12.07.2015 Views

Dyslexia in the Foreign Language Classroom

Dyslexia in the Foreign Language Classroom

Dyslexia in the Foreign Language Classroom

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>


SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITIONSeries Editor: Professor David S<strong>in</strong>gleton, Tr<strong>in</strong>ity College, Dubl<strong>in</strong>, IrelandThis series br<strong>in</strong>gs toge<strong>the</strong>r titles deal<strong>in</strong>g with a variety of aspects of languageacquisition and process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> situations where a language or languages o<strong>the</strong>r than<strong>the</strong> native language is <strong>in</strong>volved. Second language is thus <strong>in</strong>terpreted <strong>in</strong> its broadestpossible sense. The volumes <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> series all offer <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir different ways, on<strong>the</strong> one hand, exposition and discussion of empirical f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs and, on <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r, somedegree of <strong>the</strong>oretical reflection. In this latter connection, no particular <strong>the</strong>oreticalstance is privileged <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> series; nor is any relevant perspective – sociol<strong>in</strong>guistic,psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic, neurol<strong>in</strong>guistic, etc. – deemed out of place. The <strong>in</strong>tended readershipof <strong>the</strong> series <strong>in</strong>cludes f<strong>in</strong>al-year undergraduates work<strong>in</strong>g on second languageacquisition projects, postgraduate students <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> second language acquisitionresearch, and researchers and teachers <strong>in</strong> general whose <strong>in</strong>terests <strong>in</strong>clude a secondlanguage acquisition component.Full details of all <strong>the</strong> books <strong>in</strong> this series and of all our o<strong>the</strong>r publications canbe found on http://www.multil<strong>in</strong>gual-matters.com, or by writ<strong>in</strong>g to Multil<strong>in</strong>gualMatters, St Nicholas House, 31-34 High Street, Bristol BS1 2AW, UK.


SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITIONSeries Editor: David S<strong>in</strong>gleton, Tr<strong>in</strong>ity College, Dubl<strong>in</strong>,Ireland<strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong><strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Joanna NijakowskaMULTILINGUAL MATTERSBristol • Buffalo • Toronto


Library of Congress Catalog<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Publication DataA catalog record for this book is available from <strong>the</strong> Library of Congress.Nijakowska, Joanna.<strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>/Joanna Nijakowska.Second <strong>Language</strong> AcquisitionIncludes bibliographical references and <strong>in</strong>dex.1. <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. 2. Second language acquisition. 3. Dyslexic children–Education. 4. Englishlanguage–Study and teach<strong>in</strong>g–<strong>Foreign</strong> speakers. I. Title. II. Series: Second language acquisition(Clevedon, England)[DNLM: 1. <strong>Dyslexia</strong>–etiology. 2. <strong>Dyslexia</strong>–<strong>the</strong>rapy. 3. <strong>Language</strong>. 4. Learn<strong>in</strong>g. WL 340.6 N691d2010]RJ496.A5N55 2010616.85’53–dc22 2010018314British Library Catalogu<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Publication DataA catalogue entry for this book is available from <strong>the</strong> British Library.ISBN-13: 978-1-84769-280-1 (hbk)ISBN-13: 978-1-84769-279-5 (pbk)Multil<strong>in</strong>gual MattersUK: St Nicholas House, 31-34 High Street, Bristol BS1 2AW, UK.USA: UTP, 2250 Military Road, Tonawanda, NY 14150, USA.Canada: UTP, 5201 Duffer<strong>in</strong> Street, North York, Ontario M3H 5T8, Canada.Copyright © 2010 Joanna Nijakowska.All rights reserved. No part of this work may be reproduced <strong>in</strong> any form or by any meanswithout permission <strong>in</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g from <strong>the</strong> publisher.The policy of Multil<strong>in</strong>gual Matters/Channel View Publications is to use papers that are natural,renewable and recyclable products, made from wood grown <strong>in</strong> susta<strong>in</strong>able forests. In <strong>the</strong>manufactur<strong>in</strong>g process of our books, and to fur<strong>the</strong>r support our policy, preference is given topr<strong>in</strong>ters that have FSC and PEFC Cha<strong>in</strong> of Custody certification. The FSC and/or PEFC logoswill appear on those books where full certification has been granted to <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>ter concerned.Typeset by Datapage International Ltd.Pr<strong>in</strong>ted and bound <strong>in</strong> Great Brita<strong>in</strong> by Short Run Press Ltd.


ContentsIntroduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .vii1 Becom<strong>in</strong>g Literate . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1Def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>Dyslexia</strong> . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1Gett<strong>in</strong>g Ready for Read<strong>in</strong>g and Spell<strong>in</strong>g . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 10Read<strong>in</strong>g Strategies and Stages of Read<strong>in</strong>g Development . . . . . . 15Orthographic Depth and Gra<strong>in</strong> Size: A Cross-languagePerspective on Read<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>Dyslexia</strong> . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 212 Causes of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33Genetic Mechanisms <strong>in</strong> <strong>Dyslexia</strong> . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35Bra<strong>in</strong> Mechanisms <strong>in</strong> <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 37Phonological Cod<strong>in</strong>g Deficit Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43Double-deficit Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 54Magnocellular Deficit Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 56Cerebellar Deficit Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 613 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> and <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 66Native <strong>Language</strong>-based <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong>Learn<strong>in</strong>g Difficulties . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 66<strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disability versus Cont<strong>in</strong>uumNotion of <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g Differences . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 70Review of Research <strong>in</strong> Support of L<strong>in</strong>guistic Cod<strong>in</strong>gDifferences Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 744 Symptoms and Identification of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85Lifelong Nature of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85Emotional-motivational Disorders <strong>in</strong> <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 97Identification of <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 100Identification of <strong>Dyslexia</strong>: Bil<strong>in</strong>gual/Multil<strong>in</strong>gualPerspective . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1065 Treatment and Teach<strong>in</strong>g . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 115Issues <strong>in</strong> <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Treatment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 115Pr<strong>in</strong>ciples of <strong>the</strong> Multisensory StructuredLearn<strong>in</strong>g Approach . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 122v


vi<strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Multisensory Structured Learn<strong>in</strong>g Approach and<strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Study . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 127Educational Accommodations Towards Learnerswith <strong>Dyslexia</strong> . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1456 Sample Activities for Learners with <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>gEnglish as a <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 153Activities for Develop<strong>in</strong>g Phonological Awareness andAwareness of Sound-letter Relations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 153Orthographic Awareness Activities . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 165Morphological Awareness Activities . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 175Grammatical Awareness Activities . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 179Afterword . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 190Appendix 1 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 197Appendix 2 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 200Appendix 3 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 206Appendix 4 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 207Appendix 5 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 208Appendix 6 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 209References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 210Index . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 233


IntroductionNaturally enough, ensur<strong>in</strong>g access to <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> various fields ofknowledge, literacy skills exert considerable <strong>in</strong>fluence on our personalgrowth and professional development. Thus, researchers have been<strong>in</strong>tensively <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> phenomenon of read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>gdisorders and, even more importantly from <strong>the</strong> practitioner’s standpo<strong>in</strong>t,ways of help<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g-impaired <strong>in</strong>dividuals.Perceived as a significant commercial asset as well as a good<strong>in</strong>vestment <strong>in</strong> children, <strong>the</strong> ability to speak foreign languages constitutesano<strong>the</strong>r prerequisite of success <strong>in</strong> our fast-develop<strong>in</strong>g, multil<strong>in</strong>gualsociety. Not only does it equip a person with a useful tool for <strong>in</strong>ternationalcommunication, but it also enhances an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s chances of gett<strong>in</strong>g aposition on <strong>the</strong> professional ground. The necessity of possess<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>command of a foreign language has been well recognised by <strong>the</strong>authorities, which resulted <strong>in</strong> a foreign language requirement becom<strong>in</strong>ga compulsory part of educational systems. Viewed as an advantage,recently bil<strong>in</strong>gualism has become a social necessity, allow<strong>in</strong>g people toface <strong>the</strong> challenges thrown up by <strong>the</strong> united, frontier-free Europe.The foreign language requirement forms an <strong>in</strong>tegral and compulsorypart of educational systems. Apparently, fulfill<strong>in</strong>g this requirement seemsto br<strong>in</strong>g about considerable difficulties <strong>in</strong> many students with dyslexia.This is because <strong>the</strong> specific read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g problems <strong>the</strong>y encounterfrequently prevent <strong>the</strong>m from accomplish<strong>in</strong>g this social and educationaldemand. Weaknesses such as poor decod<strong>in</strong>g and remember<strong>in</strong>g, slowprocess<strong>in</strong>g and retriev<strong>in</strong>g l<strong>in</strong>guistic <strong>in</strong>formation, ma<strong>in</strong>ly on <strong>the</strong> phonologicallevel, are believed to be salient features of <strong>in</strong>dividuals withdyslexia, lead<strong>in</strong>g to educational achievements much below <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>in</strong>tellectualpotential and requirements posed on <strong>the</strong>m.Students with dyslexia tend to face difficulties <strong>in</strong> foreign languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g (FLL) and, <strong>in</strong> fact, <strong>the</strong> problem is associated with nativelanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g. Ganschow and Sparks (Ganschow et al., 1998; Sparks,1995; Sparks et al., 1989, 1998a) plausibly show that for <strong>in</strong>dividuals withdyslexia, native language learn<strong>in</strong>g poses variable difficulties, which <strong>the</strong>ntranslate <strong>in</strong>to similar problems <strong>in</strong> foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g. It followsthat <strong>the</strong> skills of an <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> native language components ofl<strong>in</strong>guistic cod<strong>in</strong>g form <strong>the</strong> cornerstone of successful foreign languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g. It has been shown that <strong>the</strong> performance on standard measuresof native language skill is related to <strong>the</strong> level of foreign languagevii


viii<strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>proficiency. Namely, higher levels of oral and written foreign languageproficiency are achieved by <strong>the</strong> students who exhibit higher levels ofnative language skills. Thus, difficulties <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g, writ<strong>in</strong>g, listen<strong>in</strong>g andspeak<strong>in</strong>g, be <strong>the</strong>y subtle or overt, exist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> native language are likelyto be responsible for similar difficulties <strong>in</strong> foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g. Themost noticeable deficit concerns <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>ability to consciously isolate andmanipulate <strong>the</strong> sounds of language and relate <strong>the</strong>m to <strong>the</strong> appropriatewritten symbols letters. Suffice it to say that poor native language skillsmay significantly impede <strong>the</strong> process of foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g.In <strong>the</strong> modern foreign language classroom, where emphasis is put onau<strong>the</strong>ntic situational contexts, <strong>the</strong> almost-exclusive use of <strong>the</strong> targetlanguage and <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>ductive acquisition of grammar, pronunciation andspell<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>the</strong> needs of students with dyslexia are hardly catered for.Learners with dyslexia require explicit and structured <strong>in</strong>struction. Adirect structured multisensory <strong>in</strong>struction (MSL) <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> phonological/orthographic system of a language has unequivocally proved successful<strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g skills to children with dyslexia, not only<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir native language, but also <strong>in</strong> a foreign language (Crombie &McColl, 2000; Ganschow et al., 1998; Jameson, 2000; Miller & BussmanGillis, 2000; Nijakowska, 2008; Sparks et al., 1989, 1992b, 1998c; Sparks &Ganschow, 1993).In <strong>the</strong> multisensory approach, it is assumed that <strong>the</strong> more modalitiesare <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g process, <strong>the</strong> more effective it is. Multisensorymethods utilise simultaneous engagement of several sensory channelsand <strong>the</strong> syn<strong>the</strong>sis of stimuli com<strong>in</strong>g from <strong>the</strong>se channels. Thus, teach<strong>in</strong>gread<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g is realised by <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tegration of visual, auditory,k<strong>in</strong>aes<strong>the</strong>tic and tactile stimuli and <strong>in</strong>volves simultaneous presentationof <strong>in</strong>formation com<strong>in</strong>g from various senses (Bogdanowicz, 1997a/2000;Ganschow & Sparks, 1995; Jędrzejowska & Jurek, 2003; Sparks &Ganschow, 1993; Sparks et al., 1991, 1998c; Thomson & Watk<strong>in</strong>s, 1990).In order to m<strong>in</strong>imise <strong>the</strong> literacy problems of children with dyslexia, it isnecessary to automatise <strong>the</strong>ir skills through carefully designed, monitoredand long-term tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g (Nicolson & Fawcett, 2001). Unfortunately,<strong>in</strong> many cases, nei<strong>the</strong>r special methods of teach<strong>in</strong>g foreign languagesdur<strong>in</strong>g regular teach<strong>in</strong>g hours nor special classes outside school areoffered to students with dyslexia. <strong>Foreign</strong> language teachers’ awarenessof developmental dyslexia still seems ra<strong>the</strong>r poor. Those who are familiarwith <strong>the</strong> problem and want to help <strong>the</strong>ir students with dyslexia becomediscouraged by <strong>the</strong> lack of materials designed especially for teach<strong>in</strong>gforeign languages to dyslexics.In most educational systems, <strong>the</strong> foreign language requirement iscompulsory. Many foreign language teachers are likely to be faced with<strong>the</strong> challenge of teach<strong>in</strong>g learners with dyslexia. It follows from <strong>the</strong>above that by far <strong>the</strong> most important issue seems to be rais<strong>in</strong>g awareness


Introductionixamong pre-service and <strong>in</strong>-service foreign language teachers of <strong>the</strong> natureof dyslexic difficulties and effective teach<strong>in</strong>g methods. There exist greatsocial demand and pressure on teachers to be able to understand andhelp students with special educational needs. Also, foreign languageteachers <strong>the</strong>mselves feel <strong>the</strong> need to broaden <strong>the</strong>ir professional knowledge.They repeatedly report be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> need of a comprehensibleguidance on how to work with children with dyslexia, s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>the</strong>se issuesrarely constitute a part of curricula realised dur<strong>in</strong>g teacher tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g. Itseems especially crucial <strong>in</strong> light of <strong>the</strong> necessity to create equaleducational opportunities for all children. Teachers are expected toaccommodate <strong>the</strong>ir educational requirements and exam<strong>in</strong>ation conditionsto <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual needs and abilities of students with dyslexia.However, too often, <strong>the</strong>re seems to be a considerable mismatch between<strong>the</strong> legal educational law concern<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia and <strong>the</strong>way it is executed <strong>in</strong> school practice.There is a wide spectrum of publications concern<strong>in</strong>g both <strong>the</strong><strong>the</strong>oretical and practical aspects of <strong>the</strong> phenomenon of dyslexia;however, <strong>the</strong>y are prepared ma<strong>in</strong>ly for psychologists, teachers-<strong>the</strong>rapists,native language teachers, k<strong>in</strong>dergarten and elementary education teachers.Very few publications on dyslexia are addressed specifically toforeign language teachers. This book aims to fill this gap and present <strong>the</strong>problem of dyslexia from a foreign language teacher’s and crosslanguageperspective. It is <strong>in</strong>tended to serve as a reference book forthose preoccupied with foreign language teach<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g experienced<strong>in</strong>-service teachers, novice teachers as well as teacher tra<strong>in</strong>ers andtra<strong>in</strong>ees. My <strong>in</strong>tention <strong>in</strong> this work is to provide <strong>the</strong> reader with a conciseoverview of <strong>the</strong> current research, both its <strong>the</strong>oretical and practicalaspects, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> field of specific difficulties <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and spell developmental dyslexia. Understand<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> phenomenon of dyslexiarequires <strong>in</strong>terdiscipl<strong>in</strong>ary knowledge. Advances <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> fields of sciencesuch as psychology, pedagogy, neurology, biology and l<strong>in</strong>guisticscontribute to <strong>the</strong> explanation of <strong>the</strong> nature of this learn<strong>in</strong>g disorder.Equipped with greater awareness as well as solid background knowledgeof a wide spectrum of <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>oretical and practical aspects of <strong>the</strong>problem, one can more efficiently help children with dyslexia. It is hopedthat <strong>the</strong> book will help teachers to face one of <strong>the</strong> many challenges that<strong>the</strong> educational system poses on <strong>the</strong>m nowadays, namely, <strong>the</strong> organisationof an effective teach<strong>in</strong>g process for students with dyslexia, which isaccommodated to <strong>the</strong>ir needs and abilities.Term<strong>in</strong>ological issues related to dyslexia, toge<strong>the</strong>r with a def<strong>in</strong>ition of<strong>the</strong> disorder are covered <strong>in</strong> Chapter 1. The potential to develop literacyskills and <strong>the</strong> need to do likewise are fundamental. Abilities to read andspell are complex and multilevel <strong>in</strong> character and engage several physical,physiological and psychological phenomena. Chapter 1 discusses <strong>the</strong>


x<strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>issues of general read<strong>in</strong>ess for learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and write and bestpredictors of read<strong>in</strong>g success. The focus <strong>the</strong>n shifts towards <strong>the</strong> nature of<strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g process, strategies, developmental stages, to f<strong>in</strong>ally touchupon <strong>the</strong> issues of orthographic depth and gra<strong>in</strong> size. Read<strong>in</strong>g anddyslexia are discussed from a cross-language perspective, highlight<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong> similarities and differences between dyslexia manifestations andliteracy acquisition <strong>in</strong> different languages.Due to <strong>the</strong> complex nature of dyslexia, any organised attempt tounderstand its multiple facets should <strong>in</strong>volve <strong>the</strong> analysis of its underly<strong>in</strong>gcauses. Chapter 2 is devoted to both <strong>the</strong> neurobiological substratesand cognitive correlates of dyslexia. It familiarises <strong>the</strong> reader with <strong>the</strong>major facts and hypo<strong>the</strong>ses and presents selected f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs from massiveresearch. As already mentioned, phonological process<strong>in</strong>g impairmentsresponsible for <strong>the</strong> specific read<strong>in</strong>g disability <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> native language maysimilarly impede <strong>the</strong> acquisition of foreign languages. Chapter 3 dealswith <strong>the</strong> issue of native language-based foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>gdifficulties and <strong>in</strong>cludes <strong>the</strong> verification of <strong>the</strong> notion of a disability forforeign language learn<strong>in</strong>g versus cont<strong>in</strong>uum notion of language learn<strong>in</strong>gdifferences.<strong>Dyslexia</strong> is a lifetime condition, one does not grow out of it, oncequalified to have dyslexia a child cont<strong>in</strong>ues to demonstrate symptoms of<strong>the</strong> disorder, alter<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> range and severity, <strong>in</strong>to adulthood (Gregg et al.,2005; Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2001a). The ways dyslexia leaves its impr<strong>in</strong>t onbehaviour varies across <strong>in</strong>dividuals. In addition, throughout life,symptoms of dyslexia manifest <strong>in</strong> a given person are subject to dynamicchange. These issues as well as <strong>the</strong> problem of identify<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> cases ofdyslexia among <strong>the</strong> multitude of poor readers are addressed <strong>in</strong> Chapter4. There is general agreement that early diagnosis of learn<strong>in</strong>g disorders ispivotal to <strong>the</strong> fur<strong>the</strong>r educational career of at-risk children, however,various universally applicable but also country- and language-specificfactors seem to <strong>in</strong>fluence <strong>the</strong> assessment procedures. There is <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gand widespread sensitivity and concern over <strong>the</strong> issues of literacyacquisition and assessment of read<strong>in</strong>g disabilities among school children<strong>in</strong> multicultural and multil<strong>in</strong>gual sett<strong>in</strong>gs, <strong>in</strong> particular those who learnEnglish as a second or subsequent language. This issue is also referred to<strong>in</strong> Chapter 4.Support<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir attempts to overcomeread<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g difficulties, experienced both <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir nativelanguage as well as <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> second or additional language that <strong>the</strong>yfrequently struggle to learn, is a stand<strong>in</strong>g challenge for teachers. Chapter5 is concerned with treatment and educational accommodations forchildren with dyslexia, it revises teach<strong>in</strong>g pr<strong>in</strong>ciples and methods, withspecial attention given to a multisensory structured approach and itsapplication <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> foreign language context.


IntroductionxiChapter 6 comprises a collection of activities, worksheets, games andmovable devices that are designed to enhance <strong>the</strong> sensitivity to and foster<strong>the</strong> acquisition of phonological, morphological, orthographic and, lastbut not least, grammatical aspects of <strong>the</strong> English language. Theorthographic awareness section conta<strong>in</strong>s a set of sample activitiesselected from <strong>the</strong> programme used <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> small-scale <strong>in</strong>tervention studydescribed <strong>in</strong> Chapter 5. The study concerned <strong>the</strong> effectiveness of directmultisensory <strong>in</strong>struction for improv<strong>in</strong>g word read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g skills<strong>in</strong> English as a foreign language, through <strong>the</strong> systematic study of selectedgrapheme-phoneme relations, spell<strong>in</strong>g patterns and rules. The activitiesproposed <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> book may be used dur<strong>in</strong>g additional classes conductedwith students with dyslexia, ei<strong>the</strong>r for one-to-one lessons or for groupwork, however <strong>the</strong>y can equally well be <strong>in</strong>corporated <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong> regularclassroom rout<strong>in</strong>e.The feel<strong>in</strong>g of success is especially important for students withdyslexia, who frequently experience failure <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir educational endeavourbecause, needless to say, an <strong>in</strong>dividual constantly confronted withan unatta<strong>in</strong>able challenge can, eventually, become frustrated anddiscouraged. In addition, confusion caused by an <strong>in</strong>adequate teach<strong>in</strong>gapproach and by requirements that are impossible to fulfil, deprives<strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia of <strong>the</strong> feel<strong>in</strong>g of joy and satisfaction broughtabout by learn<strong>in</strong>g. Teachers’ sensitivity and awareness of <strong>the</strong> nature ofdyslexic difficulties and ways of overcom<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m can def<strong>in</strong>itely help tosteer clear of such danger.


Chapter 1Becom<strong>in</strong>g LiterateDef<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>Dyslexia</strong>Term<strong>in</strong>ological issuesThis book is devoted to a developmental cognitive disorder, moreprecisely, to a specific learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulty <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g developmental dyslexia. To beg<strong>in</strong> with it is important to ensure term<strong>in</strong>ologicaland def<strong>in</strong>itional clarity, which would be most helpful forappropriate understand<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> described phenomena. Read<strong>in</strong>gdisorders are quite <strong>in</strong>tensively studied and several terms have beenproposed <strong>in</strong> different countries to denote read<strong>in</strong>g deficits of variousk<strong>in</strong>ds, <strong>in</strong> children and adults alike. Apparently, such a situation is aconsequence of <strong>the</strong> complex nature of <strong>the</strong> skill <strong>in</strong> question as well as <strong>the</strong>diverse causes and multiple types of read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g disorders.Let us focus first on <strong>the</strong> dist<strong>in</strong>ction between acquired and developmentaldisorders. Acquired read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g deficits result from bra<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>juryor disease and connote ei<strong>the</strong>r total or partial loss of <strong>the</strong> already possessedability to read or spell. The <strong>in</strong>tensity of symptoms largely depends on <strong>the</strong>size and location of <strong>the</strong> lesion as well as <strong>the</strong> age of <strong>the</strong> person. Acquiredalexia and acquired agraphia are spelled out as a total loss of <strong>the</strong> faculty ofread<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g, respectively, while acquired dyslexia denotes only apartial disappearance of read<strong>in</strong>g competence, and dysgraphia is a partialloss of spell<strong>in</strong>g ability. To stress aga<strong>in</strong>, acquired read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>gdisorders are connected with deficits <strong>in</strong> qualifications that a givenperson, be it an adult or a child, had already possessed prior to bra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>jury (Bogdanowicz, 1989, 1999; Krasowicz, 1997).Acquired read<strong>in</strong>g disabilities <strong>in</strong> adult patients are often taken as amodel for <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g and achiev<strong>in</strong>g fur<strong>the</strong>r understand<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong>neurological concept of developmental read<strong>in</strong>g impairments (Borkowska,1997). However, such an approach is rejected by Hulme andSnowl<strong>in</strong>g (2009), who advocate <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g developmental read<strong>in</strong>gdisorders <strong>in</strong> children by relat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m to patterns of typical read<strong>in</strong>gdevelopment <strong>in</strong> children. Much as adult models usefully specify whatnormally constitutes a result of development, <strong>the</strong>y do not describe <strong>the</strong>development itself. Read<strong>in</strong>g development undergoes certa<strong>in</strong> changesover time as children develop, thus, naturally, younger children are lessaccomplished <strong>in</strong> this respect than older children. Developmentaldisorders would typically <strong>in</strong>volve modified slowed down ratesand patterns of change. Children with dyslexia will learn to read slowly1


2 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>and with difficulty and this delay <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> rate of <strong>the</strong> development of <strong>the</strong>read<strong>in</strong>g skill is claimed to be <strong>the</strong> most salient and strik<strong>in</strong>g characteristicof this disorder. 1Developmental disorders can be divided <strong>in</strong>to specific (restricted) andgeneral difficulties/disorders. General disorders concern deficits <strong>in</strong> most, ifnot all, cognitive functions. By contrast, specific disorders refer tosituations where impairment <strong>in</strong> just one or a limited number of skillsis <strong>in</strong>volved, while function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r areas rema<strong>in</strong>s typical. Krasowicz(1997) suggests fur<strong>the</strong>r division of specific developmental read<strong>in</strong>gdisorder <strong>in</strong>to specific decod<strong>in</strong>g disorder and specific comprehension disorder.In <strong>the</strong> UK, such a specific (restricted) difficulty, which <strong>in</strong>volves aselective (occurr<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a restricted doma<strong>in</strong>) impairment <strong>in</strong> acquir<strong>in</strong>g a skillthat must be learned, is referred to as a specific learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulty; whereasglobal learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulty concerns problems <strong>in</strong> acquir<strong>in</strong>g a wide range ofskills and <strong>in</strong> understand<strong>in</strong>g concepts. In <strong>the</strong> USA, <strong>the</strong> terms learn<strong>in</strong>gdisorders and mental retardation are used to denote specific and globallearn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties, respectively. In practice, <strong>the</strong> results of a standardisedIQ test (a measure of general <strong>in</strong>telligence) are used to dist<strong>in</strong>guish betweencases of specific and general learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties. 2 The diagnosis of specificlearn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties is frequently conditioned by <strong>the</strong> achievement of IQscore with<strong>in</strong> or near <strong>the</strong> average range, while general learn<strong>in</strong>g difficultiesare diagnosed <strong>in</strong> children with IQ scores below 70.Specific developmental read<strong>in</strong>g disorder developmental dyslexia is one of<strong>the</strong> most <strong>in</strong>tensively researched, best known and understood specificlearn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties. Specific learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties experienced by childrenwith dyslexia concern acquir<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g skills, while <strong>the</strong>ability to understand concepts is normal. In addition, <strong>the</strong>y oftendemonstrate talents <strong>in</strong> various areas of study <strong>in</strong> science, sport or art(Hulme & Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2009).Developmental dyslexia has its place <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternational classificationsof diseases, mental disorders and related health problems. In <strong>the</strong>‘International Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems,Tenth Revision (ICD-10)’, specific difficulties <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and spellare classified as follows: general category specific developmental disorders ofscholastic skills; specific learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g specific read<strong>in</strong>gdisorders (developmental dyslexia); specific learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>in</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g specific spell<strong>in</strong>g disorders; specific difficulties <strong>in</strong> acquir<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> technique ofwrit<strong>in</strong>g due to lowered motor ability and motor coord<strong>in</strong>ation of hands,which is characterised by a very low graphic level of writ<strong>in</strong>g specificdevelopmental disorder of motor function. For <strong>the</strong> sake of comparison, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>‘Diagnostic and Statistic Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Revision(DSM-IV)’, <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g notions have been <strong>in</strong>troduced: general category learn<strong>in</strong>g disorders; specific read<strong>in</strong>g problems read<strong>in</strong>g disorder (or dyslexia);specific writ<strong>in</strong>g problems disorder of written expression; specifictechnical


Becom<strong>in</strong>g Literate 3problems <strong>in</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g developmental coord<strong>in</strong>ation disorder (Bogdanowicz,2007).The notion dyslexia can be understood as a narrow concept, compris<strong>in</strong>gsolely of a difficulty <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g or as a whole syndrome of specificdifficulties <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and spell, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> coexistence ofread<strong>in</strong>g difficulties with spell<strong>in</strong>g impairments (perta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g to both poororthography and graphic level) (Borkowska, 1998; Pętlewska, 1999;Zakrzewska, 1999). Draw<strong>in</strong>g on cl<strong>in</strong>ical experience, Bogdanowicz (1999)has suggested that isolated read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g disorders can bediagnosed apart from <strong>the</strong> whole syndrome of specific difficulties <strong>in</strong>learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and spell. For example, a spell<strong>in</strong>g disorder may arise thatco<strong>in</strong>cides with nei<strong>the</strong>r a read<strong>in</strong>g disorder nor poor graphic level of writ<strong>in</strong>g(developmental coord<strong>in</strong>ation disorder). However, accord<strong>in</strong>g to Szczerbiński(2007), as much as dysorthography (specific spell<strong>in</strong>g disorder) mayhappen to be dissociated from dyslexia, <strong>the</strong> reverse case is extremely rare.Most usually, low-level performance of decod<strong>in</strong>g and encod<strong>in</strong>g coexist.Bogdanowicz (1989, 1997b, 1999) uses <strong>the</strong> term developmental dyslexia tosignify <strong>the</strong> syndrome of specific learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g andspell<strong>in</strong>g as a disorder of written communication. With<strong>in</strong> this syndrome,she identifies three isolated disorders: dyslexia to symbolise specificdifficulties <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g to read (poor decod<strong>in</strong>g), dysorthography specificspell<strong>in</strong>g difficulties (poor encod<strong>in</strong>g) and dysgraphia specific difficulties <strong>in</strong>acquir<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> appropriate graphic level of writ<strong>in</strong>g. The above term<strong>in</strong>ologyhas been widely used <strong>in</strong> Polish publications (Borkowska, 1998; Juszczyk& Zając, 1997; Knobloch-Gala, 1995; Krasowicz, 1997; Miązek, 2001;Pętlewska, 1999; Sawa, 1999; Zakrzewska, 1999; Zelech, 1997 and o<strong>the</strong>rs).Much as <strong>the</strong> abovementioned term<strong>in</strong>ology has ga<strong>in</strong>ed wide acceptance <strong>in</strong>Poland, <strong>the</strong> terms may be <strong>in</strong>terpreted differently <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r countries. Byway of example, accord<strong>in</strong>g to Smy<strong>the</strong> and Everatt (2000), <strong>in</strong> Italy,dysgraphia denotes motor difficulties, while dysorthography refers tospell<strong>in</strong>g difficulties; however, <strong>the</strong> term dysgraphia is used as synonymouswith spell<strong>in</strong>g difficulties as well. Yet, <strong>in</strong> Russia, dysgraphia stands for aspell<strong>in</strong>g disorder characterised by <strong>in</strong>adequate usage of graphemes orsyntactic impairments, whereas dyslexia refers exclusively to a read<strong>in</strong>gdisorder manifest<strong>in</strong>g itself <strong>in</strong> a slower rate of read<strong>in</strong>g, paired withnumerous persistent errors.All <strong>in</strong> all, dyslexia, often qualified by <strong>the</strong> adjective developmental, is <strong>the</strong>most popular and <strong>in</strong>ternationally accepted expression denot<strong>in</strong>g specificdifficulties <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and spell, specifically with regard todecod<strong>in</strong>g and encod<strong>in</strong>g s<strong>in</strong>gle words, which come <strong>in</strong>to play <strong>in</strong> children at<strong>the</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of school education (Critchley, 1964; Krasowicz, 1997; Ott,1997; Reid, 1998). Additionally, apart from dyslexia and developmentaldyslexia, several descriptive notions, such as learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties, learn<strong>in</strong>gdisabilities, specific read<strong>in</strong>g disorders or specific learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties/disabilities,


4 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>which have been <strong>in</strong>troduced <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternational medical, psychologicaland pedagogical classifications, are <strong>in</strong>ternationally recognised as well.Snowl<strong>in</strong>g and Caravolas (2007) argue that <strong>the</strong> characteristics of dyslexia a delay <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g development makes us perceive <strong>the</strong> disorder asdimensional. It means that <strong>in</strong>dividuals can suffer from a disorder tovary<strong>in</strong>g degrees; children with dyslexia seem to occupy a bottom end of<strong>the</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>uum of normal variation <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> skill <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> population. However,despite <strong>the</strong> apparent arbitrar<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>in</strong> putt<strong>in</strong>g a cut-off po<strong>in</strong>t betweendyslexia and normal read<strong>in</strong>g, it is common, and often easier, to usecategorical labels such as ‘children with dyslexia’ ra<strong>the</strong>r than dimensional(descriptive) terms ‘children with specific severe difficulty <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g toread and spell’. Such diagnostic categorical labels are found useful <strong>in</strong>communicat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> types and nature of difficulties experienced bychildren (Hulme & Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2009).In this book, <strong>the</strong> terms dyslexia, developmental dyslexia, as well asdescriptive notions such as specific learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties, specific difficulties <strong>in</strong>learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and spell, specific developmental read<strong>in</strong>g disorder and learn<strong>in</strong>gdisorder/disability, are used <strong>in</strong>terchangeably. They denote specific difficulties<strong>in</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>t process<strong>in</strong>g, with respect to word decod<strong>in</strong>g and encod<strong>in</strong>g,which children experience <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and spell from <strong>the</strong>beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong>ir school education.Def<strong>in</strong>ition of dyslexiaEducational science qualifies children with developmental dyslexia to<strong>the</strong> special educational needs (SEN) group, toge<strong>the</strong>r with particularly<strong>in</strong>telligent and talented children as well as mentally retarded children,children with neurological diseases, deficits <strong>in</strong> motor and sensory organs,emotional disorders, culturally (environmentally) and didactically neglectedchildren, and, last but not least, those with speech disorders. It isa frequently observed fact that <strong>the</strong>se children <strong>in</strong>variably function badly<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> school environment when follow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> rout<strong>in</strong>e educationalprogrammes. They typically stand <strong>in</strong> need of: firstly, a pace of workaccommodated to <strong>the</strong>ir abilities and needs; secondly, <strong>in</strong>dividualisedteach<strong>in</strong>g programmes and requirements, and, f<strong>in</strong>ally, special methods ofteach<strong>in</strong>g, put <strong>in</strong>to practice by qualified teachers (Bogdanowicz, 1995,1997b; Tomaszewska, 2001).It is widely acknowledged that dyslexia constitutes a neurologicalcondition with genetic traces and is typically designated with phonologicalprocess<strong>in</strong>g impairment at <strong>the</strong> cognitive level. Essentially, <strong>the</strong>fundamental dyslexic difficulty relates to <strong>the</strong> below-standard pr<strong>in</strong>tprocess<strong>in</strong>g at <strong>the</strong> level of s<strong>in</strong>gle words, more precisely, <strong>in</strong>accurate and/or slow decod<strong>in</strong>g of attempted words as well as <strong>in</strong>correct word encod<strong>in</strong>g(spell<strong>in</strong>g). Children with dyslexia fail to recognise pr<strong>in</strong>ted words at <strong>the</strong>


Becom<strong>in</strong>g Literate 5level expected for <strong>the</strong>ir age. Read<strong>in</strong>g is primarily dependent onphonological process<strong>in</strong>g and visual analysis, and some importance isalso attributed to general speed of <strong>in</strong>formation process<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>the</strong> abilityto <strong>in</strong>tegrate <strong>in</strong>formation of various modalities. While morphological,syntactic and semantic factors may play a part <strong>in</strong> decod<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>the</strong>y are mostprobably language-specific and <strong>the</strong>ir role is ra<strong>the</strong>r limited here <strong>in</strong>comparison to <strong>the</strong> engagement of <strong>the</strong>se processes <strong>in</strong> comprehension.Children with dyslexia f<strong>in</strong>d accurate and fluent read<strong>in</strong>g truly difficult,while impairment of read<strong>in</strong>g comprehension is quite dist<strong>in</strong>ct fromdyslexia. A consequence of persistent difficulty <strong>in</strong> skilful recognition ofpr<strong>in</strong>ted words, not specific to dyslexia but ra<strong>the</strong>r constitut<strong>in</strong>g its result,gives rise to certa<strong>in</strong> difficulties. Frequently, trouble <strong>in</strong> understand<strong>in</strong>gwritten texts and organis<strong>in</strong>g thoughts on paper may become apparent,which, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> long run, can <strong>in</strong>tensify, <strong>in</strong>evitably lead<strong>in</strong>g to persistentproblems <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> general process of ga<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g knowledge. Importantly,notwithstand<strong>in</strong>g some read<strong>in</strong>g comprehension problems brought aboutby impaired decod<strong>in</strong>g of written text, <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia would bevery unlikely to demonstrate any limitations <strong>in</strong> understand<strong>in</strong>g complexspoken text and concepts. Last but not least, dyslexia is a life-longcondition, 3 whose characteristic features alter with age and development;symptoms are dynamic <strong>in</strong> nature, <strong>the</strong>y tend to be evident and <strong>the</strong>ndim<strong>in</strong>ish at given po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> development. Certa<strong>in</strong> deficits are compensated,for example, atta<strong>in</strong>ment of customary accurate read<strong>in</strong>g is with<strong>in</strong>reach, however, even <strong>in</strong> adulthood, spell<strong>in</strong>g rema<strong>in</strong>s a pa<strong>in</strong>ful task andread<strong>in</strong>g speed seems less susceptible to remediation (Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2001b).The complex and diverse nature of dyslexia naturally <strong>in</strong>vites multipleattempts at def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> phenomenon <strong>in</strong> question. There are severalcompet<strong>in</strong>g etiological <strong>the</strong>ories of dyslexia at large (though one mightperceive <strong>the</strong>m as actually complementary to one ano<strong>the</strong>r), hence a naturaltendency towards promot<strong>in</strong>g a def<strong>in</strong>ition that would aptly reflect <strong>the</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>assumptions of a given <strong>the</strong>ory. Most relevantly, dyslexia is claimed to be<strong>the</strong> outcome of <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluence that various pathogenic factors exert on <strong>the</strong>central nervous system, lead<strong>in</strong>g to disorders of functions underly<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>processes of read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g. Beyond doubt though, difficultiesencountered by children with dyslexia are specific, narrow and limited <strong>in</strong>range as opposed to unspecific, global learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties. Generally,read<strong>in</strong>g disorders are claimed to depend on <strong>the</strong> occurrence of a substantialmismatch between <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g achievement of a child (operationalised as<strong>the</strong> score on a standardised read<strong>in</strong>g test) and that expected, given <strong>the</strong> levelof <strong>in</strong>telligence, education and chronological age. Children with dyslexiaexperience persistent learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>in</strong> spite of adequate <strong>in</strong>telligence,lack of sensory and motor deficits, and absence of environmental anddidactic negligence (Bogdanowicz, 1997b, 1997c; Elliot & Place, 2000;Heaton & W<strong>in</strong>terton, 1996).


6 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>The follow<strong>in</strong>g def<strong>in</strong>ition was recommended by <strong>the</strong> World Federationof Neurology <strong>in</strong> 1968:Specific developmental dyslexia is a disorder manifested by difficulty<strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and write despite conventional <strong>in</strong>struction,adequate <strong>in</strong>telligence, and socio-cultural opportunity. It depends onfundamental cognitive disabilities that are frequently constitutional<strong>in</strong> orig<strong>in</strong>. (Bogdanowicz, 2002a: 56; Borkowska, 1998: 41; Krasowicz-Kupis, 2008: 49; Ott, 1997: 3)The neurobiological, constitutional character of <strong>the</strong> disorder and limitedrange of difficulties are highlighted here. However, <strong>the</strong> feature thatearned this def<strong>in</strong>ition <strong>the</strong> unfavourable attitude of practitioners is itsclearly exclusionary character (Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2001b). Exclusionary def<strong>in</strong>itionsare described as negative, <strong>the</strong>y are frequently heavily criticisedbecause exclusionary criteria may be ambiguous, which <strong>in</strong> turn multipliesdoubts (Krasowicz-Kupis, 2008). Never<strong>the</strong>less, at least to someextent, exclusionary def<strong>in</strong>itions can be useful for <strong>in</strong>troductory cl<strong>in</strong>icaldiagnosis, especially when additional explanations with regard to <strong>the</strong>concepts <strong>in</strong>cluded are added <strong>in</strong> order to avoid ambiguity.For example, on <strong>the</strong> one hand, rightly enough as one would th<strong>in</strong>k,<strong>in</strong>struction is brought to light <strong>in</strong> this def<strong>in</strong>ition, which potentially conveyscrucial implications for diagnosis. More precisely, s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>in</strong>adequateteach<strong>in</strong>g too often produces <strong>in</strong>tense difficulties <strong>in</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>t process<strong>in</strong>g of o<strong>the</strong>rthan a dyslexic character, it is of extreme importance to verify <strong>the</strong> methodsof teach<strong>in</strong>g that a person be<strong>in</strong>g assessed towards dyslexia was exposed to<strong>in</strong> order to escape misclassification (Szczerbiński, 2007). On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand,what exactly is ‘conventional <strong>in</strong>struction’? What k<strong>in</strong>ds of methods andtechniques do <strong>the</strong> authors conceive of as conventional? The same holdstrue for ‘adequate <strong>in</strong>telligence’, one might wonder why children with anIQ below average may not qualify for <strong>the</strong> diagnosis of dyslexia. 4‘Socio-cultural opportunity’ lacks precision as well. There is enoughevidence to claim that experience can alter <strong>the</strong> way genes are expressed<strong>in</strong> children predisposed to dyslexia. Variable patterns of gene expressioncan be responsible for differences <strong>in</strong> health and bra<strong>in</strong> development,which <strong>in</strong> turn may exert diverse <strong>in</strong>fluence on behaviour. Importantly,environmental factors operat<strong>in</strong>g at biological, cognitive and behaviourallevel can add to <strong>the</strong> risk of develop<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g impairments <strong>in</strong> children.Poor socio-economic background as well as poor literacy environment(literacy-related activities, children <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> books, read<strong>in</strong>g experienceopportunities at home, parents’ educational level and literacy problems,school<strong>in</strong>g, pr<strong>in</strong>t exposure) seem to generate greater chances for <strong>the</strong>occurrence of read<strong>in</strong>g problems, though with probably greater impact oncomprehension than decod<strong>in</strong>g skills (Frith, 2008; Hulme & Snowl<strong>in</strong>g,2009). Never<strong>the</strong>less, as stressed by Hulme and Snowl<strong>in</strong>g (2009: 329):


Becom<strong>in</strong>g Literate 7‘We are not born with genes that make us dyslexic, nor are we born <strong>in</strong>to<strong>the</strong> environment that makes us dyslexic. We may, however, be born withgenes that give an <strong>in</strong>creased risk of develop<strong>in</strong>g dyslexia, but this risk will<strong>in</strong> turn be moderated by environmental factors’. The <strong>in</strong>fluence of bothgenetic and environmental risk factors as well as <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>in</strong>teraction ondevelopment seems undeniable.The discrepancy between IQ level, age and scholastic atta<strong>in</strong>ments ofchildren with dyslexia are elucidated <strong>in</strong> ano<strong>the</strong>r def<strong>in</strong>ition, provided byThomson and Watk<strong>in</strong>s:Developmental dyslexia is a severe difficulty with <strong>the</strong> written form oflanguage <strong>in</strong>dependent of <strong>in</strong>tellectual, cultural, and emotional causation.It is characterized by <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual’s read<strong>in</strong>g, writ<strong>in</strong>g andspell<strong>in</strong>g atta<strong>in</strong>ments be<strong>in</strong>g well below <strong>the</strong> level expected based on<strong>in</strong>telligence and chronological age. The difficulty is a cognitive one,affect<strong>in</strong>g those language skills associated with <strong>the</strong> written form,particularly visual-to-verbal cod<strong>in</strong>g, short-term memory, order perceptionand sequenc<strong>in</strong>g. (Thomson & Watk<strong>in</strong>s, 1990: 3)Aga<strong>in</strong>, children with dyslexia are claimed to exhibit a typicallyunexpected, considerable mismatch between <strong>the</strong> low level of <strong>the</strong>irread<strong>in</strong>g skill, age and generally high <strong>in</strong>tellectual ability. In this def<strong>in</strong>ition,a reference is also made to <strong>the</strong> emotional aspect of dyslexia. Importantly,emotional causation of <strong>the</strong> disorder is def<strong>in</strong>itely denied. It needsstress<strong>in</strong>g that even though emotional-motivational disturbances frequentlyaccompany dyslexia, <strong>the</strong>y are not responsible for <strong>the</strong> occurrenceof <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g failure, quite <strong>the</strong> contrary, <strong>the</strong>y may follow from it. 5The follow<strong>in</strong>g, widely cited, def<strong>in</strong>ition was compiled <strong>in</strong> 1994 by <strong>the</strong>Orton <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Society Research Committee <strong>in</strong> conjunction with <strong>the</strong>National Centre for Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities and <strong>the</strong> National Institute ofChild Health and Human Development:<strong>Dyslexia</strong> is one of several dist<strong>in</strong>ct learn<strong>in</strong>g disabilities. It is a specificlanguage-based disorder of constitutional orig<strong>in</strong> characterized bydifficulties <strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle word decod<strong>in</strong>g, usually reflect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>sufficientphonological process<strong>in</strong>g. These difficulties <strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle word decod<strong>in</strong>gare often unexpected <strong>in</strong> relation to age and o<strong>the</strong>r cognitive andacademic abilities; <strong>the</strong>y are not <strong>the</strong> result of generalized developmentaldisability or sensory impairment. <strong>Dyslexia</strong> is manifested byvariable difficulty with different forms of language, often <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong>addition to problems with read<strong>in</strong>g, a conspicuous problem withacquir<strong>in</strong>g proficiency <strong>in</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g. (Bogdanowicz, 1999:821; Borkowska, 1998: 42; Cieszyńska, 2001: 11; Krasowicz-Kupis, 2008:53; Lyon, 1995: 9; Ott, 1997: 4; Reid, 1998: 3; Tomaszewska, 2001: 30)


8 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>It is an <strong>in</strong>clusionary def<strong>in</strong>ition; it focuses on characteris<strong>in</strong>g what dyslexiais ra<strong>the</strong>r than what it is not (Schneider, 1999). Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Reid (1998), itis an example of a research-based def<strong>in</strong>ition, which means that it drawson <strong>the</strong> constructs that can be measured directly and clearly <strong>in</strong>dicateswhen an <strong>in</strong>dividual may be diagnosed as dyslexic. Bogdanowicz (1999)states that this def<strong>in</strong>ition is cl<strong>in</strong>ical <strong>in</strong> nature, s<strong>in</strong>ce it perta<strong>in</strong>s to <strong>the</strong>characteristic symptoms of dyslexia. The def<strong>in</strong>ition clarifies that dyslexiais <strong>in</strong>herited, clearly stresses difficulties <strong>in</strong> process<strong>in</strong>g l<strong>in</strong>guistic stimuli,and po<strong>in</strong>ts to <strong>in</strong>sufficient phonological process<strong>in</strong>g skills as <strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>gcause of dyslexic read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g difficulties. However, o<strong>the</strong>rscholars seem to voice a contradictory op<strong>in</strong>ion on <strong>the</strong> list<strong>in</strong>g of possiblecauses of <strong>the</strong> disorder <strong>in</strong> its def<strong>in</strong>ition. Much as Uppstad and Tønnessen’s(2007) postulate that a def<strong>in</strong>ition of dyslexia should concentrate on<strong>the</strong> symptoms, <strong>the</strong>y also advocate not stat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> causes, such asphonological deficit, <strong>in</strong> order to avoid promot<strong>in</strong>g circular argumentsand sett<strong>in</strong>g limits on <strong>the</strong> search for causes.Still, it seems that formulat<strong>in</strong>g a def<strong>in</strong>ition of dyslexia based on itssymptoms <strong>in</strong>deed poses substantial problems. First of all, we lackunanimous agreement as to <strong>the</strong> understand<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> phenomenon andspecify<strong>in</strong>g its symptoms, and second, it goes without say<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>the</strong>cl<strong>in</strong>ical picture of dyslexia is quite dynamic, symptoms change with timedur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> course of development and can be <strong>in</strong>fluenced, for <strong>in</strong>stance, bymethods of teach<strong>in</strong>g, remedial <strong>in</strong>struction and compensation abilities(depend<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>telligence level, range and severity of deficit). Asstressed by Frith (2008), on <strong>the</strong> one hand, lack of severe problems <strong>in</strong>acquir<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g skill does not necessarily exclude dyslexia, on <strong>the</strong>o<strong>the</strong>r hand, frequently, read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties manifest <strong>in</strong> children are not ofa dyslexic nature. Moreover, development is characterised by change,and behavioural signs of dyslexia can reduce with time as a result ofeducation and compensation, however, <strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>g cognitive cause,most probably <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> form of <strong>the</strong> phonological deficit, may rema<strong>in</strong>impaired. Thus, to avoid faults and mistakes, diagnosis towards dyslexiacannot be based solely on <strong>the</strong> outcome of read<strong>in</strong>g tests. In order todist<strong>in</strong>guish dyslexic difficulties from read<strong>in</strong>g problems of o<strong>the</strong>r k<strong>in</strong>ds,dyslexia should be def<strong>in</strong>ed as a neurodevelopmental disorder, imply<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong> existence of a complex causal cha<strong>in</strong> embrac<strong>in</strong>g biological, cognitiveand behavioural factors, present s<strong>in</strong>ce birth and characterised by a set ofbehavioural symptoms subject to change over time.Despite <strong>the</strong> most noticeable tendencies <strong>in</strong> dyslexia research concern<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong> language deficit highlighted <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> def<strong>in</strong>ition, <strong>the</strong> fact that allchildren with dyslexia are said to encounter problems with phonologicalprocess<strong>in</strong>g seems an oversimplification s<strong>in</strong>ce, as cl<strong>in</strong>ical experiencetoge<strong>the</strong>r with research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs (White et al., 2006) show, <strong>the</strong>re is agroup of children with <strong>the</strong> visual-spatial pathomechanism of dyslexia


Becom<strong>in</strong>g Literate 9(Borkowska, 1998; Cieszyńska, 2001). Never<strong>the</strong>less, beyond doubt, <strong>the</strong>phonological process<strong>in</strong>g deficit is perceived as a core deficit by <strong>the</strong>majority of academics and practitioners, which is, as a matter of fact,quite well reflected <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>tervention programmes.Determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> approximate number of children with dyslexia isimportant <strong>in</strong> order to estimate <strong>the</strong> scale of <strong>the</strong> phenomenon. Admittedly, arough calculation differs strik<strong>in</strong>gly across countries from 0.1% to asmuch as 20% or even 30% (Bogdanowicz, 1989; Zakrzewska, 1999). Elliottand Place (2000) quote <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g numbers: 10% of children experiencemild dyslexic difficulties and 4% experience severe dyslexic problems.Ste<strong>in</strong> (2001) claims that 510% of children, ma<strong>in</strong>ly boys, are found to sufferfrom dyslexia. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Hulme and Snowl<strong>in</strong>g (2009), <strong>the</strong> disorderappears to be quite common and affects about 36% of children.It seems that <strong>the</strong> apparent <strong>in</strong>consistency with regard to estimates of<strong>the</strong> prevalence of dyslexia, not only <strong>in</strong> different countries but even acrossa given country, results from <strong>the</strong> lack of a commonly accepted def<strong>in</strong>itionof <strong>the</strong> disorder, naturally <strong>in</strong>vit<strong>in</strong>g diverse <strong>in</strong>terpretations of <strong>the</strong> natureand severity of disorders qualified as dyslexia (Zakrzewska, 1999). As<strong>in</strong>dicated earlier, read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties can be placed on <strong>the</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>uum,rang<strong>in</strong>g from mild to severe, thus def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> disorder is not just stat<strong>in</strong>gdyslexia versus its lack, but it <strong>in</strong>volves select<strong>in</strong>g, quite arbitrarily, <strong>the</strong>po<strong>in</strong>t on <strong>the</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>uum <strong>the</strong> scores below which denote poor readers(Szczerbiński, 2007). The situation referred to above is conditioned byadopt<strong>in</strong>g various, not uniform (apparently not quite reliable and valid)criteria used for identification (Wagner et al., 2005). Last but not least,diverse results of studies on dyslexia <strong>in</strong> different countries are dependenton <strong>the</strong> specificity of a given language as well.Intense scholarly discussions set aside, <strong>the</strong>re seems to be some generalagreement mirrored <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> proposed def<strong>in</strong>itions, namely, dyslexia has abiological, constitutional basis. Moreover, this developmental disorder ismost commonly characterised <strong>in</strong> terms of <strong>in</strong>adequate facility <strong>in</strong> languageprocess<strong>in</strong>g, which is manifested by decod<strong>in</strong>g and encod<strong>in</strong>g difficulties.The most ubiquitous cause of dyslexic read<strong>in</strong>g problems is belowstandardword identification ability, which itself is brought about bypr<strong>in</strong>t decod<strong>in</strong>g impairments. Word identification is heavily dependent on<strong>the</strong> successful acquisition of <strong>the</strong> alphabetic pr<strong>in</strong>ciple, allow<strong>in</strong>g comprehensionof <strong>the</strong> phoneme-grapheme conversions, which <strong>in</strong> turn draws on<strong>the</strong> acquisition of phonological awareness def<strong>in</strong>ed as <strong>the</strong> knowledgethat spoken words are composed of <strong>in</strong>dividual speech sounds. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore,it is explicated that, firstly, dyslexic difficulties do not arise as aresult of sight or hear<strong>in</strong>g impairment, secondly, <strong>the</strong>y are not due toemotional problems, environmental or didactic negligence or mentalretardation, and f<strong>in</strong>ally, that <strong>the</strong> discrepancy between <strong>the</strong>ir potential andscholastic achievement is <strong>in</strong>disputably tangible. The failure <strong>in</strong> acquir<strong>in</strong>g


10 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g skills is <strong>in</strong>contestable despite conventional teach<strong>in</strong>gmethods, understood as those that proved effective as relates to o<strong>the</strong>r<strong>in</strong>dividuals. At <strong>the</strong> same time, frequently, <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexiaprove to be high-achievers <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r academic discipl<strong>in</strong>es, which <strong>in</strong>controvertibly<strong>in</strong>dicates that dyslexic scholastic difficulties are very specific,concern<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> limited range of abilities.Gett<strong>in</strong>g Ready for Read<strong>in</strong>g and Spell<strong>in</strong>gRead<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g skillsLiteracy concerns <strong>the</strong> skills of read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g, learn<strong>in</strong>g of whichrequires <strong>in</strong>tentional and conscious control. They are evidently dynamic,complex and, above all, multilevel <strong>in</strong> character; both are l<strong>in</strong>guisticactivities (forms of communication based on language), metal<strong>in</strong>guisticactivities (grounded <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> awareness of <strong>the</strong> relationship between pr<strong>in</strong>tand spoken word, phoneme-grapheme relation and <strong>the</strong> awareness oflanguage resources used to formulate utterances), metacognitive activities(requir<strong>in</strong>g conscious control of <strong>the</strong> cognitive processes <strong>in</strong>volved understand<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g conveyed <strong>in</strong> texts), pragmatic and metapragmaticactivities (connected with purposeful use of written texts) (Krasowicz-Kupis, 2004). Read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>volves decod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> text and <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g itsmean<strong>in</strong>g, while writ<strong>in</strong>g is connected with shap<strong>in</strong>g ideas and encod<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong>m with <strong>the</strong> use of pr<strong>in</strong>t, both requir<strong>in</strong>g l<strong>in</strong>guistic skills on phonological,morphological, syntactic and semantic levels (Bogdanowicz & Krasowicz-Kupis, 2005a).As <strong>in</strong>dicated above, read<strong>in</strong>g is concerned with transmitt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> form of a written text from <strong>the</strong> author to <strong>the</strong> reader. Thus, pr<strong>in</strong>tforms a medium for send<strong>in</strong>g and receiv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation via <strong>the</strong> visualchannel (Kaczmarek, 1969). From a ra<strong>the</strong>r technical standpo<strong>in</strong>t, read<strong>in</strong>gcan be viewed as convert<strong>in</strong>g graphemes <strong>in</strong>to phonemes and, consequently,chang<strong>in</strong>g written words <strong>in</strong>to spoken words. Even though itactually allows access to higher levels of l<strong>in</strong>guistic process<strong>in</strong>g, meredecod<strong>in</strong>g does not guarantee understand<strong>in</strong>g (Shaywitz, 1997). It is <strong>the</strong>ability to comprehend <strong>the</strong> identified symbols that <strong>in</strong>dicates mastery of<strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g skill (Brzezińska, 1987; Szempruch, 1997). To recapitulate,<strong>the</strong>re exist three <strong>in</strong>terrelated aspects that are <strong>in</strong>corporated <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong>read<strong>in</strong>g process, namely, <strong>the</strong> technical aspect of match<strong>in</strong>g phonologicalunits to graphic symbols, <strong>the</strong> semantic aspect understand<strong>in</strong>g, and <strong>the</strong>reflexive one, operationalised as critical read<strong>in</strong>g. In o<strong>the</strong>r words, <strong>the</strong>multifactor character of read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>volves identify<strong>in</strong>g phonologicalelements and <strong>the</strong>ir correspond<strong>in</strong>g graphic symbols, recognis<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>mean<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong>se symbols, as well as understand<strong>in</strong>g and assess<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>value of <strong>the</strong> content <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> context of <strong>in</strong>dividual experience. Additionally,Velut<strong>in</strong>o et al. (2004) stress <strong>the</strong> fact that words <strong>in</strong> a text must be identified


Becom<strong>in</strong>g Literate 11not only accurately but also fluently enough <strong>in</strong> order to <strong>in</strong>fer <strong>the</strong>mean<strong>in</strong>gs from <strong>the</strong> text, with<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> limits posed by work<strong>in</strong>g memory.Altoge<strong>the</strong>r, read<strong>in</strong>g as a compound psychological process, consist<strong>in</strong>g ofseveral stages and <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g numerous activities, is pr<strong>in</strong>cipally aboutunlock<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> code of pr<strong>in</strong>ted symbols used to represent speech <strong>in</strong> agiven culture <strong>in</strong> order to access mean<strong>in</strong>g (Ziegler & Goswami, 2006).As it stands, <strong>the</strong> process of read<strong>in</strong>g is frequently presented as a form ofcommunication <strong>in</strong>separable from spell<strong>in</strong>g (Kaczmarek, 1969; Szczerbiński,2007). However, closely <strong>in</strong>terrelated as <strong>the</strong>y are, <strong>the</strong>y still seem to followslightly differ<strong>in</strong>g courses of development. It is a frequent observation thatsome words that are read correctly tend to be misspelled by children andadults alike. Similarly, children can appropriately write down words that<strong>the</strong>y cannot read easily (Bryant & Bradley, 1980). Frith (1985) proposed notonly <strong>the</strong> existence of qualitative differences <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> time-courses of read<strong>in</strong>gand spell<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> children, but also a causal relation between <strong>the</strong> two. Moreprecisely, at <strong>the</strong> very beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong>ir way towards literacy, childrentend to break <strong>the</strong> alphabetic code through spell<strong>in</strong>g and this knowledge ofletter-sound conversion rules is <strong>the</strong>n used <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>in</strong>itial read<strong>in</strong>g attempts.However, along <strong>the</strong> process of becom<strong>in</strong>g literate, certa<strong>in</strong> changes occurwith regard to <strong>the</strong> causal l<strong>in</strong>k between <strong>the</strong> two skills. That is to say, a yearor two later, <strong>the</strong>re is a shift of roles; orthographic patterns and rules arefirst to be recognised <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g and it is <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g experience thatpromotes subsequent application of new pr<strong>in</strong>ciples <strong>in</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g. Let us take<strong>the</strong> split-digraph rule <strong>in</strong> English (e.g. ‘a-e’ <strong>in</strong> ‘fate’) as an example. It hasbeen observed that at first, children represent vowel sounds with s<strong>in</strong>gleletters (e.g. ‘o’ <strong>in</strong> ‘dog’ or ‘a’ <strong>in</strong> ‘hat’), that is why <strong>the</strong>y f<strong>in</strong>d it harder both toread and spell long vowel CVCe words (which <strong>in</strong>volves <strong>the</strong> orthographicrule) than short vowel monosyllabic CVC words, conform<strong>in</strong>g to simpleletter-sound rules (e.g. ‘fame’ versus ‘cat’). Then, over time, <strong>the</strong>y firstimprove <strong>the</strong>ir read<strong>in</strong>g performance of CVCe words before <strong>the</strong>y <strong>in</strong>ternalise<strong>the</strong>ir spell<strong>in</strong>g, and, f<strong>in</strong>ally, this read<strong>in</strong>g upgrade leads to subsequentsuccess <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong>se words (Davis & Bryant, 2006).The educational implication of Frith’s claims is that enhancement ofchildren’s read<strong>in</strong>g experience is most likely to have a beneficial effect on<strong>the</strong>ir learn<strong>in</strong>g of spell<strong>in</strong>g choices, letter sequences and orthographic rulesbecause <strong>in</strong>itial acquisition of orthographic regularities and patternsachieved through read<strong>in</strong>g practice is later transferred and applied tospell<strong>in</strong>g.Read<strong>in</strong>ess for learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and spellMov<strong>in</strong>g on now to <strong>the</strong> notion of read<strong>in</strong>ess for learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and spell,let us elaborate on it from a more global perspective and with<strong>in</strong> a widersocial context first. School read<strong>in</strong>ess can be understood as a part of


12 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong><strong>the</strong> process of br<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g up and educat<strong>in</strong>g children, whereas <strong>in</strong> a muchnarrower scope, it is perceived as a factor condition<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> acquisition ofcerta<strong>in</strong> specialist skills and abilities, e.g. read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g orma<strong>the</strong>matics (Krasowicz-Kupis, 2004). Thus, as such, read<strong>in</strong>ess forlearn<strong>in</strong>g to read and spell constitutes an <strong>in</strong>tegral part of a more generalnotion of school read<strong>in</strong>ess, which is perceived as dependent on <strong>the</strong>advances <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> physical, mental, emotional and social developmentof a child.Look<strong>in</strong>g more closely at <strong>the</strong> issue of read<strong>in</strong>ess for learn<strong>in</strong>g to read andspell, Brzezińska (1987) proposes that, firstly, it <strong>in</strong>corporates <strong>the</strong> developmentof psychomotor functions, which enable children to acquire<strong>the</strong> technique of read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g. Secondly, it comprises <strong>the</strong>development of l<strong>in</strong>guistic-notional processes, which condition <strong>the</strong>comprehension of mean<strong>in</strong>g, and, f<strong>in</strong>ally, it <strong>in</strong>volves <strong>the</strong> development ofemotional-motivational processes, which allow discovery of writtenspeech as a form of communication. Much as <strong>the</strong> above descriptionseems to provide an <strong>in</strong>sightful perspective on <strong>the</strong> notion <strong>in</strong> question, it iscriticised for its apparent lack of a clear and direct reference to <strong>the</strong> impactof language awareness on <strong>the</strong> process of learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and spell(Krasowicz-Kupis, 2004).By and large, Krasowicz-Kupis (2004) formulates <strong>the</strong> concept ofread<strong>in</strong>ess for learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and spell, aptly def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g it as a moment<strong>in</strong> which a child reaches <strong>the</strong> level of physical, social and psychologicaldevelopment which makes him/her both sensitive and susceptible tosystematic teach<strong>in</strong>g of read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g. Be<strong>in</strong>g sensitive refers tobe<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> knowledge provided at school (emotionalmotivationalaspect of read<strong>in</strong>ess), while susceptibility is connectedwith an ability to understand, remember and acquire knowledge andskills taught at school. In her model, Krasowicz-Kupis (2004) lists sixgeneral components of read<strong>in</strong>ess for learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and spell, grouped<strong>in</strong>to two broader sections, namely, factors specific and non-specific toread<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g. Among <strong>the</strong> non-specific components, she classifiesattitude and motivation, development of perceptual-motor abilities and<strong>in</strong>tellectual development. To fur<strong>the</strong>r elaborate on <strong>the</strong> subcomponent ofperceptual-motor development, it comprises visual perception (encompass<strong>in</strong>gvisual analysis and syn<strong>the</strong>sis, identify<strong>in</strong>g, compar<strong>in</strong>g, directionality,visual memory); auditory perception (<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g differentiat<strong>in</strong>gbetween speech and environmental sounds, perceiv<strong>in</strong>g and reproduc<strong>in</strong>grhymes as well as auditory memory); motor abilities, settled pattern oflateralisation, spatial orientation (left-right, top-down) and perceptualmotor<strong>in</strong>tegration.The components specific to read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g are as follows:development of speech (understood not <strong>in</strong> terms of correct articulationbut ra<strong>the</strong>r as an ability to form and comprehend l<strong>in</strong>guistically correct oral


Becom<strong>in</strong>g Literate 13utterances, which most fully reflects <strong>the</strong> level of language acquisition) andlanguage (basic l<strong>in</strong>guistic abilities phonological, morphological, syntactic,semantic and verbal memory), language awareness, and, last but notleast, awareness of pr<strong>in</strong>t as a system of signs, with a set of rules andspecific reference to speech and <strong>the</strong> surround<strong>in</strong>g world. The role ofattention and memory is stressed as well.Predictors of read<strong>in</strong>g successThe study of predictors of read<strong>in</strong>g success has ga<strong>in</strong>ed substantial<strong>in</strong>terest among researchers. There exists an impressive amount ofempirical evidence support<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> claim of <strong>the</strong> causal l<strong>in</strong>k betweenpre-exist<strong>in</strong>g (underly<strong>in</strong>g) early phonological skills (prior to read<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>struction) and later read<strong>in</strong>g achievement (Bowey, 2005; Krasowicz-Kupis, 1999). The strength of phonological abilities measured at <strong>the</strong>beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of school education significantly predicts later ability to read,which is <strong>in</strong> accord with <strong>the</strong> causal <strong>the</strong>ory of development of <strong>the</strong> ability toread by Bryant and Goswami (Goswami, 1999; Goswami & Bryant, 1990).Apart from phonological awareness, considerable attention was given toletter knowledge, short-term memory, rapid serial nam<strong>in</strong>g speed,pseudo-word and expressive vocabulary, all of which qualify as powerfulpredictors of later read<strong>in</strong>g atta<strong>in</strong>ment identified among pre-schoolers(Carroll & Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2004; Puolakanaho et al., 2008; Snowl<strong>in</strong>g et al., 2003).The acquisition of <strong>the</strong> alphabetic pr<strong>in</strong>ciple (<strong>the</strong> idea that <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividualsounds of spoken words can be represented by letters or letter clusters) isof critical importance for master<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g skill <strong>in</strong> alphabeticalscripts. Both phoneme awareness and knowledge of letter-soundcorrespondences are essential for master<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> alphabetic pr<strong>in</strong>ciple, <strong>in</strong>addition, both strongly predict read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g achievement <strong>in</strong>children and <strong>in</strong>fluence each o<strong>the</strong>r <strong>in</strong> a reciprocal nature (Hulme et al.,2005; Vellut<strong>in</strong>o et al., 2004).Ziegler and Goswami (2006) put forward an argument that <strong>in</strong> alllanguages studied to date, read<strong>in</strong>g development depends on <strong>the</strong>children’s phonological awareness. They admit that developmentaldifferences <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> gra<strong>in</strong> size of lexical representations across languagesemerge, brought about by <strong>the</strong> vary<strong>in</strong>g consistency of phonologicalorthographicrelations, which <strong>in</strong> turn <strong>in</strong>vites <strong>the</strong> application of variabledevelopmental read<strong>in</strong>g strategies <strong>in</strong> different languages. Consequently,<strong>the</strong> awareness of onsets, rimes and phonemes studied <strong>in</strong> variouslanguages may not predict read<strong>in</strong>g success to <strong>the</strong> same extent. Generally,phonological skills and <strong>the</strong> ability to recognise letters <strong>in</strong> a pre-literacyperiod constitute <strong>the</strong> best predictors of future read<strong>in</strong>g achievement(Adams, 1990; Bradley & Bryant, 1983; Wolf et al., 2005). For <strong>in</strong>stance,phoneme awareness strongly predicts read<strong>in</strong>g success with regard to all


14 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong><strong>the</strong> alphabetical orthographies that it has been measured <strong>in</strong> (Goswami,2000). Nikolopoulos et al.’s (2006) f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs with regard to <strong>the</strong> transparentorthographic system of <strong>the</strong> Greek language support <strong>the</strong> exist<strong>in</strong>g evidencethat phoneme awareness is a strong with<strong>in</strong>-age predictor of variations <strong>in</strong>read<strong>in</strong>g rate and a unique longitud<strong>in</strong>al predictor of spell<strong>in</strong>g ability. On<strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, <strong>the</strong> strong predictive l<strong>in</strong>k between onset-rime awarenessand read<strong>in</strong>g development exists <strong>in</strong> English (Bradley & Bryant, 1983;Bryant et al., 1990), however, it is not as prom<strong>in</strong>ent <strong>in</strong> Norwegian andSwedish (Hoien et al., 1995) or <strong>in</strong> Polish (Krasowicz-Kupis, 1999), f<strong>in</strong>ally,it has not been confirmed for German (Wimmer et al., 1994). Goswami(2000) speculates that <strong>the</strong>se differences might result from <strong>the</strong> relativelyhigh degree of spell<strong>in</strong>g-sound consistency <strong>in</strong> English orthography asrelates to rimes <strong>in</strong> comparison to <strong>in</strong>dividual phonemes.Puolakanaho et al. (2008) support <strong>the</strong> claim that <strong>the</strong> level of read<strong>in</strong>gaccuracy is based on very early core phonological abilities. Theydemonstrated that early pre-read<strong>in</strong>g skills, measured as early as 3.5years of age, were strong predictors of read<strong>in</strong>g accuracy, however, thatdid not hold true for fluency. Fluency, on <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, could bepartially expla<strong>in</strong>ed by letter knowledge. Substantial doubts have beencast on <strong>the</strong> presumed equally predictive power of phonological awarenesswith regard to read<strong>in</strong>g accuracy versus fluency across orthographies.Apparently, <strong>the</strong> outcome of studies, predom<strong>in</strong>antly concern<strong>in</strong>gregular orthographies, even though <strong>the</strong>re is a grow<strong>in</strong>g body of evidenceto support <strong>the</strong> claim with reference to less transparent orthographies aswell, <strong>in</strong>dicates that although it prognosticates read<strong>in</strong>g accuracy well,phonological awareness tends not to best predict read<strong>in</strong>g fluency (Aro,2006; Hogan et al., 2005; Puolakanaho et al., 2008; Seymour, 2005).In sum, Reid (1998) considers <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g subskills crucial <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>acquisition of read<strong>in</strong>g: phonemic awareness, segment<strong>in</strong>g and blend<strong>in</strong>gabilities, letter recognition, grapheme-phoneme correspondence, recognitionof word patterns and visual memory. Hav<strong>in</strong>g access to <strong>the</strong>abovementioned subskills <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> pre-literacy stage fosters greater andfaster competence <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g. Conversely, children with poor read<strong>in</strong>gsubskills tend not to make successful readers and, consequently, lackopportunities to fur<strong>the</strong>r develop <strong>the</strong> skills via <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g activity itself.Hence, good readers seem to be <strong>in</strong> a favourable situation because <strong>the</strong>ycan improve <strong>the</strong> skills <strong>the</strong>y already possess, whereas poor readersbecome even poorer <strong>the</strong> phenomenon described by Stanovich (1986) as<strong>the</strong> ‘Mat<strong>the</strong>w effects’.Start<strong>in</strong>g school life and becom<strong>in</strong>g a student poses a considerablecognitive burden as well as psychological and social pressure onchildren, let alone <strong>the</strong> problems emerg<strong>in</strong>g due to poor read<strong>in</strong>g subskills.Thus, to escape <strong>the</strong> turmoil of read<strong>in</strong>g failure, children require assistanceand special tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g from an early stage, not only when <strong>the</strong>y start formal


Becom<strong>in</strong>g Literate 15school<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction, but also prior to that. K<strong>in</strong>dergartenchildren, <strong>in</strong> particular those at risk for developmental disorders such asdyslexia, should be given support <strong>in</strong> order to optimally develop <strong>the</strong>components of read<strong>in</strong>ess for learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and write, which wouldmake <strong>the</strong>ir early tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g easier. Still, it cannot bedenied that arriv<strong>in</strong>g at a diagnosis before a child is expected todemonstrate <strong>the</strong> skills <strong>in</strong> which difficulties are likely to arise posesproblems. What matters most is teachers’ sensitivity and awareness of<strong>the</strong> need to promote success for children from a pre-school stage,however, with <strong>the</strong> social risk of categorical labell<strong>in</strong>g dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>identification process <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d.Read<strong>in</strong>g Strategies and Stages of Read<strong>in</strong>g DevelopmentRead<strong>in</strong>g strategiesIt is undeniable that read<strong>in</strong>g primarily constitutes a l<strong>in</strong>guistic ra<strong>the</strong>rthan visual activity. Still, <strong>the</strong> role of visual-orthographic and phonologicalprocess<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-word recognition across languages is <strong>in</strong>tenselydebated, especially with reference to <strong>the</strong> orthographic depth hypo<strong>the</strong>sis(assum<strong>in</strong>g that different word recognition strategies are <strong>in</strong>vited bydifferent levels of grapheme-phoneme transparency). That is whygeneralis<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> results of research on read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g with regardto diverse languages and orthographic systems seems problematic.Additionally, <strong>the</strong> role of context and language awareness <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> courseof read<strong>in</strong>g acquisition is perceived differently by researchers (Bogdanowicz& Krasowicz-Kupis, 2005a; Goswami, 2005; Miller-Guron & Lundberg,2004; Ziegler & Goswami, 2006).Let us now concentrate on read<strong>in</strong>g strategies. When <strong>the</strong> ability to reads<strong>in</strong>gle words is taken <strong>in</strong>to consideration, two major strategies to reach <strong>the</strong>mean<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>ted <strong>in</strong>formation can be enumerated, namely, <strong>the</strong>visual (whole-word, lexical) direct and phonological <strong>in</strong>direct strategy. Dur<strong>in</strong>gwhole-word visual recognition, mean<strong>in</strong>g is attempted directly from <strong>the</strong>graphic image of a word, thus it is <strong>in</strong>dependent of <strong>the</strong> knowledge of <strong>the</strong>alphabetic pr<strong>in</strong>ciple. Children lack<strong>in</strong>g awareness of <strong>the</strong> graphemephonemerelationship, quite naturally, are <strong>in</strong> no way capable of read<strong>in</strong>gwords <strong>the</strong>y are not familiar with. By contrast, <strong>the</strong> holistic lexicalapproach proves effective with regard to well-known words, whichchildren have already come across and read several times, and it isefficient with homophones as well. Also, skilled readers use <strong>the</strong>opportunity to rely on <strong>the</strong> visual strategy for quick and automaticrecognition of familiar letter sequences.On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, dur<strong>in</strong>g phonological decod<strong>in</strong>g, particular letters <strong>in</strong>a word are first transposed <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong>ir correspond<strong>in</strong>g sounds (or <strong>the</strong>irmental representations), which are <strong>the</strong>n syn<strong>the</strong>sised <strong>in</strong>to a k<strong>in</strong>d of


16 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>acoustic code that is recognised. In that way, with <strong>the</strong> use of speechprocess<strong>in</strong>g mechanisms, <strong>the</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> word is unlocked. Phonologicaldecod<strong>in</strong>g might seem relatively complicated, however, it is<strong>in</strong>dispensable for read<strong>in</strong>g unknown words and pseudo-words. Bycontrast, it loses its efficiency with reference to read<strong>in</strong>g homophonesand homographs.Unknown, difficult words and pseudo-words can also be successfullydecoded with <strong>the</strong> use of both <strong>the</strong> analogy between <strong>the</strong> well-known andunknown words and contextual cues. When approach<strong>in</strong>g new words,readers are equipped with <strong>the</strong> ability to recognise familiar sequences ofletters, frequently occurr<strong>in</strong>g at <strong>the</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g or end of o<strong>the</strong>r words that<strong>the</strong>y are already capable of read<strong>in</strong>g. A natural tendency that childrendemonstrate towards identify<strong>in</strong>g subsyllabic elements, such as onsetsand rimes, promotes read<strong>in</strong>g through analogy.The lexical (visual) and sublexical (phonological) strategies for read<strong>in</strong>gconstitute <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>cipal components of <strong>the</strong> dual route model of read<strong>in</strong>g(Col<strong>the</strong>art et al., 2001). The model assumes that words can be read ei<strong>the</strong>rvia direct orthography-to-phonology mapp<strong>in</strong>gs or <strong>in</strong>direct phoneme-tographemecorrespondences. The former depends on knowledge of how<strong>the</strong> whole word is pronounced, and <strong>the</strong> latter <strong>in</strong>volves knowledge of how<strong>in</strong>dividual graphemes (sublexical units) are pronounced.Readers have at <strong>the</strong>ir disposal yet ano<strong>the</strong>r strategy for attempt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>mean<strong>in</strong>g of unknown words, namely, anticipation from context. Sentencesconta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a target word, illustrations accompany<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> text andknowledge about <strong>the</strong> world can be of considerable help <strong>in</strong> ga<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g accessto mean<strong>in</strong>g. Still, it is not surpris<strong>in</strong>g that a lot of guess<strong>in</strong>g is bound to takeplace when this strategy is employed. None<strong>the</strong>less, <strong>the</strong> role of contextualclues <strong>in</strong> ga<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g understand<strong>in</strong>g of pr<strong>in</strong>ted <strong>in</strong>formation is highlighted,ma<strong>in</strong>ly <strong>in</strong> reference to text read<strong>in</strong>g strategies. The follow<strong>in</strong>g is a shortdiscussion of <strong>the</strong> concept of bottom-up, top-down and <strong>in</strong>teractive waysof text process<strong>in</strong>g.Let us beg<strong>in</strong> with <strong>the</strong> bottom-up model, which presumes that <strong>the</strong> readeranalyses <strong>the</strong> visual stimuli, often transform<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m <strong>in</strong>to auditory stimuli,and <strong>the</strong>n recognises <strong>the</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> consecutive elements of <strong>the</strong> text,until he/she understands <strong>the</strong> whole phrase or sentence. This model entailstwo different mechanisms/strategies. The first one visual assumesimmediate transformation of <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation from <strong>the</strong> eye to <strong>the</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d.More precisely, a graphic representation (picture) of a fragment of a giventext is used as a code, open<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> mental lexicon, and <strong>in</strong> this way <strong>the</strong>mean<strong>in</strong>g is reached. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, <strong>the</strong> second mechanism phonological <strong>in</strong>volves a direct transformation of <strong>the</strong> visual substanceof <strong>the</strong> text (or its image) onto <strong>the</strong> phonological level or, <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r words,visually perceived elements of <strong>the</strong> text are changed <strong>in</strong>to articulationexpectancy (mental representation of pronunciation), which, <strong>in</strong> turn,


Becom<strong>in</strong>g Literate 17opens <strong>the</strong> mental lexicon, utilis<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> mechanisms of perception andcomprehension of speech. It means that <strong>the</strong> reader pronounces <strong>the</strong> wordsbe<strong>in</strong>g read, or at least imag<strong>in</strong>es <strong>the</strong>ir sounds, and <strong>in</strong> that way arrives at<strong>the</strong>ir mean<strong>in</strong>g. Apparently, <strong>the</strong>se mechanisms do not exclude each o<strong>the</strong>r;what is more, both can be used dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> process of read<strong>in</strong>g a given text,but with regard to its different elements. The phonological mechanism isused <strong>in</strong> process<strong>in</strong>g unknown words, whereas <strong>the</strong> visual mechanism isutilised <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g familiar words (Bogdanowicz & Krasowicz-Kupis,2005a; Jorm, 1985; Krasowicz, 1997; Krasowicz-Kupis, 1999; Reid, 1998).By contrast, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> top-down model <strong>the</strong> reader forms hypo<strong>the</strong>ses andpredictions about <strong>the</strong> text and <strong>the</strong>n tries to absorb <strong>the</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g from <strong>the</strong>available clues. The h<strong>in</strong>ts concern <strong>the</strong> syntactic (<strong>the</strong> structure of <strong>the</strong>sentence) and semantic (<strong>the</strong> anticipated mean<strong>in</strong>g) context of <strong>the</strong> textbe<strong>in</strong>g read as well as <strong>the</strong> graphic <strong>in</strong>formation, perta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g to what a wordor a sentence looks like. Thus, <strong>the</strong> reader draws on his/her experienceand knowledge of <strong>the</strong> world and language (Bogdanowicz & Krasowicz-Kupis, 2005a; Krasowicz, 1997; Krasowicz-Kupis, 1999; Reid, 1998).In his <strong>in</strong>teractive-compensatory model, Stanovich (1980) describes read<strong>in</strong>gas an <strong>in</strong>teractive process, <strong>in</strong> which <strong>the</strong> reader utilises both <strong>the</strong> bottom-upand <strong>the</strong> top-down strategies, thus <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation from <strong>the</strong> graphic andcontextual levels is used simultaneously. It is implied that some readersmay rely more heavily on a given way of process<strong>in</strong>g (e.g. <strong>the</strong> use ofcontext), depend<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong> level of development of <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g skill.Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, for <strong>the</strong> most part, <strong>in</strong>dividuals whose decod<strong>in</strong>g (recognition)skills are poor use contextual clues; <strong>the</strong>refore, quite naturally, children <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g stages of learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and <strong>in</strong>dividuals encounter<strong>in</strong>gread<strong>in</strong>g disorders would fall <strong>in</strong>to this category. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, for poorreaders, recognis<strong>in</strong>g words requires such a great effort that <strong>the</strong> cognitivecapacities for comprehension are reduced. Unlike poor readers, goodreaders, armed with efficient word recognition skills (quick and automaticdecod<strong>in</strong>g), demonstrate no need to lean on <strong>the</strong> context. On <strong>the</strong> contrary,<strong>the</strong>y visually process every word, even highly predictable ones. Therefore,<strong>the</strong>y may concentrate <strong>the</strong>ir full cognitive potential on understand<strong>in</strong>g(Adams, 1990; Stanovich, 1980). Never<strong>the</strong>less, a tendency for <strong>the</strong> partialuse of contextual clues may occur <strong>in</strong> good readers when approach<strong>in</strong>gparticularly difficult texts, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g complex or unclear <strong>in</strong>formation.Moreover, <strong>the</strong> model proposes that <strong>the</strong> weaknesses of <strong>the</strong> reader arecompensated for by his/her strengths (Stanovich, 1980).Read<strong>in</strong>g developmentThe development of read<strong>in</strong>g is commonly seen to proceed <strong>in</strong> phasesor stages. The way childrens’ read<strong>in</strong>g ability changes with age isspecified <strong>in</strong> various phase models of read<strong>in</strong>g, however <strong>the</strong>se models


18 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>often fail to del<strong>in</strong>eate how exactly <strong>the</strong> changes take place. In mostdevelopmental (phase) models, <strong>the</strong> acquisition of read<strong>in</strong>g beg<strong>in</strong>s with avisual stage followed by a l<strong>in</strong>guistic stage. However, <strong>the</strong> models differ <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong> way <strong>the</strong> stages of read<strong>in</strong>g development are sequenced and <strong>in</strong>descriptions of <strong>the</strong> characteristic behaviour of children dur<strong>in</strong>g particularstages, with <strong>the</strong> most salient difference regard<strong>in</strong>g when (how early) andhow children beg<strong>in</strong> to rely on phonological <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g torecognise words (Ehri, 2005; Treiman & Kessler, 2007). Still, <strong>the</strong> fact that<strong>the</strong> models lack convergence with respect to certa<strong>in</strong> areas, while, at <strong>the</strong>same time show<strong>in</strong>g certa<strong>in</strong> similarities <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r aspects, allows <strong>in</strong>terpretationof a relatively more complete and reliable picture of <strong>the</strong>process. It needs stress<strong>in</strong>g though that <strong>the</strong> stages are not discrete, butra<strong>the</strong>r overlapp<strong>in</strong>g and reflect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> specific demands of <strong>the</strong> texts be<strong>in</strong>gdealt with.Generally, when children start learn<strong>in</strong>g to read, <strong>the</strong>y use a visualapproach and form quite arbitrary connections between pr<strong>in</strong>ted wordsand <strong>the</strong>ir pronunciation, <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong>y beg<strong>in</strong> to perceive and understandhow letters of pr<strong>in</strong>ted words map onto sounds <strong>in</strong> a systematic way. Asread<strong>in</strong>g skill develops, children consolidate <strong>the</strong> knowledge of lettersoundcorrespondences (alphabetic stage), <strong>the</strong> application of whichbecomes progressively more automatic and less effortful. F<strong>in</strong>ally, <strong>the</strong>orthographic representations become fully specified (<strong>the</strong> orthographicstage).Models of read<strong>in</strong>g are numerous and diverse (e.g. Sochacka, 2004; Wolfet al., 2005); however, it is not with<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> scope of this book to discuss <strong>the</strong>mextensively. Let us <strong>in</strong>stead draw <strong>the</strong> attention of <strong>the</strong> reader to <strong>the</strong> generalcharacterisation of <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g models: <strong>the</strong> phase model of wordread<strong>in</strong>g proposed by Ehri; <strong>the</strong> three-stage model of read<strong>in</strong>g by Frith,claim<strong>in</strong>g universality across languages; and <strong>the</strong> balance model of read<strong>in</strong>gby Bakker, pretend<strong>in</strong>g to possess an explanatory power with regard tospecific difficulties <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and spell developmentaldyslexia.Ehri (Ehri, 1995, 2005, 2008; Ehri & McCormick, 1998) dist<strong>in</strong>guishesfour phases <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> process of learn<strong>in</strong>g words pre-alphabetic phase andthree alphabetic stages, namely, partial, full and consolidated. The prealphabeticstage is characterised by form<strong>in</strong>g connections betweenselected visual features of words and <strong>the</strong>ir pronunciation and mean<strong>in</strong>g,children are not yet capable of mak<strong>in</strong>g use of systematic letter-soundmapp<strong>in</strong>gs. There are tight bonds between <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g three stages,mov<strong>in</strong>g from partial, through full towards consolidated alphabetic phasereflect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> process of development from immature towards matureand automatised read<strong>in</strong>g. Dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> partial alphabetic stage, beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>greaders, who are aware of several letter names and <strong>the</strong>ir correspond<strong>in</strong>gsounds, learn how to remember words through creat<strong>in</strong>g conventional,


Becom<strong>in</strong>g Literate 19though still partial, matches between <strong>the</strong> letters and sounds <strong>in</strong> words.Very often, first and last letters leave most last<strong>in</strong>g traces <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir memory.Children become familiar with selected letter-sound relations, <strong>the</strong>y arealso capable of perform<strong>in</strong>g simple phonological analysis tasks. Equippedwith <strong>the</strong> knowledge of <strong>the</strong> alphabetic system, children become moreadvanced <strong>in</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> powerful tool, enhanc<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> process of form<strong>in</strong>gand remember<strong>in</strong>g l<strong>in</strong>ks between pr<strong>in</strong>ted words and <strong>the</strong>ir pronunciations.The strategy of rely<strong>in</strong>g on phonological h<strong>in</strong>ts is beyond doubt muchmore efficient and productive than depend<strong>in</strong>g on visual clues specificto pre-alphabetic stage. In full alphabetic phase, beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g readers learnto read words through creat<strong>in</strong>g full letter-sound connections, <strong>in</strong> addition,<strong>the</strong>y ga<strong>in</strong> access to <strong>the</strong> decod<strong>in</strong>g strategy and <strong>the</strong> strategy of read<strong>in</strong>gwords by analogy. F<strong>in</strong>ally, <strong>the</strong> consolidated alphabetic phase <strong>in</strong>volvesautomatic application of grapheme-phoneme conversion rules. Moreover,children learn how to make use of bigger units syllables andsubsyllable spell<strong>in</strong>g patterns (e.g. alliterations, rimes), which <strong>the</strong>yrepeatedly come across <strong>in</strong> different words. Rely<strong>in</strong>g on multiletter units<strong>in</strong> order to decode words reduces memory load.Frith (Dockrell & McShane, 1993; Frith, 1985; Krasowicz, 1997;Krasowicz-Kupis, 1999; Ott, 1997; Reid, 1998; Sochacka, 2004) identifiesthree stages <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g acquisition. The first is <strong>the</strong> logographic stage,dur<strong>in</strong>g which a child visually recognises <strong>the</strong> overall word patterns. Moreprecisely, words are perceived as units based on <strong>the</strong> first or last letter, acluster of letters or a general shape of a word. Thus, recognition dependson <strong>the</strong> visual memory of words that children are already acqua<strong>in</strong>tedwith. Therefore, <strong>the</strong>y f<strong>in</strong>d it very difficult to read unfamiliar or nonsensewords. Dur<strong>in</strong>g this stage, <strong>the</strong> relations between letters and sounds are notyet <strong>in</strong>tegrated, which is why children frequently misspell <strong>the</strong> words <strong>the</strong>ycan already read.The next stage <strong>in</strong> Frith’s model of read<strong>in</strong>g is <strong>the</strong> alphabetic stage. Achild becomes aware of <strong>the</strong> fact that words can be segmented <strong>in</strong>tophonemes and, fur<strong>the</strong>r on, that <strong>the</strong>se phonemes can be mapped onletters. The ability to match <strong>the</strong> sounds to <strong>the</strong> correspond<strong>in</strong>g symbols letters is def<strong>in</strong>ed as <strong>the</strong> alphabetic competence. As such, thiscompetence constitutes a necessary but, <strong>in</strong> fact, <strong>in</strong>sufficient conditionfor appropriate read<strong>in</strong>g, as it does not allow differentiat<strong>in</strong>g betweenirregular words. Spell<strong>in</strong>g is believed to enhance <strong>the</strong> alphabetic stage ofread<strong>in</strong>g by virtue of more direct l<strong>in</strong>ks to <strong>the</strong> letter-sound relationships.When a child enters <strong>the</strong> third stage <strong>the</strong> orthographic stage his/herread<strong>in</strong>g is a result of an amalgamation of <strong>the</strong> skills acquired dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>previous stages. Words are recognised quickly, automatically and with<strong>the</strong> use of adequate decod<strong>in</strong>g strategy.As already mentioned, <strong>the</strong> course of <strong>the</strong> three stages differs for <strong>the</strong>acquisition of read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g. That is to say, <strong>the</strong> visual, logographic


20 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>code is used faster dur<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g, while <strong>the</strong> alphabetic stage is firstobservable <strong>in</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g, and <strong>in</strong> fact <strong>the</strong> reverse is <strong>the</strong> case as relates to <strong>the</strong>orthographic code. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Reid (1998), children with dyslexia mayhave some difficulties <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> visual stage, but <strong>the</strong>y will almost certa<strong>in</strong>lyencounter problems <strong>in</strong> translat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> letters <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong>ir phonologicalequivalents.The physiology of <strong>the</strong> human bra<strong>in</strong> toge<strong>the</strong>r with bra<strong>in</strong> function<strong>in</strong>g asrelates to <strong>the</strong> cognitive processes, form <strong>the</strong> basis for <strong>the</strong> balance model oflearn<strong>in</strong>g to read, proposed by Bakker (Bakker, 1984, 1990, 1995; Bakkeret al., 1991, 1995; Bednarek, 1999; Bogdanowicz & Krasowicz, 1995, 1996/1997; Dryer et al., 1999; Kappers, 1997; Krasowicz, 1997; Robertson, 2000a,2000b). The left hemisphere is said to be specialised or dom<strong>in</strong>ant forlanguage <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> majority of people, whereas <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere dealswith perception of shape, form and direction (visual-spatial <strong>in</strong>formation).The balance model of learn<strong>in</strong>g to read presumes that both hemispheresare simultaneously <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> process of read<strong>in</strong>g, though notequally. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, <strong>the</strong> level of <strong>the</strong> right- and left-hemisphericentailment shifts accord<strong>in</strong>g to both <strong>the</strong> ability to read and <strong>the</strong> perceptualfeatures of <strong>the</strong> text. To be more precise, <strong>the</strong> right and left hemispherespredom<strong>in</strong>antly mediate beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g and advanced read<strong>in</strong>g, respectively.Greater activity of <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere is noted when <strong>the</strong> text <strong>in</strong>cludessome perceptually challeng<strong>in</strong>g features, for <strong>in</strong>stance, unusual fonts ordraw<strong>in</strong>gs. In <strong>the</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g stages of learn<strong>in</strong>g to read, spatial-perceptualanalysis of letter shapes and letter str<strong>in</strong>gs, identification of letter namesand unfamiliar words require greater right hemisphere <strong>in</strong>volvement. On<strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, advanced read<strong>in</strong>g is characterised by <strong>the</strong> use of l<strong>in</strong>guisticstrategies that are under <strong>the</strong> control of <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere. Hence, <strong>in</strong>successful read<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>the</strong> visual-spatial process<strong>in</strong>g becomes <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>glyautomatic and <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistic strategies (syntactic and semanticanalyses) become superior, thus a shift from right hemisphere dom<strong>in</strong>anceto left hemisphere dom<strong>in</strong>ance is presupposed and it is claimed to takeplace at around eight years of age.In his balance model, Bakker proposes a quite plausible, though notwithout faults, explanation of specific difficulties <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g to read.Apparently, some children are unable to adopt <strong>the</strong> appropriate strategies,ei<strong>the</strong>r perceptual or l<strong>in</strong>guistic, at <strong>the</strong> right time dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> process oflearn<strong>in</strong>g to read; consequently, some disturbances may emerge. Bakkerproposes <strong>the</strong> existence of two types of dyslexia <strong>the</strong> P-type and L-type which result from <strong>the</strong> overuse of <strong>the</strong> perceptual (P) and l<strong>in</strong>guistic (L)strategy, respectively. The difficulties emerge if dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> process oflearn<strong>in</strong>g to read, <strong>the</strong> shift of dom<strong>in</strong>ance from <strong>the</strong> right to <strong>the</strong> lef<strong>the</strong>misphere takes place ei<strong>the</strong>r too late (P-type dyslexia) or too early(L-type dyslexia). A child with P-type dyslexia fails to utilise <strong>the</strong> lef<strong>the</strong>misphericstrategies, necessary to achieve read<strong>in</strong>g fluency, at <strong>the</strong> same


Becom<strong>in</strong>g Literate 21time heavily rely<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong> visual-spatial features of <strong>the</strong> texts. That iswhy read<strong>in</strong>g is slow, fragmented, but still characterised by relativelyhigh accuracy. Conversely, L-type dyslexics are claimed to adoptl<strong>in</strong>guistic (left-hemispheric) strategies from <strong>the</strong> very beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g oflearn<strong>in</strong>g to read, <strong>the</strong>y lack <strong>the</strong> automatic ability to recognise lettershapes and str<strong>in</strong>gs. S<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>the</strong>y do not focus on <strong>the</strong> surface (visual-spatial)features of <strong>the</strong> text, <strong>the</strong>y read relatively quickly, however, <strong>the</strong>y tend toproduce numerous substantive mistakes (low accuracy), which <strong>in</strong> turnmay lead to comprehension problems. Thus, L-type and P-type dyslexicsmay be dist<strong>in</strong>guished by read<strong>in</strong>g speed, accuracy and type of errors <strong>the</strong>ycommit. Two types of errors are generally taken <strong>in</strong>to consideration:substantive (omissions, chang<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> sequence of sounds and o<strong>the</strong>rdeformations) and time errors (frequent pauses, repetitions, read<strong>in</strong>gletter by letter). Substantive errors <strong>in</strong>dicate <strong>the</strong> superiority of <strong>the</strong> lef<strong>the</strong>misphere and are to be expected <strong>in</strong> L-type dyslexics. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rhand, time errors <strong>in</strong>dicate <strong>the</strong> supremacy of <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere and<strong>the</strong>ir existence is to be assumed <strong>in</strong> P-type dyslexics. However useful <strong>the</strong>abovementioned typology may seem, some children with dyslexiacannot be classified <strong>in</strong> this way. Approximately 60% (Bednarek, 1999;Bogdanowicz & Krasowicz, 1996/1997; Kappers, 1997) to 65% (Bakkeret al., 1995) of children with dyslexia fall <strong>in</strong>to ei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> P-type or L-typegroup, whereas <strong>the</strong> rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g 3540% cannot be classified accord<strong>in</strong>g toBakker’s typology. 6The abovementioned developmental models of read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>vited criticismow<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> lack of description of <strong>the</strong> causal conditions. Bryant andGoswami (Goswami, 1999; Goswami & Bryant, 1990) <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir causal <strong>the</strong>oryof development of <strong>the</strong> ability to read, concentrate on specify<strong>in</strong>g factors thatcondition progress <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g to read ra<strong>the</strong>r than on describ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>phases of read<strong>in</strong>g acquisition. Causal <strong>in</strong>fluence on <strong>the</strong> process of learn<strong>in</strong>gto read has been assigned to several factors. Firstly, to early phonologicalabilities (e.g. recognis<strong>in</strong>g and creat<strong>in</strong>g rhymes), secondly, to formalteach<strong>in</strong>g of read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g, with special emphasis placed on <strong>the</strong>acquisition of grapheme-phoneme conversion rules, and, f<strong>in</strong>ally, to <strong>the</strong>mutual <strong>in</strong>fluence that read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g exert on each o<strong>the</strong>r.Orthographic Depth and Gra<strong>in</strong> Size: A Cross-languagePerspective on Read<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>Dyslexia</strong>Orthographic depthAlphabetic orthographic systems can be classified accord<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong>consistency of <strong>the</strong> letter-to-sound relations, def<strong>in</strong>ed as orthographic depth.Deep orthographies demonstrate considerably unpredictable andunequivocal grapheme-phoneme correspondences and complexitiessuch as, for <strong>in</strong>stance, multiletter graphemes or frequent irregularities.


