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry<strong>and</strong> Allied Disciplines 42(2): 181-7Duffy, B. (1998) Supporting Creativity <strong>and</strong>Imagination in the Early Years Buckingham:Open University PressDunn,J. (1984) Sisters <strong>and</strong> BrothersLondon: FontanaDu n n ,J. ( 1987) Underst<strong>and</strong>ing fe e l i n g s :t h eearly stages. In J. Bruner <strong>and</strong> H. Haste (eds)Making Sense London: Methuen pp.26-40Dunn,J. (1988) The Beginnings of SocialUnderst<strong>and</strong>ing Cambridge Mass: HarvardUniversity PressDunn,J. (1993) Young Children’s CloseRelationships. Beyond Attachment NewburyPark California: SageDunn,J. (1999) Mindreading <strong>and</strong> SocialRelationships. In M. Bennett (ed)Developmental Psychology London: Taylor<strong>and</strong> Francis 55-71Dwivedi,K.N. (1996) Race <strong>and</strong> the Child’sPerspective. In R. Davie, G. Up<strong>to</strong>n <strong>and</strong>V. Varma (eds) The Voice of the Child: aH<strong>and</strong>book for Professionals London: Falmerpp.153-69Dyson,A.H. (2001) Where are theChildhoods in Childhood Literacy?An exploration in outer (school) spaceJournal of Early Childhood Literacy 1(1):9-38153


Early Years Curriculum Group (1989)Early Childhood Education S<strong>to</strong>ke-on-Trent:Trentham BooksEchenrode, J., Laird, M.<strong>and</strong> Doris, J. (1993)School performance <strong>and</strong> disciplinaryproblems among abused <strong>and</strong> neglectedchildren. Developmental Psychology29:53-62Edwards, C., G<strong>and</strong>ini,L. <strong>and</strong> Foreman,G.(1998) The Hundred Languages of ChildrenNY: AblexEgel<strong>and</strong>, B., Sroufe, L.A. <strong>and</strong> Erickson,M.(1983) The developmental consequencesof different patterns of maltreatment. ChildAbuse <strong>and</strong> Neglect 7:459-69Elder, G. (1998) The life course asdevelopmental theory. Child Development69:1-12Elfer, P. (1996) Building intimacy inrelationships with young children innurseries. Early Years 16(2):30-34Elfer, P. <strong>and</strong> Selleck, D. (1999) Childrenunder <strong>three</strong> in nurseries. Uncertainty as acreative fac<strong>to</strong>r in child observations.European Early Childhood EducationResearch Journal 7(1):69-82Elfer, P., Goldschmeid, E. <strong>and</strong> Selleck, D.(2002) Key Persons in Nurseries: BuildingRelationships for Quality Provision London:NEYNEliot, L. (1999) Early Intelligence: How theBrain <strong>and</strong> Mind Develop in the First FiveYears of Life. London: Penguin Books Ltd.Emde, R.N., Gaensbauer, T.J.<strong>and</strong> Harmon,R.J. (1976) Emotional Expression in InfancyNew York: International Universities PressEngle, P.L., Castle, S.<strong>and</strong> Menon, P. (1996)Child development, vulnerability <strong>and</strong>resilience. Social Science <strong>and</strong> Medicine43(5):621-35EPPI Early Years Group (forthcoming)A Review of Research on Quality in ECECErikson, E. (1963) Childhood <strong>and</strong> SocietyHarmondsworth: PenguinErikson, E. (1968) Identity: Youth <strong>and</strong> CrisisLondon: FaberEvangelou, M.<strong>and</strong> Sylva, K. (2002) TheEffects of the Peers Early EducationPartnership (PEEP) on Children’sDevelopmental Progress. Confidential FinalReport for the study funded by the DfESTender no. 4/RP/236/2000 Oxford:University of Oxford Department ofEducationEvans, G.W., Maxwell, L.E. <strong>and</strong> Hart, B.(1999) Parental language <strong>and</strong> verbalresponsiveness in crowded homes.Developmental Psychology 35(4): 1020-3Fa l l ,C. H . D. , Gog g i n ,P. M .,Ha r s t i n ,P. , Fi n e, D.<strong>and</strong> Duggleby, S. (1997) Growth in infancy,infant feeding, childhood living conditions<strong>and</strong> Heliobacter pylori infection at age 70.Archives of Disease in Childhood 77:310-314Farver, J.M.<strong>and</strong> Frosch, D.L. (1996) L.A.S<strong>to</strong>ries: ‘aggression in preschoolers’spontaneous narratives after the riotsof 1992. Child Development 67: 19-32154 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Fawcett, A. (2001) Special EducationalNeeds/Early Years Literature Review. A report<strong>to</strong> the Department for Education <strong>and</strong> SkillsSheffield: University of SheffieldFeldman, C.F. (1992) The theory of theoryof mind. In Human Development 35: 107-17Feldman, M.A., Hancock,C.L., Rielly, N.,Minnes, P. <strong>and</strong> Cairns, C. (2000) Behaviourproblems in young children with or at riskfor developmental delay. Journal of Child<strong>and</strong> Family Studies 9(2):247-61Fen<strong>to</strong>n, D. <strong>and</strong> Martello, J. (1996) BathtimeTalk. In N. Hall <strong>and</strong> J. Martello (eds)Listening <strong>to</strong> Children Think: Exploring Talk inthe Early Years Sevenoaks: Hodder <strong>and</strong>S<strong>to</strong>ugh<strong>to</strong>n pp.18-33Ferguson,C.A.<strong>and</strong> Slobin, D.I. (eds) (1973)Studies of Child Language DevelopmentNew York: Holt Rinehart <strong>and</strong> WilsonFerri, E. <strong>and</strong> Smith,K. (1996) Parenting inthe 1990s London: Family Policy StudiesUnit/Joseph RowntreeFonagy, P., Steele, H. <strong>and</strong> Steele, M. (1991)Maternal representations of attachmentpredict the organization of infant-motherattachment at one year of age. ChildDevelopment 62:891-905Fo n a gy, P. , Ste e l e, M ., Ste e l e, H ., Hi g g i t t, A .<strong>and</strong> Target, M. (1994) The theory <strong>and</strong>practice of resilience. Journal of ChildPsychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry 35:231-57Forrest, G. (ed) (1997) Bonding <strong>and</strong>Attachment: Current Issues in Research <strong>and</strong>Practice Occasional Paper 14 London:Association of Child Psychology <strong>and</strong>PsychiatryFox ,N. A ., Ki m m e rl ey, N . L .<strong>and</strong> Schafe r, W. D.(1991) Attachment <strong>to</strong> mother/attachment<strong>to</strong> father a meta-analysis ChildDevelopment 62:210-25Fürstenberg, F. <strong>and</strong> Cherlin, A. (1991)Divided Families Cambridge Mass: HarvardUniversity PressGalinsky, E., Howes, C., Kon<strong>to</strong>s, S.<strong>and</strong>Shinn, M. (1994) The Study of Children inFamily Child Care <strong>and</strong> Relative Care NewYork: Families <strong>and</strong> Work InstituteGallagher, J.J. (1990) The family as a focusfor intervention. In S.J. Meisels <strong>and</strong> J.P.Shonkoff (eds) (1990) H<strong>and</strong>book of EarlyChildhood Interventions Cambridge:Cambridge University Press pp.540-59Gammage, P. (1999) Early childhoodeducation in a postmodern world.In L. Abbott <strong>and</strong> H. Moylett (eds) EarlyEducation Transformed London: Falmerpp.153-163Garbarino, J. (1993) Children’s response <strong>to</strong>community violence: what do we know?Infant Mental Health Journal 14: 103-115Gardner, F. (1994) The quality of jointactivity between mothers <strong>and</strong> theirchildren with behaviour problems.Journal of Child Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry35:935-48Gardner, H. (1993) The Unschooled MindLondon: Fontana155