22 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Conversely, shallow orthographies have simple letter-sound relations. Insome languages, a given letter or letter cluster is always pronounced <strong>the</strong>same way (e.g. Greek, Italian, Spanish), whereas <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r languages it canhave several dist<strong>in</strong>ct pronunciations (e.g. English, Danish). It workssimilarly <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r direction a phoneme can be represented withmultiple spell<strong>in</strong>g choices (e.g. English, French, Hebrew) or is nearlyalways spelled <strong>the</strong> same way (e.g. Italian) (Ziegler & Goswami, 2006;Frost & Ziegler, 2007). Among deep, opaque orthographies <strong>the</strong>re are, for<strong>in</strong>stance, English, French, Danish and Portuguese, while Spanish, F<strong>in</strong>nishor Turkish fall <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong> category of shallow, transparent orthographies.Spencer (2007) sees <strong>the</strong> need for a less <strong>in</strong>tuitive and more ref<strong>in</strong>eddef<strong>in</strong>ition of <strong>the</strong> orthographic depth, with adequate attention given to <strong>the</strong>actual direction of <strong>the</strong> grapheme-phoneme relation. Highly transparentorthographies, such as F<strong>in</strong>nish or Turkish, have one-to-one correspondence<strong>in</strong> both phonology-orthography (spell<strong>in</strong>g) and orthographyphonology(read<strong>in</strong>g). By contrast, English lacks consistency <strong>in</strong> ei<strong>the</strong>rdirection. German or Greek, however, show highly regular mapp<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>read<strong>in</strong>g, but <strong>the</strong>y tend to be considerably complex with regard tospell<strong>in</strong>g. Goswami (2000) uses <strong>the</strong> terms feedforward (spell<strong>in</strong>g-to-sound)and feedback (sound-to-spell<strong>in</strong>g) consistency, both of which can bemeasured and both exert substantial <strong>in</strong>fluence on <strong>the</strong> phoneme-levelrestructur<strong>in</strong>g.In addition, <strong>in</strong>dividual words with<strong>in</strong> deep orthographies may becharacterised by vary<strong>in</strong>g degrees of depth, which has a strong effect onread<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g accuracy. Some resemble words from shalloworthographies with simple one-to-one mapp<strong>in</strong>gs between graphemesand phonemes, whereas o<strong>the</strong>r words pose considerable difficulty due tographeme complexity. Spencer (2007) cites an example of two threephonemewords ‘might’ and ‘sly’ which appear to represent differentdegrees of grapheme complexity, operationalised as word letter-length.Generally, <strong>in</strong>creased word letter-length was found to correlate withmore complex orthographic structures with<strong>in</strong> words. The effects of wordletter-length can be modified by word frequency with high-frequencywords be<strong>in</strong>g resistant to word letter-length effect. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Spencer(2007), word orthographic depth, especially <strong>the</strong> level of <strong>in</strong>dividualphonemes transparency, word complexity and frequency, uniquely<strong>in</strong>fluence word spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> young children. Spencer (2007)<strong>in</strong>vestigated <strong>the</strong> comparative strength of both orthography-phonology andphonology-orthography measures of transparency at <strong>the</strong> f<strong>in</strong>e-gra<strong>in</strong>phoneme-grapheme level <strong>in</strong> high-frequency English words <strong>in</strong> predict<strong>in</strong>gyoung children’s difficulty with word spell<strong>in</strong>g. Both orthographic <strong>in</strong>consistenciesas well as complexities were shown to <strong>in</strong>fluence <strong>the</strong> degreeof difficulty with word spell<strong>in</strong>g, with <strong>the</strong> orthographic complexities


Becom<strong>in</strong>g Literate 23(translated simply <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong> differences between <strong>the</strong> number of letters andphonemes <strong>in</strong> a word) hav<strong>in</strong>g greater effect size.Still, given that greater or lesser transparency or orthographic depthcharacterises particular alphabetic languages, it is <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g, whe<strong>the</strong>rthis vary<strong>in</strong>g degree of consistency <strong>in</strong> mapp<strong>in</strong>g graphemes onto phonemescan be responsible for qualitative differences <strong>in</strong> literacy acquisitionand, consequently, for <strong>the</strong> way read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties manifest<strong>the</strong>mselves <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>se languages. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, this<strong>in</strong>deed is <strong>the</strong> case (e.g. Davies et al., 2007).The orthographic depth hypo<strong>the</strong>sis (Frost et al., 1987; Katz & Frost, 1992),based on <strong>the</strong> dual route model of read<strong>in</strong>g (Col<strong>the</strong>art et al., 2001; Treiman &Kessler, 2007), assumes that differences <strong>in</strong> literacy acquisition depend on<strong>the</strong> orthographic system of a language. In shallow orthographies, <strong>the</strong>process is based ma<strong>in</strong>ly on language phonology; readers tend to rely on<strong>the</strong> sublexical or phonological route, simply due to unambiguous,transparent letter-to-sound relations. Deep orthographies require <strong>the</strong>application of <strong>the</strong> logographic strategy, based on <strong>the</strong> visual-orthographicstructure. Shallow orthographies permit greater use of sublexicalcorrespondences because <strong>the</strong>y provide reliable pronunciations, whereasdeep orthographies <strong>in</strong>vite ma<strong>in</strong>ly lexical mapp<strong>in</strong>gs or orthographic routebecause sublexical correspondences repeatedly turn out to be unreliable,thus lead<strong>in</strong>g to erroneous pronunciations.Inasmuch as <strong>the</strong> orthographic depth hypo<strong>the</strong>sis suggests that,depend<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong> nature of a given orthography, readers choose to relyon <strong>the</strong> whole word recognition/orthographic route or phonological one,it does not propose that <strong>the</strong> vary<strong>in</strong>g psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic units may developdue to differences <strong>in</strong> orthographic systems (Ziegler & Goswami, 2006).However, it appears that <strong>the</strong> importance of a type of l<strong>in</strong>guistic chunk (e.g.onset, rime, phoneme) <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> process of develop<strong>in</strong>g phonologicalawareness and <strong>the</strong> ability to read is different across languages. In moretransparent or shallow orthographies, <strong>the</strong> awareness of <strong>in</strong>dividualphonemes is seem<strong>in</strong>gly more important for <strong>the</strong> effects of read<strong>in</strong>g.Children learn<strong>in</strong>g to read <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>se orthographies rapidly form orthographicrepresentations of phoneme-level <strong>in</strong>formation. By contrast, <strong>in</strong>languages with deep, <strong>in</strong>consistent orthographies, <strong>the</strong> awareness of onsetsand rimes seems crucial. Group<strong>in</strong>g words accord<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> sounds <strong>the</strong>ybeg<strong>in</strong> with or common end<strong>in</strong>gs, and decod<strong>in</strong>g new words throughanalogy is ra<strong>the</strong>r typical of non-transparent orthographies such asEnglish. For <strong>in</strong>stance, <strong>the</strong> onset-rime level representations provide asufficiently effective tool for dist<strong>in</strong>guish<strong>in</strong>g between numerous similarsound<strong>in</strong>gwords. Words like ‘cat’, ‘hat’, ‘rat’, ‘bat’ or ‘hen’, ‘pen’, ‘ten’,‘men’ form similarity neighbourhoods (Bogdanowicz & Krasowicz-Kupis, 2005a; Goswami, 2000; Krasowicz-Kupis & Bryant, 2004;Sochacka, 2004).


24 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic gra<strong>in</strong> sizeThe psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic gra<strong>in</strong> size <strong>the</strong>ory (Ziegler & Goswami, 2005, 2006)attempts to expla<strong>in</strong> substantial differences <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g accuracy and speedacross languages. These differences are said to reflect both <strong>the</strong> fundamentaldisparities <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> nature of <strong>the</strong> system for mapp<strong>in</strong>g visualsymbols onto phonological segments (units of sound) and read<strong>in</strong>gstrategies, whose development is considerably <strong>in</strong>fluenced by a givenorthography. Davies et al. (2007) specify gra<strong>in</strong> size as <strong>the</strong> size of anorthography-to-phonology mapp<strong>in</strong>g unit (<strong>the</strong> number of letters correspond<strong>in</strong>gto a phonological unit). Hence, gra<strong>in</strong> sizes vary fromgrapheme-phoneme, through rime to lexical level. Individual soundsare represented by graphemes (consist<strong>in</strong>g of one or more letters). S<strong>in</strong>glesyllable words comprise onset (<strong>in</strong>itial consonant or a consonant cluster)and rime (<strong>the</strong> rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g part). F<strong>in</strong>ally, lexical mapp<strong>in</strong>gs relate tocomplete phonology to orthography representations of a word.The proponents of <strong>the</strong> psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic gra<strong>in</strong> size <strong>the</strong>ory argue thatlearn<strong>in</strong>g to read <strong>in</strong> more transparent orthographies (e.g. F<strong>in</strong>nish, Italianor Greek) <strong>in</strong>volves heavy reliance on <strong>the</strong> grapheme-phoneme correspondencesfor word recognition because <strong>the</strong>se grapheme-phoneme mapp<strong>in</strong>gsare relatively consistent. Still, it is not denied that larger gra<strong>in</strong> sizerepresentations may successfully develop <strong>in</strong> mature readers of orthographieswith a high degree of consistency (Goswami & Ziegler, 2006).Unlike <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> case of transparent orthographies, while learn<strong>in</strong>g to read <strong>in</strong>languages that lack substantial regularity as relates to graphemephonemeconversion, like English or unvowelled Hebrew, childrencannot resort to smaller gra<strong>in</strong> sizes as easily because such languagesare characterised by greater <strong>in</strong>consistency with regard to smaller (s<strong>in</strong>gleletters or letter clusters represent<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividual phonemes) than largerread<strong>in</strong>g units (rimes or syllables). As a consequence, English-speak<strong>in</strong>gchildren may face <strong>the</strong> need to master mapp<strong>in</strong>g strategies at more thanone gra<strong>in</strong> size (multiple gra<strong>in</strong> size strategy). Hence employ<strong>in</strong>g, apartfrom <strong>the</strong> grapheme-phoneme conversion strategy, which does not seemto be sufficiently effective, <strong>the</strong> strategy of recognition of letter patterns forrimes and <strong>the</strong> whole word recognition strategy. However, <strong>the</strong> bigger <strong>the</strong>gra<strong>in</strong> size, <strong>the</strong> more orthographic units to learn. Some implications for<strong>in</strong>structional practices can be drawn from <strong>the</strong> above discussion, withprobably <strong>the</strong> most obvious suggestion that small gra<strong>in</strong> size units shouldbe given special attention <strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> consistent orthographies.On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, children learn<strong>in</strong>g to read <strong>in</strong> less consistentorthographies could possibly benefit more from a comb<strong>in</strong>ation ofteach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> small and large gra<strong>in</strong> size units and whole-word approach(‘look and say’).


Becom<strong>in</strong>g Literate 25Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, Vousden (2008), <strong>in</strong> order to form a rationale for choos<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong> amount and types of spell<strong>in</strong>g-to-sound units for read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction,views differently sized unit knowledge from an environmental ra<strong>the</strong>rthan a developmental perspective. She reports evidence on <strong>the</strong> potentialusefulness of English sound-to-symbol relations with regard to wholewords, onsets, rimes and phonemes <strong>in</strong> monosyllabic text read<strong>in</strong>g. In fact,<strong>the</strong> most frequently occurr<strong>in</strong>g mapp<strong>in</strong>gs at each level allow read<strong>in</strong>g of alarge proportion of monosyllabic text, with small-size (graphemephoneme)units seem<strong>in</strong>gly be<strong>in</strong>g more effective than onset/rime correspondences.This potentially bears some important implications forread<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction as long as support is provided to generalise <strong>the</strong>f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs to multisyllabic text as well.Much as <strong>the</strong> evidence <strong>in</strong> favour of <strong>the</strong> larger gra<strong>in</strong> size effects onread<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> less transparent orthographies is strik<strong>in</strong>g (e.g. Ziegler et al.,2003), it is worth consider<strong>in</strong>g a view that is alternative to <strong>the</strong> strongversion of <strong>the</strong> psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic gra<strong>in</strong> size <strong>the</strong>ory and <strong>the</strong> orthographicdepth hypo<strong>the</strong>sis, assum<strong>in</strong>g heavy reliance on grapheme-phonemecorrespondences <strong>in</strong> transparent orthographic systems. Davies et al.(2007) highlight that resort<strong>in</strong>g to sublexical mapp<strong>in</strong>gs can be characteristicof early read<strong>in</strong>g, followed by <strong>the</strong> establishment of larger gra<strong>in</strong> sizemapp<strong>in</strong>gs. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, <strong>the</strong>y propose that large gra<strong>in</strong> size correspondencesmay well be formed quite early <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g development as can be<strong>the</strong> case, for <strong>in</strong>stance, <strong>in</strong> Italian, where lexical and morphologicalproperties exert considerable <strong>in</strong>fluence on pr<strong>in</strong>t (Burani et al., 2002;Barca et al., 2007). Davies et al. (2007) aptly expla<strong>in</strong> that such aphenomenon may depend on <strong>the</strong> relative frequency of multisyllabic(between two and four) words <strong>in</strong> transparent orthographies such asItalian or Spanish. Approach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>se words armed with a grapheme-tophonemeconversion strategy may turn out <strong>in</strong>effective <strong>in</strong> terms of time.Hence <strong>in</strong>vit<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> strategy of large gra<strong>in</strong> size mapp<strong>in</strong>g (e.g. lexical level),which enhances read<strong>in</strong>g efficiency by allow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> pronounc<strong>in</strong>g of largergroups of graphemes at a time.In addition, factors such as word frequency, word length and orthographicneighbourhood size may modify read<strong>in</strong>g speed and accuracyga<strong>in</strong>s (Davies et al., 2007). Across languages vary<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> orthographicconsistency, longer words <strong>in</strong> comparison to shorter words are typicallyread more slowly, and less accurately at times. This length size effect isespecially manifest <strong>in</strong> younger and less able readers (Burani et al., 2002;Ziegler et al., 2003). Greater length effects are more evident with regard toread<strong>in</strong>g low-frequency words than high-frequency words, which usuallyrema<strong>in</strong> unaffected. The latter are also, more often than not, read quiteaccurately and fast, at least <strong>in</strong> some languages (e.g. Italian; Barca et al.,2007; Burani et al., 2002), a fact probably attributable to experience. F<strong>in</strong>ally,orthographic neighbourhood size, def<strong>in</strong>ed as <strong>the</strong> number of words that


26 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>can possibly be formed by chang<strong>in</strong>g a s<strong>in</strong>gle letter of a word or non-word,with letter positions preserved, reflects <strong>the</strong> facilitat<strong>in</strong>g impact of <strong>the</strong> largegra<strong>in</strong> size mapp<strong>in</strong>gs. Words with numerous orthographic neighbours arerepeatedly read more accurately and with greater speed, by children andadults alike, <strong>in</strong> comparison to words from sparse orthographic neighbourhoods.Importantly, orthographic neighbourhood size effect, be<strong>in</strong>g greaterfor younger and less-skilled readers, is also more evident <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g lowra<strong>the</strong>r than high-frequency words (Davies et al., 2007). For example,‘marsh’ has two neighbours, ‘harsh’ and ‘march’, while ‘cover’ has asmany as 13, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g ‘coven’, ‘cower’, ‘hover’, ‘lover’, ‘mover’ and‘rover’ (Lavidor et al., 2006).Lavidor et al. (2006) present a case study of developmental dyslexiawhere orthographic neighbourhood was tested. They observed largerreliance on right hemisphere orthographic process<strong>in</strong>g strategies <strong>in</strong>dyslexics when compared to normal readers. It appears that dyslexics,because of <strong>the</strong>ir preference towards more global process<strong>in</strong>g, which canbe traced back to <strong>the</strong> absence of clear phonological representations andf<strong>in</strong>e-gra<strong>in</strong>ed orthography-to-phonology cod<strong>in</strong>g, demonstrate considerableskill <strong>in</strong> process<strong>in</strong>g words that belong to a given neighbourhood, andthus share certa<strong>in</strong> letter str<strong>in</strong>gs. Generally, it is argued that, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> face of<strong>the</strong> apparent failure <strong>in</strong> achiev<strong>in</strong>g phonology-based left hemisphere wordrecognition process<strong>in</strong>g, some <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia can be characterisedby an <strong>in</strong>creased sensitivity to orthographic cues. All <strong>in</strong> all, while itis not argued that <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere holds an absolute advantage over<strong>the</strong> left one <strong>in</strong> orthographic process<strong>in</strong>g, it seems that even though <strong>the</strong>phonological, orthographic and semantic representations of words areprocessed <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> two hemispheres <strong>in</strong> vary<strong>in</strong>g proportions, <strong>the</strong>se proportionsmay be different <strong>in</strong> dyslexia.There is grow<strong>in</strong>g evidence that orthography, <strong>in</strong> particular <strong>the</strong>grapheme-phoneme consistency concern<strong>in</strong>g vowels, exerts considerable<strong>in</strong>fluence on <strong>the</strong> word read<strong>in</strong>g strategies used by readers <strong>in</strong> a givenlanguage. In cross-l<strong>in</strong>guistic comparisons, readers of English typicallycommit more errors with regard to both word and pseudo-word read<strong>in</strong>gthan readers of more regular orthographies. This situation may be <strong>the</strong>result of <strong>the</strong> strategy choice, namely, grapheme-phoneme assembly isapparently more heavily relied on by readers of shallow orthographies.Seymour et al. (2003) report <strong>the</strong> results of a cross-language read<strong>in</strong>gcomparison. Children from 14 European countries, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir first year ofread<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction, were given a matched set of items of simple realwords and non-words. Strik<strong>in</strong>gly, <strong>the</strong> children learn<strong>in</strong>g to read <strong>in</strong>consistent orthographies demonstrated excellent skill <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g bothfamiliar real words and pseudo-words by <strong>the</strong> middle of <strong>the</strong> first grade(Greek: 98 and 92% correct real words and non-words, respectively,F<strong>in</strong>nish: 98 and 95%, German: 98 and 94%, Italian: 95 and 89%, Spanish: 95


Becom<strong>in</strong>g Literate 27and 89%). By contrast, <strong>the</strong> ga<strong>in</strong>s of children learn<strong>in</strong>g to read Englishalmost reached floor levels (real words: 34% correct, non-words: 29%correct); slightly reduced results were also found for Danish (71 and 54%correct real words and non-words, respectively), Portuguese (73 and77%) and French (79 and 85%).Miller-Guron and Lundberg’s (2004) comparative <strong>in</strong>vestigation ofword read<strong>in</strong>g efficiency <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> analysis of frequency and types oferrors committed by early readers of English and Swedish <strong>in</strong>dicates that<strong>the</strong>y adopt differ<strong>in</strong>g strategies. The f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs suggest that Englishchildren were faster on <strong>the</strong> task because <strong>the</strong>y attempted more task items,however, <strong>the</strong>y made significantly more errors (ma<strong>in</strong>ly skipp<strong>in</strong>g items)than <strong>the</strong> more careful Swedish readers, for whom it took longer tocomplete <strong>the</strong> task but <strong>the</strong>y corrected <strong>the</strong>ir errors more often. Thedifferences apparently follow from <strong>the</strong> strategies adopted <strong>in</strong> approach<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong> task, more precisely, whole-word recognition <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> cases of Englishspeak<strong>in</strong>gchildren and grapheme-to-phoneme conversion <strong>in</strong> Swedishreaders. Similar pattern holds true for poor readers.The general f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g that it seems easier to learn to read <strong>in</strong> a transparentra<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>in</strong> a deep orthography is <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>gly extended andstreng<strong>the</strong>ned by Spencer and Hanley (2003), who compare <strong>the</strong> acquisitionof read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> transparent Welsh and deep English orthographies, both ofwhich constitute a part of <strong>the</strong> same educational system <strong>in</strong> Wales, wheresome children are taught to read <strong>in</strong> Welsh and some <strong>in</strong> English. They aretypically, though not necessarily, <strong>the</strong> native speakers of <strong>the</strong>se languages,which makes possible <strong>the</strong> comparison between Welsh children learn<strong>in</strong>g toread English and children learn<strong>in</strong>g to read Welsh. The f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of <strong>the</strong>study appear to support <strong>the</strong> view that <strong>the</strong> degree of orthographictransparency substantially <strong>in</strong>fluences <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial choice of read<strong>in</strong>g strategies.Children learn<strong>in</strong>g to read Welsh showed a preference towardsphonological strategies while attempt<strong>in</strong>g unfamiliar words, unlike thoselearn<strong>in</strong>g to read <strong>in</strong> English, who preferred visual, logographic strategies.Hanley et al. (2004) verified <strong>the</strong> facilitat<strong>in</strong>g effects of <strong>the</strong> transparentWelsh orthography on early read<strong>in</strong>g ability. They re-exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>the</strong>children from <strong>the</strong> abovementioned study three years later and foundthat children learn<strong>in</strong>g to read English rema<strong>in</strong>ed beh<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g lowand medium frequency irregular words but, apparently, had caught upwith those learn<strong>in</strong>g to read <strong>in</strong> Welsh with respect to read<strong>in</strong>g regularwords and non-words. The f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs demonstrate that <strong>the</strong> slowed downacquisition of read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> English can be traced back to <strong>the</strong> effects of itsnon-transparent deep orthography.In addition to read<strong>in</strong>g skill, <strong>the</strong> acquisition of spell<strong>in</strong>g ability alsoseems to be <strong>in</strong>fluenced by <strong>the</strong> type of orthographic depth of a language.Juul and Sigurdsson (2005) demonstrated a detrimental effect of <strong>the</strong>opaque Danish orthography on slower development of encod<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong>


28 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>comparison to <strong>the</strong> transparent Icelandic orthography. They compared adeep orthography to a handicap for children learn<strong>in</strong>g to spell <strong>in</strong> it andspecifically showed that Icelandic children markedly outperformedDanish-speak<strong>in</strong>g children on word medial consonant doublets and onword <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters.There is no deny<strong>in</strong>g that, as it stands, <strong>the</strong> psychological gra<strong>in</strong> size<strong>the</strong>ory is attractive and rich <strong>in</strong> potential to expla<strong>in</strong> variations <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>gaccuracy and speed across languages, but it has not escaped somecriticism. It has been po<strong>in</strong>ted out that greater attention should be devotedto <strong>the</strong> role of morphology <strong>in</strong> predict<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> degree of sensitivity to smallergra<strong>in</strong> sizes and expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g sound units sizes preferred for decod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>particular languages. This observation seems especially valid withreference to Turkish, an agglut<strong>in</strong>ative language <strong>in</strong> which morphemesoccupy a predef<strong>in</strong>ed order of attachment and a s<strong>in</strong>gle phoneme <strong>in</strong> a suffixcan alter <strong>the</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g (Durgunoğlu, 2006). Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, <strong>the</strong> particularchoice of certa<strong>in</strong> gra<strong>in</strong> sizes for decod<strong>in</strong>g may be determ<strong>in</strong>ed, for example,by unavailability of o<strong>the</strong>r sizes, not necessarily by orthographic consistency.Frost (2006) claims that <strong>the</strong> Hebrew unpo<strong>in</strong>ted pr<strong>in</strong>t consonantstr<strong>in</strong>g is ambiguous because different vowels may be <strong>in</strong>serted <strong>in</strong>to it, thusform<strong>in</strong>g different words. Consequently, root and word pattern morphemes(large gra<strong>in</strong> size) have to be used to provide <strong>in</strong>formation about <strong>the</strong> miss<strong>in</strong>gvowels. In fact, this miss<strong>in</strong>g phonemic <strong>in</strong>formation about vowels isresponsible for <strong>the</strong> opaqueness of Hebrew orthography. Interest<strong>in</strong>gly,even though Hebrew and English are irregular orthographies, <strong>the</strong>ir natureis apparently different. In English, unlike <strong>in</strong> Hebrew, <strong>the</strong> perceivedorthographic depth results from <strong>in</strong>consistency of letter clusters used torepresent sounds. However, <strong>in</strong>sert<strong>in</strong>g diacritical marks (‘po<strong>in</strong>ts’) represent<strong>in</strong>gvowels <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong> text converts deep Hebrew orthography <strong>in</strong>to ashallow one. Frost’s (2006) conclusion as to <strong>the</strong> character of <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>gprocess <strong>in</strong> Hebrew is that readers activate both letter-by-letter units andmorphemic units <strong>in</strong> parallel.It seems that, <strong>in</strong>deed, <strong>the</strong> differences <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> orthographic mapp<strong>in</strong>g onphonological elements across languages can be responsible for <strong>the</strong>discrepancies <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> word recognition strategies adopted by children,as well as <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>cidence and types of errors. However, it would besimplistic to assign <strong>the</strong> responsibility for <strong>the</strong>se differences to <strong>the</strong>orthographic depth only. O<strong>the</strong>r factors, such as morphemic complexity,apparently play a role as well.Gra<strong>in</strong> size and dyslexiaAs may be expected, especially <strong>in</strong> view of <strong>the</strong> propositions posed bypsychol<strong>in</strong>guistic gra<strong>in</strong> size <strong>the</strong>ory, <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tensity of dyslexic difficultiesdepends on <strong>the</strong> nature of a language and <strong>the</strong> range of skills required for


Becom<strong>in</strong>g Literate 29read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> that language. Reliability of <strong>the</strong> letter-sound mapp<strong>in</strong>gs iscrucial: it seems that <strong>the</strong> more transparent or shallow <strong>the</strong> orthography ofa given language, <strong>the</strong> fewer <strong>the</strong> difficulties encountered by dyslexicslearn<strong>in</strong>g to read <strong>in</strong> it (Miles, 2000; Snowl<strong>in</strong>g & Caravolas, 2007). As<strong>in</strong>dicated by Wimmer (1993), it applies ma<strong>in</strong>ly with regard to accuracy.The effect of phonological impairment on read<strong>in</strong>g speed <strong>in</strong> children withdyslexia is apparently evident even <strong>in</strong> highly consistent orthographies,however, poor word and non-word read<strong>in</strong>g fluency rema<strong>in</strong>s <strong>the</strong> primarybehavioural marker for dyslexia <strong>in</strong> more transparent orthographies. In asimilar ve<strong>in</strong>, Hanley et al. (2004) proved that read<strong>in</strong>g is very slow <strong>in</strong> poorWelsh readers. Ziegler et al. (2003) observed that German childrenrout<strong>in</strong>ely outperformed <strong>the</strong>ir English counterparts <strong>in</strong> terms of accuracyand fluency alike <strong>in</strong> both languages, while children with dyslexia did notdiffer <strong>in</strong> accuracy; <strong>the</strong>y were significantly poorer with respect to read<strong>in</strong>gspeed <strong>in</strong> comparison to read<strong>in</strong>g ability-matched controls.Opaque, deep, <strong>in</strong>consistent orthographies such as English or Frenchtend to pose much more pronounced problems on <strong>in</strong>dividuals withdyslexia than transparent languages such as Italian, Spanish or Greek(Goswami, 2000; Lundberg, 2002; Reid & Fawcett, 2004). Let us takeEnglish spell<strong>in</strong>g as an example: firstly, a s<strong>in</strong>gle sound may be writtendown with one letter or a comb<strong>in</strong>ation of letters, secondly, a s<strong>in</strong>gle soundmay be noted down with different letters or comb<strong>in</strong>ations of letters, and,f<strong>in</strong>ally, a given letter or a comb<strong>in</strong>ation of letters may represent more thanone sound, let alone irregular words (Payne, 1995). Thus, unsurpris<strong>in</strong>gly,while attempt<strong>in</strong>g to write a word <strong>in</strong> English, several potentially correctspell<strong>in</strong>g patterns for certa<strong>in</strong> phonemes may be taken <strong>in</strong>to consideration(e.g. ‘rane’ <strong>in</strong>stead of ‘ra<strong>in</strong>’) (Lundberg & Hoien, 2001). However, Kesslerand Treiman (2003) make a successful attempt to reject some misconceptionsabout <strong>the</strong> irregularity and chaotic nature of <strong>the</strong> English orthographicsystem, po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g to several useful patterns and pr<strong>in</strong>ciplescapable of reduc<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> complexity of <strong>the</strong> system.Davies et al. (2007) <strong>in</strong>vestigated read<strong>in</strong>g development and dyslexia <strong>in</strong>a relatively transparent Spanish orthography, <strong>the</strong>ir sample compris<strong>in</strong>g ofnormally develop<strong>in</strong>g (both chronological age-matched and abilitymatchedcontrols) and dyslexic children. Read<strong>in</strong>g accuracy was considerablyhigh for all children, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g dyslexics, which was ananticipated result given <strong>the</strong> orthographic consistency level of Spanish.Dyslexics and ability-matched controls committed significantly moreerrors than older age-matched controls, importantly though, <strong>the</strong> types oferrors (ma<strong>in</strong>ly substitutions, additions and deletions) were similar acrossgroups. With regard to speed, dyslexic and ability-matched controlgroups appeared to read slower than age-matched controls. Thesignificant modify<strong>in</strong>g effects of word frequency, length and orthographicneighbourhood size on read<strong>in</strong>g times were observed. Altoge<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong>


30 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>dicate delayed ra<strong>the</strong>r than abnormal read<strong>in</strong>g development <strong>in</strong>Spanish-speak<strong>in</strong>g dyslexic children.Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, Oren and Breznitz (2005) analysed read<strong>in</strong>g processes<strong>in</strong> Hebrew (L1) and English (L2) <strong>in</strong> dyslexics and chronological agematchedregular bil<strong>in</strong>gual readers. The behavioural and electrophysiologicalevidence collected <strong>in</strong> this study seems to support <strong>the</strong> propositionof a universal basis of dyslexia <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong>, however, at <strong>the</strong> same time<strong>the</strong> observed manifestations of dyslexia <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g activity turned outto be orthography-specific. Dyslexics performed consistently better <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong> shallow orthography of a po<strong>in</strong>ted Hebrew than <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> deep Englishorthographic system, where <strong>the</strong>ir difficulties were considerablyaggravated.In <strong>the</strong> study by Spencer and Hanley (2003), even <strong>the</strong> least-skilled,under-achiev<strong>in</strong>g Welsh young readers were anyth<strong>in</strong>g but close to <strong>the</strong>low-perform<strong>in</strong>g English readers, a f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dicative of less pronounceddyslexic difficulties present <strong>in</strong> children learn<strong>in</strong>g to read <strong>in</strong> Welsh than <strong>in</strong>English. The differences rema<strong>in</strong>ed valid three years later, when <strong>the</strong> samechildren were re-exam<strong>in</strong>ed by Hanley et al. (2004), suggest<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>long run <strong>the</strong> most harmful and damag<strong>in</strong>g effects of a deep orthographyon read<strong>in</strong>g development are evident <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> poorest readers.All <strong>in</strong> all, <strong>the</strong>re is a greater prom<strong>in</strong>ence of <strong>the</strong> causal relationshipbetween problems <strong>in</strong> word identification and deficits <strong>in</strong> phonologicalskills <strong>in</strong> dyslexics learn<strong>in</strong>g to read <strong>in</strong> opaque orthographies such asEnglish (Vellut<strong>in</strong>o et al., 2004). On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, <strong>the</strong> core phonologicaldeficit <strong>in</strong> dyslexics is harder to detect and not so persever<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> moretransparent orthographies with regular relationships between letters andsounds (Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2001a). Impaired fluency and speed <strong>in</strong> wordidentification and text process<strong>in</strong>g that lead to read<strong>in</strong>g comprehensiondifficulties are claimed to be <strong>the</strong> key markers for dyslexia <strong>in</strong> suchlanguages (Vellut<strong>in</strong>o et al., 2004). Several potential areas of difficulty forstudents with dyslexia might be enumerated even <strong>in</strong> highly transparentand regular orthographies. Hungarian, for example, may pose problemsconnected with visual process<strong>in</strong>g (diacritical marks) and with auditoryshort-term memory due to its agglut<strong>in</strong>ative nature. Theoretically, givenenough time, a child should be able to read a particular word bytranslat<strong>in</strong>g each of <strong>the</strong> graphemes <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong> correspond<strong>in</strong>g sounds.However, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> case of Hungarian, very slow speed of process<strong>in</strong>g,responsible for poor access to mean<strong>in</strong>g, seems to be a better predictor of aread<strong>in</strong>g failure (Smy<strong>the</strong> & Everatt, 2000).As mentioned above, orthographic features may be conducive todiverse occurrence and manifestations of dyslexia <strong>in</strong> different scripts.Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, Ch<strong>in</strong>ese children with dyslexia, apart from a visualorthographicdeficit, considered to be more specific to read<strong>in</strong>g problems<strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, encounter phonological process<strong>in</strong>g deficits similar to <strong>the</strong>ir


Becom<strong>in</strong>g Literate 31alphabetic counterparts. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, bear<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong> peculiarcharacteristics of <strong>the</strong> two scripts, Ch<strong>in</strong>ese children learn<strong>in</strong>g English asa second language (L2) demonstrate generally low-grade phonologicalprocess<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> both languages. What follows from <strong>the</strong>se facts is <strong>the</strong>presumed existence of multiple factors contribut<strong>in</strong>g to dyslexic difficulties<strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese children, additionally, both common and specific causesresponsible for read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese and English can beenumerated (Ho et al., 2000; Ho & Fong, 2005).Given <strong>the</strong> vital importance of phonemic awareness <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>gacquisition, ano<strong>the</strong>r crucial issue to be addressed is its cross-languagetransfer. Phonological process<strong>in</strong>g skills are consistently found to becritically related to word recognition faculty across languages. Thismeans that phonological process<strong>in</strong>g abilities <strong>in</strong> one language, no matterwhe<strong>the</strong>r first language (L1) or L2, can predict <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong>word recognition skills with<strong>in</strong> and cross-l<strong>in</strong>guistically, which ga<strong>in</strong>sspecial importance <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> context of assessment <strong>in</strong> multil<strong>in</strong>gual sett<strong>in</strong>gs(see Chapter 4) (Geva, 2000).Atwill et al. (2007) <strong>in</strong>vestigated <strong>the</strong> effect that L1 proficiency has oncross-language transfer of phonemic awareness <strong>in</strong> Spanish-speak<strong>in</strong>gchildren <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> USA, enter<strong>in</strong>g k<strong>in</strong>dergarten classrooms with <strong>in</strong>struction<strong>in</strong> English. They found that sufficient L1 skills determ<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong> successfultransfer of phonemic awareness from L1 to L2 and, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> long run, <strong>the</strong>L2 read<strong>in</strong>g ability <strong>in</strong> young children. In fact, children with below-averageL1 faculty showed no evidence of cross-language transfer of phonemicawareness from L1 to L2.Petrus and Bogdanowicz (2004) suggest that <strong>the</strong> simultaneousacquisition of both native (Polish) and foreign (English) language couldhave a supplementary effect on <strong>the</strong> development of particular skillsmak<strong>in</strong>g up phonological awareness, especially <strong>the</strong> awareness of onsetsand rimes. This assumption is based on <strong>the</strong> abovementioned claim that<strong>the</strong>re exists a relation between phonological competence <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> nativeand foreign language, more specifically, that <strong>the</strong>re occurs a positivetransfer of a well-developed phonological competence, at least asperta<strong>in</strong>s to <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial stage of its acquisition, from L1 to consecutivelanguages. It is concluded <strong>the</strong>n that s<strong>in</strong>ce phonological awareness isreflected <strong>in</strong> all languages acquired by a given child, one does not have todevelop it separately <strong>in</strong> each language. It is fur<strong>the</strong>r argued that <strong>the</strong><strong>in</strong>tervention programmes for children who, for various reasons, fail toproperly develop phonological skills, could be equally well provided <strong>in</strong> aforeign language, on condition that a child understands and uses itconfidently. The outcome of <strong>the</strong> research conducted on Polish k<strong>in</strong>dergartenchildren by Petrus and Bogdanowicz (2004) implies strongpositive correlation between <strong>the</strong> phonological skills measured <strong>in</strong> Polish(L1) and <strong>in</strong> English (L2). Additionally, <strong>the</strong> greater <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tensity of


32 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>learn<strong>in</strong>g a foreign language, <strong>the</strong> more pronounced <strong>the</strong> transfer ofphonological skills from Polish to English and vice versa.In sum, it appears that <strong>the</strong> phonological process<strong>in</strong>g impairmentsresponsible for <strong>the</strong> specific read<strong>in</strong>g disability <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> native language maysimilarly impede <strong>the</strong> acquisition of foreign languages. Core phonologicaldeficit <strong>in</strong> dyslexia is believed to be transferred from L1 to L2. This l<strong>in</strong>e ofexplanation provides for <strong>the</strong> difficulties experienced by many poorforeign language learners. Sparks (1995) enumerates several reasons whyphonological process<strong>in</strong>g may have an adverse effect on foreign languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g. He mentions <strong>the</strong> fact that a student may have difficultiesperceiv<strong>in</strong>g and produc<strong>in</strong>g novel phonological str<strong>in</strong>gs as well as comprehend<strong>in</strong>gspoken language. What is more, poor native language read<strong>in</strong>gskills will generalise to poor read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a foreign language, which <strong>in</strong> turnnegatively <strong>in</strong>fluences listen<strong>in</strong>g comprehension, oral expression, read<strong>in</strong>gcomprehension, syntax, general knowledge and verbal memory. Chapter3 is devoted to a more <strong>in</strong>-depth analysis of native language-based foreignlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties.Notes1. Various methodological designs can be applied <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> study of developmentalread<strong>in</strong>g disorders longitud<strong>in</strong>al versus cross-sectional, group versus casestudies. As for <strong>the</strong> choice of control groups, read<strong>in</strong>g ability/read<strong>in</strong>g-agematchedgroups (younger normally develop<strong>in</strong>g children matched for read<strong>in</strong>gability) or chronological age groups (typically develop<strong>in</strong>g children of <strong>the</strong>same age, usually matched for o<strong>the</strong>r variables such as gender, IQ, school) canbe selected for comparison with cl<strong>in</strong>ical cases.2. The average IQ for <strong>the</strong> population is 100, with a standard deviation of15 po<strong>in</strong>ts. In <strong>the</strong> UK, obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g IQ scores between 50 and 70 means hav<strong>in</strong>gmoderate learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties, people with IQ scores below 50 are believed tohave severe learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties (Hulme & Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2009).3. See Chapter 4 for a discussion on <strong>the</strong> dynamic change of symptoms <strong>in</strong>dyslexia.4. See Chapter 4: ‘Identification of dyslexia’, for a discussion of <strong>the</strong> usefulness ofread<strong>in</strong>g/IQ discrepancy for diagnosis.5. See Chapter 4: ‘Emotional-motivational disorders <strong>in</strong> dyslexia’, on emotionalmotivationalproblems <strong>in</strong> dyslexia.6. See Chapter 5 for a discussion of <strong>in</strong>tervention techniques (HSS and HAS)proposed by Bakker.


Chapter 2Causes of <strong>Dyslexia</strong>Introduction<strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong>volves an <strong>in</strong>terdiscipl<strong>in</strong>ary study and consensus ofneuroscience, cognitive science and learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ory, quite naturally<strong>in</strong>vit<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ir application <strong>in</strong> education. It seems to be a very <strong>in</strong>trigu<strong>in</strong>gand controversial phenomenon, widely <strong>in</strong>vestigated from many diversestandpo<strong>in</strong>ts. Over <strong>the</strong> past few decades, a considerable amount ofresearch has been devoted to identify<strong>in</strong>g its probable causes, withseveral important <strong>in</strong>sights from science pathways whose frameworkswere not extensively used before to substantiate <strong>the</strong> nature of dyslexia.Admittedly, <strong>the</strong> outstand<strong>in</strong>g progress <strong>in</strong> such scientific fields asneuroscience, bra<strong>in</strong> imag<strong>in</strong>g and genetics has confirmed several <strong>in</strong>tuitivelyplausible hypo<strong>the</strong>ses lack<strong>in</strong>g earlier empirical verification and hasrevealed multiple, previously unknown facts <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> complex,polietiological nature of dyslexia (Bogdanowicz & Adryjanek, 2004).<strong>Dyslexia</strong> may be caused by a number of factors operat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dependentlyor <strong>in</strong>teract<strong>in</strong>g with o<strong>the</strong>r factors to produce <strong>the</strong> outcome; moreover,various causes can be applicable to different children, and, last but notleast, <strong>the</strong>re may be several causes of dyslexic problems <strong>in</strong> place withrelation to a particular child. Hulme and Snowl<strong>in</strong>g (2009: 30) conv<strong>in</strong>c<strong>in</strong>glyargue that <strong>in</strong> light of current knowledge, causality and causesshould be treated <strong>in</strong> terms of probability ra<strong>the</strong>r than certa<strong>in</strong>ty ‘Causesare th<strong>in</strong>gs that <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>the</strong> likelihood of an outcome’.All <strong>in</strong> all, <strong>the</strong>re seems to be general agreement that dyslexia hasneurobiological orig<strong>in</strong>s with reference to genetic construction as well asstructural and functional features of <strong>the</strong> central nervous system (Knight &Hynd, 2008). These distal causes br<strong>in</strong>g about certa<strong>in</strong> malfunctions on <strong>the</strong>cognitive level, which, <strong>in</strong> turn, serve as more proximal causes of read<strong>in</strong>gfailure.Notwithstand<strong>in</strong>g certa<strong>in</strong> characteristics of difficulties encountered bychildren with dyslexia and manifest causes of <strong>the</strong>ir read<strong>in</strong>g disability,which are generally agreed upon (Velut<strong>in</strong>o et al., 2004), several conceptionsconcern<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>g causes of dyslexia are currently at large,<strong>in</strong>deed generat<strong>in</strong>g sizeable chaos <strong>in</strong> terms of available, at times<strong>in</strong>consistent and often contradict<strong>in</strong>g evidence and its critical evaluation.Given <strong>the</strong> complex nature of dyslexia, any decent attempt to understandits multiple facets would necessarily <strong>in</strong>volve a description and explanationwith regard to three levels: biological, cognitive and behavioural,with a range of environmental <strong>in</strong>fluences operat<strong>in</strong>g at each of <strong>the</strong>m33


34 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>(Frith, 1999, 2008; Morton & Frith, 1995). Explanation at <strong>the</strong> biologicallevel p<strong>in</strong>po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>g bra<strong>in</strong> mechanism, for example, disorganisation<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> cerebral cortex <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> language areas, abnormal magnocellularpathways or abnormal cerebellum. The cognitive level provides adescription as regards <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>oretical constructs from cognitive psychologysuch as reduced work<strong>in</strong>g memory, poor phonological process<strong>in</strong>g,<strong>in</strong>complete automatisation or slow central process<strong>in</strong>g. F<strong>in</strong>ally, <strong>the</strong>behavioural level refers to symptoms such as poor read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g,difficulty with rhymes, poor motion sensitivity, poor rapid auditoryprocess<strong>in</strong>g and difficulty ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g balance. No one level of explanationis assigned a more important role than <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r levels; all of <strong>the</strong>mare extremely useful <strong>in</strong> enhanc<strong>in</strong>g our understand<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> disorder.As for <strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>tical causal l<strong>in</strong>ks between <strong>the</strong> levels, <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicateddirection is from biological through cognitive 1 to behavioural level. Ino<strong>the</strong>r words, a genetic difference causes a bra<strong>in</strong> abnormality, which <strong>in</strong>turn is responsible for a cognitive deficit, which <strong>in</strong> turn br<strong>in</strong>gs aboutcerta<strong>in</strong> observed patterns of behaviour. As stressed by Hulme andSnowl<strong>in</strong>g (2009), none of <strong>the</strong> levels can be reduced or replaced withano<strong>the</strong>r level. In addition, <strong>the</strong>y extend <strong>the</strong> understand<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> directionof <strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>tical causal l<strong>in</strong>ks proposed <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> abovementioned causalmodel by postulat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> causal direction ‘backwards’ from behaviour,through cognition to biology. It is claimed that alterations at <strong>the</strong>behavioural level can <strong>in</strong>duce changes at <strong>the</strong> cognitive level, which <strong>in</strong>turn depend on <strong>the</strong> changes <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>g bra<strong>in</strong> mechanisms. It ispossible because experience is likely to modify connections betweennerve cells, which can result <strong>in</strong> long-last<strong>in</strong>g structural and functionalchanges. Even more surpris<strong>in</strong>gly, <strong>the</strong> genetic level is also likely to be<strong>in</strong>fluenced by changes at <strong>the</strong> cognitive and behavioural levels <strong>the</strong>re isevidence that <strong>the</strong> way genes are expressed can be altered by experience.Genes carry <strong>in</strong>formation that serves to direct development, but <strong>in</strong>fluencesfrom <strong>the</strong> environment <strong>in</strong> which <strong>the</strong> development takes place<strong>in</strong>teract with genetic <strong>in</strong>puts. The course of development, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>development of <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong>, is characterised by change and <strong>in</strong>teraction andis said to result from <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>terplay of genetic and environmental <strong>in</strong>puts.Hulme and Snowl<strong>in</strong>g (2009: 11) stress that: ‘learn<strong>in</strong>g (an <strong>in</strong>fluence from<strong>the</strong> environment) operates to modify structures <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> thatdeveloped under genetic control and <strong>in</strong> turn may <strong>in</strong>fluence subsequentlearn<strong>in</strong>g’. <strong>Dyslexia</strong> seems to be under <strong>the</strong> considerable <strong>in</strong>fluence ofgenetic risk factors whose activity is connected with alter<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>development of certa<strong>in</strong> language systems of <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere of <strong>the</strong>bra<strong>in</strong>. However, <strong>the</strong> impact of <strong>the</strong> environment, <strong>in</strong> which children learnto read, on dyslexia cannot be underestimated.Contemporary <strong>the</strong>ories of dyslexia may be applied at any of <strong>the</strong> levels:bra<strong>in</strong> based, cognitive or behavioural, however, a complete explanation


Causes of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 35of <strong>the</strong> dyslexia phenomenon would require form<strong>in</strong>g a complex modelaptly represent<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> relations and <strong>the</strong> causal l<strong>in</strong>ks between <strong>the</strong>seseparate levels of <strong>in</strong>vestigation. In addition, Hulme and Snowl<strong>in</strong>g (2009)propose that research<strong>in</strong>g atypical read<strong>in</strong>g development <strong>in</strong> childrenshould necessarily <strong>in</strong>volve compar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m to patterns of typical read<strong>in</strong>gdevelopment <strong>in</strong> children. Thus, a <strong>the</strong>ory of developmental dyslexiawould have to del<strong>in</strong>eate how and why children with dyslexia demonstrateimpairment <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> processes that are normally <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> typicalread<strong>in</strong>g development. Deep and thorough understand<strong>in</strong>g of normalread<strong>in</strong>g development constitutes a necessary prerequisite for successfuluncover<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> complex nature of this developmental read<strong>in</strong>gdisorder.The follow<strong>in</strong>g part of <strong>the</strong> chapter is devoted to neurobiologicalsubstrates (genetic and bra<strong>in</strong> mechanisms) and cognitive correlates ofdyslexia. It focuses on familiaris<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> reader with <strong>the</strong> major facts andhypo<strong>the</strong>sis brought to light by extensive <strong>in</strong>vestigations of <strong>the</strong> causes ofdyslexia.Genetic Mechanisms <strong>in</strong> <strong>Dyslexia</strong>Genetic <strong>the</strong>ory is one of <strong>the</strong> oldest attempts to expla<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> causes ofdyslexia. It presupposes <strong>the</strong> connection between <strong>the</strong> occurrence of <strong>the</strong>disorder and <strong>in</strong>herited anatomical and functional features of <strong>the</strong> centralnervous system, determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> existence of difficulties <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g andspell<strong>in</strong>g. A tendency for <strong>the</strong> specific read<strong>in</strong>g disorder to run <strong>in</strong> familieswas first reported at <strong>the</strong> turn of <strong>the</strong> last century (Bogdanowicz, 1989;Spionek, 1985); s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>the</strong>n, converg<strong>in</strong>g evidence has accumulated <strong>in</strong>support of substantial heritability <strong>in</strong> dyslexia (Mar<strong>in</strong>o et al., 2007). Thegenetic basis of dyslexia is verified <strong>in</strong> population genetic studies, which<strong>in</strong>vestigate patterns of <strong>in</strong>heritance across <strong>in</strong>dividuals, and <strong>in</strong> moleculargenetic studies, which attempt to p<strong>in</strong>po<strong>in</strong>t particular genes or genemarkers that can be l<strong>in</strong>ked to dyslexia.Conv<strong>in</strong>c<strong>in</strong>g evidence for <strong>the</strong> hereditary hypo<strong>the</strong>sis genetic componentto dyslexia comes from family and tw<strong>in</strong> studies (DeFries et al.,1987; Jaklewicz, 1982; Olson et al., 1989; Penn<strong>in</strong>gton, 1989; Penn<strong>in</strong>gtonet al., 1986; Penn<strong>in</strong>gton & Smith, 1988; Ramus, 2006; Ste<strong>in</strong>, 2001). It hasbeen speculated that about 2030% of <strong>the</strong> cases of dyslexia aregenetically conditioned (Bodganowicz, 1999; DeFries et al., 1987; Penn<strong>in</strong>gtonet al., 1986); however, some authors claim it might be even 5060% (Lundberg & Hoien, 2001; Ste<strong>in</strong>, 2001; Ste<strong>in</strong> et al., 2001). Putt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>differences <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> estimates aside, what follows is that a child of adyslexic parent can <strong>in</strong>deed be at considerable risk to develop read<strong>in</strong>gproblems. In family studies, children at genetic risk for dyslexia arefollowed from pre-school age, with <strong>the</strong>ir development and progress


36 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>be<strong>in</strong>g documented (e.g. Snowl<strong>in</strong>g et al., 2003). Snowl<strong>in</strong>g et al. (2007)confirm <strong>in</strong>creased risk of dyslexia among adolescent children of dyslexicparents, who were exam<strong>in</strong>ed when <strong>the</strong>y were 3 years and 9 months, <strong>the</strong>nat <strong>the</strong> age of 6 and <strong>the</strong>n 8. Their literacy difficulties prevail ra<strong>the</strong>r thanresolve, <strong>the</strong>y are long-last<strong>in</strong>g, with no apparent catch-up between 8 and13 years of age. Still, <strong>the</strong> genetic risk for dyslexia is not perceived asdeterm<strong>in</strong>istic <strong>in</strong> character and <strong>the</strong> correlation of genes and environmentis highlighted <strong>in</strong>stead.In all likelihood, <strong>in</strong>sufficient phoneme segmentation skills andphonological/orthographic awareness deficits qualify for explanationwith regard to genetic <strong>in</strong>fluences (Schneider, 1999). Accord<strong>in</strong>g toSnowl<strong>in</strong>g (2001a), a low-grade facility to establish relations betweenorthography and phonology may be an <strong>in</strong>herited feature of dyslexia. Asargued fur<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong> severity of <strong>the</strong> specific read<strong>in</strong>g disorder encounteredby children at risk for dyslexia appears to be directly proportional to <strong>the</strong>severity of <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g impairment of <strong>the</strong>ir parents. Lundberg and Hoien(2001) also po<strong>in</strong>t out that children genetically disposed to encounterdyslexic difficulties demonstrate a delay or a deficit primarily <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>irphonological development; almost <strong>in</strong>variably lack<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> clear-cutmental representations of <strong>the</strong> sounds of words. Olson et al. (1989) haveput forward much <strong>the</strong> same ideas, namely, that word recognition andphonological decod<strong>in</strong>g skills are strongly <strong>in</strong>fluenced genetically; additionally,<strong>the</strong>y have <strong>in</strong>dicated lower hereditability for <strong>the</strong> orthographiccod<strong>in</strong>g skills. Ste<strong>in</strong> (2001) claims that even though <strong>in</strong>itially only <strong>the</strong>phonological ability was assigned a hereditary characteristic, it appearsthat also <strong>the</strong> orthographic ability can be transmissible. 2Admittedly though, it is unlikely that dyslexia is caused by a s<strong>in</strong>glegene, but ra<strong>the</strong>r through <strong>the</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ation and <strong>in</strong>teraction of multiplegenes (Nicolson, 2001; Ramus, 2006). Molecular genetics studies aim atidentify<strong>in</strong>g certa<strong>in</strong> genes (DNA sequences) or gene markers that can beassigned <strong>the</strong> responsibility for <strong>the</strong> development of <strong>the</strong> disorder. It is mostlikely that it is <strong>the</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ation of <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluence of many genes of smalleffect and <strong>the</strong> environmental <strong>in</strong>put that <strong>the</strong> risk of <strong>in</strong>herit<strong>in</strong>g dyslexiadepends on (Hulme & Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2009). Indeed, <strong>the</strong>re is no <strong>in</strong>dication of<strong>the</strong> existence of specific genes associated with dyslexia (Vellut<strong>in</strong>o et al.,2004). Even though no s<strong>in</strong>gle gene for dyslexia has been identified, <strong>the</strong>approximate positions of <strong>the</strong> number of gene loci have been mapped(Fisher & Smith, 2001). Chromosomal regions implicated by a number ofstudies as be<strong>in</strong>g likely to conta<strong>in</strong> genes predispos<strong>in</strong>g for dyslexia <strong>in</strong>volve<strong>the</strong> regions on chromosomes 1, 2, 3, 6, 15 and 18 (Fisher & Francks, 2006;Fisher & Smith, 2001; Ste<strong>in</strong> et al., 2001). Four candidate dyslexiasusceptibility genes that are <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> developmental processes<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g neuronal migration have been identified (Galaburda et al.,2006; Ramus, 2006).


Causes of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 37Notwithstand<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> utmost difficulty and complexity of <strong>the</strong> task,establish<strong>in</strong>g transparent connections between genes and behaviour aswell as genetic mutations and behavioural disorders seems crucial. In<strong>the</strong>ir research, Galaburda et al. (2006) <strong>in</strong>dicate a speculative routebetween genetic effect, developmental bra<strong>in</strong> changes and perceptualand cognitive impairments characteris<strong>in</strong>g dyslexia. Similarly, accord<strong>in</strong>gto Fisher and Smith (2001), <strong>the</strong> isolation of gene variants that mightpossibly be responsible for dyslexia rema<strong>in</strong>s a major challenge forresearch, hav<strong>in</strong>g great potential benefits for early diagnosis of <strong>in</strong>dividualsat risk for dyslexia.In sum, dyslexia tends to run <strong>in</strong> families, thus a child of a dyslexicparent naturally falls <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong> category of children at risk for dyslexia,who are prone to develop dyslexic difficulties. The awareness of a familyhistory of a struggle with literacy acquisition may well serve as an<strong>in</strong>dicator of a necessity of early assessment, followed by appropriate<strong>in</strong>tervention with regard to pre-literacy skills, hopefully reduc<strong>in</strong>g or,preferably, prevent<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> experience of scholastic failure.Bra<strong>in</strong> Mechanisms <strong>in</strong> <strong>Dyslexia</strong>Mov<strong>in</strong>g on from <strong>the</strong> heritability aspect <strong>in</strong> dyslexia research to <strong>the</strong>analysis of bra<strong>in</strong> structure and function, let us concentrate on bra<strong>in</strong>abnormalities <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia. S<strong>in</strong>ce a major difficulty thatchildren with dyslexia experience is poor phonological process<strong>in</strong>g, onewould expect certa<strong>in</strong> impairments <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> structure and/or function of <strong>the</strong>bra<strong>in</strong> areas that are assigned <strong>the</strong> task of language process<strong>in</strong>g. Indeed,<strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia are frequently reported to demonstrate dist<strong>in</strong>ctcharacteristics of bra<strong>in</strong> anatomy, bra<strong>in</strong> activity and function from nondyslexic<strong>in</strong>dividuals. Moreover, <strong>the</strong> abovementioned differences aretentatively assigned responsibility for <strong>the</strong> occurrence of specific learn<strong>in</strong>gdifficulties. M<strong>in</strong>imal bra<strong>in</strong> damage (MBD) or alterations <strong>in</strong> bra<strong>in</strong>structure (which <strong>in</strong>vite malfunction) to <strong>the</strong>se areas of <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> that aretypically allocated <strong>the</strong> role of mediat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> processes of read<strong>in</strong>g andspell<strong>in</strong>g may constitute <strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>g cause of dyslexia. Structuralanomalies would rout<strong>in</strong>ely occur due to damag<strong>in</strong>g activity of variouspathogenic factors on <strong>the</strong> central nervous system <strong>in</strong> prenatal andper<strong>in</strong>atal life. However, as rightly highlighted by Hulme and Snowl<strong>in</strong>g(2009), despite converg<strong>in</strong>g evidence with regard to anomalous function<strong>in</strong>gof <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere language areas <strong>in</strong> dyslexia, it would bepremature to evaluate <strong>the</strong> causal status of <strong>the</strong>se f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. Possibly,reduced read<strong>in</strong>g experience <strong>in</strong> people with dyslexia becomes <strong>in</strong> itself anenvironmental factor potentially capable of exert<strong>in</strong>g certa<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluences on<strong>the</strong> structure and function of bra<strong>in</strong> circuits <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g. Viewedfrom such a perspective, differences <strong>in</strong> patterns of bra<strong>in</strong> activation


38 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>observed <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> performance ofphonological and read<strong>in</strong>g tasks can be treated as a consequence ra<strong>the</strong>rthan a cause of read<strong>in</strong>g problems.As far as <strong>the</strong> measurement of bra<strong>in</strong> function, operationalised as bra<strong>in</strong>responses to cognitive tasks, is concerned, functional neuroimag<strong>in</strong>g istypically utilised <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> current studies. Sophisticated techniques 3 allowmeasur<strong>in</strong>g and mapp<strong>in</strong>g of changes and patterns <strong>in</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> activation,<strong>in</strong>duced by <strong>the</strong> performance on particular cognitive tasks <strong>in</strong> terms ofplace and/or time. 4 Importantly, <strong>the</strong> outcome of studies us<strong>in</strong>g vary<strong>in</strong>gmethods seem to provide ra<strong>the</strong>r consistent f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs as to ascerta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gparticular areas of <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> that show habitually <strong>in</strong>creased activationdur<strong>in</strong>g tasks <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g phonological process<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g. There existdifferences <strong>in</strong> activation patterns of neural circuits dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>se tasks <strong>in</strong>dyslexic bra<strong>in</strong>s as compared to non-dyslexic bra<strong>in</strong>s. Generally, dyslexicstend to under-activate vital structures of <strong>the</strong> neural network; <strong>the</strong>connections between <strong>the</strong>se structures are also fewer than <strong>in</strong> non-dyslexicbra<strong>in</strong>s.Mar<strong>in</strong>o et al. (2007) po<strong>in</strong>t to an <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g aspect of dyslexia, namely,neurofunctional organisation pattern, be<strong>in</strong>g representative of differentwrit<strong>in</strong>g systems. Aga<strong>in</strong>, evidence seems <strong>in</strong>conclusive; data collected <strong>in</strong>some neuroimag<strong>in</strong>g studies support <strong>the</strong> existence of unitary mechanismswith regard to alphabetic scripts, irrespective of <strong>the</strong> orthographic depth.Conversely, o<strong>the</strong>r researchers favour <strong>the</strong> idea that logographic andalphabetic writ<strong>in</strong>g systems lead to diversified patterns of neurofunctionalorganisation.Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Pugh et al. (2005), generally, fundamental pr<strong>in</strong>ciples ofspeech and read<strong>in</strong>g organisation <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> seem to chiefly parallelacross languages. Despite broad consistency and uniformity <strong>in</strong> neurobiologicalfoundations, <strong>the</strong> existence of certa<strong>in</strong> language-specific featuresis not denied. Particular left hemisphere cortical regions, such as <strong>the</strong>occipitotemporal, temporoparietal and <strong>in</strong>ferior frontal networks, arerepeatedly <strong>in</strong>dicated by <strong>the</strong> outcome of neuroimag<strong>in</strong>g studies toparticipate <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g across alphabetic languages, with specific differencesbetween languages, concern<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> degree ra<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>the</strong> k<strong>in</strong>d. 5The same tendency holds true for read<strong>in</strong>g disability, as demonstrated by<strong>the</strong> results of neuroimag<strong>in</strong>g studies on read<strong>in</strong>g tasks performed by poorreaders. Ma<strong>in</strong>ly common neurobiological features, such as functionalabnormality <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere posterior cortex, are displayed acrosslanguages (English, F<strong>in</strong>nish, German, French, Italian) with regard toread<strong>in</strong>g impairment.However, Hadzibeganovic et al. (<strong>in</strong> press) believe that patterns ofactivation and neural circuits participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>gdisorders potentially vary across languages, accord<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> way writtenlanguage maps on spoken language (orthographic depth). This view is


Causes of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 39also supported by cognitive heterogeneity of dyslexia, evident <strong>in</strong> itsvary<strong>in</strong>g subtypes across cultures, which cannot possibly be trackeddown to <strong>the</strong> neural impairment of <strong>the</strong> same type. Hence <strong>the</strong> existence of acommon type of bra<strong>in</strong> abnormality <strong>in</strong> dyslexia across cultures andlanguages is perceived as oversimplification. In addition, Siok et al.(2008) provide <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g data from Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, a non-alphabetic language.They report vary<strong>in</strong>g bra<strong>in</strong> anomalies <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese dyslexic readers who,unlike alphabetic-language dyslexics, do not demonstrate functional orstructural 6 differences from regular readers <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> more posterior bra<strong>in</strong>systems. Thus, a structural and functional basis for dyslexia <strong>in</strong> alphabeticand non-alphabetic languages varies.Importantly, f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of a number of imag<strong>in</strong>g studies analys<strong>in</strong>g bra<strong>in</strong>activation patterns <strong>in</strong> English monol<strong>in</strong>gual children with dyslexia beforeand after <strong>in</strong>tervention, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> form of remedial read<strong>in</strong>g treatment orphonologically driven, <strong>in</strong>dicate <strong>the</strong> apparent <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> activation ofareas typically employed by good readers. This suggests that <strong>in</strong>structionplays a vital role <strong>in</strong> creat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> activation patterns allow<strong>in</strong>gsuccessful skilled read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> dyslexics (e.g. Blachman et al., 2004;Richards et al., 2000; Shaywitz et al., 2004). For <strong>in</strong>stance, Aylward et al.(2003) demonstrate that 28 hours of comprehensive read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>structionresults <strong>in</strong> both behavioural ga<strong>in</strong>s <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> form of improved read<strong>in</strong>g scoresand a change <strong>in</strong> dyslexic bra<strong>in</strong> function. Namely, <strong>in</strong>creased bra<strong>in</strong>activation dur<strong>in</strong>g language tasks parallel characteristic patterns of neuralprocess<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> non-disabled readers. Similarly, Simos et al. (2002) reportthat dyslexia-specific bra<strong>in</strong> activation maps become normal follow<strong>in</strong>gsuccessful eight-week <strong>in</strong>tensive remedial tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, which results <strong>in</strong>dyslexics <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ir word read<strong>in</strong>g ga<strong>in</strong>s up to average range. The<strong>in</strong>creased activation, resembl<strong>in</strong>g patterns usually demonstrated by highachiev<strong>in</strong>greaders, is observed <strong>in</strong> dyslexic left temporoparietal areas,which equals significant improvement <strong>in</strong> word read<strong>in</strong>g accuracy.One would wonder whe<strong>the</strong>r children learn<strong>in</strong>g English as a second/additional language (ESL/EAL) demonstrate comparable neurobiologicalcharacteristics to monol<strong>in</strong>gual English-speak<strong>in</strong>g children with regardto read<strong>in</strong>g acquisition and disability? In fact, Pugh et al. (2005) conclude<strong>the</strong>ir literature review with <strong>the</strong> claim that pronounced differences <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>way bra<strong>in</strong> functions between ESL/EAL 7 learners and monol<strong>in</strong>gualchildren learn<strong>in</strong>g to read <strong>in</strong> English are ra<strong>the</strong>r unlikely to emerge, <strong>the</strong>differences though may be anticipated <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> rate of acquisition. What ismore, <strong>the</strong>y stress <strong>the</strong> potential of functional neuroimag<strong>in</strong>g techniques <strong>in</strong>verify<strong>in</strong>g and evaluat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> effects of vary<strong>in</strong>g approaches to teach<strong>in</strong>gread<strong>in</strong>g to ESL/EAL learners.Post-mortem studies as well as non-<strong>in</strong>vasive anatomical magneticresonance imag<strong>in</strong>g (aMFI) are <strong>the</strong> most commonly utilised techniques <strong>in</strong>research on bra<strong>in</strong> structure. A small number of post-mortem analyses of


40 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>dyslexic bra<strong>in</strong>s, amount<strong>in</strong>g to a total of 10 cases (Vellut<strong>in</strong>o et al., 2004),notwithstand<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>in</strong>structive force is considerable <strong>in</strong> that <strong>the</strong>y<strong>in</strong>dicate a complex, and by no means reduced to a s<strong>in</strong>gle bra<strong>in</strong> area,neuroanatomical basis of dyslexia. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, methods based on<strong>the</strong> aMFI make possible <strong>the</strong> precise evaluation of bra<strong>in</strong> structures and <strong>in</strong>that way exert substantial <strong>in</strong>fluence on <strong>the</strong> processes of search<strong>in</strong>g for andestablish<strong>in</strong>g neural correlates of dyslexia.Mov<strong>in</strong>g on to a more detailed discussion of <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> regionscharacterised by some structural anomalies <strong>in</strong> dyslexics as compared tonon-dyslexics, let us beg<strong>in</strong> with <strong>the</strong> planum temporale. 8 Areas <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>upper posterior part of <strong>the</strong> left temporal lobe, <strong>in</strong> particular <strong>the</strong> planumtemporale, are claimed to be critically <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> phonological process<strong>in</strong>g.Naturally, attempts to expla<strong>in</strong> impairments <strong>in</strong> phonologicalprocess<strong>in</strong>g concentrated on abnormal or non-existent bra<strong>in</strong> laterality <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong>se areas as well as on <strong>the</strong> structural anomaly of <strong>the</strong> planum temporale(Hugdahl et al., 2003). Several researchers <strong>in</strong>vestigated reduced orreversed leftward asymmetry of <strong>the</strong> planum temporale, however, <strong>the</strong>results <strong>the</strong>y obta<strong>in</strong>ed varied.Post-mortem analyses of dyslexic bra<strong>in</strong>s have demonstrated substantialevidence for <strong>the</strong> lack of typical structural asymmetry as regards<strong>the</strong> planum temporale 9 (Galaburda et al., 1978; Galaburda, 1985, 1993).In addition, several magnetic resonance (MR) imag<strong>in</strong>g studies of <strong>the</strong>bra<strong>in</strong> structure <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia have <strong>in</strong>dicated reduced orreversed asymmetry of <strong>the</strong> language areas <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> temporal lobe. Still, thisview seems to be challenged by more recent studies of <strong>the</strong> planumtemporale. The abovementioned discrepancies <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> results regard<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong> hemispheric asymmetry of temporal regions <strong>in</strong> dyslexia might havebeen brought about by <strong>the</strong> methodological differences between <strong>the</strong>studies. For example, Heiervang et al. (2000) hypo<strong>the</strong>sised that reducedasymmetry of <strong>the</strong> planum temporale and/or smaller left planumtemporale <strong>in</strong> children with dyslexia would be connected to reducedright ear advantage (REA) 10 on <strong>the</strong> dichotic listen<strong>in</strong>g task. 11 The studyaddressed an apparently crucial issue whe<strong>the</strong>r deviant structure of <strong>the</strong>bra<strong>in</strong> areas, which are engaged <strong>in</strong> speech sound perception andprocess<strong>in</strong>g, equals functional deficit. However, both normal structuralasymmetry, assessed by <strong>the</strong> size of <strong>the</strong> planum temporale, and functionalasymmetry, <strong>in</strong>dicated by <strong>the</strong> dichotic listen<strong>in</strong>g ear advantage, wasdemonstrated <strong>in</strong> dyslexic subjects participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> study. Nosignificant differences were spotted <strong>in</strong> planum temporale asymmetrybetween <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia and controls; both groups demonstrateda mean leftward asymmetry of <strong>the</strong> area <strong>in</strong> question. Moreover,dyslexics and controls showed normal REA on <strong>the</strong> dichotic listen<strong>in</strong>g task.However, a small (10%) reduction of <strong>the</strong> planum temporale size wassignified <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia, while it was apparently absent <strong>in</strong>


Causes of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 41controls. Additionally, planum temporale asymmetry and read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dyslexic group were not correlated, while a positive correlation between<strong>the</strong>se two variables was shown <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> control group, thus it would seemthat leftward asymmetry as regards <strong>the</strong> planum temporale may beconnected with normal read<strong>in</strong>g but not with impaired read<strong>in</strong>g. F<strong>in</strong>ally,deviant shape of <strong>the</strong> planar area was demonstrated <strong>in</strong> dyslexics. Thus,altoge<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> study has shown that subtle developmental abnormalitiesmay <strong>in</strong>deed be associated with dyslexia. Ano<strong>the</strong>r study conducted by <strong>the</strong>same group of researchers (Hugdahl et al., 2003) provided support for<strong>the</strong>ir previous f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. 12Leonard et al. (2006) perceive <strong>the</strong> view that children with dyslexia arecharacterised by symmetrical planum temporale as a long-last<strong>in</strong>gmisconception. They fur<strong>the</strong>r report that f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir sample runcounter to <strong>the</strong> idea of <strong>the</strong> existence of an association between planumtemporale asymmetry and several read<strong>in</strong>g and language variables thatwere previously <strong>in</strong>dicated <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r studies.A different set of f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs refers to anomalies of cell migration(ectopias and dysplasias 13 ) reported to exist <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> perisylvian cortex ofdyslexic bra<strong>in</strong>s, pr<strong>in</strong>cipally <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere (Ramus, 2004).Ectopias and dysplasias are claimed to result from focal lesions to <strong>the</strong>matur<strong>in</strong>g bra<strong>in</strong>, which take place dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> migration of neurons to <strong>the</strong>cortex at about <strong>the</strong> fifth month of fetal life (Borkowska, 1997; Ste<strong>in</strong>, 2001).Yet ano<strong>the</strong>r anatomical difference concerns <strong>the</strong> corpus callosum. 14 Ithas been <strong>in</strong>dicated that <strong>the</strong> corpus callosum and, <strong>in</strong> particular, its backpart, called <strong>the</strong> splenium, which connects <strong>the</strong> parts realis<strong>in</strong>g languagefunctions, are considerably larger <strong>in</strong> dyslexics (Bloom & Hynd, 2005;Galaburda, 1993; Preis et al., 2000). Plessen et al. (2002) have observed ashorter corpus callosum shape <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> posterior midbody region <strong>in</strong>dyslexic bra<strong>in</strong>s as compared to control subjects. As <strong>in</strong>dicated by Bloomand Hynd (2005), most studies report certa<strong>in</strong> abnormalities <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> size of<strong>the</strong> corpus callosum, which are probably directly connected to reducedcortical asymmetry. However, <strong>the</strong> evidence regard<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> size of <strong>the</strong>corpus callosum <strong>in</strong> developmental dyslexia is conflict<strong>in</strong>g: both smallerand larger corpus callosi have been found <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia.Thus, tak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>to account <strong>the</strong> anatomical data, it might be hypo<strong>the</strong>sised(Bednarek, 1999; Ste<strong>in</strong>, 2001) that dyslexia is connected with <strong>the</strong> failure toestablish hemispheric specialisation. However, it is difficult to def<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong>activity of <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> organised <strong>in</strong> that particular way and to qualify <strong>the</strong><strong>in</strong>fluence it has on <strong>the</strong> process of read<strong>in</strong>g (Bednarek, 1999).The most direct evidence that many dyslexics may have impaireddevelopment of <strong>the</strong> visual magnocellular system 15 comes from postmortemexam<strong>in</strong>ation of dyslexic bra<strong>in</strong>s. This f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g has been fur<strong>the</strong>rconfirmed by electrophysiological studies and functional imag<strong>in</strong>gstudies (fMRI). It has been found that <strong>the</strong> magnocellular layers of <strong>the</strong>


42 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) of <strong>the</strong> thalamus were disordered and<strong>the</strong> neurons were about 30% smaller <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> area as compared to nondyslexicbra<strong>in</strong>s. These abnormalities are known to arise dur<strong>in</strong>g earlydevelopment of <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong>, as is <strong>the</strong> case with ectopias, dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> phaseof rapid neuronal growth and migration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> fourth or fifth month offetal life (Galaburda & Liv<strong>in</strong>gstone, 1993; Ramus, 2004; Ste<strong>in</strong>, 2001).Fawcett and Nicolson (2001, 2004) believe that problems suffered bypeople with dyslexia may come down to cerebellar 16 deficit. 17 Indeed,certa<strong>in</strong> structural abnormalities have been reported <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> cerebella ofdyslexic bra<strong>in</strong>s. The abnormality is characterised by greater cell size(Fawcett & Nicolson, 2004). In addition, relatively more large neuronsand fewer small neurons were found <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> cerebella of dyslexic bra<strong>in</strong>s,which might suggest problems <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>put to <strong>the</strong> cerebellum (Fawcett &Nicolson, 2001). Also, a morphological post-mortem anatomical analysishas s<strong>in</strong>gled out a significantly larger mean cellular area <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> medialposterior and <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> anterior lobe of <strong>the</strong> cerebellar cortex <strong>in</strong> dyslexicbra<strong>in</strong>s (F<strong>in</strong>ch et al., 2002). Moreover, <strong>the</strong> cerebellar hemispheres <strong>in</strong>dyslexic bra<strong>in</strong>s have been found to be symmetrical, while non-dyslexiccontrols were reported to possess a larger right hemispheric cerebellarcortical surface (Rae et al., 2002).As far as bra<strong>in</strong> activation is concerned, it was monitored <strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia and control adults dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> performanceof a pre-learned sequence and dur<strong>in</strong>g learn<strong>in</strong>g a novel sequence of f<strong>in</strong>gerpresses, known to result <strong>in</strong> substantial activation <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> cerebellum withnon-dyslexic adults (Nicolson et al., 1999). As predicted by <strong>the</strong> cerebellardeficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis, <strong>the</strong> dyslexic group activated <strong>the</strong>ir cerebella lessdur<strong>in</strong>g motor learn<strong>in</strong>g as far as pre-learned (automatic) and novelsequences were concerned. Moreover, greater activation <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir frontallobes was shown <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g a novel sequence, suggest<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>y werebypass<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> cerebellum to some extent and rely<strong>in</strong>g on consciousstrategies (Fawcett & Nicolson, 2001).Additionally, a normally greater density of white matter <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> lef<strong>the</strong>misphere tends to be decreased <strong>in</strong> people with dyslexia (Kl<strong>in</strong>gberget al., 2000). Studies also report reduced grey matter <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> orbital portionof <strong>the</strong> left <strong>in</strong>ferior frontal gyrus and superior temporal gyrus, but alsooutside language regions (Brown et al., 2001). Silani et al. (2005) providesupport for <strong>the</strong> claim that changed activation noted with<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>gsystem is associated with altered density of grey and white matter ofcerta<strong>in</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> regions, such as left middle and <strong>in</strong>ferior temporal gyri and<strong>the</strong> left arcuate fasciculus. They fur<strong>the</strong>r voice an op<strong>in</strong>ion that <strong>the</strong>irevidence is <strong>in</strong> accord with <strong>the</strong> view that dyslexia is associated with localgrey matter dysfunction and altered connectivity among phonological/read<strong>in</strong>g areas.


Causes of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 43Also, Temple (2002) supports <strong>the</strong> claim of <strong>the</strong> neurobiologicalaetiology of dyslexia, po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g out that <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia,regardless of age, language and methodology adopted <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> study,demonstrate disruptions <strong>in</strong> temporoparietal bra<strong>in</strong> responses as regardsphonological demands and <strong>in</strong> left frontal bra<strong>in</strong> responses to rapidauditory process<strong>in</strong>g tasks. In addition, <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia showunusual organisation of white matter connect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> temporoparietalcortex to o<strong>the</strong>r cortical areas, and <strong>the</strong> degree of this faulty organisationcorrelated with read<strong>in</strong>g ability, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> important role it plays<strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g. However, apparently, more attention should be devoted to<strong>the</strong> issue of causality and determ<strong>in</strong>ation of <strong>the</strong> relationship between<strong>the</strong> abovementioned disruptions <strong>in</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> structure and function and <strong>the</strong>aetiology of developmental dyslexia.The f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of <strong>the</strong> post-mortem exam<strong>in</strong>ation and bra<strong>in</strong> imag<strong>in</strong>ganalyses of dyslexic bra<strong>in</strong>s <strong>in</strong>dicate <strong>the</strong> complexity of <strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>gneuroanatomical basis of dyslexia that cannot be reduced to a s<strong>in</strong>glearea of <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> (Galaburda et al., 2006). Leonard et al. (2006) highlight<strong>the</strong> fact that recent studies have revealed a puzzl<strong>in</strong>g range of anatomicaldifferences between dyslexics and controls. The neuroanatomical substratesof dyslexia that are po<strong>in</strong>ted out <strong>in</strong>clude: reduction <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>temporal lobe, frontal lobe, candate, thalamus and cerebellum, <strong>in</strong>sula,anterior superior neocortex and occipital cortex, additionally, subtlechanges <strong>in</strong> callosal morphology, <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>ferior frontal gyrus and cerebellumhave been observed. Similarly, Ramus (2004) enumerates severaldifferences between dyslexic and control bra<strong>in</strong>s that have beendocumented by recent studies, for example, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> left perisylviancortex, <strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>g white matter, <strong>the</strong> thalamus, <strong>the</strong> corpus callosumand <strong>the</strong> cerebellum. However, it is stressed aga<strong>in</strong> that <strong>the</strong> functionalsignificance of <strong>the</strong> abovementioned bra<strong>in</strong> differences requires decentexplication.The outcome of <strong>the</strong> neuroanatomical research, even though at timescontradictory and hence <strong>in</strong>conclusive, mostly due to methodologicaldifferences, seems to suggest that <strong>in</strong>deed <strong>the</strong>re exist an anatomicalphysiologicalbasis for dyslexia.Phonological Cod<strong>in</strong>g Deficit Hypo<strong>the</strong>sisPhonological abilitiesPhonological process<strong>in</strong>g (implicit phonology) refers to children us<strong>in</strong>gspeech, without reflect<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong> structure of spoken words. Nam<strong>in</strong>g andshort-term verbal memory (STM) tasks as well as repeat<strong>in</strong>g words ornon-words are frequently used <strong>in</strong> studies on phonological process<strong>in</strong>g.Children with dyslexia repeatedly f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong>se simple tasks troublesome(Hulme & Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2009). Phonological awareness (explicit phonology) is


44 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong><strong>the</strong> ability to perform explicit judgements with regard to <strong>the</strong> structure ofspoken words and it refers to all k<strong>in</strong>ds of operations on speech sounds,engag<strong>in</strong>g memory, analysis and syn<strong>the</strong>sis of phonological elements. It isbasically def<strong>in</strong>ed as an ability to identify, dist<strong>in</strong>guish between, detect andmanipulate <strong>the</strong> sound structure of words, stretch<strong>in</strong>g from youngchildren’s sensitivity to that structure (separate from word mean<strong>in</strong>g) to<strong>the</strong> skilful handl<strong>in</strong>g of manipulation tasks, with regard to constituentphonemes <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> speech stream. It is knowledge that spoken words aremade of t<strong>in</strong>y segments sounds; it is an ability to break apart and puttoge<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>se sounds. This facility, <strong>in</strong> turn, forms a prerequisite forlater successful mapp<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> sounds on <strong>the</strong> appropriate symbols letters. Phonemic awareness is a type of phonological awareness. While<strong>the</strong> latter deals with various sizes of phonological elements (words,syllables, onset, rimes, phonemes), <strong>the</strong> former is reduced <strong>in</strong> scope andrelated to identification and manipulation of <strong>in</strong>dividual phonemes(Krasowicz-Kupis, 2008).The knowledge of and sensitivity to constituent sound parts ofwords aids understand<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> written language system and acquisitionof <strong>the</strong> alphabetic pr<strong>in</strong>ciple <strong>the</strong> idea that written words symbolisespoken words <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g way: s<strong>in</strong>gle sounds are represented bys<strong>in</strong>gle letters or groups of letters. Phonological awareness is frequentlyconfused with phonics, which constitutes a method of read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction,aimed at familiaris<strong>in</strong>g children with relationships between lettersounds and correspond<strong>in</strong>g pr<strong>in</strong>ted letters or clusters of letters (Philipset al., 2008).To ensure term<strong>in</strong>ological clarity, we shall def<strong>in</strong>e phoneme as <strong>the</strong>smallest functional unit of a given language, <strong>the</strong> smallest unit of sound<strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g a difference <strong>in</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g. It constitutes a class of speechsounds that native speakers identify as <strong>the</strong> same sound, while membersof such a class are called allophones. Allophones of a given phoneme <strong>the</strong> actual sounds produced by speakers are phonetically slightlydifferent. For example, even though /p/ <strong>in</strong> ‘p<strong>in</strong>’ and /p/ <strong>in</strong> ‘sp<strong>in</strong>’ arephonetically different sounds, aspirated and unaspirated, respectively,native speakers of English ignore (or are relatively <strong>in</strong>sensitive to) <strong>the</strong>difference because it does not cause any change <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g ofwords. However, /p/ <strong>in</strong> ‘p<strong>in</strong>’ and /b/ <strong>in</strong> ‘b<strong>in</strong>’ are perceived asmembers of different classes (phonemes) because <strong>the</strong>y are dist<strong>in</strong>ctive.As such, a phoneme is an abstract unit and it is unpronounceable. It canbe described as an image or a model of a speech sound <strong>in</strong> our m<strong>in</strong>d.Dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> course of phonological development, we learn to identifyphonemes and ignore allophonic variation because only phonemechange can result <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> change of mean<strong>in</strong>g. Phonemes allow us todist<strong>in</strong>guish between and identify <strong>in</strong>dividual sounds that constitute


Causes of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 45utterances we hear, which, <strong>in</strong> turn, makes segmentation of <strong>the</strong>seutterances possible.Turn<strong>in</strong>g now to a more thorough discussion of implicit and explicitphonological knowledge, let us beg<strong>in</strong> with a twofold division ofphonological competence <strong>in</strong>to epil<strong>in</strong>guistic and metal<strong>in</strong>guistic. The firstone is a matter of earlier development and accommodates implicitl<strong>in</strong>guistic knowledge, which is applied <strong>in</strong> an unconscious, <strong>in</strong>tuitive andautomatic way by pre-literate children. The second follows fromepil<strong>in</strong>guistic skills and is connected with a conscious, reflective and<strong>in</strong>tentionally applied explicit knowledge (Gombert, 1992; Goswami,2000; Krasowicz-Kupis, 2004; Saiegh-Haddad, 2007; Sochacka, 2004).Epil<strong>in</strong>guistic activities would <strong>in</strong>volve <strong>in</strong>tuitive judgements on similaritiesbetween words, for example, compar<strong>in</strong>g words and recognis<strong>in</strong>g whe<strong>the</strong>r<strong>the</strong>y share syllables, onsets and rimes. Such tasks as identify<strong>in</strong>g andproduc<strong>in</strong>g phonological segments require conscious knowledge andoperation, usually acquired dur<strong>in</strong>g formal tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g.In <strong>the</strong> course of development of phonological awareness, youngchildren progress from larger to smaller sound units. The awareness ofsyllables, onsets and rimes develops before <strong>the</strong> awareness of phonemes.This observation has been confirmed across alphabetic languages when<strong>the</strong> same phonological awareness tasks are used (Goswami, 2000;Goswami & Bryant, 1990; Miller-Guron & Lundberg, 2000). Consequently,children first f<strong>in</strong>d out that sentences are composed of separatewords; <strong>the</strong>se words, <strong>in</strong> turn, can be divided <strong>in</strong>to syllables, onsets andrimes (<strong>in</strong>tra-sub-syllabic elements) and, f<strong>in</strong>ally, <strong>the</strong>y are made up ofsequences of sounds. Children also learn that all <strong>the</strong>se elements can beseparated, blended toge<strong>the</strong>r and o<strong>the</strong>rwise manipulated.As already mentioned, children beg<strong>in</strong> with handl<strong>in</strong>g tasks <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>gsentences, phrases and words, for <strong>in</strong>stance, compounds (e.g. ‘hotdog’,‘ra<strong>in</strong>bow’). Then, <strong>the</strong> ability to identify and manipulate particularphonological units <strong>in</strong> words naturally refers to bigger chunks, such assyllables with<strong>in</strong> words first, which are more salient and more directlyperceivable, and <strong>the</strong>n to <strong>in</strong>dividual phonemes. Syllables (or typical,short, s<strong>in</strong>gle-syllable words <strong>in</strong> English) can be divided <strong>in</strong>to particular<strong>in</strong>tra-syllabic elements. The model that highlights <strong>the</strong> most importantelements of an <strong>in</strong>ner hierarchical structure of a syllable is as follows (C)*V(C)*. C stands for a consonant, V stands for a vowel, * shows apossible repetitiveness, and, f<strong>in</strong>ally, () <strong>in</strong>dicates a facultative element.A vowel constitutes a peak of a syllable, which can be preceded bya s<strong>in</strong>gle consonant, consonant blend (e.g. ‘fr’, ‘gl’) or digraph (e.g.‘sh’, ‘ch’), called an onset. A vowel can also be followed by a consonantor a consonant blend, named coda. A comb<strong>in</strong>ation of a vowel and af<strong>in</strong>al consonant, consonant blend or digraph forms a rime, whichconstitutes a common part of rhym<strong>in</strong>g words (e.g. ‘fan’, ‘van’, ‘pan’


46 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>or ‘brand’, ‘grand’, ‘stand’) (Kessler & Treiman, 2003; Philips et al., 2008;Sochacka, 2004). Thus, onsets and rimes form bigger <strong>in</strong>tra-syllabicchunks of a syllable or a one-syllable s<strong>in</strong>gle lexical unit. Each can befur<strong>the</strong>r divided <strong>in</strong>to <strong>in</strong>dividual phonemes.Philips et al. (2008) stress that <strong>the</strong> model described above does not holdthat a child has to master a given level or stage before mov<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong>consecutive one, ra<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> stages overlap. Thus, while still work<strong>in</strong>gtowards a decent grasp and proficiency at less complicated levels, a childbeg<strong>in</strong>s to focus on acquir<strong>in</strong>g more complex components. In addition,diversified complexity applies also to <strong>the</strong> types of tasks that vary <strong>in</strong>difficulty level. Tasks of identification, syn<strong>the</strong>sis and analysis aretypically used <strong>in</strong> both assessment and teach<strong>in</strong>g of phonological awareness.Tasks aimed at production pose more constra<strong>in</strong>t than recognitionexercises. Blend<strong>in</strong>g activities tend to be less challeng<strong>in</strong>g than analysistasks. Visual (e.g. tokens, boxes, markers, pictures, gestures) or auditory(e.g. clapp<strong>in</strong>g, tapp<strong>in</strong>g) cues used to represent a given phonologicalchunk (word, syllable, onset, rime or phoneme) can considerably back upchildren’s efforts towards complet<strong>in</strong>g a given task, because <strong>the</strong>y makeoral activities more concrete.Crucially, <strong>the</strong>re exists a reciprocal supportive relation betweenphonological awareness and letter-sounds and letter-names awarenessdevelopment. Instruction <strong>in</strong> letter-sounds and names should be explicitand systematic <strong>in</strong> character and <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> phonemic awarenessteach<strong>in</strong>g (Philips et al., 2008). Goswami and Bryant (1990) stress thatacquir<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g skills can efficiently follow <strong>the</strong> same path, namely,from bigger chunks to smaller ones. The ability to recognise onsetsand rimes allows children to categorise words accord<strong>in</strong>g to soundsimilarities. Then, sounds are mapped onto letters and letter clusters,and, f<strong>in</strong>ally, children are able to read unfamiliar words throughanalogy, by recognis<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> chunks <strong>the</strong>y already know. For example,rhym<strong>in</strong>g words often have identical orthographic sequences of letters(Krasowicz-Kupis & Bryant, 2004; Petrus & Bogdanowicz, 2004;Sochacka, 2004). Thus, <strong>the</strong> more proficient children are <strong>in</strong> identify<strong>in</strong>gsyllables, <strong>in</strong>tra-syllabic elements, phonemes and phoneme-graphemerelations, <strong>the</strong> faster and more effective is <strong>the</strong> acquisition of read<strong>in</strong>gability. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, children encounter<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g difficultiestend to be less sensitive <strong>in</strong> perceiv<strong>in</strong>g rhymes and grapheme/phonemecorrespondences (Bogdanowicz & Krasowicz-Kupis, 2005a; Bradley &Bryant, 1983). Direct <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> grapheme-phoneme relations enhances<strong>the</strong> conversion of implicit phonological knowledge <strong>in</strong>to explicit.Instruction <strong>in</strong> orthographic patterns matched with <strong>the</strong>ir correspond<strong>in</strong>gsounds proved pivotal <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> development of phonological awareness(Wise et al., 2007).


Causes of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 47Phonological difficulties <strong>in</strong> dyslexiaWith regard to <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigation on cognitive level, by far <strong>the</strong> bestdeveloped <strong>the</strong>ory of dyslexia states that phonological problems occurr<strong>in</strong>gprior to <strong>the</strong> emergence of read<strong>in</strong>g constitute a cause of later read<strong>in</strong>gimpairment. Two strong assumptions made by this <strong>the</strong>ory are as follows:<strong>in</strong> children later diagnosed to suffer from dyslexia, phonological deficitpre-dates read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction, secondly, <strong>the</strong> severity of <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>gimpairment (consequence) can be predicted by <strong>the</strong> severity of <strong>the</strong>phonological process<strong>in</strong>g (cause). Importantly, phonological deficit isclaimed to persist through time, before and after read<strong>in</strong>g has begun. Ifearly phonological deficit is remediated before read<strong>in</strong>g emerges, typicalread<strong>in</strong>g development can be expected (Hulme & Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2009).The verification of <strong>the</strong> core phonological deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis has generatedmultiple studies, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g cross-sectional studies, <strong>in</strong> which dyslexics’performance is compared to <strong>the</strong> achievements of chronological age andread<strong>in</strong>g age-matched controls, longitud<strong>in</strong>al studies conducted <strong>in</strong> searchfor best predictors of read<strong>in</strong>g faculty, and, f<strong>in</strong>ally, experimental studieswhose aim is to verify <strong>the</strong> effectiveness of early phonological tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>enhanc<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g skills (Krasowicz-Kupis, 2008). These studies producedan impressive amount of strong and converg<strong>in</strong>g evidence <strong>in</strong>support of <strong>the</strong> assumption that weak phonological cod<strong>in</strong>g constitutes <strong>the</strong>cause of dyslexia. In addition, <strong>the</strong> l<strong>in</strong>k between phonological process<strong>in</strong>gdeficits and atypical bra<strong>in</strong> activation patterns as well as anatomicaldifferences has been demonstrated repeatedly. 18 The description ofphonological deficit usually comprises three ma<strong>in</strong> elements: phonemicawareness; slow lexical retrieval best demonstrated <strong>in</strong> rapid serialnam<strong>in</strong>g/rapid automatised nam<strong>in</strong>g (RAN) tasks; and poor short-termmemory (STM) evident <strong>in</strong> non-word repetition and digit span tasks(Ramus, 2004).Phonological process<strong>in</strong>g difficulties encountered by children withdyslexia are well-documented (Hoien & Lundberg, 2000; Hulme et al.,2005; Lundberg, 2002; Ramus et al., 2003; Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2001a, 2001b;Szenkovits & Ramus, 2005; Vellut<strong>in</strong>o et al., 2004). Indeed, <strong>the</strong>y seem tobe especially evident <strong>in</strong> attempts to tackle both phonological awarenesstasks and tasks requir<strong>in</strong>g memory for phonological sequences. Impairedphonological system <strong>in</strong> dyslexics is seriously challenged by <strong>the</strong> activitiesaimed at identification and manipulation of phonological elements ofdiffer<strong>in</strong>g sizes with<strong>in</strong> words. A range of typical symptoms such asproblems of verbal short-term memory, non-word repetition difficulties,poor phonological learn<strong>in</strong>g of new verbal <strong>in</strong>formation, word retrievaland rapid nam<strong>in</strong>g problems rout<strong>in</strong>ely emerge <strong>in</strong> dyslexics (Borkowska,1997; Reid, 1998; Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2001a). To extend <strong>the</strong> list, Lundberg andHoien (2001) enumerate more characteristic <strong>in</strong>dicators of deficiency


48 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong><strong>in</strong> phonological process<strong>in</strong>g that may underlie read<strong>in</strong>g impairments:difficulty <strong>in</strong> segment<strong>in</strong>g words <strong>in</strong>to phonemes, keep<strong>in</strong>g l<strong>in</strong>guisticmaterial (str<strong>in</strong>gs of sounds) <strong>in</strong> short-term memory, repeat<strong>in</strong>g back longnon-words, read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g even short non-words, and also slownam<strong>in</strong>g of colours, numbers, letters and objects <strong>in</strong> pictures, a slower rateof speech sometimes with <strong>in</strong>dist<strong>in</strong>ct pronunciation, not to mentionproblems <strong>in</strong> phoneme manipulation. As a consequence, <strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sishas been put forward that despite relatively appropriate semanticprocess<strong>in</strong>g, phonology is not as efficiently coded <strong>in</strong> a dyslexic bra<strong>in</strong> ascompared to normally develop<strong>in</strong>g children.Accord<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> phonological cod<strong>in</strong>g deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis, children withdyslexia encounter specific disruptions <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> representation, storageand/or retrieval of speech sounds, which impedes <strong>the</strong> acquisition of <strong>the</strong>grapheme-phoneme correspondences, which <strong>in</strong> turn constitutes <strong>the</strong>foundation for read<strong>in</strong>g disorders, especially with regard to alphabeticsystems (Krasowicz-Kupis, 2008; Ramus et al., 2003). Indeed, a generalconclusion, to which <strong>the</strong>re is a grow<strong>in</strong>g consensus, is that below standardphonological cod<strong>in</strong>g, reflected <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>complete or <strong>in</strong>accurate creation ofphonological representations of words <strong>in</strong> one’s mental lexicon, provesresponsible for <strong>the</strong> failure <strong>in</strong> acquir<strong>in</strong>g phonological awareness andalphabetic cod<strong>in</strong>g skills alike, which <strong>in</strong> turn is a key to <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>gdifficulties experienced by learners with dyslexia (Vellut<strong>in</strong>o et al., 2004).Hence, <strong>the</strong> most notorious characteristics <strong>in</strong> dyslexia seem to perta<strong>in</strong> topoor quality of phonological representations of speech sounds, which,more often than not, turn out to be <strong>in</strong>accurately specified, roughly coded,lack<strong>in</strong>g dist<strong>in</strong>ctness and/or segmental specificity. As a consequence,phonological process<strong>in</strong>g difficulties and problems <strong>in</strong> apply<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>alphabetic pr<strong>in</strong>ciple (segmental hypo<strong>the</strong>sis) emerge. This typically endsup <strong>in</strong> more or less pronounced read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g impairment(Goswami, 2000; Hatcher & Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2008; Hulme & Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2009;Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2001a; Snowl<strong>in</strong>g & Caravolas, 2007). Research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs alsoappear to confirm <strong>the</strong> fact that children at risk for dyslexia tend todemonstrate failure <strong>in</strong> creat<strong>in</strong>g mental phonological representations longbefore <strong>the</strong>y beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction. A test of pseudo-wordrepetition serves well <strong>in</strong> diagnos<strong>in</strong>g such difficulties because it is anexcellent <strong>in</strong>dicator of <strong>the</strong> ability to form phonological representations(Krasowicz-Kupis, 2008).With <strong>the</strong> assumption of <strong>the</strong> predom<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g phonological cod<strong>in</strong>g deficithypo<strong>the</strong>sis <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d, presum<strong>in</strong>g only one cause of dyslexic difficulties, itappears to be truly difficult to expla<strong>in</strong> how different subtypes of dyslexiamay occur. Especially as regards <strong>the</strong> cases of <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexiawhose read<strong>in</strong>g failure is not due to phonological process<strong>in</strong>g impairment.Snowl<strong>in</strong>g (2000) proposes that diversified dyslexia manifestations do notnecessarily refer so much to differ<strong>in</strong>g underly<strong>in</strong>g deficits, but ra<strong>the</strong>r to


Causes of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 49<strong>the</strong> vary<strong>in</strong>g severity of impairment with<strong>in</strong> phonological process<strong>in</strong>g itself.Hence, surface dyslexia, <strong>in</strong>deed <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g a developmental delay ra<strong>the</strong>rthan a deficit, may possibly occur due to a much subtler phonologicalcod<strong>in</strong>g deficit than phonological dyslexia. Surface dyslexia is characterisedby dom<strong>in</strong>ant sublexical strategy <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g, 19 entail<strong>in</strong>g orthographiccod<strong>in</strong>g deficit (ra<strong>the</strong>r adequate non-word read<strong>in</strong>g, problemswith exception word read<strong>in</strong>g), whereas <strong>in</strong> phonological dyslexia, it is <strong>the</strong>lexical strategy, which <strong>in</strong>volves recognis<strong>in</strong>g whole words, that dom<strong>in</strong>ates(relatively normal exception word read<strong>in</strong>g, severe non-word read<strong>in</strong>gproblems) (Hulme & Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2009). This over-reliance on lexicalstrategy <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g suggests more pronounced phonological cod<strong>in</strong>gproblems, present due to less-specified phonological representations. Itcan be expected that <strong>the</strong> two subtypes differ with respect to neuropsychologicalbasis and represent vary<strong>in</strong>g susceptibility to <strong>in</strong>tervention, with<strong>the</strong> surface type probably promis<strong>in</strong>g greater and much more easilyachieved pedagogical ga<strong>in</strong>s than is <strong>the</strong> case with <strong>the</strong> phonological type(Krasowicz-Kupis, 2008). However, several studies provided weaksupport for <strong>the</strong> above classification and, <strong>in</strong> fact, it is suggested that abetter way may be to resign from <strong>the</strong> proposed labels of phonologicaland surface dyslexia because <strong>the</strong>y describe <strong>the</strong> patterns of read<strong>in</strong>gimpairment that tend to be unstable over time. Instead, it is proposed totreat <strong>the</strong>m ra<strong>the</strong>r <strong>in</strong> terms of cont<strong>in</strong>uous variations <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> skills thatunderp<strong>in</strong> development of read<strong>in</strong>g (Hulme & Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2009; Snowl<strong>in</strong>g &Caravolas, 2007).Goswami (2000) argues that <strong>the</strong> phonological representations hypo<strong>the</strong>sispromises a plausible explanation of <strong>the</strong> role of phonologicaldevelopment <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g and dyslexia, and proposes its potential validityacross languages. It is argued that segmental specificity of phonologicalrepresentations is subject to changes <strong>in</strong>duced by read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>gacquisition. Follow<strong>in</strong>g this l<strong>in</strong>e of argument, it is expected, with goodreason, that <strong>the</strong> way dist<strong>in</strong>ctness and segmental specificity would beaffected by different orthographies depends very much on <strong>the</strong> transparencyof a given orthography. 20 For children learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and spell <strong>in</strong>non-transparent orthographies, <strong>the</strong> process of restructur<strong>in</strong>g phonologicalrepresentations towards <strong>the</strong> phoneme-level is much more timeconsum<strong>in</strong>g and more pa<strong>in</strong>ful because of <strong>the</strong> lack of consistent feedbackfrom read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g at <strong>the</strong> phonemic level, even with regard todense phonological neighbourhoods (words with numerous similarsound<strong>in</strong>gneighbours). For <strong>in</strong>stance, words of low degree of sound-tospell<strong>in</strong>gconsistency, such as ‘birch’, ‘lurch’, ‘perch’, ‘search’ (severalspell<strong>in</strong>g choices for a given sound), are likely to rema<strong>in</strong> less specifiedphonologically. Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, for children with dyslexia this processrequires substantially greater <strong>in</strong>vestment of effort and time, frequentlywithout a well-earned profit. On <strong>the</strong> contrary, while learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and


50 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>write <strong>in</strong> transparent orthographies, children with dyslexia come acrossrelatively consistent grapheme-phoneme relations and read<strong>in</strong>g practiceallows <strong>the</strong>m to specify phoneme-level representations despite <strong>the</strong>irpronounced deficits <strong>in</strong> phonological process<strong>in</strong>g.Particular phonological elements of different sizes, which are mentallyrepresented, change over time and can be re-represented several timesdur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> process of development of phonological skills <strong>in</strong> a given child.This phenomenon has been termed restructur<strong>in</strong>g. Intensity of restructur<strong>in</strong>gdepends on several factors, for example, vocabulary size, phonologicalneighbourhood, l<strong>in</strong>guistic features, as well as word frequency andfamiliarity. Larger vocabularies <strong>in</strong>vite greater restructur<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>the</strong> sameholds true for early-acquired, high-frequency and familiar words as wellas for denser phonological neighbourhoods.Generally, early phonological representations of words are ra<strong>the</strong>rholistic and fairly global, <strong>the</strong>n, with age, <strong>the</strong> change is characterised byan <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> represent<strong>in</strong>g segments of sounds <strong>in</strong> words correspond<strong>in</strong>gto syllables, onsets and rimes and, f<strong>in</strong>ally, phonemes, and specify<strong>in</strong>gdist<strong>in</strong>ctive features of <strong>the</strong>se sounds (e.g. voic<strong>in</strong>g). Phonemic representationis ra<strong>the</strong>r rare <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> pre-literacy period, but it is believed to <strong>in</strong>creasewith literacy acquisition due to feedback of graphemic <strong>in</strong>formation(Wise et al., 2007). However, it needs stress<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>in</strong>accessibility ofphonemes to pre-literate children is by no means universal. Childrenfrom several l<strong>in</strong>guistic backgrounds may demonstrate vary<strong>in</strong>g degreesof phonemic awareness prior to formal literacy tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g; whe<strong>the</strong>r and towhat extent small gra<strong>in</strong> sizes are available to <strong>the</strong>m depends very muchon <strong>the</strong> nature of <strong>the</strong> language and its orthography. One might expectthat <strong>the</strong> rapidity of change <strong>in</strong> restructur<strong>in</strong>g towards achiev<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> levelof <strong>in</strong>dividual phonemes would be affected by <strong>the</strong> level of orthographictransparency of <strong>the</strong> language <strong>in</strong> which a child is learn<strong>in</strong>g to read andspell. The anticipated effect assumes faster acquisition of <strong>the</strong> phonemelevelrepresentation <strong>in</strong> languages with relatively transparent ra<strong>the</strong>r thanopaque orthographies, s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>the</strong>re is a consistent letter representation ofa given sound for read<strong>in</strong>g (Caravolas, 2006; Goswami, 2000; Goswami &Ziegler, 2006; Ziegler & Goswami, 2006).Slow and <strong>in</strong>adequate phonological cod<strong>in</strong>g and poorly developedphonological representations constitute a characteristic feature of childrenand adults with dyslexia. Additionally, accord<strong>in</strong>g to Lundberg andHoien (2001), phonological deficit also tends to be apparent <strong>in</strong> compensateddyslexics <strong>in</strong>dividuals who manage to achieve an almost normalread<strong>in</strong>g faculty despite experienc<strong>in</strong>g severe dyslexic problems. Snowl<strong>in</strong>g(2001a) highlights two possible sources of compensation <strong>in</strong> dyslexicswith phonological process<strong>in</strong>g impairments: visual memory and perceptualspeed. Never<strong>the</strong>less, all <strong>the</strong> phonological tasks appear to besignificantly more difficult for compensated dyslexics as compared to


Causes of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 51non-dyslexics. As po<strong>in</strong>ted out by Shaywitz (1997), despite <strong>the</strong> undeniableimprovement compensated dyslexics achieve <strong>in</strong> decod<strong>in</strong>g and recognis<strong>in</strong>gwords, thus automatically ga<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g access to <strong>the</strong> higher levels oflanguage process<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>the</strong>ir results on tests on read<strong>in</strong>g speed and accuracyprove that decod<strong>in</strong>g still requires considerable effort; words are notrecognised automatically and fluently. Poor and slow phonologicalprocess<strong>in</strong>g not only makes it <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly hard, if not impossible, toquickly and effortlessly recognise words, but it is also capable ofconsiderably hamper<strong>in</strong>g access to higher levels of language process<strong>in</strong>g(syntactic and semantic), which, even though usually <strong>in</strong>tact, can bereduced <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir potential accessibility unless decod<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong> phonologicallevel is successful.Summ<strong>in</strong>g up, a pervasive phonological impairment present <strong>in</strong> preschoolyears and persist<strong>in</strong>g after <strong>the</strong> onset of read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>structionconstitutes a characteristic feature of dyslexia, which at <strong>the</strong> cognitivelevel can be perceived as a delay or difficulty <strong>in</strong> form<strong>in</strong>g segmentalphonological representations. Non-segmental and poorly specified phonologicalrepresentations present <strong>in</strong> children beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g formal read<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>struction can considerably <strong>in</strong>fluence <strong>the</strong>ir read<strong>in</strong>g development andaggravate read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties. There exist <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong>dyslexia with reference to severity and pattern of phonological difficulties.Still, Hulme and Snowl<strong>in</strong>g (2009) propose that, despite strongevidence collected <strong>in</strong> support of <strong>the</strong> view that <strong>the</strong> phonological deficitconstitutes one cause of failure <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> proper development of read<strong>in</strong>gskill <strong>in</strong> dyslexia, it rema<strong>in</strong>s an open question whe<strong>the</strong>r o<strong>the</strong>r factors mayplay a causal role here. Indeed, it seems possible and likely. Theoperation of additional causes can be twofold: <strong>the</strong>y may impactphonological skills and hence become <strong>the</strong> ultimate cause of phonologicalimpairment or <strong>the</strong>y operate separately from <strong>the</strong> phonological deficit(read<strong>in</strong>g does not become impaired via <strong>the</strong> phonological deficit).It goes without say<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>the</strong> phonological deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis hasbecome <strong>the</strong> most powerful <strong>the</strong>oretical framework for dyslexia, but, as itstands, it did not escape criticism. Some scholars believe that <strong>the</strong>symptoms and causes of read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g difficulties are specific toevery child with dyslexia and, apparently, not all of <strong>the</strong>m can beexpla<strong>in</strong>ed by phonological deficit (Nicolson & Fawcett, 1995; Nicolson,2001). One of <strong>the</strong> ma<strong>in</strong> weaknesses of <strong>the</strong> phonological cod<strong>in</strong>g hypo<strong>the</strong>sisis its <strong>in</strong>ability to expla<strong>in</strong> motor and sensory deficits evident <strong>in</strong>children with dyslexia. As much as <strong>the</strong> advocates of <strong>the</strong> phonologicaldeficit <strong>the</strong>ory appear to accept <strong>the</strong>ir co-occurrence with dyslexia, <strong>the</strong>yreject <strong>the</strong>m as <strong>in</strong>significant and not critical manifestations of <strong>the</strong> specificread<strong>in</strong>g difficulty (Krasowicz-Kupis, 2008). No wonder that, as alreadymentioned, numerous alternative <strong>the</strong>ories have been developed. These<strong>the</strong>ories mostly hold that high-standard phonological process<strong>in</strong>g is


52 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong><strong>in</strong>deed crucial for successful development of read<strong>in</strong>g skills and thatphonological deficit may deeply compromise read<strong>in</strong>g acquisition. Never<strong>the</strong>less,<strong>the</strong>se problems can be traced back to a more general learn<strong>in</strong>gdisorder or sensorimotor impairment, which underlies phonologicaldecod<strong>in</strong>g deficit. Additionally, <strong>the</strong> high-quality controlled researchnotwithstand<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>the</strong>re seems to rema<strong>in</strong> some scepticism among scholarswith reference to such fundamental issues underly<strong>in</strong>g research designsas operationalis<strong>in</strong>g and def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> concept of ‘phonology’ (Uppstad &Tønnessen, 2007).Indeed, much as <strong>the</strong> consensus with respect to <strong>the</strong> l<strong>in</strong>k betweenliteracy problems and phonological awareness is common, it is apparentlylack<strong>in</strong>g with regard to assign<strong>in</strong>g phonological awareness ei<strong>the</strong>ra primary/core role or perceiv<strong>in</strong>g it secondary to low-level sensoryprocess<strong>in</strong>g deficits (Bishop, 2006; White et al., 2006). Low-level sensoryprocess<strong>in</strong>g impairment <strong>the</strong>ories deal with more basic causes of read<strong>in</strong>gdisability, <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> visual system, auditory system and <strong>the</strong> cerebellum.They usually refer to <strong>the</strong> faulty process<strong>in</strong>g of rapid, transient stimulimost probably orig<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g from neurobiological underp<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>form of cerebellar or/and magnocellular malfunction, and possiblyunderly<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> phonological deficit. F<strong>in</strong>ally, apart from attempt<strong>in</strong>g toaccount for <strong>the</strong> phonological deficit <strong>in</strong> terms of <strong>the</strong> secondary impairment,<strong>the</strong>y also propose a range of o<strong>the</strong>r deficits.Goswami (2006) perceives those <strong>the</strong>ories as complementary; probablyunderly<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> phonological deficit and undertak<strong>in</strong>g an attempt todecipher why o<strong>the</strong>rwise cognitively well-function<strong>in</strong>g children strugglewith phonological process<strong>in</strong>g. As an example, <strong>the</strong> doma<strong>in</strong> of process<strong>in</strong>gauditory clues to <strong>the</strong> rhythm of speech is given; it is claimed that‘dyslexics just don’t get <strong>the</strong> beat’ (Goswami, 2006: 259). The temporalprocess<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ory of dyslexia (or temporal order perception <strong>the</strong>ory)(Tallal, 1980) sees <strong>the</strong> non-l<strong>in</strong>guistic, auditory deficit, characterised byproblems <strong>in</strong> temporal process<strong>in</strong>g of rapidly chang<strong>in</strong>g auditory stimuli,responsible for dyslexic read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties. In a similar ve<strong>in</strong>, <strong>the</strong>magnocellular <strong>the</strong>ory (Ste<strong>in</strong>, 2001) proposes that <strong>the</strong> phonological deficitbe traced back to <strong>the</strong> auditory deficit. In addition, Nicolson and Fawcett(2006: 261) po<strong>in</strong>t to <strong>the</strong> fact that <strong>the</strong> ‘cerebellar deficit is <strong>the</strong> only s<strong>in</strong>gleexplanation of problems <strong>in</strong> balance, phonology and speed, and also oneexplanation of ‘‘pure’’ phonology/speed problems’.By contrast, White et al. (2006) and Ramus et al. (2006) admit that <strong>the</strong>connection between phonological dyslexia and disordered sensorimotorprocess<strong>in</strong>g is unquestionable, probably also <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g a commonbiological characteristic; <strong>in</strong> addition, <strong>the</strong>y acknowledge <strong>the</strong> existence ofa subgroup of children with dyslexia whose read<strong>in</strong>g problems cannot beplausibly expla<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> terms of phonological deficit and this is where <strong>the</strong>sensorimotor deficit comes <strong>in</strong>to play as a viable alternative. None<strong>the</strong>less,


Causes of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 53<strong>the</strong>y heavily criticise <strong>the</strong> proposition that phonological deficit could becausally related to and conv<strong>in</strong>c<strong>in</strong>gly expla<strong>in</strong>ed by low-level process<strong>in</strong>g.Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, if it were <strong>the</strong> case, <strong>the</strong>n all dyslexics with phonologicalprocess<strong>in</strong>g problems should be expected to demonstrate auditory deficit.However, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir study, only a small subgroup with clear phonologicaldeficit also showed auditory deficit. Moreover, phonological deficit notaccompanied by any sensorimotor deficits, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g visual, auditoryand motor, was observed <strong>in</strong> some children with dyslexia. White et al.(2006) and Ramus et al. (2006) <strong>in</strong>terpret <strong>the</strong> results obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>irmultiple case study as strongly supportive of a primary role ofphonological deficit, which is expected to be biologically based, at <strong>the</strong>same time discredit<strong>in</strong>g and unequivocally reject<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> sensorimotor<strong>the</strong>ories as generally <strong>in</strong>capable of expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g dyslexia. Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, asalready mentioned, <strong>the</strong>y admit though that phonological deficit itselfcannot expla<strong>in</strong> all cases of dyslexia. Thus, <strong>the</strong> rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g subgroup ofdyslexics, a non-phonological type, is believed to suffer from a visualimpairment, more precisely visual stress, caus<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ir read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties.Altoge<strong>the</strong>r, summaris<strong>in</strong>g White et al.’s (2006: 253) position, ‘specificallyl<strong>in</strong>guistic phonological deficit’ is clearly favoured and argued tobe ‘directly and exclusively’ responsible for dyslexic read<strong>in</strong>g failure,fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, it is not believed to follow from auditory or motor deficits.Last but not least, <strong>in</strong> rare cases, visual disorder possibly accounts fordyslexic read<strong>in</strong>g problems.Still, <strong>the</strong> character of this visual disorder is not totally clear <strong>in</strong> light ofevidence produced by o<strong>the</strong>r studies. Shovman and Ahissar (2006) po<strong>in</strong>tout that, <strong>in</strong> fact, <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia often compla<strong>in</strong> about visionrelatedsymptoms such as ‘jump<strong>in</strong>g letters’, ‘danc<strong>in</strong>g l<strong>in</strong>es’ or blurredtext; <strong>in</strong> addition, some of <strong>the</strong>ir read<strong>in</strong>g errors tend to be expla<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong>terms of visual process<strong>in</strong>g impairment. However, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir exam<strong>in</strong>ationof <strong>the</strong> visual aspect of read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> dyslexia, <strong>the</strong>y did not confirm this k<strong>in</strong>dof deficit to have any debilitat<strong>in</strong>g effect on <strong>the</strong> dyslexic s<strong>in</strong>gle wordread<strong>in</strong>g ability. The subjects were tested on a specially designed task thatwas <strong>in</strong>tended to be as similar as possible to s<strong>in</strong>gle word read<strong>in</strong>g withrespect to visual aspects, at <strong>the</strong> same time lack<strong>in</strong>g all <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rs phonological, morphological or semantic. Shovman and Ahissar (2006)admit that children with dyslexia more frequently reported visualdifficulties <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g compared with controls. The proposed explanationof this phenomenon <strong>in</strong>volves <strong>the</strong> idea that, ra<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> context ofs<strong>in</strong>gle word decod<strong>in</strong>g, which was simulated <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> study, <strong>in</strong>dividualswith dyslexia encounter greater visual stress dur<strong>in</strong>g normal read<strong>in</strong>gconditions, namely, when approach<strong>in</strong>g texts. The visual stress <strong>the</strong>yexperience seems to follow from <strong>the</strong> additional burden of <strong>the</strong> need foraccurate saccades and track<strong>in</strong>g along <strong>the</strong> l<strong>in</strong>es of <strong>the</strong> text, whichaltoge<strong>the</strong>r requires more visual attention necessary to compensate for


54 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>poor phonological representations. Thus, <strong>the</strong> visual stress itself appearsto stem from ra<strong>the</strong>r than to cause dyslexic read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties.In l<strong>in</strong>e with a multifactorial and multifaceted view of dyslexia, visualattention span deficit, possibly contribut<strong>in</strong>g to dyslexic difficultiesirrespective of phonological impairment, was <strong>in</strong>vestigated by Bosseet al. (2007). Reduced visual attention span is characterised by poormulti-element process<strong>in</strong>g, stemm<strong>in</strong>g from disorder <strong>in</strong> allocat<strong>in</strong>g attentionacross letter or symbol str<strong>in</strong>gs, which <strong>in</strong> turn limits <strong>the</strong> number ofelements that can be simultaneously processed from a short visualdisplay dur<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g. The outcome of <strong>the</strong> study <strong>in</strong>dicates that poorvisual attention span can expla<strong>in</strong> some dyslexic read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties<strong>in</strong>dependently of <strong>the</strong>ir phonological abilities. In fact, Bosse et al. (2007)qualified dyslexics <strong>in</strong>to three subtypes, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g those with identifieds<strong>in</strong>gle phonological process<strong>in</strong>g or visual attention span deficits and asubgroup with a double deficit.It seems that <strong>the</strong> debate on <strong>the</strong>ories of dyslexia is very unlikely to endsoon as new evidence, fresh <strong>in</strong>sights and re<strong>in</strong>terpretations of <strong>the</strong>previous f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are regularly added to <strong>the</strong> field. Such a state of affairsnaturally gives rise to numerous questions, especially as practitionerstend to f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong>mselves at a loss while try<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>in</strong>terpret and understandresearch f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs and translate <strong>the</strong>m <strong>in</strong>to plausible implications for<strong>in</strong>tervention and teach<strong>in</strong>g.Double-deficit Hypo<strong>the</strong>sisBeyond doubt, some scholars do not seem to share <strong>the</strong> view of <strong>the</strong>exclusive and primary nature of poor phonological cod<strong>in</strong>g as a cause ofdyslexia. The double-deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis (DDH) proposes <strong>the</strong> existence oftwo <strong>in</strong>dependent underly<strong>in</strong>g sources of dyslexic difficulties, namely, <strong>the</strong>phonological core deficit and nam<strong>in</strong>g speed impairment (Wolf & Bowers,1999). Cir<strong>in</strong>o et al. (2005) extend <strong>the</strong> applicability of DDH beyondchildren, support<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> fact that both phonological process<strong>in</strong>g andvisual nam<strong>in</strong>g speed seem likely to predict read<strong>in</strong>g deficits <strong>in</strong> adults.Let us move on to a more detailed revision of <strong>the</strong> nature of <strong>the</strong> secondcore deficit <strong>in</strong> dyslexia <strong>the</strong> nam<strong>in</strong>g speed impairment. Deficits <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>rate of process<strong>in</strong>g are best elucidated by <strong>the</strong> serial nam<strong>in</strong>g speed, whichis usually measured by RAN. Individuals’ responses dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>irperformance on tasks of nam<strong>in</strong>g speed are analysed with respect to <strong>the</strong>time needed for provid<strong>in</strong>g verbal labels for randomly presented highfrequencyvisual stimuli, such as numbers, letters, colours or objects(Vukovic & Siegel, 2006; Wolf & O’Brien, 2001). Slow nam<strong>in</strong>g speed maytranslate <strong>in</strong>to low-level ability to recognise words quickly, whichtypically <strong>in</strong>vites read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties. In o<strong>the</strong>r words, children whoexhibit difficulties <strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g adequate rapid process<strong>in</strong>g rates can


Causes of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 55be expected, with good reason, to experience some turbulence <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>gdevelopment. Cir<strong>in</strong>o et al. (2005) stress that poor visual nam<strong>in</strong>g speedmay be responsible for slower word decod<strong>in</strong>g, without be<strong>in</strong>g affected byaccuracy, and as a consequence, read<strong>in</strong>g comprehension may considerablydim<strong>in</strong>ish. Thus, RAN tasks seem to prove of service <strong>in</strong> formulat<strong>in</strong>gpredictions and identify<strong>in</strong>g children at risk for read<strong>in</strong>g failure. However,Vukovic and Siegel (2006), due to <strong>the</strong> lack of a clearly operationalised andconsistently used def<strong>in</strong>ition of rapid nam<strong>in</strong>g, call for <strong>the</strong> need todel<strong>in</strong>eate particular features of RAN tasks that make <strong>the</strong> processes ofnam<strong>in</strong>g speed crucial to read<strong>in</strong>g development. Hav<strong>in</strong>g reviewed <strong>the</strong>literature on read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>in</strong> children, <strong>the</strong>y arrived at <strong>the</strong>conclusion that nam<strong>in</strong>g speed deficit lacks specificity. In addition, <strong>the</strong>yraise considerable doubts as to <strong>the</strong> existence of a sufficient amount ofsupport<strong>in</strong>g evidence for DDH, especially as relates to <strong>the</strong> claimed<strong>in</strong>dependence of nam<strong>in</strong>g speed skills of phonological process<strong>in</strong>g ability<strong>in</strong> expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties. By contrast, Wolf and O’Brien (2001)propose that <strong>the</strong> processes underly<strong>in</strong>g nam<strong>in</strong>g speed <strong>in</strong>deed constitute asecond core deficit <strong>in</strong> dyslexia, generally <strong>in</strong>dependent of phonologicalprocess<strong>in</strong>g and manifest<strong>in</strong>g itself <strong>in</strong> children and adults alike, a factverified by numerous research outcomes.Admittedly, <strong>the</strong> presupposed <strong>in</strong>dependence of <strong>the</strong> rapid nam<strong>in</strong>gimpairment of <strong>the</strong> phonological deficit allows identification of threemajor subtypes of read<strong>in</strong>g-impaired <strong>in</strong>dividuals, who can be categorisedaccord<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> presence or absence of <strong>the</strong>se underly<strong>in</strong>g processes. Moreprecisely, <strong>the</strong> phonological deficit subtype concerns poor readers suffer<strong>in</strong>gsolely from <strong>the</strong> disorder of phonological process<strong>in</strong>g, at <strong>the</strong> same timedemonstrat<strong>in</strong>g normal nam<strong>in</strong>g speed. By contrast, <strong>the</strong> nam<strong>in</strong>g speeddeficit subtype refers to read<strong>in</strong>g-impaired <strong>in</strong>dividuals with averagephonological and word attack skills, but exhibit<strong>in</strong>g below standardnam<strong>in</strong>g speed and later comprehension disorders. F<strong>in</strong>ally, <strong>the</strong> mostnotoriously and severely disordered poor readers, characterised by lowgradega<strong>in</strong>s with regard to both phonological process<strong>in</strong>g and rapidnam<strong>in</strong>g, constitute <strong>the</strong> double-deficit subtype (Wolf & Bowers, 1999).Katzir et al. (2008) provided support for <strong>the</strong> valid framework of <strong>the</strong> DDHby <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g dist<strong>in</strong>ct subtyp<strong>in</strong>g of read<strong>in</strong>g-disabled <strong>in</strong>dividuals. In<strong>the</strong>ir sample, as many as 46% of poor readers were categorised under <strong>the</strong>double-deficit subtype, approximately 25% were ill-at-ease when perform<strong>in</strong>gphonological process<strong>in</strong>g tasks but proved average <strong>in</strong> terms ofnam<strong>in</strong>g speed, <strong>in</strong> 18% of cases <strong>the</strong> reverse was observed, and f<strong>in</strong>ally, <strong>the</strong>rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g 11% could not be classified <strong>in</strong>to any of <strong>the</strong> subtypes under <strong>the</strong>DDH framework. Similar f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs were reported <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r studies as well(Lovett et al., 2000). As stressed by Wolf and O’Brien (2001), <strong>the</strong>cont<strong>in</strong>ually grow<strong>in</strong>g bulk of data on <strong>the</strong> abovementioned subtypes ofread<strong>in</strong>g-impaired <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong>dicate certa<strong>in</strong> applied implications of <strong>the</strong>


56 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>DDH, namely, <strong>the</strong> need for adequate treatment and read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tervention,especially as regards two subtypes with poor rate of process<strong>in</strong>g and lowstandardfluency <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g. However, Vukovic and Siegel (2006) po<strong>in</strong>t to<strong>the</strong> improper design of <strong>in</strong>tervention studies as constitut<strong>in</strong>g an apparentgap <strong>in</strong> research devoted to verification of <strong>the</strong> DDH.Nam<strong>in</strong>g speed deficits are also existent <strong>in</strong> languages with moretransparent orthographies, pos<strong>in</strong>g readers with fewer phonologicaldemands than English. Tak<strong>in</strong>g German as an example, it has becomeevident that <strong>the</strong> nam<strong>in</strong>g speed deficits differentiated groups of readers <strong>in</strong>that language and predicted a read<strong>in</strong>g ability better than a phonemedeletion task, which is considered <strong>the</strong> most well-known phonologicalmeasure. Apart from German, serial nam<strong>in</strong>g speed also proved apowerful predictor <strong>in</strong> Dutch, F<strong>in</strong>nish and Spanish. Wolf and O’Brien(2001) highlight <strong>the</strong> fact that <strong>in</strong> languages with more regular orthographies,phonological skills play a reduced role, while it is <strong>the</strong> carry<strong>in</strong>g outof <strong>the</strong> nam<strong>in</strong>g speed tasks that becomes a stronger predictor of <strong>the</strong>read<strong>in</strong>g performance.However, language process<strong>in</strong>g disorders do not hold a total potentialfor <strong>the</strong> explanation of read<strong>in</strong>g disorders. Frequently, <strong>the</strong>se problemsseem not to be provoked by or fully dependent on poor language skills,quite <strong>the</strong> contrary, <strong>the</strong>y may be <strong>in</strong>vited by o<strong>the</strong>r deficits, thus properattention is given to <strong>the</strong>m <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> forthcom<strong>in</strong>g sections.Magnocellular Deficit Hypo<strong>the</strong>sisAs already signalled, a huge bulk of evidence has been providedsupport<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>sis that <strong>the</strong> majority of people with dyslexia haveimpaired phonological process<strong>in</strong>g (Bradley & Bryant, 1983; Hatcher &Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2008; Hulme & Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2009; Shaywitz, 1997; Snowl<strong>in</strong>g,1981, 2001a), which correlates with <strong>the</strong>ir read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties and, to agreat extent, can expla<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>m. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Vellut<strong>in</strong>o et al. (2004), visualsystem deficit <strong>the</strong>ories <strong>in</strong> dyslexia, despite poor empirical support, werefairly <strong>in</strong>fluential and occupied an important position <strong>in</strong> research from <strong>the</strong>turn of <strong>the</strong> 20th century, through <strong>the</strong> 1970s and 1980s. Then l<strong>in</strong>guisticdeficit hypo<strong>the</strong>ses started to ga<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g prom<strong>in</strong>ence. In those days,<strong>the</strong> proposed causes of read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>in</strong>cluded, for example, faultyvisualisation, visual sequences and visual memory. Some <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong>disordered visual process<strong>in</strong>g as a cause of read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties rema<strong>in</strong>ed,but <strong>the</strong> scope has changed <strong>in</strong>to analys<strong>in</strong>g visual track<strong>in</strong>g problems,visual transient system deficit, abnormalities of perception of visualmotion, <strong>the</strong> last two associated with functional distortions <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> visualmagnocellular subsystem.In <strong>the</strong> search for possible causes of dyslexic difficulties, <strong>the</strong> studieson psychophysiology of <strong>the</strong> visual system arrested <strong>the</strong> attention of


Causes of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 57researchers, especially as perta<strong>in</strong>s to <strong>the</strong> role of functional differencesbetween <strong>the</strong> two structurally <strong>in</strong>dependent subsystems of <strong>the</strong> visualsystem magnocellular and parvocellular. 21 Indeed, <strong>the</strong> fact has beenaccepted that some dyslexics show a certa<strong>in</strong> degree of visual deficit (Ste<strong>in</strong>et al., 2000b; Bednarek, 2003).Ste<strong>in</strong> (2001), Ste<strong>in</strong> et al. (2000b) and Talcott et al. (2000a) demonstratedthat <strong>the</strong> contrast sensitivity <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia is impaired ascompared to non-dyslexics, especially as low spatial frequencies, lowlum<strong>in</strong>ance and high temporal frequencies (short stimulus duration) areconcerned. Detection of such stimuli depends on <strong>the</strong> sensitivity ofmagnocells (M-cells) that comprise <strong>the</strong> ret<strong>in</strong>o-cortical M-pathway andthis transient pathway is said to be <strong>the</strong> locus of <strong>the</strong> subtle visualimpairments <strong>in</strong> dyslexia. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, people with dyslexiaperform better at high spatial frequencies realised by <strong>the</strong> parvocellularsystem, which <strong>in</strong>dicates that <strong>the</strong>y are not as poor at all visual tasks. Whatis more, dyslexics exhibit low contrast and flicker sensitivity that arewell-accepted measures of <strong>the</strong> visual transient/magnocellular systemfunction. That is why it has been suggested (Ste<strong>in</strong>, 2001; Ste<strong>in</strong> & Talcott,1999; Talcott et al., 2000a, 2000b) that <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia may havea specific impairment of <strong>the</strong> visual magnocellular/transient pathway,however <strong>the</strong> impairment is said to be mild and non-exist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> alldyslexics. Those <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia who show visual symptomsare likely to have significant magnocellular deficit. The impairedtransient sensitivity has been claimed (Ste<strong>in</strong> et al., 2000b) to result <strong>in</strong>unsteady b<strong>in</strong>ocular control, which may br<strong>in</strong>g about <strong>the</strong> impression,frequently reported by people with dyslexia, of letters mov<strong>in</strong>g aroundand over each o<strong>the</strong>r.Ste<strong>in</strong> (2001), Ste<strong>in</strong> et al. (2000a, 2001) and Talcott et al. (2000a, 2000b)fur<strong>the</strong>r state that reduced sensitivity of <strong>the</strong> magnocellular system is<strong>in</strong>dicated by reduced sensitivity to visual motion. 22 Visual motionsensitivity has been measured with <strong>the</strong> use of a random dot k<strong>in</strong>ematogram(RDK) test prepared for adults and children. Sensitivity to coherentmotion is significantly reduced <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> dyslexic group <strong>in</strong> comparison tocontrols. It appears to be <strong>in</strong>dependent of <strong>the</strong> changes <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> temporal andspatial parameters of <strong>the</strong> RDK as well as its density. It can be related topoor <strong>in</strong>tegration of <strong>the</strong> changes <strong>in</strong> time that are characteristic of dynamicvisual stimuli. Moreover, <strong>the</strong> reduction <strong>in</strong> motion sensitivity <strong>in</strong> an<strong>in</strong>dividual has been shown to correlate with his/her read<strong>in</strong>g impairment.These f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs have been confirmed by electrophysiological studies,fMRI and post-mortem exam<strong>in</strong>ation of dyslexic bra<strong>in</strong>s (Galaburda &Liv<strong>in</strong>gstone, 1993). 23As argued by Ste<strong>in</strong> (2001) and Talcott et al. (2000b), more evidence isbe<strong>in</strong>g accumulated to show a causal connection between <strong>the</strong> function of<strong>the</strong> magnocellular system and read<strong>in</strong>g. The direction of visual attention


58 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>and eye movements as well as visual search are claimed to be dependenton <strong>the</strong> magnocellular system function<strong>in</strong>g. It is ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed that dyslexicsshow reduced motion sensitivity as well as unsteady b<strong>in</strong>ocular fixation,caus<strong>in</strong>g poor visual localisation, particularly on <strong>the</strong> left side (left neglect).B<strong>in</strong>ocular and visual perceptual <strong>in</strong>stability <strong>in</strong> dyslexics can cause animpression that <strong>the</strong> letters <strong>the</strong>y are try<strong>in</strong>g to read move around.Individuals with dyslexia also tend to compla<strong>in</strong> (Bednarek, 2003; Ste<strong>in</strong>,2001; Ste<strong>in</strong> et al., 2001) that small letters <strong>the</strong>y are try<strong>in</strong>g to read seem tochange places, to merge with each o<strong>the</strong>r, to move <strong>in</strong> and out of <strong>the</strong> page,to blur, to get larger or smaller. It can happen because b<strong>in</strong>ocular<strong>in</strong>stability causes <strong>the</strong> l<strong>in</strong>es of sight of <strong>the</strong> two eyes to cross over eacho<strong>the</strong>r. Therefore, it is not surpris<strong>in</strong>g that dyslexics cannot remember <strong>the</strong>proper order of <strong>the</strong>se letters <strong>in</strong> words and that is why <strong>the</strong>y fail to formreliable memories of <strong>the</strong>se words’ orthography. It is claimed that <strong>the</strong>smaller <strong>the</strong> letter size, <strong>the</strong> greater <strong>the</strong> number of visual errors committedby children with b<strong>in</strong>ocular <strong>in</strong>stability (Cornelissen et al., 1991). Consequently,magnocellular sensitivity, be<strong>in</strong>g essential for motion sensitivityand stable b<strong>in</strong>ocular fixation, can thus predict <strong>the</strong> visual orthographiccomponent of <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g skill of good and poor readers. For example,<strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g of irregular words and homophones 24 cannot be atta<strong>in</strong>ed bysound<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> letters out; <strong>the</strong>ir orthography must be remembered visually.Ste<strong>in</strong> (2001) and Talcott et al. (2000b) demonstrated that <strong>the</strong> ability to spellirregular words and performance on <strong>the</strong> pseudo-homophone testcorrelate with visual motion sensitivity. Moreover, motion sensitivityaccounts for <strong>the</strong> orthographic skills of an <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>in</strong>dependently ofhis/her phonological skill.There is ano<strong>the</strong>r trend <strong>in</strong> research on <strong>the</strong> role of <strong>the</strong> visual system,deal<strong>in</strong>g with <strong>the</strong> specificity of eye movements <strong>in</strong> people with dyslexia(Bednarek, 1999). Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Pavlidis (1986), eye movements highlycorrelate with <strong>the</strong> ability to read. It is ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed (Bednarek, 1999;Bogdanowicz, 1999, 2002a; Borkowska, 1997, 1998; Maruszewski, 1996;Pavlidis, 1986) that <strong>the</strong> movement of eyes dur<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g is notcont<strong>in</strong>uous; it consists of moments of fixation and saccades allow<strong>in</strong>gperception of <strong>the</strong> next fragment of text. Successful read<strong>in</strong>g is said todepend on achiev<strong>in</strong>g stable visual perception dur<strong>in</strong>g each fixation on<strong>the</strong> word to be read and rapid shifts of visual attention to <strong>the</strong> next (Ste<strong>in</strong>et al., 2001). Good readers move <strong>the</strong>ir eyes more regularly along <strong>the</strong> l<strong>in</strong>esof <strong>the</strong> text than people with dyslexia. Abnormal saccade control has beendemonstrated <strong>in</strong> people with dyslexia (Biscaldi et al., 1998). The patternof eye movement <strong>in</strong> dyslexics dur<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g is disordered; <strong>the</strong> movementsare more irregular, less harmonious and smooth. Significantly,more movements dur<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g have been noticed, especially asperta<strong>in</strong>s to <strong>the</strong> occurrence of successive regressions (mov<strong>in</strong>g back and


Causes of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 59fixat<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong> already processed fragment of <strong>the</strong> text). Moreover, <strong>the</strong>reare more fixations and <strong>the</strong>y are longer (Borkowska, 1997; Pavlidis, 1986).As much as <strong>the</strong> severity of read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties may correlate withimpairment <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> pattern of eye movements, <strong>the</strong>re is no agreement as to<strong>the</strong> causal status of <strong>the</strong> disordered pattern of eye movements with regardto dyslexia. It seems that read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>in</strong>duce an abnormal patternof eye movements because poor readers seek ways of compensat<strong>in</strong>g, thuslonger fixations may be traced back to decod<strong>in</strong>g problems and numerousregressions can be brought about by <strong>the</strong> necessity to go through a givenfragment of <strong>the</strong> text aga<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> order to <strong>in</strong>tegrate and understand it. Itmight be suspected <strong>the</strong>n, with good reason, that <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>correct pattern ofeye movement <strong>in</strong> dyslexia may be caused by <strong>the</strong> difficulties <strong>in</strong> understand<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong> text, particularly <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> cases of unknown words andgrammatically complex sentences (Everatt, 2008).On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, some studies show a tendency <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals withdyslexia to demonstrate <strong>the</strong> same pattern of eye movement <strong>in</strong> tasks thatdo not require read<strong>in</strong>g and understand<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> text. For example, <strong>in</strong>watch<strong>in</strong>g numbers, pictures or fixat<strong>in</strong>g eyes on lights that are mov<strong>in</strong>gsequentially along <strong>the</strong> l<strong>in</strong>e from left to right and from right to left.Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, children with dyslexia, when compared to o<strong>the</strong>r read<strong>in</strong>gimpaired<strong>in</strong>dividuals, also perform more eye movements on read<strong>in</strong>gtasks, especially regressions. These f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs may <strong>in</strong>dicate that <strong>the</strong>y showa consistent eye movement pattern irrespective of <strong>the</strong> task. Thus, thispattern seems to be <strong>in</strong>dependent of read<strong>in</strong>g. It is probably brought aboutby <strong>the</strong> central neurological mechanism, connected with small developmentalchanges <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> cerebral cortex (Bednarek, 1999; Bogdanowicz,1999; Borkowska, 1997; Ott, 1997). Apparently, <strong>the</strong>se f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs have notbeen successfully replicated <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r studies; some children with dyslexiademonstrate typical patterns of eye movements, characteristic for nondyslexic<strong>in</strong>dividuals, <strong>in</strong> tasks that do not <strong>in</strong>volve read<strong>in</strong>g (Everatt, 2008).Ste<strong>in</strong> et al. (2001) consider <strong>the</strong> visual magnocellular system responsiblefor controll<strong>in</strong>g eye movements dur<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g, thus it might beconcluded that <strong>the</strong> disordered pattern of eye movement <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividualswith dyslexia may be caused by <strong>in</strong>adequate function<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> magnocellularsystem. Biscaldi et al. (1998) speculated that <strong>the</strong> neural substratefor <strong>the</strong> disordered saccade performance <strong>in</strong> dyslexics might be <strong>the</strong> parietalcortex.The magnocellular <strong>the</strong>ory is not reduced to <strong>the</strong> visual system. Eventhough <strong>the</strong> visual magnocellular pathway has been widely studied, <strong>the</strong>realso exist magnocellular pathways <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r sensory and motor systems,<strong>in</strong> which magnocells are specialised for temporal process<strong>in</strong>g. Forexample, Stoodley et al. (2000) tested sk<strong>in</strong> sensitivity to mechanicalvibration <strong>in</strong> dyslexics and found mild deficits; moreover, reduced tactilesensation <strong>in</strong> dyslexics has been <strong>in</strong>dicated as well (Grant et al., 1999). Even


60 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>though <strong>the</strong>se magnocellular deficits do not affect read<strong>in</strong>g directly, <strong>the</strong>projections from all <strong>the</strong> magnocellular systems are received by <strong>the</strong>cerebellum, which is also known to be important for <strong>the</strong> acquisition ofread<strong>in</strong>g (Ste<strong>in</strong>, 2001).As far as <strong>the</strong> auditory magnocellular system <strong>in</strong> dyslexia is concerned,similar to its visual counterpart, deficits <strong>in</strong> detect<strong>in</strong>g rapidly presentedand chang<strong>in</strong>g stimuli are observable (Bednarek, 1999; Talcott et al.,2000b). As ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed by Ste<strong>in</strong>, Talcott and o<strong>the</strong>rs (Ste<strong>in</strong>, 2001; Talcottet al., 1999, 2000b), <strong>the</strong> ability to differentiate between <strong>the</strong> letter sounds isdependent on <strong>the</strong> capacity to perceive changes <strong>in</strong> sound frequency andamplitude that characterise <strong>the</strong>m. Spoken language occurs at lowfrequency. Thus, sensitivity to low frequency (FM) and amplitudemodulation (AM) (ability to spot slower modulations <strong>in</strong> speech) isnecessary for speech perception and thus for <strong>the</strong> development ofphonological skills. It has been demonstrated that FM (2 Hz) and AMare significantly lower <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia than <strong>in</strong> non-dyslexics,which can possibly expla<strong>in</strong> dyslexic difficulties with phonologicalprocess<strong>in</strong>g. Auditory FM and AM sensitivity <strong>in</strong> dyslexics has beencompared to <strong>the</strong>ir ability to read nonsense words (considered to be <strong>the</strong>purest test of phonological skills) and high correlation has been found.Dyslexics are claimed to require considerably larger changes <strong>in</strong>frequency and amplitude to dist<strong>in</strong>guish between <strong>the</strong>m, however <strong>the</strong>yare good at differentiat<strong>in</strong>g much higher rates of FM (240 Hz), which arenot used for phoneme detection. Aga<strong>in</strong>, as <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> case of <strong>the</strong> visualsystem, people with dyslexia are not poor at all auditory tasks, but <strong>the</strong>ytend to show specific difficulties with modulations that are important fordist<strong>in</strong>guish<strong>in</strong>g letter sounds.Measures of auditory temporal sensitivity correlate with measures ofphonological skill, which might mean that people who are good atdetect<strong>in</strong>g modulations (changes <strong>in</strong> pitch) of low-frequency sounds arebetter at phonological process<strong>in</strong>g (dist<strong>in</strong>guish<strong>in</strong>g and manipulat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>speech sounds) and read<strong>in</strong>g. Whereas measures of visual temporalsensitivity correlate with measures of orthographic skill, thus it mightbe <strong>the</strong> case that people who are good at spott<strong>in</strong>g changes <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> motionof dots across <strong>the</strong> computer screen are better spellers (Bower, 2000).The auditory frequency modulation (FM) sensitivity at 2 Hz but not at240 Hz strongly predicts phonological ability but not orthographic skill,while visual motion sensitivity predicts orthographic skill but notphonological ability (Talcott et al., 2000b). It is fur<strong>the</strong>r argued that eventhough <strong>the</strong>se correlations do not prove a causal relationship, low-levelvisual and auditory function seem likely to be important <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>gto read.Despite solid research, some scholars (Fawcett & Nicolson, 2004) claimthat <strong>the</strong> relationship between <strong>the</strong> magnocellular deficit and read<strong>in</strong>g is not


Causes of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 61transparent, and o<strong>the</strong>rs (Vellut<strong>in</strong>o et al., 2004) consider <strong>the</strong> evidenceprovided by <strong>the</strong> advocates of <strong>the</strong> magnocellular hypo<strong>the</strong>sis as <strong>in</strong>conclusive,equivocal and controversial.Cerebellar Deficit Hypo<strong>the</strong>sisThe advocates of a different concept to expla<strong>in</strong> dyslexic difficultieshave advanced an alternative model to <strong>the</strong> phonological core deficithypo<strong>the</strong>sis (Fawcett & Nicolson, 2001, 2004; Fawcett, 2008). While notdeny<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> widely <strong>in</strong>vestigated and strongly confirmed connectionbetween dyslexic read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties and faulty phonological process<strong>in</strong>g,<strong>the</strong>y propose that problems suffered by children with dyslexia may beattributed to <strong>the</strong> more generally perceived automatisation deficit, capableof caus<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g impairments and poor phonological process<strong>in</strong>g alike.The dyslexic automatisation deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis operates on <strong>the</strong> cognitive leveland can be traced back to <strong>the</strong> biological level, namely, to an underly<strong>in</strong>gcerebellar malfunction. 25Typical signs of cerebellar malfunction such as problems with muscletone and disturbance <strong>in</strong> posture, gait or limb movements can beobserved <strong>in</strong> children with dyslexia. It has been shown (Fawcett et al.,1996; Fawcett & Nicolson, 1999) that dyslexics perform worse than nondyslexicson a wide variety of motor tests, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g balance and o<strong>the</strong>rfunctions that <strong>the</strong> cerebellum is responsible for, thus clear behaviouralevidence of <strong>the</strong> existence of cerebellar abnormalities <strong>in</strong> children withdyslexia has been provided. However, <strong>in</strong> a more recent study (Stoodley,2006), which compared adults with dyslexia and controls on rapidpo<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g and balanc<strong>in</strong>g measures, no significant between-group differenceswere <strong>in</strong>dicated with regard to balanc<strong>in</strong>g tasks; only when speedand accuracy of po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g were comb<strong>in</strong>ed, a slight advantage of controlsover dyslexics was recorded. The authors concluded that <strong>the</strong> tentativeexplanation can be twofold ei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> tasks were too easy for adultswith dyslexia participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> study, or it is possible that with time,balanc<strong>in</strong>g deficits dim<strong>in</strong>ish and <strong>the</strong>y are no longer apparent <strong>in</strong> adultswith dyslexia. If <strong>the</strong> latter is true, <strong>the</strong>n it would mean that balanc<strong>in</strong>gdeficits evident <strong>in</strong> children with dyslexia are <strong>in</strong>deed a sign of adevelopmental delay ra<strong>the</strong>r than a true deficit. There is a need formore research to verify <strong>the</strong>se propositions.Traditionally, <strong>the</strong> cerebellum has been seen as a motor area <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong>learn<strong>in</strong>g and automatisation of motor skills. Recently it has also beenclaimed that <strong>the</strong> cerebellum is important for language-related cognitivetasks, so called language dexterity. Thus, it is <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> automatisationof any skill, whe<strong>the</strong>r motor or cognitive, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g.Learn<strong>in</strong>g a skill fluently, mak<strong>in</strong>g it automatic, means that one canperform it without th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g. These facts have led to <strong>the</strong> formulation of


62 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong><strong>the</strong> automatisation deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis (Nicolson & Fawcett, 1990, 2001),which states that children with dyslexia f<strong>in</strong>d it abnormally difficult tomake any skill automatic (to become expert <strong>in</strong> any skill), despiteextensive practice, regardless of whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> skill is cognitive or motor.Even if children with dyslexia succeed <strong>in</strong> achiev<strong>in</strong>g reasonably goodliteracy skills, <strong>the</strong>ir read<strong>in</strong>g rema<strong>in</strong>s slower, more effortful and lessautomatic than normal readers of <strong>the</strong> same age. The quality of <strong>the</strong>automatised performance is lower, dyslexics are less skilled and <strong>the</strong>irperformance on any task is comparable with that of much youngerchildren (Fawcett & Nicolson, 2004). What is more, <strong>the</strong> difficulties aregreater when <strong>the</strong> time needed to master <strong>the</strong> skill <strong>in</strong>creases. 26 Even though<strong>the</strong> behaviour of children with dyslexia appears normal, <strong>the</strong>y experiencelapses of concentration and get tired more quickly than o<strong>the</strong>r childrenwhile perform<strong>in</strong>g a given activity. Hence <strong>the</strong> suggestion that dyslexicsare able to perform at normal levels by consciously compensat<strong>in</strong>g(concentrat<strong>in</strong>g, controll<strong>in</strong>g) dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> performance of <strong>the</strong> skill thatmight normally be automatic. A difference <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g rate has also beenobserved between dyslexics and controls, if a skill takes a typical child100 hours to master, it would take a dyslexic child 1000 hours to acquire<strong>the</strong> same level of performance of <strong>the</strong> skill. The dyslexic automatisationhypo<strong>the</strong>sis provides for read<strong>in</strong>g problems and also for deficit <strong>in</strong>phonological and motor skills and rapid process<strong>in</strong>g (Fawcett & Nicolson,2001). It also offers an explanation why everyth<strong>in</strong>g needs to be madeexplicit <strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g a child with dyslexia, while non-dyslexic children canpick up <strong>the</strong> skill without effort (Fawcett & Nicolson, 2004).An ontogenetic causal cha<strong>in</strong> proposed by Fawcett and Nicolson (2001,2004) highlights <strong>the</strong> connection between cerebellar abnormalities, phonologicalproblems and eventual difficulties <strong>in</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g, read<strong>in</strong>g andspell<strong>in</strong>g. It <strong>in</strong>dicates how deficits <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> structure can lead, viadisorders <strong>in</strong> cognitive processes such as automatisation and phonology,to read<strong>in</strong>g disorder. It provides an account of patterns of difficultiesencountered by dyslexics and shows how <strong>the</strong>y arise developmentally.Children with dyslexia show poor quality handwrit<strong>in</strong>g, which can beexpla<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> view of <strong>the</strong> cerebellar deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis as a motor skillthat requires precise tim<strong>in</strong>g and coord<strong>in</strong>ation of muscles. It is fur<strong>the</strong>rargued that cerebellar deficit <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>fants would first manifest by mildmotor difficulty <strong>in</strong> sitt<strong>in</strong>g, walk<strong>in</strong>g, muscle control and articulation (<strong>the</strong>most complex motor skill), result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> later start as far as babbl<strong>in</strong>g andtalk<strong>in</strong>g are concerned. Moreover, even after <strong>the</strong> development of speechand walk<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>the</strong>se skills rema<strong>in</strong> less fluent than <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>fants withoutany cerebellar impairment. Consequently, articulation and speech that isless fluent than normal may lead to less complete process<strong>in</strong>g of auditory,phonemic structure of spoken words, br<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g about <strong>the</strong> loss of onset,rime and <strong>the</strong> phonemic structure of language. Spell<strong>in</strong>g difficulties arise


Causes of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 63from over-effortful read<strong>in</strong>g, poor phonological awareness and difficulties<strong>in</strong> automatis<strong>in</strong>g skills. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> proponents of <strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sis <strong>in</strong>question, <strong>in</strong> order to m<strong>in</strong>imise read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g difficulties, <strong>the</strong><strong>in</strong>volved subskills should be automatised through specialised teach<strong>in</strong>g,with <strong>the</strong> use of carefully designed, monitored and long-term tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gprogrammes.Altoge<strong>the</strong>r, cerebellar deficit would be responsible for <strong>the</strong> phonologicalprocess<strong>in</strong>g deficit that toge<strong>the</strong>r with <strong>the</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g and automatisationdeficit lead to read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g difficulties. However, as seems to be<strong>the</strong> case with any o<strong>the</strong>r, more or less popular, assumption concern<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>possible underly<strong>in</strong>g causes of dyslexia, <strong>the</strong> cerebellar deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sisalso <strong>in</strong>vited considerable criticism from scholars deal<strong>in</strong>g with dyslexia.For <strong>in</strong>stance, Savage’s (2007) f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs discredit Fawcett et al.’s (2001)claim that <strong>the</strong> cerebellar deficit is specific to dyslexia and hence capableof dist<strong>in</strong>guish<strong>in</strong>g between groups of children with dyslexia and readerswithout IQ discrepancy, more precisely, children with moderate <strong>in</strong>tellectualdisability. In light of <strong>the</strong>se f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, <strong>the</strong> value of IQ measures <strong>in</strong>dyslexia assessment, argued <strong>in</strong> favour of by Fawcett et al. (2001),apparently loses its potential, s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>the</strong> cerebellar deficit appears not tobe reserved solely for this group.The scale and scope of research devoted to discover<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>gcause of dyslexia is beyond doubt impressive. Complicated and variableneurobiological patterns have been revealed. Apparently, <strong>the</strong> diversity ofbehavioural <strong>in</strong>dications of dyslexia does add to <strong>the</strong> complexity of <strong>the</strong>nature of <strong>the</strong> neurological and cognitive basis of <strong>the</strong> disorder. It seemsobvious that a massive amount of research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, quite frequentlycontroversial or <strong>in</strong>conclusive, <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g anatomical, physiological andbehavioural <strong>in</strong>vestigations, requires <strong>in</strong>tegrat<strong>in</strong>g with evidence of geneticand cultural <strong>in</strong>fluences as well. Altoge<strong>the</strong>r, it seems that a comprehensivecausal explanation of dyslexia, <strong>in</strong>corporat<strong>in</strong>g a huge bulk of scientificfacts, still rema<strong>in</strong>s a matter for <strong>the</strong> future.Notes1. The term cognitive refers here to <strong>the</strong> level between <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> and behaviour;<strong>the</strong> level of ‘m<strong>in</strong>d’ or ‘mental processes’ (Hulme & Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2009).2. It is implied (Ste<strong>in</strong>, 2001; Ste<strong>in</strong> et al., 2001) that at least three genes may beimportant, one for orthography alone, one for phonological skills alone andone controll<strong>in</strong>g l<strong>in</strong>ked orthographic and phonological abilities. Moreover,<strong>the</strong>re seems to be noticeable agreement that <strong>the</strong> gene responsible forphonological and orthographic control is located on <strong>the</strong> short arm ofchromosome 6 (Ste<strong>in</strong> et al., 2001).3. For <strong>in</strong>stance, electrophysiological methods, positron emission tomography(PET), functional magnetic resonance imag<strong>in</strong>g (fMRI), magnetic sourceimag<strong>in</strong>g (MSI) and magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS).


64 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>4. More precisely, what can be successfully captured dur<strong>in</strong>g cognitive process<strong>in</strong>gare, for <strong>in</strong>stance, <strong>the</strong> metabolic changes reflected by glucose utilisation orblood flow shifts from one part of <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> to ano<strong>the</strong>r, as measured by PET orfMRI (Richards, 2001).5. For example, Hadzibeganovic et al. (<strong>in</strong> press) <strong>in</strong>dicate <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> regions ofabnormal function and atypical structure <strong>in</strong> dyslexics identified <strong>in</strong> recentneuroimag<strong>in</strong>g studies with regard to alphabetic languages. Among <strong>the</strong>enumerated regions, <strong>the</strong>re are <strong>the</strong> left temporoparietal areas, claimed to be<strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> letter-to-sound decod<strong>in</strong>g dur<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>the</strong> left middlesuperiortemporal cortex, tak<strong>in</strong>g part <strong>in</strong> speech sound analysis, and, f<strong>in</strong>ally,<strong>the</strong> left <strong>in</strong>ferior temporo-occipital gyrus, participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> quick wordrecognition.6. For <strong>in</strong>stance, volumetric grey matter.7. ESL learners learn English as a second language; EAL (EFL) learners learn English as an additional (foreign) language.8. The planum temporale is a region located <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> superior temporal gyrus,between Heschl’s sulcus anteriorly and <strong>the</strong> end of <strong>the</strong> Sylvian fissureposteriorly (Hugdahl et al., 2003).9. The planum temporale is normally bigger <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere <strong>in</strong> nondyslexics,while <strong>in</strong> people with dyslexia it is of similar size <strong>in</strong> both cerebralhemispheres.10. The dichotic listen<strong>in</strong>g test constitutes a functional measure of phonologicalprocess<strong>in</strong>g. It allows <strong>the</strong> evaluation of <strong>the</strong> auditory process<strong>in</strong>g of verbal<strong>in</strong>put <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> temporal lobe. It entails simultaneous presentation of twodifferent compet<strong>in</strong>g auditory stimuli, one to each ear. Typically, a REA<strong>in</strong>dicates left hemisphere dom<strong>in</strong>ance for phonological process<strong>in</strong>g. In o<strong>the</strong>rwords, when report<strong>in</strong>g what <strong>the</strong>y have heard, <strong>in</strong>dividuals with left hemispherelanguage lateralisation more accurately repeat verbal stimuli presentedto <strong>the</strong> right ear.11. O<strong>the</strong>r studies have also po<strong>in</strong>ted to <strong>the</strong> existence of reduced or absent REA <strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia encounter<strong>in</strong>g phonological difficulties, e.g. Bloomand Hynd (2005).12. The left planum temporale was significantly larger than <strong>the</strong> right one <strong>in</strong>dyslexics and controls. However, <strong>the</strong> left planum temporale appearedsignificantly smaller (9%) <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> dyslexic group, whereas <strong>the</strong> right planumtemporale was similar <strong>in</strong> size <strong>in</strong> both groups. Aga<strong>in</strong>, both groups demonstrateda significant REA, however, right ear scores <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> dyslexic groupwere reduced as compared to controls, while <strong>the</strong> left ear score resembled <strong>the</strong>scores ga<strong>in</strong>ed by <strong>the</strong> control group. It has been suggested that normal REA<strong>in</strong> subjects with dyslexia, despite a smaller left planum temporale, meansthat <strong>the</strong> planum temporale is not unique for phonological process<strong>in</strong>g.13. Ectopias are def<strong>in</strong>ed as small areas of <strong>in</strong>adequately placed neurons clusteredaround <strong>the</strong> temporoparietal junction; <strong>the</strong>y consist of about 50100 neuronsthat missed <strong>the</strong>ir target <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> cortex dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> course of neural migration.Dysplasias described as focal malformations of <strong>the</strong> architecture of <strong>the</strong>cortex, have been shown to be bilaterally located; never<strong>the</strong>less, <strong>the</strong>ir numberwas greater <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere <strong>in</strong> all <strong>the</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>ed bra<strong>in</strong>s (Ramus, 2004).14. The tissue connect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> two cerebral hemispheres, allow<strong>in</strong>g communicationand flow of <strong>in</strong>formation between <strong>the</strong>m; <strong>the</strong> largest neural pathwayconnect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> two cerebral hemispheres. It is built of several partsconnect<strong>in</strong>g particular areas of <strong>the</strong> left and right cerebral cortex.


Causes of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 6515. See <strong>the</strong> section ‘Magnocellular deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis’ for more <strong>in</strong>formation on<strong>the</strong> magnocellular deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis <strong>in</strong> dyslexia.16. The cerebellum is a subcortical bra<strong>in</strong> structure at <strong>the</strong> back of <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong>.17. See <strong>the</strong> section ‘Cerebellar deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis’ for more <strong>in</strong>formation on <strong>the</strong>cerebellar deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis <strong>in</strong> dyslexia.18. Neuroimag<strong>in</strong>g studies of phonological process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dicate differences <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>left temporoparietal cortex <strong>in</strong> adults and children with dyslexia whencompared to non-impaired controls (Shaywitz et al., 2003; Temple et al.,2001).19. See Chapter 1: ‘Read<strong>in</strong>g strategies and stages of read<strong>in</strong>g development’20. See Chapter 1: ‘Orthographic depth and gra<strong>in</strong> size: A cross languageperspective on read<strong>in</strong>g and dyslexia’.21. The stimulus analysis <strong>in</strong> perceptual systems is conducted sequentially(hierarchically), it means that <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation sent from <strong>the</strong> lower levels of<strong>the</strong> central nervous system is fur<strong>the</strong>r analysed and <strong>in</strong>tegrated on <strong>the</strong> higherlevels. In <strong>the</strong>se systems, <strong>the</strong>re exist considerably <strong>in</strong>dependent pathways,specialis<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> transmitt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation concern<strong>in</strong>g different features of agiven stimulus (Grabowska, 1997). At all levels of <strong>the</strong> visual system, twosubsystems of cells, which fulfil different roles <strong>in</strong> analys<strong>in</strong>g visual stimuli,can be separated. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong>ir anatomical structure, <strong>the</strong>se subsystemsare called magnocellular and parvocellular (Bednarek, 1999; Bogdanowicz,1999). Thus, <strong>the</strong> ret<strong>in</strong>o-cortical visual stream consists of magno (larger) andparvo (smaller) cells, which transmit signals (electrical impulses) from <strong>the</strong>ret<strong>in</strong>a <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> eye to <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> (Grabowska, 1997; Ste<strong>in</strong>, 2001; Ste<strong>in</strong> et al., 2001;Talcott et al., 2000a). The parvocellular system is responsible for <strong>the</strong> analysisof details, recognition of small shapes, perception of colours, and clear, sharpvision, while <strong>the</strong> magnocellular system deals with peripheral vision(react<strong>in</strong>g over a larger area), perception of rapidly presented, rapidlychang<strong>in</strong>g and mov<strong>in</strong>g stimuli, it is sensitive to contrast and lum<strong>in</strong>ance(Bednarek, 1999; Grabowska, 1997) and low spatial frequencies (Grabowska,1997). Magnocells project to <strong>the</strong> primary visual area <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> occipital cortex(Ste<strong>in</strong>, 2001).22. Experiments on monkeys have demonstrated that detect<strong>in</strong>g coherent motion<strong>in</strong> a display of dots mov<strong>in</strong>g around randomly (random dot k<strong>in</strong>ematogram)proves to be a sensitive test for prob<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> whole magnocellular system.23. See <strong>the</strong> section: ‘Bra<strong>in</strong> mechanisms <strong>in</strong> dyslexia’ on bra<strong>in</strong> abnormalities <strong>in</strong>dyslexia.24. Words that sound <strong>the</strong> same but have different spell<strong>in</strong>g.25. See <strong>the</strong> section: ‘Bra<strong>in</strong> mechanisms <strong>in</strong> dyslexia’ on bra<strong>in</strong> abnormalities <strong>in</strong>dyslexia.26. Impairment is said to <strong>in</strong>crease as <strong>the</strong> square root of <strong>the</strong> necessary learn<strong>in</strong>gtime.