Garrett, M., McElroy, A.M.<strong>and</strong> Staines, A.(2002) Locomo<strong>to</strong>r miles<strong>to</strong>nes <strong>and</strong>babywalkers: cross sectional study. BritishMedical Journal 324: 1494Garvey, C. (1984) Children’s Talk Oxford:FontanaGauthier, A.H. (1996) The State <strong>and</strong> theFamily Oxford: Clarendon PressGelman,R. <strong>and</strong> Gallistel, C.R. (1978)The Child’s Underst<strong>and</strong>ing of NumberCambridge Mass: Harvard University PressGesell, A. <strong>and</strong> Armatruda,C.S. (1947)Developmental diagnosis New York: HaberGibson, E. J. (1969) Principles of PerceptualLearning New York: Prentice HallGilgun, J.F. (1996) Human development<strong>and</strong> adversity in ecological perspective,part 1: a conceptual framework Families<strong>and</strong> Society 77(7):395-402Gilligan,C. (1982) In a Different Voice:Psychological Theory <strong>and</strong> Women’sDevelopment Cambridge Mass: HarvardUniversity PressGoddard Blythe, S. (2000) Early learningin the balance Support for Learning15(4): 154-9Goin,R.P. (1998) A review of per socialdevelopment in early childhood. EarlyChild Development <strong>and</strong> Care 142:1-8Goldman-Racik, P. (1987) ‘Development ofCortical Circuitry <strong>and</strong> Cognitive Function’.Child Development, 58: 601-622.Goldschmeid, E. (1986) Infants at Work.Babies of 6-9 months exploring everydayobjects. London: National Children’s Bureau(video)Goldschmeid, E. <strong>and</strong> Jackson, S. (1994)People Under Three: Young Children in DayCare London: RoutledgeGoldschmied, E. <strong>and</strong> Selleck, D. (1996)Communication between babies in their firstyear London: National Children’s BureauEarly Childhood UnitGoldstein, L.H., Diener, M.L.<strong>and</strong>Mangelsdorf, S.C. (1996) Maternalcharacteristics <strong>and</strong> social support acrossthe transition <strong>to</strong> motherhood:associationswith maternal behaviour. Journal of FamilyPsychology 10(1): 60-71Goleman, D. (1996) Emotional Intelligence:Why it can matter more than IQ. London:Bloomsbury PublishingGoodman, R. <strong>and</strong> Scott, S. (1997) ChildPsychiatry Oxford: BlackwellGopnik, A., Melzoff, A. <strong>and</strong> Kuhl, P. (1999)How Babies Think: the Science of Childhood.London: Weidenfeld <strong>and</strong> NicolsonGordon, P., Shaw, M., Dorling, D. <strong>and</strong>Smith, G.D. (1999) Inequalities in Health:the Evidence Presented <strong>to</strong> the IndependentInquiry in<strong>to</strong> Inequalities in Health Bris<strong>to</strong>l:Policy PressGoswami,U. (1998) Cognition in ChildrenHove: Psychology PressGraham, H. (1984) Women, Health <strong>and</strong> theFamily Brigh<strong>to</strong>n: Wheatsheaf156 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Grainger, T. <strong>and</strong> Goouch,K. (1999) Youngchildren <strong>and</strong> playful language. In T. David(ed) Teaching Young Children London:Paul Chapman/Sage pp.19-29Gr<strong>and</strong>in,T. (1996) Thinking in Pictures: AndOther Reports from My Life With AutismUSA: VintageGraves, D. (1983) Writing: Teachers &Children at Work London: HeinemannGreene, S. (1999) Child development:oldthemes, new directions. In M. Woodhead,D. Faulkner <strong>and</strong> K. Little<strong>to</strong>n (eds) MakingSense of Social Development London:Routledge/ Open University pp.250-268Greenfield, S. (1997) The Human Brain.London: Weidenfeld <strong>and</strong> Nicolson.Greenough,W.T., Black, J.E. <strong>and</strong>Wallace, C.S. (1987) Experience <strong>and</strong>brain development. Child Development58:569-582Gregory E (2001) Sisters <strong>and</strong> brothers aslanguage <strong>and</strong> literacy teachers: Synergybetween siblings playing <strong>and</strong> working<strong>to</strong>gether in Journal of Early ChildhoodLiteracy 1(3): 301-322Grossman, F.K., Eichler, L.S.<strong>and</strong>Winickoff, S.A. (1980) Pregnancy, birth <strong>and</strong>parenthood: adaptations of mothers, fathers<strong>and</strong> infants. San Francisco: Jossey BassGrugeon, E. (1999) The State of Play:children’s oral culture, literacy <strong>and</strong>learning. Reading 33(1): 13-16Grych, J.H.<strong>and</strong> Clark,R. (1999) Maternalemployment <strong>and</strong> development of thefather-infant relationship in the first year.Developmental Psychology 35(4):893-903Gunnar, M.R. (1996) Quality of Care <strong>and</strong> theBuffering of Stress Physiology: Its Potential inProtecting The Developing Human BrainMinnesota: University of MinnesotaInstitute of Child DevelopmentHaddad, D.F., Green, E.S.A.<strong>and</strong> Oliver, R.E.(2000) Core Paediatrics <strong>and</strong> Child HealthLondon: Churchill Livings<strong>to</strong>neHagekull, B., Stenberg, G.<strong>and</strong> Bohlin,G.(1993) Infant-mother social referencinginteractions:description <strong>and</strong> antecedentsin maternal sensitivity <strong>and</strong> infantirritability. In Early Development <strong>and</strong>Parenting 2(3): 183-191Haglund, B. <strong>and</strong> Cnattingius, S. (1990)Cigarette smoking as a risk fac<strong>to</strong>r for SIDSAmerican Journal of Public Health 80:29-32Halliday, M.A.K. (1978) Language as SocialSemiotic London: ArnoldHa n n , D. M ., Os o fs ky, J . D. ,Ba rn a rd, K . E. a n dLe o n a rd, G . (1994) Dyadic affect regulationin <strong>three</strong> caregiving environments AmericanJournal of Orthopsychiatry 64(2):263-273Ha n n o n ,P. , We i n be rg e r, J .<strong>and</strong> Nu t b row n ,C.(1991) A study of work with parents <strong>to</strong>promote early literacy development.Research Papers in Education 6(2):77-97Harris, P. (1989) Children <strong>and</strong> Emotion:theDevelopment of PsychologicalUnderst<strong>and</strong>ing Oxford: Basil BlackwellHarste, J.C., Woodward, V.A.<strong>and</strong>Burke, C.L. (1984) Language S<strong>to</strong>ries157


<strong>and</strong> Literacy Lessons, New Hampshire:HeinemannHasselberg, M., Laflamme, L., Ringback,G.<strong>and</strong> Wei<strong>to</strong>ft, P. (2001) Socio-economicdifferences in road traffic injuries duringchildhood <strong>and</strong> youth:a closer look atdifferent kinds of road user. Journal ofEpidemiology <strong>and</strong> Community Health55:858-62Haynes-Seman, C.<strong>and</strong> Baumgarten, D.(1998) The victimization of young children.Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment <strong>and</strong>Trauma 2(1);67-86Heath,S.B. (1983) Ways with WordsCambridge: Cambridge University PressHennessy, E. <strong>and</strong> Melhuish, E.C. (1991)Early day care <strong>and</strong> the development ofschool-age children: a review Journal ofReproductive <strong>and</strong> Infant Psychology9: 117-36Henry, D.L. (1999) Resilience in maltreatedchildren: implications for special needsadoptions Child Welfare 78(5):519-40Henry, M. (1996) Young Children’s Parents<strong>and</strong> Professionals London: RoutledgeHerbert, E. (1994) Becoming a specialfamily. In T. David (ed) Working Together forYoung Children London: RoutledgeHerbert, E. <strong>and</strong> Carpenter, B. (1994)Fathers – the secondary partners:professional perceptions <strong>and</strong> a father’sreflections Children <strong>and</strong> Society 8(1):31-41Herbert, M. (1999) Clinical Child PsychologyChichester: John Wiley <strong>and</strong> SonsHil<strong>to</strong>n,M. (1996) The Children of thisWorld. In M. Hil<strong>to</strong>n (ed) Potent FictionsLondon: Routledge pp.1-16Hirose, T. <strong>and</strong> Barnard, K. (1997)Interactions between depressed mothers<strong>and</strong> their infants:maternal verbal jointattention <strong>and</strong> its effects on the infant’scognitive development Early ChildDevelopment <strong>and</strong> Care 138:83-95Hirschfield, D.R., Rosenbaum, J.F.,Biederman,J., Bolduc, E.A., Faranone, S.V.,Snidman, N., Reznick, J.S.<strong>and</strong> Kagan, J.(1992) Stable behavioural inhibition <strong>and</strong> itsassociation with anxiety disorder. Journalof the American Academy of Child <strong>and</strong>Adolescent Psychiatry 31: 103-111Hirst, J. (1999) Growing Strong CommunityCare 13 May 1999: 18-20Hobson,R.P. (1993) Autism <strong>and</strong> theDevelopment of Mind Hove & Hillsdale NJ:Lawrence ErlbaumHoffer, M. (ed) (1975) Parent-infantInteraction Amsterdam: ElsevierHome Office, DoH, DES, Welsh Office(1991) Working Together Under the ChildrenAct London: HMSOHopkins, J. (1988) Facilitating thedevelopment of intimacy between nurses<strong>and</strong> infants in day nurseries. Early ChildDevelopment <strong>and</strong> Care 33: 99-111Hornby, G., Atkinson, A.<strong>and</strong> Howard, J.(1997) Controversial Issues in SpecialEducation London: David Ful<strong>to</strong>n158 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Howes, C. (1987) Peer interaction of youngchildren. Monographs of the Society forRe s e a rch in Child Deve l o p m e n t( No. 217 ,53:1)Howes, C. (1991) A comparison of preschoolbehaviors with peers when childrenenrol in child-care as infants or olderchildren. Journal of Reproductive <strong>and</strong> InfantPsychology 9:105-15Howes, C. <strong>and</strong> Hamil<strong>to</strong>n, C.E. (1993) Childcare for young children. In B. Spodek <strong>and</strong>O. Saracho (eds) Yearbook in EarlyChildhood Education Vol. 3: Issues in ChildCare New York: Teachers’ College Presspp.31-46Howes, C., Hamil<strong>to</strong>n, C.E. <strong>and</strong> Matheson,C.C. (1994) Children’s relationships withpeers: differential associations withaspects of the teacher-child relationship.Child Development 65(1):253-63Howes, C., Hamil<strong>to</strong>n, C.E. <strong>and</strong> Matheson,C.C. (1994a) Maternal. Teacher <strong>and</strong> childcare his<strong>to</strong>ry correlates of children’srelationships with peers. ChildDevelopment 65(1):264-73Hutchins, T. <strong>and</strong> Sims, M. (1999) ProgramPlanning for Infants <strong>and</strong> Toddlers, anEcological Approach London: Prentice HallHutt, C. (1970) Specific <strong>and</strong> diverseexploration. Advances in Child Development<strong>and</strong> Behaviour 5: 119-80Hutt, C. (1979) Early education <strong>and</strong> childcare in India <strong>and</strong> Sri Lanka:a critical review.Bulletin of the British Psychological Society32:377- 80Hutt, C. (1966) Exploration <strong>and</strong> play inchildren. In Play, Exploration <strong>and</strong> Terri<strong>to</strong>ryin Mammals. Symposium of the ZoologicalSociety 18: 61-8Hutt, C. (1972) Males <strong>and</strong> FemalesHarmondsworth: PenguinHutt, S.J., Tyler, S., Hutt, C. <strong>and</strong>Chris<strong>to</strong>pherson, H. (1989) Play, Exploration<strong>and</strong> Learning London: RoutledgeIs a a c s, S . ( 1930) In te l l e ctual Growth in Yo u n gChildren London: Routledge & Kegan PaulIsaacs, S. (1933) Social Development inYoung Children London: Routledge &Kegan PaulJagger, G.<strong>and</strong> Wright, C. (eds) ChangingFamily Values London: RoutledgeJarousse, J-P., Mingat, A.<strong>and</strong> Richard, M(1991) La scolarisation maternelle à deuxans:analyse des effets pedagogiques <strong>and</strong>sociaux. Bourgogne: L’Université deBourgogneJohnson,M. (1999) Developmentalcognitive neuroscience. In M. Bennett (ed)Developmental Psychology London: Taylor<strong>and</strong> Francis pp.147-64Johnson, M. (2002) Brain building.Interplay Summer 2002:36-42Johnson, M.<strong>and</strong> Mareschal, D. (2001)‘Cognitive <strong>and</strong> perceptual developmentduring infancy’. Current Opinion inNeurobiology 11:213—218.Kagan,J. (1984) The Nature of the ChildNew York: Basic Books159