Chapter 3<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong>Learn<strong>in</strong>gNative <strong>Language</strong>-based <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong>Learn<strong>in</strong>g DifficultiesObviously, familiarity with foreign languages is a must <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>multil<strong>in</strong>gual society we live <strong>in</strong> today. Thus, a foreign language requirementhas to be fulfilled at schools and universities, pos<strong>in</strong>g a substantialburden on a number of students, who repeatedly demonstrate vary<strong>in</strong>gdegrees of difficulty <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g a foreign language, quite frequently at<strong>the</strong> same time do<strong>in</strong>g well at o<strong>the</strong>r courses. Many explanations have beenproposed and debated <strong>in</strong> order to expla<strong>in</strong> this phenomenon. Suggestions<strong>in</strong>cluded lack of foreign language aptitude, poor attitude, low motivation,high levels of anxiety, failure to use appropriate learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies,mismatch of teacher-student learn<strong>in</strong>g styles, much less personalityvariables. Seem<strong>in</strong>gly, some correlational l<strong>in</strong>kages between affectivevariables and successful foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g appeared to bestrong. However, no s<strong>in</strong>gle variable or set of variables have been provedto account for successful or unsuccessful foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g.<strong>Foreign</strong> language researchers have not managed to explicitly specify anyclear pattern of <strong>the</strong> relationship between foreign language achievementand cognitive, attitud<strong>in</strong>al and personality variables (Brown, 2000; Ellis,1994; Lundberg, 2002; Sparks, 1995, 2006).In <strong>the</strong> 1960s, Carroll attributed <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> foreignlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> overall language ability of <strong>the</strong> learner and tovariables related to <strong>in</strong>struction. Pimsleur observed that foreign languageunderachievers tend to demonstrate poor sound discrim<strong>in</strong>ation skillsand hardness <strong>in</strong> sound-symbol learn<strong>in</strong>g, responsible for <strong>the</strong> foreignlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g differences that could not be expla<strong>in</strong>ed by lowmotivation or <strong>in</strong>telligence. D<strong>in</strong>klage documented cases of studentsachiev<strong>in</strong>g well at o<strong>the</strong>r courses, yet fail<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ir foreign languagecomponent at Harvard University. Aga<strong>in</strong>, <strong>the</strong> failure could not be tracedback to lack of motivation or poor attitude, as <strong>the</strong>ir attempts to performwell <strong>in</strong> foreign language courses and positive stance were evident.D<strong>in</strong>klage was <strong>the</strong> first to suggest that <strong>the</strong> h<strong>in</strong>drance experienced by thosestudents resembled dyslexic problems, namely, difficulties <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g toread and spell, occurrence of letter/symbol reversals, sound confusions,poor discrim<strong>in</strong>ation of sounds <strong>in</strong> a foreign language and verbal memorydeficit (Ganschow et al., 1998; Schneider, 1999; Sparks, 1995; Sparks et al.,66


<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 671989, 1991, 1992b, 1995b). In <strong>the</strong> 1980s, a plausible bond between foreignlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties and problems with native languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g was put forward. Apparently, foreign language learners withspecific learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties share various aspects of language function<strong>in</strong>gthat might negatively <strong>in</strong>fluence <strong>the</strong>ir ability to learn a foreignlanguage (Sparks et al., 1989). Skehan believes that second or foreignlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g is <strong>the</strong> equivalent of <strong>the</strong> first language learn<strong>in</strong>g facultyand children who develop faster <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir first language also score higheron foreign language aptitude tests (Ganschow & Sparks, 1995; Schneider,1999; Sparks et al., 1995b). It is claimed that successful foreign languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g draws on <strong>in</strong>tact language skills. Thus, logically, foreignlanguage acquisition can be blocked by any physiological or biologicaldeterrents that handicap <strong>the</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g of one’s native language (Sparkset al., 1995b; Spolsky, 1989).Ganschow and Sparks (1986) have presented detailed case studies offour college students, who faced foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g problemsmost probably related to <strong>the</strong>ir native language learn<strong>in</strong>g impairment.Gajar (1987) compared a group of students with identified specificlearn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties with a control foreign language-enrolled groupfor <strong>the</strong>ir aptitude for learn<strong>in</strong>g a foreign language, measured by <strong>the</strong>Modern <strong>Language</strong> Aptitude Test (MLAT; Carroll & Sapon, 1959). Specificlearn<strong>in</strong>g-disabled students performed significantly more poorly than<strong>the</strong> control group on all five subtests of <strong>the</strong> MLAT, namely, auditorycomprehension <strong>in</strong> a listen<strong>in</strong>g memory task, sound-symbol associationability, vocabulary knowledge, sensitivity to grammatical structureand visual rote memory for words. This emerg<strong>in</strong>g body of evidencesuggested that students with foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties hadearly histories of language problems, which were ei<strong>the</strong>r unrecognised orcompensated for due to <strong>the</strong>ir high IQ and remedial help (Sparks et al.,1989).In <strong>the</strong> same ve<strong>in</strong>, Chodkiewicz (1986) highlights <strong>the</strong> fact that<strong>in</strong>dividuals struggl<strong>in</strong>g with read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir native language are proneto face failure <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir attempts to become fluent <strong>in</strong> foreign languages. Aclose <strong>in</strong>terdependence between <strong>the</strong> ability to read <strong>in</strong> one’s native andforeign language is suggested. Thus, <strong>in</strong>dividuals who are low-gradereaders <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir native language are very likely to read poorly <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>foreign language, while, conversely, good readers apply <strong>the</strong>ir competenciesequally well <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> native as well as foreign language.Kahn-Horwitz et al. (2006) borrow <strong>the</strong> notion of <strong>the</strong> so called ‘Mat<strong>the</strong>weffects’ from native language (L1) read<strong>in</strong>g research (Stanovich, 1986) andaptly use it to characterise weak English as a foreign language (EFL)readers. Possess<strong>in</strong>g stronger read<strong>in</strong>g subskills such as, for example,phonological awareness and knowledge of letter sounds and names at <strong>the</strong>beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g stages of learn<strong>in</strong>g to read <strong>in</strong> a second/foreign language (L2)


68 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>typically assures faster and more accurate L2 read<strong>in</strong>g acquisition. This, <strong>in</strong>turn, by and large secures greater read<strong>in</strong>g experience, almost <strong>in</strong>variablynecessary to produce skilled readers. Be<strong>in</strong>g disabled with respect to <strong>the</strong>abovementioned decod<strong>in</strong>g skills puts children at an obvious disadvantage,s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>the</strong>ir slow and laborious read<strong>in</strong>g experience results <strong>in</strong>difficulties with comprehension, decreases motivation and, f<strong>in</strong>ally, makes<strong>the</strong>m lag beh<strong>in</strong>d good foreign language (FL) readers. Additionally,develop<strong>in</strong>g an ability to read <strong>in</strong> a foreign language is believed to triggerprogress of <strong>the</strong> parallel skill <strong>in</strong> a native language and vice versa. However,such a transfer can take place after a learner has reached a certa<strong>in</strong> level ofproficiency <strong>in</strong> a foreign language (Chodkiewicz, 1986). It seems that <strong>in</strong>this way a vicious circle closes, unless learners with dyslexia are providedwith appropriate tutor<strong>in</strong>g assistance.S<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>the</strong> 1980s, Sparks et al. have conducted pioneer<strong>in</strong>g research <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong> matter of foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties of <strong>in</strong>dividuals forwhom read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir native language constituted anobvious challenge. Among <strong>the</strong>se poor FL learners, <strong>the</strong>re are studentsclassified as learn<strong>in</strong>g disabled (LD)/dyslexia and low-achiev<strong>in</strong>g (at-risk)students without diagnosis towards LD. In 1989, Sparks and Ganschow(Ganschow et al., 1998; Ganschow & Sparks, 2000, 2001; Sparks, 1995;Sparks et al., 1989; Sparks & Ganschow, 1991) proposed <strong>the</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guisticcod<strong>in</strong>g deficits hypo<strong>the</strong>sis (LCDH) as a model for expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g languageproblems encountered by poor foreign language learners. They revived<strong>the</strong> speculations of Pimsleur, Carroll and o<strong>the</strong>rs about <strong>the</strong> salience of <strong>the</strong>native language skills, language aptitude differences, and, particularly,phonological process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g. It is claimed thatpoor foreign language learners have <strong>in</strong> common a disability <strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guisticcod<strong>in</strong>g. There are three types of l<strong>in</strong>guistic cod<strong>in</strong>g deficits: phonological(<strong>in</strong>volves identify<strong>in</strong>g and dist<strong>in</strong>guish<strong>in</strong>g between speech sounds andprocess<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> sound/symbol connections), syntactic (<strong>in</strong>volves understand<strong>in</strong>gand apply<strong>in</strong>g grammatical, structural concepts of a languagesystem) and semantic (connected with understand<strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>gs). Phonologicaland syntactic tasks are fundamental to language acquisition <strong>in</strong>its earliest stages, whereas semantic undertak<strong>in</strong>gs depend on <strong>the</strong>conceptual understand<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> messages conveyed through languageunits (Sparks et al., 1989). The model proposed by Sparks and Ganschowis based on f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> native language research on read<strong>in</strong>g disabilitiesby Vellut<strong>in</strong>o and Scanlon (Ganschow & Sparks, 1995; Sparks et al., 1989),who claim that poor readers exhibit difficulties with phonological andsyntactic aspects of <strong>the</strong>ir native language, with phonological cod<strong>in</strong>gbe<strong>in</strong>g especially weak.To recapitulate, LCDH puts forward an idea that foreign languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g is built on native language skills, that phonological/orthographic,syntactic and semantic competences <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> native language form


<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 69<strong>the</strong> foundation for foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g (and FL aptitude). Thus, <strong>the</strong>strength of <strong>the</strong> native language codes considerably determ<strong>in</strong>es <strong>the</strong> extentto which a learner can become proficient <strong>in</strong> a foreign language. Weak L1skills tend to <strong>in</strong>hibit FL proficiency development. Students with strongernative language skills will demonstrate higher FL proficiency andachievement than students with weaker native language skills. In asimilar ve<strong>in</strong>, poor L1 readers are expected to transpose low automaticityand efficiency to L2 read<strong>in</strong>g.It is also speculated that both native and foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>gdepend on basic language learn<strong>in</strong>g mechanisms, moreover problemswith one language skill are likely to have a negative effect on both <strong>the</strong>native and foreign language systems. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, it is assumed that <strong>the</strong>majority of poor foreign language learners experience most difficultywith <strong>the</strong> phonological/orthographic rule system of L2. LCDH predom<strong>in</strong>antlyfocuses on l<strong>in</strong>guistic variables because foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>gis <strong>the</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g of language (Ganschow et al., 1998; Ganschow & Sparks,1995; Sparks, 1995; Sparks et al., 1989; Sparks & Ganschow, 1991).The comparison between good and poor language learners shows thatpoor students show significant degrees of variation ra<strong>the</strong>r than deficits <strong>in</strong>language performance. As already mentioned, it especially perta<strong>in</strong>s to<strong>the</strong> tasks that place specific demands on phonological process<strong>in</strong>g, with<strong>in</strong>which <strong>the</strong> ability to isolate and manipulate consciously <strong>the</strong> sounds of <strong>the</strong>language phonemic awareness 1 tends to be particularly deficient.Measures of phonological awareness appear to be significant predictorsof <strong>the</strong> disparities between children with and without impaired read<strong>in</strong>g(Sparks, 1995). Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Sparks (Sparks, 1995; Sparks et al., 1989),students with obstructions <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> phonological component of <strong>the</strong>ir nativelanguage are likely to confront an immediate hurdle <strong>in</strong> foreign languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g.The native language difficulties of at-risk learners may be overt orsubtle; <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> latter case, <strong>the</strong>y may only be apparent <strong>in</strong> one languagecode phonological/orthographic, syntactic or semantic. Four prototypesof poor foreign language learners, characterised by diversel<strong>in</strong>guistic profiles, have been described. Weak phonology, average orstrong syntax and strong semantics constitute one prototype; <strong>the</strong> secondprototype is characterised by strong phonology, average or strong syntaxand weak semantics; <strong>the</strong> third has weak phonology, syntax andsemantics; and <strong>the</strong> last prototype entails average to strong phonology,syntax and semantics, but low motivation and/or high anxiety. The mostcommonly occurr<strong>in</strong>g comb<strong>in</strong>ation is <strong>the</strong> first prototype (with weakphonology) (Ganschow & Sparks, 1995; Sparks, 1995).As already <strong>in</strong>dicated, Spark and Ganschow (Sparks et al., 1995b) suggestthat foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g problems of students at secondary andpost-secondary level may be due to earlier problems with phonological/


70 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>orthographic process<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong>ir native language. It is fur<strong>the</strong>r hypo<strong>the</strong>sisedthat even though <strong>the</strong>se students may be able to compensate for <strong>the</strong>irphonological/orthographic process<strong>in</strong>g problems and achieve average orabove-average grades <strong>in</strong> most school subjects, when it comes to learn<strong>in</strong>g anew sound-symbol system of a foreign language, <strong>the</strong> difficulties withphonological/orthographic process<strong>in</strong>g re-emerge.Bear<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d that <strong>the</strong>se difficulties may hamper <strong>the</strong> process of FLlearn<strong>in</strong>g to a lesser or greater extent, one might, quite naturally, consideran extreme case and ask whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y are capable of mak<strong>in</strong>g a decentcommand of a foreign language a totally unatta<strong>in</strong>able goal. In o<strong>the</strong>rwords, should we conceptualise <strong>the</strong> notion of an absolute <strong>in</strong>ability tolearn a foreign language as a plausible explanation of FL learn<strong>in</strong>gdifficulties <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> cases of LD students, and only those students?<strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disability versus Cont<strong>in</strong>uumNotion of <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g DifferencesThe aim of this section is to provide arguments, supported byevidence from research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, for <strong>the</strong> negative answer to <strong>the</strong> question,whe<strong>the</strong>r a disability for FL learn<strong>in</strong>g (FLLD) can be perceived and treatedas a dist<strong>in</strong>ct type of disability. On <strong>the</strong> contrary, <strong>the</strong> position that languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g ability, with reference to LD as well as non-LD (low-achiev<strong>in</strong>g)students, exists on a cont<strong>in</strong>uum, with <strong>the</strong> foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>gdifficulties rang<strong>in</strong>g from mild to severe, is advocated. It seems reasonableto expect that <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> majority of cases, learners with dyslexia (LD) couldbe placed on this cont<strong>in</strong>uum more towards its severe end.Sparks et al. (1989) <strong>in</strong>itially termed <strong>the</strong>ir hypo<strong>the</strong>sis, <strong>the</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guisticcod<strong>in</strong>g deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis, and concentrated on describ<strong>in</strong>g barriers thatstudents with specific learn<strong>in</strong>g disabilities (SLD) encounter <strong>in</strong> foreignlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g. SLD students are claimed to face subtle or overtdeficits <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> oral and written aspects of <strong>the</strong>ir native language and toexhibit a discrepancy between <strong>the</strong>ir measured <strong>in</strong>tellectual ability (IQ) andspecific areas of academic achievement (e.g. read<strong>in</strong>g, spell<strong>in</strong>g, ma<strong>the</strong>matics).Then, Sparks et al. expanded <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>in</strong>terest onto a large group ofstudents without SLD, who, never<strong>the</strong>less, experience substantial trouble<strong>in</strong> foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g. The term, at-risk foreign language learners,has been <strong>in</strong>troduced to refer to students with identified learn<strong>in</strong>gdisabilities, most of whom experience obstructions <strong>in</strong> foreign languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g, and students who are not ascerta<strong>in</strong>ed as LD but still strugglewith substantial pitfalls <strong>in</strong> foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> formalclassroom environment (Ganschow & Sparks, 1995). S<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> USA <strong>the</strong>study of foreign languages does not usually start until high school, <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong>ir studies, Sparks and colleagues have concentrated on <strong>the</strong>se olderstudents.


<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 71The name, l<strong>in</strong>guistic cod<strong>in</strong>g deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis, was changed to <strong>the</strong>l<strong>in</strong>guistic cod<strong>in</strong>g differences hypo<strong>the</strong>sis (LCDH) <strong>in</strong> 1994 (Ganschow et al.,1998), firstly, <strong>in</strong> order to highlight <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> basiclanguage propensities; secondly, to focus attention on <strong>the</strong> fact thatlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g ability exists on a cont<strong>in</strong>uum, with foreign languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties rang<strong>in</strong>g from mild to severe; and thirdly, to placeemphasis on <strong>the</strong> fact that <strong>the</strong>re is no such phenomenon as a foreignlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g disability (FLLD). Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Schneider (1999), <strong>the</strong>differences between poor LD and non-LD foreign language learnersperta<strong>in</strong> to <strong>the</strong> degree of complication and appear primarily <strong>in</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>gand performance on foreign language aptitude tests. On <strong>the</strong> proposedcont<strong>in</strong>uum, <strong>the</strong> students with diagnosed LD constitute <strong>the</strong> most severelyaffected cases.Sparks (2006) proposes conv<strong>in</strong>c<strong>in</strong>g arguments aga<strong>in</strong>st <strong>the</strong> existence ofa dist<strong>in</strong>ct entity, such as a disability for FL learn<strong>in</strong>g (FLLD), as a notionseparate from terms more neutral <strong>in</strong> character, for example, learners withFL learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties, or at-risk, poor, low-achiev<strong>in</strong>g FL learners. Theterm foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g disability (FLLD) seems to have ga<strong>in</strong>ed someacceptance over <strong>the</strong> past couple of years, <strong>in</strong> both learn<strong>in</strong>g disabilities andforeign learn<strong>in</strong>g field, and even when it is not used explicitly, a relationbetween LD and FL difficulties is apparently implied (e.g. Shaw, 1999;Reed & Stansfield, 2004; Smith, 2002). In <strong>the</strong>ir earlier work on FL learn<strong>in</strong>gproblems, Sparks and colleagues (e.g. Ganschow & Sparks, 1986, 1987;Ganschow et al., 1991) also <strong>the</strong>orised about a plausible explanation of <strong>the</strong>abovementioned difficulties <strong>in</strong> terms of an assumed existence of <strong>the</strong> l<strong>in</strong>kbetween a classification of LD and FL learn<strong>in</strong>g problems. However,hav<strong>in</strong>g conducted a number of studies <strong>in</strong> order to verify <strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sisedconnection, <strong>the</strong>y revised <strong>the</strong>ir stance on this matter and acknowledgedthat research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs had not confirmed <strong>the</strong>ir speculations, andthat <strong>the</strong> earlier use of <strong>the</strong> term FLLD was unjustified and <strong>in</strong>correct(Sparks, 2006; Sparks et al., 2002, 2003).It turned out that secondary and post-secondary students, bothclassified as hav<strong>in</strong>g LD, and poor FL learners without LD diagnosis,signed on for FL courses, rarely significantly differed with respect tocognitive, native language and FL aptitude measures (e.g. Sparks et al.,1996, 1998b). LD students did not always experience problems with FLlearn<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> fact, many of <strong>the</strong>m easily got credits <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir FL courses,complet<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m with little or no obvious difficulty (Sparks & Javorsky,1999; Sparks et al., 2002, 2003), while many students who could notformally be classified as hav<strong>in</strong>g LD experienced difficulty and failure <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong>ir attempts to complete FL requirements. Additionally, it has beenshown that LD students do not substantially differ from non-LD lowachiev<strong>in</strong>gstudents when it comes to <strong>the</strong> severity of <strong>the</strong> FL problems


72 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong><strong>the</strong>y experience or <strong>the</strong>ir language learn<strong>in</strong>g profiles (Sparks et al., 2002,2003, 2006).Ferrari and Pallad<strong>in</strong>o (2007) f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong> results described above concern<strong>in</strong>grelative similarity between LD and non-LD high-risk FL studentssomewhat mislead<strong>in</strong>g. On <strong>the</strong> one hand, low-achiev<strong>in</strong>g students identifiedas at-risk for FL learn<strong>in</strong>g problems lack formal (and former)diagnosis of LD, on <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, <strong>the</strong>y closely resemble LD studentswith regard to phonological and syntactic difficulties. Potentially, <strong>the</strong>answer may lie <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> analysis of FL learn<strong>in</strong>g profiles demonstrated byhigh-risk students, with <strong>the</strong> most frequently and consistently emerg<strong>in</strong>gpoor phonological and syntactic abilities but ra<strong>the</strong>r average semanticfaculty. In addition, age could be a factor as well. S<strong>in</strong>ce most of <strong>the</strong> studiesby Sparks et al. concentrate on young adults, high school or collegestudents, arguably, it seems a challeng<strong>in</strong>g task to actually reconstruct<strong>in</strong>dividual’s development and native learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties, which mighthave been compensated to some extent by <strong>the</strong> time <strong>the</strong>y advanced <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>iracademic career to <strong>the</strong> level of high school or college. Thus, track<strong>in</strong>gdown <strong>the</strong>ir FL learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties to poor phonological and syntacticabilities <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir native language poses a considerable problem.The abovementioned research outcomes <strong>in</strong>deed seem to run counter to<strong>the</strong> commonly accepted proposition that students with LD struggle withmarkedly more severe native and foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g problemsthan non-LD learners. The controversy may have also been caused by <strong>the</strong>lack of a congruous, unambiguously operationalised and empiricallywell-grounded def<strong>in</strong>ition of LD (Sparks et al., 1995a, 1998a, 2002, 2003,2006, 2008a) that would allow <strong>the</strong> diagnosis of a large number of childrenwho do not function well <strong>in</strong> regular classroom sett<strong>in</strong>gs and do not exhibito<strong>the</strong>r malfunctions such as mental retardation, visual or hear<strong>in</strong>gimpairments, or physical handicaps as LD. The lack of scientificallysound criteria for <strong>the</strong> identification of LD has been very problematic andhas led to <strong>the</strong> formation of heterogeneous research samples of LDstudents (Sparks et al., 1995a, 1995c, 1998b). For example, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir study,Sparks et al. (2003) report that at least 50% of LD students did not meetei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> legal or research criteria for LD classification. The fact wasattributed to problematic def<strong>in</strong>ition and classification system of LD that,quite logically, produces problematic students’ classifications as LD.Although poor foreign language learners exhibit statistically significantnative language and foreign language dissimilarities when comparedto good foreign language learners, <strong>the</strong>ir performance, as already<strong>in</strong>dicated, may still be <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> average to low-average range (Sparks, 1995).The analysis of <strong>the</strong> grades of LD foreign language students showed that anumber of <strong>the</strong>m had successfully passed at least one foreign languagecourse <strong>in</strong> college. The majority of petition students also admit hav<strong>in</strong>gpassed FL courses <strong>in</strong> high school, often with average and above-average


<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 73grades. Thus, one might wonder why <strong>the</strong>se students had been able tofulfil <strong>the</strong> FL requirement <strong>in</strong> high school and <strong>the</strong>n, surpris<strong>in</strong>gly, weregranted FL course substitutions or waivers <strong>in</strong> college. Accord<strong>in</strong>g toSparks et al. (2003, 2008b), <strong>the</strong>re exist several unfounded assumptionsadopted by students, diagnosticians, university officials and disabilityservice providers. The prevail<strong>in</strong>g belief among LD students anddiagnosticians is that <strong>the</strong>se students are bound to confront excessivedifficulty with FL learn<strong>in</strong>g and are most likely to fail FL courses.Moreover, diagnosticians may falsely assume that students of averageand above-average <strong>in</strong>telligence, who struggle with and achieve lowgrades <strong>in</strong> FL courses, must be LD, notwithstand<strong>in</strong>g high scores obta<strong>in</strong>edon tests verify<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ir native language ability. Even more importantly,<strong>the</strong>re is a possibility that educational officials and disabled serviceproviders adopt a view that only students classified as LD are prone toexperience extreme problems <strong>in</strong> FL learn<strong>in</strong>g and fail FL courses, and<strong>the</strong>refore should be provided with educational accommodations <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>form of special <strong>in</strong>struction, course substitution or waiver. Educators mayfur<strong>the</strong>r assume that non-LD low-achiev<strong>in</strong>g students with L2 learn<strong>in</strong>gproblems do not encounter subtle or overt L1 difficulties and, unlike <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong> cases of LD poor L2 learners, <strong>the</strong>y do not need and would not benefitfrom direct, explicit <strong>in</strong>struction and o<strong>the</strong>r educational adjustments.However, <strong>the</strong> aforementioned assumptions have been negatively verifiedby <strong>the</strong> research results and, <strong>in</strong> all likelihood, <strong>the</strong>y seem to be false.Sparks (Sparks, 2006; Sparks et al., 2008b) claims that research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsunequivocally denote LD classification as an irrelevant factor <strong>in</strong>determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> ease of FL acquisition. Still, despite quite <strong>in</strong>controvertibleresearch conclusions, typically <strong>the</strong> practice <strong>in</strong> many high schools,universities and colleges is that <strong>in</strong> order to be eligible for FL coursesubstitution or waiver, students need to be diagnosed as LD. At <strong>the</strong> sametime non-LD FL underachievers, with legitimate special needs <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> areaof FL learn<strong>in</strong>g, are generally not granted <strong>the</strong> abovementioned accommodationsbecause <strong>the</strong>y lack formal identification and evaluation of <strong>the</strong>irlearn<strong>in</strong>g problems. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, LD students are too frequently automaticallyassumed to be likely to exhibit difficulties <strong>in</strong> FL learn<strong>in</strong>g, andlow-achievers struggl<strong>in</strong>g with FL learn<strong>in</strong>g are rout<strong>in</strong>ely expected to beLD (Sparks et al., 2002, 2003; Sparks, 2006). S<strong>in</strong>ce, at present, <strong>the</strong>re is noempirically supported sound ground to offer FL course substitutions orwaivers to LD students at <strong>the</strong> same time deny<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m to non-LDstudents with FL learn<strong>in</strong>g problems it would seem fair to ei<strong>the</strong>r offer<strong>the</strong> educational adjustments, follow<strong>in</strong>g some school-selected criteria, toall students struggl<strong>in</strong>g with FL provisions or not to make any studentseligible for such options (Sparks et al., 2003; Sparks, 2006).It needs stress<strong>in</strong>g here that FL learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties experienced bylow-achiev<strong>in</strong>g non-LD students as well as <strong>the</strong>se diagnosed to have LD


74 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>can be, <strong>in</strong> all probability, attributed to <strong>the</strong> same factor, namely, weaker L1skills, which, almost <strong>in</strong>variably, are l<strong>in</strong>ked to lower L2 aptitude,achievement and proficiency. LD and non-LD poor FL learners, whoseresults on measures of L1 skills and FL aptitude are on <strong>the</strong> wholesignificantly poorer than those of high achievers, have been repeatedlyobserved to demonstrate similar skills on measures of L1 and L2 literacy.In addition, <strong>the</strong>y commonly exhibit comparable L2 aptitudes, cognitiveabilities (IQ) as well as levels of motivation for L2 learn<strong>in</strong>g (Sparks, 2001,2006; Sparks et al., 2008a, 2008b).What follows from <strong>the</strong> above is that, firstly, most probably <strong>the</strong>re is nosuch phenomenon as FL learn<strong>in</strong>g disability and language learn<strong>in</strong>g abilityexists on a cont<strong>in</strong>uum, with foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g difficultiesrang<strong>in</strong>g from mild to severe. Secondly, <strong>in</strong>telligent students struggl<strong>in</strong>gwith FL courses should not be perceived as hav<strong>in</strong>g any k<strong>in</strong>d of specialdisability. Thirdly, non-disabled low-achiev<strong>in</strong>g learners may have, justlike <strong>the</strong>ir LD counterparts, subtle or overt language learn<strong>in</strong>g problems.F<strong>in</strong>ally, students classified as LD can, to some extent, become proficient<strong>in</strong> a foreign language. Beyond doubt, native language problems willmanifest <strong>the</strong>mselves <strong>in</strong> students’ attempts to learn a foreign language,never<strong>the</strong>less, <strong>the</strong>y do not necessarily have to impede successful foreignlanguage acquisition (Mabbott, 1995). Thus, obviously enough, poor FLlearners should ra<strong>the</strong>r not be withdrawn from foreign language coursesbut encouraged to enrol and provided with tutor<strong>in</strong>g assistance(Ganschow et al., 1998; Sparks et al., 1998b, 2008a; Sparks, 2006). 2What follows is a comprehensive, however, by no means exhaustive,review of research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> support of <strong>the</strong> LCDH, promot<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> claimthat native language skills form <strong>the</strong> basis for FL aptitude and learn<strong>in</strong>g,so, logically, students with weaker native language skills are expected torout<strong>in</strong>ely demonstrate markedly lower FL aptitude, achievement andproficiency than students with stronger native language skills. Inaddition, l<strong>in</strong>guistic factors have repeatedly proved responsible for FLlearn<strong>in</strong>g success or failure, while affective variables seem to be secondarywith respect to this matter. Poor attitude, unfavourable self-perception,as well as motivational-emotional disorders are likely to occur as a resultra<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>the</strong> cause of <strong>the</strong> difficulties <strong>in</strong> question.Review of Research <strong>in</strong> Support of L<strong>in</strong>guistic Cod<strong>in</strong>gDifferences Hypo<strong>the</strong>sisSparks and Ganschow <strong>in</strong>itiated <strong>the</strong>ir research on <strong>the</strong> aetiology offoreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties concentrat<strong>in</strong>g on cognitive, affectiveand l<strong>in</strong>guistic factors. They accumulated a sound empirical supportfor <strong>the</strong>ir hypo<strong>the</strong>sis that foreign language potential is <strong>in</strong> essence directlyl<strong>in</strong>ked to <strong>the</strong> native language capacity. In <strong>the</strong>ir studies, a rich battery of


<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 75tests <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g phonological, syntactic and semantic measures oflanguage and a foreign language aptitude test were adm<strong>in</strong>istered togood and poor foreign language learners, who consistently turned out tohave substantial native language learn<strong>in</strong>g deficits (Sparks, 1995).It needs highlight<strong>in</strong>g that, as far as <strong>the</strong> cognitive doma<strong>in</strong> is concerned,no significant unlikeness <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> level of <strong>in</strong>telligence (IQ) has been foundbetween <strong>the</strong> students with and without foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>gdifficulties. As relates to <strong>the</strong> affective doma<strong>in</strong>, poor attitude and lack ofmotivation <strong>in</strong>variably turned out to be <strong>the</strong> result of hardness withlanguage ra<strong>the</strong>r than its cause. In <strong>the</strong> face of it, <strong>the</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistic factors arebelieved to constitute <strong>the</strong> primary causal determ<strong>in</strong>ants <strong>in</strong> successful orunsuccessful foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g. Thus, let us stress aga<strong>in</strong>, poorforeign language learners almost <strong>in</strong>evitably suffer from ei<strong>the</strong>r subtle orovert native language impairments, which are blame-worthy for <strong>the</strong>foreign language acquisition problems <strong>the</strong>se students encounter(Ganschow et al., 1998; Sparks et al., 1998a).Anxiety and perception studiesAs perta<strong>in</strong>s to <strong>the</strong> affective variables, it is speculated that students’low level of motivation and high level of anxiety regard<strong>in</strong>g foreignlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g exist due to certa<strong>in</strong> deficits <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir native languageskills (Sparks, 1995; Sparks & Ganschow, 1991; Sparks et al., 1989). In oneof <strong>the</strong> studies provid<strong>in</strong>g support for <strong>the</strong> above hypo<strong>the</strong>sis, <strong>the</strong> studentswere grouped by <strong>the</strong> level of foreign language anxiety. Native languageskill and foreign language aptitude differences were documented acrosshigh-, average- and low-anxiety groups (Ganschow et al., 1994). Inano<strong>the</strong>r study (Sparks et al., 1997b) with similar group<strong>in</strong>g criteria,significant differences were spotted <strong>in</strong> oral and written foreign languageproficiency among high-, average- and low-anxiety groups. In general,it can be concluded that students with lower levels of anxiety aboutforeign language learn<strong>in</strong>g, stronger native language skills and greaterforeign language aptitude score significantly higher on measuresof foreign language proficiency than students with higher levels ofanxiety about foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g, lower native language skillsand lower foreign language aptitude.In order to study affective states, several <strong>in</strong>vestigations with referenceto students’ self-perceptions about <strong>the</strong>ir foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g havebeen conducted (Javorsky et al., 1992; Sparks et al., 1993). O<strong>the</strong>r studiesconcerned teachers’ perceptions about <strong>the</strong>ir students’ foreign languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g aptness and affective characteristics (Sparks & Ganschow, 1995).Moreover, parents’ perceptions about <strong>the</strong>ir children’s native and foreignlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g faculties (Sparks & Ganschow, 1996) have also been<strong>in</strong>vestigated.


76 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>In one of <strong>the</strong> self-perception studies (Javorsky et al., 1992), both LDand non-LD foreign language college students were equally motivated tolearn a foreign language. However, <strong>the</strong> LD foreign language learnersperceived <strong>the</strong>mselves as less capable of master<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> content of foreignlanguage courses <strong>in</strong> both <strong>the</strong> oral and written requirements. Additionally,<strong>the</strong>y admitted feel<strong>in</strong>g more anxious when tak<strong>in</strong>g tests and study<strong>in</strong>gfor foreign language lessons. In ano<strong>the</strong>r study of this k<strong>in</strong>d (Sparks et al.,1993), high school first-year foreign language course students characterisedas high-risk, low-risk or diagnosed as LD were compared withrespect to <strong>the</strong>ir academic skills and attitudes towards foreign languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g. Low-risk students reported significantly higher foreign languagegrades and demonstrated markedly more positive attitudes about<strong>the</strong>ir language learn<strong>in</strong>g potential than high-risk and LD students.Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, high-risk and LD students perceived <strong>the</strong>mselves asdeprived of <strong>the</strong> academic capacities to acquire satisfactory competence<strong>in</strong> a foreign language. Never<strong>the</strong>less, all students participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>study admitted <strong>the</strong>y possessed read<strong>in</strong>ess and a desire to learn a foreignlanguage. As ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed by Sparks et al. (1993), <strong>the</strong> fact that high-riskand LD students perceived <strong>the</strong>mselves less positively resulted from <strong>the</strong>irsubstantially weaker native language propensities and foreign languageaptitude as compared to low-risk students.In <strong>the</strong> perception study conducted by Sparks and Ganschow (1996),<strong>the</strong> high school students were allocated <strong>in</strong>to high, average and lowgroups <strong>in</strong> compliance with <strong>the</strong>ir scores on <strong>the</strong> native language andforeign language aptitude tests. Students who scored lower on <strong>the</strong> test<strong>in</strong>gmeasures were perceived by <strong>the</strong>ir teachers as possess<strong>in</strong>g weaker foreignlanguage academic skills and less hearten<strong>in</strong>g affective characteristics,perta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g to higher anxiety, lower motivation and less optimisticattitude than students who demonstrated superior native language skillsand foreign language aptitude.In <strong>the</strong> study on parents’ perceptions of <strong>the</strong>ir children’s languageability, first-year high school foreign language learners were divided <strong>in</strong>tolow-risk, average-risk and high-risk groups with regard to <strong>the</strong> scores of<strong>the</strong> questionnaire (completed by <strong>the</strong>ir parents) on <strong>the</strong> subject of <strong>the</strong>irnative language learn<strong>in</strong>g history. The students also underwent <strong>the</strong> nativelanguage faculty and foreign language aptitude test<strong>in</strong>g. All-embrac<strong>in</strong>gsignificant group differences were detected favour<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> low- andaverage-risk groups over <strong>the</strong> high-risk group on <strong>the</strong> native language,foreign language aptitude and cognitive measures. What follows from<strong>the</strong> above studies is that students who are perceived, both by <strong>the</strong>irparents and foreign language teachers, as hold<strong>in</strong>g weaker native andforeign language skills <strong>in</strong>deed exhibit <strong>in</strong>ferior competencies touch<strong>in</strong>gon <strong>the</strong> native and foreign language and <strong>the</strong>y achieve lower grades <strong>in</strong>


<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 77foreign language courses than students who are perceived as <strong>the</strong> onesdemonstrat<strong>in</strong>g strong native and foreign language abilities.The results of <strong>the</strong> aforementioned studies clearly provide support for<strong>the</strong> claim that foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties are not <strong>in</strong>duced by<strong>the</strong> affective variables. Quite <strong>the</strong> opposite, <strong>the</strong> affective differencesbetween good and poor foreign language learners seem to be dependenton <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tensity of difficulties <strong>the</strong>y struggle with dur<strong>in</strong>g foreign languageacquisition.Comparison studies between good and poorforeign language learnersMov<strong>in</strong>g on from perception to comparison studies, let us note thatSparks and Ganschow conducted a number of studies <strong>in</strong> which <strong>the</strong>ycompared good and poor foreign language learners. In <strong>the</strong> studiesexam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g native language skills and foreign language aptitude differences,it was shown that successful college foreign language learnersexhibited significantly stronger native language phonological/orthographicskills (but not semantic) and greater foreign language aptitude(as measured by <strong>the</strong> MLAT) than unsuccessful foreign language learners(Ganschow et al., 1991). This dist<strong>in</strong>ctness has been demonstrated not onlyat post-secondary, but also at secondary level (Sparks et al., 1992a), with<strong>the</strong> use of a similar battery of native language strength (read<strong>in</strong>g, spell<strong>in</strong>g,vocabulary and writ<strong>in</strong>g) and foreign language aptitude measures. Firstyearforeign language learners <strong>in</strong> high school were grouped <strong>in</strong>to low andhigh risk <strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>e with <strong>the</strong>ir first quarter foreign language grade (A and B low risk, D and F high risk) and <strong>the</strong> recommendation of <strong>the</strong>ir foreignlanguage teachers. Low-risk foreign language learners displayed impressivelystronger native language phonological/orthographic and syntacticbut not semantic capabilities and greater foreign language aptitude thanhigh-risk foreign language learners.In ano<strong>the</strong>r study (Sparks et al., 1992b), LD first-year foreign languagelearners were compared to low- and high-risk foreign language learnerson <strong>the</strong> same measures. Aga<strong>in</strong>, low-risk foreign language learnersmanifested stronger native language phonological/orthographic andsyntactic but not semantic skills and greater foreign language aptitudethan LD and high-risk foreign language learners. The salient outcome of<strong>the</strong> study is that no significant contrasts were identified on most nativelanguage and foreign language aptitude tests between <strong>the</strong> LD and highriskforeign language learners. High-risk foreign language learnersoutperformed LD foreign language learners only on <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>gmeasure. Hence, <strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sis that high-risk non-LD foreign languagelearners and LD foreign language learners present similar native andforeign language aptitude complexity.


78 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>F<strong>in</strong>ally, foreign language grades of poor and good foreign languagelearners were compared, relevant to <strong>the</strong>ir native language abilities andforeign language aptitude. As expected, students who obta<strong>in</strong> higherforeign language grades reveal impressively stronger native languageskills and foreign language aptitude than students who earn lowergrades (Ganschow et al., 1998).Prediction studiesSparks and Ganschow tested <strong>the</strong> efficacy of <strong>the</strong> LCDH by exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong> best predictors of foreign language grades <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> first year of study.Sparks et al. (1995b) conducted two experiments <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g n<strong>in</strong>th andtenth grade students. They found that <strong>the</strong> best predictor of foreignlanguage grades <strong>in</strong> both experiments appeared to be <strong>the</strong> native languagevariable <strong>the</strong> eighth English grade. This grade is related to one’s oral(listen<strong>in</strong>g and speak<strong>in</strong>g) and written (read<strong>in</strong>g and writ<strong>in</strong>g) l<strong>in</strong>guisticabilities, both of which are crucial for foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g. Ano<strong>the</strong>rpredictor was <strong>the</strong> result on <strong>the</strong> MLAT long form, consist<strong>in</strong>g of four<strong>in</strong>dependent variables, namely, <strong>the</strong> phonetic cod<strong>in</strong>g, grammatical sensitivity,<strong>in</strong>ductive language learn<strong>in</strong>g ability and rote memory. Nativelanguage spell<strong>in</strong>g turned out to be a predictor variable <strong>in</strong> one of <strong>the</strong>experiments, thus confirm<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> importance of <strong>the</strong> native phonological/orthographic ability for foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, separateanalyses of various foreign languages (Spanish and French) wereconducted. The f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>dicate that similar test<strong>in</strong>g measures bestpredict <strong>the</strong> achievement <strong>in</strong> Spanish and French.Sparks et al. (1997c) exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>the</strong> best predictors of overall (oral andwritten) proficiency <strong>in</strong> a foreign language after two years of study <strong>in</strong> twoexperiments. <strong>Foreign</strong> language word decod<strong>in</strong>g, which is a direct measureof <strong>the</strong> phonological-orthographic (sound and sound-symbol) componentof a foreign language, and end of first-year foreign language grade (as an<strong>in</strong>dicator of achievement <strong>in</strong> a foreign language course) were claimed topredict comprehensive foreign language proficiency <strong>in</strong> both experiments.Additionally, <strong>the</strong> native language vocabulary contributed to <strong>the</strong> generalforeign language proficiency <strong>in</strong> experiment 1.In ano<strong>the</strong>r study conducted by Sparks et al. (2006), students werefollowed from first through tenth grade and tested on L1 read<strong>in</strong>g andspell<strong>in</strong>g, L1 receptive vocabulary and listen<strong>in</strong>g comprehension, as well asgeneral IQ at specific time <strong>in</strong>tervals over this period. The scores on <strong>the</strong>abovementioned measures were used as predictor variables <strong>in</strong> order toanalyse <strong>the</strong>ir effects on oral and written FL proficiency and FL aptitude.All students participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> this study completed a two-year high schoolFL course <strong>in</strong> Spanish, French or German. The study f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs stronglysupport <strong>the</strong> existence of <strong>the</strong> connection between L1 (read<strong>in</strong>g, spell<strong>in</strong>g)


<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 79and subsequent L2 skills (read<strong>in</strong>g, spell<strong>in</strong>g, listen<strong>in</strong>g, speak<strong>in</strong>g) and FLaptitude, as well as <strong>the</strong> importance of lower-level phonological abilitiesfor oral and written FL proficiency. A vital implication that follows from<strong>the</strong> outcome of <strong>the</strong> study is that <strong>in</strong>telligence cannot be treated as a goodpredictor of students’ future FL proficiency. Thus, to determ<strong>in</strong>e placementlevels and to predict FL proficiency, native and foreign languageeducators are advised to use measures of students’ L1 achievementra<strong>the</strong>r than IQ scores. Explicit teach<strong>in</strong>g of FL phonological andphonological/orthographic skills to secondary school FL students isstrongly recommended by <strong>the</strong> authors. Touch<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong> native languageelementary graders, direct <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> L1 sound and sound/symbolsystems would better prepare those students for L2 study.Proficiency studiesProficiency studies aim to determ<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong> relationship between nativeand foreign language skills. The results of research conducted by Sparkset al. (1998a) disclosed <strong>the</strong> existence of fair overall differences <strong>in</strong> nativelanguage capability, foreign language aptitude and <strong>the</strong> end-of-yearforeign language grade. High school students tak<strong>in</strong>g part <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> studywere grouped accord<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> level of <strong>the</strong>ir oral and written competencies<strong>in</strong> a foreign language (Spanish, French and German) <strong>in</strong>to highforeign language proficiency group, average foreign language proficiencygroup and low foreign language proficiency group. It turned outthat students who obta<strong>in</strong> higher scores on oral and written foreignlanguage proficiency tests have stronger native language skills <strong>in</strong>phonology/orthography and semantics than students who ga<strong>in</strong> lowerscores. The outcome of <strong>the</strong> study also suggests that generally averageand low foreign language proficiency students did not differ noticeablyon <strong>the</strong> phonology/orthography measures, thus only def<strong>in</strong>ed differences<strong>in</strong> phonological/orthographic skills differentiated low and high foreignlanguage proficiency students. Moreover, students with low foreignlanguage competence would score significantly lower than students withhigh foreign language faculty on <strong>the</strong> foreign language aptitude measure.F<strong>in</strong>ally, <strong>the</strong> abovementioned f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs imply that <strong>the</strong> students exhibit<strong>in</strong>gdifferent levels (high, average, low) of foreign language proficiency showdisparities <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir foreign language course grades, especially at <strong>the</strong> endof <strong>the</strong> first year, on <strong>the</strong> side of <strong>the</strong> high foreign language ability group. Ithas been argued that even though <strong>the</strong> students’ grades <strong>in</strong> foreignlanguage courses do not compose a measure of actual foreign languageproficiency, <strong>the</strong> results of <strong>the</strong> studies suggest that, at least to some extent,higher grades may reflect superior oral and written foreign languageskills. Follow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> results of <strong>the</strong>se studies, it is quite apparent that <strong>the</strong>performance on <strong>the</strong> standard measures of native language skill is


80 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>straightforwardly related to <strong>the</strong> level of foreign language proficiency.Students with higher levels of native language abilities tend to achievegreater oral and written foreign language proficiency. In a similar ve<strong>in</strong>,groups of students who manifest critical variance <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> overall foreignlanguage proficiency almost certa<strong>in</strong>ly display decent divergence <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>native language skills. Hence <strong>the</strong> conclusion that learners faced wi<strong>the</strong>i<strong>the</strong>r subtle or overt difficulties <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g, writ<strong>in</strong>g, listen<strong>in</strong>g andspeak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir native language are likely to experience similar pitfalls<strong>in</strong> foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g.To sum up, <strong>the</strong> research evidence cited above suggests that native andforeign language aptitude differences between good and poor foreignlanguage learners <strong>in</strong>evitably exist. Poor foreign language learnersconsistently score lower on <strong>the</strong> native and foreign language aptitudemeasures than good foreign language learners. Moreover, most of <strong>the</strong>poor foreign language learners have trouble primarily with <strong>the</strong> phonologicalcode <strong>the</strong> ability to break down, put toge<strong>the</strong>r and relate <strong>the</strong>sounds of <strong>the</strong> language to <strong>the</strong> appropriate letters or letter comb<strong>in</strong>ations,while semantic difficulties (language comprehension problems) do notappear to trigger most foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties (Sparkset al., 1992b).Studies on English as a foreign languageSparks’ claims concern<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g profiles of native Englishspeak<strong>in</strong>gyoung adults, learn<strong>in</strong>g various foreign languages, attracted <strong>the</strong>attention and <strong>in</strong>terest of many researchers. The results of numerousrecent studies, with reference to various L1 languages, with English as aforeign language, seem to be <strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>e with Sparks’ f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs; however, <strong>in</strong>some experiments, <strong>the</strong> LCDH assumptions were not quite positivelyverified.For example, Chen (2001) supported <strong>the</strong> validity of <strong>the</strong> LDCH byconclud<strong>in</strong>g that difficulties which Ch<strong>in</strong>ese college learners of English <strong>in</strong>Taiwan experience <strong>in</strong> FL learn<strong>in</strong>g can be attributed to <strong>the</strong>ir underly<strong>in</strong>gproblems with <strong>the</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese language. Ho and Fong (2005) exam<strong>in</strong>edCh<strong>in</strong>ese dyslexic learners of English <strong>in</strong> Hong Kong who had difficultieslearn<strong>in</strong>g English as a second language and found that, regardless of <strong>the</strong>dist<strong>in</strong>ctive characteristics of <strong>the</strong> two scripts, Ch<strong>in</strong>ese non-dyslexicchildren outperformed children with dyslexia on FL measures. Ch<strong>in</strong>esedyslexic children with a phonological deficit <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir native languageproved at high risk of encounter<strong>in</strong>g difficulties learn<strong>in</strong>g English. The factthat children participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> study demonstrated weak phonologicalawareness <strong>in</strong> both Ch<strong>in</strong>ese (L1) and English (L2) supports <strong>the</strong> notion ofcross-l<strong>in</strong>guistic transfer and <strong>the</strong> proposition that L1 skills form afoundation for <strong>the</strong> development of L2 skills. However, importantly,


<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 81weak phonological awareness demonstrated by <strong>the</strong> participants of <strong>the</strong>study <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese did not translate <strong>in</strong>to read<strong>in</strong>g problems <strong>in</strong> thatlanguage, unlike <strong>in</strong> English, where <strong>the</strong>ir poor phonological skills weremost often, though not without exceptions, l<strong>in</strong>ked to read<strong>in</strong>g failure. Inconclusion, despite be<strong>in</strong>g concomitant at a high rate, read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties<strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese (L1) and English (L2) occur due to both common and specificcauses.Helland and Kaasa (2005) focused on Norwegian 12-year-old studentswith dyslexia, who learned English as a second language. Generally, <strong>the</strong>dyslexic group, when compared to non-dyslexic controls, demonstratedsignificantly poorer results on a specially designed English proficiencytest, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> measures of morphology, syntax, semantics andorthography. However, group differences among learners with dyslexia,relat<strong>in</strong>g to success on all l<strong>in</strong>guistic skills measures except for L2 spell<strong>in</strong>g,have been noted. The assumption that dyslexics, who show L1 skillimpairments of differ<strong>in</strong>g severity, confront more hurdles <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>gforeign languages than non-dyslexic students also holds true <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>Polish secondary school environment (Jurek, 2004a, 2004b, 2004c;Nijakowska, 2004).However, by way of contrast, Miller-Guron and Lundberg (2000) seemto challenge <strong>the</strong> assumption that L1 dyslexic read<strong>in</strong>g problems are boundto translate <strong>in</strong>to L2 read<strong>in</strong>g failure. They report a truly surpris<strong>in</strong>g andextraord<strong>in</strong>ary preference for read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a foreign language (English),exhibited by adult Swedish dyslexics. The authors termed <strong>the</strong> phenomenon<strong>the</strong> dyslexic preference for English read<strong>in</strong>g (DPER) and hypo<strong>the</strong>sisedthat its occurrence may be caused by several socio-cultural andemotional factors, such as enhanced L2 <strong>in</strong>put through television andmusic media, early exposure to L2 literature, along with factors specificto English orthography. The relatively shallow Swedish orthographyputs high demands on readers with regard to phoneme by phonemedecod<strong>in</strong>g. That is why Swedish readers with dyslexia, who possess weakphonological skills at <strong>the</strong> level of phonemes, may paradoxically developa preference for read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> deeper English orthography, most probablybecause <strong>the</strong>y are cont<strong>in</strong>uously <strong>in</strong>efficient <strong>in</strong> apply<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> graphemephoneme(small gra<strong>in</strong> size phonological unit) strategy when approach<strong>in</strong>gSwedish texts. Read<strong>in</strong>g English texts, on <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, requires wordrecognition strategies concern<strong>in</strong>g larger orthographic segments such asrimes or whole words. 3 The proposed explanation seems to be <strong>in</strong> accordwith <strong>the</strong> phonological gra<strong>in</strong> size <strong>the</strong>ory (Ziegler & Goswami, 2006). Still,Swedish, as a highly <strong>in</strong>flected and derivational language, places greatermorphosyntactic demands on its readers than English, which is <strong>the</strong>fact that may additionally contribute to <strong>the</strong> explanation of DPER.DPER proponents (Lundberg, 2002; Miller-Guron & Lundberg, 2000)also seek supportive arguments for <strong>the</strong>ir claims among <strong>the</strong> results of


82 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>neurobiological <strong>in</strong>vestigations, speculat<strong>in</strong>g that neurological dissociationof languages <strong>in</strong> bil<strong>in</strong>guals may help to expla<strong>in</strong> DPER. As much as <strong>the</strong>assumption that <strong>the</strong> neurological substrate of L2 <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> mayfunction more effectively than L1 tissue is <strong>in</strong>deed a tempt<strong>in</strong>g speculation,it seems a matter of future research to be confirmed.Van der Leij and Morfidi (2006) suggest that even though <strong>the</strong> universalphonological core deficit is responsible for transferr<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g difficultiesfrom L1 to L2, variable orthographic competence may possibly exist<strong>in</strong>dependently of phonological decod<strong>in</strong>g and be capable of expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gdifferences between read<strong>in</strong>g-disabled <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> L2 with a deeporthography and L1 with a shallow one. Follow<strong>in</strong>g this l<strong>in</strong>e of argument,<strong>the</strong>y speculate that <strong>the</strong> subgroup of Swedish dyslexics with preferencefor English read<strong>in</strong>g from Miller-Guron and Lundberg’s (2000) studymight have demonstrated advantageous orthographic competence,which was ma<strong>in</strong>ly applied to L2 <strong>in</strong>consistent orthography, while <strong>the</strong>rewas limited opportunity to use it <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> quite shallow orthography of L1.It is fur<strong>the</strong>r implied that poor readers exhibit differ<strong>in</strong>g cognitive profilesand may vary <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir sensitivity to irregularities of <strong>the</strong> non-transparentEnglish orthography.Sparks et al. predom<strong>in</strong>antly focused on young adults consistentlyexhibit<strong>in</strong>g m<strong>in</strong>or to more severe phonological and syntactic cod<strong>in</strong>gimpairment <strong>in</strong> L1. However, <strong>the</strong> apparent constra<strong>in</strong>t of such research isthat direct analysis of developmental and academic advancement as wellas early histories of L1 acquisition hurdle, which <strong>the</strong> FL learn<strong>in</strong>gproblems might be traced back to, is ra<strong>the</strong>r impossible. It can only bereconstructed, usually from a self-report assessment. Hence <strong>the</strong> need toextend <strong>the</strong> exist<strong>in</strong>g f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs to younger children and vary<strong>in</strong>g languages.In this ve<strong>in</strong>, Ferrari and Pallad<strong>in</strong>o (2007) <strong>in</strong>vestigated seventh- andeighth-grade Italian students qualified ei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>in</strong>to a group of high or lowachievers with regard to learn<strong>in</strong>g EFL. Native language read<strong>in</strong>gcomprehension was consistently deficient <strong>in</strong> low-achiev<strong>in</strong>g FL students,whereas <strong>the</strong>ir read<strong>in</strong>g speed and accuracy, even though slightly poorer(though usually not reach<strong>in</strong>g statistical significance) when compared tohigh-achiev<strong>in</strong>g FL counterparts, proved average accord<strong>in</strong>g to Italiannorms. The f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are not quite <strong>in</strong> accord with <strong>the</strong> LCDH assumptions,accord<strong>in</strong>g to which poor FL learners are expected to demonstrateimpairment <strong>in</strong> particular <strong>in</strong> phonological-orthographic skills with regardto L1. All <strong>in</strong> all, <strong>the</strong> pattern of learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>in</strong> Italian children wasnot parallel with that of native English-speak<strong>in</strong>g young adult students atrisk of FL learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties described earlier.By contrast, analogous f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs to those of Sparks et al. concern<strong>in</strong>gbest predictors of FL proficiency have been reported by o<strong>the</strong>r researcherswith regard to younger students as well. For example, Kahn-Horwitzet al. (2005, 2006) analysed <strong>in</strong>dividual differences among beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g


<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 83readers of EFL and concluded that L1 skills <strong>in</strong> Hebrew strongly predicted<strong>the</strong>ir L2 skills. Similar to Sparks et al. (2006), Kahn-Horwitz et al. (2006)opt for early detection of L1 difficulties <strong>in</strong> word recognition, phonologicalawareness and vocabulary knowledge <strong>in</strong> students beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>irFL acquisition <strong>in</strong> order to identify at-risk FL learners. It is expected andsuggested that such identification followed by appropriate <strong>in</strong>terventionand <strong>in</strong>struction would very likely prevent those students from fall<strong>in</strong>gbeh<strong>in</strong>d good FL readers. Dufva and Voeten (1999) studied F<strong>in</strong>nishchildren and found that L1 literacy and phonological process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> firstgrade predicted <strong>the</strong>ir performance <strong>in</strong> foreign language (English) <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>third grade. Similarly, a significant relationship between L1 phonological/orthographicskills (especially an ability to decode words) of Spanishfirst-graders and <strong>the</strong>ir later decod<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong> EFL has been<strong>in</strong>dicated by L<strong>in</strong>dsay et al. (2003).Oren and Breznitz (2005) <strong>in</strong>vestigated <strong>the</strong> differences <strong>in</strong> accuracy andread<strong>in</strong>g rate of words <strong>in</strong> L1 and L2 <strong>in</strong> adult dyslexic and regular readers.They ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> that as much as dyslexic deficits <strong>in</strong> L1 (Hebrew) and L2(English) seem to share some universal underly<strong>in</strong>g features, <strong>the</strong>irbehavioural manifestations may well be dependent on specific orthographiccharacteristics, <strong>in</strong>herent to each and every language. Thusproposed is <strong>the</strong> idea of <strong>the</strong> possibly complementary relation between<strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g assumptions: <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> foreign languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g can be predicted and caused by native language deficits andparticular features of different orthographies may cause vary<strong>in</strong>g problems<strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>se languages. Hence, frequently, certa<strong>in</strong>impairments, not necessarily evident <strong>in</strong> L1, may emerge with <strong>the</strong>commencement of second or foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g. In Oren andBreznitz’s study (2005) <strong>in</strong> both languages dyslexic atta<strong>in</strong>ments onmeasures of speed of <strong>in</strong>formation process<strong>in</strong>g, phonological short-termand work<strong>in</strong>g memory, and phonological process<strong>in</strong>g proved considerablypoorer <strong>in</strong> comparison to regular readers. In Hebrew and English alike,<strong>the</strong> dyslexic read<strong>in</strong>g rate was substantially slower than <strong>in</strong> regularbil<strong>in</strong>gual readers. With regard to accuracy measures, regular readersoutperformed <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia <strong>in</strong> English but not <strong>in</strong> Hebrew,<strong>the</strong> effect most likely traceable to <strong>the</strong> non-transparent, irregular characterof English orthography. All <strong>in</strong> all, dyslexics performed consistently better<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> shallow orthography of a po<strong>in</strong>ted Hebrew than <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> deep Englishorthographic system, where <strong>the</strong>ir difficulties were considerably moresevere and apparent. What follows from <strong>the</strong> outcome of this study is apresupposed importance of <strong>the</strong> orthography-specific mechanisms <strong>in</strong>determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g success <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> acquisition of <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g skill.It seems that, generally, native language competence serves as a basisfor foreign language faculty and children who develop faster <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>irnative language tend to manifest higher foreign language aptitude. Then,


84 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>logically enough, hampered ability to learn a foreign language <strong>in</strong>students with dyslexia can be attributed to <strong>the</strong> lack of solid foundation<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir native language. Implications for <strong>the</strong> assessment of foreignlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties explicitly <strong>in</strong>dicate both native languageability and foreign language aptitude measures as a must.Notes1. See Chapter 2: ‘Phonological cod<strong>in</strong>g deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis’, for a discussion onphonological process<strong>in</strong>g.2. A review of <strong>the</strong> research on <strong>the</strong> effectiveness of <strong>the</strong> MSL <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> FLteach<strong>in</strong>g can be found <strong>in</strong> Chapter 5: ‘Multisensory Structured Learn<strong>in</strong>gApproach and <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Study’.3. See Chapter 1: ‘Orthographic Depth and Gra<strong>in</strong> Size: A Cross-languagePerspective on Read<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>Dyslexia</strong>’ for a discussion on orthographic depthand gra<strong>in</strong> size.


Chapter 4Symptoms and Identificationof <strong>Dyslexia</strong>Lifelong Nature of <strong>Dyslexia</strong><strong>Dyslexia</strong> is a lifetime and chronic condition; children once diagnosed asdyslexic do not outgrow dyslexia, but rema<strong>in</strong> dyslexic through <strong>the</strong>ir youth<strong>in</strong>to adulthood (Downey et al., 2000; Gregg et al., 2005; Oren & Breznitz,2005). However, substantial <strong>in</strong>ter- and <strong>in</strong>tra-<strong>in</strong>dividual variance makes<strong>the</strong> overall picture of dyslexia quite complicated (Krasowicz-Kupis, 2008).The way dyslexia leaves its impr<strong>in</strong>t on behaviour varies across <strong>in</strong>dividuals.In addition, throughout life, behavioural symptoms of dyslexiamanifest <strong>in</strong> a given person are subject to a dynamic change, under <strong>the</strong><strong>in</strong>fluence of education, effectiveness of <strong>the</strong>rapeutic activities andefficiency of compensatory strategies. Thus, some features of dyslexiaalter with age characteristic symptoms tend to be evident but candim<strong>in</strong>ish or disappear at given po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> development because certa<strong>in</strong>deficits get compensated, while o<strong>the</strong>r disorders prevail <strong>in</strong>to adulthood(Bogdanowicz, 1997c, 1999; Mickiewicz, 1997; Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2001a).The most fundamental and primary behavioural symptom of dyslexiaseems to be a pronounced and persistent difficulty <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> acquisition ofskilful word decod<strong>in</strong>g (read<strong>in</strong>g) and encod<strong>in</strong>g (spell<strong>in</strong>g), forc<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> childto lag beh<strong>in</strong>d his/her peers with regard to literacy development.Decod<strong>in</strong>g and encod<strong>in</strong>g are <strong>in</strong>terrelated, <strong>the</strong>y can be collectivelyperceived as pr<strong>in</strong>t process<strong>in</strong>g or a mechanical aspect of read<strong>in</strong>g andspell<strong>in</strong>g ability (Szczerbiński, 2007). The difficulties often rema<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>tractable,despite special educational efforts at alleviat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m. The keydyslexic difficulty related to decod<strong>in</strong>g words is reflected <strong>in</strong> slower rateand poor accuracy of read<strong>in</strong>g. Dyslexic word decod<strong>in</strong>g problems are bestevidenced dur<strong>in</strong>g s<strong>in</strong>gle word and pseudo-word read<strong>in</strong>g tasks. Difficulties<strong>in</strong> non-word process<strong>in</strong>g are highly <strong>in</strong>dicative of late or deficientphonological development (poor phonological representations). Read<strong>in</strong>gdifficulties are frequently accompanied by low-grade orthographicspell<strong>in</strong>g. Phonological process<strong>in</strong>g disorders, by def<strong>in</strong>ition, constitute acharacteristic trait of dyslexia, while l<strong>in</strong>guistic function<strong>in</strong>g with referenceto syntactic, semantic or pragmatic levels may well be with<strong>in</strong> average. Allo<strong>the</strong>r symptoms associated with dyslexia, which are discussed <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>follow<strong>in</strong>g sections, have been reported to exist <strong>in</strong> some <strong>in</strong>dividuals withdyslexia, so <strong>the</strong>y can, but by no means have to, go along with basic85


86 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>read<strong>in</strong>g impairment. These symptoms may form multiple diversified setsand constellations, specific to each child (Krasowicz-Kupis, 2008).Risk for dyslexiaMultiple warn<strong>in</strong>g signs and areas of poor performance, to a considerableextent <strong>in</strong>dicative of later low-grade read<strong>in</strong>g skill, can be quiteaccurately identified <strong>in</strong> children before or at <strong>the</strong> very beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong>irschool education, when any adeptness regard<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>gskills is naturally not yet evident (Bogdanowicz, 2002a; Johnson et al.,2001; Ott, 1997). Areas of weakness identified <strong>in</strong> post-<strong>in</strong>fantile and preschoolstages, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g, for <strong>in</strong>stance, late development of speech, poorepiphonological skills or late development of motor ability, constitute <strong>the</strong>warn<strong>in</strong>g signs or <strong>in</strong>dicators of <strong>the</strong> risk for dyslexia, denot<strong>in</strong>g highprobability of later learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties (Bogdanowicz, 2002a; Ott,1997). With <strong>the</strong> commencement of school education and formal literacy<strong>in</strong>struction, isolated difficulties <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and spell, frequentlyemerge <strong>in</strong> children with <strong>the</strong>se early signs.A group at risk for dyslexia is composed of children with hereditarytransmission com<strong>in</strong>g from families with a history of dyslexia (Snowl<strong>in</strong>get al., 2007), children show<strong>in</strong>g speech delay, ambidextrousness or lefthandednessand partial/fragmentary disorders of psychomotor development.Children from a pathological pregnancy and delivery and thoseprematurely born qualify to <strong>the</strong> at-risk group, as do pupils who mightnot have explicitly demonstrated any worry<strong>in</strong>g signs of developmentaldelay or deficit at a very young age, but with <strong>the</strong> onset of formal<strong>in</strong>struction, <strong>the</strong>y clearly beg<strong>in</strong> to face <strong>in</strong>tensified problems <strong>in</strong> literacyacquisition. They experience learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties despite normal <strong>in</strong>telligence,good sight and hear<strong>in</strong>g as well as favourable educational anddidactic conditions (Bogdanowicz, 2002a). The malfunctions observed <strong>in</strong>at-risk children are mild, not result<strong>in</strong>g from <strong>the</strong> more global developmentaldelay or reduced cognitive capability.Detailed <strong>in</strong>ventories of symptoms, which parents and teachers ofyoung pre-school and school children should be aware of and especiallysensitive to, have been compiled by several authors (Bogdanowicz, 1999,2002a, 2003a; Bogdanowicz & Adryjanek, 2004; Ott, 1997; Tomaszewska,2001). As already <strong>in</strong>dicated, <strong>the</strong> ma<strong>in</strong> prevail<strong>in</strong>g symptom of dyslexia isread<strong>in</strong>g difficulty, <strong>in</strong> particular s<strong>in</strong>gle word decod<strong>in</strong>g, tracked downdirectly to <strong>the</strong> core l<strong>in</strong>guistic deficit at <strong>the</strong> level of phonologicalprocess<strong>in</strong>g. All <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r symptoms related to dyslexia, grouped <strong>in</strong>tocollections specific to each child, can but, let us stress aga<strong>in</strong>, do notnecessarily accompany <strong>the</strong> basic read<strong>in</strong>g problem. Moreover, <strong>the</strong>ir<strong>in</strong>tensity varies as well. One can imag<strong>in</strong>e that <strong>the</strong> more characteristic


Symptoms and Identification of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 87signs (listed below) one notices <strong>in</strong> a particular child, <strong>the</strong> greater <strong>the</strong>likelihood of <strong>the</strong> risk for <strong>the</strong> disorder.Spheres of child development and activity demand<strong>in</strong>g careful attentionof caretakers <strong>in</strong>clude language functions and speech, sequenc<strong>in</strong>g(<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g both visual and auditory sequential memory), f<strong>in</strong>e and grossmotor skills, visual functions and visual-motor co-ord<strong>in</strong>ation, orientation<strong>in</strong> body schemata, space and time, and, last but not least, read<strong>in</strong>g andspell<strong>in</strong>g competence. Unusual, low-grade performance of young childrenwith<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>se areas cannot be missed or ignored, s<strong>in</strong>ce failure to recogniseproblematic spheres and risks may result <strong>in</strong> far-reach<strong>in</strong>g consequences.Individual differences among regular readers as well as children withdyslexia can be, to a great extent, expla<strong>in</strong>ed by <strong>the</strong> degree of efficiencywith respect to phonological process<strong>in</strong>g. Children at risk for dyslexia tendto demonstrate late or <strong>in</strong>complete phonological development, evidenced<strong>in</strong> low scores on non-word process<strong>in</strong>g tasks. It seems that <strong>the</strong>se childrenexperience marked impairment <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir ability to form phonologicalrepresentations before <strong>the</strong>y make a start with read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction and <strong>the</strong>yare disadvantaged <strong>in</strong> comparison to normally develop<strong>in</strong>g children, s<strong>in</strong>ce<strong>the</strong>ir representations are much less specified and stable 1 (Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2000).This, <strong>in</strong> turn, has a debilitat<strong>in</strong>g effect on <strong>the</strong> ability to map orthography onphonology.<strong>Language</strong> impairment can be manifest <strong>in</strong> several ways, for example,late development of speech, word-nam<strong>in</strong>g problems, word mispronunciations,jumbl<strong>in</strong>g words, difficulties with rhyme and alliteration, andalso poor use of syntax (<strong>in</strong>appropriate word order and ungrammaticalforms). O<strong>the</strong>r problematic aspects <strong>in</strong>clude poor memorisation of nurseryrhymes, short poems and songs, below-standard aptitude to repeatmessages and follow a series of <strong>in</strong>structions, and a tendency to usecircumlocutions. Difficulty <strong>in</strong> remember<strong>in</strong>g names and common sequences(e.g. <strong>the</strong> alphabet, days of <strong>the</strong> week, months of <strong>the</strong> year) andretriev<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m from memory is also unexceptional. Sound discrim<strong>in</strong>ationand manipulation, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g blend<strong>in</strong>g, sequenc<strong>in</strong>g, add<strong>in</strong>g anddelet<strong>in</strong>g tasks usually poses tangible difficulty as well.The symptomatic behaviour with regard to poor automatisation 2 ofgross motor skills (arms and legs) <strong>in</strong>volves lack of a crawl<strong>in</strong>g stage,awkwardness <strong>in</strong> keep<strong>in</strong>g balance, laboriousness <strong>in</strong> hopp<strong>in</strong>g, catch<strong>in</strong>g,throw<strong>in</strong>g or kick<strong>in</strong>g a ball, frequent bump<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>to people and objects,knock<strong>in</strong>g th<strong>in</strong>gs over or dropp<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m, struggl<strong>in</strong>g to learn to ride abicycle, to swim or to dance. In view of <strong>the</strong> above, playground gamesmay constitute a problem, which can be additionally <strong>in</strong>tensified by <strong>the</strong>use of commands such as left/right, up/down, backwards/forwards, <strong>in</strong>front of/beh<strong>in</strong>d. Poor co-ord<strong>in</strong>ation when climb<strong>in</strong>g ropes, stand<strong>in</strong>g onone leg or walk<strong>in</strong>g along a bench are fur<strong>the</strong>r signs. Late development off<strong>in</strong>e motor skills (f<strong>in</strong>gers and hands) leads to low dexterity <strong>in</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g


88 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>cutlery, scissors, rubber, trac<strong>in</strong>g, dress<strong>in</strong>g up, ty<strong>in</strong>g up shoe laces,button<strong>in</strong>g a shirt, manipulat<strong>in</strong>g small objects (build<strong>in</strong>g blocks) and, lastbut not least, draw<strong>in</strong>g and writ<strong>in</strong>g (due to an awkward pencil grip).Disorders of visual function and visual-motor co-ord<strong>in</strong>ation concernlow-grade grapho-motor activity, difficulty arrang<strong>in</strong>g build<strong>in</strong>g blocks orpuzzles accord<strong>in</strong>g to a given pattern and draw<strong>in</strong>g. What is more, poorability to remember letter shapes, to dist<strong>in</strong>guish between similar shapes(geometric figures or letters, e.g. m-n, l-t) or letters of similar shape butdifferent position <strong>in</strong> space (p-d-g-b), low graphic level of draw<strong>in</strong>gs andwritten work as well as mirror writ<strong>in</strong>g are frequently observed. Childrenat risk for dyslexia may experience problems <strong>in</strong> orientation <strong>in</strong> bodyschemata and space, at times can be confused when discrim<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g rightfrom left, moreover, <strong>the</strong>y can exhibit low-level orientation <strong>in</strong> time(yesterday, tomorrow, later, earlier) and poor concentration.Persistent below-standard abilities to decode words, encountered at<strong>the</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of school education, qualify as symptoms of <strong>the</strong> risk fordyslexia. Slow and laborious read<strong>in</strong>g can be so time consum<strong>in</strong>g that evenif accuracy approximates normal, comprehension is <strong>in</strong>variably hampered.A tendency towards skipp<strong>in</strong>g certa<strong>in</strong> fragments, repeat<strong>in</strong>g l<strong>in</strong>esand los<strong>in</strong>g a place <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> text can be habitually observed. In addition,<strong>in</strong>stead of be<strong>in</strong>g properly read (decoded), words are often guessed,draw<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong> first letter, syllable or on overall appearance of a word, orsubstituted with a semantic counterpart (‘was’ for ‘lived’, ‘car’ for ‘bus’).Decod<strong>in</strong>g seems to arrest most attention, which, unsurpris<strong>in</strong>gly, oftenleads to problems with comprehension.As for spell<strong>in</strong>g ability, some young learners at risk for dyslexiatypically commit multiple mistakes <strong>in</strong> rewrit<strong>in</strong>g and dictation; <strong>the</strong>ycont<strong>in</strong>ually f<strong>in</strong>d it oddly difficult, <strong>in</strong> comparison to o<strong>the</strong>r children, toremember and properly dist<strong>in</strong>guish between <strong>the</strong> language sounds and<strong>the</strong>ir correspond<strong>in</strong>g graphic symbols letters. At times, <strong>the</strong>y also usemirror images of letters and often write down words awkwardly, for<strong>in</strong>stance <strong>in</strong> reverse direction from right to left.Generally, <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g types of errors are pr<strong>in</strong>cipally committed bychildren with dyslexia <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g: omission, <strong>in</strong>sertion,displacement, condensation, rotation, reversal, substitution and guess<strong>in</strong>g(Critchley, 1964; Kaja, 2001b; Lev<strong>in</strong>son, 1980; Ott, 1997). Thus, letters,parts of words, syllables and whole words are often omitted or<strong>in</strong>appropriately <strong>in</strong>serted. Peculiarly, skipped elements can resurface <strong>in</strong>distant parts of <strong>the</strong> same sentence and letters or syllables of successivewords can be condensed and read as new words. In addition, read<strong>in</strong>g isoften characterised by noticeably awkward <strong>in</strong>tonation. Handwrit<strong>in</strong>g canbe barely legible and full of corrections, while rate of writ<strong>in</strong>g tends to beunnaturally slow.


Symptoms and Identification of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 89Quite naturally, screen<strong>in</strong>g for early dyslexia signs has great potentialwith regard to <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g children at risk, who can <strong>the</strong>n undergo specialtra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g aimed at reduc<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> areas of difficulty and, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> long run,avoid scholastic failure and <strong>the</strong> potential danger of emotional damage.Hopefully, such a procedure becomes a widespread and commonpractice. 3Notwithstand<strong>in</strong>g massive evidence for early predispositions to literacydifficulties as well as sound usefulness of screen<strong>in</strong>g, L<strong>in</strong>dsay (2001)ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>s that any attempts to develop early educational screen<strong>in</strong>gmethods frequently prove problematic due to low accuracy, result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>high rates of false positives (children <strong>in</strong>correctly identified as be<strong>in</strong>g atrisk for dyslexia, who later show no read<strong>in</strong>g impairment) and falsenegatives (children not identified as be<strong>in</strong>g at risk for dyslexia, who laterencounter read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties) (Kalka, 2003).Regardless of <strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>tical difficulties, children at risk for dyslexiastand <strong>in</strong> need of early detection, most valuable dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> pre-schoolstages, which, <strong>in</strong>deed, is undeniably dependent on both teachers’ andparents’ alertness and sensitivity to early signs of late development ordifficulty. Obviously, <strong>the</strong> role of observant and attentive adults, who areknowledgeable about <strong>the</strong> symptoms and <strong>in</strong>escapable consequences ofdyslexia <strong>in</strong> later life, especially when <strong>the</strong>y are not properly identified at<strong>the</strong> right time, cannot be overestimated.Provided considerable attention and time is devoted to <strong>the</strong> remedialactivities, <strong>the</strong>re is a realistic chance that read<strong>in</strong>g problems can substantiallydecrease or even disappear. Spell<strong>in</strong>g difficulties tend to be muchmore persistent and manifest <strong>in</strong> childhood through adolescence toadulthood, despite an adequate knowledge of <strong>the</strong> orthographic rules.Thus, even <strong>in</strong> adulthood, spell<strong>in</strong>g usually rema<strong>in</strong>s a pa<strong>in</strong>ful task, <strong>in</strong> spiteof <strong>the</strong> atta<strong>in</strong>ment of typically fluent read<strong>in</strong>g. If <strong>the</strong> usually isolateddifficulties <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and spell, evident at <strong>the</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g ofschool education, are not correctly diagnosed and subject to adequatepedagogical <strong>in</strong>tervention, almost <strong>in</strong>variably <strong>the</strong>y can grow <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong> globallearn<strong>in</strong>g difficulty. Read<strong>in</strong>g impairments may hamper educationalprogress <strong>in</strong> many areas of <strong>the</strong> school curriculum and beyond it.Individuals with dyslexia, be<strong>in</strong>g poor readers, to a great extent can bedeprived of unlimited access to a wide range of <strong>in</strong>formation, touch<strong>in</strong>g onmultiple scientific discipl<strong>in</strong>es and spheres of life (Bogdanowicz, 1999;Dockrell & McShane, 1993).All <strong>in</strong> all, dyslexia is a life-long condition, whose behavioural signsdiffer and change as a function of age, from pre-school age (risk fordyslexia) to adulthood. Several phenomena may shape <strong>the</strong> dynamism ofsymptom change, for <strong>in</strong>stance, an <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> range of difficultieswith regard to specificity of various school subjects; possible occurrenceof <strong>the</strong> emotional-motivational and personality disorders (consequence of


90 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>scholastic failure), but also compensation or reduction of <strong>the</strong> disordersand improvement <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g skills due to <strong>in</strong>tensiveremedial activities and learn<strong>in</strong>g. Importantly, withdrawal from specialeducational activities and tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, lack of sufficient self-control whileread<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong>tensive stress, tiredness or <strong>in</strong>ability to concentratecan br<strong>in</strong>g about <strong>the</strong> re-occurrence or <strong>in</strong>tensification of specific learn<strong>in</strong>gdifficulties (Bogdanowicz & Adryjanek, 2004).Signs of dyslexia <strong>in</strong> older <strong>in</strong>dividualsLet us now concentrate on older <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia. It appearsthat certa<strong>in</strong> symptoms described above with regard to younger childrenare no longer discernible <strong>in</strong> older learners, but some o<strong>the</strong>r difficultiesendure. What is more, new areas of difficulty, for example, problems <strong>in</strong>foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g (FLL) or <strong>in</strong> undertak<strong>in</strong>g sports with success mayemerge. O<strong>the</strong>r problematic doma<strong>in</strong>s <strong>in</strong>clude geography (read<strong>in</strong>g maps;directions), arithmetic, geometry, chemistry, music (musical notation),biology (complex term<strong>in</strong>ology) or history (dates, names, chronology), tomention just a few.Frequently, <strong>in</strong>tensified read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties largely dim<strong>in</strong>ish and whatusually rema<strong>in</strong>s unchanged is a slow rate of read<strong>in</strong>g and a negativeattitude towards <strong>the</strong> activity of read<strong>in</strong>g as such. Still, <strong>in</strong>tensity, types andprevalence of read<strong>in</strong>g errors would certa<strong>in</strong>ly depend on <strong>the</strong> orthographicdepth of a given language and adopted read<strong>in</strong>g strategies. 4 For <strong>in</strong>stance,vowel misread<strong>in</strong>gs frequently occur <strong>in</strong> English. It can be attributable tocomplicated letter-to-sound mapp<strong>in</strong>gs, with yet much more complexrelations for vowels than for consonants. Whereas <strong>in</strong> Italian or Spanish,vowel misread<strong>in</strong>gs are ra<strong>the</strong>r rare because of <strong>the</strong> roughly equallyconsistent grapheme-phoneme relations for vowels and consonants. Infact, young Spanish-speak<strong>in</strong>g children tend to commit most read<strong>in</strong>gerrors with regard to three context-dependent consonants (c, g, r which have two dist<strong>in</strong>ct pronunciations, depend<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r letters<strong>in</strong> a word) and not <strong>in</strong> visually similar consonants (d, b, g, p, q)(Goikoetxea, 2006). Davies et al. (2007) observe that Spanish dyslexic andregular readers differ significantly with respect to quantity but not <strong>the</strong>type of errors <strong>the</strong>y make. Children with dyslexia are less efficient readersand produce significantly more errors than controls. The errors <strong>in</strong>decod<strong>in</strong>g pr<strong>in</strong>t ma<strong>in</strong>ly take <strong>the</strong> form of additions, deletions or substitutions,reflect<strong>in</strong>g possibly both impaired phonological and orthographic(grapheme-to-phoneme) process<strong>in</strong>g.The number of specific spell<strong>in</strong>g mistakes, such as additions, deletionsor substitutions of parts of words, or reversals, may slightly decreasewith age and education, however, recurrent orthographic mistakes(difficulty choos<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> appropriate spell<strong>in</strong>g choice for a given sound,


Symptoms and Identification of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 91which is especially conspicuous <strong>in</strong> deep orthographies) notoriouslyprevail. Tell<strong>in</strong>g similar sounds apart and writ<strong>in</strong>g dictations rema<strong>in</strong>s ademand<strong>in</strong>g task both <strong>in</strong> terms of s<strong>in</strong>gl<strong>in</strong>g out and recognis<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividualsounds <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> right sequence as well as apply<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> phoneme-graphemeconversion rules.The follow<strong>in</strong>g are examples of dyslexic spell<strong>in</strong>gs with skipped, addedor changed letters, syllables or parts of words: ‘trick’ for ‘tick’, ‘walk’ for‘walk<strong>in</strong>g’, ‘sudly’ for ‘suddenly’, ‘rember’ for ‘remember’, ‘amt’ for‘amount’, ‘merember’ for ‘remember’, ‘tow’ for ‘two’, ‘pakr’ for ‘park’,‘sitesr’ for ‘sister’. Peculiar dyslexic difficulty also lies <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> loweredability to divide sentences <strong>in</strong>to words or to keep word boundaries, for<strong>in</strong>stance ‘a no<strong>the</strong>r’ for ‘ano<strong>the</strong>r’, ‘firstones’ for ‘first ones’, ‘halfanhour’for ‘half an hour’. Similarly, divid<strong>in</strong>g words <strong>in</strong>to syllables and constituentphonemes and difficulty <strong>in</strong> differentiat<strong>in</strong>g between similar sounds,which leads to choos<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>correct graphic representation for <strong>the</strong>targeted word, proves problematic. Time and time aga<strong>in</strong>, phoneticspell<strong>in</strong>g (e.g. ‘yoos’ for ‘use’, ‘wokt’ for ‘walked’, ‘mendid’ for ‘mended’)but also bizarre and <strong>in</strong>consistent spell<strong>in</strong>g, unusual sequenc<strong>in</strong>g of lettersor multiple attempts at spell<strong>in</strong>g a target word (e.g. ‘schule’, ‘skchool’,‘school’) may occur <strong>in</strong> one piece of dyslexic writ<strong>in</strong>g. Spell<strong>in</strong>g of<strong>in</strong>dividual words can be so deformed that <strong>the</strong>ir decod<strong>in</strong>g provesvirtually impossible.Occasionally, some older students with dyslexia can cont<strong>in</strong>ually showbelow-standard ability to dist<strong>in</strong>guish between letters of similar shapeand to rewrite texts correctly, which is characteristic for youngerchildren. Low-grade ability to discrim<strong>in</strong>ate and remember letter shapesapplies, <strong>in</strong> particular, to letters similar <strong>in</strong> shape (a-o, m-n, l-t, hence ‘cat’for ‘cot’, ‘moon’ for ‘noon’) as well as letters similar <strong>in</strong> shape but differ<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir position <strong>in</strong> space (p-g-b-d; m-w, n-u, hence ‘bady’ for ‘baby’,‘dot’ for ‘got’, ‘brown’ for ‘drown’, ‘pig’ for ‘dig’, ‘pug’ for ‘bud’).Invert<strong>in</strong>g words dynamically (‘no’ for ‘on’, ‘was’ for ‘saw’, ‘dog’ for‘god’, ‘gip’ for ‘pig’), neglect<strong>in</strong>g diacritical marks and misus<strong>in</strong>g lowercaseand uppercase letters (‘daDDy’ for ‘daddy’) happens at times (seeAppendix 3 and Appendix 4).The traditional division of read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g errors <strong>in</strong>to visual andauditory, be<strong>in</strong>g directly traced back to disorders of visual and auditoryperception, respectively, though not completely free from faults, isfrequently presented <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> literature and, at least <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Polish context,heavily relied on by diagnosticians and practitioners (Czajkowska &Herda, 1998; Górniewicz, 1998; Jędrzejowska & Jurek, 2003; Mickiewicz,1997; Opolska, 1997; Saduś, 2000, 2003; Skibińska, 2001; Zakrzewska,1999). Beyond doubt, <strong>the</strong> plausibility of divid<strong>in</strong>g dyslexic errors <strong>in</strong>toauditory and visual seems questionable because an unequivocal qualificationof <strong>the</strong> nature of an error <strong>in</strong>to visual or auditory seems highly


92 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>problematic (e.g. confus<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> letters ‘b’ and ‘p’) (Mickiewicz, 1997).Despite <strong>the</strong> apparent unreliability of such divisions, <strong>the</strong>y serve asfoundations for typologies of dyslexia based on symptoms (read<strong>in</strong>gspell<strong>in</strong>gpattern and types of errors). For example, such a typology ofdyslexia, frequently referred to <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> literature (Bakker et al., 1995;Bednarek, 1999; Borkowska, 1998; Dockrell & McShane, 1993; Johnson,1978; Krasowicz, 1997; Krasowicz-Kupis, 2008; Ott, 1997; Reid, 1998;Stamboltzis & Pumfrey, 2000), was proposed by Boder (1973). Threesubtypes of dyslexia suggested by Boder are as follows: dysphonetic(auditory), dyseidetic (visual) and mixed. 5 Bogdanowicz (1997a/2000,1999) suggests <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g subtypes visual, auditory, <strong>in</strong>tegrative andmixed, aga<strong>in</strong> based on differ<strong>in</strong>g underly<strong>in</strong>g pathomechanisms, responsiblefor certa<strong>in</strong> types of errors. 6The reliability of classifications based on symptoms may be reducedby uncerta<strong>in</strong>ties regard<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> credibility of qualify<strong>in</strong>g errors as visual orauditory (Borkowska, 1998). Notwithstand<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> considerable popularityof such classifications among practitioners, <strong>in</strong> light of <strong>the</strong> presentknowledge about <strong>the</strong> causes of dyslexia, <strong>the</strong>y seem to have ra<strong>the</strong>rhistorical value (Krasowicz-Kupis, 2008). For example, research verify<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong> causal mechanisms of visual and auditory spell<strong>in</strong>g mistakes <strong>in</strong> Polishdid not <strong>in</strong>dicate that <strong>the</strong>y are caused by dist<strong>in</strong>ct mechanisms, hence <strong>the</strong>reliability of <strong>the</strong> abovementioned typology rema<strong>in</strong>s unconfirmed (Pietras,2007, 2008).Pietras (2007) proposes that spell<strong>in</strong>g errors be qualified <strong>in</strong>to threecategories. <strong>Language</strong> errors constitute <strong>the</strong> first category, which a givenspell<strong>in</strong>g attempt is phonologically <strong>in</strong>accurate <strong>the</strong> phonological structureof a word is distorted, thus <strong>the</strong> correspond<strong>in</strong>g orthographicmapp<strong>in</strong>g is <strong>in</strong>accurate for <strong>the</strong> targeted word. The second category memory errors are phonologically accurate spell<strong>in</strong>g attempts butorthographically <strong>in</strong>accurate, <strong>the</strong>y are decodable and reta<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> phonologicalstructure of a word, however <strong>the</strong> choice of sound-to-lettermapp<strong>in</strong>gs is <strong>in</strong>appropriate (e.g. ‘re<strong>in</strong>’ for ‘ra<strong>in</strong>’, ‘eeg’ for ‘egg’). F<strong>in</strong>ally,errors <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g skipp<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> t<strong>in</strong>y elements, such as diacritical marks, orconfus<strong>in</strong>g letters similar <strong>in</strong> shape are called visual. Krasowicz-Kupis(2008) suggests that, generally, it seems to be <strong>the</strong> quantitative factor thatis more important <strong>in</strong> evaluation of dyslexic errors, hence tak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> totalnumber of errors as an <strong>in</strong>dicator of dyslexic spell<strong>in</strong>g difficulty. Whilewith regard to qualitative analysis, a broad division <strong>in</strong>to phonologicallyaccurate or <strong>in</strong>accurate spell<strong>in</strong>gs is advised.Low-standard level of precision of hand and f<strong>in</strong>ger movement (f<strong>in</strong>emotor skills), caus<strong>in</strong>g un<strong>in</strong>telligible handwrit<strong>in</strong>g, is aga<strong>in</strong> and aga<strong>in</strong>reported <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia. Due to an awkward pen grip,writ<strong>in</strong>g tends to be slow and subject to numerous corrections. Additionally,co-movements of o<strong>the</strong>r parts of <strong>the</strong> body (legs, tongue) can be


Symptoms and Identification of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 93observed dur<strong>in</strong>g writ<strong>in</strong>g. Letters tend to be poorly formed and notappropriately connected; <strong>the</strong>y may well drift off <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tended angle,direction and spatial position (see Appendix 5 and Appendix 6). F<strong>in</strong>ally,details such as periods, commas and capitals are very often omitted,spaces between letters and words can be irregular, and, as alreadymentioned, l<strong>in</strong>es are cont<strong>in</strong>ually skipped.<strong>Language</strong> deficit <strong>in</strong> dyslexia, by def<strong>in</strong>ition connected with poorphonological process<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> some cases may also extend to loweredmorphological, syntactic and/or syntactic abilities, which accompanydisordered phonological functions. Students with dyslexia have beenreported to produce ungrammatical utterances, especially with regard toapply<strong>in</strong>g phrases that describe spatial relations (Borkowska, 1997, 1998;Krasowicz, 1997; Krasowicz-Kupis, 2003, 2006; Oszwa, 2000; Schneider,1999). Comprehend<strong>in</strong>g and retriev<strong>in</strong>g abstract and complex sentences,especially those composed of multiple clauses, such as relative orsubord<strong>in</strong>ate clauses, frequently poses irrefutable difficulty on childrenwith dyslexia. The most pronounced problems <strong>in</strong> understand<strong>in</strong>g syntacticstructures can concern questions, relations between direct and <strong>in</strong>directobjects as well as <strong>the</strong> passive voice (Rosenthal, 1970; Schneider, 1999;Slob<strong>in</strong>, 1971; Wiig & Semel, 1984). Learners with dyslexia may demonstratepoor familiarity with syntactic rules, perta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> transformationalcomplexity and length of <strong>the</strong> sentences; as a consequence, <strong>the</strong>y mayproduce less structurally advanced sentences mostly short, positive,declarative and <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> active voice. Often, numerous syntactic mistakesoccur, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g grammatically <strong>in</strong>correct structures and <strong>in</strong>appropriateuse of parts of speech. These deficits have been observed <strong>in</strong> dyslexicchildren and adolescents alike (Wiig & Semel, 1976).Similarly, research conducted by Krasowicz (1997) among Polishchildren regard<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>terdependence between <strong>the</strong> level of languageacquisition and read<strong>in</strong>g ability <strong>in</strong> children, has shown that <strong>the</strong> syntacticstructure of <strong>the</strong> utterances produced by children with dyslexia ischaracterised by <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g factors: reduced content of <strong>the</strong> utterance,use of significantly shorter sentences, more <strong>in</strong>stances of simple thancomplex sentences, and, f<strong>in</strong>ally, greater number of <strong>in</strong>complete sentenceswhen compared with <strong>the</strong> utterances produced by non-dyslexic children.Oszwa (2000) discusses <strong>in</strong>dications of agrammatisms as perta<strong>in</strong>s to <strong>the</strong>use of prepositions by Polish children with dyslexia. The number ofprepositions used by children with dyslexia while describ<strong>in</strong>g a picture issignificantly smaller as compared to non-dyslexics. Additionally, derivationof nouns and adjectives as well as <strong>the</strong> use of complex prepositionstend to be difficult for <strong>the</strong>se children.Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Vogel (1983), <strong>the</strong> use of <strong>in</strong>flectional, grammaticalmorphemes is sometimes considerably reduced <strong>in</strong> children with dyslexiaas compared to good readers. Correct use of <strong>the</strong> third person s<strong>in</strong>gular


94 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>present tense marker ‘-s’, <strong>the</strong> progressive aspect marker ‘-<strong>in</strong>g’, tensemarkers, regular and irregular plural markers, <strong>the</strong> comparative marker‘-er’, <strong>the</strong> superlative marker ‘-est’, <strong>the</strong> adverb marker ‘-ly’ as well assuffixes can constitute a source of perceivable confusion for children withdyslexia who learn English (Wiig et al., 1973; Wiig & Semel, 1984).Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, children with dyslexia can also show a subtle deficit on<strong>the</strong> semantic level of language process<strong>in</strong>g. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Krasowicz(1997), this is ma<strong>in</strong>ly reflected <strong>in</strong> disordered organisation of <strong>the</strong> narrativeutterances, <strong>the</strong>ir poor structure and content. Syntactic deficit concernsdifficulties <strong>in</strong> identify<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> sett<strong>in</strong>g of a story (characters, time andplace), <strong>the</strong> ma<strong>in</strong> plot and <strong>the</strong> resolution. The utterances formed bylearners with dyslexia conta<strong>in</strong> a considerable number of omissions anddeformations of <strong>the</strong> basic <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> story. The most oftenneglected elements concern <strong>the</strong> identification of characters and <strong>the</strong> sphereof spatial-temporal relations. The observed disorders may be caused bydeficits <strong>in</strong> cognitive functions, relat<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistic process<strong>in</strong>g andorganisation of <strong>in</strong>formation. Similarly, Wiig and Semel (1984) mentiondyslexic semantic difficulty <strong>in</strong> discrim<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g between <strong>the</strong> spatial (‘<strong>in</strong>’,‘on’, ‘by’, ‘at’, ‘to’) and temporal (‘yesterday’, ‘today’, ‘tomorrow’)references, adjective and adverbial time markers (‘last’, ‘first’, ‘before’,‘after’), directional changes (‘up’, ‘down’) and <strong>in</strong>def<strong>in</strong>ite (‘someone’,‘anyone’, ‘someth<strong>in</strong>g’), demonstrative (‘that’, ‘those’) and reflexive (‘a girlwashed himself’) pronouns. Moreover, it is argued (Wiig & Semel, 1976)that some <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia may experience plausible difficulties<strong>in</strong> differentiat<strong>in</strong>g between <strong>the</strong> various mean<strong>in</strong>gs of a word, whichdepends on <strong>the</strong> changes of context. Sometimes, <strong>the</strong>y also demonstratesizeable difficulty <strong>in</strong> understand<strong>in</strong>g synonyms, semantic transformations,connected with recognis<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> alterations <strong>in</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g, and semanticimplications def<strong>in</strong>ed as an ability to comprehend <strong>in</strong>formation that is<strong>in</strong>directly presented.As Borkowska and Tarkowski (1990) claim, l<strong>in</strong>guistic competence,def<strong>in</strong>ed as a capacity to use language, as well as communicativecompetence, signified as an ability to use language efficiently <strong>in</strong> social<strong>in</strong>teractions, are beyond doubt limited <strong>in</strong> children with dyslexia.Borkowska (1998) fur<strong>the</strong>r argues that <strong>the</strong>re exists a relationship between<strong>the</strong> level of acquisition of semantic rules as well as communicativeness ofdiscourse and <strong>the</strong> occurrence of specific difficulties <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g andspell<strong>in</strong>g. Learners with dyslexia may show deficits <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> acquisition ofsemantic rules, which leads to construal of poorly organised narrativediscourse (e.g. <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> situation of describ<strong>in</strong>g a picture and tell<strong>in</strong>g a story),which is characterised by significantly simpler, shorter or <strong>in</strong>completeepisodic structure. Moreover, <strong>in</strong> reported research, children with dyslexiaconstruct <strong>the</strong>ir narrative discourse from <strong>the</strong> reduced number of pieces of


Symptoms and Identification of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 95<strong>in</strong>formation, thus mak<strong>in</strong>g it appreciably less communicative. Additionally,children with dyslexia often encounter difficulties <strong>in</strong> accommodat<strong>in</strong>gdiscourse to <strong>the</strong> social situation that <strong>the</strong>y f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong>mselves <strong>in</strong> and<strong>the</strong>se difficulties tend to <strong>in</strong>crease with age (Borkowska, 1998).Some older students with dyslexia are reported to display poor grossmotor skills and deficient balance, hence an impression of overallclums<strong>in</strong>ess, which, unfortunately, does not help <strong>the</strong>m <strong>in</strong> undertak<strong>in</strong>gsports activities. For that reason, <strong>the</strong>se students may f<strong>in</strong>d PE classes leastenjoyable. In addition, <strong>the</strong>y can be characterised by poor orientation <strong>in</strong>space and body schemata, especially with reference to denot<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> rightside versus left. Proper time management seems to pose a noticeableburden on <strong>the</strong>se students, as <strong>the</strong>y struggle try<strong>in</strong>g to organise <strong>the</strong>ir timeefficiently as well as to remember dates and events, particularly <strong>in</strong>chronological order. In addition, phonological sequential memory can bedeficient, which translates <strong>in</strong>to extreme difficulty <strong>in</strong> recall<strong>in</strong>g sequencesof terms and names, such as months of <strong>the</strong> year, or call<strong>in</strong>g to m<strong>in</strong>d andfollow<strong>in</strong>g a series of complex multistep <strong>in</strong>structions. What is more, do<strong>in</strong>gsimple mental ma<strong>the</strong>matical calculations and us<strong>in</strong>g multiplication tablesmay seem obscure. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, students happen to pronounce distortedwords and spoonerisms, <strong>the</strong>y are often lost for words andforgetful, and apparently require more time for retriev<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formationfrom memory. As for <strong>the</strong> poor manual precision or low-grade f<strong>in</strong>e motorskills, draw<strong>in</strong>gs can be of a particularly poor quality, written work messyor even un<strong>in</strong>telligible, with badly formed letters and spell<strong>in</strong>gs crossedout. However, handwrit<strong>in</strong>g may be surpris<strong>in</strong>gly neat but, at <strong>the</strong> sametime, writ<strong>in</strong>g speed substantially decreases. Generally, an ability toorganise and compose a piece of writ<strong>in</strong>g is much below standard <strong>in</strong>terms of accuracy, content and time efficiency, unlike frequent advancedoral capability (Bogdanowicz & Adryjanek, 2004).It needs stress<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>in</strong>dividual students with dyslexia demonstratecerta<strong>in</strong>ly not all but only diversified sets or clusters of <strong>the</strong> abovementioneddifficulties. There apparently exists an impressive variance asto <strong>the</strong> areas and severity of difficulties, levels of creativeness or sport<strong>in</strong>gfaculties across <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia, let alone various talents andstrengths <strong>the</strong>y have at <strong>the</strong>ir disposal.Most of <strong>the</strong> symptoms described above may persevere <strong>in</strong> secondaryschool students with dyslexia. In addition, problems more specific toparticular school subjects, for <strong>in</strong>stance foreign languages, can <strong>in</strong>tensify.Generally, <strong>the</strong> difficulties encountered by learners with dyslexia at highereducational stages undeniably become more global <strong>in</strong> character.Among adults with dyslexia, two types of readers are usuallyobserved and reported <strong>in</strong> research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs those who, despiteconsiderable problems experienced dur<strong>in</strong>g literacy acquisition, manage


96 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>to achieve high-grade read<strong>in</strong>g proficiency, allow<strong>in</strong>g access to highereducation facilities. The second group is composed of <strong>in</strong>dividuals whosecompensation mechanisms did not prevent failure <strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>gsufficient read<strong>in</strong>g skills, hence <strong>the</strong>ir enormous struggle <strong>in</strong> adapt<strong>in</strong>g tocommon formal school<strong>in</strong>g requirements. Still, even those adults who arecapable of compensat<strong>in</strong>g for <strong>the</strong>ir dyslexic difficulties and demonstratecomparatively high read<strong>in</strong>g performance are characterised by a relativelystable cognitive profile, with prevail<strong>in</strong>g phonological process<strong>in</strong>g difficulties.They may demonstrate <strong>in</strong>adequate phonological representationsand f<strong>in</strong>d it abnormally difficult to form new ones as well (Snowl<strong>in</strong>g &Nation, 1997). These difficulties are most evident <strong>in</strong> approach<strong>in</strong>g tasks,which call for heavy reliance on phonological skills, such as unfamiliarword read<strong>in</strong>g or pseudo-word decod<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> addition, spell<strong>in</strong>g seemsquite pa<strong>in</strong>ful (Oren & Breznitz, 2005). All <strong>in</strong> all, dyslexic symptoms <strong>in</strong>adulthood can be summarised as <strong>the</strong> impairment of vary<strong>in</strong>g degrees <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong> accuracy of decod<strong>in</strong>g and speed of word recognition (fluency)(Breznitz, 2003). Inasmuch as <strong>the</strong> former can possibly improve <strong>in</strong>adulthood, especially with regard to text, <strong>the</strong> latter may rema<strong>in</strong> slower<strong>in</strong> comparison to regular adult readers. It appears that <strong>the</strong> skill of<strong>in</strong>ferr<strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>g from contextual cues enables some adult dyslexicreaders to achieve high-standard read<strong>in</strong>g comprehension. None<strong>the</strong>less,ra<strong>the</strong>r poor accuracy still seems quite prom<strong>in</strong>ent <strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle word andpseudo-word decod<strong>in</strong>g. Converg<strong>in</strong>g research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>deed confirmthat dyslexic students’ accuracy ga<strong>in</strong>s <strong>in</strong>crease as <strong>the</strong>y progress <strong>in</strong> school,however <strong>the</strong>y cont<strong>in</strong>ually show low-grade read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g fluency.In fact, read<strong>in</strong>g fluency level can best discrim<strong>in</strong>ate between collegestudents with and without dyslexia (Gregg et al., 2005).Some <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia also self-report be<strong>in</strong>g disorganisedand forgetful throughout life, which can probably be traced back to poorconcentration abilities. As children, <strong>the</strong>y may often fail to recall <strong>the</strong>irhomework, while as adults <strong>the</strong>y admit lett<strong>in</strong>g slip telephone numbers,messages, names, appo<strong>in</strong>tments and dates; gett<strong>in</strong>g an impression that<strong>the</strong>y most certa<strong>in</strong>ly are <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> wrong place at <strong>the</strong> wrong time. Sometimesadults with dyslexia fail <strong>in</strong> dist<strong>in</strong>guish<strong>in</strong>g left from right and f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong>ir way <strong>in</strong> an unknown place, not to mention gett<strong>in</strong>g on a wrong busbecause of erroneous perception of <strong>the</strong> position of figures (e.g. 69 for 96).In addition, <strong>the</strong>y report misarticulat<strong>in</strong>g multisyllabic words, mispronounc<strong>in</strong>gnames and surnames, committ<strong>in</strong>g spell<strong>in</strong>g mistakes andavoid<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g. Driv<strong>in</strong>g a car, especially <strong>in</strong> an unfamiliar area, fill<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> forms, learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> sequences of movements <strong>in</strong> an aerobics or danceclass, quickly retriev<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation from memory all constituteexamples of activities that can be confus<strong>in</strong>g for adults with dyslexia,who are o<strong>the</strong>rwise perceived as <strong>in</strong>telligent, talented and creative people


Symptoms and Identification of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 97(Bogdanowicz, 2003a; Bogdanowicz & Adryjanek, 2004; Bogdanowicz &Krasowicz-Kupis, 2005a).Emotional-motivational Disorders <strong>in</strong> <strong>Dyslexia</strong>The advocates of <strong>the</strong> psychogenic aetiological <strong>the</strong>ory of dyslexia (psychodyslexia)claim that specific read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>in</strong> children are caused bydisorders <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> emotional sphere. In view of <strong>the</strong> above statement,dyslexia should be treated as a symptom of emotional disorders(Krasowicz, 1997). However, it needs stress<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>in</strong> light of recentf<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs perta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> causes of dyslexia, this <strong>the</strong>ory underwen<strong>the</strong>avy criticism and was discredited, as no conv<strong>in</strong>c<strong>in</strong>g evidence wasprovided to support it (Zakrzewska, 1999). Naturally, an <strong>in</strong>appropriatecourse of <strong>the</strong> emotional-motivational processes may exert negative<strong>in</strong>fluence on <strong>the</strong> ability to read and spell, however, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>se cases <strong>the</strong>result<strong>in</strong>g difficulties are not of a dyslexic character (Bogdanowicz,2003a).In fact, <strong>the</strong> emotional-motivational and social perturbations that canbe experienced by <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia are beyond doubt secondaryto <strong>the</strong>ir scholastic failure <strong>in</strong> acquir<strong>in</strong>g literacy skills. Their onset is notevident at <strong>the</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of children’s educational career but later, whenread<strong>in</strong>g problems become <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly pronounced and, sometimes,close to impossible to overcome. Thus, disorders of emotional and socialfunction<strong>in</strong>g and personality disorders can <strong>in</strong>deed coexist with dyslexia,but as a consequence ra<strong>the</strong>r than a cause of difficulties <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g andspell<strong>in</strong>g (Bogdanowicz, 1989, 1999, 2002a; Czajkowska & Herda, 1998;Krasowicz-Kupis, 2008; Zakrzewska, 1999). Individuals with dyslexia arereported to frequently suffer from emotional-motivational problems ofvarious <strong>in</strong>tensity with regard to low self-perception, low self-esteem, lackof confidence, feel<strong>in</strong>gs of shame, fear, embarrassment and frustration,caused by experienc<strong>in</strong>g constant failure <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> acquisition of read<strong>in</strong>g andspell<strong>in</strong>g skills. Common negative attitudes of peers and, sometimes,parents, lack of <strong>the</strong>ir acceptance and support can markedly enhance <strong>the</strong>abovementioned negative feel<strong>in</strong>gs (Bogdanowicz, 1985a; Elliott & Place,2000; G<strong>in</strong>drich, 2002; Ledwoch, 1999). Possible frustration experiencedby children with dyslexia, brought about by <strong>the</strong>ir lowered ability tosuccessfully acquire literacy skills, can frequently be aggravated byfeel<strong>in</strong>g unable to meet <strong>the</strong> expectations of <strong>the</strong>ir relatives, teachers andpeers (Ryan, 1992).Importantly, dyslexics constitute a group at considerable risk foremotional disorders, still <strong>the</strong>y do not constitute a homogeneous group asregards <strong>the</strong>ir emotional function<strong>in</strong>g (Porter & Rourke, 1985). Somechildren with dyslexia do not categorically manifest any evidentsymptoms of emotional disorders, which would noticeably dist<strong>in</strong>guish


98 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong><strong>the</strong>m from <strong>the</strong>ir peers at school and <strong>the</strong>y function well <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> educationalenvironment (Krasowicz-Kupis, 2008). By contrast, <strong>in</strong> some children withdyslexia <strong>the</strong> relationship between read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g difficulties andemotional disorders appears to be more apparent and undeniable than <strong>in</strong>o<strong>the</strong>rs. Research outcomes <strong>in</strong>dicate <strong>the</strong> occurrence of difficulties <strong>in</strong>gett<strong>in</strong>g to sleep, nightmares, aversion to school, constant worry<strong>in</strong>g,<strong>in</strong>creased self-criticism, shyness, timidity, all too often lead<strong>in</strong>g todifficulty <strong>in</strong> establish<strong>in</strong>g contacts with peers, isolation at school, grosslydisordered <strong>in</strong>terpersonal encounters and even symptoms of childdepression. What is more, several somatic disorders such as los<strong>in</strong>gconsciousness, frequent headaches and dizz<strong>in</strong>ess, <strong>the</strong> feel<strong>in</strong>g of constanttiredness and exhaustion, not to mention stomach aches and pa<strong>in</strong>s <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>chest, may at times be evidenced (Porter & Rourke, 1985).Failure <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and spell and <strong>in</strong> meet<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> expectations ofo<strong>the</strong>rs, unsurpris<strong>in</strong>gly, can <strong>in</strong>vite misbehaviour of diverse harshness,from daydream<strong>in</strong>g, not focus<strong>in</strong>g and gett<strong>in</strong>g distracted by virtuallyeveryth<strong>in</strong>g to employ<strong>in</strong>g avoidance tactics (sharpen<strong>in</strong>g pencils, look<strong>in</strong>gfor books, go<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> toilet) and disruptive (class clown) or withdrawaltendencies. In rare cases, low resistance to frustration can culm<strong>in</strong>ate <strong>in</strong>attacks of anger and rage, hostility or aggressive manner towards o<strong>the</strong>rpeople and, unfortunately, sometimes even turn to juvenile del<strong>in</strong>quency.Ryan (1992) claims that <strong>the</strong> obvious target of this anger would be school,teachers and parents.Anxiety appears to be frequently reported as an emotional symptom <strong>in</strong>children with dyslexia, who become fearful due to cont<strong>in</strong>ual dissatisfaction,disappo<strong>in</strong>tment and confusion at school. Anxiety forces people toavoid whatever frightens <strong>the</strong>m. As a consequence, new situations andexperiences may become extremely anxiety provok<strong>in</strong>g, hence steer<strong>in</strong>gclear of undertak<strong>in</strong>g challenges. All too often <strong>the</strong>re seems to exist amarked tendency among teachers and parents to <strong>in</strong>terpret such avoidancebehaviour as laz<strong>in</strong>ess. Carroll and Iles (2006) extend <strong>the</strong> research ondyslexic vulnerability to anxiety as an emotional consequence ofscholastic failure to higher education students, who, be<strong>in</strong>g highlycapable, most probably manage to adapt successful cop<strong>in</strong>g strategies <strong>in</strong>order to compensate for <strong>the</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>the</strong>y experience. Hence<strong>the</strong> shift of focus from detrimental feel<strong>in</strong>gs of <strong>in</strong>adequacy and academic<strong>in</strong>competence <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> context of literacy acquisition <strong>in</strong> children, towards<strong>the</strong> current worries of <strong>the</strong> university undergraduates. Observed are <strong>the</strong>elevated trait anxiety (def<strong>in</strong>ed as a ra<strong>the</strong>r stable personality feature) levels<strong>in</strong> students with dyslexia, much higher <strong>in</strong> comparison to what is shownby age-matched read<strong>in</strong>g unimpaired students. Sadly, <strong>the</strong> dyslexicemotional perturbation is not limited to academic tasks, but it generalisesto <strong>in</strong>volve social function<strong>in</strong>g as well. In addition, tasks and situationsdemand<strong>in</strong>g literacy accuracy, e.g. read<strong>in</strong>g skills, repeatedly evoke <strong>the</strong>


Symptoms and Identification of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 99feel<strong>in</strong>gs of stress and worry, translatable <strong>in</strong>to state anxiety (def<strong>in</strong>ed as asubjective feel<strong>in</strong>g of emotional tension evoked by a particular situationperceived as self-threaten<strong>in</strong>g).With regard to foreign language study, Piechurska-Kuciel (2008)<strong>in</strong>vestigates <strong>the</strong> effect of dyslexia on language anxiety at different stagesof language process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> high school students. Individuals withsymptoms of dyslexia typically and repeatedly demonstrate significantlyhigher levels of language anxiety at all stages of language process<strong>in</strong>g(<strong>in</strong>put, process<strong>in</strong>g and output) than regular foreign language learners.Dyslexic language anxiety at <strong>in</strong>put and process<strong>in</strong>g stages seems toparallel <strong>the</strong> general tendency observed among unimpaired students todecrease substantially after <strong>the</strong> first year of learn<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>the</strong>n to rema<strong>in</strong>stable till <strong>the</strong> end of <strong>the</strong> three-year study period at secondary school.However, when it comes to output anxiety, unlike <strong>the</strong>ir peers, learnerswith dyslexia evidence permanently high levels throughout <strong>the</strong> time ofhigh school foreign language study, which potentially detracts from <strong>the</strong>standard of <strong>the</strong>ir foreign language production.Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, Ledwoch (1999) claims that particular developmentaldeficits of cognitive processes have a diverse <strong>in</strong>fluence on <strong>the</strong> emotionalconduct of children with dyslexia. Thus, on <strong>the</strong> one hand, children whorepeatedly demonstrate a hurdle <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> auditory-l<strong>in</strong>guistic process<strong>in</strong>g areoften observed to experience more perplexity <strong>in</strong> establish<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terpersonalcontacts, manifested by apprehensiveness, lack of confidence and lowself-esteem. Never<strong>the</strong>less, <strong>the</strong>y seem to function quite adequately <strong>in</strong>social situations requir<strong>in</strong>g good visual-spatial abilities and <strong>in</strong>ter-modal<strong>in</strong>tegration. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, children with non-verbal learn<strong>in</strong>gdisabilities seem to be markedly more prone to experienc<strong>in</strong>g emotionaldisorders due to perceptual and cognitive deficits, which are crucial forsocial function<strong>in</strong>g. The difficulties perta<strong>in</strong> to visual-spatial <strong>in</strong>formationprocess<strong>in</strong>g, responsible for <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g such non-verbal <strong>in</strong>dicators as, forexample, face expression, gesticulation or body movements. In addition,as stressed by Ryan (1992), some <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia may show atendency to encounter problems <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g social clues; moreover,<strong>the</strong>ir reluctance to <strong>in</strong>itiate and ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> relationships with o<strong>the</strong>r people(draw<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong> habit of avoidance of potentially difficult and failure<strong>in</strong>stigat<strong>in</strong>g situations) can give an impression of <strong>the</strong>m be<strong>in</strong>g awkward,which may lead to ill social function<strong>in</strong>g.The follow<strong>in</strong>g is a seem<strong>in</strong>gly obvious conclud<strong>in</strong>g remark it is ofparamount importance that educators and parents possess an adequateunderstand<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> nature of dyslexia and are sensitive to problemsexperienced by <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia. Then, hopefully, fewerchildren will be prone to develop<strong>in</strong>g emotional and personality disordersdue to scholastic failure.


100 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Identification of <strong>Dyslexia</strong>A detailed description of all aspects of assessment, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g a broadbattery of diagnostic tests, is beyond <strong>the</strong> scope of <strong>the</strong> present section,ra<strong>the</strong>r our aim here is to draw <strong>the</strong> attention of <strong>the</strong> reader to <strong>the</strong> generalobjectives and ma<strong>in</strong> issues related to identification.It appears that <strong>in</strong>tensive research on dyslexia <strong>in</strong>vites multiple attempts,which often depend on general discrepancy and exclusion criteria, todef<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong> condition. The discrepancy criterion concerns significant<strong>in</strong>congruity between <strong>the</strong> unexpectedly poor scholastic achievement of achild <strong>in</strong> a certa<strong>in</strong> area of study and his/her age and/or level of<strong>in</strong>telligence. The exclusionary criterion <strong>in</strong>dicates critical differencesexist<strong>in</strong>g between dyslexic and o<strong>the</strong>r read<strong>in</strong>g disordered <strong>in</strong>dividualswhose read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g problems, unlike <strong>in</strong> dyslexics, may stemfrom mental retardation, sensory and emotional impairment, or <strong>the</strong><strong>in</strong>fluence of an environment disadvantaged with respect to f<strong>in</strong>ancial,economical, cultural or pedagogical aspects (Kasowicz-Kupis, 2008).The discrepancy criterion basically allows to dist<strong>in</strong>guish betweengeneral and specific learn<strong>in</strong>g disorders. It touches upon <strong>the</strong> significantgap between <strong>the</strong> actual read<strong>in</strong>g ability and <strong>the</strong> level of this ability thatcould be expected of age and/or IQ. The exclusionary criterion <strong>in</strong>dicatesthat read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties stemm<strong>in</strong>g from lowered <strong>in</strong>tellectual capacity,severe sensory, health or emotional impairment and <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluence of adisadvantaged background should not be qualified as dyslexia (e.g.Vellut<strong>in</strong>o, 1979). F<strong>in</strong>ally, a cl<strong>in</strong>ical criterion is anchored <strong>in</strong> identification ofkey symptoms. However, <strong>the</strong> reliability of this criterion seems to beconsiderably reduced by <strong>the</strong> prevail<strong>in</strong>g lack of consensus as to <strong>the</strong>collection of <strong>the</strong> most tell<strong>in</strong>g symptoms of dyslexia. Much as it is agreedthat persist<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g difficulty constitute <strong>the</strong> key signs <strong>in</strong>dyslexia, however a precise description and def<strong>in</strong>ition of this difficultyrema<strong>in</strong>s a matter of discussion (Kasowicz-Kupis, 2008). It seems to be <strong>the</strong>case that cl<strong>in</strong>icians conduct<strong>in</strong>g diagnosis towards dyslexia are confrontedwith <strong>the</strong> lack of def<strong>in</strong>itional consensus of <strong>the</strong> disorder (Helland, 2007;Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2001b). 7<strong>Dyslexia</strong> is rout<strong>in</strong>ely diagnosed no sooner than a child starts learn<strong>in</strong>gto read and spell. In Poland, for <strong>in</strong>stance, it is usually at <strong>the</strong> end of <strong>the</strong>second grade of primary school (Bogdanowicz, 1997c, 1999). Similarly,Wagner et al. (2005) report that <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> USA, children with learn<strong>in</strong>gdisabilities are typically identified and provided with remedial servicesnot until <strong>the</strong> second or third grade, due to <strong>the</strong> formal requirement of <strong>the</strong>significant discrepancy between expected (based on IQ) and observedlevels of achievement.While <strong>the</strong>re is common agreement that early diagnosis of learn<strong>in</strong>gdisorders is pivotal to <strong>the</strong> fur<strong>the</strong>r educational career of at-risk children,


Symptoms and Identification of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 101especially <strong>in</strong> light of research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that with age, read<strong>in</strong>gdifficulties become progressively <strong>in</strong>tractable, it appears that too frequentlythis postulate cannot be easily put <strong>in</strong>to practice. Variousuniversally applicable but also country- and language-specific factorsseem to contribute to such a state of affairs, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g lack of agreementas to <strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>g causes, def<strong>in</strong>ition and assessment criteria amongresearchers, unsatisfactory degree of awareness among some of <strong>the</strong>stakeholders or lack of resources <strong>in</strong> terms of appropriate standardiseddiagnostic measures, formal regulations, let alone money. This alreadycomplex situation is apparently aggravated <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> bil<strong>in</strong>gual or multil<strong>in</strong>gualcontext.Assessment of dyslexia <strong>in</strong> speakers of vary<strong>in</strong>g languages proves<strong>in</strong>tricate, especially <strong>in</strong> light of <strong>the</strong> fact that <strong>the</strong> same phonological deficitcan be capable of br<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g about different manifestations of read<strong>in</strong>gproblems across languages, depend<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong>ir orthographic depth.Wagner et al. (2005) speculate that as much as a mild phonologicalimpairment <strong>in</strong> Spanish-speak<strong>in</strong>g children tends not to deter <strong>the</strong>m fromacquir<strong>in</strong>g accurate word read<strong>in</strong>g ability, though slightly hamper<strong>in</strong>gfluency, <strong>in</strong> Spanish due to its transparent orthography, it would domore harm if <strong>the</strong>y were English L1 speakers. Hypo<strong>the</strong>tically, if <strong>the</strong> samechildren had an English native-language family background and were tofulfil <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g requirement <strong>in</strong> English, <strong>the</strong> same phonologicalimpairment would be very likely to <strong>in</strong>vite considerable problems <strong>in</strong>accurate and fluent read<strong>in</strong>g, possibly even fur<strong>the</strong>r exacerbated by<strong>in</strong>appropriate <strong>in</strong>struction, neglect<strong>in</strong>g explicit reference to <strong>the</strong> alphabeticpr<strong>in</strong>ciple. Such a view can be substantiated by <strong>the</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of research onread<strong>in</strong>g acquisition <strong>in</strong> alphabetic languages (Ziegler & Goswami, 2005,2006) <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that, first, read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>in</strong> all alphabetic languagesstudied to date can be tracked down to phonological process<strong>in</strong>gdisorders, second, <strong>the</strong> way letter-to-sound mapp<strong>in</strong>g is executed <strong>in</strong>different languages <strong>in</strong>fluences <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g strategies adopted by readers<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>se languages. 8Naturally, identify<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> cases of dyslexia among <strong>the</strong> multitude ofpoor readers is a difficult task. In <strong>the</strong> Polish context at least, diagnosistowards dyslexia is <strong>in</strong>terdiscipl<strong>in</strong>ary <strong>in</strong> character and requires a reliable,discern<strong>in</strong>g and multidimensional op<strong>in</strong>ion of various specialists, ma<strong>in</strong>ly ateacher <strong>the</strong>rapist and an educational psychologist. However, time andtime aga<strong>in</strong>, depend<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual needs and set of symptoms, <strong>the</strong>consultation and co-operation of o<strong>the</strong>r experts, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g a speech<strong>the</strong>rapist, a laryngologist, an ophthalmologist, a psychiatrist, a paediatricianor a neurologist may be necessary (Bogdanowicz, 1999, 2002b;Czajkowska & Herda, 1998; Jędrzejowska & Jurek, 2003; Mickiewicz,1997; Tomaszewska, 2001). A medical exam<strong>in</strong>ation, if it takes place, isusually selective and aimed at exclud<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> conceivable defects (e.g. of


102 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>vision and hear<strong>in</strong>g) and illnesses, potentially bear<strong>in</strong>g responsibility for<strong>the</strong> occurrence of difficulties <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g (Jaklewicz, 1997).Generally, <strong>the</strong> aim of <strong>the</strong> assessment procedures is to provide wellgrounded<strong>in</strong>formation concern<strong>in</strong>g: firstly, <strong>the</strong> classification of <strong>the</strong>disorder, secondly, an unequivocal identification of <strong>the</strong> deficient functionsthat directly condition <strong>the</strong> processes of read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g,thirdly, a tentative <strong>in</strong>dication of <strong>the</strong> possible underly<strong>in</strong>g causes whichdyslexic difficulties can be traced back to (Bogdanowicz, 2003b; Reid,1998). Last but not least, a diagnostic policy should <strong>in</strong>volve <strong>the</strong>anticipation and prognosis of fur<strong>the</strong>r development as well as <strong>in</strong>spire<strong>the</strong> design of an <strong>in</strong>dividual educational plan, tailored to <strong>the</strong> needs of agiven child (Bogdanowicz, 2002a; Górniewicz, 1998; Jędrzejowska &Jurek, 2003). Likewise, Reid (1998) rightly claims that assessment shouldbe l<strong>in</strong>ked to teach<strong>in</strong>g at all times. Thus, recommendations as to <strong>the</strong>design of <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>rapeutic programme make up an <strong>in</strong>dispensable part ofan official post-assessment op<strong>in</strong>ion. In fact, Vellut<strong>in</strong>o et al. (1998)advocate that <strong>in</strong>tervention should constitute a vital part of <strong>the</strong> longitud<strong>in</strong>aldiagnostic process, thus mak<strong>in</strong>g it more reliable. Pedagogicalassessment should be conducted repeatedly <strong>in</strong> order to verify <strong>the</strong> effectsof <strong>the</strong>rapeutic <strong>in</strong>tervention (Górniewicz, 1998; Oszwa, 2000). F<strong>in</strong>ally, <strong>the</strong>appropriate formal (m<strong>in</strong>isterial) regulations perta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividualisationof <strong>the</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g process, types of accommodations of scholasticrequirements to <strong>the</strong> needs and abilities of children with dyslexia as wellas provision of special help ought to be cited <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> assessment report aswell (Bogdanowicz, 1999; Tomaszewska, 2001). Read<strong>in</strong>g impairedchildren, deprived of access to professional <strong>the</strong>rapeutic activities, rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong> danger of <strong>in</strong>tensify<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> already persistent difficulties <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g andspell<strong>in</strong>g, which <strong>in</strong> turn often negatively <strong>in</strong>fluences <strong>the</strong>ir overall scholasticcareer (lower marks, repeat<strong>in</strong>g classes, resignation from fur<strong>the</strong>r education)and personality development (low self-esteem, group-dependence,emotional disorders) (Bogdanowicz & Wszeborowska-Lipińska, 1992;Jaklewicz, 1997).Assessment procedures <strong>in</strong>volve both cl<strong>in</strong>ical and experimentalmethods. Cl<strong>in</strong>ical methods such as <strong>in</strong>terview, observation, analysis ofsamples of writ<strong>in</strong>g, draw<strong>in</strong>gs, medical and educational documentationare useful <strong>in</strong> collect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation with reference to developmentalhistory, learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties and <strong>the</strong> ways <strong>the</strong>y have been dealt with,home and school environment, personality features and behaviourpatterns. Diagnos<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> family and school environment yields an<strong>in</strong>valuable <strong>in</strong>sight <strong>in</strong>to a multitude of factors <strong>in</strong>fluenc<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> child’sdevelopment and contemporary situation at school, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g a recordof up-to-date successes and failures concern<strong>in</strong>g all school subjects.Consultation with <strong>the</strong> teacher <strong>the</strong>rapist, native and foreign languageteachers as well as physical education teacher constitutes a source of


Symptoms and Identification of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 103particularly useful data (Bogdanowicz, 1997a/2000, 1999, 2002b, 2003b;Bogdanowicz & Krasowicz-Kupis, 2005b; Górniewicz, 1998; Mickiewicz,1997; Szepietowska, 2002).The <strong>in</strong>troductory stage of <strong>the</strong> assessment process described above isfollowed by <strong>the</strong> evaluation of read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g ability, perta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g toaccuracy, rate, strategy of read<strong>in</strong>g and level of comprehension, all ofwhich constitute <strong>the</strong> essential part of <strong>the</strong> diagnostic procedure. It covers:verification of <strong>the</strong> letter knowledge with regard to <strong>the</strong>ir shape, name andsound, s<strong>in</strong>gle word decod<strong>in</strong>g, pseudo-word read<strong>in</strong>g, read<strong>in</strong>g text aloud,silent read<strong>in</strong>g with comprehension, dictation, writ<strong>in</strong>g an essay andsometimes rewrit<strong>in</strong>g and writ<strong>in</strong>g from memory (Górniewicz, 1998;Jędrzejowska & Jurek, 2003; Krasowicz-Kupis, 2008; Turner, 2003). Infact, def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g dyslexia with reference to decod<strong>in</strong>g and encod<strong>in</strong>g difficultiesnaturally <strong>in</strong>vites certa<strong>in</strong> diagnostic procedures. S<strong>in</strong>ce pseudo-wordread<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g tests constitute <strong>the</strong> most direct measures of decod<strong>in</strong>gand encod<strong>in</strong>g skills, <strong>the</strong>n, logically, <strong>the</strong>y should become an <strong>in</strong>tegralelement of <strong>the</strong> assessment battery (Szczerbiński, 2007).Ano<strong>the</strong>r crucial component part of such a battery would be <strong>the</strong>evaluation of <strong>the</strong> cognitive functions. The description of <strong>the</strong> phonologicaldeficit usually comprises three ma<strong>in</strong> elements, namely, phonologicalawareness def<strong>in</strong>ed as conscious ability to identify and manipulate speechsounds, slow lexical retrieval, best demonstrated <strong>in</strong> rapid serial nam<strong>in</strong>gtasks, and, f<strong>in</strong>ally, poor short-term verbal memory, evident <strong>in</strong> non-wordrepetition and digit span tasks (Ramus, 2004; Turner, 2003). Krasowicz-Kupis (2008) enumerates <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g areas of cognitive developmentthat require decent evaluation: speech and language functions, ma<strong>in</strong>lywith regard to phonological process<strong>in</strong>g but also morphological, syntacticand semantic (pragmatic) process<strong>in</strong>g, language awareness, aga<strong>in</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>lywith reference to <strong>the</strong> phonological level (e.g. phonemic analysis andsyn<strong>the</strong>sis), rapid automatised nam<strong>in</strong>g, phonological memory, and lastbut not least, <strong>in</strong>tellectual ability (IQ).Admittedly, some doubts have been cast on <strong>the</strong> role of <strong>in</strong>telligencetests <strong>in</strong> diagnos<strong>in</strong>g dyslexia (Stanovich, 1996), <strong>the</strong> issue is subject to<strong>in</strong>tense debate, <strong>in</strong>deed generat<strong>in</strong>g substantial controversy. Traditionally,a discrepancy between <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g capacity of children and that expectedof <strong>the</strong>ir IQ and age is used to dist<strong>in</strong>guish dyslexics from o<strong>the</strong>r poorreaders (L<strong>in</strong>dsay, 2001; Reid, 1998; Schneider, 1999). The probable scoreon <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g test can be estimated <strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>e with <strong>the</strong> score obta<strong>in</strong>ed on <strong>the</strong><strong>in</strong>telligence test. Considerable disparities between <strong>the</strong> expected andactual read<strong>in</strong>g scores are claimed to <strong>in</strong>dicate <strong>the</strong> presence of specificdifficulties <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g (Elliot & Place, 2000). However, Miles(1996) believes that <strong>the</strong> concept of <strong>the</strong> global IQ may be mislead<strong>in</strong>gbecause some of <strong>the</strong> items <strong>in</strong> IQ tests are, <strong>in</strong> fact, <strong>in</strong>appropriate forchildren with dyslexia <strong>in</strong> that <strong>the</strong>y cannot possibly reveal <strong>the</strong> real


104 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>capabilities of <strong>the</strong>se <strong>in</strong>dividuals. There is no deny<strong>in</strong>g that children withdyslexia repeatedly achieve significantly reduced scores <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>gsubtests: Information, Arithmetic, Digit Span and Cod<strong>in</strong>g form<strong>in</strong>g aprofile called ACID, hence a worry that <strong>the</strong> evaluation of <strong>the</strong> general IQcan be imprecise (Bogdanowicz, 2003b; Krasowicz-Kupis, 2008; L<strong>in</strong>dsay,2001; Reid, 1998; Wszeborowska-Lipińska, 1996).Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, Reid (1998) highlights <strong>the</strong> fact that <strong>the</strong> unexpecteddiscrepancy between low literacy achievement and high IQ <strong>in</strong> dyslexicsmay not necessarily be predicted by IQ but by some o<strong>the</strong>r cognitivemeasure, e.g. listen<strong>in</strong>g comprehension. Szczerbiński (2007) adds that asmuch as <strong>in</strong>tellectual ability highly correlates with read<strong>in</strong>g comprehension,its relation with decod<strong>in</strong>g is poor, <strong>in</strong> fact, decod<strong>in</strong>g ability can bebetter predicted by phonological process<strong>in</strong>g ability. Moreover, <strong>the</strong> causalrelationship between IQ and read<strong>in</strong>g faculty appears doubtful <strong>in</strong> light ofevidence from hyperlexia studies, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that children with poorcomprehension and low general IQ can demonstrate good decod<strong>in</strong>gskills, which means <strong>the</strong>y can be good mechanical readers (Siegal, 1989).Yet ano<strong>the</strong>r argument is that dyslexia can <strong>the</strong>oretically exist <strong>in</strong> allchildren, <strong>in</strong>dependently of <strong>in</strong>telligence. What follows from <strong>the</strong> above isthat it is possible to diagnose dyslexia across <strong>the</strong> levels of <strong>in</strong>tellectualdevelopment, provided children’s read<strong>in</strong>g achievement is below atypical read<strong>in</strong>g score for <strong>the</strong>ir age and <strong>in</strong>telligence level. However, it isnot recommended with respect to mentally retarded children, as it wouldchange noth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir educational situation. It can be expected though,with good reason, that <strong>the</strong>y may manifest decod<strong>in</strong>g difficulties ofdyslexic type and benefit from pedagogical <strong>in</strong>terventions designed forchildren with dyslexia. All <strong>in</strong> all, Szczerbiński (2007) concludes thatdecod<strong>in</strong>g and encod<strong>in</strong>g difficulties are not directly caused by low<strong>in</strong>telligence because, simply enough, pr<strong>in</strong>t process<strong>in</strong>g is not dependenton <strong>in</strong>telligence, hence <strong>the</strong> secondary role that IQ measurement holds <strong>in</strong>dyslexia assessment. Never<strong>the</strong>less, despite <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tense debate on <strong>the</strong>usefulness of <strong>the</strong> discrepancy between <strong>the</strong> IQ level and <strong>the</strong> ability toread, it cont<strong>in</strong>ually tends to be treated as a dyslexia <strong>in</strong>dicator <strong>in</strong> everydaycl<strong>in</strong>ical practice (Bogdanowicz, 2003b; Bogdanowicz & Krasowicz-Kupis,2005b; Elliot & Place, 2000). Moreover, with reference to research practice,<strong>the</strong> use of IQ-discrepancy-def<strong>in</strong>ed groups proves to be a commonapproach <strong>in</strong> studies on dyslexia. This cautious selection of poor readersof average IQ is conducted <strong>in</strong> hope of exclud<strong>in</strong>g more general learn<strong>in</strong>gdifficulties and <strong>in</strong> that way creat<strong>in</strong>g better chances for depict<strong>in</strong>g cognitivedisorders that read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties can be traced back to (Hulme &Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2009).Mov<strong>in</strong>g on from <strong>the</strong> role of IQ to o<strong>the</strong>r components of <strong>the</strong> assessmentbattery <strong>in</strong> dyslexia diagnosis, let us enumerate <strong>the</strong> evaluation ofperceptual-motor functions (e.g. visual perception and memory) and <strong>the</strong>ir


Symptoms and Identification of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 105<strong>in</strong>tegration (e.g. visual-motor or auditory-visual-motor coord<strong>in</strong>ation) aswell as motor function (gross and fane motor skills) and lateralisation,which are rout<strong>in</strong>ely perceived valid <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Polish context for <strong>in</strong>stance(Krasowicz-Kupis, 2008). However, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> face of non-existent unanimousagreement with regard to <strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>g causes and pathomechanismsof dyslexia, its diagnosis, unsurpris<strong>in</strong>gly, proves a complex anddifficult process. For <strong>the</strong> same reason, <strong>the</strong>re are differences with referenceto <strong>the</strong> choice of tests and functions to be measured. Szczerbiński (2007)suggests that s<strong>in</strong>ce it can be concluded from research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs thatdyslexia does not follow from visual memory deficit or is not significantlyrelated to <strong>the</strong> lateralisation formula, it seems reasonable to give upon <strong>the</strong>ir verification to concentrate on more tell<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dicators of <strong>the</strong>disability. At <strong>the</strong> same time though, he admits that among all familiarpivotal dyslexia <strong>in</strong>dicators, none is, <strong>in</strong> fact, totally specific to this disorder(<strong>the</strong>y occur <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r contexts as well), thus <strong>the</strong> assessment towardsdyslexia is to a great extent probabilistic <strong>in</strong> nature. The presence ofparticular features <strong>in</strong>creases <strong>the</strong> chances that we are deal<strong>in</strong>g withdyslexia, however some doubts and uncerta<strong>in</strong>ty may rema<strong>in</strong>. Importantly,a high degree of severity and persistency of occurrence of <strong>the</strong>secharacteristics magnifies <strong>the</strong> likelihood that <strong>the</strong> problem we cope with is<strong>in</strong>deed dyslexia.In fact, <strong>the</strong> level of severity and persistency can be verified by <strong>the</strong>responsiveness to <strong>in</strong>tervention, which most desirably should become acritical element of <strong>the</strong> assessment procedure. It would translate <strong>in</strong>topreced<strong>in</strong>g f<strong>in</strong>al diagnostic decision about <strong>the</strong> nature of read<strong>in</strong>g andspell<strong>in</strong>g difficulties by <strong>in</strong>tensive and <strong>in</strong>dividualised treatment, aimed atenhanc<strong>in</strong>g decod<strong>in</strong>g and encod<strong>in</strong>g abilities. Only non-respond<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>dividuals, who do not benefit from such a treatment and still strugglewith read<strong>in</strong>g, could be diagnosed dyslexic. Thus, <strong>in</strong>effectiveness ofspecial remedial teach<strong>in</strong>g (which never<strong>the</strong>less proved useful for o<strong>the</strong>rpoor readers) serves as an <strong>in</strong>dicator of dyslexia. It needs stress<strong>in</strong>g herethat low-respond<strong>in</strong>g children are not impossible to teach, <strong>the</strong>y simplyrequire much more <strong>in</strong>tensified and longer treatment (Szczerbiński, 2007;Vellut<strong>in</strong>o et al., 1998).Helland et al. (2008) verify <strong>the</strong> relation between school performance,operationalised as responsiveness to remedial activities and support, anda dichotic listen<strong>in</strong>g (DL) task with consonant-vowel (CV) syllablesrealisation <strong>in</strong> dyslexic 12-year-olds. Typically, children with dyslexiademonstrate reduced REA 9 on <strong>the</strong> DL task, which is a frequently usedmeasure tapp<strong>in</strong>g phonological process<strong>in</strong>g. Given that <strong>the</strong> phonologicalprocess<strong>in</strong>g deficit is <strong>the</strong> core deficit <strong>in</strong> dyslexia, <strong>the</strong>n reduced REA andlack of <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere dom<strong>in</strong>ance would be related to dyslexicread<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g difficulties. Children with dyslexia who do notrespond well to special education show significantly lower REA <strong>in</strong>


106 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>comparison to non-dyslexic age-matched children as well as childrenwith dyslexia who positively react to remedial teach<strong>in</strong>g. DL scores highlycorrelate with scores on language test, and Helland et al. (2008) speculatethat children achiev<strong>in</strong>g average REA on <strong>the</strong> DL test should be capable ofproperly dist<strong>in</strong>guish<strong>in</strong>g between similar sounds, unlike those withreduced DL scores. The potential validity and usefulness of <strong>the</strong> DLtask as a tool that can be successfully applied <strong>in</strong> cl<strong>in</strong>ical practice, withregard to differential diagnosis, is <strong>in</strong>dicated. It is fur<strong>the</strong>r implied thattra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> differentiat<strong>in</strong>g between and sequenc<strong>in</strong>g speech sounds canimprove dyslexic l<strong>in</strong>guistic skills and exert a normalis<strong>in</strong>g effect on <strong>the</strong>activation pattern <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> left planum temporal, as evidenced by fMRIbra<strong>in</strong> imag<strong>in</strong>g.F<strong>in</strong>ally, <strong>in</strong> order to get an overall picture of an <strong>in</strong>dividual withdyslexia, it is worth devot<strong>in</strong>g some thought to <strong>the</strong>ir emotional function<strong>in</strong>g.It is proposed that emotional state and well-be<strong>in</strong>g, self-esteem, selfcontrolas well as attitude and motivation become a part of <strong>the</strong>assessment of <strong>in</strong>dividual needs of children with dyslexia (Carroll &Iles, 2006; Krasowicz-Kupis, 2008; Turner, 2003).Identification of <strong>Dyslexia</strong>: Bil<strong>in</strong>gual/Multil<strong>in</strong>gual PerspectiveOn <strong>the</strong> one hand, <strong>the</strong>re is a grow<strong>in</strong>g literature touch<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong>identification and remediation of dyslexia <strong>in</strong> a monol<strong>in</strong>gual population;on <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, very little research has been <strong>in</strong>itiated with respect to<strong>in</strong>dividuals from bil<strong>in</strong>gual and multil<strong>in</strong>gual backgrounds (Deponio et al.,2000; Everatt et al., 2000a, 2000b; Ganschow et al., 2000; Stamboltzis &Pumfrey, 2000). There is an <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g and widespread sensitivity andconcern over <strong>the</strong> issues of literacy acquisition and assessment of read<strong>in</strong>gdisabilities among school children <strong>in</strong> multicultural and multil<strong>in</strong>gualsett<strong>in</strong>gs, <strong>in</strong> particular, those who learn English as a second or subsequentlanguage. Inasmuch as <strong>the</strong> assessment under such conditions is <strong>in</strong>deed agreat challenge, achiev<strong>in</strong>g it early enough may equal enhanced effects <strong>in</strong>terms of treatment and later read<strong>in</strong>g success. Conversely, delay<strong>in</strong>gdiagnosis and <strong>in</strong>tervention leads to under-identification and, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>long run, to disadvantage and cumulative educational failure, not tomention <strong>the</strong> fact that it proves cost <strong>in</strong>effective. As Geva (2000) rightlystresses, <strong>in</strong> attempt<strong>in</strong>g diagnosis towards dyslexia <strong>in</strong> a multil<strong>in</strong>gualenvironment, first of all <strong>the</strong>re is a need to differentiate between <strong>the</strong>phenomena that could be characterised as common, regular problemsrelated to second language (L2) acquisition and true warn<strong>in</strong>g signs ofread<strong>in</strong>g fiasco. In addition, it is vital to understand whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>abovementioned signs prevail <strong>in</strong> L1 and L2 alike.


Symptoms and Identification of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 107Children from ethnic and l<strong>in</strong>guistic m<strong>in</strong>orities, raised <strong>in</strong> a bil<strong>in</strong>gualenvironment (liv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a target/L2 speak<strong>in</strong>g country and us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>irnative language (L1) at home), frequently possess considerably weak L2language skills before <strong>the</strong> commencement of formal school<strong>in</strong>g. Manyeducators apparently share a view that <strong>the</strong>se children necessarily need toacquire a decent spoken command of L2 <strong>in</strong> order to learn to read it. Suchan attitude also extends to promot<strong>in</strong>g a view that wait<strong>in</strong>g for <strong>the</strong> L2 oralproficiency to fully develop is required to ensure <strong>the</strong> reliable and valididentification of learn<strong>in</strong>g/read<strong>in</strong>g problems. In research on at-risklearners of English as a second language (ESL), Geva (2000) confirmsthat elementary classroom teachers are ra<strong>the</strong>r reluctant and hesitant <strong>in</strong>qualify<strong>in</strong>g ESL children as potentially at-risk for read<strong>in</strong>g failure due to<strong>the</strong> prevail<strong>in</strong>g belief that read<strong>in</strong>g disabilities cannot be identified until<strong>the</strong>se children reach a certa<strong>in</strong> threshold of L2 oral proficiency. However,accord<strong>in</strong>g to Geva (2000) <strong>the</strong>re is no need to wait because, notwithstand<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong> crucial effect of L2 oral proficiency on read<strong>in</strong>g comprehension, itseems to play a m<strong>in</strong>or role <strong>in</strong> expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g poor word-based read<strong>in</strong>g skills(word and pseudo-word decod<strong>in</strong>g abilities) <strong>in</strong> L2, which prevail despiteadequate <strong>in</strong>struction. Hence <strong>the</strong> need for early assessment of worddecod<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong> L2, even if children still lack adequate L2 facility. As amatter of fact, it may be <strong>the</strong> case that children decode words much moreaccurately <strong>in</strong> L2 than <strong>in</strong> L1, which would depend on how letters maponto sounds (orthographic depth) <strong>in</strong> a given language. On <strong>the</strong> one hand,substantial differences may not be spotted between <strong>the</strong> accuracy and rateof word decod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> L1 and L2 <strong>in</strong> spite of vary<strong>in</strong>g levels of proficiency.Under such circumstances, read<strong>in</strong>g comprehension seems greater <strong>in</strong> L1than <strong>in</strong> L2 due to <strong>the</strong> facilitat<strong>in</strong>g effect of children’s command of L1. On<strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, along with not fully developed L2 oral proficiency, alsopoor decod<strong>in</strong>g skills are capable of <strong>in</strong>hibit<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g comprehension.Ano<strong>the</strong>r important issue is that poor word recognition abilities can betraced back to low-grade phonological process<strong>in</strong>g, which holds trueacross languages. Skilful phoneme manipulation enhances <strong>the</strong> developmentof good decod<strong>in</strong>g skill, which, <strong>in</strong> turn, translates <strong>in</strong>to high-levelread<strong>in</strong>g from <strong>the</strong> first grades. By contrast, below-standard phonememanipulation ability has a debilitat<strong>in</strong>g effect on word decod<strong>in</strong>g skill,possibly lead<strong>in</strong>g to read<strong>in</strong>g failure. Importantly, several cross-l<strong>in</strong>guisticstudies consistently report <strong>the</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistic <strong>in</strong>terdependence of phonologicalprocess<strong>in</strong>g skills (and related processes, such as nam<strong>in</strong>g speed andphonological memory), <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r words, <strong>the</strong>se skills <strong>in</strong> one language arecapable of predict<strong>in</strong>g word recognition ability with<strong>in</strong> and acrosslanguages, irrespective of exist<strong>in</strong>g differences between alphabetic systemsof phoneme-to-grapheme conversion. All <strong>in</strong> all, most important fordiagnos<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g disabilities <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> multil<strong>in</strong>gual context is <strong>the</strong> fact thatdifferences <strong>in</strong> phonological process<strong>in</strong>g abilities are critically related to


108 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>word recognition skills across languages, which means that phonologicalprocess<strong>in</strong>g abilities <strong>in</strong> one language, no matter whe<strong>the</strong>r L1 or L2, canpredict <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> word decod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> ano<strong>the</strong>r language.Diagnos<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>in</strong> a cross-l<strong>in</strong>guistic context provesplausible even <strong>in</strong> view of not yet fully developed L2 proficiency (Geva,2000).In sum, Geva (2000) suggests, first of all, that heavy reliance on L2 oralproficiency <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> identification of ESL learners who can be at-risk forread<strong>in</strong>g disability is unreasonable, second, that two sets of complementaryprocedures (useful <strong>in</strong> L1) can be successfully applied <strong>in</strong> assess<strong>in</strong>gtowards dyslexia <strong>in</strong> bil<strong>in</strong>gual or ESL <strong>in</strong>dividuals. The procedures <strong>in</strong>volvephonological process<strong>in</strong>g abilities and rapid basic read<strong>in</strong>g skills assessmentas well as look<strong>in</strong>g for <strong>the</strong> gap between listen<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g skills.A small gap is expected <strong>in</strong> average-achiev<strong>in</strong>g children, by contrast, <strong>the</strong>discrepancy between much greater scores on listen<strong>in</strong>g comprehensiontasks and poor read<strong>in</strong>g comprehension is a vital <strong>in</strong>dicator of a childexperienc<strong>in</strong>g pr<strong>in</strong>t process<strong>in</strong>g difficulties ra<strong>the</strong>r than problems withprocess<strong>in</strong>g and comprehend<strong>in</strong>g verbal <strong>in</strong>formation. To stress aga<strong>in</strong>,postpon<strong>in</strong>g diagnostic activities <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> case of ESL children whoencounter substantial difficulties <strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g word recognition facultycan have far-reach<strong>in</strong>g damag<strong>in</strong>g consequences for <strong>the</strong>se children, as <strong>the</strong>yare simultaneously deprived of <strong>the</strong> chance for <strong>in</strong>tensive <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong>word recognition/attack skills.Nowadays, <strong>the</strong> ethnographic reality poses new requirements oneducators and cl<strong>in</strong>icians <strong>in</strong> terms of dyslexia screen<strong>in</strong>g procedures andtools as well as <strong>in</strong>dividual education plans <strong>in</strong> order to cater for <strong>the</strong> needsof children from ethnic and language m<strong>in</strong>orities, e.g. English as anadditional language (EAL) learners. The estimates are that ESL childrenconstitute almost 10% of <strong>the</strong> entire public school enrolment <strong>in</strong> 20002001<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> USA, with <strong>the</strong> numbers expected to grow to 40% by <strong>the</strong> year 2030.Still, <strong>the</strong> vast majority of <strong>the</strong> non-English-speak<strong>in</strong>g population (about80%) is ma<strong>in</strong>ly Spanish (Wagner et al., 2005), unlike <strong>in</strong> Canada, where <strong>the</strong>student population is much more diverse <strong>in</strong> terms of native languages ofESL children and more numerous (approximately 18% as of 2001) (Geva,2000; Lovett et al., 2008).In fact, it seems that early screen<strong>in</strong>g procedures designed primarilywith monol<strong>in</strong>gual children <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d can benefit bil<strong>in</strong>guals, as demonstrated<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> case of learners of EAL. Everatt et al. (2000a) ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> that,<strong>in</strong> particular, phonological measures seem to be useful <strong>in</strong> identify<strong>in</strong>gstudents with literacy problems, regardless of language background.Everatt et al. (2000b) found that young English/Sylheti bil<strong>in</strong>gual childrendemonstrat<strong>in</strong>g below-standard spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g skills can beaccurately dist<strong>in</strong>guished from <strong>the</strong> control group by <strong>the</strong> scores <strong>the</strong>yachieve on several tests, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> measures of phonological


Symptoms and Identification of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 109process<strong>in</strong>g ability and rapid nam<strong>in</strong>g. Apart from <strong>the</strong>se two, <strong>the</strong> ability torecite high frequency sequences and repeat novel sequences of unknownor non-l<strong>in</strong>guistic <strong>in</strong>formation, as well as <strong>the</strong> ability to recognisepreviously seen shapes also proved vital. Everatt et al. (2000b) stressthat phonological measures <strong>in</strong>deed provide consistent results withreference to bil<strong>in</strong>gual and monol<strong>in</strong>gual learners alike.Hutch<strong>in</strong>son et al. (2004) support <strong>the</strong> view that early identification andassessment towards dyslexia of children with EAL translates <strong>in</strong>to moredesirable effects <strong>in</strong> terms of <strong>the</strong>rapy and treatment. They, never<strong>the</strong>less,admit that <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> cases of children with EAL such a diagnosis is muchmore difficult than <strong>in</strong> monol<strong>in</strong>gual children due to frequently poor L2proficiency, <strong>the</strong> lack of L1 assessment and poor teachers’ awareness of<strong>the</strong> effect dyslexia has on bil<strong>in</strong>gual language process<strong>in</strong>g, which altoge<strong>the</strong>roften leads to under-identification. In <strong>the</strong> UK, almost 10% ofchildren attend<strong>in</strong>g primary schools speak EAL, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g British Asianchildren <strong>in</strong>vestigated by Hutch<strong>in</strong>son et al. (2004). They report <strong>the</strong>phonological assessment battery (PhAB) of service <strong>in</strong> identify<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>dividuals with poor phonological skills, responsible for read<strong>in</strong>gaccuracy difficulties <strong>in</strong> both EAL and monol<strong>in</strong>gual children, with <strong>the</strong>EAL learners ready for PhAB <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> second year at school and above, oncondition that <strong>the</strong>ir language skills allow to understand test <strong>in</strong>structions.All <strong>in</strong> all, as <strong>in</strong>dicated by Lovett et al. (2008), it seems that, pr<strong>in</strong>cipally, <strong>the</strong>components of <strong>the</strong> assessment of read<strong>in</strong>g disorders <strong>in</strong> ESL childrenwould be similar to those used <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> diagnosis of read<strong>in</strong>g deficits <strong>in</strong> L1.Wagner et al. (2005), as mentioned above, report that approximatelyevery fifth child <strong>in</strong> US public schools speaks an L1 o<strong>the</strong>r than English, <strong>in</strong> arough estimate about 80% are Spanish-L1 speakers, however some urbandistricts document around 100 languages spoken among students.Understandably, such a situation forces educators and policy makers torespond to <strong>the</strong> educational needs of such learners, also with respect to <strong>the</strong>appropriate identification procedures. Given <strong>the</strong> facts cited so far about<strong>the</strong> necessity to dist<strong>in</strong>guish between problems regularly occurr<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>FLL context and signs of forthcom<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g failure as well as <strong>the</strong> factsabout assessment procedures orig<strong>in</strong>ally designed for monol<strong>in</strong>gual speakersbut applicable also <strong>in</strong> multil<strong>in</strong>gual context, whe<strong>the</strong>r such childrenshould undergo <strong>the</strong> identification process <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir L1 or L2 seems torema<strong>in</strong> an open question. Generally, that would depend on <strong>the</strong> levels ofproficiency <strong>in</strong> both native language and English. On <strong>the</strong> one hand,assessment of ESL learners <strong>in</strong> English may prove troublesome <strong>in</strong> that <strong>the</strong>ymay simply f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong> task <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong>comprehensible, despite <strong>the</strong>potential for perform<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> task itself. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, whileassessment conducted <strong>in</strong> L1 may offer greater <strong>in</strong>sight <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong> range ofabilities and knowledge, it cannot possibly prognosticate read<strong>in</strong>gacquisition <strong>in</strong> English to <strong>the</strong> same extent as <strong>the</strong> assessment <strong>in</strong> English can.


110 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Wagner et al. (2005) rightly po<strong>in</strong>t to <strong>the</strong> fact that <strong>the</strong> identification oflearn<strong>in</strong>g disabilities <strong>in</strong> ESL learners needs to comprise <strong>the</strong> evaluation of<strong>the</strong> course of literacy <strong>in</strong>struction, which is useful for <strong>the</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>ationof both opportunity (access) to learn and <strong>the</strong> skills learned, especiallywhen children are given such <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir native language prior totra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> English. Thus, if children demonstrate good phonologicalskills developed <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir L1 and at <strong>the</strong> same time experience difficulty <strong>in</strong>master<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g ability <strong>in</strong> English, it may possibly be attributed to <strong>the</strong>quality and amount of <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> English ra<strong>the</strong>r than to <strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>gphonological deficit. If a student who gets literacy tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> his L1does not successfully develop read<strong>in</strong>g ability <strong>in</strong> L1, it can be suspected,with good reason, that <strong>the</strong> prevalent phonological deficit constitutes <strong>the</strong>underly<strong>in</strong>g cause. However, such factors as <strong>the</strong> vary<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>structional andcultural background of speakers shar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> L1 but hav<strong>in</strong>g differentorig<strong>in</strong>s, as is <strong>the</strong> case with Spanish speakers who are Puerto Rican,Mexican or Spanish, can play a role as well.All <strong>in</strong> all, Wagner et al. (2005) advocate develop<strong>in</strong>g a comparableassessments procedure, grounded on exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g at-risk ESL learners withregard to both native and English language. Ideally, <strong>the</strong>se comparableassessments should touch on <strong>the</strong> same doma<strong>in</strong>s and levels with similarprecision. In practice, availability of such compatible, valid and highstandardprocedures is extremely rare. Such an enterprise posesconsiderable demands for multiple reasons, for <strong>in</strong>stance, design<strong>in</strong>gcomparable assessments across languages vary<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> terms of orthography(both <strong>the</strong> type of <strong>the</strong> alphabet and orthographic depth) or alphabeticand non-alphabetic languages is <strong>in</strong>deed a challenge. Never<strong>the</strong>less,Wagner et al. (2005) report successful work on compil<strong>in</strong>g a comparableassessment procedure for Spanish, developed from an exist<strong>in</strong>g one <strong>in</strong>English and compris<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g doma<strong>in</strong>s: knowledge of letternames and sounds, phonological awareness, rapid nam<strong>in</strong>g, word read<strong>in</strong>gaccuracy and efficiency, and read<strong>in</strong>g comprehension.Faced with mount<strong>in</strong>g problems concern<strong>in</strong>g assessment procedures <strong>in</strong>EAL students, one might also turn to <strong>the</strong> model of assessment <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong> dynamism of response to <strong>in</strong>struction (Szczerbiński, 2007; Vellut<strong>in</strong>oet al., 1998; Wagner et al., 2005). The model assumes that <strong>the</strong> identificationof students at-risk for read<strong>in</strong>g failure, who require additional remedialteach<strong>in</strong>g, should be based on <strong>the</strong>ir reduced response to <strong>in</strong>struction. In <strong>the</strong>context of EAL students, limited reaction to ESL or EFL services isapplicable.Yet ano<strong>the</strong>r perspective on <strong>the</strong> assessment of learn<strong>in</strong>g difficultyfollows from <strong>the</strong> research on <strong>the</strong> native language-based FLL difficulties. 10Here we concentrate on <strong>in</strong>dividuals who f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong> study of L2/foreignlanguage, ma<strong>in</strong>ly executed <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> formal, classroom environment ra<strong>the</strong>rthan <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> naturalistic sett<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> target language-speak<strong>in</strong>g country, a


Symptoms and Identification of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 111considerably demand<strong>in</strong>g task. Identify<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> problems <strong>the</strong>y encounter,which can most possibly be traced back, as mentioned above, to <strong>the</strong>irpoor native language skills, is helpful <strong>in</strong> design<strong>in</strong>g effective remedialteach<strong>in</strong>g methods with regard to foreign language study.The f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g that students encounter<strong>in</strong>g ei<strong>the</strong>r subtle or overt difficulties<strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g, spell<strong>in</strong>g, listen<strong>in</strong>g and speak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir native languageare likely to experience similar difficulties <strong>in</strong> FLL is, naturally, of vitalimportance for <strong>the</strong> assessment of <strong>the</strong>ir potential performance <strong>in</strong> foreignlanguage courses. Generally, a comprehensive evaluation procedureshould best <strong>in</strong>volve several components, such as a review of adevelopmental history of <strong>the</strong> student and a scrupulous survey of his/her native as well as FLL accounts (Ganschow et al., 1998). Implicationsfor <strong>the</strong> assessment of difficulties <strong>in</strong> FLL drawn from <strong>the</strong> studiesconducted by Sparks et al. (Ganschow & Sparks, 1995; Sparks et al.,1995b, 1997c) explicitly <strong>in</strong>dicate both native language (English) andforeign language aptitude measures as a must. Adm<strong>in</strong>istration of <strong>the</strong>standardised measures of <strong>the</strong> native language skills (read<strong>in</strong>g, phonological/orthographicprocess<strong>in</strong>g, grammar, writ<strong>in</strong>g, vocabulary and orallanguage) and <strong>the</strong> foreign language aptitude test (MLAT) sheds light onFLL potential (Ganschow et al., 1998). Native language phonological/orthographic measures consist of tests of phonemic awareness, pseudowordread<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g, which is an additional <strong>in</strong>dicator of futureforeign language performance. It is known that <strong>the</strong> score on <strong>the</strong> longversion of <strong>the</strong> MLAT turned out to be a key predictor of <strong>the</strong> foreignlanguage performance, hence <strong>the</strong> proposition to use it as a screen<strong>in</strong>g toolfor diagnos<strong>in</strong>g students’ aptness for FLL. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>the</strong> subtestsof <strong>the</strong> MLAT measure different aspects of language learn<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>the</strong>y couldserve as diagnostic tools for detect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> possible problem areas <strong>in</strong> FLL.Additionally, attention should be paid to whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> language background,toge<strong>the</strong>r with <strong>the</strong> current native language difficulty, areadequately documented as well as whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>re exists a corroboratedrecord of a failure <strong>in</strong> or an excessive struggle with <strong>the</strong> foreign languagecourses. F<strong>in</strong>ally, it needs stress<strong>in</strong>g aga<strong>in</strong> that <strong>the</strong> data collected dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>psycho-educational assessment pave <strong>the</strong> way for <strong>the</strong> development ofalternative foreign language <strong>in</strong>struction, with adequate adjustmentsmade with reference to <strong>the</strong> FLL methods and strategies (Sparks et al.,1989). 11A different approach towards evaluat<strong>in</strong>g learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>in</strong> L2,though shar<strong>in</strong>g some elements with <strong>the</strong> one presented above, especiallywith regard to <strong>the</strong> assessment procedures <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g L1, is <strong>the</strong> oneproposed by Helland (2008). Helland gives an account of <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>ciplesthat served as guidel<strong>in</strong>es <strong>in</strong> design<strong>in</strong>g a test battery ‘English 2 <strong>Dyslexia</strong>Test’, used for test<strong>in</strong>g English as L2 <strong>in</strong> Norwegian learners with dyslexiaof Grade 6 and 7. Such a L2 test would necessarily <strong>in</strong>tegrate <strong>the</strong>


112 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>follow<strong>in</strong>g: differences between L1 and L2, with reference to languagetypology and features of <strong>the</strong> orthographic system, customary signs ofdyslexia <strong>in</strong> L1 and typical symptoms of <strong>the</strong> disorder <strong>in</strong> L2, also vitalelements of a language test and crucial components of a dyslexia test. Infact, Helland (2008) proposes that <strong>the</strong> above assessment tenets couldequally be applied to o<strong>the</strong>r languages learned as L2 besides English.Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, functional neuroimag<strong>in</strong>g techniques can also be potentiallyutilized dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> diagnosis of ESL/EAL learners <strong>in</strong> order toidentify certa<strong>in</strong> markers of read<strong>in</strong>g disability (Pugh et al., 2005).Admittedly, despite <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g recognition of dyslexia throughout<strong>the</strong> world, tests for identification of <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia exist <strong>in</strong>relatively few languages (Smy<strong>the</strong> & Everatt, 2000). Not only <strong>in</strong> deeporthographies, such as English, but also <strong>in</strong> highly transparent andlogographic languages <strong>the</strong>re are reports of <strong>in</strong>dividuals faced withproblems <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and spell. As ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed by Vellut<strong>in</strong>o et al.(2004), impaired fluency and speed <strong>in</strong> word identification and textprocess<strong>in</strong>g, which lead to read<strong>in</strong>g comprehension difficulties, are claimedto be <strong>the</strong> key markers for dyslexia <strong>in</strong> such languages. Thus, quitenaturally, diagnos<strong>in</strong>g dyslexia <strong>in</strong> different languages cannot be reducedto <strong>the</strong> assessment of phonological awareness, but requires <strong>the</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>ationof a broad scope of cognitive identifiers, for example, <strong>the</strong> speed ofprocess<strong>in</strong>g or visual recognition skills, depend<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong> orthographicfeatures of a given language, possibly conducive to diverse occurrenceand manifestations of dyslexia across scripts. It is suggested that forstudents with dyslexia learn<strong>in</strong>g English, <strong>the</strong> processes related tophonological awareness constitute <strong>the</strong> ma<strong>in</strong> obstacle. However, <strong>the</strong>likelihood of occurrence of o<strong>the</strong>r deficits <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>se learners is not denied,still, <strong>the</strong> nature of <strong>the</strong> script seems most problematic. Thus, deficits <strong>in</strong>visual process<strong>in</strong>g are more liable to be responsible for dyslexia <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>esedyslexics, while Hungarian dyslexics would probably experience deficits<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> speed of process<strong>in</strong>g (Smy<strong>the</strong> & Everatt, 2000).Summ<strong>in</strong>g up, beyond doubt, timely diagnosis provides opportunitiesfor immediate <strong>in</strong>tervention, designed to prevent a child from encounter<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>tensified, severe dyslexic difficulties. Early diagnosis, condition<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong> efficient help, paired with an opportunity to compensate for<strong>the</strong>ir developmental delays, should be offered to children at risk fordyslexia. The necessity of preventive treatment, <strong>the</strong> prophylaxis,draw<strong>in</strong>g on early diagnosis has been highlighted by practitioners(Benton, 1978; Bogdanowicz, 2002a; Elliot & Place, 2000; Górniewicz,1998; Jędrzejowska & Jurek, 2003; Johnson et al., 2001; Ott, 1997). Theearlier <strong>the</strong> pedagogical <strong>in</strong>tervention, <strong>the</strong> more efficient it is <strong>in</strong> terms oftime and effort. Moreover, it can possibly hold back high anxiety and lowself-esteem from develop<strong>in</strong>g (Everatt et al., 2000a). On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, if<strong>the</strong> dyslexic difficulties are not adequately recognised and dealt with


Symptoms and Identification of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 113<strong>in</strong>stantly, <strong>the</strong>y are forceful enough to exert considerable <strong>in</strong>fluence on <strong>the</strong>course of <strong>the</strong> scholastic career of <strong>the</strong> child. The assessment towardsdyslexia is a typically complex task <strong>in</strong> monol<strong>in</strong>guals, it gets even morecomplicated <strong>in</strong> bil<strong>in</strong>gual and multil<strong>in</strong>gual environment, however, <strong>the</strong>re isno doubt that it is worth every effort.Notes1. See Chapter 2: ‘Phonological cod<strong>in</strong>g deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis’, on <strong>the</strong> phonologicaldeficit <strong>in</strong> dyslexia.2. See Chapter 2: ‘Cerebellar deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis’, on <strong>the</strong> cerebellar deficit <strong>in</strong>dyslexia.3. Several early screen<strong>in</strong>g tests are available, for example <strong>the</strong> phonologicalassessment battery (PhAB) (Frederickson et al., 1997), <strong>the</strong> dyslexia earlyscreen<strong>in</strong>g test (DEST) devised by Nicolson and Fawcett (1996) and <strong>the</strong> scale of<strong>the</strong> risk for dyslexia (SRD) developed by Bogdanowicz (2002a). DEST can beadm<strong>in</strong>istered by teachers or school health professionals who could test allpupils on entry to school. It is suitable for children aged from 4.6 to 6.5 yearsand provides <strong>in</strong>formation concern<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> areas of strengths and weaknessesof <strong>the</strong> children. It is <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> pencil and paper form and requires 30 m<strong>in</strong>utes perpupil to adm<strong>in</strong>ister. SRD can be applied to pupils aged 67. Its aim is toreveal delays <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> psychomotor development and denote <strong>the</strong> functions thatcan be disordered. The adm<strong>in</strong>istration of <strong>the</strong> SRD should most suitably beaccompanied by an early assessment of read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g skills. Theadvantage of <strong>the</strong> SRD is its uncomplicated form <strong>the</strong> questionnaire canbe relatively quickly filled and compiled by teachers or parents.4. See Chapter 1: ‘Read<strong>in</strong>g Strategies and Stages of Read<strong>in</strong>g Development’, onread<strong>in</strong>g strategies and Chapter 1: ‘Orthographic Depth and Gra<strong>in</strong> Size:A Cross-language Perspective on Read<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>Dyslexia</strong>’, on <strong>the</strong> orthographicdepth.5. Dysphonetic dyslexics (63%) suffer from deficits <strong>in</strong> phonological decod<strong>in</strong>g,given rise to by disorders of auditory-l<strong>in</strong>guistic process<strong>in</strong>g. This type ischaracterised by problems <strong>in</strong> differentiat<strong>in</strong>g and identify<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividualspeech sounds, poor phonological word analysis, and difficulties <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>tegrat<strong>in</strong>gwritten words with <strong>the</strong>ir sounds. In o<strong>the</strong>r words, dysphonetic dyslexicsface problems apply<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> grapheme-phoneme rules and, for that reason,<strong>the</strong>y are forced to rely on <strong>the</strong>ir sight-vocabulary when recognis<strong>in</strong>g words.Thus, dysphonetic writ<strong>in</strong>g exploits <strong>the</strong> visual memory of words. Typicalmisspell<strong>in</strong>gs are phonetically <strong>in</strong>accurate (e.g. ‘catteg’ for ‘cottage’, ‘coetere’for ‘character’), while <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g, words are often guessed or replaced(semantic substitution, e.g. ‘laugh’ for ‘funny’, ‘bus’ for ‘car’). By contrast, <strong>in</strong>dyseidetic dyslexia (9%) visual perception and memory as well as visualspatialprocess<strong>in</strong>g is deficient. Dyseidetic dyslexics f<strong>in</strong>d it particularlydifficult to make use of visual whole-word <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g;additionally, <strong>the</strong>ir performance on irregular words is also significantly belowstandard. Confusion of letter shapes and phonetic writ<strong>in</strong>g are characteristicfeatures of this type of dyslexia. Thus, visual-spatial reversals, static anddynamic <strong>in</strong>version are usually observed (‘dab’ for ‘bad’, ‘was’ for ‘saw’).Typical misspell<strong>in</strong>gs, as suggested above, are phonetically accurate and canbe easily decoded (e.g. ‘laf’ for ‘laugh’, ‘berd’ for ‘bird’, ‘wok’ for ‘walk’),while read<strong>in</strong>g is based on phonological analysis and syn<strong>the</strong>sis. However,dyseidetic dyslexics appear to be relatively better than dysphonetic dyslexics


114 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong><strong>in</strong> apply<strong>in</strong>g grapheme-phoneme conversion rules. F<strong>in</strong>ally, <strong>the</strong> third, mixedtype (28%) presumes co-occurrence of dysphonetic and dyseidetic difficulties.As can be noticed from <strong>the</strong> percentages cited above, dysphoneticdyslexics constitute <strong>the</strong> majority of cases, which is <strong>in</strong> accord with acommonly accepted claim that phonological process<strong>in</strong>g deficit is a coredeficit <strong>in</strong> dyslexia.6. Disorders of visual or visual-spatial process<strong>in</strong>g, caused by impairments ofvisual attention, perception and memory, markedly connected with handmotor disorder as well as visual-motor coord<strong>in</strong>ation deficits, generate visualdyslexia. By way of analogy, auditory dyslexia is provoked by disorders oflanguage process<strong>in</strong>g brought about by deficits <strong>in</strong> auditory attention,perception and memory, toge<strong>the</strong>r with poor representation of speech sounds.Mixed type entails cumulative characteristics, <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g co-occurrence ofboth visual and auditory deficits. F<strong>in</strong>ally, disorders of <strong>in</strong>tegration ofperceptual-motor and language functions produce <strong>in</strong>tegrative dyslexia. Inthis case, no disturbances <strong>in</strong> simple perceptual-motor functions measured <strong>in</strong>isolation are signified, but <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tegration and co-ord<strong>in</strong>ation of stimulicom<strong>in</strong>g from different receptors on <strong>the</strong> cortical level is disordered.7. See Chapter 1: ‘Def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g dyslexia’, on <strong>the</strong> def<strong>in</strong>ition of dyslexia.8. See Chapter 1: ‘Orthographic Depth and Gra<strong>in</strong> Size: A Cross-languagePerspective on Read<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>Dyslexia</strong>’, on <strong>the</strong> cross-language perspective onread<strong>in</strong>g and dyslexia.9. Right ear advantage (REA) <strong>in</strong>dicates left hemisphere superiority forlanguage, with <strong>the</strong> activation of <strong>the</strong> left planum temporale.10. See Chapter 3 for a discussion of <strong>the</strong> native language-based FLL difficulties.11. The f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of multiple studies conducted on successful <strong>in</strong>structionalmethods to at-risk foreign language learners are referred to <strong>in</strong> Chapter 5.


Chapter 5Treatment and Teach<strong>in</strong>gIssues <strong>in</strong> <strong>Dyslexia</strong> TreatmentThe issue of support<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir attempts toovercome read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g difficulties, experienced both <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>irnative language as well as <strong>in</strong> a second or additional language, which <strong>the</strong>yfrequently struggle to learn, <strong>in</strong>vites three (at least) diverse perspectives<strong>in</strong>to play, namely, that of a researcher, teacher-<strong>the</strong>rapist and languageteacher. Serv<strong>in</strong>g one overall goal search<strong>in</strong>g for <strong>the</strong> best practices <strong>in</strong> orderto enhance <strong>the</strong> chances of successful literacy acquisition for children withdyslexia each is predom<strong>in</strong>antly preoccupied with research-based<strong>in</strong>terventions and treatments, special education services (pedagogical<strong>the</strong>rapy) and teach<strong>in</strong>g approaches and techniques, respectively.Theoretically, it seems that teachers’ choices can be well <strong>in</strong>formed byresearch f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, provid<strong>in</strong>g evidence that ei<strong>the</strong>r supports or fails tosupport <strong>the</strong> effectiveness of specific educational approaches and <strong>in</strong>structionalpractices. However, an apparent lack of enthusiasm and relativereluctance of teachers with regard to implement<strong>in</strong>g research-validatededucational activities can be observed (Philips et al., 2008; Ritchey &Goeke, 2006), thus form<strong>in</strong>g a gap between research and practice.Similarly, Hurry et al. (2005) highlight a particular difficulty and, at <strong>the</strong>same time, salience of transform<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> educational research <strong>in</strong>to teacherpractice, thus mak<strong>in</strong>g research-based and verified techniques availableto teachers.This complex process of transformation consists of several levels andsteps, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g a national policy and curriculum design to f<strong>in</strong>ally end at<strong>the</strong> level of <strong>the</strong> classroom. Philips et al. (2008) suggest that one of <strong>the</strong>reasons for <strong>the</strong> mismatch might be that teachers lack awareness as well asspecialised thorough knowledge and understand<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> concepts thatare to be successfully converted from research and applied <strong>in</strong> practice.Indeed, one can be truly confused with <strong>the</strong> massive amount of conflict<strong>in</strong>gresearch outcomes, compet<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ories and alternative treatmentsoffered <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> study of dyslexia. In light of <strong>the</strong> above, some reluctanceseems justified.It appears that bridg<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> research-practice gap would first <strong>in</strong>volvetranslat<strong>in</strong>g and dissem<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs so that one would notfail to dist<strong>in</strong>guish between <strong>the</strong> issues that still constitute a matter of<strong>in</strong>tense debate and <strong>the</strong> facts that have already ga<strong>in</strong>ed widespreadrecognition and acceptance <strong>in</strong> dyslexia research and practice. For<strong>in</strong>stance, <strong>the</strong> importance and necessity of phonological tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g for115


116 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>children with dyslexia is widely approved by researchers, practitionersand teachers alike, and supported by constantly accumulat<strong>in</strong>g scholarlyand pedagogical knowledge. Naturally, <strong>the</strong>re have been several propositionswith regard to alternative modes of treatment <strong>in</strong> dyslexia, to whichsome attention is devoted <strong>in</strong> this chapter as well.In order to m<strong>in</strong>imise <strong>the</strong> literacy problems of <strong>in</strong>dividuals withdyslexia, it is necessary to enhance automatisation of <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g andspell<strong>in</strong>g skills through carefully designed, monitored and long-termread<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction (Nicolson & Fawcett, 2001). Importantly, direct<strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> phonological awareness and letter-sound correspondencestypically br<strong>in</strong>gs highly positive effects <strong>in</strong> terms of enhanced wordidentification, spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g ability (Vellut<strong>in</strong>o et al., 2004). The<strong>in</strong>fluence of phonological tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g on read<strong>in</strong>g acquisition is deemedespecially effective when it is comb<strong>in</strong>ed with <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> letter-soundmapp<strong>in</strong>gs (Bogdanowicz, 2002a; Gustafson et al., 2007; Reid, 1998; Wiseet al., 2007).What additionally follows from <strong>the</strong> research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs is that neuralnetworks necessary to mediate word recognition can be successfullyproduced through <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>teraction of bra<strong>in</strong> and environment (pedagogical<strong>in</strong>tervention, <strong>in</strong>struction or early literacy support) (Vellut<strong>in</strong>o et al., 2004) probably <strong>the</strong> most crucial implication for all those preoccupied withdevelop<strong>in</strong>g literacy faculty. As has been mentioned, 1 f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of severalimag<strong>in</strong>g studies <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g bra<strong>in</strong> activation patterns <strong>in</strong> Englishmonol<strong>in</strong>gual dyslexics before and after phonologically driven read<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>in</strong>dicate <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>creased activation of areas typically employedby regular readers, which means that <strong>in</strong>struction plays a vital role <strong>in</strong>creat<strong>in</strong>g bra<strong>in</strong> activation patterns, allow<strong>in</strong>g successful read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> childrenwith dyslexia. In o<strong>the</strong>r words, dyslexia-specific bra<strong>in</strong> activation mapsmay become normal follow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tensive remedial tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g (e.g. Aylwardet al., 2003; Blachman et al., 2004; Richards et al., 2000; Shaywitz et al.,2004; Simos et al., 2002). Similarly, functional neuroimag<strong>in</strong>g techniquescan be potentially applied to assess<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> effectiveness and success ofvarious read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>structions/<strong>in</strong>terventions <strong>in</strong> English as a second/foreign language (ESL/EAL) learners (Pugh et al., 2005).Wagner et al. (2005) imply that <strong>in</strong>asmuch as special education activitiestypically serve well <strong>in</strong> putt<strong>in</strong>g a stop to fur<strong>the</strong>r distanc<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> observedand expected achievement, <strong>the</strong>y are <strong>in</strong>capable of normalis<strong>in</strong>g performance.At <strong>the</strong> same time, research-based treatments lead more frequentlyto improvement and larger ga<strong>in</strong>s, though ma<strong>in</strong>ly with regard to accuracyra<strong>the</strong>r than fluency or comprehension. Ano<strong>the</strong>r problem seems to be <strong>the</strong>apparent lack of universality of a given <strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>in</strong> terms ofeffectiveness. Significant differences between treatment and controlgroups are shown, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g usefulness and efficiency of a given setof educational actions. Still, more often than not, <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong>


Treatment and Teach<strong>in</strong>g 117reaction to treatment emerge, evidenc<strong>in</strong>g little effect or even totally<strong>in</strong>tractable cases of non-responders, most typically fall<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong>category of <strong>the</strong> poorest-achiev<strong>in</strong>g, lowest-perform<strong>in</strong>g and, at <strong>the</strong> sametime, most severely disordered children. Hence, <strong>the</strong> suggestion thatassess<strong>in</strong>g treatments <strong>in</strong> terms of <strong>the</strong>ir overall effectiveness requires go<strong>in</strong>gbeyond group mean differences.In fact, Whiteley et al. (2007) report that about 30% of young childrenidentified as at-risk for literacy difficulties fail to take advantage of earlyconventional, phonologically based <strong>in</strong>tervention, even <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> context ofone-to-one tuition. Those non-respondents or non-beneficiaries arepredicted by particularly below-standard letter knowledge and low levelof expressive vocabulary. Naturally, <strong>the</strong>y require <strong>in</strong>tensive special<strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>se aspects of knowledge <strong>in</strong> addition to phonologicaltra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g.In a similar ve<strong>in</strong>, Gustafson et al. (2007) note that phonologicaltra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, notwithstand<strong>in</strong>g its empirically supported effectiveness withregard to general average improvements <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g skill, tends not to beequally effective for all participants. Children with dyslexia vary withrespect to phonological and orthographic word decod<strong>in</strong>g; Gustafson andcolleagues <strong>in</strong>dicate that <strong>the</strong>se skills can have critical effect on <strong>the</strong><strong>in</strong>tervention results. In fact, <strong>the</strong>y verified <strong>the</strong> effectiveness of two diversecomputerised tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g programmes for poor readers with predom<strong>in</strong>antlyphonological or pr<strong>in</strong>cipally orthographic problems, one programmebe<strong>in</strong>g phonologically oriented, and <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r orthographically focused.Children receiv<strong>in</strong>g phonological and orthographic tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g alike, onaverage strongly improved <strong>the</strong>ir text read<strong>in</strong>g and general word decod<strong>in</strong>gskills. However, <strong>the</strong> improvement did not prove statistically greater thanthat evidenced by children from <strong>the</strong> comparison groups, namely, regularreaders and children who received ord<strong>in</strong>ary special <strong>in</strong>struction (run byteacher-<strong>the</strong>rapists, accommodat<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction to <strong>in</strong>dividualneeds and abilities of children). All <strong>in</strong> all, phonologically deficientreaders improved <strong>the</strong>ir general word decod<strong>in</strong>g skills more fromphonological than from orthographic tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g. The opposite was demonstratedfor children with poor orthographic skills. Thus, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> cases ofchildren show<strong>in</strong>g a considerable gap between <strong>the</strong>ir phonological andorthographic word decod<strong>in</strong>g abilities, read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terventions and tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gprogrammes should focus on <strong>the</strong>ir relative weakness ra<strong>the</strong>r than strength<strong>in</strong> word decod<strong>in</strong>g. Similarly, White et al. (2006) strongly suggest thattreatments for dyslexia should deal with observed <strong>in</strong>dividual deficitsra<strong>the</strong>r than claim to constitute a solution to all dyslexic problems.Naturally, <strong>the</strong> problem of denot<strong>in</strong>g effective read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terventionalso applies to bil<strong>in</strong>gual and multil<strong>in</strong>gual context, for example, withregard to ESL/EAL. It seems that research data on read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>structionfor read<strong>in</strong>g-disabled ESL learners is <strong>in</strong>deed sparse (Lovett et al., 2008).