Kagan,J. (1998) Three Seductive IdeasCambridge Mass: Harvard University PressKagan,S.L. (1996) Meeting family <strong>and</strong>community needs: the <strong>three</strong> Cs of earlychildhood education. Paper presented atthe Australia <strong>and</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong> Conferenceon the First Years at School, HobartTasmania, JanuaryKa l f f, A . C .,Kroe s, M .,V l e s, J . S . H .,He n d ri k s e n ,T. G . M .,Fe ro n ,F. J . M .,Steya e nt, J .,van Ze be n ,T. M . C . B. ,Jo l e s, J . <strong>and</strong> van Os, J . (2001)Neighbourhood level <strong>and</strong> individual levelSES effects on child problem behaviour: amultilevel analysis. Journal of Epidemiology<strong>and</strong> Community Health 55: 246-50Kamel, H.<strong>and</strong> Dockrell,J.E. (2000)Divergent perspectives, multiplemeanings:a comparison of caregivers’ <strong>and</strong>observers’ interpretations of infantbehaviour. Journal of Reproductive <strong>and</strong>Infant Psychology 18(1):41-60Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992) BeyondModularity: a Developmental Perspectiveon Cognitive Science Cambridge, Mass:MIT PressKarmiloff-Smith, A. (1994) Baby It’s YouLondon: Ebury PressKarmiloff-Smith, A. (1995) Annotation:The Extraordinary Cognitive Journey fromFoetus through Infancy. Journal of ChildPsychology, Psychiatry 36(8): 1293-1313Karmiloff, K <strong>and</strong> Karmiloff-Smith, A.(2001) Pathways <strong>to</strong> Language: From Fetus<strong>to</strong> Adolescent. Cambridge Mass: HarvardUniversity PressKatz, I. (1996) The Construction of RacialIdentity in Children of Mixed Parentage:Mixed Metaphors London: KingsleyKatz,L.<strong>and</strong> Shatz,C. (1997) ‘Synapticactivity <strong>and</strong> the construction of corticalcircuits’. Science, 274: 1133.Kellmer-Pringle, M. (1980) The Needs ofChildren (2nd edition) London: HutchinsonKelly, B. (1996) The significance ofpreschool behaviour problems foradjustment in later life. Early ChildDevelopment <strong>and</strong> Care 117:1-19Kendall, L. (2002) The First Twelve MonthsLondon: IPPRKinard, E.M. (1998) Methodological issuesin assessing resilience in maltreatedchildren. Child Abuse <strong>and</strong> Neglect 22(7):669-80Kinra, S., Nelder, R.P. <strong>and</strong> Lewendon,G.J.(2000) Deprivation <strong>and</strong> childhood obesity:a cross-sectional study of 20,973 childrenin Plymouth,UK. Journal of Epidemiology<strong>and</strong> Community Health 54:456-460Kochanska, G., Coy, K.C.<strong>and</strong> Murray, K.T.(2001) The development of self-regulationin the first four years of life ChildDevelopment 72(4): 1091-111Kohlberg, L. (1984) Essays on MoralDevelopment. Vol.2 The Psychology ofMoral Development San Francisco CA:Harper <strong>and</strong> RowKokkinaki, T. <strong>and</strong> Kugiumutzakis, G.(2000) Basic aspects of vocal initation ininfant-parent interactions in the first six160 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


months Journal of Reproductive <strong>and</strong> InfantPsychology 18(3): 173-87Kong, D.S. (1995) Psychiatric disorders inpre-schoolers. Singapore Medical Journal36(3):318-21Konner, M. (1991) Childhood London:Ebury PressKoopman, L.P., Smit. H.A., Heijnen, M.L.,Wijga, A. (2001) Respira<strong>to</strong>ry infections ininfants: interaction of parental allergy,childcare <strong>and</strong> siblings. The PLAMA Study.Pediatrics 108:943-48Kornhaber, M.<strong>and</strong> Marcos, H. (2000)Young children’s communication withmothers <strong>and</strong> fathers: functions <strong>and</strong>contents British Journal of DevelopmentalPsychology 18(2): 187-210Kotch, J.B. (1997) Stress, social support <strong>and</strong>substantiated maltreatment in the second<strong>and</strong> third years of life Child Abuse <strong>and</strong>Neglect 21(11): 1025-37Kress G (1997) Before Writing: Rethinkingthe paths <strong>to</strong> literacy London: RoutledgeKress G (2000) Early Spelling, Betweenconvention <strong>and</strong> creativity London:RoutledgeKristensen, I., Aaby, P., Jensen, H.<strong>and</strong>Fine, P. (2002) Routine vaccinations <strong>and</strong>child survival: follow up study in Guinea-Bessau British Medical Journal 321:1435-9Kuhl, P.K., Williams, K.A., Lacerda, F.,Stevens, K.N.<strong>and</strong> Lindblom, B. (1992)Linguistic experience alters phoneticperceptions in infants by six months ofage Science 255: 606-608Kumar, R.C. (1997) ‘Anybody’s child’: severedisorders of mother-<strong>to</strong>-infant bonding.British Journal of Psychiatry Vol 171:175-81Kurtz, P.D., Gaudin, J.M., Wodarski, J.S.<strong>and</strong> Howing, P.T. (1993) Maltreatment <strong>and</strong>the school-aged child: school performanceconsequences. Child Abuse <strong>and</strong> Neglect17:581-9Laakso, M-L., Poikkeus, A-M., Katajäki <strong>and</strong>Lyytinen, P. (1999) Early intentionalcommunication as a predic<strong>to</strong>r of languagedevelopment in young <strong>to</strong>ddlers FirstLanguage 19:207-31Labrell, F. (1994) A typical interactionbehaviour between fathers <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ddlers:teasing Early Development <strong>and</strong> Parenting3(2): 125-30Laevers, F. (2000) Forward <strong>to</strong> basics!Deep-level learning <strong>and</strong> the experientialapproach. Early Years 20(2):20-29Laucht, M., Esser, G.<strong>and</strong> Schmidt, M.H.(1994) Parental mental disorder <strong>and</strong> earlychild development European Child <strong>and</strong>Adolescent Psychiatry 3(3): 125-37Law, J. (2000) The politics of breastfeeding.Signs: Journal of Women in Culture <strong>and</strong>Society 25(2):407-50Lawson, M.<strong>and</strong> Thomas, M. (1999) VitaminD concentrations in Asian children agedtwo years living in Engl<strong>and</strong>: populationsurvey BMJ 318(7175):28Leach, P. (1994) Children First London:Michael JosephLeach, P. (1997) Your Baby <strong>and</strong> ChildLondon: Penguin161


Leon, D.A., Vagero, D. <strong>and</strong> Olansson, P.O.(1992) Social class differences in infantmortality in Sweden: comparison withEngl<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> Wales British Medical Journal305:687-91LeVine, R.A.<strong>and</strong> Miller, P.M. (1990)Commentary. Human Development33:73-80Lieberman, A.F. (1993) The Emotional Lifeof the Toddler New York: Free PressLier, L. (1997) The Copenhagen model ofearly preventive intervention aimed athigh risk families. Social Work in Europe4(2): 15-18Light, P. (1983) Social interaction <strong>and</strong>cognitive development: a review of post-Piagetian research. In S. Meadows (ed)Developing Thinking London: Methuenpp.67-88Lindon, J. (1999) Too safe for their owngood? Helping children learn about risk<strong>and</strong> lifeskills London:NEYNLindon, J. (2001) Underst<strong>and</strong>ing Children’sPlay. Cheltenham: Nelson ThornesLo n g, F. <strong>and</strong> Ga rd u q u e, L . ( 1987) Co nt i n u i tybetween home <strong>and</strong> family daycare:caregivers’ <strong>and</strong> mothers’ perceptions <strong>and</strong>children’s social experiences. In D.L. Peters<strong>and</strong> S. Kon<strong>to</strong>s (eds) Continuity <strong>and</strong>Discontinuity of Experience in Child CareNorwood, New Jersey: Ablex pp.69-90Lowenthal, B. (1998) The effects of earlychildhood abuse <strong>and</strong> the development ofresiliency. Early Child Development <strong>and</strong> Care142:43-52Lowenthal, B. (1999) Effects ofmaltreatment <strong>and</strong> ways <strong>to</strong> promotechildren’s resiliency Childhood Education75(4):204-9Lubeck,S. (1986) S<strong>and</strong>box Society London:FalmerLuthar, S.S. (1993) Methodological <strong>and</strong>conceptual issues in research onchildhood resilience:annotation. Journalof Child Psychology, Psychiatry <strong>and</strong> AlliedDisciplines 34(4):441-53Luthar, S.S.<strong>and</strong> Zigler, E. (1991)Vulnerability <strong>and</strong> competence:a reviewof research on resilience in childhood.Am e ri can Jo u rnal of Ort h o p s yc h i a t ry 61:6-22Luthar, S.S., Cicchetti, D. <strong>and</strong> Becker, B.(2000) The construct of resilience:a criticalevaluation <strong>and</strong> guidelines for future work.Child Development 71(3): 543-75Lynch,M. (1998) Making health equalitya reality. Poverty 101: 8-10Lyons-Ruth,K.<strong>and</strong> Zeanah,C.H. Jr. (1993)The family context of infant mental health:I – affective development in the primar ycaregiving relationship. In C.H. Zeanah Jr.(ed) H<strong>and</strong>book of Infant Mental Health NewYork: Guildford Press pp.14-37Ma c ro ry, G . ( 2001) Language deve l o p m e nt :what do early years practitioners need <strong>to</strong>know? Early Years 21(1):33-40Makin, L., Cambell, J.<strong>and</strong> Jones Diaz, C.(1995) One Childhood Many LanguagesSydney: Harper Educational162 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


M<strong>and</strong>ler, J. (1999) Preverbal representation<strong>and</strong> language. In P. Bloom,M. Peterson,L. Nadel <strong>and</strong> M. Garrett Language <strong>and</strong>Space Cambridge Mass: A Bradford Book/MIT Press pp.365-84Manning-Mor<strong>to</strong>n, J. & Thorp, M. (2001)Key Times, A Framework for Developing HighQuality Provision for Children Under ThreeYears Old. London: Print EmporiumLtd/Camden Early Years Under ThreesDevelopment GroupMarchant R (2001) Working with DisabledChildren. In P. Foley, J. Roche <strong>and</strong> S. Tucker(eds) Children in Society: ContemporaryTheory, Policy <strong>and</strong> Practice Buckingham:Open University Press 215-23Marcos, H. (1995) Mother-child <strong>and</strong> fatherchildcommunication in the second year:a functional approach. Early Development<strong>and</strong> Parenting 4(2): 49-61Ma rku s, J .,Mu n dy, P. , Mo ra l e s, M .,De l g a d o,C.E.F. <strong>and</strong> Yale, M. (2000) Individualdifferences in infant skills as predic<strong>to</strong>rs ofchild-caregiver joint attention <strong>and</strong>language Social Development 9(3):302-15Martin, C.J., Platt, S.D. <strong>and</strong> Hunt, S.M.(1987) Housing conditions <strong>and</strong> ill healthBritish Medical Journal 294: 1125-7Martin, W. <strong>and</strong> Dombey, H. (2002) Findinga Voice: Language <strong>and</strong> Play in the HomeCorner in Language <strong>and</strong> Education16(1):48-61Mason, M. (1992) Disability Equality in theClassroom:A Human Rights Issue London:Disability Equality in EducationMason, M.<strong>and</strong> Davies, A. (1993) Inclusion,the Way Forward:a Guide <strong>to</strong> Integration forYoung Disabled Children London: VOLCUFMasten, A.S.<strong>and</strong> Coatsworth, J.D. (1998)The development of competence infavourable <strong>and</strong> unfavourableenvironments: lessons from research onsuccessful children. American Psychologist53(2):205-20Matthews, J. (1998) The Representationof Events <strong>and</strong> Objects in the Drawingsof Young Children from Singapore <strong>and</strong>London: Implications for the CurriculumEarly Years 18(1):90-109Matthews, J. (2002) Developing Drawing<strong>and</strong> Painting with Young Children London:Paul Chapman/SageMayall, B. (1996) Children, Health <strong>and</strong> theSocial Order. Buckingham: Open UniversityPress.McGlone, P., Dobson, B., Dowler, E. <strong>and</strong>Nelson, M. (1999) Food Projects <strong>and</strong> HowThey Work York: Joseph RowntreeFoundationMcGuire, J. (1991) Social interactionsof young, withdrawn children in daynurseries Journal of Reproductive <strong>and</strong>Infant Psychology 9: 169-79McGurk, H., Caplan, M., Hennessy, E. <strong>and</strong>Moss, P. (1993) Controversy, theory <strong>and</strong>social conytext in contemporary daycareresearch. Journal of Child Psychology <strong>and</strong>Psychiatry 34(1): 3-23Meade, A (2001) One Hundred BillionNeurons: How do they become organised?163


In T. David (ed) Advances in Applied EarlyChildhood Education. Vol.1. PromotingEvidence-based Practice in Early ChildhoodEducation: Research <strong>and</strong> its ImplicationsLondon:JAI pp.3-26Meadows, S. (1993) The Child as Thinker.London: RoutledgeMeadows, S. (1996) Parenting Behaviour<strong>and</strong> Children’s Cognitive Development.Hove: Psychology Press.Meek, M. (1985) Play <strong>and</strong> Paradoxes: SomeConsiderations of Imagination <strong>and</strong>Language. In G. Wells <strong>and</strong> J. Nicholls (eds)Language <strong>and</strong> Learning: An InteractionalPerspective Buckingham: Open UniversityPress pp. 41-57Meek, M. (1998) Important ReadingLessons. In B. Cox (ed) Literacy is NotEnough Manchester: Manchester BookTrust pp.116-23Meek, M. (2000) TransitionalTransformations. In E. Bearne <strong>and</strong> V. Watson(eds) Where Texts <strong>and</strong> Children MeetLondon: Routledge pp.198-212Meggitt, C. (2001) Baby <strong>and</strong> Child Health.Oxford: Heinemann Educational PublishersMeisels, S.J.<strong>and</strong> Shonkoff, J.P. (eds) (1990)H<strong>and</strong>book of Early Childhood InterventionsCambridge: Cambridge University PressMelhuish, E.C.<strong>and</strong> Moss, P. (eds) (1991)Day care for Young Children: InternationalPerspectives London: Tavis<strong>to</strong>ck/RoutledgeMeltzoff, A.N. (1988) Infant imitiation afterone week delay: long term memory fornovel acts <strong>and</strong> multiple stimuli.Developmental Psychology 24:470-76Meltzoff, A.N.<strong>and</strong> Bor<strong>to</strong>n, R.W. (1979)Intermodal matching by human neonatesNature 282:403-4Menzies Lyth, I. (1995) The developmen<strong>to</strong>f the self in children in institutions.In J. Trowell <strong>and</strong> M. Bower (eds) TheEmotional Needs of Young Children <strong>and</strong>Their Families: Using Psychoanalytic Ideasin the Community London: Routledgepp.189-208Millard, E., Taylor, S.<strong>and</strong> Watson, S. (2000)Books for Babies means books for parents<strong>to</strong>o: Research in Progress in Reading 34(3):130-33Miller, M. (1996) Relevance of resilience<strong>to</strong> individuals with learning disabilities.International Journal of Disability,Development <strong>and</strong> Education 43(3):255-269Minde, K. (1993) Prematurity <strong>and</strong> seriousmedical illness in infancy: implications fordevelopment <strong>and</strong> intervention.In C.H.Zeanah Jr (ed) H<strong>and</strong>book of InfantMental Health New York: Guildford Presspp.87-105Molfese, V.J., DiLalla, L.F. <strong>and</strong> Lovelace, L.(1996) Perinatal,home environment, <strong>and</strong>infant measures as successful predic<strong>to</strong>rsof preschool cognitive <strong>and</strong> verbal abilities.International Journal of BehaviouralDevelopment 19(1): 101-19Moreno, E. (1993)‘Healthy families makehealthy babies’. World Health 3, May-June1993:23-25.164 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Mo r s s, J . ( 1990) The Bi o l ogising of Ch i l d h oodHove: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesMortimer, H. (2001) Special Needs <strong>and</strong> EarlyYears Provision London: ContinuumMor<strong>to</strong>n,N. <strong>and</strong> Browne, K.D. (1998) Theory<strong>and</strong> observation of attachment <strong>and</strong> itsrelation <strong>to</strong> child maltreatment:a reviewChild Abuse <strong>and</strong> Neglect 22(11): 1093-1104Moses, L.J., Baldwin, D.A., Rosicky, J.G.<strong>and</strong> Tidball,G. (2001) Evidence forreferential underst<strong>and</strong>ing in the emotionsdomain at 12 <strong>and</strong> 18 months ChildDevelopment 72(3): 718-35Moss, P. (1990) Work, family <strong>and</strong> the careof children:equality <strong>and</strong> responsibility.Children <strong>and</strong> Society 4(2): 145-66Moyles, J. (2002) Face <strong>to</strong> face with SusanGreenfield. Interplay Summer 2002:20-22Murray, L.<strong>and</strong> Trevarthen, C. (1985)Emotional regulation of interactionsbetween two-month-olds <strong>and</strong> theirmothers. In T.M. Field <strong>and</strong> N.A. Fox (eds)Social Perception in Infants Norwood, NJ:Ablex pp.177-97Murray, L., Fiori-Cowley, A., Hooper, R.<strong>and</strong>Cooper, P. (1996) The impact of postnataldepression <strong>and</strong> associated adversity onearly mother-infant interactions <strong>and</strong> laterinfant outcome. Child Development67(5):2512-26Murray, L.<strong>and</strong> Cooper, P.J. (1997)Postpartum depression <strong>and</strong> childdevelopment Psychological Medicine27(2): 253-60Murray, L., Sinclair, D., Cooper, P.,Dudournau, P., Turner, P. <strong>and</strong> Stein, A.(1999) The socioemotional developmen<strong>to</strong>f five-year-old children of post-natallydepressed mothers. Journal of ChildPsychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry <strong>and</strong> AlliedDisciplines 40(8): 1259-71Murray, L.<strong>and</strong> Andrews, E. (2000) SocialBaby. London: Richmond PressMyers, H.F. <strong>and</strong> Taylor, S. (1998) Familycontributions <strong>to</strong> risk <strong>and</strong> resilience inAfrican American children Journal ofComparative Family Studies 29(1):215-29National Heart Forum (2002) Towards ageneration free from coronary heart disease.Policy action for children’s <strong>and</strong> youngpeople’s health <strong>and</strong> well-being. London:National Heart ForumNakamura, W.M., Stewart, K.B. <strong>and</strong>Tat a rka ,M. E. (2000) Assessing fat h e r- i n f a ntinteractions using the NCAST teachingscale:a pilot study. American Journal ofOccupational Therapy 54(1):44-51Nelson,C. (1987) The recognition of facialexpressions in the first year of life:mechanisms of development ChildDevelopment 48:854-61Nelson,C. (1999)‘Neural Plasticity <strong>and</strong>Human Development’ Current Directionsin Psychological Science 8(2):42-45.Nelson,C.<strong>and</strong> Bloom, F. (1997)‘ChildDevelopment <strong>and</strong> Neuroscience’. ChildDevelopment 68(5): 970-87.Nelson,K. (ed) (1985) Children’s LanguageHillsdale NJ: Erlbaum165