118 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>In fact, as is <strong>the</strong> case with early screen<strong>in</strong>g procedures designed primarilyfor monol<strong>in</strong>gual children, <strong>the</strong> critical components of read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terventionprogrammes prepared with monol<strong>in</strong>guals <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d can most probablybenefit bil<strong>in</strong>guals. Lovett et al. (2008) report no overall differencesbetween read<strong>in</strong>g-disabled native English-speak<strong>in</strong>g children and ESLschool-age students <strong>in</strong> response to systematic, explicit, phonologicallybased read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tervention. The tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g proves effective irrespective of<strong>the</strong> primary language background of poor readers, however, it seems tobe conditioned by <strong>the</strong> former achievement of <strong>the</strong> basic level of Englishlanguage competence.As for some alternative types of treatment, let us beg<strong>in</strong> with methodsof <strong>the</strong>rapy for dyslexia type P (perceptual) and L (l<strong>in</strong>guistic) compiled byBakker. 2 This <strong>in</strong>tervention programme is based on <strong>the</strong> assumption that<strong>the</strong> normal developmental process of literacy acquisition, ei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>in</strong> a firstor a subsequent language, beg<strong>in</strong>s with more substantial <strong>in</strong>volvement of<strong>the</strong> right hemisphere and <strong>the</strong>n transfers to <strong>the</strong> left (Robertson, 2000b;Robertson & Bakker, 2008; Stamboltzis & Pumfrey, 2000). Read<strong>in</strong>gdifficulties manifest <strong>the</strong>mselves if, dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> process of learn<strong>in</strong>g toread, <strong>the</strong> shift of dom<strong>in</strong>ance from <strong>the</strong> right to <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere takesplace ei<strong>the</strong>r too late (P-type dyslexia) or too early (L-type dyslexia),br<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g about <strong>the</strong> lack of balance between <strong>the</strong> perceptual and l<strong>in</strong>guisticstrategies used for read<strong>in</strong>g. P-type and L-type can be treated bystimulation of <strong>the</strong> left and right hemispheres, respectively, with <strong>the</strong> useof various sensory modalities tactile, auditory and visual. Such anactivity should presumably result <strong>in</strong> alterations <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> use of <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>gstrategies and, consequently, <strong>in</strong> changes <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g performance.Two remedial techniques have been proposed: hemisphere-specificstimulation (HSS) and hemisphere-allud<strong>in</strong>g stimulation (HAS). The HSStechnique <strong>in</strong>volves direct unilateral presentation of <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g materialto <strong>the</strong> right or left visual field, to <strong>the</strong> right or left ear and/or to <strong>the</strong> f<strong>in</strong>gersof <strong>the</strong> right or left hand <strong>in</strong> P-type and L-type dyslexics, respectively. Theright visual field and <strong>the</strong> right hand project onto <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere; <strong>the</strong>left visual field and <strong>the</strong> left hand project onto <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere.However, as far as <strong>the</strong> auditory channel is concerned, <strong>the</strong> dissociation <strong>in</strong>hemispheric projection is not total. Although contralateral projectionsdom<strong>in</strong>ate, ipsilateral ones also exist. Still, <strong>the</strong> activity of <strong>the</strong> ipsilateralhemisphere may be reduced dur<strong>in</strong>g listen<strong>in</strong>g tasks through <strong>the</strong> simultaneouspresentation of verbal <strong>in</strong>formation to one ear and non-verbal<strong>in</strong>formation to <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r ear.HAS provides for <strong>in</strong>direct bilateral presentation of <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>gmaterial to stimulate <strong>the</strong> left or right hemisphere. The stimulus isperceived by both hemispheres, but HAS engages each of <strong>the</strong> hemispheresby specifically manipulat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> nature of <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g task. Forexample, a perceptually difficult text (atypical fonts, pictures), which


Treatment and Teach<strong>in</strong>g 119necessitates greater <strong>in</strong>volvement of <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere, should bepresented to L-type dyslexics. It is fur<strong>the</strong>r recommended that read<strong>in</strong>gmaterials for P-type dyslexics, to activate <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere, beperceptually simple and require <strong>the</strong> use of l<strong>in</strong>guistic strategies (e.g.fill<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> miss<strong>in</strong>g words <strong>in</strong> a text, recognis<strong>in</strong>g and form<strong>in</strong>g rhymes,form<strong>in</strong>g sentences from <strong>the</strong> given words). HAS is transferable to <strong>the</strong>classroom situation, while, unsurpris<strong>in</strong>gly, HSS is not transferablebecause sophisticated equipment is required for carry<strong>in</strong>g out <strong>the</strong> tasks.A substantial bulk of behavioural and electrophysiological researchhas been conducted to validate Bakker’s balance <strong>the</strong>ory, particularlyrelat<strong>in</strong>g to classify<strong>in</strong>g dyslexics <strong>in</strong>to P and L types <strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>e with <strong>the</strong> patternof read<strong>in</strong>g errors and read<strong>in</strong>g speed and to <strong>the</strong> efficiency of <strong>the</strong>rapy(Bakker, 1984, 1990, 1995; Bakker et al., 1995; Bogdanowicz & Krasowicz,1995, 1996/1997; Kappers, 1997; Robertson, 2000a, 2000b). Bakker et al.(1995) argue that electrophysiological studies <strong>in</strong>dicate different patternsof hemispheric activation dur<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g and process<strong>in</strong>g of letters <strong>in</strong>P- and L-type dyslexics, thus support<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> argument about diverse<strong>in</strong>volvement of <strong>the</strong> left and right bra<strong>in</strong> areas. Additionally, left versusright hemisphere stimulation results <strong>in</strong> different effects on read<strong>in</strong>gaccuracy and speed. More precisely, greater effects of direct and <strong>in</strong>direc<strong>the</strong>mispheric stimulation, irrespective of sensory modality, have beenobta<strong>in</strong>ed with reference to <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere <strong>in</strong> L-type dyslexia. Somepositive <strong>in</strong>fluence of HSS and HAS has also been observed with regard to<strong>the</strong> left hemisphere <strong>in</strong> P-type dyslexia. Never<strong>the</strong>less, despite <strong>the</strong>apparently high efficiency of hemispheric stimulation, unfortunately itcannot change a dyslexic reader <strong>in</strong>to a non-dyslexic reader (Bakker,1990). Still, as Kappers (1997) and Robertson (2000a) claim, it has broughtabout considerable improvements <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g ability of many severelydisordered children with dyslexia.However, as reported by o<strong>the</strong>r studies (Dryer et al., 1999), treatmentaimed at stimulat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> underactive hemisphere when compared totreatment aimed at stimulat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> overactive hemisphere, did not result<strong>in</strong> greater read<strong>in</strong>g ga<strong>in</strong>s. Admittedly, <strong>the</strong>se f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs seem to run counterto <strong>the</strong> predictions based on <strong>the</strong> balance model. They suggest that <strong>the</strong>ga<strong>in</strong>s made dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tervention programme may have beenattributable to o<strong>the</strong>r variables ra<strong>the</strong>r than to <strong>the</strong> specific nature ofremedial methods. In addition, <strong>the</strong> reliability of dyslexia types proposedby Bakker has been questioned by Dryer et al. (1999).Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Bogdanowicz and Krasowicz (1995, 1996/1997), <strong>the</strong>efficiency of <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>rapy proposed by Bakker is, to some extent,guaranteed by <strong>the</strong> multimodal activity, and <strong>the</strong> controversies referredto earlier may result from <strong>in</strong>adequate identification of dyslexia type.Consequently, a given k<strong>in</strong>d of <strong>the</strong>rapy may not br<strong>in</strong>g about <strong>the</strong>anticipated results. It is fur<strong>the</strong>r argued that <strong>the</strong> application of HSS and


120 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>HAS could be l<strong>in</strong>ked to <strong>the</strong> diagnosis of <strong>the</strong> cognitive function<strong>in</strong>g,namely, visual-spatial functions deficits would be treated by <strong>the</strong> righ<strong>the</strong>misphere stimulation, while language deficits would require <strong>the</strong>activation of <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere.Ano<strong>the</strong>r suggestion for <strong>the</strong>rapeutic activities can be traced back to <strong>the</strong>visual and auditory magnocellular hypo<strong>the</strong>sis. The fact that monocularocclusion (blank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> vision of one eye) can improve <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g abilityof children with visual b<strong>in</strong>ocular <strong>in</strong>stability has been confirmed (Ste<strong>in</strong>,2001; Ste<strong>in</strong> et al., 2000a). In <strong>the</strong> cases of such children, read<strong>in</strong>g with oneeye (<strong>the</strong> right one), with <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r blanked, reduces <strong>the</strong>ir b<strong>in</strong>ocularperceptual confusion and allows improvement <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir read<strong>in</strong>g performance.The effects are claimed to be dramatic and progress is far greaterthan <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r remediation methods for dyslexics (Ste<strong>in</strong>, 2001; Ste<strong>in</strong> et al.,2001).Yet ano<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>rapeutic proposition touches on <strong>the</strong> sensory tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>detection of rapidly presented acoustic stimuli, which leads to betterphonological process<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>the</strong>refore to read<strong>in</strong>g improvement (Bower,2000; Ste<strong>in</strong>, 2001). Highly accurate process<strong>in</strong>g of temporal change by <strong>the</strong>auditory system is important for proper development of <strong>the</strong> phonologicalskills (Talcott et al., 2000b; Tallal et al., 1996). It has been found(Bogdanowicz, 1999; Horgan, 1997; Merzenich et al., 1996; Ste<strong>in</strong>, 2001)that tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g children with language learn<strong>in</strong>g impairment, us<strong>in</strong>g acomputer program <strong>in</strong> which <strong>the</strong> sound frequency changes can be sloweddown and amplitude changes can be <strong>in</strong>creased (stretched speech),greatly ameliorated <strong>the</strong>ir performance. In all likelihood, <strong>in</strong>dividualswith dyslexia undergo<strong>in</strong>g similar tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g would display analogousresults (Ste<strong>in</strong>, 2001), namely, greater ability to dist<strong>in</strong>guish betweenrapidly occurr<strong>in</strong>g acoustic stimuli and <strong>the</strong>ir sequence, lead<strong>in</strong>g to betterphonological process<strong>in</strong>g, which presupposes <strong>the</strong> improvement <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>read<strong>in</strong>g skill.Ano<strong>the</strong>r k<strong>in</strong>d of <strong>the</strong>rapy for children with dyslexia suffer<strong>in</strong>g fromscotopic sensitivity syndrome (SSS) was <strong>in</strong>troduced by Irlen. SSS isconnected with sensitivity to light. Students report <strong>the</strong> perception of aglare from white paper, which makes it hard to decipher a text, anddifficulty see<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>t clearly, let alone an impression that it movesaround <strong>the</strong> page. SSS is additionally connected with eyestra<strong>in</strong> eyesoften water, itch or burn (Jameson, 2000; Jędrzejowska & Jurek, 2003; Ott,1997). It has been suggested that coloured overlays or t<strong>in</strong>ted lenses canhelp. Children’s responses to colour filters for view<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> text aremeasured by an apparatus called <strong>the</strong> Intuitive Colorimeter and <strong>the</strong>nadequate coloured lenses can be prescribed. However, <strong>the</strong> nature of <strong>the</strong>treatment seems to be controversial and <strong>the</strong>re is no conclusive evidencethat it can improve poor read<strong>in</strong>g performance. Coloured lenses or filtersare likely to reduce <strong>the</strong> feel<strong>in</strong>g of sore, tired eyes, headaches and,


Treatment and Teach<strong>in</strong>g 121consequently, enhance <strong>the</strong> child’s motivation to read, but <strong>the</strong>y are notlikely to change a dyslexic reader <strong>in</strong>to a good reader (Ott, 1997).However, some accommodation of classroom materials may provebeneficial for children with dyslexia, for example, large, widely spacedpr<strong>in</strong>t, clear text on an uncluttered page tend to decrease <strong>the</strong> visualperceptual impression of <strong>the</strong> letters and words mov<strong>in</strong>g around <strong>the</strong> page,blurr<strong>in</strong>g or sp<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, small, newspaper-like greypr<strong>in</strong>t, fancy or unusual fonts, capitalisation of whole words and phrasescan <strong>in</strong>tensify <strong>the</strong> disturbances (Jameson, 2000; Lev<strong>in</strong>son, 1980).Summ<strong>in</strong>g up, I will share <strong>the</strong> sceptical view expressed by Kirk et al.(2001: 293) with reference to <strong>the</strong> proposed effectiveness and alleviat<strong>in</strong>geffect on <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g ability of <strong>the</strong> ‘almost weekly addition of dyslexiacures’, such as fish oil capsules, massage <strong>the</strong>rapy, vitam<strong>in</strong>s, colouredlenses, neurol<strong>in</strong>guistic programm<strong>in</strong>g, reorientation of <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> ando<strong>the</strong>rs. Apparently, sensational media reports about wonderful cures fordyslexia certa<strong>in</strong>ly do no good, quite <strong>the</strong> opposite, <strong>the</strong>y are unhelpful bothfor <strong>the</strong> professionals and <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia. <strong>Dyslexia</strong> is a lifetimeand diversify<strong>in</strong>g condition <strong>in</strong> terms of types and severity of impairment.Bear<strong>in</strong>g that fact <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> construction of <strong>the</strong>rapeutic programmes,allowances should be made for <strong>the</strong> educational needs and abilities of<strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia. All <strong>in</strong> all, dyslexia is not a disease it cannotbe cured, however, its negative effects certa<strong>in</strong>ly can be reduced or evensometimes completely elim<strong>in</strong>ated due to <strong>in</strong>tensive, adequate and regulareducational efforts, focus<strong>in</strong>g on read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g skills enhancement.In a similar ve<strong>in</strong>, White et al. (2006: 252) suggest: ‘There is no po<strong>in</strong>ttra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> auditory abilities of children who have no auditory deficit,<strong>the</strong> b<strong>in</strong>ocular control of children who have no visual impairment, <strong>the</strong>balance of children who have no balance problem, and <strong>the</strong> phonologicalskills who have no phonological deficit’. White et al. (2006) stronglyadvise aga<strong>in</strong>st <strong>in</strong>discrim<strong>in</strong>ately apply<strong>in</strong>g a given type of treatment to allchildren with dyslexia <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> hope of ameliorat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ir impairments. Infact, <strong>the</strong>y claim that <strong>the</strong> majority of dyslexics with def<strong>in</strong>ite phonologicaldeficit should receive phonological treatment, while much fewer childrenwith dyslexia who suffer from various types of visual disorders wouldbenefit most from visual <strong>in</strong>terventions, tailored to a given k<strong>in</strong>d of visualdeficit. They also voice a claim that it is unreasonable to expect anyauditory or motor treatment to have advantageous results with regard toread<strong>in</strong>g. In fact, White et al. (2006) <strong>in</strong>itiated an <strong>in</strong>tense debate on <strong>the</strong> roleof <strong>the</strong> sensorimotor impairments <strong>in</strong> dyslexia (<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g implications forcl<strong>in</strong>ical practice) among researchers, receiv<strong>in</strong>g some bitter criticism from<strong>the</strong>ir opponents and advocates of <strong>the</strong> abovementioned treatmentmethods.In sum, highly structured phonic read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction, paired withphonemic awareness tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g seems to be a sound, <strong>the</strong>oretically based


122 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong><strong>in</strong>tervention whose effectiveness <strong>in</strong> alleviat<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>in</strong>dyslexia has been repeatedly shown. Impressive effects of <strong>the</strong> equivalentapproach (<strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g explicit phonemic awareness tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, coupled withletter-sound tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g) <strong>in</strong> prevent<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g failure <strong>in</strong> children at-risk fordyslexia have also been reported. Still, ameliorat<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g difficultiesdoes not necessarily equal elim<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m, what is more, it usuallyrequires great <strong>in</strong>vestment <strong>in</strong> terms of effort and time. Thus, prevent<strong>in</strong>gand overcom<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties heavily depends on <strong>in</strong>tensive andspecialist teach<strong>in</strong>g, often extend<strong>in</strong>g over long periods of time.Pr<strong>in</strong>ciples of <strong>the</strong> Multisensory StructuredLearn<strong>in</strong>g ApproachPedagogical <strong>the</strong>rapy is def<strong>in</strong>ed as an <strong>in</strong>tervention employ<strong>in</strong>g specialforms of educational and didactic help, undertaken <strong>in</strong> order to m<strong>in</strong>imiseor elim<strong>in</strong>ate disorders, stimulate learn<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>in</strong>duce positive changes <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong> cognitive and emotional-motivational function<strong>in</strong>g of a given child.Thus, <strong>the</strong> ma<strong>in</strong> aim of pedagogical <strong>the</strong>rapy is to create opportunities forall-round mental, psychological and social development, through levell<strong>in</strong>gdisharmonies and deficits, and reduc<strong>in</strong>g scholastic failure, and, <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong> long run, its emotional and social consequences (Czajkowska &Herda, 1998). Therapeutic activities are realised <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> form of correctioncompensation/special<strong>in</strong>struction classes, concentrat<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong> correctionof developmental deficits as well as on compensation, def<strong>in</strong>ed as fur<strong>the</strong>rimprovement of <strong>the</strong> functions that are not disordered, so that <strong>the</strong>y cansupport or replace <strong>the</strong> disordered ones (Czajkowska & Herda, 1998;Jędrzejowska & Jurek, 2003; Kaja, 2001b).The <strong>in</strong>troduction of systematic remedial work, run by a teacher<strong>the</strong>rapistat school or on a one-to-one basis, can be fur<strong>the</strong>r cont<strong>in</strong>ued byparents at home or, if necessary, by specialists at a psychologicalpedagogicalcl<strong>in</strong>ic. Such a l<strong>in</strong>e of conduct typically allows decreas<strong>in</strong>g orsuppress<strong>in</strong>g developmental disorders, thus facilitat<strong>in</strong>g a successfulbeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of school career (Bogdanowicz, 2002a). Unsurpris<strong>in</strong>gly, <strong>the</strong>later <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>rapeutic activities are undertaken, <strong>the</strong> more difficult andeffortful <strong>the</strong>y are, because read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties rout<strong>in</strong>ely become <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly<strong>in</strong>tractable with age (Jędrzejowska & Jurek, 2003; Zakrzewska, 1999).As already po<strong>in</strong>ted out, a start<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>t for <strong>the</strong> construction of <strong>the</strong>remedial teach<strong>in</strong>g programme are recommendations draw<strong>in</strong>g on adetailed assessment. Individual plans of <strong>the</strong>rapeutic activities arenaturally subject to verifications and modifications, applied depend<strong>in</strong>gon <strong>the</strong> ongo<strong>in</strong>g identification of <strong>the</strong> needs and achievements (Reid, 1998;Tod, 2008; Zakrzewska, 1999).In general, several pr<strong>in</strong>ciples regulate <strong>the</strong> course of organisation of <strong>the</strong>remedial teach<strong>in</strong>g process. Individualisation of measures and methods is


Treatment and Teach<strong>in</strong>g 123particularly important due to considerable differentiation as to <strong>the</strong> type,range and <strong>in</strong>tensity of developmental disorders and learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties.Graduation of <strong>the</strong> complexity of tasks and activities is aimed atdevelop<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong> proportion to <strong>the</strong> capabilitiesof a given child. Thus, after complet<strong>in</strong>g elementary, simple exercises, wecan move towards more complicated tasks. Appropriate match<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong>perplexity of a task to <strong>the</strong> abilities of a child with dyslexia conditionssuccessful completion of <strong>the</strong> assignment (Czajkowska & Herda, 1998).Reid (1998) stresses <strong>the</strong> need for <strong>the</strong> application of a structured approach,so that learn<strong>in</strong>g can appear <strong>in</strong> a l<strong>in</strong>ear developmental manner, whichenables learners to grasp a particular skill before advanc<strong>in</strong>g to asubsequent one. A sequential and cumulative approach may help tomake <strong>the</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g process more mean<strong>in</strong>gful and effective. Needless tosay, <strong>the</strong> choice of materials relevant for students’ <strong>in</strong>terests as well asemployment of diverse and attractive teach<strong>in</strong>g techniques and exercisesexerts positive <strong>in</strong>fluence on <strong>the</strong> way <strong>the</strong> tasks are executed by students.Additionally, children with dyslexia require plenty of re<strong>in</strong>forcement,repetition and overlearn<strong>in</strong>g, which are meant to eventually lead toautomaticity (Deponio et al., 2000; Reid, 1998; Thomson & Watk<strong>in</strong>s; 1990).Frequent recapitulation <strong>in</strong>evitably is a must for students with dyslexiabecause <strong>the</strong>y usually f<strong>in</strong>d it abnormally troublesome to anchor <strong>in</strong>formation<strong>in</strong> memory, moreover <strong>in</strong>tensive rehearsal <strong>in</strong>vites more completemastery of a certa<strong>in</strong> part of material before <strong>the</strong> new <strong>in</strong>formation is<strong>in</strong>troduced.The most poorly acquired skills require <strong>the</strong> most <strong>in</strong>tense practice,realised through multiple diverse activities. Ano<strong>the</strong>r crucial aspect isregularity and relatively high frequency of special education activities.Systematic and recurrent practice provides <strong>the</strong> most welcome results,while long <strong>in</strong>tervals tend to br<strong>in</strong>g about partial or total regress.Cont<strong>in</strong>uous psycho<strong>the</strong>rapeutic efforts to prevent neurotic behaviour,negative attitudes towards school and learn<strong>in</strong>g, and emotional-motivationalproblems from develop<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>in</strong>tensify<strong>in</strong>g are <strong>in</strong>escapable.Teachers strive not only to make children with dyslexia able to learn,but also to make <strong>the</strong>m want to do so. The fulfilment of such a goal isconditioned by creat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> success-oriented tasks, notic<strong>in</strong>g and appreciat<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong> smallest atta<strong>in</strong>ments and, last but not least, refra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g fromcompar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> results of dyslexic students’ work with <strong>the</strong> achievementsof <strong>the</strong>ir peers, but ra<strong>the</strong>r evaluat<strong>in</strong>g dyslexics’ progress measuredcommensurate with <strong>the</strong>ir previous scores (Czajkowska & Herda, 1998;Gąsowska & Pietrzak-Stępowska, 1994; Juszczyk & Zając, 1997; Kaja,2001b; Zakrzewska, 1999).The direct multisensory structured approach is advocated for teach<strong>in</strong>gread<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> native language to children with dyslexia.The multisensory structured learn<strong>in</strong>g (MSL) style has been found effective


124 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>with regard to foreign language <strong>in</strong>struction as well (Crombie & McColl,2000; Jameson, 2000; Miller & Bussman Gillis, 2000). Orig<strong>in</strong>ally, Gill<strong>in</strong>ghamand Stillman devised <strong>the</strong> multisensory teach<strong>in</strong>g programme, basedon <strong>the</strong> pioneer<strong>in</strong>g work of Orton. The programme became known as <strong>the</strong>Orton-Gill<strong>in</strong>gham (OG) <strong>in</strong>structional approach (Gill<strong>in</strong>gham & Stillman,1997) and has s<strong>in</strong>ce been successfully used to teach read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>gto students with native language learn<strong>in</strong>g problems.Numerous remedial programmes for teach<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g and writ<strong>in</strong>g arebased on <strong>the</strong> fundamental pr<strong>in</strong>ciples of <strong>the</strong> multisensory method (Sparkset al., 1992a). For example, Alpha to Omega, <strong>the</strong> AZ of Teach<strong>in</strong>g Read<strong>in</strong>gWrit<strong>in</strong>g and Spell<strong>in</strong>g (Hornsby et al., 1999) or The Bangor <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Teach<strong>in</strong>gSystem by Miles, The Hickey Multi-sensory <strong>Language</strong> Course by Augur andBriggs, as well as <strong>the</strong> Units of Sound by Bramley (Ott, 1997). Richey andGoeke (2006) enumerate <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g: Alphabetic Phonics, Wilson Read<strong>in</strong>gSystem, The Herman Method, Project ASSIST, The Sl<strong>in</strong>gerland Approach, TheSpald<strong>in</strong>g Method, Start<strong>in</strong>g Over and Project Read, which by no meansconstitute an exhaustive list. Reid (1998) provides a comprehensive andthorough review of programmes for students with dyslexia, divid<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong>m <strong>in</strong>to four groups: <strong>in</strong>dividualised programmes, support approachesand strategies, assisted learn<strong>in</strong>g techniques and whole-school approaches.All <strong>the</strong> programmes mentioned above relate to teach<strong>in</strong>gEnglish as a native language. In Poland, for example, <strong>the</strong> Method of <strong>the</strong>Good Start compiled by Bogdanowicz (1985b) is based on <strong>the</strong> visualauditoryk<strong>in</strong>aes<strong>the</strong>tictactile (VAKT) approach and is used both <strong>in</strong>prevention and <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>rapy (Bogdanowicz, 1997a). In <strong>the</strong> tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gprogramme for teach<strong>in</strong>g German as a foreign/second language to atrisklearners, Schneider (1999) <strong>in</strong>troduced <strong>the</strong> multisensory structuredmetacognitive language <strong>in</strong>struction (MSML). She additionally addressed <strong>the</strong>metacognitive component more directly than <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> traditional MSLapproach. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Schneider (1999), <strong>the</strong> tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g of matacognitiveskills is necessary for students with language learn<strong>in</strong>g disabilitiesbecause of <strong>the</strong>ir poor ability to recognise and understand <strong>the</strong> rule systemof a foreign/second language.The fact that children with dyslexia require a multisensory approach<strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g is nowadays widely known and commonlyaccepted (Ganschow & Sparks, 1995; Jędrzejowska & Jurek, 2003;Johnson, 1978; Ott, 1997; Sparks & Ganschow, 1993; Sparks et al., 1991;1998c; Thomson & Watk<strong>in</strong>s, 1990). Schneider (1999), Szczerbiński (2007),Sparks and Miller (2000), Ritchey and Goeke (2006) enumerate <strong>the</strong>follow<strong>in</strong>g general features of teach<strong>in</strong>g methods with regard to pr<strong>in</strong>tprocess<strong>in</strong>g (read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g) that children with dyslexia f<strong>in</strong>dbeneficial multisensory, direct and explicit (rules do not have to beguessed or <strong>in</strong>ferred by students), systematic, highly structured, sequential,cumulative, syn<strong>the</strong>tic, phonetic, phonics-driven and, f<strong>in</strong>ally, giv<strong>in</strong>g


Treatment and Teach<strong>in</strong>g 125sufficient practice and consolidation, and preferably conducted <strong>in</strong> smallgroups or <strong>in</strong>dividually.More precisely, simultaneous activation of <strong>the</strong> auditory, tactile, visualand k<strong>in</strong>aes<strong>the</strong>tic pathways, which supports compensation, makes up abasic component of <strong>the</strong> MSL. Thus, <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r words, <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation<strong>in</strong>tegrated via unaffected routes can lead to <strong>the</strong> development of writtenlanguage skills. The multimodal perception is usually more advantageousthan mono-modal with regard to both <strong>the</strong> amount of rememberedmaterial and <strong>the</strong> pace of learn<strong>in</strong>g (Wlodarski, 1998). The simultaneouspresentation of l<strong>in</strong>guistic material with <strong>the</strong> use of as many sensorychannels as possible benefit <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia, <strong>in</strong> fact, it isassumed that <strong>the</strong> more modalities <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g process, <strong>the</strong>more effective it appears to be. Teach<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g with <strong>the</strong>help of multisensory methods is realised by <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tegration of visual,auditory, k<strong>in</strong>aes<strong>the</strong>tic and tactile stimuli and <strong>in</strong>volves simultaneouspresentation of <strong>in</strong>formation com<strong>in</strong>g from various senses. Thus, multisensoryteach<strong>in</strong>g is based on <strong>the</strong> constant use of <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g: what aletter or a word looks like, how it sounds and how <strong>the</strong> speech organs andhand feel when produc<strong>in</strong>g it (Bogdanowicz, 1997a; Ott, 1997). A personwith dyslexia learns how to read and spell words by hear<strong>in</strong>g, see<strong>in</strong>g andpronounc<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m, by mak<strong>in</strong>g models of plastic<strong>in</strong>e, form<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m fromwooden, sponge or plastic letters; f<strong>in</strong>ally, by trac<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m on varioussurfaces, such as paper, carpet, floor, sand and by writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m. Themore <strong>the</strong> perceptual channels are open, <strong>the</strong> greater <strong>the</strong> possibility ofform<strong>in</strong>g associations between <strong>the</strong> graphic (visual) and phonologicalaspects of a word as well as its mean<strong>in</strong>g. If a stimulus is complex, itactivates several receptors and perception of <strong>in</strong>formation is realisedsimultaneously through several sensory channels.Ano<strong>the</strong>r rule perta<strong>in</strong>s to <strong>the</strong> direct and explicit familiaris<strong>in</strong>g ofstudents with <strong>the</strong> phonological (phonological awareness), alphabetic(letter-sound correspondences), morphemic (roots, prefixes, suffixes) aswell as syntactic structure of language. Thus, for example, children withdyslexia are specifically made aware of <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual phonemes <strong>in</strong>words and how to relate <strong>the</strong>m to <strong>the</strong>ir written representations (graphemes),and <strong>the</strong>y also learn how to sequence <strong>the</strong>m <strong>in</strong> words. Childrenwith dyslexia require carefully planned (<strong>in</strong> terms of scope and sequence),direct and explicit <strong>in</strong>struction, as opposed to implicit and <strong>in</strong>cidental.Philips et al. (2008) see <strong>the</strong> backbone of <strong>the</strong> explicit <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> way<strong>the</strong> teacher participates <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction, namely, he/sheissues statements and behaves <strong>in</strong> a way that clarifies <strong>the</strong> task’s demandsto students and gives a model of <strong>the</strong> task outcome that is expected. Thus,def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, modell<strong>in</strong>g and expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g are <strong>the</strong> most commonly referred to<strong>in</strong>structional techniques <strong>in</strong> explicit teach<strong>in</strong>g. In order to successfullycomplete a task, children with dyslexia require a lot of repetition


126 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>and practice until <strong>the</strong>y are ready to come up with <strong>the</strong> answer unaided.Self-dependence is achieved <strong>in</strong> a step-by-step fashion, beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g withguided practice, through supported practice to <strong>in</strong>dependent practice.Importantly, children get constant support tak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> form of scaffold<strong>in</strong>g(verbal prompts, modell<strong>in</strong>g, additional examples), accommodated to<strong>in</strong>dividual needs. Last but not least, frequent, immediate, positive andspecific feedback characterises explicit <strong>in</strong>struction.The effective <strong>in</strong>struction should also be syn<strong>the</strong>tic/analytic, wherestudents are taught how to break down larger units <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong>ir constituentcomponents as well as how to put <strong>the</strong> parts back <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> proper sequence.Sight words, which do not follow any rule patterns, are taught as wholeunits. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, MSL teach<strong>in</strong>g is structured, which means that it isorganised <strong>in</strong>to small units with logical and straightforward presentationand practice of <strong>the</strong> rules. It is also sequential <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> course of tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gwe move from <strong>the</strong> simple and regular patterns to more complex andirregular ones, and cumulative new <strong>in</strong>formation is built on what isalready thoroughly <strong>in</strong>tegrated. Thus, only a small amount of material ispresented at a time, with a full mastery of <strong>the</strong> content via multisensorytechniques (simultaneous say<strong>in</strong>g, see<strong>in</strong>g, hear<strong>in</strong>g and writ<strong>in</strong>g) beforeadvanc<strong>in</strong>g to new components. The crucial component of <strong>the</strong> MSL isrepetition and overlearn<strong>in</strong>g, employed <strong>in</strong> order to ensure automaticity aswell as to <strong>in</strong>crease memory and rate of access. The teach<strong>in</strong>g process is<strong>in</strong>dividualised with ongo<strong>in</strong>g assessment and diagnostic <strong>in</strong>formation as toprogress and needs.Ritchey and Goeke (2006) review literature on <strong>the</strong> effectiveness of <strong>the</strong>OG and OG-based read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction programmes <strong>in</strong> contrast to o<strong>the</strong>r<strong>in</strong>structional approaches. The overall conclusion is that OG and OGbased<strong>in</strong>struction br<strong>in</strong>gs about positive effects <strong>in</strong> terms of word read<strong>in</strong>g,word attack/decod<strong>in</strong>g, spell<strong>in</strong>g and comprehension. Moreover, it provesvalid across age groups, sett<strong>in</strong>gs and populations. However, somestudies reported <strong>the</strong> lack of statistically significant differences favour<strong>in</strong>gany of <strong>the</strong> analysed programmes or found <strong>the</strong> OG and OG-basedprogrammes <strong>in</strong>ferior <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> results <strong>the</strong>y produced to alternative programmes.All <strong>in</strong> all, <strong>the</strong> small number of studies selected for <strong>in</strong>vestigationand <strong>the</strong>ir apparent methodological diversity and faults makes <strong>the</strong>undertaken task of compar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m and generalis<strong>in</strong>g as to <strong>the</strong>ir overalleffectiveness extremely difficult. Notwithstand<strong>in</strong>g this, <strong>the</strong> OG and OGbasedprogrammes have been widely accepted and used for five decades,and repeatedly reported as overwhelm<strong>in</strong>gly efficient (also acrosssett<strong>in</strong>gs, age groups and languages) by practitioners and teachers. Aga<strong>in</strong>,Ritchey and Goeke (2006) stress <strong>the</strong> mismatch or a gap between researchand practice and suggest <strong>the</strong> need for more thorough and comprehensiveresearch on <strong>the</strong> OG and OG-based <strong>in</strong>structional programmes.


Treatment and Teach<strong>in</strong>g 127As much as <strong>the</strong> MSL approach proved to be widely valued acrosseducational sett<strong>in</strong>gs and ages, still Wearmouth and Reid (2008) voice anop<strong>in</strong>ion that it seem<strong>in</strong>gly lacks certa<strong>in</strong> elements <strong>the</strong>y consider crucial <strong>in</strong>learn<strong>in</strong>g and teach<strong>in</strong>g. Such aspects as metacognition, learn<strong>in</strong>g styles andreason<strong>in</strong>g abilities should be given more attention, as students withdyslexia quite often <strong>in</strong>sufficiently realise <strong>the</strong>ir own th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g and reason<strong>in</strong>gprocesses and f<strong>in</strong>d it troublesome to control <strong>the</strong>m. In view of <strong>the</strong>above, a teacher’s role would also <strong>in</strong>volve evaluation and re<strong>in</strong>forcementof <strong>the</strong> development of matacognitive awareness. Know<strong>in</strong>g how to learnand be<strong>in</strong>g able to reflect on and analyse <strong>the</strong> process of learn<strong>in</strong>g as such,not only its content and f<strong>in</strong>al outcome, are <strong>the</strong> skills that need to be<strong>in</strong>corporated <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong> course of tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g designed to overcome dyslexicdifficulties.Multisensory Structured Learn<strong>in</strong>g Approachand <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> StudyAdaptation of <strong>the</strong> multisensory structured learn<strong>in</strong>gapproach to foreign language teach<strong>in</strong>gCurrently, <strong>the</strong> emphasis <strong>in</strong> foreign language <strong>in</strong>struction has been on<strong>the</strong> communicative approaches, <strong>in</strong> which <strong>the</strong> aspect of <strong>in</strong>ferr<strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>gfrom <strong>the</strong> context is highlighted, while <strong>the</strong> direct teach<strong>in</strong>g of sound,sound-symbol and grammatical rule system is de-emphasized. However,<strong>the</strong>re seems to exist limited evidence confirm<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> success of <strong>the</strong>natural approaches <strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g foreign languages to students withlearn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties (Ganschow et al., 1998; Ganschow & Sparks, 2000;Sparks et al., 1992b). On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, efficient lower-level verbalprocess<strong>in</strong>g operations (letter identification and word recognition) areclaimed to play a crucial role <strong>in</strong> successful foreign language read<strong>in</strong>gcomprehension (Koda, 1992). What is more, early direct and explicit<strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> orthographic (sound-symbol) system of a foreignlanguage as well as <strong>in</strong>creased exposure to pr<strong>in</strong>t is highly recommended,particularly for learners with relatively weak native language skills(Sparks et al., 2006; Kahn-Horwitz et al., 2006). Still, as ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed bySparks et al. (1997c, 1998c), it is likely to benefit all foreign languagelearners. Naturally, for literate students, read<strong>in</strong>g is likely to enhanceforeign language learn<strong>in</strong>g (FLL), which seems <strong>in</strong> accord with <strong>the</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> native language research concern<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> ‘Mat<strong>the</strong>w effects’ <strong>in</strong>read<strong>in</strong>g good readers, unsurpris<strong>in</strong>gly, fur<strong>the</strong>r improve <strong>the</strong>ir read<strong>in</strong>gskills through <strong>the</strong> sheer activity of read<strong>in</strong>g, whereas poor readers becomeeven poorer (Stanovich, 1986; Kahn-Horwitz et al., 2006).Sparks et al. (1998c) hypo<strong>the</strong>sised that s<strong>in</strong>ce oral native language skillsare enhanced by exposure to written language, <strong>the</strong>n, logically, early andfrequent presentation of pr<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> a foreign language, comb<strong>in</strong>ed with


128 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>listen<strong>in</strong>g to and speak<strong>in</strong>g, is potentially profitable for at-risk foreignlanguage learners because it provides a multisensory <strong>in</strong>put. Even thoughfor good native and foreign language learners such an approach mayseem a bit unnatural, it is necessary for <strong>the</strong> students with weak languageskills, who are unable to <strong>in</strong>tuitively grasp <strong>the</strong> knowledge as to howspoken words are composed of smaller segments and represented byletters.Sparks et al. (Ganschow & Sparks, 2000) transferred <strong>the</strong> OG methodology,a specialised approach to teach<strong>in</strong>g basic literacy skills to<strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir native language, <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong> field ofFLL. At that time, nei<strong>the</strong>r special needs educators nor foreign languageteachers, who, as a matter of fact, were hardly familiar with <strong>the</strong> approach<strong>in</strong> question, had considered such a transfer of <strong>in</strong>structional philosophyand practice across languages feasible.Adaptation of <strong>the</strong> multisensory (MSL) methodology to teach<strong>in</strong>gforeign languages to at-risk foreign language learners resulted <strong>in</strong>improvement of both <strong>the</strong> oral and written aspects of <strong>the</strong> student’s nativelanguage performance as well as foreign language aptitude (on <strong>the</strong>modern language aptitude test; MLAT) (Ganschow et al., 1998; Ganschow& Sparks, 1995; Sparks et al., 1991, 1997a). The most desirable option forlearn<strong>in</strong>g disabled (LD) students would entail <strong>the</strong> application of <strong>the</strong> MSLapproach to foreign language teach<strong>in</strong>g, with reference to all componentsof language learn<strong>in</strong>g, with special emphasis put on phonology. Additionally,it proves advantageous to employ a foreign language as alanguage of classroom <strong>in</strong>struction, with <strong>the</strong> native language reserved forclarification of areas of special difficulty. F<strong>in</strong>ally, frequent review is amust (Sparks et al., 1991).The research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs that at-risk foreign language learners experienceh<strong>in</strong>drance expressly with <strong>the</strong> phonological code of language constitute<strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>structional rationale (Ganschow et al., 1991; Sparks et al., 1989,1992a). It has been demonstrated that students with FLL difficulties whoreceive direct multisensory <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> phonology/orthography ofa foreign language make significant ga<strong>in</strong>s and ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>m over time(Ganschow & Sparks, 1995; Sparks et al., 1992b). Never<strong>the</strong>less, despite <strong>the</strong>undeniable progress <strong>in</strong> development of <strong>the</strong> phonological/orthographiccompetence that at-risk learners make, it is possible that <strong>the</strong>y keeplagg<strong>in</strong>g beh<strong>in</strong>d good foreign language learners (Sparks et al., 1991,1997a).Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, any improvement <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sound and sound-symbolperformance as regards <strong>the</strong> native language is frequently reflected <strong>in</strong>enhanced foreign language performance. This idea is clearly supportedby research outcome, show<strong>in</strong>g that students with stronger nativelanguage read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g skills obta<strong>in</strong> higher grades <strong>in</strong> foreignlanguage courses and are more verbally proficient <strong>in</strong> a foreign language


Treatment and Teach<strong>in</strong>g 129(Sparks et al., 1995b). Sparks et al. (1992b) conducted a prelim<strong>in</strong>ary<strong>in</strong>vestigation of <strong>the</strong> effects of <strong>the</strong> multisensory structured learn<strong>in</strong>g (MSL)approach on native and foreign language performance over one year. Thepre- and post-test scores on <strong>the</strong> native language and foreign languageaptitude tests of three groups of at-risk high school students, enrolled <strong>in</strong>special sections of a first-year Spanish course, were compared. Twogroups were <strong>in</strong>structed with <strong>the</strong> use of <strong>the</strong> MSL approach, one wastaught <strong>in</strong> both English (native language) and Spanish (foreign language)(MSL/ES), <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r only <strong>in</strong> Spanish (MSL/S), while <strong>the</strong> rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>ggroup (NO-MSL) was under <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluence of <strong>the</strong> traditional foreignlanguage teach<strong>in</strong>g methodology. The hypo<strong>the</strong>sis that both <strong>the</strong> nativelanguage phonological skills as well as foreign language aptitude of <strong>the</strong>students receiv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> MSL <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> a foreign language willimprove was supported. However, <strong>the</strong> two MSL groups differed <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ga<strong>in</strong>s <strong>the</strong>y achieved. More precisely, <strong>the</strong> MSL/ES group made significantprogress on all <strong>the</strong> native language phonological measures and <strong>the</strong> longand short forms of <strong>the</strong> modern language aptitude test (MLAT) as well asits subtests. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, <strong>the</strong> MSL/S group demonstrated vitalatta<strong>in</strong>ment only with reference to <strong>the</strong> long and short forms of <strong>the</strong> MLATand not its subtests and did not show any noticeable procurement as faras <strong>the</strong> native language phonological measures were concerned. Additionally,<strong>the</strong> MSL/ES group were critically superior <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> matter ofreceptive vocabulary and verbal short-term memory measures. Bycontrast, <strong>the</strong> NO-MSL group made no significant ga<strong>in</strong>s on <strong>the</strong> nativelanguage and foreign language aptitude measures. It clearly follows from<strong>the</strong> abovementioned f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs that students with weak native languageskills f<strong>in</strong>d it helpful and beneficial to use <strong>the</strong>ir native language <strong>in</strong> order tosupport foreign language <strong>in</strong>struction with respect to phonological andsyntactical systems. In addition, it may seem ra<strong>the</strong>r unreasonable toexpect students with weak phonological process<strong>in</strong>g skills to succeed <strong>in</strong>FLL, which starts out with listen<strong>in</strong>g to a foreign language and assumesstudents to learn to comprehend and speak a foreign language similar to<strong>the</strong> way <strong>the</strong>y acquired <strong>the</strong>ir native language. Thus, logically, <strong>the</strong><strong>in</strong>struction should rely more on <strong>the</strong> simultaneous oral (listen<strong>in</strong>g andspeak<strong>in</strong>g) and written (read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g) practice <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>sound/symbol system <strong>in</strong> a foreign language <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> cases of at-risk foreignlanguage learners.The above study was replicated with <strong>the</strong> same students (cohort 1) anda new group of students (cohort 2) to fur<strong>the</strong>r scrut<strong>in</strong>ise <strong>the</strong> effectivenessof <strong>the</strong> MSL/ES <strong>in</strong>struction on native and foreign language performance.Students from cohort 2 made significant ga<strong>in</strong>s on <strong>the</strong> native languagephonological test and foreign language aptitude test. Students fromcohort 1 were followed over <strong>the</strong> second year of foreign language<strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> order to more precisely determ<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong> efficacy of <strong>the</strong>


130 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>MSL tutor<strong>in</strong>g. The group ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>in</strong>itial acquirement on all <strong>the</strong>native and foreign language aptitude measures over <strong>the</strong> second year offoreign language study (Sparks & Ganschow, 1993). Evidence from <strong>the</strong>sestudies suggests that <strong>the</strong> at-risk foreign language students are able todisplay and ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> acquisition <strong>in</strong> a foreign language as well asimprove <strong>the</strong>ir foreign language aptitude due to <strong>the</strong> effect of <strong>the</strong> directMSL/ES <strong>in</strong>struction.In ano<strong>the</strong>r study, Ganschow and Sparks (1995) analysed <strong>the</strong> results ofdirect tutor<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> foreign language phonology (Spanish) on <strong>the</strong> nativelanguage skills and foreign language aptitude of at-risk foreign languagelearners. The multisensory approach to teach<strong>in</strong>g phonological/orthographicaspects of a foreign language aga<strong>in</strong> resulted <strong>in</strong> improvement <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong> native language performance, thus allow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> at-risk foreignlanguage learners to catch up with not-at-risk learners <strong>in</strong> at least someaspects of <strong>the</strong> phonological/orthographic measures. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, foreignlanguage coach<strong>in</strong>g (multisensory structured language <strong>in</strong>structionfor at-risk learners and traditional for not-at-risk learners) resulted <strong>in</strong>amelioration, touch<strong>in</strong>g on foreign language aptitude as regards both atriskand not-at-risk learners. It is <strong>in</strong>dicated <strong>in</strong> this study that foreignlanguage <strong>in</strong>struction alone is powerful enough to amend one’s phonological/orthographic,syntactic and semantic skills as well as rotememory as measured by <strong>the</strong> MLAT. The odds are that it is <strong>in</strong>deed<strong>the</strong> specific <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> phonology/orthography that makes <strong>the</strong>difference for <strong>the</strong> at-risk foreign language learners, who show noimprovement <strong>in</strong> ei<strong>the</strong>r foreign language aptitude or <strong>in</strong> any nativelanguage skill after one year of foreign language traditional <strong>in</strong>struction(without direct and explicit teach<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> sound system of a foreignlanguage) (Sparks et al., 1992b).Still, despite substantial native language and foreign languageaptitude upgrad<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>the</strong> at-risk learners repeatedly and significantlyevidence fall<strong>in</strong>g beh<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong> not-at-risk students, relevant to foreignlanguage aptitude measured by <strong>the</strong> MLAT (Ganschow & Sparks, 1995).Thus, direct <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> phonology/orthography, even though apparentlybeneficial for at-risk learners, tends not to guarantee <strong>the</strong> equalis<strong>in</strong>gof <strong>the</strong> scores for <strong>the</strong> at-risk and not-at-risk students on <strong>the</strong> foreignlanguage aptitude test.Sparks et al. (1997a) obta<strong>in</strong>ed similar results, consistent with <strong>the</strong>f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of <strong>the</strong> previous study (Ganschow & Sparks, 1995), with regardto <strong>the</strong> performance on <strong>the</strong> native language and foreign language aptitudemeasures of <strong>the</strong> at-risk and not-at-risk learners, after two years of foreignlanguage (Spanish) tutor<strong>in</strong>g. Aga<strong>in</strong>, <strong>the</strong> at-risk group underwent multisensorystructured language tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, while <strong>the</strong> not-at-risk groupreceived <strong>in</strong>struction via traditional methodology. Additionally, <strong>the</strong>experiment aimed at answer<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> question, whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> two-year


Treatment and Teach<strong>in</strong>g 131multisensory structured study <strong>in</strong> Spanish phonology/orthographyexerted a positive <strong>in</strong>fluence on <strong>the</strong> native language abilities and foreignlanguage aptitude of <strong>the</strong> at-risk learners. The outcome of <strong>the</strong> researchsuggests that both <strong>the</strong> at-risk and not-at-risk students achieved decentga<strong>in</strong>s <strong>in</strong> foreign language aptitude over one year of foreign languageteach<strong>in</strong>g. Moreover, <strong>the</strong> at-risk learners managed to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir ga<strong>in</strong>sover <strong>the</strong> second year of direct multisensory structured <strong>in</strong>struction,however, <strong>the</strong>y were unable to <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>the</strong>m, unlike <strong>the</strong> not-at-risklearners. What follows is that <strong>the</strong> multisensory structured languagecoach<strong>in</strong>g aga<strong>in</strong> proves particularly salient and efficient for <strong>the</strong> at-risk<strong>in</strong>dividuals; never<strong>the</strong>less, despite explicit tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>y repeatedly tendto fall beh<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong> not-at-risk students with respect to phonological/orthographic competence.The benefits of MSL <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> Spanish as a foreign language werealso exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> ano<strong>the</strong>r study by Sparks et al. (1998c). Four groups ofhigh school at-risk and not-at-risk students participated <strong>in</strong> this study. Theat-risk students were assigned to three groups: MSL multisensorySpanish tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> self-conta<strong>in</strong>ed classrooms, SC traditional teach<strong>in</strong>gof Spanish, provided <strong>in</strong> self-conta<strong>in</strong>ed classrooms, and NSC traditionaltutor<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Spanish, <strong>in</strong> regular (not self-conta<strong>in</strong>ed) classes. The at-riskgroups varied <strong>in</strong> terms of both <strong>the</strong> type of foreign language classroom(self-conta<strong>in</strong>ed versus not self-conta<strong>in</strong>ed) and <strong>the</strong> type of foreignlanguage <strong>in</strong>struction (MSL versus traditional with <strong>in</strong>structional accommodations).Not-at-risk students constituted <strong>the</strong> fourth group (NAR) andreceived traditional lessons <strong>in</strong> Spanish <strong>in</strong> regular classes, similar to <strong>the</strong><strong>in</strong>struction provided to <strong>the</strong> NSC group. The three different at-risk groupswere compared on native language, foreign language aptitude, andforeign language oral and written proficiency measures. The performanceof <strong>the</strong> not-at-risk group on <strong>the</strong> same measures was compared to<strong>the</strong> three at-risk groups. In light of <strong>the</strong> previously mentioned researchf<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, it is unsurpris<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>the</strong> MSL group evidenced significantga<strong>in</strong>s <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> foreign language aptitude as measured by <strong>the</strong> MLAT, while<strong>the</strong> SC and NSC groups did not. Thus, <strong>the</strong> proposition of <strong>the</strong> superiorityof MSL for at-risk students over <strong>the</strong> traditional textbook-based <strong>in</strong>structionga<strong>in</strong>ed clear support. Similarly, <strong>the</strong> NAR group manifested vitalga<strong>in</strong>s on <strong>the</strong> MLAT. Aga<strong>in</strong>, consistently with <strong>the</strong> outcome of o<strong>the</strong>rstudies, this time, even though <strong>the</strong> MSL group improved <strong>the</strong>ir scores on<strong>the</strong> MLAT, <strong>the</strong>y aga<strong>in</strong> appeared to be unable to keep pace with <strong>the</strong> NARgroup. It needs stress<strong>in</strong>g that all four groups demonstrated noticeableachievements over time on some native language skills. Still, expectedly,<strong>the</strong> scores of <strong>the</strong> at-risk foreign language learners on <strong>the</strong>se measuresusually fell beh<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong> scores of <strong>the</strong> not-at-risk students. The MSLand NAR groups obta<strong>in</strong>ed substantially greater ga<strong>in</strong>s on <strong>the</strong> foreignlanguage aptitude and native language tests (read<strong>in</strong>g comprehension,


132 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>word recognition and pseudo-word read<strong>in</strong>g). In addition, <strong>the</strong>y scoredsignificantly higher on <strong>the</strong> measure of oral and written foreign languageproficiency than SC and NSC groups; <strong>in</strong>trigu<strong>in</strong>gly, no differences relat<strong>in</strong>gto this particular measure were <strong>in</strong>dicated between <strong>the</strong> MSL and NARgroups. Seem<strong>in</strong>gly, such a f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g constitutes a strong piece of evidencefor <strong>the</strong> efficacy of <strong>the</strong> MSL <strong>in</strong>struction for teach<strong>in</strong>g a foreign language toat-risk foreign language learners. All <strong>in</strong> all, even though <strong>the</strong> at-riskstudents from <strong>the</strong> SC and NSC groups achieved certa<strong>in</strong> levels of foreignlanguage proficiency, which eventually allowed <strong>the</strong>m to pass <strong>the</strong> foreignlanguage courses, <strong>the</strong> learners who received MSL <strong>in</strong>struction appeared tobe more competent <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> foreign language than SC and NSC students.Thus, draw<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong> abovementioned research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, a conclusioncan be put forward that, apparently, at-risk foreign language learners arecapable of acquir<strong>in</strong>g specific levels of foreign language capacity and thatit is most effectively realised when MSL techniques are employed. Theapplication of <strong>the</strong> MSL techniques comparatively frequently enablesforeign language at-risk students to atta<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> levels of foreign languageability commensurate with those reached by <strong>the</strong> not-at-risk foreignlanguage learners.Descriptive and empirical studies perta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> effectiveness of <strong>the</strong>MSL approach <strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g a foreign language to at-risk foreign languagelearners have also been reported <strong>in</strong> languages o<strong>the</strong>r than Spanish. Sparkset al. (1996) report an attempt <strong>the</strong>y undertook to determ<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong> effect of<strong>the</strong> study of Lat<strong>in</strong> by means of MSL <strong>in</strong>struction on <strong>the</strong> native languageskills and foreign language aptitude of LD students. Three groups offoreign language learners were selected for <strong>the</strong> comparisons: non-LDlearners who were taught Lat<strong>in</strong> with <strong>the</strong> use of traditional methodology(NLD group), LD students who received MSL tutor<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>(LD-MSL group), and LD <strong>in</strong>dividuals who experienced a study of Lat<strong>in</strong>via traditional method (LD-NO/MSL group). Ostensibly, <strong>the</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs ofthis study generally support <strong>the</strong> conclusions of <strong>the</strong> previously citedresearch on students learn<strong>in</strong>g Spanish as a foreign language. Asexpected, <strong>the</strong> tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong> enhanced <strong>the</strong> scores on one or more of<strong>the</strong> native language phonological measures and foreign languageaptitude test of <strong>the</strong> NLD and LD/MSL groups, but not <strong>the</strong> LD/NO-MSL group. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, <strong>in</strong> spite of <strong>the</strong> fact that <strong>the</strong> LD-MSL groupshowed substantial improvements on some of <strong>the</strong> phonological/orthographicmeasures, <strong>the</strong>y kept scor<strong>in</strong>g well below <strong>the</strong> NLD students, who,<strong>in</strong> addition, were superior to <strong>the</strong> LD groups as regards <strong>the</strong> foreignlanguage aptitude test. To sum up, <strong>the</strong> at-risk foreign language learners’native language faculty and foreign language aptitude can improve withforeign language study. Unfortunately, <strong>the</strong>y are unlikely to be comparableto <strong>the</strong> language skills of <strong>the</strong> not-at-risk foreign language learners.


Treatment and Teach<strong>in</strong>g 133Schneider (1999) successfully adapted <strong>the</strong> MSL approach to teachGerman as a foreign language by expand<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> native language MSLpr<strong>in</strong>ciples to <strong>the</strong> tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> foreign language phonology/orthography,grammar and vocabulary/morphology. The author referred to heradaptations of <strong>the</strong> MSL as MSML, which stands for multisensorystructured metacognitive <strong>in</strong>struction, because she explicitly addressed<strong>the</strong> development of metacognitive process<strong>in</strong>g skills. Similarly, Downeyet al. (2000) demonstrated that Lat<strong>in</strong> classes conducted <strong>in</strong> accordancewith <strong>in</strong>structional modifications <strong>in</strong>troduced <strong>in</strong> order to cater to <strong>the</strong> needsof college students with dyslexia proved successful.The f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of <strong>the</strong> studies described above yield several implicationsfor teach<strong>in</strong>g foreign languages to students with dyslexia. They support<strong>the</strong> effectiveness of direct multisensory <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> phonology/orthography of foreign languages. As already mentioned, an accumulat<strong>in</strong>gbody of evidence <strong>in</strong>dicates that difficulties, whe<strong>the</strong>r subtle or overt,experienced by a great majority of poor foreign language learners are ofphonological nature (Sparks & Ganschow, 1993). Thus, learners withdyslexia, who hardly benefit from methodologies that force <strong>the</strong>m to<strong>in</strong>tuitively discover <strong>the</strong> phonological structure of a new language, willmost probably take advantage of <strong>the</strong> direct <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> phonologyof <strong>the</strong> target language.The reluctance of foreign language educators to accept <strong>the</strong> importanceof explicit multisensory <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> phonology and grammar is,presumably, a natural consequence of <strong>the</strong>ir be<strong>in</strong>g good language learners<strong>the</strong>mselves (Sparks et al., 1991). Arguably, more attention should be givento acqua<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g pre-service and <strong>in</strong>-service teachers of foreign languageswith <strong>the</strong> constitutional nature of dyslexic difficulties <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> first place, aswell as with <strong>the</strong> effective teach<strong>in</strong>g methods and <strong>the</strong> techniques that couldbe employed while work<strong>in</strong>g with students with dyslexia.To fur<strong>the</strong>r benefit students with dyslexia, <strong>the</strong>re should be morecollaboration between special educators and foreign language teachers.S<strong>in</strong>ce native and foreign language skills are <strong>in</strong>terrelated, consequently,weak native language skills, especially phonological, have a naturalimpact on FLL. Direct multisensory <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> a foreign language notonly substantially improves FLL, but also has a potential to <strong>in</strong>creasenative language phonology (Ganschow & Sparks, 1995; Sparks et al.,1992b). Moreover, <strong>the</strong> improvement <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> performance on <strong>the</strong> soundidentification and manipulation tasks, and <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> awareness of <strong>the</strong>spell<strong>in</strong>g choices of students <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir native language may have a positiveeffect on <strong>the</strong>ir foreign language skills it has been shown that studentswith stronger native language read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g skills achieve highergrades <strong>in</strong> foreign language courses and are more verbally proficient <strong>in</strong> aforeign language (Sparks et al., 1995).


134 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Small-scale <strong>in</strong>tervention study <strong>in</strong> dyslexia andforeign language learn<strong>in</strong>gIntroduction and methodThis subsection presents <strong>the</strong> outcome of research conducted <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>field of teach<strong>in</strong>g English as a foreign language to Polish students withdyslexia. The study concerned <strong>the</strong> effectiveness of <strong>the</strong> direct multisensory<strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> improv<strong>in</strong>g word read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong>English, through <strong>the</strong> systematic study of selected grapheme-phonemerelations, spell<strong>in</strong>g patterns and rules. 3 Three groups of learners participated<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> study, one experimental and two control groups. Progresswas measured by a set of read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g pre- and post-tests.With<strong>in</strong>- and between-group comparisons concern<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> scores on pretestsand post-tests were used. After six months of tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, significantimprovement <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> ability to relate phonemes to graphemes was shown<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimental group, which outperformed non-dyslexics. Eventhough <strong>the</strong> results are very promis<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>the</strong>y must be treated with cautiondue to <strong>the</strong> small number of participants and absence of <strong>the</strong> comparisontreatment.In languages with deep orthographies, like English, <strong>the</strong> graphemephonemerelations are especially difficult to learn. The follow<strong>in</strong>gcharacteristics of <strong>the</strong>se relations seem particularly troublesome, first,<strong>the</strong> capacity of a s<strong>in</strong>gle sound to be graphically represented by more thanone letter; second, <strong>the</strong> fact that a s<strong>in</strong>gle sound may be represented bydifferent letters or letter comb<strong>in</strong>ations <strong>in</strong> different words (<strong>in</strong>consistentspell<strong>in</strong>g); and third, <strong>the</strong> fact that a given letter or a comb<strong>in</strong>ation of lettersmay represent more than one sound and that <strong>the</strong>re is a great number ofexceptions and irregular words.To elucidate <strong>the</strong> operation of <strong>the</strong> MSL, and, specifically, to show <strong>the</strong>rationale for <strong>the</strong> explicit teach<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> grapheme-phoneme conversionrules, three groups of English language learners were selected. Onegroup, def<strong>in</strong>ed as <strong>the</strong> experimental, consisted of five <strong>in</strong>dividuals withdyslexia. The rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g two groups were labelled <strong>the</strong> control groups: <strong>the</strong>first was made up of 10 <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia, and <strong>the</strong> second wascomposed of 10 students without dyslexia.The study took place <strong>in</strong> a secondary school <strong>in</strong> one of <strong>the</strong> major cities <strong>in</strong>Poland, where 15 students were randomly chosen from a dyslexic groupof 100 <strong>the</strong> number amount<strong>in</strong>g to about 12% of <strong>the</strong> school population and assigned to two groups. Five students (two males, three females,mean age 17.6) formed <strong>the</strong> experimental group and ten students (eightmales, two females, mean age 17) formed one of <strong>the</strong> control groups. In afur<strong>the</strong>r selection, ten students (five males, five females, mean age 17)from <strong>the</strong> non-dyslexic part of <strong>the</strong> school population were assigned to <strong>the</strong>second control group. All <strong>the</strong> students from <strong>the</strong> experimental group and


Treatment and Teach<strong>in</strong>g 135<strong>the</strong> control group with dyslexia had an up-to-date assessment, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>gspecific difficulties <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and write. They had clearly nongenerallearn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties, but limited and specific difficulties <strong>in</strong>learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and spell. Their development was normal, as was <strong>the</strong>ir<strong>in</strong>telligence, which ranked average or above average. There were noemotional or personality disorders, nor any hear<strong>in</strong>g or sight deficits.F<strong>in</strong>ally, no environmental or educational negligence was observed. Allstudents with dyslexia were receiv<strong>in</strong>g compensatory teach<strong>in</strong>g services <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong>ir native language, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> form of self-conta<strong>in</strong>ed classes, run by <strong>the</strong>school teacher-<strong>the</strong>rapist. However, no compensatory teach<strong>in</strong>g to m<strong>in</strong>imiseforeign language difficulties was officially granted. All three groupswere enrolled <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir regular English classes at school, conducted with<strong>the</strong> use of traditional foreign language methodology by <strong>the</strong> same teacher,who refra<strong>in</strong>ed from devot<strong>in</strong>g time to explicit presentation of phonemegraphemeconversion rules. Students were expected to <strong>in</strong>fer <strong>the</strong>serelations naturally and spontaneously from <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>put <strong>the</strong>y received <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong> language classroom. The author provided tutorial MSL sessions for<strong>the</strong> experimental group, whereas <strong>the</strong> control group with dyslexia and <strong>the</strong>control group without dyslexia attended no such classes. No tutor<strong>in</strong>gsessions with <strong>the</strong> application of any comparison approach (alternative toMSL) were <strong>in</strong>troduced <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> study.As far as <strong>the</strong>ir ability to read and spell <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> native language isconcerned, no additional tests were given to <strong>the</strong> participants withdyslexia apart from <strong>the</strong> formal assessment of <strong>the</strong>se skills that <strong>the</strong>y hadundergone <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> pedagogical-psychological dispensary. Interviews with<strong>the</strong> teacher of Polish and <strong>the</strong> teacher-<strong>the</strong>rapist, who run <strong>the</strong> correctioncompensationclasses, were conducted <strong>in</strong> order to collect qualitative dataon <strong>the</strong> estimated level of <strong>the</strong> participants’ skills <strong>in</strong> Polish. The dyslexics’native language skills were described as considerably poorer than <strong>the</strong>skills of <strong>the</strong> non-dyslexic participants of <strong>the</strong> study, <strong>in</strong> most cases lowerthan satisfactory.Prior to <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>troduction of tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimental group, skills<strong>in</strong> English as a foreign language of all <strong>the</strong> students participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>study were evaluated by <strong>the</strong>ir teacher of English on <strong>the</strong> 6-po<strong>in</strong>t scale,where 1 denoted unsatisfactory, while 6 stood for excellent. Additionally,all <strong>the</strong> students were asked for self-assessment, apply<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> samecriteria. The follow<strong>in</strong>g aspects were assessed: read<strong>in</strong>g aloud, read<strong>in</strong>gsilently with comprehension, listen<strong>in</strong>g comprehension, speak<strong>in</strong>g, writtenassignments, pronunciation, vocabulary, spell<strong>in</strong>g and grammar. Studentswith dyslexia were assessed by <strong>the</strong>ir teacher of English as very poorlearners of English, usually represent<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> level lower than satisfactory.Students with dyslexia perceived <strong>the</strong>mselves as poor foreign languagelearners as well. Generally, learners with dyslexia were assessed by <strong>the</strong>


136 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>teacher of English as much poorer learners of English than <strong>the</strong>irnon-dyslexic peers participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> study.The study sought answers to <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g research questions: DoPolish learners with dyslexia experience greater difficulties <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong> English phonological-orthographic systems than <strong>the</strong>ir non-dyslexicpeers? Does direct, multisensory and structured <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> selectedgrapheme-phoneme correspondences and spell<strong>in</strong>g rules improve dyslexiclearners’ ability to decode and encode <strong>the</strong> language? The proposedhypo<strong>the</strong>ses for <strong>the</strong> present study assumed that:(1) Polish learners with dyslexia experienced greater difficulties <strong>in</strong>learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> English phonological-orthographic system with <strong>the</strong> useof traditional <strong>in</strong>struction than <strong>the</strong>ir non-dyslexic peers.(2) Direct, multisensory and structured <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> selected Englishgrapheme-phoneme correspondences and spell<strong>in</strong>g rules wouldimprove dyslexic learners’ ability to decode and encode <strong>the</strong>language (read and spell).(3) Pre- and post-test comparisons <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimental group wouldreveal significant differences between <strong>the</strong> results of pre- and posttests,<strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g a considerable ga<strong>in</strong>.(4) Pre- and post-test comparisons <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> control group with dyslexiawould not reveal significant differences between <strong>the</strong> results of preandpost-tests, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g no considerable ga<strong>in</strong>.(5) Pre- and post-test comparisons <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> control group withoutdyslexia would reveal significant differences between <strong>the</strong> resultsof pre- and post-tests, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g a considerable ga<strong>in</strong>.(6) There would not be significant differences between <strong>the</strong> results of<strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g pre-tests <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimental group and <strong>the</strong>control group with dyslexia. Significant differences between <strong>the</strong>results of <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g post-tests <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimentalgroup and <strong>the</strong> control group with dyslexia would be found. Theexperimental group would show greater pre- and post-test ga<strong>in</strong>sthan <strong>the</strong> control group with dyslexia.(7) Significant differences between <strong>the</strong> results of <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g andread<strong>in</strong>g pre- and post-tests <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimental group and <strong>the</strong>control group without dyslexia would be found. Despite considerableprogress and ga<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> knowledge of English graphemephonemecorrespondences, <strong>the</strong> experimental group would still lagslightly beh<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong> control group without dyslexia or, <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rwords, would not show greater pre- or post-test ga<strong>in</strong>s.(8) Significant differences between <strong>the</strong> results of <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g andread<strong>in</strong>g pre- and post-tests <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> control group with dyslexia and<strong>the</strong> control group without dyslexia would be found. The control


Treatment and Teach<strong>in</strong>g 137group without dyslexia would show greater pre- and post-test ga<strong>in</strong>sthan <strong>the</strong> control group with dyslexia.In order to verify <strong>the</strong> proposed hypo<strong>the</strong>ses, <strong>the</strong> research wasconducted with <strong>the</strong> use of <strong>the</strong> experimental procedure, aim<strong>in</strong>g toproduce an observable ga<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimental group, stemm<strong>in</strong>g from<strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>troduction of an <strong>in</strong>novative factor, responsible for improvement oflearn<strong>in</strong>g and teach<strong>in</strong>g (Komorowska, 1982). The <strong>in</strong>dependent variable <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong> present study is <strong>the</strong> direct multisensory language <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>English phonological/orthographic system. The dependent variable ismade up of <strong>the</strong> results of teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> English grapheme/phonemecorrespondence us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> MSL approach. Thus, <strong>the</strong> dependent variableis operationalised as <strong>the</strong> test scores after tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimentalgroup.The design of <strong>the</strong> study departs slightly from <strong>the</strong> experimental schemeof <strong>the</strong> classic type, which normally <strong>in</strong>volves a pre-test, a post-test and acontrol group (Komorowska, 1982). The present research employed twodifferent control groups and two post-tests, as shown <strong>in</strong> Table 5.1.In Table 5.1, PRE-S denotes a spell<strong>in</strong>g pre-test, PRE-R read<strong>in</strong>g pretest,POST-S1 first spell<strong>in</strong>g post-test and POST-R1 first read<strong>in</strong>g posttestadm<strong>in</strong>istered immediately after <strong>the</strong> treatment and, f<strong>in</strong>ally, POST-S2stands for <strong>the</strong> second spell<strong>in</strong>g post-test and POST-R2 for <strong>the</strong> secondread<strong>in</strong>g post-test, adm<strong>in</strong>istered two weeks after <strong>the</strong> treatment.The pre-test (PRE-S and PRE-R) and <strong>the</strong> post-test 1 (POST-S1 andPOST-R1), as well as <strong>the</strong> post-test 2 (POST-S2 and POST-R2), consistedof a word level read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g task. The pre-test and post-test 1comprised exactly <strong>the</strong> same words, whereas <strong>the</strong> post-test 2 featured adifferent list (see Appendix 1). The words <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> pre- and <strong>the</strong>post-tests were carefully selected to conta<strong>in</strong> all <strong>the</strong> phoneme/graphemeregularities and spell<strong>in</strong>g choices covered <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> programme (seeAppendix 2). Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, <strong>the</strong> number of words with <strong>the</strong> same spell<strong>in</strong>gpatterns was equal across <strong>the</strong> tests.Before <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>troduction of <strong>the</strong> experimental factor <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimentalgroup, spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g pre-tests were adm<strong>in</strong>istered to all threeTable 5.1 The design of <strong>the</strong> studyExperimentalgroup0 PRE-S,PRE-R0 Treatment 0 POST-S1,POST-R10 POST-S2,POST-R2Control groupwith dyslexia0 PRE-S,PRE-R0 0 POST-S1,POST-R10 POST-S2,POST-R2Control groupwithout dyslexia0 PRE-S,PRE-R0 0 POST-S1,POST-R10 POST-S2,POST-R2


138 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>groups. Each group was tested separately: <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g test had a writtenform and was conducted with <strong>the</strong> whole group. The read<strong>in</strong>g test wasadm<strong>in</strong>istered <strong>in</strong>dividually and students were recorded. A list of 63words was first dictated to <strong>the</strong> students; <strong>the</strong>n, after a break, a record<strong>in</strong>gwas made of <strong>the</strong> students read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> same list of words. Each word wasdictated <strong>in</strong> isolation twice, <strong>in</strong>terspers<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> dictation with a s<strong>in</strong>gleread<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> word <strong>in</strong> a sentence. The students were to write down<strong>in</strong>dividual words onto <strong>the</strong> form provided. There was no differencebetween <strong>the</strong> groups regard<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> time allotted for test<strong>in</strong>g, and no timelimits were set. A special education teacher, who works at <strong>the</strong> schoolwhere <strong>the</strong> study took place, assisted <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istration of <strong>the</strong> pre-test.In <strong>the</strong> assessment of <strong>the</strong> students’ performance, only <strong>the</strong> words that werespelled and read correctly were accepted. One po<strong>in</strong>t was given for eachcorrectly spelt or read word. The handwrit<strong>in</strong>g of all <strong>the</strong> studentsparticipat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> study was <strong>in</strong>telligible enough, so <strong>the</strong>re were nodubious cases. The students scored a po<strong>in</strong>t for every read word as long as<strong>the</strong>y could make <strong>the</strong>mselves understood. Even though 63 words makeup a long list, test<strong>in</strong>g did not seem to tire <strong>the</strong> students, on <strong>the</strong> contrary,<strong>the</strong>y were eager to co-operate all through.In <strong>the</strong> next stage of <strong>the</strong> research, <strong>the</strong> experimental group was exposedto <strong>the</strong> operation of <strong>the</strong> experimental variable (MSL <strong>in</strong>struction), whichwas not applied to <strong>the</strong> control groups. The classes for <strong>the</strong> experimentalgroup were conducted by <strong>the</strong> author herself. They were all 90-m<strong>in</strong>utecontact sessions, which took place once a week, over a period of sixmonths. Dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> treatment <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimental group, direct multisensoryteach<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> phoneme/grapheme relations and spell<strong>in</strong>g rules<strong>in</strong> English was conducted <strong>in</strong> a structured and step-by-step fashion. Thespecific problem areas covered dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g were <strong>the</strong> same as <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong> pre- and post-tests. The <strong>in</strong>structional style followed <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>ciples of<strong>the</strong> direct multisensory structured <strong>in</strong>struction, described earlier <strong>in</strong> thischapter. Sample tasks, teach<strong>in</strong>g aids and worksheets used <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>programme can be found <strong>in</strong> Chapter 6.Immediately after <strong>the</strong> treatment had ended, <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g post-test 1and <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g post-test 1 were adm<strong>in</strong>istered to all three groups. Twoweeks later, <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g post-test 2 and <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g post-test 2 wereadm<strong>in</strong>istered to all participants. Analogous procedure to that useddur<strong>in</strong>g pre-test adm<strong>in</strong>istration was applied.The current data analysis consists of <strong>the</strong> pre- and post-test comparisonsboth with<strong>in</strong> and between <strong>the</strong> groups. The pre- and post-testscomparisons for each group were generated by determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> meansand standard deviations. The differences between <strong>the</strong> pre- and post-tests’means were analysed us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Wilcoxon Test. The pre- and post-testscomparisons between groups were generated by determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> meansand standard deviations. The MannWhitney U-test was used to


Treatment and Teach<strong>in</strong>g 139Table 5.2 Results of <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g pre- and post-tests <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>experimental group, <strong>the</strong> control group with dyslexia and <strong>the</strong> control groupwithout dyslexiaTestExperimentalgroupControl groupwith dyslexiaControl groupwithout dyslexiaMean% SD Mean% SD Mean% SDPRE-S 27.62 6.47 24.60 8.06 38.41 8.13PRE-R 57.46 7.16 59.21 7.83 76.51 6.73POST-S1 71.74 13.54 26.19 8.10 46.35 9.54POST-S2 87.62 6.76 62.38 5.85 79.37 6.31POST-R1 82.86 7.85 27.78 8.58 48.73 11.62POST-R2 83.81 3.56 57.46 8.97 74.76 5.76determ<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong> possible existence of any significant differences between<strong>the</strong> experimental group, <strong>the</strong> control group with dyslexia and <strong>the</strong> controlgroup without dyslexia (Jóźwiak & Podgórski, 1994; Luszniewicz &Slaby, 2001; Stanisz, 1998).Table 5.2 gives <strong>the</strong> results of all <strong>the</strong> tests <strong>in</strong> all three groups. Thepre-test results were low <strong>in</strong> all three groups, but it can also be seen that<strong>the</strong> non-dyslexic group performed better, <strong>in</strong> both spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>gtasks, than <strong>the</strong>ir dyslexic peers. It is also observable that spell<strong>in</strong>g posedmore severe difficulties for learners with dyslexia than read<strong>in</strong>g:<strong>the</strong> experimental and <strong>the</strong> dyslexic control group achieved 27.62 and24.60%, respectively on spell<strong>in</strong>g, but approximately 60% each on read<strong>in</strong>g.Figures 5.1 and 5.2 illustrate <strong>the</strong> results of <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g preandpost-tests <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimental group, <strong>the</strong> control group with dyslexiaand <strong>the</strong> control group without dyslexia, respectively.Pre- and post-test comparisons with<strong>in</strong> and between groupsIn <strong>the</strong> experimental group, <strong>the</strong> POST-R1 resulted <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> highest meanscore (87.62%). The low mean score of <strong>the</strong> PRE-S (27.62%) and <strong>the</strong>relatively high mean scores of <strong>the</strong> POST-S1 (71.74%) and <strong>the</strong> POST-S2(82.86%) <strong>in</strong>dicated an improvement <strong>in</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimentalgroup. Analogously, <strong>the</strong> low mean score of <strong>the</strong> PRE-R (57.46%) and <strong>the</strong>relatively high mean scores of <strong>the</strong> POST-R1 (87.62%) and POST-R2(83.81%) marked an improvement <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimentalgroup. Statistically significant differences emerged between <strong>the</strong> results of<strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g pairs of variables: PRE-S and POST-S1, PRE-S and POST-S2,PRE-R and POST-R1, as well as PRE-R and POST-R2 (<strong>in</strong> all cases T0.0,p0.04). Conversely, no significant differences were found between <strong>the</strong>


140 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>90 82,868071,7470Test score <strong>in</strong> %605040302027,6224,6 26,19 27,7838,4146,3530,7PRE-SPOST-S1POST-S2100Experimental groupControl group withdyslexiaControl groupwithout dyslexiaFigure 5.1 Results of <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g pre- and post-tests <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimentalgroup, <strong>the</strong> control group with dyslexia and <strong>the</strong> control group without dyslexia908087,6283,8176,5179,3774,76Test score <strong>in</strong> %706050403057,4659,2162,3857,46PRE-RPOST-R1POST-R220100Experimental groupControl group withdyslexiaControl groupwithout dyslexiaFigure 5.2 Results of <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g pre- and post-tests <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimentalgroup, <strong>the</strong> control group with dyslexia and <strong>the</strong> control group without dyslexiaresults of <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g pairs of tests: POST-S1 and POST-S2 (T0.0,p0.07), POST-R1 and POST-R2 (T2.0, p0.14).Students <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> control group with dyslexia achieved <strong>the</strong> highest meanscore on <strong>the</strong> POST-R1 (62.38%) and <strong>the</strong> lowest on <strong>the</strong> PRE-S (24.60%). Themean scores of all <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g tests PRE-S (24.60%), POST-S1 (26.19%)and POST-S2 (27.78%) had similar values, which <strong>in</strong>dicated no improvement<strong>in</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> control group with dyslexia. The mean scores


Treatment and Teach<strong>in</strong>g 141of all <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g tests PRE-R (59.21%), POST-R1 (62.38%) and POST-R2(57.46%) had similar values, which aga<strong>in</strong> shows no improvement <strong>in</strong>read<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> control group with dyslexia. In contrast to what wasobserved <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimental group, <strong>the</strong>re were no statistically significantdifferences between <strong>the</strong> results of <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g pairs of tests <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> controlgroup with dyslexia: PRE-S and POST-S1 (T11.5, p0.36), PRE-S andPOST-S2 (T9.0, p0.21), PRE-R and POST-R1 (T12.0, p0.21), aswell as PRE-R and POST-R2 (T13.0, p0.26).In <strong>the</strong> control group without dyslexia, <strong>the</strong> highest mean score wasyielded by <strong>the</strong> POST-R1 (79.37%), while <strong>the</strong> lowest by <strong>the</strong> PRE-S(38.41%). The mean score of <strong>the</strong> PRE-S (38.41%) was lower than <strong>the</strong>mean scores of <strong>the</strong> POST-S1 (46.35%) and <strong>the</strong> POST-S2 (48.73%), whichmight <strong>in</strong>dicate some enhancement of <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> controlgroup without dyslexia. Even though <strong>the</strong> differences were not asconsiderable as <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimental group, <strong>the</strong>y were still significant <strong>in</strong>statistical terms. The mean scores of all <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g tests PRE-R(76.51%), POST-R1 (79.37%) and POST-R2 (74.76%) had similar values,which showed that <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> control group withoutdyslexia underwent no improvement. There were statistically significantdifferences between <strong>the</strong> results of <strong>the</strong> PRE-S and POST-S1 (T0.0,p0.007), as well as <strong>the</strong> PRE-S and POST-S2 (T1.0, p0.01).No statistically significant differences between <strong>the</strong> PRE-R and POST-R1(T5.0, p0.07) or between <strong>the</strong> PRE-R and POST-R2 (T11.0, p0.33)emerged <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> control group without dyslexia.The results of <strong>the</strong> PRE-S (U23.5, p0.85) and PRE-R (U20.0,p0.54) <strong>in</strong>dicated no differences between <strong>the</strong> experimental group and<strong>the</strong> control group with dyslexia. By contrast, statistically significantdifferences were observed between <strong>the</strong> results of all post-tests, with <strong>the</strong>experimental group outperform<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> control group with dyslexia(POST-S1 (U2.0, p0.005), POST-S2 (U0.0, p0.002), POST-R1(U2.5, p0.006) and POST-R2 (U1.0, p0.003)).As regards <strong>the</strong> comparison between <strong>the</strong> experimental and <strong>the</strong> controlgroup without dyslexia, significant differences were yielded on threemeasures: PRE-R (U4.0, p0.01) <strong>in</strong> favour of <strong>the</strong> control groupwithout dyslexia, POST-S1 (U8.0, p0.04) and POST-S2 (U3.5,p0.008) both <strong>in</strong> favour of <strong>the</strong> experimental group. The observabledifferences between <strong>the</strong> means of <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r tests were small and ofno statistical significance: PRE-S (U18.0, p0.39), POST-R1 (U11.0,p0.09), POST-R2 (U9.5, p0.06).As anticipated, <strong>the</strong> control group without dyslexia performed betteron all measures than <strong>the</strong> control group with dyslexia. The differenceswere statistically significant between <strong>the</strong> results of <strong>the</strong> PRE-R (U12.5,p0.005), POST-S1 (U15.0, p0.008), POST-S2 (U18.0, p0.015),POST-R1 (U10.5, p0.003) and POST-R2 (U18.5, p0.02).