Nelson,K. (1986) Event knowledge, structure<strong>and</strong> function in development: Hillsdale NJ:ErlbaumNelson,K. (1999) The developmentalpsychology of language <strong>and</strong> thought.In M.Bennett (ed) DevelopmentalPsychology London: Taylor <strong>and</strong> Francispp.185-204Newson, J.<strong>and</strong> Newson, E. (1963) Patternsof Infant Care in an Urban CommunityHarmondsworth: PenguinNewson, E. <strong>and</strong> Newson, J. (1979) Toys<strong>and</strong> Playthings London: PenguinNew Zeal<strong>and</strong> Ministry of Education(1996) Te Whariki Welling<strong>to</strong>n,NZ: LearningMedia LtdNICHD Early Childhood ResearchNetwork (1996) Characteristics of infantchild care:fac<strong>to</strong>rs contributing <strong>to</strong> positivecaregiving. Early Childhood ResearchQuarterly 11:269-306Norman,K. (ed) (1992) Thinking VoicesSevenoaks: Hodder <strong>and</strong> S<strong>to</strong>ugh<strong>to</strong>nNSPCC (2000) Protecting Babies from Harm:The Professional’s Response London: NSPCCNutbrown,C. (1994) Threads of ThinkingLondon: Paul ChapmanNuttall, W. (1999) The effects of postureon learning: insights from the Alex<strong>and</strong>erTechnique Early Years 20(1):65-76Odofsky, J.D. (ed) (1987) H<strong>and</strong>book ofInfant Development New York: WileyOpie, I.<strong>and</strong> Opie, P. (1959) The Lore <strong>and</strong>Language of Schoolchildren Oxford: OxfordUniversity PressOsborn, A.F. (1990) Resilient children:a longitudinal study of high achievingsocially disadvantaged children. Early ChildDevelopment <strong>and</strong> Care 62: 23-47Oyserman D, Radin,N.<strong>and</strong> Saltz, E. (1994)Predictions of nurturant parenting in teenmothers living in <strong>three</strong> generationalfamilies Child Psychiatry <strong>and</strong> HumanDevelopment 24(4):215-30Pahl, K. (1999) Transformations: Children’sMeaning Making in a Nurser y S<strong>to</strong>ke onTrent: TrenthamPahl,K. (2001) Texts as artefacts crossingsites:map making at home <strong>and</strong> schoolReading 35(3): 120-125Pa po us˘e k , M ., Pa po us˘e k ,H. <strong>and</strong> Ha rri s, B. J .(1987) The emergence of play in parentinfantinteractions. In D.Görlitz <strong>and</strong>J.F.Wohlwill (eds) Curiosity, Imagination <strong>and</strong>Play Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaumpp.214-246Parke, T. <strong>and</strong> Drury, R. (2001) Languagedevelopment at home <strong>and</strong> school:gains<strong>and</strong> losses in young bilinguals. Early Years21(2): 117-127Parkes, C.M., Stevenson-Hinde, J.<strong>and</strong> Marris, P. (eds) (1996) AttachmentAcross the Life Cycle (2nd edition)London: Routledge166 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Patel, P., Mendall, M.A., Khulusi, S.,Northfield, T.C.<strong>and</strong> Strachan, D.P. (1994)Heliobacter pylori infection in childhood:risk fac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> effect on growth BritishMedical Journal 309: 1119-23Pearl, M., Braveman, P. <strong>and</strong> Abrams, B.(2001) The relationship of neighbourhoodsocio-economic characteristics <strong>to</strong> birthweight among five ethnic groups inCalifornia American Journal of Public Health91: 1808-14Pedro-Carroll, J. (2001) The promotion ofwellness in children <strong>and</strong> families:challenges <strong>and</strong> opportunities. AmericanPsychologist 56(11):993-1004Penn,H. (2001) Research in the MajorityWorld. In T.David (ed) Advances in AppliedEarly Childhood Education. Vol.1. PromotingEvidence-Based Practice in Early ChildhoodEducation London: JAI pp.289-308Perner, J. (1999) Theory of Mind. InM.Bennett (ed) Developmental PsychologyLondon: Taylor <strong>and</strong> Francis pp.205-230Perry, B.D. (1994) Neurobiological sequelaeof childhood trauma: post-traumatic stressdisorders in children. In M. Murberg (ed)Catecholamine function in post-traumaticstress disorder: emerging conceptsWashing<strong>to</strong>n DC: American Psychiatric Presspp.233-55Petrie, P. (1997) Communicating withChildren <strong>and</strong> Adults: Interpersonal Skills forEarly Years <strong>and</strong> Play Work. 2nd Edition.London: ArnoldPi a g e t, J . ( 1932) The Mo ral Judgement of theChild London: Routledge <strong>and</strong> Kegan PaulPi a g e t, J . ( 1955) The Language <strong>and</strong>Thought of the Ch i l d Lo n d o n : Ro u t l e d g e<strong>and</strong> Kegan Pa u lPiaget, J. (1962) Play, Dreams <strong>and</strong> Imitationin Childhood New York: Nor<strong>to</strong>nPi e rce, D. (2000) Mate rnal Ma n a g e m e nt ofthe Home as a Deve l o p m e ntal Pl ay Sp a cefor In f a nts <strong>and</strong> Toddlers The Am e ri ca nJo u rnal of Occupational Th e ra py 54 :290- 299Plomin,R. <strong>and</strong> Bergeman,C.S.(1991) Thenature of nurture: genetic influence on‘environmental’ measures. Behavioural <strong>and</strong>Brain Sciences 14:373-427Pollack, S.D., Cicchetti, D., Hornung, K.<strong>and</strong> Reed, A. (2000) Recognizing emotionin faces: developmental effects of childabuse <strong>and</strong> neglect DevelopmentalPsychology 36(5):679-88Polnay, L.<strong>and</strong> Hull, D. (1998) CommunityPaediatrics London: Churchill Livings<strong>to</strong>nePost, J.<strong>and</strong> Hohmann, M. (2000) TenderCare <strong>and</strong> Early Learning, Supporting Families<strong>and</strong> Toddlers in Childcare Settings. Michigan:High/Scope PressPowell, G.J. (1985) Self-concepts amongAfro-American students in racially isolatedminority schools: some regionaldifferences Journal of the AmericanAcademy of Child Psychiatry 24: 142-9Powell,J. (1997) Who is listening?Protecting young children from abuse.In L. Abbott <strong>and</strong> H. Moylett (eds) Working167


with Under Threes: Training <strong>and</strong> ProfessionalDevelopment Buckingham: OpenUniversity Press pp. 129-140Pridham,K., Becker, P. <strong>and</strong> Brown,R.(2000) Effects of infant <strong>and</strong> caregivingconditions on an infant’s focusedexploration of <strong>to</strong>ys. Journal of AdvancedNursing 31(6): 1439-1448Pridmore, P. <strong>and</strong> Stephens, D. (2000)Children as Partners for Health:A criticalreview of the child-<strong>to</strong>-child approach.London: Zed Books.Pugh,G., De’Ath, E. <strong>and</strong> Smith, C. (1994)Confident Parents, Confident Children: Policy<strong>and</strong> Practice in Parent Education <strong>and</strong>Support London: NCBPugh, G.<strong>and</strong> Selleck, D. (1996) Listening<strong>to</strong> <strong>and</strong> Communicating with YoungChildren in Diane, R., Up<strong>to</strong>n,G.<strong>and</strong> Varma,V. (eds) The Voice of the Child: A H<strong>and</strong>bookfor Professionals. London: Falmer Presspp.120-36Raban, B., Waniganayake, M., Deans, J.,Brown,R., Ure, C.<strong>and</strong> Reynolds, B. (2002)Describing quality in practice:developinga framework. Paper presented at the10th Australian Research in Early ChildhoodEducation Annual Conference, CanberraJanuary 2002Raikes, H. (1996) A secure base for babies:applying attachment concepts <strong>to</strong> theinfant care setting. Young Children 51(5):59-67Ramey, C.T. <strong>and</strong> Ramey, S.L. (1998) Earlyintervention <strong>and</strong> early experience.American Psychologist 53(2): 109-20Rayna, S. (2001) The very beginnings of<strong>to</strong>getherness in shared play among youngchildren. International Journal of Early YearsEducation 9(2): 109-15Read, J. (2000) Disability, the Family <strong>and</strong>Society: Listening <strong>to</strong> Mothers Buckingham:Open University PressReading, R., Raybould, S.<strong>and</strong> Jarvis, S.(1993) Deprivation, low birth weight <strong>and</strong>children’s height:a comparison betweenrural <strong>and</strong> urban areas British MedicalJournal 307: 1458-62Reading, R., Colver, A., Openshaw, S.<strong>and</strong>Jarvis, S. (1994) Do interventions thatimprove immunisation uptake also reducesocial inequalities? British Medical Journal308: 1142-4Rhein,L.M., Ginsburg, K.R.,Schwarz, D.F.,Pin<strong>to</strong>-Martin, J.A., Zhao, H., Morgan, A.P.<strong>and</strong> Slap, G.B. (1997) Teen fatherparticipation in child rearing: familyperspectives Journal of Adolescent Health21:244-52Richman,N. (1978) Depression in mothersof young children. Journal of the RoyalSociety of Medicine 71:489-93Ri c h m a n ,N., Steve n s o n , J . <strong>and</strong> Gra h a m ,P. J .(1982) Pre-school <strong>to</strong> school a behaviouralstudy. London: Academic PressRoberts, H. (2001) What Works in ReducingInequalities in Child Health London:Barnado’s168 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Roberts, H. (2001a) Fit or fat? Coordinate81: 12-13Roberts, I.<strong>and</strong> Pless, B. (1993) Social policyas a cause of childhood accidents: thechildren of lone mothers British MedicalJournal 311: 925-8Roberts, I.<strong>and</strong> Power, C. (1996) Does thedecline in child injury mortality vary bysocial class? A comparison of class specificmortality in 1981-1991 British MedicalJournal 313:784-6Roberts, R. (2002) Developing Self-esteemin Young Children London: PaulChapman/SageRobertson, J.<strong>and</strong> Robers<strong>to</strong>n, J. (1989)Separation <strong>and</strong> the Very Young. London:Free Association PressRobinson,K. (2001) Out of Our Minds:Learning <strong>to</strong> Be Creative Oxford: Caps<strong>to</strong>neRobinson, J.L.<strong>and</strong> Acevedo, M.C. (2001)Infant reactivity <strong>and</strong> reliance on motherduring emotion challenges:prediction ofcognitive <strong>and</strong> language skills in a lowincomesample. Child Development 72(2):402-15Rochat, P., Querido, J.G.<strong>and</strong> Striano, T.(1999) Emerging sensitivity <strong>to</strong> the timing<strong>and</strong> structure of pro<strong>to</strong>conversation in earlyinfancy. Developmental Psychology35(4):950-7Rodd, J. (1996) Underst<strong>and</strong>ing YoungChildren’s Behaviour London: Allen <strong>and</strong>UnwinRosen,H. (1988) S<strong>to</strong>ries <strong>and</strong> MeaningsLondon: NATERosenthal,M.K. (2000) Quality in earlychildhood education <strong>and</strong> care:a culturalcontext. Keynote address, SymposiumCurriculum, Policies <strong>and</strong> Practices in EarlyChildhood Education, Malta,28 Nov –3 Dec 2000Rosser, R. (1994) Cognitive Development:Psychological <strong>and</strong> Biological PerspectivesNeedham Heights Mass: Allyn <strong>and</strong> BaconRossiter, J.C. (1998) Promoting breastfeeding: the perceptions of Vietnamesemothers in Sydney, Australia Journal ofAdvanced Nursing 28(3):598-605Rouse Selleck, D. (1995) Managing <strong>to</strong>Change. Module 4: Playing <strong>and</strong> LearningLondon: National Children’s BureauRutter, M. (1972) Maternal DeprivationReassessed Harmondsworth: PenguinRutter, M. (1989) Pathways from childhood<strong>to</strong> adult life. Journal of Child Psychology <strong>and</strong>Psychiatry 30: 23-51Rutter, M. (1995) Clinical implications ofattachment concepts: retrospect <strong>and</strong>prospect Child Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry36(4):549-571Rutter, M. (1999) Psychosocial adversity<strong>and</strong> child psychopathology British Journalof Psychiatry 174:480-93Rutter, M. (2000) Children in substitutecare:some conceptual considerations <strong>and</strong>research implications Children <strong>and</strong> YouthServices Review 22(9/10): 685-703169


Rutter, M.<strong>and</strong> the English <strong>and</strong> RomanianAdoptees (ERA) Study Team (1998)Developmental catch-up, <strong>and</strong> deficit,following adoption after severe globalearly privation. Journal of Child Psychology<strong>and</strong> Psychiatry 39:465-473Sameroff, A.J. (1993) Models ofdevelopment <strong>and</strong> developmental risk. InC.H. Zeanah,Jnr. (ed) H<strong>and</strong>book of InfantMental Health New York: Guildford Presspp.3-13Schaffer, H.R. (1998) Making Decisionsabout Children: Psychological Questions <strong>and</strong>Answers Oxford: BlackwellSchieffelin, B.B. <strong>and</strong> Ochs, E. (1986)Language Socialisation across CulturesCambridge: Cambridge University PressSchuler, M.E., Black, M.M.<strong>and</strong> Starr, R.H.(1995) Determinants of mother-infantinteraction:effects of prenatal drugexposure, social support, <strong>and</strong> infanttemperament. Journal of Clinical ChildPsychology 24(4):397-405Schweinhart, L., Barnes, H. <strong>and</strong> Weikart, D.(1993) The High/Scope Perry Pre-schoolStudy through Age 27. Michigan:High/Scope PressSeifer, R. <strong>and</strong> Sameroff, A.J. (1986) Theconcept, measurement <strong>and</strong> interpretationof temperament in young children.Advances in Developmental <strong>and</strong> BehavioralPediatrics 7:1- 43Seifer, R., Clark, G.N.<strong>and</strong> Sameroff, A.J.(1991) Positive effects of interactioncoaching on infants with developmentaldisabilities <strong>and</strong> their mothers. AmericanJournal on Mental Retardation 96:1-11Selleck, D. <strong>and</strong> Griffin,S. (1996) Qualityfor the under <strong>three</strong>s. In G. Pugh (ed)Contemporary Issues in the Early Years (2ndedition) London: Paul Chapman/Sagepp.152-169Selwyn,J. (2000) Infancy. In M. Boushel,M. Fawcett <strong>and</strong> J. Selwyn (eds) Focus onEarly Childhood: Principles <strong>and</strong> RealitiesOxford: Blackwell pp.35-48Sendak,M. (1970) Where the Wild ThingsAre Harmondsworth: PenguinShonkoff, J.<strong>and</strong> Phillips, D. (2000) FromNeurons <strong>to</strong> Neighbourhoods: The Science ofEarly Childhood Development. Washing<strong>to</strong>n:National Academy Press.Shore, R. (1997) Rethinking the Brain: NewInsights in<strong>to</strong> Early Development. New York:Families <strong>and</strong> Work InstituteSiegel, D. (1999) The Developing Mind NewYork: GuilfordSims, M., Hutchins, T. <strong>and</strong> Taylor, M. (1997)Conflict as social interaction:buildingrelationship skills in child care settings.Child <strong>and</strong> Youth Care Forum 26(4):247-60Sinclair, H. (1971) Sensorimo<strong>to</strong>r actionpatterns as the condition for theacquisition of syntax. In R.Huxley <strong>and</strong>E.Ingrams (eds) Language Acquisition:Models <strong>and</strong> Methods Academic Presspp. 121-36170 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Singer, E. (1992) Child Care <strong>and</strong> thePsychology of Development London:RoutledgeSinger, E. (1993) Shared care for children.Theory <strong>and</strong> Psychology 3(4): 129-49Singer, E. (1998) Shared care for children InM. Woodhead, D. Faulkner <strong>and</strong> K. Little<strong>to</strong>n(eds) Cultural Worlds of Early ChildhoodLondon: Routledge/Open Universitypp.64-83Singer, E. (2001) The logic of youngchildren’s (nonverbal) behavior.Keynote Address, European EarlyChildhood Educational ResearchAssociation conference, HogeschoolAlkmaar,The Netherl<strong>and</strong>s, 29 August –1 September 2001Siraj-Blatchford, I. (2001) Diversity <strong>and</strong>learning in the early years. In G. Pugh (ed)Contemporary Issues in the Ear ly Years(3rd edition) London: Paul Chapman/Sagewith Coram Family pp.96-108Siraj-Blatchford, I.<strong>and</strong> Clarke, P. (2000)Supporting Identity, Diversity <strong>and</strong> Languagein the Early Years Buckingham: OpenUniversity PressSkinner, B.F. (1938) The Behaviour ofOrganisms Englewood Cliffs NJ:Prentice-HallSlobin, D.I. (ed) (1985) The Cross-linguisticStudy of Language Acquisition Vol 2:Theoretical Issues Hillsdale NJ: ErlbaumSmith, A. (1999) Quality childcare <strong>and</strong> jointattention. International Journal of EarlyYears Education 7(1):85-98Smith,A.B., Grima,G., Gaffney, M.<strong>and</strong>Powell,K.with Masse, L.<strong>and</strong> Barnett, S.(2000) Early Childhood Education. LiteratureReview Report <strong>to</strong> the Ministry of EducationDunedin: New Zeal<strong>and</strong> Ministry ofEducationSmith,C. (1996) Developing ParentingProgrammes London: NCBSm i t h ,C. <strong>and</strong> Pu g h ,G. ( 1996) Le a rning <strong>to</strong> bea Parent:a Survey of Parenting ProgrammesLondon: Family Policy Studies CentreSmith,G.C.S.<strong>and</strong> Pell, J.P. (2001) Teenagepregnancy <strong>and</strong> risk of adverse perinataloputcomes associated with first <strong>and</strong>second birth: population basedretrospective cohort study British MedicalJournal 323:476-9Smith, K.E., L<strong>and</strong>ry, S.H., Swank, P.R.<strong>and</strong>Baldwin,C.D. (1996) The relation ofmedical risk <strong>and</strong> maternal stimulationwith pre-term infants’ development ofcognitive, language <strong>and</strong> daily livingJournal of Child Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry<strong>and</strong> Allied Disciplines 37(7):855-64Smokowski, P.R. (1998) Prevention <strong>and</strong>intervention strategies for promotingresilience in disadvantaged children. SocialScience Review 72(3):337-64Smolucha, F. (1991) Social origins ofprivate speech in pretend play. In R.M. Diaz<strong>and</strong> L.E. Berk (eds) Private Speech: FromSocial Interaction <strong>to</strong> Self-regulation.Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum pp.123-41171


Snow, C.E. (1977) The development ofconversation between mothers <strong>and</strong> babiesJournal of Child Language 4: 1-22Snow, C.E. <strong>and</strong> Ferguson,C.A. (eds) (1977)Talking <strong>to</strong> Children Cambridge: CambridgeUniversity PressSpencer, M.B. (1985) Cultural cognition<strong>and</strong> social cognition as identity fac<strong>to</strong>rsin black children’s personal-social growth.In M.B. Spencer, G.K. Brookins <strong>and</strong>W.R. Allen (eds) Beginnings: the Social <strong>and</strong>Affective Development of Black ChildrenHillsdale NJ: Erlbaum pp.215-30Spencer, N. (2000) Poverty <strong>and</strong> Child HealthOxford: Radcliffe Medical PressSpencer, N., Lewis, M.A.<strong>and</strong> Logan,S.(1993) Multiple admission <strong>and</strong> deprivation.Archives of Disease in Childhood 68:760-2Spencer, N., Bamburg, S., Logan,S.<strong>and</strong>Leicester, G. (1999) Socio-economic status<strong>and</strong> birth weight: comparison of an areabasedmeasure with the RegistrarGeneral’s Social Class. Journal ofEpidemiology <strong>and</strong> Community Health53:4995-498Steele, M., Steele, H.<strong>and</strong> Fonagy, P.(1995) Associations among attachmentclassifications of mothers, fathers <strong>and</strong>infants Child Development 67:541-55Ste rn , D. N . ( 1974) The goal <strong>and</strong> stru ct u re ofmother-infant play. Journal of the AmericanAcademy of Child Psychiatry 13:402-21Stern, D.N. (1977) The First Relationship:Mother <strong>and</strong> Infant Cambridge Mass:Harvard University PressStern, D.N. (1985) The Interpersonal Worldof the Infant New York: Basic BooksStevenson,J. (ed) (1985) Recent Researchin Developmental Psychopathology Oxford:PergamonStriano, T. <strong>and</strong> Rochat, P. (1999)Developmental Link Between Dyadic <strong>and</strong>Triadic Social Competence in InfancyBritish Journal of Developmental Psychology17:551-562Suess, G.J., Grossman,K.E. <strong>and</strong> Sroufe, L.A.(1992) Effects of infant attachment <strong>to</strong>mother <strong>and</strong> father on quality ofadaptation <strong>to</strong> pre-school InternationalJournal of Behavioural Development15:43-65Sunday Times (2002) Stars join Hornbyin MMR crusade. The Sunday Times23 June. p.9Tabors, P.O. (1997) One Child,TwoLanguages:a Guide for Pre-schoolEduca<strong>to</strong>rs of Children Learning Englishas a Second Language Baltimore MD:Paul Brookes PublishingTaylor, J.<strong>and</strong> Daniel, B. (1999) Interagencypractice in children with non-organicfailure <strong>to</strong> thrive: is there a gap betweenhealth <strong>and</strong> social care? Child Abuse Review8(5):325-38Tebes, J.K., Kaufman, J.S., Adnopoz, J.<strong>and</strong> Racusin,G.(2001) Resilience <strong>and</strong>family psychosocial processes amongchildren of parents with serious mentaldisorders. Journal of Child <strong>and</strong> FamilyStudies 10(1): 115-36172 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Teti, D.M.<strong>and</strong> Gelf<strong>and</strong>, D.M. (1991)Behavioural competence among mothersof infants in the first year: the mediationalrole of maternal self efficacy ChildDevelopment 62:918-29Teti, D.M., Wolfe Sakin, J., Kucera, E. <strong>and</strong>Corns, K.M. (1996) And baby makes four:predic<strong>to</strong>rs of attachment security amongpre-school age firstborns during thetransition <strong>to</strong> siblinghood. ChildDevelopment 67(2):579-96The Times (1995) Edi<strong>to</strong>rial: Three kindmice. The Times 12 September. p.17Thiessen,I. (1993) The impact of divorceon children. Child Development <strong>and</strong> Care96: 19-26Thomas, R.M. (1996) Comparing Theories ofChild Development Pacific Grove CA:Brooks/Cole PublishingThompson,L. (1999) Young BilingualLearners at Nursery School Clevedon:Multilingual MattersThompson, M., Petticrew, M.<strong>and</strong>Morrison, D. (2001) Health effects ofhousing improvement: systematic reviewof intervention studies British MedicalJournal 323: 187-90Thompson,R. (1991) Emotional regulation<strong>and</strong> emotional development EducationalPsychology Review 3:269-307Tillery, K.L., Katz, J.<strong>and</strong> Keller, W.D. (2000)Effects of methylphenidate (Ritalin) onaudi<strong>to</strong>ry performance in children withattention <strong>and</strong> audi<strong>to</strong>ry processingdisorders. Journal of Speech, Language <strong>and</strong>Hearing Research 43(4):893-901Tite, R. (1993) How teachers define <strong>and</strong>respond <strong>to</strong> child abuse. The distinctionbetween theoretical <strong>and</strong> reportable cases.Child Abuse <strong>and</strong> Neglect 17(5):591-603Tizard, B. <strong>and</strong> Hughes, M. (1984) YoungChildren Learning London: FontanaTownsend, P. <strong>and</strong> Davidson,N. (eds) (1982)The Black Report Harmondsworth: PenguinTravis, A. (2002) ‘Three out of four parentsfavour single jabs for MMR’. GuardianUnlimited. Available athttp://www.guardian.co.uk (accessed9 April 2002).Trevarthen, C. (1995) The child’s need<strong>to</strong> learn a culture. Children <strong>and</strong> Society9(1): 5-19Trevarthen,C. (2000) Early childhood brainresearch. Keynote address, BAECE-OMEPNational Brainwaves Conference, WarwickUniversity, November 2000Trevarthen, C., Aitken,K., Papoudi, D. <strong>and</strong>Robarts, J. (1998) Children with Autism:Diagnosis <strong>and</strong> Interventions <strong>to</strong> Meet theirNeeds London: Jessica KingsleyTrevarthen,C.<strong>and</strong> Aitken,K.J. (2001)Infant intersubjectivity: research,theory,<strong>and</strong> clinical applications. Journal of ChildPsychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry <strong>and</strong> AlliedDisciplines 42(1):3-48173