142 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>However, <strong>the</strong>re were no statistically significant differences between <strong>the</strong>setwo groups as far as <strong>the</strong> result of PRE-S (U27.5, p0.09) is concerned.Discussion of resultsPolish learners with dyslexia experience greater problems <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong> English phonological-orthographic system than <strong>the</strong>ir non-dyslexicpeers, as was revealed by <strong>the</strong> poorer results of <strong>the</strong> word spell<strong>in</strong>g andread<strong>in</strong>g pre-tests <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimental group and <strong>the</strong> control group withdyslexia <strong>in</strong> comparison to <strong>the</strong> control group without dyslexia. This hasbeen fur<strong>the</strong>r confirmed by <strong>the</strong> poor achievement <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g post-testof <strong>the</strong> dyslexic control group (Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis 1).Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis 3 was also supported as <strong>the</strong> pre- and post-test comparisonswith<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimental group revealed significant differencesbetween <strong>the</strong> results of <strong>the</strong> pre- and post-tests, which <strong>in</strong>dicated aconsiderable ga<strong>in</strong>. A very low mean score of <strong>the</strong> PRE-S (27.62%) andrelatively high mean scores of <strong>the</strong> POST-S1 (71.74%) and POST-S2(82.86%) <strong>in</strong>dicated an improvement of spell<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimentalgroup. A similar situation was found for <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g tasks a low meanscore of <strong>the</strong> PRE-R (57.46%) and relatively high mean scores of <strong>the</strong> POST-R1 (87.62%) and POST-R2 (83.81%) meant enhanced read<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>experimental group. Moreover, <strong>the</strong> above differences were all statisticallysignificant. Thus, <strong>the</strong> experimental group performed significantly betteron completion of <strong>the</strong> tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g than at <strong>the</strong> onset of <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>structionalprogramme. Hence, <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>creased efficiency of <strong>the</strong> students <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>experimental group on <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g tasks after <strong>the</strong> tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g,as compared to <strong>the</strong>ir scores before, can be attributed predom<strong>in</strong>antly to<strong>the</strong> MSL <strong>in</strong>tervention ra<strong>the</strong>r than to o<strong>the</strong>r factors (Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis 2).The above assertion is streng<strong>the</strong>ned by <strong>the</strong> fact that no significantdifferences between <strong>the</strong> mean scores on <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g pre- andpost-tests were found <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> control group with dyslexia. Such f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>ghad been anticipated <strong>in</strong> view of <strong>the</strong> absence of MSL <strong>in</strong>struction. Themean scores of all <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g tests had similar values, which <strong>in</strong>dicatedno improvement <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong> this group. Likewise, <strong>the</strong> valuesof <strong>the</strong> mean scores of all <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g tests were similar, which, aga<strong>in</strong>,<strong>in</strong>dicated no improvement. F<strong>in</strong>ally, no statistically significant differencesemerged between <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g pairs of variables: PRE-S and POST-S1,PRE-S and POST-S2, PRE-R and POST-R1, as well as PRE-R and POST-R2. Thus, it can be concluded that, despite <strong>the</strong> traditional <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>the</strong>group received at school, <strong>the</strong>y did not make any progress <strong>in</strong> terms of<strong>the</strong>ir word spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g skills (Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis 4).In <strong>the</strong> control group without dyslexia, statistically significant differenceswere found between <strong>the</strong> results of <strong>the</strong> PRE-S and <strong>the</strong> POST-S1, aswell as <strong>the</strong> PRE-S and <strong>the</strong> POST-S2, which <strong>in</strong>dicates some improvement<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong> this group. However, one should not lose sight of


Treatment and Teach<strong>in</strong>g 143<strong>the</strong> fact that <strong>the</strong>y were, from <strong>the</strong> start, <strong>in</strong> a much more beneficial positionthan <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r groups, s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>the</strong>y demonstrated no specific developmentalread<strong>in</strong>g or spell<strong>in</strong>g disorders. It could <strong>the</strong>n be assumed that <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong>ir case <strong>the</strong> traditional <strong>in</strong>struction was successful. Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, <strong>the</strong>mean scores on all <strong>the</strong>ir read<strong>in</strong>g tests had similar values, with nostatistically significant differences, which <strong>in</strong>dicated no improvement <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g skills of this group after six months of regular <strong>in</strong>struction.The control group without dyslexia did not achieve any progress <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>irword read<strong>in</strong>g skills. Hence, Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis 5 that <strong>the</strong> pre- and post-testcomparisons <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> control group without dyslexia would revealsignificant differences between <strong>the</strong> results of <strong>the</strong> pre- and post-testswas only partially supported.The results of <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g pre-tests were poor <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> caseof <strong>the</strong> experimental group and <strong>the</strong> control group with dyslexia, and <strong>the</strong>scores did not differ significantly between <strong>the</strong>se two groups. By contrast,Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis 6, stat<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>the</strong>re would be significant differences between<strong>the</strong> results of <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g post-tests, received clearconfirmation, as did <strong>the</strong> comparative postulate that <strong>the</strong> experimentalgroup would show greater pre-and post-test ga<strong>in</strong>s than <strong>the</strong> control groupwith dyslexia. Both groups received traditional <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> English as aforeign language dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> experiment, but <strong>the</strong> control group withdyslexia, unlike <strong>the</strong> experimental group, did not undergo MSL tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g,which could be <strong>the</strong> reason why <strong>the</strong>y did not make any progress on <strong>the</strong>spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g post-test. Apparently, <strong>the</strong> experimental groupdemonstrated enormous progress <strong>in</strong> acquir<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> relationships between<strong>the</strong> graphemes and phonemes and, consequently, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> word read<strong>in</strong>gand spell<strong>in</strong>g (Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis 2). The role of <strong>the</strong> direct multisensory<strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> success of <strong>the</strong> experimental group cannot thus beoverestimated.Surpris<strong>in</strong>gly, Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis 7, stat<strong>in</strong>g that significant differences wouldoccur between <strong>the</strong> results of <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g pre- and post-tests<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimental group and control group without dyslexia <strong>the</strong>scores favour<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> control group, was only partially validated. To evengreater surprise, <strong>the</strong> ensu<strong>in</strong>g hypo<strong>the</strong>sis that, despite considerableprogress, <strong>the</strong> experimental group would still be slightly lagg<strong>in</strong>g beh<strong>in</strong>d<strong>the</strong> control group without dyslexia, was not verified ei<strong>the</strong>r. The latterhypo<strong>the</strong>sis was based on recent empirical f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs suggest<strong>in</strong>g that, <strong>in</strong>spite of explicit <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> phonological/orthographic skills, at-risklearners are still likely to cont<strong>in</strong>ue to lag beh<strong>in</strong>d good foreign languagelearners <strong>in</strong> terms of <strong>the</strong>ir phonological/orthographic skills (Sparks et al.,1991, 1997a). However, as can be seen from <strong>the</strong> present study, studentswith dyslexia (experimental group) may be no worse than <strong>the</strong>irnon-dyslexic peers; <strong>in</strong> fact, <strong>the</strong>y may well be capable of achiev<strong>in</strong>gbetter scores <strong>in</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g tests. It needs stress<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>the</strong>


144 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>experimental group’s scores on <strong>the</strong> post-tests after <strong>the</strong> implementation ofdirect multisensory tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, a performance markedly better than thatof <strong>the</strong> control group without dyslexia, was <strong>the</strong> most surpris<strong>in</strong>g outcomeof <strong>the</strong> study. Even allow<strong>in</strong>g for a considerable ga<strong>in</strong>, <strong>the</strong> experimentalgroup was still expected to situate itself on <strong>the</strong> achievement scale slightlybeh<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong> non-dyslexic students.Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis 8 that <strong>the</strong>re would be significant differences between <strong>the</strong>control group with dyslexia and <strong>the</strong> control group without dyslexia wasconfirmed by <strong>the</strong> results of all <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g pre- and posttests.The control group without dyslexia received much higher scores.There were statistically significant differences between <strong>the</strong> results of all<strong>the</strong> tests, apart from <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g pre-test. These f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs prove thatstudents with developmental dyslexia experience greater difficulties <strong>in</strong>learn<strong>in</strong>g English as a foreign language with <strong>the</strong> use of traditional<strong>in</strong>struction than <strong>the</strong>ir non-dyslexic peers, as supposed <strong>in</strong> Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis 1.Although orig<strong>in</strong>ally weaker, <strong>the</strong> students from <strong>the</strong> experimental groupshowed considerable improvement <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir performance on all <strong>the</strong>spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g tests. The calibre of <strong>the</strong>ir academic growth as wellas its rapidity <strong>in</strong>dicate <strong>the</strong> effectiveness of direct multisensory <strong>in</strong>struction<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> area of grapheme/phoneme correspondences and spell<strong>in</strong>g rules <strong>in</strong>English, contribut<strong>in</strong>g to an enhanced performance of Polish students on<strong>the</strong> word spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g tests.The results of <strong>the</strong> study yield several implications for teach<strong>in</strong>g foreignlanguages to students with dyslexia. They support <strong>the</strong> effectiveness ofdirect multisensory <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> phonology/orthography of <strong>the</strong>English language. Learners with dyslexia, who hardly benefit frommethodologies that force <strong>the</strong>m to <strong>in</strong>tuitively discover <strong>the</strong> phonologicalstructure of a new language, will most probably take advantage of direct<strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> phonology of <strong>the</strong> target language. Never<strong>the</strong>less,however promis<strong>in</strong>g a start, <strong>the</strong> outcome of <strong>the</strong> study must be treatedcautiously because of <strong>the</strong> small sample size and <strong>the</strong> absence of acomparison approach <strong>in</strong> tutor<strong>in</strong>g sessions. These restrictions def<strong>in</strong>itelylimit <strong>the</strong> generalisability of <strong>the</strong> results, which should be consideredtentative and subject to fur<strong>the</strong>r <strong>in</strong>vestigation. Even though <strong>the</strong>re areseveral sound <strong>the</strong>oretical reasons for believ<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>the</strong> MSL approachapplied <strong>in</strong> tutor<strong>in</strong>g sessions contributed significantly to <strong>the</strong> progressobserved <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimental group, this supposition rema<strong>in</strong>s just that.What can be safely <strong>in</strong>ferred from <strong>the</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of <strong>the</strong> study is that<strong>in</strong>tervention itself exerted a causal effect, however similar <strong>in</strong>ferences asto <strong>the</strong> nature of <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tervention cannot be applied at this stage.Ano<strong>the</strong>r issue concern<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> degree of difficulty that <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividualstudents had <strong>in</strong> Polish read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g would certa<strong>in</strong>ly require morethorough <strong>in</strong>vestigation, as this might have been a relevant factordeterm<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> outcome of <strong>the</strong> study. Moreover, it would have been


Treatment and Teach<strong>in</strong>g 145<strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to f<strong>in</strong>d out whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> multisensory direct <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong>English phonology had any <strong>in</strong>fluence on <strong>the</strong> native language skills of <strong>the</strong>students with dyslexia from <strong>the</strong> experimental group. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, itwould be necessary to carry out longitud<strong>in</strong>al research upon dyslexicversus non-dyslexic students, to study <strong>the</strong> effects regard<strong>in</strong>g both nativeand foreign language skills of direct multisensory <strong>in</strong>struction asopposed to <strong>the</strong> traditional approach. Compar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> efficacy of <strong>the</strong>MSL structured <strong>in</strong>struction with o<strong>the</strong>r than traditional <strong>in</strong>structionalpractices <strong>in</strong> foreign language teach<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> alternative comparisonapproach applied <strong>in</strong> tutor<strong>in</strong>g sessions, with regard to <strong>the</strong>ir effects onword read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g skills would yield some <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g results. Italso has to be noted that <strong>the</strong> results of <strong>the</strong> study <strong>in</strong>dicate that, perhaps,direct multisensory <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> English as a foreign language wouldalso be beneficial for non-dyslexic students, as it would allow <strong>the</strong>m toachieve even greater success <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g skills. While itwas beyond <strong>the</strong> scope of this study to check out such a possibility, <strong>the</strong>specific effects of MSL <strong>in</strong>struction on non-dyslexic students should stillbe determ<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> future research. This study has been an attempt to<strong>in</strong>spire fur<strong>the</strong>r experimentation with <strong>the</strong> use of <strong>the</strong> direct multisensoryapproach, an activity promis<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> long run, a more successfulforeign language <strong>in</strong>struction offered to learners with dyslexia andpotentially non-LD students as well.Educational Accommodations TowardsLearners with <strong>Dyslexia</strong>The fundamental issue seems to be rais<strong>in</strong>g awareness of dyslexiaamong all parties <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> creation and function<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong>educational environment. It appears particularly important <strong>in</strong> light of<strong>the</strong> multiple formal regulations, which have been <strong>in</strong>troduced <strong>in</strong> manycountries, concern<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> conditions and ways of assess<strong>in</strong>g, classify<strong>in</strong>g,promot<strong>in</strong>g and conduct<strong>in</strong>g exam<strong>in</strong>ations for students with specialeducational needs. Children with special educational needs can benefitfrom <strong>the</strong> type of school<strong>in</strong>g available to <strong>the</strong> majority of children, usuallythrough a set of special educational arrangements, which adapt <strong>the</strong>system towards <strong>the</strong>ir needs and abilities it is referred to as <strong>in</strong>clusioneducation or ma<strong>in</strong>stream<strong>in</strong>g. The prevail<strong>in</strong>g positive attitude of <strong>the</strong>educational stakeholders towards <strong>in</strong>clusive education for children withdyslexia needs to be translated <strong>in</strong>to significant changes implemented <strong>in</strong>to<strong>the</strong> exist<strong>in</strong>g educational systems if <strong>the</strong>se children are to be truly <strong>in</strong>cluded.Unique, diverse abilities, needs, <strong>in</strong>terests and ways of learn<strong>in</strong>g reflect<strong>in</strong>dividual differences between children, and <strong>the</strong>se attributes requireunderstand<strong>in</strong>g, support and <strong>in</strong>corporat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong> educational systemsthat aspire to be <strong>in</strong>clusive. Pr<strong>in</strong>cipally, <strong>in</strong>clusion policy <strong>in</strong>volves


146 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>rearrangements at <strong>the</strong> level of school management and those <strong>in</strong>troducedby <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual teachers <strong>in</strong> order to address <strong>the</strong> special needs of childrenwith dyslexia (e.g. <strong>in</strong>dividual education plans). Teachers’ and parents’awareness and alertness can trigger early identification of learn<strong>in</strong>gdifficulties, ideally followed by <strong>the</strong> offer of a wide range of support<strong>in</strong>gactivities and services, matched to <strong>the</strong> severity of <strong>the</strong> disorder.As much as <strong>the</strong> appropriate <strong>in</strong>clusion essentially does its job <strong>in</strong>support<strong>in</strong>g educational endeavours <strong>in</strong> most cases, <strong>the</strong> most severelydisordered children with dyslexia may still require a more <strong>in</strong>dividualised,exclusive approach to help <strong>the</strong>m overcome <strong>the</strong>ir impairments.Dyslexic problems are of vary<strong>in</strong>g degrees of severity, and <strong>the</strong> disorder isprobably best illustrated as a cont<strong>in</strong>uum of difficulties, rang<strong>in</strong>g fromsubtle through mild to severe. Thus, it seems that <strong>the</strong>re is enough logicbeh<strong>in</strong>d view<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tensity and range of educational <strong>in</strong>tervention andhelp as parallel to <strong>the</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>uum of difficulties. Some children withdyslexia can be <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> educational system with ease, given <strong>the</strong>fact that m<strong>in</strong>imum alterations and accommodations are <strong>in</strong>corporated <strong>in</strong>toit; however, for o<strong>the</strong>r children more <strong>in</strong>dividualised, small group or oneto-onetreatment seems a must (Bogdanowicz & Sayles, 2004).The legal status of <strong>the</strong> special rights def<strong>in</strong>ed as specific enabl<strong>in</strong>gsolutions and arrangements offered to children with dyslexia variesconsiderably across countries, schools and teachers. Thus, <strong>the</strong> availabilityof particular accommodations, <strong>in</strong>fluenc<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> actual everyday school lifeof children with dyslexia, may be regulated by a national policy, schoolpolicy or can be entirely at a teacher’s discretion. Indeed, this last optionseems to be <strong>the</strong> case <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> majority of <strong>in</strong>stances. The aforementionedspecial rights may cover <strong>the</strong> areas of assessment, alternative ways ofperform<strong>in</strong>g at school, special conditions dur<strong>in</strong>g exam<strong>in</strong>ations andforeign language study (<strong>the</strong> data for <strong>the</strong> analysis were collected <strong>in</strong> 20countries; Bogdanowicz & Sayles, 2004).The legislation concern<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>ciples of provision and organisationof psychological-pedagogical help <strong>in</strong> public k<strong>in</strong>dergartens, schoolsand <strong>in</strong>stitutions, suggests that any special, more <strong>in</strong>dividualised help forstudents with specific learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties should be organised <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>form of correction-compensation classes. However, <strong>the</strong>re is usually nomention of foreign language <strong>in</strong>struction, whereby, as it stands, studentswith dyslexia do not receive any specialised help <strong>in</strong> FLL. To offset <strong>the</strong>negative consequences of <strong>the</strong> above, quite often a regulation isimplemented to <strong>the</strong> effect that students with deep dyslexia be exemptedfrom learn<strong>in</strong>g a second foreign language (L3), temporarily or evenpermanently. As a result, fac<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> un<strong>in</strong>vit<strong>in</strong>g alternative of <strong>the</strong>irchildren’s failure <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g ano<strong>the</strong>r foreign language, ra<strong>the</strong>r unsurpris<strong>in</strong>gly,parents of learners with dyslexia would most probably takeadvantage of this option.


Treatment and Teach<strong>in</strong>g 147S<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>the</strong> reception of a standard educational programme by childrenwith dyslexia is usually h<strong>in</strong>dered, <strong>the</strong>ir abilities and needs ought to becatered for through <strong>the</strong> implementation of specially adapted, more orless <strong>in</strong>dividualised (depend<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong> severity of difficulties) conditions.These would <strong>in</strong>volve <strong>the</strong> appropriate pace of work, <strong>in</strong>dividual learn<strong>in</strong>gplans, special teach<strong>in</strong>g techniques and activities, preferably conducted bywell-qualified teachers (Bogdanowicz, 1995; Tomaszewska, 2001). Ingeneral, teachers are obliged to adjust <strong>the</strong>ir educational practices,requirements as well as conditions and forms of external exam<strong>in</strong>ationsto suit <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual needs and abilities of students with dyslexia,however, how exactly this translates <strong>in</strong>to practical applications significantlydiffers across schools and teachers.Ensur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> active participation of students with dyslexia <strong>in</strong> classroomactivities can be successfully executed by adjust<strong>in</strong>g, alter<strong>in</strong>g anddifferentiat<strong>in</strong>g teach<strong>in</strong>g practices. It needs stress<strong>in</strong>g though that accommodat<strong>in</strong>gdoes not necessarily mean lower<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> requirements, butra<strong>the</strong>r teach<strong>in</strong>g children with dyslexia <strong>the</strong> way <strong>the</strong>y learn best and deviseconditions for <strong>the</strong>m to demonstrate <strong>the</strong>ir knowledge and full potential.Because of <strong>the</strong>ir underly<strong>in</strong>g impairment, children with dyslexia getslowed down <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> course of process<strong>in</strong>g new <strong>in</strong>formation, especially <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>ted form, and retriev<strong>in</strong>g knowledge from memory. Much can bedone to make <strong>the</strong>ir learn<strong>in</strong>g a less pa<strong>in</strong>ful experience. It can be achievedthrough creat<strong>in</strong>g a support<strong>in</strong>g atmosphere and a generally positivelearn<strong>in</strong>g environment. What <strong>the</strong>y need is more time for complet<strong>in</strong>g tasksand help <strong>in</strong> susta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g concentration so that <strong>the</strong>y can show <strong>the</strong>irstrengths and atta<strong>in</strong>ment. Signall<strong>in</strong>g that we would like <strong>the</strong>m to respondand secur<strong>in</strong>g enough time for preparation as well as provid<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>advance <strong>the</strong> questions and issues we want to discuss <strong>in</strong> a lesson, ra<strong>the</strong>rthan call<strong>in</strong>g on students for spontaneous responses, markedly mitigates<strong>the</strong>ir anxiety and fear. Teachers’ teach<strong>in</strong>g style, flexibility, openness,read<strong>in</strong>ess to implement changes <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir teach<strong>in</strong>g rout<strong>in</strong>es <strong>in</strong> order tofoster scholastic atta<strong>in</strong>ment <strong>in</strong> children with dyslexia greatly <strong>in</strong>fluences<strong>the</strong> way <strong>the</strong>y function at school. Regular consultations and feedback fromstudents makes it possible for teachers to tailor <strong>the</strong>ir teach<strong>in</strong>g accommodationsmost accurately towards <strong>the</strong> needs and abilities of childrenwith dyslexia.When we take students’ performance <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> classroom <strong>in</strong>to account, aset of alterations, <strong>the</strong> description of which follows, can successfully createa more fertile ground for active and more efficient function<strong>in</strong>g of childrenwith dyslexia. Students with dyslexia can be released from read<strong>in</strong>g aloud<strong>in</strong> front of <strong>the</strong> whole class. Instead, <strong>the</strong>y can provide <strong>the</strong> teacher with arecord<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong>ir read<strong>in</strong>g aloud at home. They work with a selection oftexts agreed upon by <strong>the</strong> teacher, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g some texts of <strong>the</strong>ir own choice,on topics relevant and <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong>m. Poor handwrit<strong>in</strong>g should not


148 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>disqualify children’s work along with poor spell<strong>in</strong>g. Accept<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> use oflaptop computers (typewriters) for edit<strong>in</strong>g, note-tak<strong>in</strong>g and generallytyp<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> class <strong>in</strong>stead of hand writ<strong>in</strong>g is of great help, toge<strong>the</strong>r withautomated spellers (spellcheckers) and dictionaries.The use of technology <strong>in</strong>deed seems <strong>in</strong>valuable <strong>in</strong> support<strong>in</strong>g classroomprocedures. Computers, spellcheckers, books on tape, overheadprojectors, PowerPo<strong>in</strong>t presentations, tape recorders, <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternet, websites,e-mail, let alone more sophisticated assistive technological support,which <strong>in</strong>cludes specialised computer software, speech-control taperecorders, read<strong>in</strong>g mach<strong>in</strong>es with optical character recognition, listen<strong>in</strong>gaids that use a microphone and headset, and voice output systems thatread back texts displayed on a computer screen (Ganschow et al., 2000:188), can considerably aid <strong>the</strong> efforts of teachers and students alike (cf.Schneider & Crombie, 2003). Even if access to <strong>the</strong> abovementionedequipment is limited, simply allow<strong>in</strong>g oral <strong>in</strong>stead of a writtenperformance as well as <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g a reader, who would read <strong>the</strong> materialaloud, and/or a scribe, who would do <strong>the</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g part for <strong>the</strong> childrenwith dyslexia, aids efficiency of work and saves time. This idea can beeasily put <strong>in</strong>to practice <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> classroom through pair and group work,where students are allocated different tasks and roles. With reference tohomework, learners can be encouraged to hand <strong>in</strong> various assignmentsprepared <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> form of a tape record<strong>in</strong>g or a multimedia presentationra<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>in</strong> written form.Frequent repetition and overlearn<strong>in</strong>g, be<strong>in</strong>g a must for children withdyslexia, br<strong>in</strong>gs a danger of boredom and decreas<strong>in</strong>g motivation.Schneider and Crombie (2003) advocate avoid<strong>in</strong>g it through practic<strong>in</strong>ga given learn<strong>in</strong>g step <strong>in</strong> a variety of contexts and <strong>in</strong>corporat<strong>in</strong>g tasks that<strong>in</strong>volve <strong>the</strong> use of movable teach<strong>in</strong>g aids. Diverse, more or less guidedactivities us<strong>in</strong>g coloured cards, blackboard, picture-to-text match<strong>in</strong>gactivities are followed by regular paper-pencil tasks. Moreover, a gradualchange from larger to smaller pr<strong>in</strong>t, coloured to black and white pr<strong>in</strong>t oncards is suggested, eventually giv<strong>in</strong>g up card-sort<strong>in</strong>g tasks <strong>in</strong> favour ofpaper and pencil activities. A similar rule, mov<strong>in</strong>g from easier to moredifficult, is also applied to <strong>the</strong> character of <strong>the</strong> task receptive beforeproductive.Colour cod<strong>in</strong>g and shape cod<strong>in</strong>g (cf. Schneider, 1999; Schneider &Crombie, 2003) of various l<strong>in</strong>guistic concepts (e.g. parts of speech; roots,prefixes, suffixes) proves helpful <strong>in</strong> anchor<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m <strong>in</strong> memory.Numerous movable devices (e.g. illustrat<strong>in</strong>g letter-sound patterns,word formation processes or orthographic regularities), for <strong>in</strong>stanceorthographic slides, sentence strips, flashcards 4 or trac<strong>in</strong>g pads withvary<strong>in</strong>g surfaces, can be designed to enhance learn<strong>in</strong>g and remember<strong>in</strong>g.All of <strong>the</strong> abovementioned aids can be lam<strong>in</strong>ated (with <strong>the</strong> exception oftrac<strong>in</strong>g pads of course) for <strong>the</strong> sake of durability, collected and kept by


Treatment and Teach<strong>in</strong>g 149students <strong>in</strong> special boxes/files to be easily accessed for reference, reviewand <strong>in</strong>teractive language games, <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> k<strong>in</strong>aes<strong>the</strong>tic-tactile elementsof learn<strong>in</strong>g.K<strong>in</strong>aes<strong>the</strong>tic-tactile experience can be easily re<strong>in</strong>forced by studentspractic<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> position of <strong>the</strong> mouth and tongue when produc<strong>in</strong>gparticular sounds and modell<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> sound-letter structure of wordswith <strong>the</strong> use of tokens; f<strong>in</strong>ger trac<strong>in</strong>g of letters and words on varioussurfaces; colour- and shape-coded card sort<strong>in</strong>g with simultaneousmodell<strong>in</strong>g and verbalis<strong>in</strong>g language concepts <strong>the</strong>y represent; us<strong>in</strong>gtokens, cards with letters and body movements to illustrate l<strong>in</strong>guisticprocesses (word formation, grammar structures 5 ). Also mnemonicteach<strong>in</strong>g aids (Jurek, 2008), which enhance <strong>the</strong> ability to remember andretrieve <strong>in</strong>formation through unique, sometimes humorous, surpris<strong>in</strong>g oreven nonsensical associations (e.g. a sentence with a picture anddemonstration to illustrate its mean<strong>in</strong>g ‘I keep my elbow on a yellowpillow under <strong>the</strong> w<strong>in</strong>dow’ to remember <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g choice ‘-ow’ for <strong>the</strong>/3y/ sound), are students’ favourite choice. Such activities allowunlimited practice and repetition, aimed at reach<strong>in</strong>g automaticity <strong>in</strong> amultisensory, boredom- and stress-free fashion. Moreover, engag<strong>in</strong>gstudents <strong>in</strong> produc<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ir own sets of such teach<strong>in</strong>g aids proves avaluable learn<strong>in</strong>g experience <strong>in</strong> itself due to <strong>the</strong> k<strong>in</strong>aes<strong>the</strong>tic-tactilelearn<strong>in</strong>g opportunities it provides.Importantly, as noted by Schneider and Crombie (2003), k<strong>in</strong>aes<strong>the</strong>tictactilecontext can be successfully applied to produc<strong>in</strong>g longer texts.Paper outl<strong>in</strong>es are written <strong>in</strong> keywords on coloured cards. Basicparagraph features topic sentence with <strong>the</strong> ma<strong>in</strong> idea of <strong>the</strong> paragraph;support<strong>in</strong>g details, facts and reasons; examples that support each detail when colour coded are much more easily understood and manipulatedby students, who f<strong>in</strong>ally get ready to write texts of several paragraphs.Additionally, conjunction words are provided on separate cards.The multimodal/multisensory structured <strong>in</strong>put (characterised earlier <strong>in</strong>this chapter), divided <strong>in</strong>to digestible, logically sequenced (from easier tomore difficult) chunks, can be summarised <strong>in</strong> figures, charts, tables,graphs and illustrations. It is also worth encourag<strong>in</strong>g students to organiseand represent <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>dmaps and spidergrams. Handoutsprepared by teachers, conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g well-organised notes, summaries, crucialpo<strong>in</strong>ts and conclusions of <strong>the</strong> lesson, constitute an <strong>in</strong>valuable help forstudents with dyslexia as regards later study and consolidation of <strong>the</strong>material as well as a model that <strong>the</strong>y can follow while prepar<strong>in</strong>g notes<strong>the</strong>mselves. However, pages cluttered with <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> a ra<strong>the</strong>r unclearand chaotic way should be avoided; <strong>in</strong>stead, highlight<strong>in</strong>g salient po<strong>in</strong>tstoge<strong>the</strong>r with us<strong>in</strong>g larger fonts would most certa<strong>in</strong>ly be appreciated.Students are responsible for fil<strong>in</strong>g all <strong>the</strong> handouts toge<strong>the</strong>r with notes,assignments, projects and homework <strong>in</strong> portfolios (electronic forms be<strong>in</strong>g


150 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>possible as well). The plan of <strong>the</strong> lesson, given to students at <strong>the</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gof <strong>the</strong> lesson, toge<strong>the</strong>r with clear sets of <strong>in</strong>structions and modelledresponses preced<strong>in</strong>g particular tasks back up and direct <strong>the</strong> successfulcompletion of <strong>the</strong> tasks and allow <strong>the</strong> students to concentrate better on <strong>the</strong>content of <strong>the</strong> lesson.The need to develop stronger metacognitive skills, reason<strong>in</strong>g abilitiesand self-correction strategies is often highlighted with regard to studentswith dyslexia (Schneider, 1999; Schneider & Crombie, 2003; Wearmouth &Reid, 2008). Ga<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g awareness of <strong>the</strong> nature of <strong>the</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g process assuch and be<strong>in</strong>g able to reflect on it requires undertak<strong>in</strong>g a series of smallsteps, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> application of <strong>the</strong> verbalisation strategy. At <strong>the</strong>beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g, students can be encouraged to simply comment on whatexactly <strong>the</strong>y are do<strong>in</strong>g and how, while complet<strong>in</strong>g various tasks. Teachersstimulate and elicit responses through mime, gesture and thoughtprovok<strong>in</strong>gquestions (e.g. Why would you spell it this way? Where wouldyou <strong>in</strong>sert this word and why? Can you see <strong>the</strong> pattern? What are youth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g?). This ability to identify <strong>the</strong> mechanisms and strategies underly<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong> actions we take extends to verbalis<strong>in</strong>g (out aloud) <strong>the</strong> discoverycourse of l<strong>in</strong>guistic process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> area of read<strong>in</strong>g, spell<strong>in</strong>g, pronunciation,grammar structures, vocabulary and writ<strong>in</strong>g (express<strong>in</strong>g thoughts onpaper). Schneider and Crombie (2003) claim <strong>the</strong> need to enhancedevelopment of <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong> learners with dyslexia: <strong>the</strong> abilityto identify a problem (e.g. a spell<strong>in</strong>g mistake), knowledge of a set ofstrategies to solve it, <strong>the</strong> ability to choose and apply <strong>the</strong> most suitablestrategy to solve <strong>the</strong> problem quickly and successfully, and, last but notleast, <strong>the</strong> ability to double check (to ask check questions) <strong>the</strong> outcome of<strong>the</strong> applied strategy. The role of <strong>the</strong> teacher <strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> matacognitivestrategies <strong>in</strong> students with dyslexia <strong>in</strong>volves, firstly, explicitmodell<strong>in</strong>g of a variety of strategies to aid self-reflection and self-correction,and secondly, provid<strong>in</strong>g multiple opportunities for students to extensivelypractice <strong>the</strong>se strategies <strong>in</strong> a stress-free environment.Schneider and Crombie (2003) mention several actions that can beundertaken with regard to creat<strong>in</strong>g opportunities for obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g satisfactorytest results. They highlight <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluential role of an explicit<strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> preparation strategies, such as <strong>the</strong> use of mnemonicdevices, multisensory structured study<strong>in</strong>g, summary <strong>in</strong>formation charts,mock exam<strong>in</strong>ations, time management and task organisation. Apart from<strong>the</strong> test preparation strategies, also test-tak<strong>in</strong>g strategies, which refer tobehaviour dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> test/exam<strong>in</strong>ation, should be pla<strong>in</strong>ly taught todyslexics. Still, by far <strong>the</strong> most important seems to be <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>troduction of<strong>the</strong> proper test-tak<strong>in</strong>g modifications, such as, for <strong>in</strong>stance, extend<strong>in</strong>g timeand provid<strong>in</strong>g a separate, distraction-free room. Ano<strong>the</strong>r option constitutes<strong>the</strong> alternative test modes, such as take-home tests, which can beapplied to <strong>the</strong> whole exam<strong>in</strong>ation or only parts of it, or tests prepared to


Treatment and Teach<strong>in</strong>g 151be completed with <strong>the</strong> use of a computer. Secur<strong>in</strong>g a scribe or a reader <strong>in</strong>a language test-tak<strong>in</strong>g situation also requires a separate room so thato<strong>the</strong>r test takers are not disturbed. F<strong>in</strong>ally, careful selection of test tasksseems crucial, for <strong>in</strong>stance, cloze procedure tasks should be avoided, as<strong>the</strong>y prevent learners with dyslexia, who tend to rely heavily on <strong>the</strong>contextual clues, from demonstrat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ir actual knowledge. In addition,match<strong>in</strong>g tasks may prove difficult, especially for students withimpaired visual process<strong>in</strong>g. Dur<strong>in</strong>g oral exam<strong>in</strong>ations, it is best to avoidimmediate forced responses, but ra<strong>the</strong>r distribute questions <strong>in</strong> advance,<strong>in</strong> that way giv<strong>in</strong>g enough time for retriev<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation from memory.When correct<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> written work of learners with dyslexia, onlycerta<strong>in</strong> aspects of it can undergo assessment, for <strong>in</strong>stance, <strong>the</strong> argumentspresented, <strong>the</strong> vocabulary use, <strong>the</strong> choice of grammatical structures orspell<strong>in</strong>g. As far as spell<strong>in</strong>g mistakes are concerned, it is recommended(Jurek, 2008) not to highlight or circle <strong>the</strong>m (especially with a red pen) oro<strong>the</strong>rwise expose <strong>the</strong>m, <strong>in</strong> order to prevent students from concentrat<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong>ir attention on and consolidat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> erroneous forms. It seems morejustified to cross <strong>the</strong> misspelled word and provide <strong>the</strong> correct spell<strong>in</strong>gabove or next to it so that it is <strong>the</strong> correct form that is focused on and<strong>in</strong>tegrated. Such an approach proved more effective than, for <strong>in</strong>stance,writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> words on <strong>the</strong> blackboard and ask<strong>in</strong>g students to comparewith <strong>the</strong>ir own spell<strong>in</strong>g attempts. Teachers also often <strong>in</strong>dicate <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>marg<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> occurrence of a mistake <strong>in</strong> a given l<strong>in</strong>e of <strong>the</strong> text, however,<strong>the</strong>y do not specify where exactly it is, expect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> students to do so, or<strong>the</strong>y simply count down <strong>the</strong> mistakes and provide <strong>the</strong> total number at<strong>the</strong> bottom of <strong>the</strong> text, aga<strong>in</strong> with no <strong>in</strong>dication of <strong>the</strong> position of <strong>the</strong>mistakes. Unfortunately, such expectations expressed towards childrenwith dyslexia are bound to produce ra<strong>the</strong>r negative results. Even verycareful and <strong>in</strong>tensive look<strong>in</strong>g at words and search<strong>in</strong>g for mistakes cannotguarantee spott<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> misspelled words. What is more, frequently,words that are perfectly well spelled get corrected. Thus, it seems moretime wise and effect wise to cross misspell<strong>in</strong>gs and provide correct formsfor students. Naturally, decent practice with regard to words which donot seem well <strong>in</strong>tegrated yet is a must, however, mere mechanicalrewrit<strong>in</strong>g is useless and monotonous, it kills motivation and enthusiasm.Instead, teachers may f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong> multisensory techniques andmovable devices described earlier useful. Students can also be asked tocolour <strong>the</strong> troublesome spell<strong>in</strong>g patterns <strong>in</strong> words, to verbalise orthographicrules, to provide o<strong>the</strong>r examples of words where <strong>the</strong> rulesapply, to form word families for <strong>the</strong> misspelled words, to formnegations, to form sentences with <strong>the</strong> words, to design mnemonicsthat would help to remember <strong>the</strong> correct spell<strong>in</strong>g (especially <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> casesof irregular words and exceptions).


152 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Students with dyslexia can greatly benefit from frequent feedback on<strong>the</strong>ir educational progress from teachers, both oral and written as well asquantitative and qualitative. Especially relevant is that <strong>the</strong>y comparewhat <strong>the</strong>y have already learned with <strong>the</strong>ir own previous achievementsra<strong>the</strong>r than with <strong>the</strong> atta<strong>in</strong>ments of <strong>the</strong>ir peers. It is important that <strong>the</strong>ysee how <strong>the</strong>y advance <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir study, consequently realis<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ir<strong>in</strong>dividual teach<strong>in</strong>g plans, and that <strong>the</strong>y can count on <strong>the</strong> attention,understand<strong>in</strong>g and help of <strong>the</strong>ir teachers.Notes1. See Chapter 2: ‘Bra<strong>in</strong> mechanisms <strong>in</strong> dyslexia’ for a discussion of bra<strong>in</strong>abnormalities <strong>in</strong> dyslexia.2. See Chapter 1: ‘Read<strong>in</strong>g strategies and stages of read<strong>in</strong>g development’ formore <strong>in</strong>formation on <strong>the</strong> balance model of read<strong>in</strong>g by Bakker.3. For a more thorough discussion see Nijakowska (2008).4. See Chapter 6 for examples.5. See Chapter 6 for examples.


Chapter 6Sample Activities for Learners with<strong>Dyslexia</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g English as a<strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong>Activities for Develop<strong>in</strong>g PhonologicalAwareness and Awareness ofSound-letter RelationsAs already <strong>in</strong>dicated, explicit phonological awareness tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g isbelieved to be necessary for children with dyslexia, who fail tosufficiently develop phonological awareness skills. It has also beenstressed that such tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g needs to follow a developmental sequence,start<strong>in</strong>g from bigger chunks with<strong>in</strong> words and subsequently mov<strong>in</strong>g to<strong>in</strong>dividual phonemes. Thus, f<strong>in</strong>ally, <strong>in</strong>dividuals should be able to hearand discrim<strong>in</strong>ate between sounds, to identify <strong>the</strong>m <strong>in</strong> various positions<strong>in</strong> words (<strong>in</strong>itial, f<strong>in</strong>al, medial) and to manipulate <strong>the</strong>m. Master<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>seabilities <strong>in</strong> an auditory context translates <strong>in</strong>to more rapid success <strong>in</strong>spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g (written context).Pure phonemic awareness activities are based on spoken words andare responded to orally. However, it is recommended to use auditory(clapp<strong>in</strong>g) and visual (boxes, blocks, tokens, cards) cues to help childrenunderstand that <strong>the</strong> sounds <strong>in</strong> words are separate entities. Apparently,visible, movable representations of sounds help to clarify and guidecount<strong>in</strong>g, segment<strong>in</strong>g and blend<strong>in</strong>g tasks and children should beencouraged to use auditory and visual cues.In all phonological awareness activities, <strong>the</strong> visual representations ofsounds (boxes, blocks, tokens, cards) can be substituted with letters.The aim of such tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g is to eventually make <strong>the</strong> letter-to-soundrelations def<strong>in</strong>ite. Letters correspond<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> sounds be<strong>in</strong>g masteredcan be <strong>in</strong>troduced and placed on <strong>the</strong> tokens. It has been demonstratedthat <strong>the</strong>re is an advantage to comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g phoneme awareness withletter knowledge tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, over <strong>the</strong> phonological awareness tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gwithout <strong>the</strong> letter-to-sound relations, as well as over <strong>the</strong> letter knowledgetra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g without phonological awareness <strong>in</strong>struction (Maurer,2003). The aim of teach<strong>in</strong>g phonics is predom<strong>in</strong>antly to ga<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>knowledge that written words are built of letters or clusters of letters153


154 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>that represent <strong>the</strong> sounds of spoken words and to acquire thosesystematic relations between letters and sounds. Not until sound<strong>in</strong>gout letters and spell<strong>in</strong>g patterns is automatic can readers naturallyconcentrate on mean<strong>in</strong>g ra<strong>the</strong>r than on decod<strong>in</strong>g while read<strong>in</strong>g. Skilfuland automatic decod<strong>in</strong>g allows a shift of focus from recognition ofletters and words to comprehension, which, <strong>in</strong> turn, leads to a criticalassessment and creative use of <strong>the</strong> content of <strong>the</strong> text. The time andeffort allotted to decod<strong>in</strong>g should be gradually reduced dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>course of learn<strong>in</strong>g to read. Summ<strong>in</strong>g up, a truly effective read<strong>in</strong>gprogramme should comprise a direct and explicit <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> both<strong>the</strong> phonological awareness and letter/sound correspondences.In addition, <strong>the</strong>re is a debate on what gra<strong>in</strong> sizes are optimal forteach<strong>in</strong>g ra<strong>the</strong>r <strong>in</strong>consistent English letter-sound correspondences.Should <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>struction concentrate on small or larger units of sound?It has been suggested that English is relatively more consistent withregard to larger units such as rimes or syllables, while graphemephonemerelations tend to be irregular. Thus, English-speak<strong>in</strong>gchildren need both small- and large-size decod<strong>in</strong>g strategies. All <strong>in</strong>all, Ziegler and Goswami (2006) suggest that comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g phonics-basedmethods with regard to small and large units with whole wordteach<strong>in</strong>g, hence form<strong>in</strong>g a k<strong>in</strong>d of complementary balanced approach,is important <strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g high-grade word recognition ability <strong>in</strong>English. Some English words have to be learned as dist<strong>in</strong>ct patterns(e.g. ‘choir’, ‘people’), o<strong>the</strong>r words have rich orthographic neighboursand share consistent rime spell<strong>in</strong>gs and pronunciations with numerouswords (e.g. ‘light’), f<strong>in</strong>ally, <strong>the</strong>re are words with relatively consistentgrapheme-phoneme relations, which can be easily decoded at thissmall gra<strong>in</strong> size level (e.g. ‘cat’, ‘hen’).The follow<strong>in</strong>g is a sequence of activities for develop<strong>in</strong>g phonologicalawareness and <strong>the</strong> awareness of sound-letter relations, based on word,syllable, onset-rime and phoneme-level manipulations, all of which canbe easily <strong>in</strong>corporated <strong>in</strong>to classroom procedures. In all <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>gtasks, tokens are used to represent phonological units, and later, letterscan be <strong>in</strong>troduced pr<strong>in</strong>ted on tokens to make <strong>the</strong> phonologyorthographyrelations more explicit.


Sample Activities for Learners with <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g English 155Activities for Develop<strong>in</strong>g Phonological Awareness and <strong>the</strong> Awareness of Sound-letter RelationsType of task Procedure1. Segment<strong>in</strong>gcompound wordsand sentences <strong>in</strong>towords.a) " Students say ‘toothbrush’ without a ‘tooth;’ ‘ra<strong>in</strong>bow’ without ‘bow,’ ‘girlfriend’ without ‘girl.’b) " Students listen to <strong>the</strong> teacher read<strong>in</strong>g a sentence and place a marker from <strong>the</strong> left to <strong>the</strong> right for eachword heard.‘Ben had a hen’ ‘Ben....had......a......hen’" To make <strong>the</strong> boundaries between words clear teachers can use a device made of tokens fastened to <strong>the</strong>elastic band; <strong>the</strong>y simply need to stretch <strong>the</strong> band." Later, <strong>the</strong> words can be put on <strong>the</strong> tokens.The fat cat is on <strong>the</strong> matThe fat cat is on <strong>the</strong> mat


156 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Type of task Procedure2. Segment<strong>in</strong>g andblend<strong>in</strong>gsyllables." Students listen to <strong>the</strong> teacher say<strong>in</strong>g words, count <strong>the</strong> syllables and place a marker from <strong>the</strong> left to <strong>the</strong>right for each syllable heard. The procedure with an elastic band can be used here as well. For example: ‘enlist,’‘per-form,’ ‘dis-em-bark,’ ‘en-joy-<strong>in</strong>g.’en list enlist3. Recogniz<strong>in</strong>gand creat<strong>in</strong>grhymes.a) " Recogniz<strong>in</strong>g rhymes. Students name <strong>the</strong> objects that are <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> pictures and say <strong>the</strong>ir names aloud. Then<strong>the</strong>y listen to <strong>the</strong> teacher say<strong>in</strong>g pairs of words and if <strong>the</strong>y rhyme students raise <strong>the</strong>ir thumbs up, circle <strong>the</strong>pictures and colour <strong>the</strong>m.‘bat’ – ‘cat’‘rat’ – ‘men’


Sample Activities for Learners with <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g English 157Type of task Procedureb) " Students name <strong>the</strong> objects <strong>in</strong> each row and say <strong>the</strong>ir names aloud. They circle <strong>the</strong> picture whose namedoes not rhyme with <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r names.‘ten’ ‘men’ ‘cat’‘hen’ ‘rat’ ‘ten’c) " Creat<strong>in</strong>g rhymes. Students name <strong>the</strong> pictures <strong>in</strong> each row and say <strong>the</strong>ir names aloud. In each row <strong>the</strong>ydraw a picture whose name rhymes with <strong>the</strong> names of <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r two pictures.4. Segment<strong>in</strong>g andblend<strong>in</strong>g onsetsand rimes." Flashcards with pictures, tokens as well as flashcards with onsets, rimes and words are needed. Colourcod<strong>in</strong>g is used to help students dist<strong>in</strong>guish between <strong>the</strong> onsets and rimes. Movable devices slides are usedto manipulate letters to form words. First students are shown three pictures and provide <strong>the</strong> names for <strong>the</strong>me.g. ‘men,’ ‘ten,’ ‘hen’ or ‘cat,’ ‘hat,’ ‘rat.’ They listen to <strong>the</strong> teacher say<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> words and are asked if <strong>the</strong>y canhear any common parts <strong>in</strong> all of <strong>the</strong> words. They use tokens to represent onsets and rimes. They are givenflashcards with <strong>the</strong> ‘en’ rime and with <strong>the</strong> onsets ‘m,’ ‘t,’ ‘h’ and are asked to form words. They match <strong>the</strong>


158 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Type of task Procedurepictures to <strong>the</strong> words. The procedure is <strong>the</strong>n repeated with o<strong>the</strong>r examples of rimes and onsets. Studentsmanipulate slides to form words.h en h ent en t enm en m enhhbten t en c atmmr


Sample Activities for Learners with <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g English 159Type of task Procedure5. Identify<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>alsounds <strong>in</strong> words.a) " Students name <strong>the</strong> objects <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> pictures <strong>in</strong> each row; say <strong>the</strong>ir names aloud. Then <strong>the</strong>y listen to <strong>the</strong>teacher say<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m; if <strong>the</strong> words start with <strong>the</strong> same sound students raise <strong>the</strong>ir thumbs up, circle <strong>the</strong>m andcolour <strong>the</strong>m.‘pig’ ‘pen’‘car’ ‘rat’‘key’ ‘cat’


160 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Type of task Procedureb) " Students say aloud <strong>the</strong> names of <strong>the</strong> objects <strong>in</strong> each row and choose a picture whose name doesn’t startwith <strong>the</strong> same sound as <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r names.‘car’ ‘cat’ ‘pig’‘pig’ ‘ten’ ‘pen’c) " Students listen to <strong>the</strong> words and identify <strong>the</strong> sounds at <strong>the</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g.Do ‘cup’ and ‘cake’ beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> same?Do ‘sun’ and ‘ship’ beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> same?Do ‘shop,’ ‘ship,’ ‘sheep,’ ‘shoe’ start with <strong>the</strong> same sound?Do ‘shop,’ ‘cheap,’ ‘shoe,’ ‘ship’ start with <strong>the</strong> same sound?Do ‘pen,’ ‘pet,’ ‘pill,’ ‘pot’ start with <strong>the</strong> same sound?d) " Students listen to <strong>the</strong> words and identify <strong>the</strong> sounds at <strong>the</strong> end?Do ‘pet,’ ‘pot,’ ‘cat,’ ‘mat’ end with <strong>the</strong> same sound?Do ‘sun,’ ‘man,’ ‘gone,’ ‘goat’ end with <strong>the</strong> same sound?


Sample Activities for Learners with <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g English 161Type of task Procedure6. Segment<strong>in</strong>g andblend<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividualsounds <strong>in</strong>words.a) " Students look at <strong>the</strong> pictures, listen to <strong>the</strong> teacher say<strong>in</strong>g words, count <strong>the</strong> phonemes and place amarker from <strong>the</strong> left to <strong>the</strong> right for each phoneme heard. Later letters can be put on tokens.‘hen’‘bat’b) " Students look at <strong>the</strong> pictures, listen to <strong>the</strong> teacher say<strong>in</strong>g words, count <strong>the</strong> phonemes and cut <strong>the</strong>pictures <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong> number of pieces correspond<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> number of phonemes.


162 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Type of task Procedurec) " Students listen to <strong>the</strong> teacher say<strong>in</strong>g words, count <strong>the</strong> phonemes and place a marker from <strong>the</strong> left to <strong>the</strong>right for each phoneme heard. Then <strong>the</strong>y blend <strong>the</strong>m to make a word aga<strong>in</strong>. The procedure with an elasticband can be used here as well. For example: ‘rat’.r a t r a td) " Students listen to <strong>the</strong> sets of words and decide whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y have <strong>the</strong> same sound <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> middle. Tokensare used to represent sounds <strong>in</strong> words. For example: ‘lip,’ ‘rib,’ ‘flip,’ ‘chick’ or ‘pet,’ ‘p<strong>in</strong>,’ ‘sun,’ ‘bun’ or‘sun,’ ‘bun,’ ‘cup,’ ‘nut.’e) " Students listen to <strong>the</strong> words and identify <strong>the</strong> sounds <strong>the</strong>y hear at <strong>the</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g, end and <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> middleof <strong>the</strong>se words. Tokens are used to represent sounds <strong>in</strong> words.f) " Students listen to <strong>the</strong> words first, <strong>the</strong>n to <strong>in</strong>dividual sounds and signal whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y are heard at <strong>the</strong>beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g, end or <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> middle of <strong>the</strong> word. Tokens are used to represent sounds <strong>in</strong> words.7. Manipulat<strong>in</strong>gsounds (delet<strong>in</strong>g,add<strong>in</strong>g, substitut<strong>in</strong>g).a) " Leave off <strong>the</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g sound of a given word to make a new word.p a t p a t


Sample Activities for Learners with <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g English 163Type of task ProcedureFor example:‘pat’ it starts with /p/ and ends with /æt/, take <strong>the</strong> first sound away and it says /æt/.Take /k/ out of ‘cat’ and it says /æt/. Say ‘mat’ without /m/. What word will be left if you take /p/ off pat?What is miss<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> ‘eat’ that you can hear <strong>in</strong> ‘meat’?b)" Leave off <strong>the</strong> end sound of a given word to make a new word. The procedure is similar to <strong>the</strong> one usedwith <strong>in</strong>itial sounds.c) " Listen to <strong>the</strong> word and substitute <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial sound.What is <strong>the</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g sound <strong>in</strong> ‘cat’? Say ‘cat’ with /h/ <strong>in</strong>stead of /k/. What is <strong>the</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g sound <strong>in</strong>‘hen’? Say ‘hen’ with /p/ <strong>in</strong>stead of /h/.c a th a th a t


164 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Type of task Procedured) " Listen to a word and substitute <strong>the</strong> f<strong>in</strong>al sound.What is <strong>the</strong> end/f<strong>in</strong>al sound <strong>in</strong> ‘bet’? Say ‘bet’ with /l/ <strong>in</strong>stead of /t/.b e tb e ll b e lle) " Listen to a word and substitute <strong>the</strong> middle sound.What is <strong>the</strong> middle sound of ‘met’? Say ‘met’ with /æ/ <strong>in</strong>stead of /e/.m e t mt m a taf) " Listen to a word and change one sound to form a new word. eg. ‘met’ ‘mat’ ‘sat’ ‘sit’ ‘s<strong>in</strong>’ ‘b<strong>in</strong>’ ‘bun’ ‘nun’ ‘nut’ ‘hut’


Sample Activities for Learners with <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g English 165Orthographic Awareness Activities 1English orthography is widely perceived as non-transparent anddifficult, however, Kessler and Treiman (2003) provide conv<strong>in</strong>c<strong>in</strong>gevidence that it is <strong>in</strong> fact more regular than one would th<strong>in</strong>k. There areseveral productive spell<strong>in</strong>g patterns and pr<strong>in</strong>ciples whose exploration canaid <strong>the</strong> effectiveness of teach<strong>in</strong>g. Position (<strong>in</strong>itial, f<strong>in</strong>al) and environment(surround<strong>in</strong>g sounds) have an effect on <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g of a given sound.English vowels tend to be notoriously misspelled. The analysis of syllables<strong>in</strong> words, <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> division of syllables <strong>in</strong>to onsets and rimes(consist<strong>in</strong>g of vowels and codas), supports spell<strong>in</strong>g. Rimes prove to play<strong>the</strong> most crucial role here. Present<strong>in</strong>g words <strong>in</strong> sets classified accord<strong>in</strong>g to<strong>the</strong> rimes <strong>the</strong>y share is very useful, and this also applies to onsets withmultiple spell<strong>in</strong>gs (e.g. /k/) <strong>in</strong> words grouped by <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g vowel.Obviously, several patterns have exceptions, for <strong>in</strong>stance certa<strong>in</strong> rimeshave vary<strong>in</strong>g spell<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> different words. Kessler and Treiman (2003)demonstrated that adults rely on <strong>the</strong> environmental characteristics (onsetand coda) of a word when attempt<strong>in</strong>g to spell vowels, thus childrenlearn<strong>in</strong>g spell<strong>in</strong>g patterns at school later resort to <strong>the</strong>m <strong>in</strong> adulthood.The follow<strong>in</strong>g is a set of sample activities selected from <strong>the</strong> programmeused <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> small-scale <strong>in</strong>tervention study described <strong>in</strong> Chapter 5. The studyconcerned <strong>the</strong> effectiveness of direct multisensory <strong>in</strong>struction for improv<strong>in</strong>gword read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong> English through <strong>the</strong> systematicstudy of selected grapheme-phoneme relations, spell<strong>in</strong>g patterns and rules.The follow<strong>in</strong>g activities predom<strong>in</strong>antly concern practic<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>gchoices for <strong>the</strong> /ai/ sound, namely ‘i-e’ <strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-syllable words, ‘-y’ at<strong>the</strong> end of a word and ‘igh’ <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> middle of a s<strong>in</strong>gle-syllable word.Orthographic Awareness ActivitiesType of task1. Short andlong vowels comparison.Procedure" Teacher asks <strong>the</strong> students to give <strong>the</strong> key words to unlock<strong>the</strong> sounds of short vowel sounds. The words: ‘apple,’ ‘egg,’‘Indian,’ ‘octopus,’ ‘umbrella’ are written on <strong>the</strong> board.Students are asked to read <strong>the</strong> words and say what <strong>the</strong>beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g sound <strong>in</strong> each of <strong>the</strong>m is. Teacher stresses that<strong>the</strong>y are short vowel sounds and writes <strong>the</strong> correspond<strong>in</strong>gsymbols of phonetic transcription-/æ/, /e/, /I/, / / and /ffl/. Students are asked to provide some examples of wordsconta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g short vowel sounds, e.g. ‘hat,’ ‘hen,’ ‘zip,’ ‘hot,’‘nut’." Teacher elicits from <strong>the</strong> students <strong>the</strong> vowels’s names (<strong>the</strong>yrevise <strong>the</strong> alphabet if necessary). The teacher writes <strong>the</strong>follow<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong> board: ‘a’-/ei/, ‘e’-/i:/, ‘i’-/ai/, ‘o’-/1y/,‘u’-/ju:/ or /u:/.


166 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Type of task2. Auditorydifferentiationbetween shortand longvowel sounds m<strong>in</strong>imal pairs.Procedure" It is expla<strong>in</strong>ed to <strong>the</strong> students that a m<strong>in</strong>imal pair is a pairof words that differ only <strong>in</strong> one sound. Here <strong>the</strong>y differ <strong>in</strong> avowel sound. Students listen to teacher read<strong>in</strong>g m<strong>in</strong>imalpairs of words and decide <strong>in</strong> which word <strong>the</strong>y hear a shortvowel sound and <strong>in</strong> which one a long vowel sound." e.g. a) ‘ship - sheep’, b) ‘note - not’, c) ‘bed - bead’, d) ‘fit -fight’, e) ‘read-red’, f) ‘cut-cute’, g) ‘bat-bait’M<strong>in</strong>imalpairShort vowelsoundLong vowelsounda 1 2b 2 1c 1 2d 1 23. Auditorydifferentiationbetween shortand longvowels oddone out." Students listen to teacher read<strong>in</strong>g sets of one-syllablewords conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g ei<strong>the</strong>r short or long vowels and choose <strong>the</strong>odd one out. Each set consists of four words. The studentsmark <strong>the</strong>ir answers <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> chart, e.g. a) ‘cut’, ‘sun’, ‘nut’,‘cute’; b) ‘w<strong>in</strong>e’, ‘five’, ‘w<strong>in</strong>’, ‘nice’; c) ‘hop’, ‘hot’, ‘hope’, ‘cot’Nr 1 2 3 4abcvvv4. ‘Magic, silentor leng<strong>the</strong>n<strong>in</strong>ge’ rule." It is made explicit to students that <strong>the</strong>re are several waysof spell<strong>in</strong>g long vowel sounds but we will concentrate on oneof <strong>the</strong>m. Teacher draws <strong>the</strong> chart* on <strong>the</strong> board with <strong>the</strong>head<strong>in</strong>gs and <strong>the</strong> first column filled up. Then she says <strong>the</strong>words ‘mad’, ‘met’, ‘w<strong>in</strong>’, ‘hop’, ‘cut’ and asks studentswhe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y conta<strong>in</strong> short or long vowel sounds. Students<strong>in</strong>dicate <strong>in</strong> which row of <strong>the</strong> second column <strong>the</strong>y should bewritten down. Next teacher asks students to write <strong>the</strong>sewords aga<strong>in</strong> add<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> ‘e’ at <strong>the</strong> end of each of <strong>the</strong>m <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>correspond<strong>in</strong>g rows of <strong>the</strong> third column. Teacher reads <strong>the</strong>words from <strong>the</strong> third column, <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> m<strong>in</strong>imal pairs andasks <strong>the</strong> students what spell<strong>in</strong>g pattern <strong>the</strong>y can see <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>words from <strong>the</strong> third column. We add <strong>the</strong> ‘e’ after <strong>the</strong> f<strong>in</strong>alconsonant and this makes <strong>the</strong> vowel long (or say its name).


Sample Activities for Learners with <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g English 167Type of taskProcedure" Rule card: Add<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> ‘e’ after <strong>the</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle f<strong>in</strong>al consonant of aone-syllable word makes <strong>the</strong> vowel <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> middle of that word long(or say its name).+1 2 3Vowel Short sound Long sounda mad madee met metei w<strong>in</strong> w<strong>in</strong>eo hop hopeu cut cute5. The /ai/sound spelledwith ‘i-e.’6. Read<strong>in</strong>gdrill ‘i-e.’" Teacher writes <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g words on <strong>the</strong> board: ‘white’,‘time’, ‘write’, ‘five’, ‘nice’, ‘p<strong>in</strong>e’, ‘like’ and reads <strong>the</strong>m.Students are asked what spell<strong>in</strong>g pattern <strong>the</strong>y can see <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong>se words. Teacher highlights <strong>the</strong> ‘i-e’ pattern <strong>in</strong> all <strong>the</strong>words and writes <strong>the</strong> ‘i-e’ rule on <strong>the</strong> board." Rule card ‘i-e’: When we hear <strong>the</strong> sound /ai/ (‘i’ say<strong>in</strong>g itsname) <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> middle of a one-syllable word followed by a s<strong>in</strong>gleconsonant sound (‘e’ is silent), we most frequently spell it with <strong>the</strong>letters ‘i-e’." Students are given flash cards with ‘i-e’ words for read<strong>in</strong>gpractice. They look at <strong>the</strong> underl<strong>in</strong>ed letter, say <strong>the</strong> sound itmakes and read a word. There is <strong>the</strong> sound /ai/ pr<strong>in</strong>ted at<strong>the</strong> left top corner and a word <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> middle of a flash card.The letters used to spell a given sound are pr<strong>in</strong>ted <strong>in</strong> boldtype, e.g.:/ai/time/ai/nice/ai/smile7. Hands onactivity ‘i-e.’" Students are given movable devices slides. The task is toform and read words with <strong>the</strong> /ai/ sound. Later <strong>the</strong>y write<strong>the</strong> words <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong> chart. The consonants are already filled <strong>in</strong>,<strong>the</strong> miss<strong>in</strong>g element <strong>in</strong> all words is <strong>the</strong> ‘i-e’ pattern.


168 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Type of taskProcedure‘i-e’ - /ai/1 t i m e2 s z3 n cstnpizmcne" Students are provided with a movable device compris<strong>in</strong>gfour piles of flash cards jo<strong>in</strong>ed at <strong>the</strong> top with a spr<strong>in</strong>g. Thefirst pile consists of white cards with consonants or <strong>in</strong>itialconsonant blends or digraphs, <strong>the</strong> second pile consists of agrey card with <strong>the</strong> letter ‘i,’ <strong>the</strong> third pile <strong>in</strong>cludes whitecards with consonants and <strong>the</strong> forth pile consists of a greycard with <strong>the</strong> letter ‘e.’ Colour cod<strong>in</strong>g is used to enhancevisual perception. Students are asked to form words bymov<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> cards, <strong>the</strong>y read and write <strong>the</strong> words. Thewords are pr<strong>in</strong>ted on <strong>the</strong> reverse side of <strong>the</strong> white cards sothat <strong>the</strong> students can check whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y have formed acorrect word.••••t• •••/ai/i••• •m• •••e• • • • • • • •••••n••••i••••c••••e8. Auditory/visual<strong>in</strong>tegration." Students are given charts with <strong>the</strong> ‘i-e’ pattern, <strong>the</strong>y listento <strong>the</strong> teacher read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> words and fill <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> miss<strong>in</strong>gconsonants, e.g.:1 _ i _ e time2 _ i _ e w<strong>in</strong>e3 _ i _ e like


Sample Activities for Learners with <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g English 169Type of taskProcedure9. Grid. " Students are asked to solve <strong>the</strong> puzzles on <strong>the</strong> left andwrite <strong>the</strong> words <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> boxes on <strong>the</strong> right. To make <strong>the</strong>exercise easier <strong>the</strong> list of words can be provided for students.Worksheet adapted from Shemesh and Waller (2000), e.g.:A number, half of ten i eA colour i eBelongs to me i eA reward i e10. Letter leads. " Students solve <strong>the</strong> puzzles and write <strong>the</strong> letters <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>boxes, one letter <strong>in</strong> each box. The letter <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> marked boxwill be <strong>the</strong> first letter of <strong>the</strong> next word. To make <strong>the</strong> exerciseeasier <strong>the</strong> list of words can be provided for studentsWorksheet adapted from Shemesh and Waller (2000), e.g.:The river of Egypt.N I L EA citrus fruit.L I M EA measure of distance.M11. Spell<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>/ai/ sound at<strong>the</strong> end of aword with <strong>the</strong>letter‘-y.’12. Hands-onactivity." Teacher writes <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g words on <strong>the</strong> board: ‘try’,‘cry’, ‘spy’, ‘fly’, ‘by’, ‘why’, ‘sky’, ‘rely’, ‘reply’, ‘shy’, ‘my’,‘July’, ‘dry’, ‘deny’ and reads <strong>the</strong>m underl<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> end<strong>in</strong>g‘-y.’ Then she asks students whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y can observe anyspell<strong>in</strong>g patterns. F<strong>in</strong>ally, she writes <strong>the</strong> rule on <strong>the</strong> board." Rule card: When we hear <strong>the</strong> /ai/ sound (‘i’ say<strong>in</strong>g its name) at<strong>the</strong> end of a word, we most frequently spell it with <strong>the</strong> letter ‘-y’.Exceptions: ‘high’, ‘sigh’, ‘thigh’, ‘nigh’, ‘tie’, ‘lie’, ‘pie’, ‘die’,‘eye’, ‘buy’, ‘bye’. When add<strong>in</strong>g a suffix to a word end<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>a consonant ‘-y,’ change <strong>the</strong> ‘-y’ <strong>in</strong>to ‘i’, e.g.: ‘sky’ ‘skies’,‘try’ ‘tries’, ‘spy’ ‘spied’." Students are given movable devices slides andmanipulate <strong>the</strong>m to form words. They are asked to readand write <strong>the</strong> words down. Colour cod<strong>in</strong>g is used to enhancevisual perception.


170 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Type of taskProcedure/ai/ytr cr sp fl b sk m" Students manipulate <strong>the</strong> movable device compris<strong>in</strong>g twopiles of cards jo<strong>in</strong>ed at <strong>the</strong> top with a spr<strong>in</strong>g to form wordsconta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> /ai/ sound spelled with <strong>the</strong> letter ‘-y’ at <strong>the</strong>end. They form, read and spell <strong>the</strong> words, e.g.:••••sk• •••y13. Fill<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>miss<strong>in</strong>g words.14. Consolidat<strong>in</strong>gspell<strong>in</strong>gpatterns for <strong>the</strong>/ai/ sound." Students are given sentences with <strong>the</strong> ‘-y’ words miss<strong>in</strong>gto fill. The words a provided, e.g.: ‘fly’, ‘by’, ‘why’, ‘my’,‘sky’, ‘July’; a) ........ do you want to buy this book?; b) Thestars are on <strong>the</strong> .......; c) In ....... we go for holiday." Students visually perceive a list of words conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>/ai/ sound and cover <strong>the</strong>m. Then, <strong>the</strong>y listen to <strong>the</strong> teacherread<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> words and decide whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> /ai/ sound isspelled with <strong>the</strong> letters ‘i-e’ or ‘-y’. The students arerem<strong>in</strong>ded that <strong>the</strong> ‘i-e’ pattern occurs when <strong>the</strong> /ai/ soundis followed by a s<strong>in</strong>gle consonant sound, whereas ‘-y’appears at <strong>the</strong> end of a word. They fill <strong>the</strong> chart by writ<strong>in</strong>ga tick under <strong>the</strong> appropriate head<strong>in</strong>g. F<strong>in</strong>ally, <strong>the</strong>y write <strong>the</strong>whole word, e.g.: 1. ‘cry’, 2. ‘w<strong>in</strong>e’, 3. ‘time’Nr ‘i-e’ ‘-y’ word1 v cry2 v w<strong>in</strong>e3 v time15. Spell<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>/ai/ soundfollowed by<strong>the</strong> letter ‘t’with <strong>the</strong> letters‘igh’." Teacher writes <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g words on <strong>the</strong> board: ‘night’,‘light’, ‘sight’, ‘bright’, ‘fight’, ‘fright’, ‘might’, ‘right’,‘tight’, ‘slight’ and reads <strong>the</strong>m underl<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> letters ‘igh’.Then she asks <strong>the</strong> students whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y can observe acommon spell<strong>in</strong>g patterns <strong>in</strong> all <strong>the</strong> words. F<strong>in</strong>ally, shewrites <strong>the</strong> rule on <strong>the</strong> board." Rule card: When we hear <strong>the</strong> /ai/ sound (‘i’ say<strong>in</strong>g its name)followed by <strong>the</strong> letter ‘t’, we often spell it with <strong>the</strong> letters ‘igh’. Thisspell<strong>in</strong>g pattern is most commonly found followed by <strong>the</strong>letter ‘t’ but it also appears as a f<strong>in</strong>al vowel sound <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>follow<strong>in</strong>g words: ‘sigh’, ‘high’, ‘thigh’, ‘nigh’. The mostcommon spell<strong>in</strong>g pattern of <strong>the</strong> sound /ai/ (‘i’ say<strong>in</strong>g its name) <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong> middle of a one-syllable word followed by <strong>the</strong> sound of a s<strong>in</strong>gleconsonant is ‘i-e’.


Sample Activities for Learners with <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g English 171Type of task16. Hands-onactivity.Procedure" Students manipulate a movable device compris<strong>in</strong>g threepiles of cards jo<strong>in</strong>ed at <strong>the</strong> top with a spr<strong>in</strong>g to form wordsconta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> /ai/ sound spelled with <strong>the</strong> letters ‘igh’ <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>middle. They read and spell <strong>the</strong> words.••••n••••igh••••t" Students are given movable devices slides andmanipulate <strong>the</strong>m to form words. They read and write <strong>the</strong>words down./ai/r fl fr br s l sl ight f t17. Consolidat<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong>‘igh’ spell<strong>in</strong>gpattern." Students are given sentences with <strong>the</strong> ‘igh’ words miss<strong>in</strong>g.They write down <strong>the</strong> appropriate words <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong> grid. Thenumber of letters <strong>in</strong> a word equals <strong>the</strong> number of boxes <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong> grid, <strong>the</strong> ‘igh’ pattern is already written <strong>in</strong>, e.g.: 1. Switchon <strong>the</strong> ...... please; 2. Did you sleep well last ......?3.Itwas a ......, sunny day.1 l i g h t2 n i g h t3 b r i g h t18. Consolidat<strong>in</strong>gspell<strong>in</strong>gpatternsfor <strong>the</strong> /ai/sound." Students perceive visually a list of words conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>/ai/ sound and cover <strong>the</strong>m. Then, <strong>the</strong>y listen to <strong>the</strong> teacherread<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> words and decide whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> /ai/ sound isspelled with <strong>the</strong> letters ‘igh’ or ‘-y’. The students arerem<strong>in</strong>ded that <strong>the</strong> ‘igh’ pattern is usually followed by ‘t’,whereas ‘-y’ appears at <strong>the</strong> end of a word. They fill <strong>the</strong> chartby writ<strong>in</strong>g a tick under <strong>the</strong> appropriate head<strong>in</strong>g and f<strong>in</strong>ally,<strong>the</strong>y write <strong>the</strong> whole word, e.g.: 1. ‘cry’, 2. ‘light’, 3. ‘bright’Nr ‘igh’ ‘-y’ word1 v cry2 v light3 v bright


172 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Type of task19. Read<strong>in</strong>gand spell<strong>in</strong>gpractice.Procedure" Students are given a chart divided <strong>in</strong>to three parts andfilled with <strong>the</strong> words conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> /ai/ sound spelled with<strong>the</strong> letters ‘i-e’, ‘igh’ and ‘-y’. The words are pr<strong>in</strong>ted without<strong>the</strong>se letters, <strong>in</strong>stead <strong>the</strong>re are dashes whose numbercorresponds to <strong>the</strong> number of <strong>the</strong> miss<strong>in</strong>g letters. Studentsrevise <strong>the</strong> rules stat<strong>in</strong>g when a given spell<strong>in</strong>g pattern shouldbe used, fill <strong>the</strong> miss<strong>in</strong>g letters, say <strong>the</strong> sound <strong>the</strong>y give andread <strong>the</strong> whole word, e.g.:/ai/‘i-e’‘igh’‘-y’t_m_, sm_l_,wh_t_,s _z_,n___t, f___t,r___t, br___t,sk _, tr _, fl _, m_,20. Read<strong>in</strong>gdrill." Students are given flash cards with words conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>/ai/ sound spelled with <strong>the</strong> letters ‘i-e,’ ‘-y’ or ‘igh’ forread<strong>in</strong>g practice. They look at <strong>the</strong> highlighted letters, say<strong>the</strong>ir names and <strong>the</strong> sound <strong>the</strong>y give. F<strong>in</strong>ally, <strong>the</strong>y read <strong>the</strong>word./ai/time/ai/sky/ai/night21. B<strong>in</strong>gogame auditory/visualmatch<strong>in</strong>g." Every student is given a different b<strong>in</strong>go board and a set oflittle white cards. First students look carefully at <strong>the</strong>ir b<strong>in</strong>goboards and try to remember <strong>the</strong> position of words, <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong>ylisten to <strong>the</strong> teacher read<strong>in</strong>g a word, f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong> word on <strong>the</strong>b<strong>in</strong>go board and cover it with a card. The first student tocover all words <strong>in</strong> one column and one row says ‘b<strong>in</strong>go’ andw<strong>in</strong>s <strong>the</strong> game, e.g.:white try right timecry might my crimefright nice shy brightwrite flight slight fly22. F<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong>odd one out." Students are given sets of words, <strong>the</strong>y are asked to listento teacher read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m, to look at <strong>the</strong>m and to choose <strong>the</strong>odd one out. They cross out <strong>the</strong> word that does not belong to<strong>the</strong> group, write it down and <strong>in</strong>dicate <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g patternthat all <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r words conta<strong>in</strong>, e.g.:


Sample Activities for Learners with <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g English 173Type of taskProcedureWordswhite timetry crimeright nicefright brightfly sky cryslightOdd oneouttryniceslightCommonpart‘i-e’‘igh’‘-y’23. Dom<strong>in</strong>oes. " Each student is given a board for dom<strong>in</strong>oes and a set ofcards. On every card <strong>the</strong>re are two words. Students aresupposed to match identical words until <strong>the</strong> board is full.When <strong>the</strong>y are all ready, <strong>the</strong>y are asked to read and write <strong>the</strong>words down, e.g.:right crime crime cry24. Consolidation spell<strong>in</strong>gboard game ‘Battleships.’" Players choose <strong>the</strong> words with <strong>the</strong> /ai/ sound <strong>the</strong>yworked on and put <strong>the</strong> counters with letters on <strong>the</strong>ir boardsor write <strong>the</strong>m down <strong>in</strong>to a grid vertically or horizontally.Players are supposed to shoot and immerse <strong>the</strong> word-shipsof <strong>the</strong>ir adversary, to read and write <strong>the</strong>m down. The studentwho manages to immerse all <strong>the</strong> word-ships of his/heradversary first is <strong>the</strong> w<strong>in</strong>ner. They shoot giv<strong>in</strong>g a possibleposition of <strong>the</strong> letter, for example, 3B or 9K, and a letter canbe ei<strong>the</strong>r missed or hit. A sample grid:12 l i g h t3 c4 n r5 i y6 c7 t e8 i f l i g h t9 m10 eA B C D E F G H I J


174 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Type of task25. Consolidation spell<strong>in</strong>gboard game ‘Letter maze’.Procedure" Players throw a dice ten times <strong>in</strong> turns and move <strong>the</strong>ircounters towards <strong>the</strong> centre of <strong>the</strong> maze collect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> letters.They are supposed to form as many words with <strong>the</strong> /ai/sound <strong>the</strong>y worked on as <strong>the</strong>y can from <strong>the</strong> letters <strong>the</strong>ycollected. They write and read <strong>the</strong> words. If <strong>the</strong> words arecorrect, <strong>the</strong> players score one po<strong>in</strong>t for every letter that hasbeen used. The one to score <strong>the</strong> most po<strong>in</strong>ts is <strong>the</strong> w<strong>in</strong>ner.Players can decide to play for more than one round hencecollect<strong>in</strong>g more letters and form<strong>in</strong>g more words." Each player chooses three words with <strong>the</strong> /ai/ sound <strong>the</strong>yworked on and writes <strong>the</strong>m down, <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong>y throw a dice <strong>in</strong>turns and move <strong>the</strong>ir counters towards <strong>the</strong> centre of <strong>the</strong> mazecollect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> letters. The players are supposed to collect all<strong>the</strong> letters that <strong>the</strong>ir words are built of, <strong>the</strong> first one to do it is<strong>the</strong> w<strong>in</strong>ner. A sample letter maze:s x u t m z i w k pv f j e k f a k g oa h d w i t h e j nj r o b c e m v o rc i c i p g w s f ee p b l z o a l w hm d n u s m c a u jh g o z b u r n x lk o t l i v q a p h→ d g a z j c u b f26. Dictation. " Students perceive visually a list of words and cover <strong>the</strong>m.Then, <strong>the</strong>y listen to teacher read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> words with <strong>the</strong> ‘i-e,’‘igh’ and ‘-y’ spell<strong>in</strong>g patterns and write <strong>the</strong>m down <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong>chart under <strong>the</strong> appropriate head<strong>in</strong>g, e.g.:‘i-e’/ai/ ‘igh’ /ai/ ‘-y’ /aï¨/1.2.3.


Sample Activities for Learners with <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g English 175Morphological Awareness ActivitiesThe supportive role of morphological awareness <strong>in</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g Englishwords is stressed by several researchers (Chliounaki & Bryant, 2007;Deacon & Bryant, 2005, 2006; Hurry et al., 2005; Jurek, 2008). Morphologymay form a clear basis for <strong>the</strong> way some words are spelled. Chliounakiand Bryant (2007) give <strong>the</strong> example of <strong>the</strong> identical end soundsrepresented by diverse spell<strong>in</strong>gs of <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g pairs of words: ‘kissed’and ‘list’, ‘socks’ and ‘box’, ‘trees’ and ‘freeze’. The first word <strong>in</strong> each pairconsists of two morphemes, <strong>the</strong> f<strong>in</strong>al morpheme <strong>in</strong> each case is ei<strong>the</strong>r apast tense or plural <strong>in</strong>flectional morpheme. Hurry et al. (2005: 188) claimthat ‘language is a morphological jigsaw which we manipulate all <strong>the</strong>time to <strong>in</strong>crease our word power’, thus equipp<strong>in</strong>g children with <strong>the</strong>knowledge and understand<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> morphological pr<strong>in</strong>ciples enables<strong>the</strong>m to enhance <strong>the</strong>ir performance on spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g tasks.Basically, morphology focuses on how words are structured fromconstituent parts. Many words can be divided <strong>in</strong>to smaller units morphemes, which constitute <strong>the</strong> smallest l<strong>in</strong>guistic units with amean<strong>in</strong>g (e.g. ‘unhappy’, ‘unlucky’, ‘unsatisfied’) or a grammaticalfunction (e.g. ‘looks’, ‘looked’). Some words cannot be fur<strong>the</strong>r brokendown (e.g. ‘car’), <strong>the</strong>y are free morphemes s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>the</strong>y can stand alone,while bound morphemes (e.g. ‘un-’, ‘-s’, or ‘-ed’) are always attachedto some o<strong>the</strong>r morphemes. There are various classes of morphemes(e.g. free and bound) and ways <strong>in</strong> which certa<strong>in</strong> morphemes cancomb<strong>in</strong>e with o<strong>the</strong>r morphemes (e.g. ‘retake’, ‘rewrite’; ‘re-’ can only beattached to verbs). Bound morphemes (or affixes) are classified <strong>in</strong>toprefixes, which attach to <strong>the</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of words, and suffixes, whichattach to <strong>the</strong> end of words. Bound morphemes can be also divided,accord<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong>ir function <strong>in</strong> complex words, <strong>in</strong>to derivational and<strong>in</strong>flectional morphemes. When derivational morphemes are added towords, <strong>the</strong>y have a power to derive (create) new words, ei<strong>the</strong>r bychang<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> orig<strong>in</strong>al word’s mean<strong>in</strong>g (e.g. ‘un-’ added to ‘happy’) orits part of speech (e.g. ‘-ness’ added to ‘quick’). Inflectional morphemesdo not form new words, but change <strong>the</strong> exist<strong>in</strong>g ones <strong>in</strong> a differentway by ref<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g and giv<strong>in</strong>g extra <strong>in</strong>formation (e.g. ‘-s’ added to ‘cat’).In English, <strong>the</strong>re are eight <strong>in</strong>flectional morphemes, and all are suffixes(‘-s’ third person s<strong>in</strong>gular present, ‘-s’ plural, ‘-ed’ past tense, ‘-<strong>in</strong>g’progressive, ‘-en’ past participle, ‘-s’ possessive, ‘-er’ comparative and‘-est’ superlative). They po<strong>in</strong>t to a syntactic and semantic relationbetween words <strong>in</strong> a given sentence and occur at <strong>the</strong> end of a word,follow<strong>in</strong>g derivational morphemes. Each word is composed of at leastone morpheme. Complex words consist of a free morpheme, <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rwords a base, root or stem, and a number of affixes (Jannedy et al.,1994).


176 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Morphological Awareness ActivitiesType of task Procedure1. Wordfamilies.Students use cards and movable devices with words and morphemes to form words belong<strong>in</strong>g to one family.For example: a) ‘educate’, ‘re-educate’, ‘educated’, ‘education’, ‘educational’, ‘uneducated’, ‘educators’;b) ‘employ’, ‘employment’, ‘unemployment’, ‘employee’, ‘employer’;c) ‘fresh’, ‘refreshments’, ‘freshly’, ‘freshman’, ‘freshness’, ‘refresher’.educate -tion educationeeun employ menteducation -al educationalerun- educational uneducationaleducate -ed educatedereducate -or educatorre fresheducator -s educatorslyment smanun- educated uneducatednessre- educate re-educate


Sample Activities for Learners with <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g English 177Type of task ProcedureSuch activities can help students realize that <strong>the</strong>y do not have to learn <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g of all <strong>the</strong> words <strong>in</strong> a familyseparately but know<strong>in</strong>g how to spell <strong>the</strong> base word and common affixes allows for spell<strong>in</strong>g impressive numberof words. O<strong>the</strong>r types of exercises <strong>in</strong>volve analys<strong>in</strong>g groups of words belong<strong>in</strong>g to a given family <strong>in</strong> order toidentify and to split <strong>the</strong>m <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong> base word and affixes, classify<strong>in</strong>g words <strong>in</strong>to word families, f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>words that do not belong to a given family (Jurek, 2008).2. F<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong>words that donot belong to a‘belief’ family.beliefs disbelief believable beautifully disbelieveunbeatable unbelievably believer unbelievable non-believer3. Choose <strong>the</strong>odd one out <strong>in</strong>each l<strong>in</strong>e.differently differential difficult difference different differcentral certa<strong>in</strong> centralize centrally center centralizationperfect perfection prefect imperfection perfectionist imperfect4. Sort <strong>the</strong>cards to formtwo wordfamilies.friendpa<strong>in</strong>killerfriendlypa<strong>in</strong>fully pa<strong>in</strong>fulfriendshipunfriendlypa<strong>in</strong>pa<strong>in</strong>less


178 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Type of task Procedure5. Divide <strong>the</strong>words <strong>in</strong>to twofamilies.cleaner cleanl<strong>in</strong>ess cleanser close closet cleanly cleanestdisclosure cleans<strong>in</strong>g closely cleanest enclosed clean<strong>in</strong>g6. Do <strong>the</strong>follow<strong>in</strong>g pairsof wordsbelong to <strong>the</strong>same family.friend/friendl<strong>in</strong>ess casual/causal enable/unablepray<strong>in</strong>g/play<strong>in</strong>g fil<strong>in</strong>g/fill<strong>in</strong>g science/scientist7. Split <strong>the</strong>words <strong>in</strong>to itsconstituentmorphemeswrite <strong>the</strong>m oncards.formalize <strong>in</strong>formallyformally formalist<strong>in</strong>formal8. Split <strong>the</strong>words <strong>in</strong>to itsconstituentmorphemes.correct corrective correctional <strong>in</strong>correct <strong>in</strong>correctlycorrections correctly correctness


Sample Activities for Learners with <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g English 179Grammatical Awareness ActivitiesA number of activities for develop<strong>in</strong>g grammatical awareness presentedbelow are helpful <strong>in</strong> analys<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> separate constituent elements of alanguage (Nijakowska, 2007). Some students do not develop sufficientgrammatical awareness along with communicative effectiveness despitefully experiential and mean<strong>in</strong>gful classroom second language learn<strong>in</strong>g.These limitations can be reduced by <strong>the</strong> pedagogical <strong>in</strong>terventions<strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g rais<strong>in</strong>g grammatical awareness or, <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r words, focus<strong>in</strong>gon form. Thornbury (1999) stresses <strong>the</strong> need to po<strong>in</strong>t out language items<strong>in</strong> order to help <strong>the</strong> learners notice <strong>the</strong>m and, <strong>in</strong> so do<strong>in</strong>g, support andspeed up <strong>the</strong> acquisition process.The study of grammar, provided it is not treated as an end <strong>in</strong> itself butone of <strong>the</strong> ways to achieve a thorough mastery of a language, helps tolearn to use <strong>the</strong> language, and by no means has to be perceived as abor<strong>in</strong>g, dry and de-motivat<strong>in</strong>g experience. S<strong>in</strong>ce fun and play<strong>in</strong>gcompose a vital part of children’s grow<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>the</strong>y naturallyform a fertile ground for <strong>in</strong>corporat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m <strong>in</strong>to grammar practiceactivities and create opportunities for multiple repetition and reproductionof grammatical material, which, consequently, leads to automatisation.The abovementioned natural characteristic of children allowsteachers to use games, project work, crafts (e.g. various movable teach<strong>in</strong>gdevices made by children), surveys (simple repetitive activities but withclear communicative focus), movement and multisensory techniques <strong>in</strong>order to make children focus on grammatical concepts. This, <strong>in</strong> turn,allows children to acquire <strong>the</strong> feel<strong>in</strong>g of what is grammatically accurateand offers chances for <strong>in</strong>tegrat<strong>in</strong>g language skills as well. Learnersbecome aware of <strong>the</strong> constituent elements and <strong>the</strong>ir proper order as wellas <strong>the</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g of a given grammatical structure. Stimulat<strong>in</strong>g andlearner-friendly activities, <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g play<strong>in</strong>g, manipulat<strong>in</strong>g and mov<strong>in</strong>garound, provide an excellent opportunity for practis<strong>in</strong>g grammar. Whatfollows is a set of sample grammar activities.


180 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Grammatical Awareness ActivitiesType of task Procedure Comments1. Colourpars<strong>in</strong>g." <strong>Language</strong>: Present Simple general questions; materials: coloured chalk,coloured pencils; time: 2030 m<strong>in</strong>." Four colours are needed: red for verbs (‘like‘, ’love‘, ’hate’); blue for subjectpronouns (‘I‘, ’you‘, ’she‘, ’he‘, ’etc’.); yellow for auxiliary verbs (‘do‘, ’does’)and green for nouns for food (‘pizza‘, ’coffee‘, ’tea‘, ’bananas‘, ’tomatoes’)." Teacher divides <strong>the</strong> board <strong>in</strong>to two halves. On <strong>the</strong> left she writes some wordsthat fit <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong> chosen sentence structure." Teacher underl<strong>in</strong>es <strong>the</strong> verb <strong>in</strong> red and <strong>in</strong>vites children to underl<strong>in</strong>e o<strong>the</strong>r redwords. Teacher does <strong>the</strong> same with blue, yellow and green words."Teacher writes her model sentence on <strong>the</strong> right of <strong>the</strong> board and asks <strong>the</strong>children to underl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong> words <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> appropriate colours." Teacher shows <strong>the</strong> children how to make o<strong>the</strong>r sentences like hers, us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>words on <strong>the</strong> left. Then <strong>the</strong>y make some of <strong>the</strong>ir own. They read <strong>the</strong> sentencesand write under <strong>the</strong> model." Children do a questionnaire. Before children go around <strong>the</strong> room, teacherasks <strong>the</strong> questions and notes down <strong>the</strong> answers ‘Yes’ and ‘No,’ makes <strong>the</strong>mform <strong>the</strong> questions and writes <strong>the</strong>m on <strong>the</strong> board as well as makes sure <strong>the</strong>yknow how to correctly pronounce <strong>the</strong> questions.The activity is an example ofa simple language analysiscomb<strong>in</strong>ed with <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>troductionof matalanguage. Childrenstudy sentences such as‘Do you like bananas?’ analys<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong> component parts of <strong>the</strong>structure auxiliary pronoun verb noun. Colour cod<strong>in</strong>gis used to help childrenconduct <strong>the</strong> analysis. Whatfollows is a controlled practicestage <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> form of a spokentask with clear communicativepurpose (<strong>in</strong>formation gapactivity), though with alanguage scope limited to <strong>the</strong>target structure only.


Sample Activities for Learners with <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g English 181Type of task Procedure CommentsF<strong>in</strong>d: Name1. someone wholikes bananas2. three people wholove coffee3. two people wholike pizza4. three people wholove chocolate5. someone whohates sp<strong>in</strong>ach6. two people whohate milkAlice John Tom JaneNoNoYesYesNoNoIn a communicative drill,such as <strong>the</strong> one used <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>activity, learners are supposedto use given grammar structuresseveral times <strong>in</strong> order tof<strong>in</strong>d out <strong>the</strong> lack<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation.This multiple repetitiongreatly aids automatisation,while <strong>the</strong> focus on mean<strong>in</strong>gis exercised through <strong>the</strong> use of<strong>in</strong>formation gap. Learnersdemonstrate a clear communicativepurpose and satisfy<strong>the</strong> need for real communicationby bridg<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> gap.Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, by nature, <strong>the</strong><strong>in</strong>teraction is reciprocal, itrequires both, listen<strong>in</strong>g andspeak<strong>in</strong>g." As a follow up, children may be asked to analyse <strong>the</strong> answers and formaffirmative sentences (revision), for example, ‘John likes bananas’. ‘Alice, Tomand Jane like coffee’ (idea adapted from Phillips, 1993).


182 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Type of task Procedure Comments2. Demonstrationwithmovement." <strong>Language</strong>: Present Simple Passive; materials: colourful cardboard, flashcards;time: 3040 m<strong>in</strong>." Three piles of colourful cardboard green, yellow, blue are placed on <strong>the</strong>floor <strong>in</strong> a row. The first one represents subject, <strong>the</strong> second one verb and <strong>the</strong> lastone object.subject verb objectto be (am, is are) + past participlesubject verb objectbyACTIVE VOICEPASSIVE VOICE" By mov<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> blank cards from <strong>the</strong> tops of <strong>the</strong> files and plac<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>appropriate positions we demonstrate how <strong>the</strong> Present Simple Passiveisformedsubject becomes object, object becomes subject, verbischangedaccord<strong>in</strong>gto<strong>the</strong>pattern.Colour cod<strong>in</strong>g can be aidedby movement <strong>in</strong> tasks devotedto language analysis and metalanguage<strong>in</strong>troduction. Explanationprocedure does nothave to<strong>in</strong>volve a blackboard,though a teacher is free toattach colourful cardboards to<strong>the</strong> board. In activity 2 enoughspace must be secured on <strong>the</strong>floor for students to movealong accord<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> patternand thus demonstrat<strong>in</strong>g howlanguage structure is formed.This activity <strong>in</strong>volves varioussensory modalities visual,auditory, and k<strong>in</strong>aes<strong>the</strong>tic. Itsmultisensory character allowsbetter understand<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong>presented grammar structureadditionally enhanc<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terestand motivation. The


Sample Activities for Learners with <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g English 183Type of task Procedure Comments" Then we write parts of a sample sentence on <strong>the</strong> appropriate cards and askthree volunteers to help to demonstrate <strong>the</strong> example. Each of <strong>the</strong> volunteersstands beh<strong>in</strong>d one pile <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> first row, <strong>the</strong>n takes <strong>the</strong> card with <strong>the</strong> word, movestowards <strong>the</strong> appropriate pile <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> second row and, f<strong>in</strong>ally, puts <strong>the</strong> card down." Teacher writes more examples on <strong>the</strong> cards, asks students to form somesentences <strong>in</strong> passive voice by mov<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> cards from <strong>the</strong> piles <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> upper rowand plac<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> appropriate positions <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> lower row. Next, he or shemakes <strong>the</strong>m draw slips of paper with sentences <strong>in</strong> active voice and <strong>the</strong>ntransform <strong>the</strong> sentence <strong>in</strong>to passive voice. This time <strong>the</strong>y only move accord<strong>in</strong>g to<strong>the</strong> pattern while pronounc<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> sentences.drill performed <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> activityis far from dull and bor<strong>in</strong>g.John loves MaryACTIVE VOICEMary is lovedbyJohnPASSIVE VOICE


184 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Type of task Procedure Comments3. Stepp<strong>in</strong>gstones." Real-life-size game or a board game; language: any grammar structure;materials: two pieces of ribbon to mark <strong>the</strong> banks of <strong>the</strong> river on <strong>the</strong> floor; somecardboard stones across it; cardboard frogs for each team; one pile of cards withquestions and, optionally, piles of cards with answers for each team; time:2030m<strong>in</strong>." Students are divided <strong>in</strong>to teams. Teams take turns <strong>in</strong> draw<strong>in</strong>g cards withquestions, <strong>the</strong>y read questions aloud and try to give <strong>the</strong> answer (or match <strong>the</strong>answer). If <strong>the</strong> answer is correct, <strong>the</strong> team scores one po<strong>in</strong>t. For each po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>the</strong>frog goes forward one stone. The first team to reach <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r side of <strong>the</strong> riverw<strong>in</strong>s <strong>the</strong> game (idea adapted from Phillips, 1993).Game is a technique that hasbeen repeatedly proved towork well with children. Ur(1988) argues that it is apleasurable tension felt by<strong>the</strong> participants that makesup <strong>the</strong> success of <strong>the</strong>game-like grammar practiceprocedures.Game-like activities 3, 4, 5and 6 can be successfully<strong>in</strong>corporated <strong>in</strong>to grammarpractice procedures and usedas templates accommodatedtowards <strong>the</strong> requirements of<strong>the</strong> program and needs ofstudents. Various simple andmore sophisticated grammarpatters can be practiced thisway.


Sample Activities for Learners with <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g English 185Type of task Procedure Comments4. Board game. " <strong>Language</strong>: any aspect of grammar; materials: boards snakes and ladders; apile of cards with questions on one side and answers on <strong>the</strong> reverse side; a pileof cards with forfeits and risks; a dice; counters; time: 2030m<strong>in</strong>." The first player throws a dice and moves <strong>the</strong> counter. If he or she lands on asquare with a question mark on it, he/she takes a card from <strong>the</strong> pile and tries toanswer it and <strong>the</strong>n checks <strong>the</strong> answer on <strong>the</strong> reverse side of <strong>the</strong> card." If <strong>the</strong> student gets it wrong, he/she goes to <strong>the</strong> previous position. If <strong>the</strong>student gets it right, he/she moves forward <strong>the</strong> same number of squares aga<strong>in</strong>.If students land on <strong>the</strong> squares with <strong>the</strong> snakes or lea<strong>the</strong>rs, <strong>the</strong>y climb up orslide down. The w<strong>in</strong>ner is <strong>the</strong> player who reaches <strong>the</strong> last square first." Forfeits and risk spaces are <strong>in</strong>cluded along <strong>the</strong> game path. If children land ona square with a star, <strong>the</strong>y must take a card from a special pile and do whatever iswritten on it, for example, jump, s<strong>in</strong>g, say a poem, wait a turn, move twosquares back, roll a dice once aga<strong>in</strong>, spell <strong>the</strong>ir name, move forward threespaces, etc. (idea adapted from R<strong>in</strong>volucri, 1985).


186 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Type of task Procedure Comments5. Play<strong>in</strong>gcards." <strong>Language</strong>: any aspect of grammar, e.g. Present Simple Passive, materials: oneset of cards (12 pairs one additional card) for each group of four students;time: 1520m<strong>in</strong>." Teacher prepares sets of 12 pairs of cards consist<strong>in</strong>g of one card with asentence <strong>in</strong> an active voice and <strong>the</strong> correspond<strong>in</strong>g card with <strong>the</strong> sentencetransformed <strong>in</strong>to passive voice. One card without a pair is added." Teacher shuffles <strong>the</strong> cards and deals between <strong>the</strong> players. They cannot show<strong>the</strong>ir cards to <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r players." Students take turns <strong>in</strong> draw<strong>in</strong>g cards from each o<strong>the</strong>r clockwise. If <strong>the</strong>y f<strong>in</strong>dany pairs, <strong>the</strong>y put <strong>the</strong>m on <strong>the</strong> table face up, so that o<strong>the</strong>r players can check,whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y are correctly matched or not." The first person to get rid of all of <strong>the</strong> cards is <strong>the</strong> w<strong>in</strong>ner, o<strong>the</strong>r studentscont<strong>in</strong>ue <strong>the</strong> game until one of <strong>the</strong>m stays with a s<strong>in</strong>gle card which cannot bematched to any o<strong>the</strong>r card.