Tronick, E.Z., Morelli,G.A.<strong>and</strong> Ivey, P.K.(1992) The Efe Forager infant <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ddler’spattern of social relationships:multiple<strong>and</strong> simultaneous. DevelopmentalPsychology 28:568-77Underdown,A. (2000) When feeding fails:parents’ experiences of faltering growthLondon: The Children’s SocietyUnited Nations (1989) United NationsConvention on the Rights of the ChildGeneva: UNUtting, D. (1995) Family <strong>and</strong> Parenthood.Supporting families, preventing breakdown.York: Joseph Rowntree FoundationVan En k , A .,Bri te n d e i d i j k ,S. E. ,van der Pa u l ,K.M., van Eck, W.J.J. <strong>and</strong> Schulpen, T.W.J.(1998) Perinatal death in ethnic minoritiesin the Netherl<strong>and</strong>s Journal of Epidemiology<strong>and</strong> Community Health 52:735-9Vo l l i n g, B. L .<strong>and</strong> Be l s ky, J .( 1992) In f a nt, f at h e r<strong>and</strong> marital ante ce d e nts of infant - f at h e rat t a c h m e nt securi ty in dual-earner <strong>and</strong>s i n g l e - e a rner families. Inte rn ational Jo u rn a lof Be h av i o u ral Deve l o p m e nt 15 :83- 100Vondra, J.A., Barnett, D. <strong>and</strong> Cicchetti, D.(1989) Perceived <strong>and</strong> actual competenceamong maltreated <strong>and</strong> comparison schoolchildren. Development <strong>and</strong>Psychopathology 1: 237-255Vondra, J.A., Barnett, D. <strong>and</strong> Cicchetti, D.(1990) Self-concept, motivation <strong>and</strong>competence among pre-schoolers frommaltreating <strong>and</strong> comparison families. ChildAbuse <strong>and</strong> Neglect 14: 525-40Vygotsky, L.S. (1978) Mind in Society: theDevelopment of Higher PsychologicalProcesses Cambridge Mass: HarvardUniversity PressVygotsky, L.S. (1986) Thought <strong>and</strong>Language Cambridge Mass: MIT PressWachs, T. (2000) ‘Nutritional deficits <strong>and</strong>behavioural development’. InternationalJournal of Behavioural Development24(4): 435-441.Wade, B. <strong>and</strong> Moore, M. (1993) BookstartLondon: Book TrustWade, B. <strong>and</strong> Moore, M. (1996) Children’searly book behaviour. Educational Review48:283-8Wade, B. <strong>and</strong> Moore, M. (2000) A sure startwith books. Early Years 20(2):39-46Wadsworth, J., Burnell, J., Taylor, B. <strong>and</strong>Butler, N. (1983) Family type <strong>and</strong> accidentsin pre-school children Journal ofEpidemiology <strong>and</strong> Community Health37: 100-104Walker, J. (1989) The behaviour of <strong>three</strong>year-oldchildren who were born pre-termChild: care, health <strong>and</strong> development15: 297-313Walker-Andrews, A.S. (1997) Infants’perception of expressive behaviours:differentiation of multimodal informationPsychological Bulletin 121(3):437-56Walker-Hall,J.<strong>and</strong> Sylva,K. (2001) Whatworks for families of children withbehaviour problems? Evidence fromresearch. In G. Pugh (ed) Contemporary174 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Issues in the Early Years London:Paul Chapman/Sage with Coram Familypp.161-76Wall,K. (forthcoming) Special Needs <strong>and</strong>Early Years: a Practitioner’s Guide London:PCP/SageWalsh,K. (2002) Early childhood teachers<strong>and</strong> child abuse:a critical study of theirwork <strong>and</strong> knowledge. Unpublished PhDthesis Brisbane, Aus: Queensl<strong>and</strong>University of TechnologyWa l s h ,S. S . <strong>and</strong> Ja rv i s, S . N . ( 1992) Me a s u ri n gthe frequency of ‘severe’ accidental injuryin childhood Journal of Epidemiology <strong>and</strong>Community Health 46:26-32Ward, S. (2000) Babytalk London: CenturyWasserman,C.R., Shaw, G.M., Selvin, S.,Gould,J.B. <strong>and</strong> Syme, S.L. (1998) Socioeconomicstatus, neighbourhood socialconditions <strong>and</strong> neural tube defectsAmerican Journal of Public Health88: 1674-80Watson,R.J. (1994) Affective <strong>to</strong>ne ina <strong>to</strong>ddler measured during transitionsbetween multiple caregivers. Early ChildDevelopment <strong>and</strong> Care 97:135-44Webb, E., Shankleman,J., Evans, M.K.<strong>and</strong>Brooks, R. (2001) The health of children inrefuges for women victims of domesticviolence:a cross sectional descriptivesurvey. British Medical Journal 323:210-3Webb, S., Monk,C.<strong>and</strong> Nelson,C. (2001)‘Mechanisms of Postnatal NeurobiologicalDevelopment: Implications for HumanDevelopment’. DevelopmentalNeuropsychology 19(2): 147-171.Weir, R.H. (1962) Language in the CribThe Hague: Mou<strong>to</strong>n <strong>and</strong> CoWeiss, S.J., Wilson, P., Seed, M.S.J.<strong>and</strong>Paul,S.M. (2001) Early tactile experienceof low birth weight children: links <strong>to</strong> latermental health <strong>and</strong> social adaptation.Infant <strong>and</strong> Child Development 10(3):93-115Wells, G. (1986) The Meaning Makers:children learning language <strong>and</strong> usinglanguage <strong>to</strong> learn London: Hodder <strong>and</strong>S<strong>to</strong>ugh<strong>to</strong>nWells, G. (1987) The Meaning MakersSevenoaks: Hodder <strong>and</strong> S<strong>to</strong>ugh<strong>to</strong>nWerner, E.E. (1996) Vulnerable butInvincible: high risk children from birth <strong>to</strong>adulthood European Child <strong>and</strong> AdolescentPsychiatry 5 (Suppl.1):47-51West, D. (2000) Paedophilia: plague orpanic? Journal of Forensic Psychiatry11(3):511-31Westbrook,J. (1999) Two points of view:young children’s literacy development inprint <strong>and</strong> the audio visual imageInternational Journal of Early Childhood31(2):37-44Whaley, K.L.<strong>and</strong> Rubenstein, T.S. (1994)How <strong>to</strong>ddlers ‘do’ friendship: a descriptiveanalysis of naturally occurring friendshipsin a group child care setting. Journal ofSocial <strong>and</strong> Personal Relationships11(3):383-400175


BibliographyAbbott, L. <strong>and</strong> Moylett, H. (1997) (eds)Working with the Under-3s: Training <strong>and</strong>Professional Development Buckingham:Open University PressAbbott, L.<strong>and</strong> Moylett, H. (1997a) (eds)Working with the Under-3s: Responding <strong>to</strong>Children’s Needs Buckingham: OpenUniversity PressBruce, T. (1996a) Tuning in <strong>to</strong> childrenLondon: BBC (book – part of pack withvideo <strong>and</strong> audio material)Carnegie Corporation of New York (1994)Starting Points. Meeting the Needs of OurYoungest Children New York: CarnegieCorporationDeLoache, J. <strong>and</strong> Gottlieb, A. (eds) (2000)A World of Babies Cambridge: CambridgeUniversity PressDES (1990) Starting with Quality.(Rumbold Report) London:HMSOMacNaugh<strong>to</strong>n,G.<strong>and</strong> Williams, G. (1998)Techniques for Teaching Young ChildrenFrenchs Forest NSW Australia: LongmansManning Mor<strong>to</strong>n,J. <strong>and</strong> Thorp, K. (2001)Key Times: a Framework for Developing HighQuality Provision for Children Under ThreeYears Old. London: Camden Early YearsUnder Threes Development Group LondonBorough of CamdenMiller, L., Rustin,M., Rustin,M. <strong>and</strong>Shuttleworth, J. (eds) (1989) CloselyObserved Infants London: DuckworthMooney, A.<strong>and</strong> Mun<strong>to</strong>n, A. (1997)Research <strong>and</strong> Policy in Early ChildhoodServices: Time for a New Agenda London:University of London IoE Thomas CoramResearch UnitMooney, A., Knight, A., Moss, P. <strong>and</strong>Owen,C. (2001) A profile of childminding:implications for planning Children <strong>and</strong>Society 15:253-62177


National Association for SpecialEducational Needs (1999) Policy documen<strong>to</strong>n Early Years Tamworth:NASENNew Zeal<strong>and</strong> Ministry of Education (2001)(video) Empowered <strong>to</strong> Learn: Te Whariki forInfants <strong>and</strong> Toddlers Welling<strong>to</strong>n NZ:Ministry of Education (www.media.co.nz)O’Brien, M. (1997) Inclusive Child Care forInfants <strong>and</strong> Toddlers: Meeting IndividualSpecial Needs Baltimore: Paul BrookesPublishingOECD (2001) Starting Strong: EarlyChildhood Education <strong>and</strong> Care Paris: OECDSe l l e c k ,D. ( 2001) Being under <strong>three</strong> years ofage. In G. Pugh (ed) Contemporary Issues inthe Early Years. London:PCP/Sage pp.78-95178 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


AppendixMethodology – how the literature review was carried outThe lite rat u re rev i ew was ca rried out by firstly bra i n s <strong>to</strong> rming a list of key wo rds <strong>and</strong> phra s e swhich would be used in accessing libra ry data bases. These we re organised as a table:-PEOPLE FOCUSEDInfantBaby/BabiesNeonateToddlerParentMotherFatherSisterBrotherSiblingGr<strong>and</strong>parentStaffEduca<strong>to</strong>r/sCarerMinder/childminder/child-minderNanny/iesAu pairFamily/iesAdultHealth visi<strong>to</strong>rNurseDoc<strong>to</strong>rAGE FOCUSEDUnder oneUnder Two(<strong>to</strong> searchfor research ontwo-year-olds)ThreeUnder five /sixToddler(obviously a few in the‘people’ column wouldalso do this eg baby,neonate)ISSUE FOCUSEDBrainNeuro-DevelopmentGrowthPerson/alPhysicalEmotion/alSocialCognitive/intellectual/underst<strong>and</strong>La n g u a g e / l i n g u i s t i c /b i l i n g u a l / m u l t i l i n g u a lTalkPlayLiteracyCommunication etcNegotiation, etcImaginationSelf esteemLearningCurriculumCreative/etcRelations(-ships)Snuggling179


PEOPLE FOCUSED continued/setting focusedDaycare/day carePre-school/ Pre-schoolNurseryLibrary/iesSports (centre)Parent <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ddlerAGE FOCUSED continuedISSUE FOCUSED continuedBelongSafe/ protect/abuseEsteemCompetentVoiceIdentitymoralspiritualculturalmaths/mathematics/number etcmusicartscienceworld (K&U of World?)holisticPedagog(y-ista-ue)Resource/sEnvironmentSENDisabilityEthnicRace/racialGenderBoy/girlEqual (opportunity/ies)HealthPovertyPolicyPoliticContinuityInteractionTrainingQualificationsConditions180 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


Other terms – for example ADHD,Ritalin, were later added <strong>to</strong> this extensivefirst trawl.Members of the team then exploredlibrary data bases. These included:-ASSIA; BIDS; British Humanities Index;Medline;ERIC; PsysINFO; Social ScienceCitation Index; OVID Biomed;BEI; IBSS/BIDS;BHI;Science Citation Index;BookFind-Online; Caredata;BOPCAS; MMU,London IoE, <strong>and</strong> CCCUC library catalogues;NCB library catalogue; Blackwells,PCP/Sage, <strong>and</strong> other relevant publishers’on-line catalogues.Colleagues on the teams at the two HEIs(Manchester Metropolitan University <strong>and</strong>Canterbury Christ Church UniversityCollege);the project Steering <strong>and</strong> WorkingGroups; <strong>and</strong> those in the UK <strong>and</strong> abroad,who might know of key texts, were alsocontacted for suggestions.The results from the data bases were insome cases surprising – for example whena nursing data base appeared <strong>to</strong> have noreferences <strong>to</strong> neonates. In other cases theresults were so vast they were impossible<strong>to</strong> cover. Others were manageable but stilllarger than we would expect <strong>to</strong> be dealtwith in one review which includes not justthe <strong>to</strong>pic raising that list but a largenumber of other foci. For example, theterms resilience <strong>and</strong> risk produced 375references from one data base alone, ofwhich 91 were selected for downloading.Thirty-nine were used subsequently in thisliterature review.The question of which<strong>to</strong> de-select was made on the basis ofrelevance <strong>and</strong> then often it was a matterof using one’s knowledge <strong>and</strong> experiencein the field <strong>to</strong> further discriminatebetween those which would be mostuseful <strong>and</strong> those which were subsidiary.In order <strong>to</strong> ensure that the team did notduplicate searches, the data bases weredivided up <strong>and</strong> team members exchangedreferences found relating <strong>to</strong> areas of thereview which were the responsibility ofanother member of the team. Each teammember <strong>to</strong>ok responsibility for the initialdrafting of particular components of theframework ‘<strong>Birth</strong> <strong>to</strong> Three Matters’ as dealtwith in the literature review.When a data base had been searchedaccording <strong>to</strong> a particular term orcombination of terms, the collectionof chosen citations, abstracts <strong>and</strong> locationsof references were downloaded <strong>and</strong>saved <strong>to</strong> disk. The records could thenbe printed off <strong>and</strong> used for sorting <strong>and</strong>for margin notes.In order <strong>to</strong> facilitate common approaches<strong>to</strong> reading selected articles <strong>and</strong> books, apro forma was devised. (See opposite).181


BIRTH TO THREE MATTERSPro Forma for the Literature ReviewReviewer id + number of review . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .Data base search/ source . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .Key search terms . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .Publication infoAuthor/s . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .Date of publication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .Title . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .Book title if a chapter <strong>and</strong> edi<strong>to</strong>rs/Journal name . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .Publisher (book) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .Place of publication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .Page numbers (of article or chapter) . . . . . . . . . .ISBN/ISSN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .DetailsDiscipline/ area . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .Country/ies involved . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .Type of literature (eg. meta-analysis; ‘issues‘ book; text for practitioners;popular jnl/periodical; acad journal;professional journal; etc) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .Type of research (eg quantitative, qualitative,…paradigm espoused/philos of authors if given or identifiable) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .Methods used (methods, sample size, age range, socio-econ info, etc) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .Focus of study (eg. emotional development) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .Relevant for which Aspect/Component of the pack (eg A1,B3,etc) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .Criteria (tick or make notes)Minimises possible biasHas external validity/authenticityConclusions fit data,sufficient evidenceHas been assessed by others (eg refereed for journal, peer review, in public domain etc)Generalisations have been made only where/when appropriateResumé of relevant findingsUseful quote <strong>and</strong> page no.A= A Strong Child;B= A Skilful Communica<strong>to</strong>r; C= A Competent Learner; D= A Healthy Child. Thus A1= Me, myself <strong>and</strong> I,<strong>and</strong> so on.182 EDUCATION ANDSKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S


In many cases what surprised the reviewteam was the overall support whichdifferent pieces of research lent <strong>to</strong> thegeneral picture which was emerging.Generally, disagreements betweenresearchers seemed <strong>to</strong> be more concernedwith methodology or research approaches<strong>and</strong> the validity of previous studies, ratherthan about the main findings concerningchildren’s development <strong>and</strong> learningin this age group. In the event, becauseof time constraints, the pro forma wasused simply as an aide memoire <strong>and</strong> noteswe re wri t ten beside the data base pri n<strong>to</strong> u t s,or attached <strong>to</strong> hard copies of articles.As each team member gatheredinformation she would add it <strong>to</strong> a draftsection for which she was responsible,concerned with a particular Componen<strong>to</strong>f the Framework. As a result of the verybroad coverage of the literature review,there were some <strong>to</strong>pics which receivedless attention than others. However, itmust be said that there are, in any case,certain <strong>to</strong>pics which have been neglectedby research. These include, in particular,young children’s spiritual <strong>and</strong> moraldevelopment, research about ECEC <strong>and</strong>children in this age group, especially thatabout two year olds.the Aspects <strong>and</strong> Components of theFramework <strong>and</strong> the chapters <strong>and</strong> sectionsof the literature review.The first draft of the review was sent out<strong>to</strong> members of the Steering <strong>and</strong> WorkingGroups <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> the MMU team members.Further, two colleagues at CanterburyChrist Church UC provided a helpfulcommentary on the review, <strong>to</strong> strengthenits inclusiveness of young children withspecial needs.Since the review is intended for use mainlyby practitioners, it would have been idealhad time allowed for it <strong>to</strong> be read at thisstage by a sample of practitionercolleagues, so augmenting the practitionerrepresentation on the Working Group whoreceived the draft. However, time was veryshort, <strong>and</strong> this meant that when all thesuggested amendments had beenreceived, the text was redrafted <strong>and</strong>reviewed only by DfES colleagues <strong>and</strong> theproject Direc<strong>to</strong>r, Professor Lesley Abbott.We would hope that this literature reviewwill complement those referenced in ourtext <strong>and</strong> the forthcoming reviews by theBERA Early Years Special Interest Group<strong>and</strong> the EPPI Early Years Group.Initial drafts of sections of the literaturereview were put <strong>to</strong>gether by one memberof the team, who also incorporated therelevant quotations <strong>and</strong> definitions fromthe Framework itself. These links weremade in order <strong>to</strong> help readers readilyrecognise the connections between183


184 EDUCATIONAND SKILLS B I RT H T O T H R E E M AT T E R S

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!