Sample Activities for Learners with <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g English 187Type of task Procedure CommentsQuestion cardsMrs Harriscooks ourmeals.Answer cardsOur meals arecooked byMrs Harris.Question cardsPolicemen helpold ladies.Answer cardsOld ladies arehelped bypolicemen.Ann loves dogs.Dogs are lovedby Ann.My bro<strong>the</strong>rwrites books.Books arewritten by mybro<strong>the</strong>r.People drivecars.Cars are drivenby people.They makedelicious cakes.Deliciouscakes aremade by <strong>the</strong>m.He growsbeautiful plants.Beautiful plantsare grown byhim.Millions ofpeople watchthis program.This programis watched bymillions of people.Our mum cleans<strong>the</strong> house.The house iscleaned by ourmum.A lot of peoplespeak English.English isspoken by alot of people.My sisterstudies history.History isstudied by mysister.John kissedMary.Marry waskissed byJohn.You lose <strong>the</strong>gameThis sentence doesnot have a pair.


188 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Type of task Procedure Comments6. Play<strong>in</strong>gcards Snap." <strong>Language</strong>: any grammar structure, e.g. Present Simple Passive; materials: oneset of cards (20 pairs) for each pair of students; <strong>the</strong> cards from <strong>the</strong> previous gamecan be used here; time: 1520m<strong>in</strong>." Teacher prepares sets of 20 pairs of cards consist<strong>in</strong>g of one card with asentence <strong>in</strong> an active voice and <strong>the</strong> correspond<strong>in</strong>g card with <strong>the</strong> sentencetransformed <strong>in</strong>to passive voice. Teacher shuffles <strong>the</strong> cards and deals between <strong>the</strong>players." Students keep <strong>the</strong>ir cards on a pile face down. Each of <strong>the</strong>m takes a card from<strong>the</strong> top of <strong>the</strong>ir files and puts it on <strong>the</strong> table face up. If <strong>the</strong> cards do not match,<strong>the</strong>y leave <strong>the</strong>m on <strong>the</strong> table. However, if <strong>the</strong>y constitute a pair, <strong>the</strong> students areto say ‘Snap!’ and cover <strong>the</strong>m with a hand. The first one to do it takes all <strong>the</strong>cards that are on <strong>the</strong> table. The student who collects all <strong>the</strong> cards is <strong>the</strong> w<strong>in</strong>ner.


Sample Activities for Learners with <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g English 189Note1. Sample task types, teach<strong>in</strong>g aids and worksheets used <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> small-scale<strong>in</strong>tervention study <strong>in</strong> dyslexia and EFL described <strong>in</strong> Chapter 5.


AfterwordDevelopmental dyslexia can br<strong>in</strong>g about serious educational consequences,which leave <strong>the</strong>ir impr<strong>in</strong>t on <strong>the</strong> broadly perceived well-be<strong>in</strong>g compris<strong>in</strong>g social, emotional and also economic aspects of an<strong>in</strong>dividual, dur<strong>in</strong>g school years and later on <strong>in</strong> adulthood. Thoroughunderstand<strong>in</strong>g of this cognitive disorder constitutes a prerequisite forundertak<strong>in</strong>g any action aimed at reduc<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> impact that dyslexia canexert on one’s function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> school environment, which too oftenreduces future choices with regard to a professional career. To preventsuch consequences through creat<strong>in</strong>g a dyslexia-friendly approach, fairattention and effort of all <strong>the</strong> stakeholders <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> process ofupbr<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g on different levels home, school, government seems amust.However, understand<strong>in</strong>g dyslexia is an extremely difficult task <strong>in</strong>itself, especially <strong>in</strong> light of <strong>the</strong> complex, and, <strong>in</strong> many aspects, conflict<strong>in</strong>goutcome of a massive research, conducted <strong>in</strong> an attempt to del<strong>in</strong>eate <strong>the</strong>mechanisms underly<strong>in</strong>g specific read<strong>in</strong>g disorders. As such, <strong>the</strong> study ofdyslexia is very much <strong>in</strong>terdiscipl<strong>in</strong>ary <strong>in</strong> nature; it <strong>in</strong>vites expertise andappreciates knowledge <strong>in</strong> such scientific discipl<strong>in</strong>es as neurobiology,genetics, cognitive psychology, l<strong>in</strong>guistics and pedagogy. Search<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>sefields for converg<strong>in</strong>g pathways, hopefully allow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> creation of a morecomplete picture of <strong>the</strong> phenomenon <strong>in</strong> question, is constantly <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>course of action, with new bits and pieces added regularly. For <strong>in</strong>stance,<strong>the</strong> advances <strong>in</strong> technology greatly enhance <strong>the</strong> grasp of dyslexia; <strong>in</strong>particular, <strong>the</strong> use of non-<strong>in</strong>vasive bra<strong>in</strong> imag<strong>in</strong>g techniques for <strong>the</strong>exam<strong>in</strong>ation of regularities and abnormalities <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> structure andfunction<strong>in</strong>g is of significant help <strong>in</strong> understand<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> disorder. Thisneurobiological <strong>in</strong>sight <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong> mechanisms of dyslexia uncovers newdetails and facts, which could not have been unveiled previously.Never<strong>the</strong>less, however promis<strong>in</strong>g a start such an <strong>in</strong>terdiscipl<strong>in</strong>ary and<strong>in</strong>tensive research approach may seem, it can well turn out to be abless<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> disguise, produc<strong>in</strong>g more questions and controversies thananswers. Indeed, this seems <strong>the</strong> case with dyslexia. Clash<strong>in</strong>g researchf<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are pil<strong>in</strong>g up, particularly with reference to <strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>gcauses of <strong>the</strong> disorder, <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretation of which poses considerableproblems some see <strong>the</strong> evidence as complementary, some as ra<strong>the</strong>rcontradict<strong>in</strong>g perspectives.190


Afterword 191Apparently, dyslexia has become one of <strong>the</strong> most <strong>in</strong>tensively <strong>in</strong>vestigatedand best known cognitive disorders; still, <strong>the</strong> rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g controversyand uncerta<strong>in</strong>ty is also mirrored <strong>in</strong> teachers’ hesitation anddoubtfulness as to how <strong>the</strong>y can most successfully deal with childrenwith dyslexia (e.g. dyslexia diagnosis <strong>in</strong> a bil<strong>in</strong>gual sett<strong>in</strong>g). Severalprevail<strong>in</strong>g misconceptions about <strong>the</strong> mechanisms, symptoms and teach<strong>in</strong>gapproaches are at large, which, most probably, can be attributed to <strong>the</strong>gap <strong>the</strong> mismatch between <strong>the</strong> contemporary sophisticated scientificknowledge and practice. From <strong>the</strong> practitioner’s standpo<strong>in</strong>t, <strong>the</strong> outcomeof <strong>the</strong> prolific multidirectional research on dyslexia needs translat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>topractical applications <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> school sett<strong>in</strong>g and, vice versa, research canalso be <strong>in</strong>formed by <strong>the</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>valuable teacher classroomobservation. In fact, it nicely matches <strong>the</strong> idea of <strong>the</strong> three different levelsof explanation biological, cognitive and behavioural and <strong>the</strong> dynamic,bidirectional <strong>in</strong>teraction between <strong>the</strong>m, with no one level of greaterimportance than <strong>the</strong> rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g levels.How does this idea affect practitioners teachers, who confrontvarious behavioural facets of dyslexia every day? It does, even thoughneuroscience seems so distant from didactics, it lends considerablesupport <strong>in</strong> br<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g answers to <strong>the</strong> questions about <strong>the</strong> most effectivetreatments, tutor<strong>in</strong>g and teach<strong>in</strong>g approaches. It serves well <strong>in</strong> assess<strong>in</strong>gand verify<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> environmental <strong>in</strong>fluences (e.g. <strong>the</strong> effects of teach<strong>in</strong>gand learn<strong>in</strong>g) on bra<strong>in</strong> function<strong>in</strong>g. The neurobiological foundations are,<strong>in</strong> turn, claimed to underlie and determ<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong> cognitive function<strong>in</strong>g ofan <strong>in</strong>dividual, hence an <strong>in</strong>-depth <strong>in</strong>vestigation of <strong>the</strong> cognitive processesand <strong>the</strong>ir disorders, to which dyslexia can be traced. The l<strong>in</strong>k betweenbiology, cognition and behaviour, all of which undergo environmental<strong>in</strong>fluences, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> study of dyslexia seems most apparent. A thoroughunderstand<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> processes specific to each of <strong>the</strong> three levels isnecessary to build up a complete image of <strong>the</strong> phenomenon (Frith, 1999).This is so because a s<strong>in</strong>gle symptom at <strong>the</strong> behavioural level may equallyfollow from different impairments at <strong>the</strong> biological and cognitive levels.The picture becomes even more complicated because a particularimpairment at <strong>the</strong> cognitive or biological level is well capable ofcontribut<strong>in</strong>g to more than one deficit at <strong>the</strong> behavioural level (Helland,2007). Nicolson (2001) uses a medical model to illustrate <strong>the</strong> situation.Such a model comprises three elements: a cause, a symptom and atreatment. Obviously, several diseases may have similar symptoms, but<strong>the</strong>y markedly differ as to causes and treatments, which is why <strong>the</strong> use offur<strong>the</strong>r, more sensitive tests, adm<strong>in</strong>istered by specialists, constitutes anecessary condition to determ<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong> true underly<strong>in</strong>g cause and thus <strong>the</strong>proper treatment.The complexity of <strong>the</strong> phenomenon of dyslexia, largely brought aboutby its multiple behavioural manifestations, has given rise to numerous


192 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>and, <strong>in</strong>deed, at times controversial etiological <strong>the</strong>ories of <strong>the</strong> disorder.Never<strong>the</strong>less, <strong>the</strong>y have ga<strong>in</strong>ed substantial <strong>in</strong>terest and generated a greatdeal of research, <strong>in</strong>vit<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tense academic debates <strong>in</strong> recent years. Inorder to avoid confusion <strong>in</strong> dyslexia research, a need for consensus ishighlighted, s<strong>in</strong>ce, quite naturally, <strong>the</strong> understand<strong>in</strong>g of such a complicatedcondition would require a complex, <strong>in</strong>terdiscipl<strong>in</strong>ary cooperation,lead<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> formulation of a causal <strong>the</strong>ory of dyslexia. Such a <strong>the</strong>ory, asit stands, should be capable of expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> symptoms, <strong>the</strong>ir underly<strong>in</strong>gcauses as well as an optimal <strong>the</strong>rapeutic and support system(Fawcett & Nicolson, 2001; Nicolson, 2001; Reid & Fawcett, 2004).<strong>Dyslexia</strong> is a neurodevelopmental disorder. Adopt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> developmentalperspective <strong>in</strong> an approach to <strong>in</strong>vestigate dyslexia requires deepawareness of <strong>the</strong> typical development of <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g skill first and <strong>the</strong>ncompar<strong>in</strong>g it to what can be observed <strong>in</strong> children with dyslexia. Fromsuch a standpo<strong>in</strong>t, dyslexia can be described as a delay <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>gdevelopment, hence perceiv<strong>in</strong>g it <strong>in</strong> dimensional terms seems mostjustified. It means that read<strong>in</strong>g ability can be illustrated <strong>in</strong> a form ofcont<strong>in</strong>uum, thus considerable <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> acquir<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>skill come <strong>in</strong>to play. Individuals may suffer from dyslexia to vary<strong>in</strong>gdegrees; still, <strong>the</strong>y would occupy <strong>the</strong> bottom end of <strong>the</strong> proposedcont<strong>in</strong>uum. However, categorical descriptions of <strong>the</strong> disorder apparentlyare <strong>in</strong> use, despite an obvious difficulty to decide on <strong>the</strong> arbitrary cut-offpo<strong>in</strong>t on <strong>the</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>uum, after reach<strong>in</strong>g which, one would qualify to bediagnosed as hav<strong>in</strong>g dyslexia (Hulme & Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2009).Perceiv<strong>in</strong>g dyslexia as a dimensional construct, as a delay, alters <strong>the</strong>way we look at <strong>the</strong> improvement <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g skill. Development isdynamic and characterised by change; quite naturally, <strong>the</strong> same holdstrue for <strong>the</strong> symptoms of dyslexia. Some of <strong>the</strong> symptoms are observableat <strong>the</strong> onset of formal school<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction and, later, canreduce or disappear, which by no means implies that <strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>gimpairment, most probably <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> form of phonological process<strong>in</strong>gdeficit, vanishes (Frith, 2008). O<strong>the</strong>r signs, i.e. spell<strong>in</strong>g difficulty, seem topersist <strong>in</strong>to adulthood. So, <strong>the</strong> behavioural manifestations of dyslexiachange over <strong>the</strong> course of time and development. Does this mean thatone can grow out of dyslexia? This is where <strong>the</strong> categorical labels becomeproblematic, while a dimensional description offers a more plausibleexplanation dyslexia is a life-long condition, with considerable <strong>in</strong>terand<strong>in</strong>tra-variation with reference to behavioural facets. Once diagnosed,one rema<strong>in</strong>s dyslexic; however, as a result of compensation activities andlearn<strong>in</strong>g, one becomes able to successfully handle tasks that used to posegreat demand and moves up along <strong>the</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>uum of <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g ability.As for <strong>the</strong> characteristic symptoms, <strong>the</strong> fundamental difficulty lies <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong> acquisition of skilful word decod<strong>in</strong>g (read<strong>in</strong>g) and encod<strong>in</strong>g(spell<strong>in</strong>g), leav<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> child beh<strong>in</strong>d his/her peers with regard to literacy


Afterword 193development. The language process<strong>in</strong>g system is selectively impaired <strong>in</strong>children with dyslexia, ma<strong>in</strong>ly with reference to phonological process<strong>in</strong>g,while o<strong>the</strong>r aspects of language are ra<strong>the</strong>r normal. Thus, <strong>the</strong> characteristicdeficit <strong>in</strong> dyslexia seems to perta<strong>in</strong> to poor quality of phonologicalrepresentations of speech sounds, which are to support spoken wordrecognition and production. Dyslexic phonological representations tendto be <strong>in</strong>accurately specified and deprived of dist<strong>in</strong>ctness, which is how<strong>the</strong> phonological process<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia arise(Goswami, 2000; Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2001a). Additionally, learn<strong>in</strong>g to readrequires <strong>the</strong> capacity to map letter str<strong>in</strong>gs of pr<strong>in</strong>ted words (orthography)on phonemic sequences that spoken words are built of (phonology).Normally develop<strong>in</strong>g children are able to grasp <strong>the</strong> relations between <strong>the</strong>spell<strong>in</strong>g patterns and <strong>the</strong>ir pronunciations (<strong>the</strong> alphabetic pr<strong>in</strong>ciple) andacquire a powerful tool that allows <strong>the</strong>m to fur<strong>the</strong>r explore <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>t(Lundberg & Hoien, 2001; Shaywitz, 1997; Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, 2001a). On <strong>the</strong>contrary, children with dyslexia are claimed to have noticeable trouble <strong>in</strong>establish<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> connections between phonemes and <strong>the</strong>ir graphicrepresentations as well as <strong>in</strong> generalis<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ir knowledge <strong>in</strong> order toread unfamiliar words.In fact, across numerous studies, poor readers consistently demonstratesignificantly worse performances on phonological awareness andletter-to-sound decod<strong>in</strong>g tasks <strong>in</strong> comparison to good readers. Inaddition, <strong>the</strong> most <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g and excit<strong>in</strong>g evidence for poor phonologicalskills constitut<strong>in</strong>g a manifest cause of read<strong>in</strong>g failure <strong>in</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>greaders comes from tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g and <strong>in</strong>tervention studies (Vellut<strong>in</strong>o et al.,2004), where direct <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> phonological awareness and lettersoundcorrespondences repeatedly proved highly effective <strong>in</strong> enhanc<strong>in</strong>gword identification facility as well as spell<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g skills. Thestrongest effects on read<strong>in</strong>g acquisition are noticed when phonologicaltra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g is comb<strong>in</strong>ed with <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> letter-sound mapp<strong>in</strong>gs.Th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g about <strong>the</strong> time and course of development of <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>gskill, one cannot ignore <strong>the</strong> multitude of factors <strong>in</strong>fluenc<strong>in</strong>g it, amongwhich a prom<strong>in</strong>ent position is held by <strong>the</strong> language-specific factors.Differences <strong>in</strong> literacy acquisition read<strong>in</strong>g accuracy and speed dependto a great extent on <strong>the</strong> orthographic system of a given language. Thus,we can expect that typical development of a read<strong>in</strong>g skill differs acrosslanguages and so do <strong>the</strong> behavioural manifestations of dyslexia. Themore transparent <strong>the</strong> phoneme-grapheme mapp<strong>in</strong>g system of a language,<strong>the</strong> fewer difficulties it poses on <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia learn<strong>in</strong>g toread <strong>in</strong> it, irrespective of whe<strong>the</strong>r it is a native, a second or a foreignlanguage. This is so because <strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>g core phonological deficit,caus<strong>in</strong>g delay <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g development <strong>in</strong> one’s native language, exertssubstantial <strong>in</strong>fluence on <strong>the</strong> acquisition of <strong>the</strong> consecutive languages.


194 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Hardly anyone would argue aga<strong>in</strong>st a decent command of a foreignlanguage, or even languages, constitut<strong>in</strong>g a tremendous asset <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>contemporary world. <strong>Foreign</strong> language faculty is no longer perceived aluxury and an extra quality enhanc<strong>in</strong>g job prospects, quite <strong>the</strong> opposite,foreign language requirement has become a regular, compulsory part ofeducational systems and one of <strong>the</strong> elementary skills expected ofcandidates for many professional positions. How does such expectationsrelate to dyslexia? Individuals with dyslexia are prone to encounterdifficulties <strong>in</strong> foreign language study, vary<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> scope and <strong>in</strong>tensity,because foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g is built upon native language skills.Strong native language competence fosters foreign language acquisition,while poor native language codes significantly determ<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong> degree ofproficiency <strong>in</strong> a foreign language. Most poor foreign language learnersseem to f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong> phonological/orthographic process<strong>in</strong>g particularlytroublesome (Sparks et al., 1989).An <strong>in</strong>trigu<strong>in</strong>g question arises whe<strong>the</strong>r all poor foreign languagelearners are dyslexic and, more importantly, whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>in</strong>dividuals withdyslexia can achieve average or above average competence <strong>in</strong> a foreignlanguage. Similar to read<strong>in</strong>g ability, also <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> case of foreign languageproficiency, a dimensional ra<strong>the</strong>r that categorical perspective is adopted.<strong>Foreign</strong> language ability, with regard to <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia as wellas low-achiev<strong>in</strong>g but non-disabled learners, can be represented on acont<strong>in</strong>uum, with <strong>the</strong> foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties rang<strong>in</strong>g frommild to severe. It seems reasonable to expect that most students withdyslexia would occupy <strong>the</strong> bottom part <strong>the</strong> more severe end of <strong>the</strong>cont<strong>in</strong>uum. We can tentatively expect that <strong>the</strong> degree to which <strong>the</strong>placement of a given <strong>in</strong>dividual with dyslexia on <strong>the</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>uum ofread<strong>in</strong>g development <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> native and foreign language parallel woulddepend on numerous factors. Among <strong>the</strong>se factors, <strong>the</strong> type and severityof <strong>the</strong> disorder, <strong>in</strong> particular <strong>the</strong> strength of <strong>the</strong> phonological skills, and<strong>the</strong> characteristics of <strong>the</strong> phonological/orthographic system of a languagecan be listed. So, it seems a <strong>the</strong>oretically sound assumption that,even though <strong>the</strong> majority of students with dyslexia would be expected toshow vary<strong>in</strong>g degrees of difficulty <strong>in</strong> a foreign language study, <strong>the</strong>y arecapable of achiev<strong>in</strong>g average results, provided an adequate teach<strong>in</strong>gapproach is adopted. Still, despite undeniable progress, <strong>the</strong>y would stillslightly lag beh<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong> non-dyslexic <strong>in</strong>dividuals. However, <strong>the</strong>re wouldalso be learners with dyslexia reach<strong>in</strong>g above-average foreign languagecompetence.As much as <strong>the</strong>re is a wide consensus on <strong>the</strong> need for early diagnosisand <strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> monol<strong>in</strong>gual sett<strong>in</strong>gs, it is lack<strong>in</strong>g with respect tomultil<strong>in</strong>gual backgrounds. It is true that <strong>in</strong>terest and sensitivity isconstantly grow<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong> issue of diagnos<strong>in</strong>g dyslexia <strong>in</strong> children be<strong>in</strong>gbrought up <strong>in</strong> bil<strong>in</strong>gual, multil<strong>in</strong>gual and multicultural contexts, which is


Afterword 195reflected <strong>in</strong> research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. The situation concerns children from ethnicand l<strong>in</strong>guistic m<strong>in</strong>orities, who use <strong>the</strong>ir native language at home andsecond language at school. Frequently, it is <strong>the</strong> case that <strong>the</strong>y possessra<strong>the</strong>r limited competence <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> second language prior to <strong>the</strong> commencementof formal <strong>in</strong>struction at school. The controversy that ariseshere is ma<strong>in</strong>ly whe<strong>the</strong>r we should wait for <strong>the</strong>se children to acquire anacceptable level of spoken proficiency <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> second language before <strong>the</strong>ycan become liable to <strong>the</strong> valid identification towards dyslexia <strong>in</strong> thatlanguage. This assumption is apparently based on <strong>the</strong> commonly sharedview that <strong>in</strong> order to develop skilful read<strong>in</strong>g, one needs to have adequatespeak<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> second language. However, <strong>the</strong> crucial effect thatoral proficiency <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> second language exerts on read<strong>in</strong>g comprehensiondoes not seem to apply with regard to word and pseudo-word decod<strong>in</strong>gabilities. Thus, <strong>the</strong> early assessment of <strong>the</strong>se skills is highly recommended.What is more, s<strong>in</strong>ce phonological process<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong> onelanguage are capable of predict<strong>in</strong>g word recognition ability with<strong>in</strong> andacross languages, it means that phonological process<strong>in</strong>g abilities <strong>in</strong> onelanguage, no matter whe<strong>the</strong>r native or second, can predict <strong>in</strong>dividualdifferences <strong>in</strong> word decod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> ano<strong>the</strong>r language, even <strong>in</strong> view of not yetfully developed proficiency <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> second language (Geva, 2000). Beyonddoubt, whe<strong>the</strong>r conducted <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> monol<strong>in</strong>gual or bil<strong>in</strong>gual sett<strong>in</strong>g, timelydiagnosis ensures greater opportunities for effective <strong>in</strong>tervention andread<strong>in</strong>g success, while postpon<strong>in</strong>g assessment can br<strong>in</strong>g about <strong>the</strong> farreach<strong>in</strong>gdamag<strong>in</strong>g effects of cumulative scholastic failure.Recommendations drawn from <strong>the</strong> detailed identification constitute afoundation for design<strong>in</strong>g remedial activities and <strong>in</strong>tervention programmesfor students with dyslexia. Such an approach should mostpreferably apply <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> context of <strong>the</strong> native, second and foreignlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g. In pr<strong>in</strong>ciple, tutor<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> dyslexia should be <strong>in</strong>dividualisedand tailored to <strong>the</strong> particular needs and abilities of <strong>the</strong> students,especially with regard to <strong>the</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g approach, pace of presentation andwork, as well as <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tensity and frequency of repetition andconsolidation. Regularity, re<strong>in</strong>forcement and overlearn<strong>in</strong>g are to lead toautomaticity, which, never<strong>the</strong>less, needs <strong>the</strong> back up of <strong>the</strong> explicitdevelopment of <strong>the</strong> metacognitive skills.With regard to treatment, teach<strong>in</strong>g methods and techniques <strong>in</strong>dyslexia, <strong>the</strong> aforementioned alliance of research and practice is mostwelcome. The noticeable reluctance of teachers, especially foreignlanguage teachers, towards accept<strong>in</strong>g and apply<strong>in</strong>g research-based<strong>in</strong>terventions is most probably triggered by <strong>the</strong> often controversial andconflict<strong>in</strong>g research outcomes, let alone <strong>the</strong> confusion <strong>in</strong>duced by <strong>the</strong>frequent occurrence of <strong>the</strong> miracle cures for dyslexia on <strong>the</strong> market.However, it seems especially important to dist<strong>in</strong>guish between issues thatare still <strong>in</strong>tensely debated and those well verified, widely accepted and


196 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>agreed upon. There is no doubt that translat<strong>in</strong>g and dissem<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>confirmed and replicated f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs concern<strong>in</strong>g research-based treatmentsamong teachers and implement<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m <strong>in</strong>to school practice is wor<strong>the</strong>very effort. For <strong>in</strong>stance, research-validated educational activitiesrecommended for widespread use <strong>in</strong>volve direct <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> phonologicalawareness and letter-sound correspondences, which typically br<strong>in</strong>gshighly positive effects <strong>in</strong> terms of enhanced word identification, spell<strong>in</strong>gand read<strong>in</strong>g ability (Vellut<strong>in</strong>o et al., 2004). Phonological tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g comb<strong>in</strong>edwith <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> letter-sound mapp<strong>in</strong>gs is claimed to exert <strong>the</strong> mostpronounced facilitat<strong>in</strong>g effect on read<strong>in</strong>g acquisition (Bogdanowicz,2002a; Gustafson et al., 2007; Reid, 1998; Wise et al., 2007). In addition,<strong>the</strong> direct multisensory structured approach has repeatedly been shownto be effective and is advocated for teach<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g tochildren with dyslexia <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> native and foreign language alike.All <strong>in</strong> all, <strong>in</strong>dividuals with dyslexia can become regular beneficiariesof <strong>the</strong> educational system available to <strong>the</strong> majority of children <strong>in</strong> a givencultural context, as long as certa<strong>in</strong> provisions are <strong>in</strong>troduced <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong>system. Successful ma<strong>in</strong>stream<strong>in</strong>g or <strong>in</strong>clusion education for <strong>in</strong>dividualswith dyslexia requires <strong>the</strong> understand<strong>in</strong>g, engagement and collectiveeffort of all parties <strong>in</strong>volved parents, teachers, policy makers and, lastbut not least, students with dyslexia <strong>the</strong>mselves. Educational provisionsthat can be applied <strong>in</strong> order to reduce and soften <strong>the</strong> dyslexic difficultiesand allow <strong>the</strong>m to demonstrate <strong>the</strong>ir potential would vary depend<strong>in</strong>g on<strong>the</strong> severity of <strong>the</strong> disorder, which is basically consistent with <strong>the</strong> idea of<strong>in</strong>dividualisation. What follows from <strong>the</strong> above is that some studentswith dyslexia require m<strong>in</strong>imal alterations to feel relatively comfortably <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong> educational system, while o<strong>the</strong>r learners with dyslexia might need amuch more <strong>in</strong>dividualised, for example one-to-one teach<strong>in</strong>g approach.The teacher’s role cannot be overestimated <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> all-embrac<strong>in</strong>gdyslexic educational endeavour. Teachers can form <strong>the</strong> fertile groundfor <strong>the</strong> overall mental, psychological and social development throughreduc<strong>in</strong>g disharmonies, lessen<strong>in</strong>g scholastic failure as well as lower<strong>in</strong>gemotional tension. Prevent<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> devastat<strong>in</strong>g consequences of <strong>the</strong>extensive encounter with non-success and disappo<strong>in</strong>tment is essential.Rebuild<strong>in</strong>g confidence and restor<strong>in</strong>g self-image can be achieved throughnotic<strong>in</strong>g even <strong>the</strong> smallest educational advances and encourag<strong>in</strong>g fur<strong>the</strong>reffort <strong>in</strong> students with dyslexia.


Appendix 1Words Used <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Pre-test, Post-test 1 and Post-test 2NrPre-test andpost-test 1 Problem area Post-test 21 tag Short vowel sounds, CVC fan2 fixwordsb<strong>in</strong>3 mug rug4 shift Initial and f<strong>in</strong>al consonant shed5 crushblends and digraphsblush6 such much7 whip when8 chick ch<strong>in</strong>9 fl<strong>in</strong>t cloth10 froth twist11 slick crisp12 th<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ft13 clamp Nasal sounds, assimilation plump14 trench clench15 shunt pr<strong>in</strong>t16 frond brand17 cl<strong>in</strong>k bl<strong>in</strong>k18 prong cl<strong>in</strong>g197


198 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Appendix 1 (Cont<strong>in</strong>ued)NrPre-test andpost-test 1 Problem area Post-test 219 thrust Initial triple consonant blends strung20 strand shrimp21 sprung spr<strong>in</strong>g22 shr<strong>in</strong>k splash23 miss Doubl<strong>in</strong>g rule s<strong>in</strong>gle vowel spill24 fuzzfollowersbuzz25 stiff sniff26 swan ‘W’ rules swamp27 wharf ward28 word worm29 lark Vowel/consonant digraphs smart30 cord north31 fern verb32 have ‘V’ rules live33 cover glove34 cane ‘Magic e’ rule flame35 cube duke36 p<strong>in</strong>e kite37 dole rope38 pick Spell<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> /k/ sound track39 milk bark40 kilt kettle41 cactus clip42 cl<strong>in</strong>ic basic43 quit quest


Appendix 1 199Appendix 1 (Cont<strong>in</strong>ued)NrPre-test andpost-test 1 Problem area Post-test 244 mix fax45 gypsy Spell<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> /da/ and /tR/ gender46 jugsoundsjunk47 edge lodge48 large wage49 fetch stretch50 fry Spell<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> long vowel spy51 sightsounds /ai/, /ei/, /1y/, /ju:/,/u:/, /i:/flight52 tray bay53 bra<strong>in</strong> fail54 grow blow55 moan soak56 glue clue57 grew flew58 boot broom59 keep wheel60 east meal61 copy w<strong>in</strong>dy62 niece piece63 receive ceil<strong>in</strong>g


Appendix 2List of Words Used <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> ProgrammeSound Letters Words/æ/ ‘a’ bad, Dad, had, lad, mad, pad, sad, bag,rag, tag, ham, jam, Pam, ram, Sam, can,fan, man, pan, ran, cap, map, nap, tap, at,bat, cat, fat, hat, mat, pat, rat, sat/e/ ‘e’ bed, fed, led, Ned, red, Ted, wed, beg, keg,leg, Meg, peg, Ben, hen, men, pen, ten, bet,get, jet, let, met, net, pet, set, wet, yet, yes/I/ ‘i’ bid, did, hid, lid, rid, big, dig, fig, pig, wig,him, b<strong>in</strong>, p<strong>in</strong>, t<strong>in</strong>, w<strong>in</strong>, dip, pip, rip, tip,zip, is, his, it, bit, fit, hit, lit, pit, sit, wit, six/ / ‘o’ rob, cod, nod, rod, bog, dog, fog, hog, log,on, Ron, hop, mop, pop, top, cot, dot, got,hot, jot, lot, not, pot, rot, tot, box, fox/^/ ‘u’ cub, mud, tub, bug, hug, jug, mug, rug,hum, Mum, rum, sum, bun, fun, gun, run,sun, up, but, cup, cut, gut, hut, jut, nut,pup, us, busInitial consonant blendsand digraphsF<strong>in</strong>al consonant blendsand digraphsblob, crab, fret, plug, scab, smog, step, ch<strong>in</strong>,blot, cram, frog, plus, smug, stop, chop, brag,drip, glad, pram, sk<strong>in</strong>, snap, swim, ship,brim, drum, glum, prop, skip, snip, swot,shop, clap, flag, gran, tram, slim, sp<strong>in</strong>, twig,thug, clip, flap, gr<strong>in</strong>, trip, slot, spot, tw<strong>in</strong>, th<strong>in</strong>gift, soft, camp, lamp, bank, dust, p<strong>in</strong>k,must, milk, hand, m<strong>in</strong>t, bench, silk, land,rent, lunch, belt, long, desk, felt, song, risk200


Appendix 2 201Appendix 2 (Cont<strong>in</strong>ued)Sound Letters WordsInitial and f<strong>in</strong>al consonantblends and digraphsbrush, crisp, frost, chest, dr<strong>in</strong>k, grand,cloth, fl<strong>in</strong>t, shift, th<strong>in</strong>k, trust, twist, slang,spend, stamp/a:/ ‘ar’ car, bar, far, scar, star, arm, farm, harm,charm, march, dark, bark, park, shark, art,chart, part, smart, start, card, hard, sharp/ :/ ‘or’ or, for, born, corn, form, storm, port, sort,short, sport, fork, lord, forth, north, horse/ :/ ‘er’ her, herd, herb, verb, nerve, serve, sister,silver, expert, letter, mo<strong>the</strong>r, butter, fa<strong>the</strong>r/w / ‘wa’; ‘wha’ what, water, watch, swab, swat, swap,wasp, wand, wad, swamp, swan, was,want, wash, wander/w :/ ‘war’;‘whar’warrant, wharf, ward, swarm, dwarf, war,warm, warn, toward, reward/w :/ ‘wor’ word, work, world, worth, worship,worse, worm, worst, worthy, worthless/l/ ‘ll’ *all, *ball, *call, *doll, *fall, *hall, *small,*tall, *wall, shall, bell, fell, hell, sell, shell,smell, spell, tell, well, ill, bill, fill, grill, hill,pill, spill, still, till, will, thrill, doll, dull,gull, hull, *bull, *pull, *full/s/ ‘ss’ mass, *brass, *glass, *class, *grass, *pass,chess, dress, press, stress, miss, boss, cross,loss, fuss, kiss/z/ ‘zz’ jazz, fizz, whizz, buzz, fuzz/f/ ‘ff’ cliff, sniff, stiff, whiff, cuff, puff, bluff,*staff/v/ ‘ve’ have, live, give, five, serve, nerve/^v/ ‘ov’ love, dove, oven, above, cover, glove


202 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Appendix 2 (Cont<strong>in</strong>ued)Sound Letters Words/k/ ‘c’ clip, cream, correct, crazy, carpet, coffee,cloud, curta<strong>in</strong>, cactus, cup, cold, candle,computer, cream, clown, calendar, canary,clock, cat, cage, crown, cable, corn, car,curta<strong>in</strong>, cap‘k’‘-k’‘-ck’keep, kettle, k<strong>in</strong>d, ketchup, key, kid, kite,killer, kiss, kennel, kitten, k<strong>in</strong>d, kilt, kick,kiss, k<strong>in</strong>gspeak, week, book, ask, bank, desk, talk,work, th<strong>in</strong>k, look, cloak, hook, croak, took,shook, beak, Greek, cheek, cook, p<strong>in</strong>k,mask, s<strong>in</strong>k, tank, talk, shark, fork, bark,walk, break, park, mark, thank, milk,break, riskback, black, neck, stick, luck, clock, quick,duck, sick, lock, suck, trick, sock, block,check, rock, track, click/kw/ ‘qu’ question, quite, square, quit, quick, queen,equip, quiz, quake, quest, aquarium,equator, aqua, squeeze, quarrel, equal,quiet, quarter, quack, quality/ks/ ‘x’ fox, six, mix, fax, taxi, sixty, text, extra,example, excellent, relax, expert, complex,box, next, wax, fix, exam, exit/ik/ ‘ic’ cl<strong>in</strong>ic, classic, traffic, basic, plastic, logic,magic, electric, genetic, public, music,cosmetic, l<strong>in</strong>guistic, pediatric, fantastic,scientific, graphic, analytic, panic, comic,problematic, athletic


Appendix 2 203Appendix 2 (Cont<strong>in</strong>ued)Sound Letters Words/d / ‘-dge’ judge, fridge, sledge, badge, bridge, ledge,lodge, edge, ridge, fudge‘-ge’‘j’‘g’age, change, huge, large, orange, page,stage, wage, cage, chargejail, jazz, job, jo<strong>in</strong>, joke, judge, jump, junk,just, jacketgender, general, genetic, gentle, g<strong>in</strong>, g<strong>in</strong>ger,giant, gym, gypsy, gesture/g/ ‘-g’ dog, frog, big, jug, leg, pig‘g’girl, go, give, game, get, great/tR/ ‘ch’ chance, change, check, child, choice, Ch<strong>in</strong>a,chop, chapter, cha<strong>in</strong>, chapel, channel,charm, charge, cheap, choose, cheese,charity, chest, church‘-ch’‘-tch’beach, each, reach, speech, teach, touch,bench, lunch, march, coach, peach, French,branch, crunch, punch, search, churchwitch, catch, sketch, stitch, Dutch, match,fetch, switch, watch, stretch, scratch/ai/ ‘igh’ night, light, sight, bright, fight, fright,might, right, tight, slight, flight‘-y’‘i-e’try, cry, spy, fly, by, why, sky, rely, reply,shy, my, July, dry, denytime, ride, m<strong>in</strong>e, f<strong>in</strong>e, side, mile, kite, n<strong>in</strong>e,bite, pipe, like, hide, life, size, w<strong>in</strong>e, white,quite, bride, nice, five, pride, drive, smile,prize, crime


204 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Appendix 2 (Cont<strong>in</strong>ued)Sound Letters Words/ei/ ‘ai’ tra<strong>in</strong>, sail, jail, bra<strong>in</strong>, ra<strong>in</strong>, fail, pa<strong>in</strong>t, afraid,wait, mail, raise, pla<strong>in</strong>, maid, ma<strong>in</strong>, tail,sta<strong>in</strong>, pa<strong>in</strong>, nail, fa<strong>in</strong>t‘-ay’‘a-e’say, may, play, tray, way, day, May, stay,holiday, Friday, subway, replay, yesterday,pay, gray, bay, today, delay, Sundayplace, shake, grape, brake, plane, flame,shame, snake, bake, name, cake, make,cage, race, late, face, hate, take, rate, tape,made, male, mate, same, pale/1y/ ‘oa’ coast, load, float, toast, coal, goat, toad,throat, soak, soap, coach, goal, road, coat,moan, boat‘-ow’‘o-e’snow, grow, row, blow, know, show, slow,low, below, elbow, shadow, arrow, w<strong>in</strong>dow,follow, pillow, throw, yellow, borrowhome, bone, globe, hope, stone, nose, rope,tone, smoke, close, phone, joke, code, note,clone, spoke, drove, wrote, hole, choke,froze, chose, stole, woke, broke/u:/, /ju:/ ‘ue’ blue, glue, true, clue, due, value, rescue,issue, cont<strong>in</strong>ue, argue, statue, sue, cue,avenue, barbecue, tissue‘-ew’‘oo’‘u-e’new, grew, nephew, few, view, blew, crew,knew, flew, threw, screw, stew, dewmoon, root, boot, shoot, afternoon, noon,spoon, soon, room, broom, food, mood,fool, school, tool, cool, pool, gloom, stoolcube, cute, huge, use, tune, mule, fuse,tube, duke, mute, nude, June, rule, Luke,rude, flute, crude, plume, lute, prune,prude


Appendix 2 205Appendix 2 (Cont<strong>in</strong>ued)Sound Letters Words/i:/ ‘ee’ teeth, green, seem, speech, sweet, wheel,feel, keep, sleep, week, cheese, need,queen, speed, feet, street, meet, feed, free,knee, see, three, tree, agree, bee, coffee,degree‘ea’‘-y’‘-ey’‘ie’‘ei’‘e-e’please, peach, speak, peace, mean, leave,cheap, reach, beat, read, dream, teach,stream, team, beach, real, meal, meat,clean, eat, easy, eagle, ear, eager, east, tea,seahappy, funny, sleepy, lucky, sorry, angry,snowy, cloudy, w<strong>in</strong>dy, sunny, ra<strong>in</strong>y, city,family, lady, daddy, baby, granny, mummy,twenty, thirty, forty, chemistry, biology,history, anatomy, Billy, Teddy, Sallychimney, donkey, hockey, honey, jockey,journey, kidney, money, monkey, valley,alley, attorney, turkeypiece, niece, field, cashier, believe, priest,chief, thief, reliefceil<strong>in</strong>g, receive, deceive, receipt, conceive,perceivemete, complete, athlete, extreme, recede


Appendix 3Sample of writ<strong>in</strong>g (two pages from <strong>the</strong> English language notebook;personal details were removed) of a 12-year-old Polish boy with dyslexia,who learns English as a foreign language <strong>in</strong> a regular primary classroomsett<strong>in</strong>g. The two pages differ noticeably <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> quality of <strong>the</strong> graphic levelof writ<strong>in</strong>g and organisation of <strong>the</strong> notes on <strong>the</strong> page. The difference arisesfrom <strong>the</strong> fact that <strong>the</strong> left-hand side page conta<strong>in</strong>s <strong>the</strong> homeworkassignment, on which a considerable amount of time was spent athome, while <strong>the</strong> right-hand side page conta<strong>in</strong>s <strong>the</strong> notes from <strong>the</strong> lesson,where <strong>the</strong> learner was under time pressure, which <strong>in</strong> turn resulted <strong>in</strong>poorer handwrit<strong>in</strong>g. In addition, mistakes specific to dyslexia occur, for<strong>in</strong>stance: ‘slreet’ for ‘street’, ‘havean’ for ‘have an’, ‘compuler’ for‘computer’, ‘somenlimes’ for ‘sometimes’, ‘competitiom’ for ‘competition’,‘ald’ for ‘old’, ‘mammols’ for ‘mammals’, ‘an<strong>in</strong>ols’ for ‘animals’, ‘Engl’ for‘English’, ‘ejght’ for ‘eight’.206


Appendix 4Sample of writ<strong>in</strong>g (two pages from <strong>the</strong> English language notebook) of a15-year-old Polish boy with dyslexia, who learns English as a foreignlanguage <strong>in</strong> a regular classroom sett<strong>in</strong>g of a secondary school. Severalmistakes can be spotted, such as: <strong>in</strong>consistent spell<strong>in</strong>g (several spell<strong>in</strong>gattempts of a given word <strong>in</strong> one piece of writ<strong>in</strong>g): ‘dolls’/‘dols’, ‘wyth’/‘with’, ‘Holiwood’/‘Hollywooa’, ‘thanks’/‘th<strong>in</strong>ks’, ‘story’/‘stoy’, ‘end<strong>in</strong>g’/‘endr<strong>in</strong>g’, ‘natulal’/‘natural’; phonetic spell<strong>in</strong>g: ‘ajs r<strong>in</strong>k’ for ‘ice r<strong>in</strong>k’, ‘koton’for ‘cotton’; add<strong>in</strong>g parts of words: ‘ekspresion<strong>in</strong>g’ for ‘express<strong>in</strong>g’; skipp<strong>in</strong>gletters and diacritical marks: ‘stoy’ for ‘story’, ‘anxious’ for ‘anxious’; chang<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong> position of letters: ‘trheat’ for ‘threat’, ‘trhead’ for ‘thread’; difficultydiscrim<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g letters of similar shape, differ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> t<strong>in</strong>y details: ‘an<strong>in</strong>al’ for‘animal’, ‘habilat’ for ‘habitat’, ‘fife’ for ‘life’, ‘comnested’ for ‘connected’;poorly <strong>in</strong>tegrated spell<strong>in</strong>g/<strong>in</strong>correct spell<strong>in</strong>g choices: ‘distroyd’ for ‘destroyed’,‘terible’ for ‘terrible’; ‘impresion’ for ‘impression’, ‘woryy’ for‘worry’, ‘mowies’ for ‘movies’, ‘discower’ for ‘discover’ (‘w’ is pronouncedas /v/ <strong>in</strong> Polish), ‘dols’ for ‘dolls’, ‘wyth’ for ‘with’, ‘trhead/trheat’ for‘threat’; ‘trheatd/trhead’ for ‘thread’; bizarre spell<strong>in</strong>g: ‘witol’ for ‘vehicle’,‘endangires’ for ‘endangered’, ‘edryth<strong>in</strong>g’ for ‘everyth<strong>in</strong>g’. In addition,letters are poorly formed, ra<strong>the</strong>r wobbly and drift<strong>in</strong>g off <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tended angles.207


Appendix 5Sample of writ<strong>in</strong>g (two pages from <strong>the</strong> English language notebook) of a15-year-old Polish boy with dyslexia, who learns English as a foreignlanguage <strong>in</strong> a regular classroom sett<strong>in</strong>g of a secondary school. Eventhough <strong>the</strong> handwrit<strong>in</strong>g is ra<strong>the</strong>r poor, still it is possible to decode, unlike<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sample presented <strong>in</strong> Appendix 6. Still, numerous spell<strong>in</strong>g mistakesof various types occur, toge<strong>the</strong>r with corrections and cross<strong>in</strong>gs. Thispiece of writ<strong>in</strong>g lacks graphic precision. It took a long time (as comparedto non-dyslexic peers) and considerable effort to produce it because ofkeep<strong>in</strong>g a tight grip on <strong>the</strong> pen and press<strong>in</strong>g down too strongly.208


Appendix 6Sample of un<strong>in</strong>telligible handwrit<strong>in</strong>g (two pages from <strong>the</strong> Englishlanguage notebook) of a Polish 19-year-old college student with dyslexia,who learns English as a foreign language.209


ReferencesAdams, M. (1990) Beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g to Read: Th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g and Learn<strong>in</strong>g about Pr<strong>in</strong>t. Cambridge,MA: MIT Press.Aro, M. (2006) Learn<strong>in</strong>g to read: The effect of orthography. In R.M. Joshi andP.G. Aaron (eds) Handbook of Orthography and Literacy (pp. 531550). Hillsdale,NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Atwill, K., Blanchard, J., Gor<strong>in</strong>, J.S. and Burste<strong>in</strong>, K. (2007) Receptive vocabularyand cross-language transfer of phonemic awareness <strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>dergarten children.The Journal of Educational Research 100 (6), 336345.Aylward, E.H., Richards, T.L., Bern<strong>in</strong>ger, V.W., Nagy, W.E., Field, K.M., Grimme,A.C., Richards, A.L., Thomson, J.B. and Cramer, S.C. (2003) Instructionaltreatment associated with changes <strong>in</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> activation <strong>in</strong> children withdyslexia. Neurology 61, 212219.Bakker, D.J. (1984) The bra<strong>in</strong> as a dependent variable. Journal of Cl<strong>in</strong>icalNeuropsychology 6 (1), 116.Bakker, D.J. (1990) Neuropsychological Treatment of <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. Oxford: OxfordUniversity Press.Bakker, D.J. (1995) The will<strong>in</strong>g bra<strong>in</strong> of dyslexic children. In C.K. Leongand R.M. Joshi (eds) Developmental and Acquired <strong>Dyslexia</strong> (pp. 3339). TheNe<strong>the</strong>rlands: Kluwer Academic.Bakker, D.J., Licht, R. and van Strien, J. (1991) Biopsychological validation ofL- and P- type dyslexia. In B.P. Rourke (ed.) Neuropsychological Validation ofLearn<strong>in</strong>g Disability Subtypes (pp. 124139). New York: The Guilford Press.Bakker, D.J., Licht, R. and Kappers, E.J. (1995) Hemispheric stimulationtechniques <strong>in</strong> children with dyslexia. In M.G. Tramontana and S.R. Hooper(eds) Advances <strong>in</strong> Child Neuropsychology (Vol. 3) (pp. 144177). New York:Spr<strong>in</strong>ger-Verlag.Bednarek, D. (1999) Neurobiologiczne podloże dysleksji [Neurobiological basis ofdyslexia]. Przegląd Psychologiczny 42 (12), 1726.Bednarek, D. (2003) Dysleksja a zaburzenia sluchu fonematycznego oraz kanaluwielkokomórkowego w ukladzie wzrokowym [<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and phonological andmagnocellular impairments]. In B. Kaja (ed.) Diagnoza dysleksji [<strong>Dyslexia</strong>Assessment] (pp. 128132). Bydgoszcz: Wydawnictwo Akademii Bydgoskiejim. Kazimierza Wielkiego.Barca, L., Burani, C., di Filippo, G. and Zoccolotti, P. (2007) Italian developmentaldyslexic and proficient readers: Where are <strong>the</strong> differences? Bra<strong>in</strong> and <strong>Language</strong>98 (3), 347351.Benton, A. (1978) <strong>Dyslexia</strong>: An appraisal of current knowledge. In L. Benton andD. Pearl (eds) <strong>Dyslexia</strong>: An Appraisal of Current Knowledge (pp. 451476). NewYork: Oxford University Press.Biscaldi, M., Gezek, S. and Stuhr, V. (1998) Poor saccadic control correlates withdyslexia. Neuropsychologia 36, 11891202.210


References 211Bishop, D.V.M. (2006) <strong>Dyslexia</strong>: What’s <strong>the</strong> problem? Developmental Science 9 (3),256257.Blachman, B.A., Fletcher, J.M., Schatschneider, C., Francis, D.J., Clonan, S.M.,Shaywitz, B.A. and Shaywitz, S.E. (2004) Effects of <strong>in</strong>tensive read<strong>in</strong>gremediation for second and third graders and a 1-year follow-up. Journal ofEducational Psychology 96, 444461.Bloom, J.S. and Hynd, G.W. (2005) The role of <strong>the</strong> corpus callosum <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terhemispherictransfer of <strong>in</strong>formation: Excitation or <strong>in</strong>hibition? NeuropsychologyReview 15 (2), 5971.Boder, E. (1973) Developmental dyslexia: A diagnostic approach based on threeatypical read<strong>in</strong>g-spell<strong>in</strong>g patterns. Developmental Medic<strong>in</strong>e and Child Neurology15, 663687.Bogdanowicz, M. (1985a) Psychologia kl<strong>in</strong>iczna dziecka w wieku przedszkolnym[Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Psychology of Pre-school Children]. Warszawa: Wydawnictwa Szkolne iPedagogiczne.Bogdanowicz, M. (1985b) Metoda dobrego startu w pracy z dzieckiem w wieku od 5 do10 lat [The Method of <strong>the</strong> Good Start for Children Aged 510]. Warszawa:Wydawnictwa Szkolne i Pedagogiczne.Bogdanowicz, M. (1989) Trudności w pisaniu u dzieci [Writ<strong>in</strong>g Difficulties <strong>in</strong>Children]. Gdańsk: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Gdańskiego.Bogdanowicz, M. (1995) Uczeń o specjalnych potrzebach edukacyjnych [Studentswith special educational needs]. Psychologia Wychowawcza 3, 216223.Bogdanowicz, M. (1997a/2000) Integracja percepcyjno-motoryczna: Teoria diagnoza terapia [Perceptual-motor Integration: Theory Diagnosis Therapy]. Warszawa:Centrum Metodyczne Pomocy Psychologiczno-Pedagogicznej M<strong>in</strong>isterstwaEdukacji Narodowej.Bogdanowicz, M. (1997b) Specyficzne trudności w czytaniu i pisaniu w świetleklasyfikacji medycznych, psychologicznych i pedagogicznych [Specificdifficulties <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternational medical, psychologicaland pedagogical classifications]. Audiofonologia 10, 145157.Bogdanowicz, M. (1997c) Dysleksja rozwojowa symptomy, patomechanizmy,terapia pedagogiczna [Developmental dyslexia symptoms, pathomechanisms,pedagogical <strong>the</strong>rapy]. Terapia Numer Specjalny (Special issue), 1216.Bogdanowicz, M. (1999) Specyficzne trudności w czytaniu i pisaniu [Specificdifficulties <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g]. In T. Galkowski and G. Jastrzębowska(eds) Logopedia. Pytania i odpowiedzi [Logopaedics. Questions and Answers](pp. 815859). Opole: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Opolskiego.Bogdanowicz, M. (2002a) Ryzyko dysleksji. Problem i diagnozowanie [Risk for<strong>Dyslexia</strong>. Def<strong>in</strong>ition and Diagnosis]. Gdańsk: Wydawnictwo Harmonia.Bogdanowicz, M. (2002b) Diagnoza dysleksji rozwojowej [Developmental dyslexiaassessment]. In W. Turewicz (ed.) Zajęcia korekcyjno-kompensacyjne wklasach IIII [Correction-compensation Classes <strong>in</strong> Grades IIII] (pp. 57). ZielonaGóra: Ośrodek Doskonalenia Nauczycieli w Zielonej Górze.Bogdanowicz, M. (2003a) Specyficzne trudności w czytaniu i pisaniu [Specificdifficulties <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g]. In T. Galkowski and G. Jastrzębowska(eds) Logopedia. Pytania i odpowiedzi. Wydanie drugie poprawione i rozszerzone[Logopaedics. Questions and Answers] (2nd edn) (pp. 491535). Opole: WydawnictwoUniwersytetu Opolskiego.Bogdanowicz, M. (2003b) Diagnoza dysleksji rozwojowej w Polsce [Developmentaldyslexia assessment <strong>in</strong> Poland]. In B. Kaja (ed.) Diagnoza dysleksji[<strong>Dyslexia</strong> Assessment] (pp. 935). Bydgoszcz: Wydawnictwo AkademiiBydgoskiej im. Kazimierza Wielkiego.


212 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Bogdanowicz, M. (2007) Świadomość dysleksji w Polsce badania porównawcze[The awareness of dyslexia <strong>in</strong> Poland comparative studies]. In M. Kostka-Szymańska and G. Krasowicz-Kupis (eds) Dysleksja. Problem znany czynieznany? [<strong>Dyslexia</strong>. Familiar or Unfamiliar Problem?] (pp. 1335). Lubl<strong>in</strong>:Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Marii-Sklodowskiej.Bogdanowicz, M. and Adryjanek, A. (2004) Uczeń z dysleksją w szkole. Poradnik nietylko dla polonistów [Dyslexic Student at School. A Guide not only for Polish<strong>Language</strong> Teachers]. Gdynia: Wydawnictwo Pedagogiczne Operon.Bogdanowicz, M. and Krasowicz, G. (1995) Diagnoza i leczenie dysleksjirozwojowej neuropsychologiczna koncepcja D.J. Bakkera [Assessment andtreatment of developmental dyslexia a neuropsychological conception ofD.J. Bakker]. Psychologia Wychowawcza 2, 116130.Bogdanowicz, M. and Krasowicz, G. (1996/1997) Typy dysleksji rozwojowej.Diagnoza i terapia wedlug koncepcji D.J. Bakkera [Types of developmentaldyslexia. Diagnosis and <strong>the</strong>rapy accord<strong>in</strong>g to D.J. Bakker’s conception].Annales Universitatis Mariae Curie-Sklodowska 9/10, 1326.Bogdanowicz, M. and Krasowicz-Kupis, G. (2005a) Czytanie i pisanie jakoformy komunikacji językowej [Read<strong>in</strong>g and writ<strong>in</strong>g as forms of l<strong>in</strong>guisticcommunication]. In T. Galkowski, E. Szeląg and G. Jastrzębowska (eds)Podstawy neurologopedii [Introduction to Neurologopaedics] (pp. 9861015).Opole: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Opolskiego.Bogdanowicz, M. and Krasowicz-Kupis, G. (2005b) Diagnoza dysleksji rozwojowej[Developmental dyslexia assessment]. In T. Galkowski, E. Szeląg andG. Jastrzębowska (eds) Podstawy neurologopedii [Introduction to Neurologopaedics](pp. 967985). Opole: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Opolskiego.Bogdanowicz, M. and Sayles, H. (2004) Prawa uczniów z dysleksją w Europie [Rightsof Dyslexic Children <strong>in</strong> Europe]. Gdańsk: Wydawnictwo Harmonia.Bogdanowicz, M. and Wszeborowska-Lipińska, B. (1992) Rozwój psychomotoryczny.Kariera szkolna i osobowość mlodzieży dyslektycznej. [Psychomotordevelopment. School career and personality of dyslexic adolescents]. Scholasticus6, 3538.Borkowska, A. (1997) Zaburzenia językowe u dzieci z trudnościami w czytaniu ipisaniu [<strong>Language</strong> disorders <strong>in</strong> children with read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g difficulties].In A. Herzyk and D. Kądzielawa (eds) Związek mózg zachowanie w ujęciuneuropsychologii kl<strong>in</strong>icznej [The Relation Between <strong>the</strong> Bra<strong>in</strong> and Behaviour from <strong>the</strong>Perspective of Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Neuropsychology] (pp. 269292). Lubl<strong>in</strong>: WydawnictwoUniwersytetu Marii Curie-Sklodowskiej.Borkowska, A. (1998) Analiza dyskursu narracyjnego u dzieci z dysleksją rozwojową[The Analysis of Narrative Discourse <strong>in</strong> Dyslexic Children]. Lubl<strong>in</strong>: WydawnictwoUniwersytetu Marii Curie-Sklodowskiej.Borkowska, A. and Tarkowski, Z. (1990) Kompetencja językowa i komunikacyjnadzieci z trudnościami w czytaniu i pisaniu [L<strong>in</strong>guistic and communicativecompetence <strong>in</strong> children with read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g difficulties]. Logopedia 17,3542.Bosse, M-L., Ta<strong>in</strong>turier, M.J. and Valdoi, S. (2007) Developmental dyslexia: Thevisual attention span deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis. Cognition 104, 198230.Bower, B. (2000) Good readers may get perceptual lift. Science News 157, 180.Bowey, J.A. (2005) Predict<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g to read. InM.J. Snowl<strong>in</strong>g and C. Hulme (eds) The Science of Read<strong>in</strong>g: A Handbook (pp. 155172). Oxford: Blackwell.Bradley, L. and Bryant, P. (1983) Categoriz<strong>in</strong>g sounds and learn<strong>in</strong>g to read acausal connection. Nature 301, 419421.


References 213Breznitz, Z. (2003) Speed of phonological and orthographic process<strong>in</strong>g as factors<strong>in</strong> dyslexia: Electrophysiological evidence. Genetic, Social and General PsychologyMonographs 129 (2), 183206.Brown, H.D. (2000) Pr<strong>in</strong>ciples of <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g and Teach<strong>in</strong>g. New York:Longman.Brown, W.E., Eliez, S., Menon, V., Rumsey, J.M., White, C.D. and Reiss, A.L. (2001)Prelim<strong>in</strong>ary evidence of widespread morphological variations of <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dyslexia. Neurology 56, 781783.Bryant, P. and Bradley, L. (1980) Why children sometimes write words which <strong>the</strong>ydo not read. In U. Frith (ed.) Cognitive Processes <strong>in</strong> Spell<strong>in</strong>g (pp. 355370).London: Academic Press.Bryant, P., Maclean, M., Bradley, L. and Crossland, J. (1990) Rhyme, alliteration,phoneme detection, and learn<strong>in</strong>g to read. Developmental Psychology 26,429438.Brzezińska, A. (1987) Czytanie i pisanie nowy język dziecka [Read<strong>in</strong>g and Writ<strong>in</strong>g aNew <strong>Language</strong> of a Child]. Warszawa: Wydawnictwa Szkolne i Pedagogiczne.Burani, C., Marcol<strong>in</strong>i, S. and Stella, G. (2002) How early does morpho-lexicalread<strong>in</strong>g develop <strong>in</strong> readers of a shallow orthography? Bra<strong>in</strong> and <strong>Language</strong> 81,568586.Caravolas, M. (2006) Ref<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic gra<strong>in</strong> size <strong>the</strong>ory: Effects ofphonotactics and word formation on <strong>the</strong> availability of phonemes topreliterate children. Developmental Science 9 (5), 445447.Carrol, J.B. and Sapon, S.M. (1959) Modern <strong>Language</strong> Aptitude Test. Chicago, IL:The Psychological Corporation. Harcourt Brace Javanovich, Inc.Carroll, J.M. and Iles, J.E. (2006) An assessment of anxiety levels <strong>in</strong> dyslexicstudents <strong>in</strong> higher education. British Journal of Educational Psychology 76,651662.Carroll, J.M. and Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, M. (2004) <strong>Language</strong> and phonological skills <strong>in</strong>children at high risk of read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties. Journal of Child Psychology andPsychiatry 45, 631640.Chen, T-Y. (2001) Test<strong>in</strong>g Ch<strong>in</strong>ese learners of English for language learn<strong>in</strong>gdifficulties by <strong>the</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistic Cod<strong>in</strong>g Deficit/Difference Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis. RELCJournal 32 (1), 3451.Chliounaki, K. and Bryant, P. (2007) How children learn about morphologicalspell<strong>in</strong>g rules. Child Development 78 (4), 13601373.Chodkiewicz, H. (1986) O sprawności czytania w nauczaniu języka obcego [A Read<strong>in</strong>gSkill <strong>in</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Teach<strong>in</strong>g]. Warszawa: Wydawnictwa Szkolne iPedagogiczne.Cieszyńska, J. (2001) Nauka czytania krok po kroku. Jak przeciwdzialać dysleksji?[Learn<strong>in</strong>g to Read Step by Step. How to Prevent <strong>Dyslexia</strong>?]. Kraków: WydawnictwoNaukowe Akademii Pedagogicznej.Cir<strong>in</strong>o, P.T., Israelian, M.K., Morris, M.K. and Morris, R.D. (2005) Evaluation of<strong>the</strong> double-deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis <strong>in</strong> college students referred for learn<strong>in</strong>gdifficulties. Journal of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 38 (1), 2943.Col<strong>the</strong>art, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R. and Ziegler, J. (2001) DRC: A dualroute cascaded model of visual word recognition and read<strong>in</strong>g aloud.Psychological Review 108 (1), 204256.Cornelissen, P., Bradley, L., Fowler, M.S. and Ste<strong>in</strong>, J.F. (1991) What children seeaffects how <strong>the</strong>y read. Developmental Medic<strong>in</strong>e & Child Neurology 33, 755762.Critchley, M. (1964) Developmental <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. London: The Whitefriars Press Ltd.Crombie, M. and McColl, H. (2000) Teach<strong>in</strong>g modern foreign languages todyslexic learners: A Scottish perspective. In L. Peer and G. Reid (eds)


214 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Multil<strong>in</strong>gualism, Literacy and <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. A Challenge for Educators (pp. 211217).London: David Fulton.Czajkowska, I. and Herda, K. (1998) Zajęcia korekcyjno-kompensacyjne w szkole[Correctio-compensation Classes at School]. Warszawa: Wydawnictwa Szkolne iPedagogiczne.Davies, R., Cuetos, F. and Glez-Seijas, R.M. (2007) Read<strong>in</strong>g development anddyslexia <strong>in</strong> a transparent orthography: Survey of Spanish children. Annals of<strong>Dyslexia</strong> 57, 179198.Davis, C. and Bryant, P. (2006) Causal connections <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> acquisition of anorthographic rule: A test of Uta Frith’s developmental hypo<strong>the</strong>sis. Journal ofChild Psychology and Psychiatry 47 (8), 849856.Deacon, S.H. and Bryant, P. (2005) What young children do and do not knowabout <strong>the</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>flections and derivations. Developmental Science 8 (6),583594.Deacon, S.H. and Bryant, P. (2006) This turnip’s not for turn<strong>in</strong>g: Children’smorphological awareness and <strong>the</strong>ir use of root morphemes <strong>in</strong> spell<strong>in</strong>g. BritishJournal of Developmental Psychology 24, 567575.DeFries, J.C., Fulker, D.W. and LaBuda, M.C. (1987) Read<strong>in</strong>g disability <strong>in</strong> tw<strong>in</strong>s:Evidence for a genetic aetiology. Nature 329, 537539.Deponio, P., Landon, J. and Reid, G. (2000) <strong>Dyslexia</strong> and bil<strong>in</strong>gualism implications for assessment, teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g. In L. Peer and G. Reid(eds) Multil<strong>in</strong>gualism, Literacy and <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. A Challenge for Educators (pp.5260). London: David Fulton.Dockrell, J. and McShane, J. (1993) Children’s Learn<strong>in</strong>g Difficulties. A CognitiveApproach. Oxford: Blackwell.Downey, D.M., Snyder, L.E. and Hill, B. (2000) College students with dyslexia:Persistent l<strong>in</strong>guistic deficits and foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g. <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 6, 101111.Dryer, R., Beale, I.L. and Lambert, A.J. (1999) The balance model of dyslexia andremedial tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g: An evaluative study. Journal of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 32 (2),174187.Dufva, M. and Voeten, M. (1999) Native language literacy and phonologicalmemory as prerequisites for learn<strong>in</strong>g English as a foreign language. AppliedPsychol<strong>in</strong>guistics 20 (3), 329348.Durgunoğlu, A.Y. (2006) Learn<strong>in</strong>g to read <strong>in</strong> Turkish. Developmental Science 9 (5),437439.Ehri, L.C. (1995) Phases of development <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g to read words by sight.Journal of Research <strong>in</strong> Read<strong>in</strong>g 18, 116125.Ehri, L.C. (2005) Development of sight word read<strong>in</strong>g: Phasis and f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. InM.J. Snowl<strong>in</strong>g and C. Hulme (eds) The Science of Read<strong>in</strong>g: A Handbook (pp. 135154). Oxford: Blackwell.Ehri, L.C. (2008) Procesy czytania. Rozwijanie tej umiejętności i konsekwencje dlanauczania [Read<strong>in</strong>g processes, acquisition, and <strong>in</strong>structional implications]. InG. Reid and J. Wearmouth (eds) Dysleksja. Teoria i praktyka [<strong>Dyslexia</strong> andLiteracy. Theory and Practice] (pp. 235260). Gdańsk: Gdańskie WydawnictwoPsychologiczne.Ehri, L.C. and McCormick, S. (1998) Phases of word learn<strong>in</strong>g: Implications for<strong>in</strong>struction with delayed and disordered readers. Read<strong>in</strong>g and Writ<strong>in</strong>gQuarterly 14, 135163.Ellis, R. (1994) Understand<strong>in</strong>g Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition. Oxford: OxfordUniversity Press.Elliott, J. and Place, M. (2000) Dzieci i mlodzież wklopocie. Poradnik nie tylko dlapsychologów [Children and Adolescents <strong>in</strong> Trouble. A Guide not Only for


References 215Psychologists]. Warszawa: Wydawnictwa Szkolne i Pedagogiczne. SpólkaAkcyjna.Everatt, J. (2008) Procesy wzrokowe [Visual processes]. In G. Reid andJ. Wearmouth (eds) Dysleksja. Teoria i praktyka [<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and Literacy. Theoryand Practice] (pp. 121140). Gdańsk: Gdańskie Wydawnictwo Psychologiczne.Everatt, J., Adams, E. and Smyth, I. (2000a) Bil<strong>in</strong>gual children’s profiles ondyslexia screen<strong>in</strong>g measures. In L. Peer and G. Reid (eds) Multil<strong>in</strong>gualism,Literacy and <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. A Challenge for Educators (pp. 3644). London: DavidFulton.Everatt, J. Smy<strong>the</strong>, I., Adams, E. and Ocampo, D. (2000b) <strong>Dyslexia</strong> screen<strong>in</strong>gmeasures and bil<strong>in</strong>gualism. <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 6, 4256.Fawcett, A.J. (2008) Dysleksja oraz umiejętność czytania i pisania. Podstawowezagadnienia [<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and literacy]. In G. Reid and J. Wearmouth (eds)Dysleksja. Teoria i praktyka [<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and Literacy. Theory and Practice] (pp. 2550). Gdańsk: Gdańskie Wydawnictwo Psychologiczne.Fawcett, A.J. and Nicolson, R.I. (1999) Performance of dyslexic children oncerebellar and cognitive tests. Journal of Motor Behaviour 31, 6878.Fawcett, A.J. and Nicolson, R.I. (2001) <strong>Dyslexia</strong>: The role of <strong>the</strong> cerebellum. InA.J. Fawcett (ed.) <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. Theory and Good Practice (pp. 89105). London:Whurr.Fawcett, A.J. and Nicolson, R.I. (2004) <strong>Dyslexia</strong>: The role of <strong>the</strong> cerebellum. InG. Reid and A.J. Fawcett (eds) <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> Context. Research, Policy and Practice(pp. 2547). London: Whurr.Fawcett, A.J., Nicolson, R.I. and Dean, P. (1996) Impaired performance of childrenwith dyslexia on a range of cerebellar tasks. Annals of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 46, 259283.Fawcett, A.J., Nicolson, R.I. and Maclagan, F. (2001) Cerebellar tests differentiatebetween groups of poor readers with and without IQ discrepancy. Journal ofLearn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 34, 119135.Ferrari, M. and Pallad<strong>in</strong>o, P. (2007) <strong>Foreign</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>in</strong>Italian children: Are <strong>the</strong>y associated with o<strong>the</strong>r learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties? Journal ofLearn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 40 (3), 256269.F<strong>in</strong>ch, A.J., Nicolson, R.I. and Fawcett, A.J. (2002) Evidence for neuroanatomicaldifference with<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> olivo-cerebellar pathway of adults with dyslexia. Cortex38, 529539.Fisher, S.E. and Francks, C. (2006) Genes, cognition and dyslexia: Learn<strong>in</strong>g to read<strong>the</strong> genome. Trends <strong>in</strong> Cognitive Sciences 10, 250257.Fisher, S.E. and Smith, S.D. (2001) Progress towards <strong>the</strong> identification of genes<strong>in</strong>fluenc<strong>in</strong>g developmental dyslexia. In A.J. Fawcett (ed.) <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. Theory andGood Practice (pp. 3964). London: Whurr.Frederickson, N., Frith, U. and Reason, R. (1997) Phonological Assessment Battery.W<strong>in</strong>dsor: NFER-Nelson.Frith, U. (1985) Beneath <strong>the</strong> surface of developmental dyslexia. In K. Patterson,J. Marshall and M. Col<strong>the</strong>art (eds) Surface <strong>Dyslexia</strong> (pp. 287295). Baltimore,MD: University Park Press.Frith, U. (1999) Paradoxes <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> def<strong>in</strong>ition of dyslexia. <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 5, 192214.Frith, U. (2008) Rozwiązywanie paradoksów dysleksji [Resolv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> paradoxesof dyslexia]. In G. Reid and J. Wearmouth (eds) Dysleksja. Teoria i praktyka[<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and Literacy. Theory and Practice] (pp. 71102). Gdańsk: GdańskieWydawnictwo Psychologiczne.Frost, R. (2006) Becom<strong>in</strong>g literate <strong>in</strong> Hebrew: The gra<strong>in</strong> size hypo<strong>the</strong>sis andsemitic orthographic systems. Developmental Science 9 (5), 439440.


216 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Frost, R., Katz, L. and Bent<strong>in</strong>, S. (1987) Strategies for visual word recognition andorthographical depth: A multil<strong>in</strong>gual comparison. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology: Human Perception and Performance 13 (1), 104115.Frost, R. and Ziegler, J.C. (2007) Speech and spell<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>teraction: The <strong>in</strong>terdependenceof visual and auditory word recognition. In M.G. Gaskell (ed.) TheOxford Handbook of Psychol<strong>in</strong>guistics (pp. 107118). Oxford: Oxford UniversityPress.Gajar, A.H. (1987) <strong>Foreign</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g disabilities; <strong>the</strong> identification ofpredictive and diagnostic variables. Journal of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 20 (6), 327330.Galaburda, A.M. (1985) Developmental dyslexia: A review of biological <strong>in</strong>teractions.Annals of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 35, 2135.Galaburda, A.M. (1993) Neurology of developmental dyslexia. Current Op<strong>in</strong>ion <strong>in</strong>Neurobiology 3, 237242.Galaburda, A.M., Lemay, M., Kemper, T.M. and Geschw<strong>in</strong>d, N. (1978) R/Lasymmetries <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> may underlie cerebral dom<strong>in</strong>ance. Science 199,852856.Galaburda, A.M. and Liv<strong>in</strong>gstone, M. (1993) Evidence for a magnocellular defect<strong>in</strong> developmental dyslexia. In P. Tallal, A.M. Galaburda, R.R. Ll<strong>in</strong>as and C. vonEuler (eds) Temporal Information Process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Nervous System. Annals of <strong>the</strong>New York Academy of Science 682 (pp. 7082). New York: New York Academy ofSciences.Galaburda, A.M., Lo Turco, L., Ramus, F., Fitch, R.H. and Rosen, G.D. (2006) Fromgenes to behaviour <strong>in</strong> developmental dyslexia. Nature Neuroscience 9 (10),12131217.Ganschow, L. and Sparks, R. (1986) Learn<strong>in</strong>g disabilities and foreign languagedifficulties: Deficits <strong>in</strong> listen<strong>in</strong>g skills? Journal of Read<strong>in</strong>g, Writ<strong>in</strong>g, and Learn<strong>in</strong>gDisabilities International 2, 306319.Ganschow, L. and Sparks, R. (1987) The foreign language requirement. Learn<strong>in</strong>gDisabilities Focus 2, 115123.Ganschow, L. and Sparks, R. (1995) Effects of direct <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> Spanishphonology on <strong>the</strong> native-language skills and foreign-language aptitude of atriskforeign-language learners. Journal of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 28 (2), 107120.Ganschow, L. and Sparks, R. (2000) Reflections on foreign language study forstudents with foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g problems: Research, issues, andchallenges. <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 6, 87100.Ganschow, L. and Sparks, R. (2001) Learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties and foreign languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g: A review of research and <strong>in</strong>struction. <strong>Language</strong> Teach<strong>in</strong>g 34, 7998.Ganschow, L., Sparks, R., Javorsky, J., Pohlman, J. and Bishop-Marbury, A. (1991)Identify<strong>in</strong>g native language difficulties among foreign language learners <strong>in</strong>college: A ‘foreign’ language learn<strong>in</strong>g disability? Journal of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities24, 530541.Ganschow, L., Sparks, R., Anderson, R., Javorsky, J. and Sk<strong>in</strong>ner, S. (1994)Differences <strong>in</strong> language performance among high and low anxious collegeforeign language learners. Modern <strong>Language</strong> Journal 78, 4155.Ganschow, L., Sparks, R. and Javorsky, J. (1998) <strong>Foreign</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>gdifficulties: A historical perspective. Journal of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 31 (3),248258.Ganschow, L., Schneider, E. and Evers, T. (2000) Difficulties of English as a foreignlanguage (EFL) for students with language-learn<strong>in</strong>g disabilities (dyslexia). InL. Peer and G. Reid (eds) Multil<strong>in</strong>gualism, Literacy and <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. A Challenge forEducators (pp. 182191). London: David Fulton.


References 217Gąsowska, T. and Pietrzak-Stępkowska, Z. (1994) Praca wyrównawcza z dziećmimającymi trudności w czytaniu i pisaniu. [Remedial Work with Children Encounter<strong>in</strong>gRead<strong>in</strong>g and Spell<strong>in</strong>g Difficulties]. Warszawa: Wydawnictwa Szkolne iPedagogiczne.Geva, E. (2000) Issues <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> assessment of read<strong>in</strong>g disabilities <strong>in</strong> L2 children beliefs and research evidence. <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 6, 1328.Gill<strong>in</strong>gham, A. and Stillman, B.W. (1997) The Gill<strong>in</strong>gham Manual: Remedial Tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gfor Children with Specific Disability <strong>in</strong> Read<strong>in</strong>g, Spell<strong>in</strong>g and Penmanship (8th edn).Cambridge, MA: Educators Publish<strong>in</strong>g Service.G<strong>in</strong>drich, P.A. (2002) Funkcjonowanie psychospoleczne uczniów dyslektycznych[Psychosocial Function<strong>in</strong>g of Dyslexic Students]. Lubl<strong>in</strong>: Wydawnictwo UMCS.Goikoetxea, E. (2006) Read<strong>in</strong>g errors <strong>in</strong> first- and second-grade readers of shalloworthography: Evidence from Spanish. British Journal of Educational Psychology76, 333350.Gombert, J.E. (1992) Metal<strong>in</strong>guistic Development. Hemel Hempstead: HavesterWheatsheaf.Goswami, U. (1999) Causal connections <strong>in</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g: The importance ofrhyme. Journal of Research <strong>in</strong> Read<strong>in</strong>g 22 (3), 217240.Goswami, U. (2000) Phonological representations, read<strong>in</strong>g development anddyslexia: Towards a cross-l<strong>in</strong>guistic <strong>the</strong>oretical framework. <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 6, 133151.Goswami, U. (2005) Syn<strong>the</strong>tic phonics and learn<strong>in</strong>g to read: A cross-languageperspective. Educational Psychology <strong>in</strong> Practice 21 (4), 273282.Goswami, U. (2006) Sensorimotor impairments <strong>in</strong> dyslexia: Gett<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> beat.Developmental Science 9 (3), 257259.Goswami, U. and Bryant, P.E. (1990) Phonological Skills and Learn<strong>in</strong>g to Read.Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Goswami, U. and Ziegler, J.C. (2006) Fluency, phonology, and morphology: Aresponse to <strong>the</strong> commentaries on becom<strong>in</strong>g literate <strong>in</strong> different languages.Developmental Science 9 (5), 451453.Górniewicz, E. (1998) Pedagogiczna diagnoza specyficznych trudności w czytaniu ipisaniu [Pedagogical Assessment of Specific Difficulties <strong>in</strong> Read<strong>in</strong>g and Spell<strong>in</strong>g].Toruń: Wydawnictwo Adam Marszalek.Grabowska, A. (1997) Percepcja wzrokowa i jej analogie do <strong>in</strong>nych form percepcji[Visual perception and its analogy to o<strong>the</strong>r forms of perception]. In T. Górska,A. Grabowska and J. Zagrodzka (eds) Mózg a zachowanie [Bra<strong>in</strong> vs. Behaviour](pp. 147183). Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN.Grant, A.C., Zangaladze, A., Thiagarajah, M. and Saathian, K. (1999) Tactileperception <strong>in</strong> dyslexics. Neuropsychologia 37, 12011211.Gregg, N., Hoy, C., Flaherty, D.A., Norris, P., Coleman, C., Davis, M. and Jordan,M. (2005) Decod<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g accommodations for postsecondary studentswith dyslexia It’s more than process<strong>in</strong>g speed. Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities: AContemporary Journal 3 (2), 117.Gustafson, S., Ferreira, J. and Rö¨nnberg, J. (2007) Phonological or orthographictra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g for children with phonological or orthographic decod<strong>in</strong>g deficits.<strong>Dyslexia</strong> 13, 211229.Hadzibeganovic, T., van den Noort, M., Bosch, P., Perc, M., van Kral<strong>in</strong>gen, R.,Mondt, K. and Col<strong>the</strong>art, M. (<strong>in</strong> press) Cross-l<strong>in</strong>guistic neuroimag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dyslexia: A critical view. Cortex.Hanley, J.R., Masterson, J., Spencer, L.H. and Evans, D. (2004) How long do <strong>the</strong>advantages of learn<strong>in</strong>g to read a transparent orthography last? An <strong>in</strong>vestigationof <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g skills and read<strong>in</strong>g impairment of Welsh children at 10 yearsof age. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 57A (8), 13931410.


218 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Hatcher, J. and Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, M.J. (2008) Hipoteza reprezentacji fonologicznych jakosposób rozumienia dysleksji: od teorii do praktyki [The phonological representationshypo<strong>the</strong>sis of dyslexia]. In G. Reid and J. Wearmouth (eds) Dysleksja.Teoria i praktyka [<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and Literacy. Theory and Practice](pp.103120). Gdańsk:Gdańskie Wydawnictwo Psychologiczne.Heaton, P. and W<strong>in</strong>terton, P. (1996) Deal<strong>in</strong>g with <strong>Dyslexia</strong> (2nd edn). London:Whurr.Heiervang, E., Hugdahl, H., Ste<strong>in</strong>metz, H., Smievoll, A.I., Stevenson, J., Lund, A.,Ersland, L. and Lundervold, A. (2000) Planum temporale, planum parietaleand dichotic listen<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 38, 17041713.Helland, T. (2007) <strong>Dyslexia</strong> at a behavioural and cognitive level. <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 13,2541.Helland, T. (2008) Second language assessment <strong>in</strong> dyslexia: Pr<strong>in</strong>ciples andpractice. In J. Kormos and E.H. Kontra (eds) <strong>Language</strong> Learners with SpecialNeeds. An International Perspective (pp. 6885). Bristol: Multil<strong>in</strong>gual Matters.Helland, T., Asbjørnsen, A.E., Hushovd, A.E. and Hugdahl, K. (2008) Dichoticlisten<strong>in</strong>g and school performance <strong>in</strong> dyslexia. <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 14, 4253.Helland, T. and Kaasa, R. (2005) <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> English as a second language.<strong>Dyslexia</strong> 11, 4160.Ho, C.S-H. and Fong, K-M. (2005) Do Ch<strong>in</strong>ese dyslexic children have difficultieslearn<strong>in</strong>g English as a second language? Journal of Psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic Research34 (6), 603618.Ho, C.S-H., Law, T.P-S. and Ng, P.M. (2000) The phonological deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis<strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese developmental dyslexia. Read<strong>in</strong>g and Writ<strong>in</strong>g: An Interdiscipl<strong>in</strong>aryJournal 13, 5779.Hoien, T. and Lundberg, I (2000) <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. From Theory to Intervention. Dordrecht,Ne<strong>the</strong>rlands: Kluwer Academic.Hoien, T., Lundberg, I., Stanovich, K.E. and Bjaalid, I.K. (1995) Components ofphonological awareness. Read<strong>in</strong>g and Writ<strong>in</strong>g 7, 171188.Hogan, T.P., Catts, H.W. and Little T.D. (2005) The relationship betweenphonological awareness and read<strong>in</strong>g: Implications for <strong>the</strong> assessment ofphonological awareness. <strong>Language</strong>, Speech, and Hear<strong>in</strong>g Services <strong>in</strong> Schools 36,285293.Horgan, J. (1997) Komputer lekarstwem na trudności w czytaniu [Computer as acure for read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties]. Świat Nauki 1, 6263.Hornsby, B., Shear, F. and Pool, J. (1999) Alpha to Omega. The AZ Teach<strong>in</strong>g Read<strong>in</strong>g,Writ<strong>in</strong>g and Spell<strong>in</strong>g (5th edn). Oxford: He<strong>in</strong>emann Educational.Hugdahl, K., Heiervang, E., Erland, L., Lundervold, A., Ste<strong>in</strong>metz, H. andSmievoll, A.I. (2003) Significant relation between MR measures of planumtemporale area and dichotic process<strong>in</strong>g of syllables <strong>in</strong> dyslexic children.Neuropsychologia 41, 666675.Hulme, C. and Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, M. (2009) Developmental Disorders of <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>gand Cognition. Oxford/Malden: Wiley-Blackwell.Hulme, C., Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, M., Caravolas, M. and Carroll, J. (2005) Phonological skillsare (probably) one cause of success <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g to read: A comment on Castlesand Col<strong>the</strong>art. Scientific Studies of Read<strong>in</strong>g 9 (4), 351365.Hurry, J., Nunes, T., Bryant, P., Pretzlik, U., Parker, M., Curno, T. and Midgley, L.(2005) Transform<strong>in</strong>g research on morphology unto teacher practice. ResearchPapers <strong>in</strong> Education 20 (2), 187206.Hutch<strong>in</strong>son, J.M., Whiteley, H.E., Smith, C.D. and Connors, L. (2004) The earlyidentification of dyslexia: Children with English as an additional language.<strong>Dyslexia</strong> 10, 179195.


References 219Jaklewicz, H. (1982) Zaburzenia mowy pisanej i czytanej u dzieci [Disorders ofspeech and read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> children]. In J. Szumska (ed.) Zaburzenia mowy u dzieci[Speech Disorders <strong>in</strong> Children] (pp. 5157). Warszawa: PZWL.Jaklewicz, H. (1997) Dysleksja problemy medyczne [<strong>Dyslexia</strong> medical issues].Terapia Numer Specjalny (Special issue), 1718.Jameson, M. (2000) <strong>Dyslexia</strong> and modern foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g strategiesfor success. In L. Peer and G. Reid (eds) Multil<strong>in</strong>gualism, Literacy and <strong>Dyslexia</strong>.A Challenge for Educators (pp. 229234). London: David Fulton.Javorsky, J., Sparks, R. and Ganschow, L. (1992) Perceptions of college studentswith and without learn<strong>in</strong>g disabilities about foreign language courses.Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities: Research and Practice 7, 3144.Jennedy, S., Poletto, R. and Weldon, T.L. (eds) (1994) <strong>Language</strong> Files. Materials foran Introduction to <strong>Language</strong> & L<strong>in</strong>guistics. Columbus, OH: Ohio State UniversityPress.Jędrzejowska, E. and Jurek, A. (2003) Diagnoza i terapia dysleksji rozwojowej[Assessment and <strong>the</strong>rapy of developmental dyslexia]. In T. Galkowski andG. Jastrzębowska (eds) Logopedia. Pytania i odpowiedzi, Wydanie drugie poprawionei rozszerzone [Logopaedics. Questions and Answers] (2nd edn) (pp. 536608).Opole: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Opolskiego.Johnson, D.J (1978) Remedial approaches to dyslexia. In A.L. Benton and D. Pearl(eds) <strong>Dyslexia</strong>: An Appraisal of Current Knowledge (pp. 399423). New York:Oxford University Press.Johnson, M., Peer, L. and Lee, R. (2001) Pre-school children and dyslexia: Policy,identification and <strong>in</strong>tervention. In A.J. Fawcett (ed.) <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. Theory and GoodPractice (pp. 231255). London: Whurr.Jorm, A.M. (1985) The Psychology of Read<strong>in</strong>g and Spell<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities. London:Routledge and Kegan Paul.Jurek, A. (2004a) Trudności w nauce języków obcych uczniów z dysleksjąrozwojową [<strong>Foreign</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>in</strong> dyslexic students]. InM. Bodganowicz and M. Smoleń (eds) Dysleksja w kontekście nauczania językówobcych [<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Teach<strong>in</strong>g] (pp. 98120). Gdańsk: WydawnictwoHarmonia.Jurek, A. (2004b) Języki obce w nauczaniu uczniów z dysleksją rozwojową. CzęśćI [Teach<strong>in</strong>g foreign languages to dyslexic students. Part I]. Języki Obce w Szkole1, 5772.Jurek, A. (2004c) Języki obce w nauczaniu uczniów z dysleksją rozwojową. CzęśćII [Teach<strong>in</strong>g foreign languages to dyslexic students. Part II]. Języki Obce wSzkole 2, 4654.Jurek, A. (2008) Ksztalcenie umiejętności ortograficznych uczniów z dysleksją [Develop<strong>in</strong>gOrthographic Skills <strong>in</strong> Dyslexic Students]. Gdańsk: Wydawnictwo Harmonia.Juszczyk, S. and Zając, W. (1997) Komputerowa edukacja uczniów z zaburzeniami wczytaniu i pisaniu [IT Education for Students with Read<strong>in</strong>g and Spell<strong>in</strong>g Disorders].Katowice: Śląsk.Juul, H. and Sigurdsson, B. (2005) Orthography as a handicap? A directcomparison of spell<strong>in</strong>g acquisition <strong>in</strong> Danish and Icelandic. Scand<strong>in</strong>avianJournal of Psychology 46, 263272.Józwiak, J. and Podgórski, J. (1994) Statystyka od podstaw [Introduction to Statistics].Warszawa: Państwowe Wydawnictwo Ekonomiczne.Kaczmarek, L. (1969) Cybernetyczne podstawy ksztaltowania się mowy ugluchych [Cybernetic basis of speech development <strong>in</strong> deaf]. Logopedia 89,315.


220 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Kahn-Horwitz, J., Shimron, J. and Sparks, R. (2005) Predict<strong>in</strong>g foreign languageread<strong>in</strong>g achievement <strong>in</strong> elementary school students. Read<strong>in</strong>g and Writ<strong>in</strong>g: AnInterdiscipl<strong>in</strong>ary Journal 18 (6), 161185.Kahn-Horwitz, J., Shimron, J. and Sparks, R. (2006) Weak and strong novicereaders of English as a foreign language: Effects of first language andsocioeconomic status. Annals of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 56 (1), 161185.Kaja, B. (2001a) Trudności w uczeniu się. Psychopatologia w uczeniu się czymikrouszkodzenia mózgu? [Learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties. Learn<strong>in</strong>g psycho-pathologyor m<strong>in</strong>imal bra<strong>in</strong> damage?] In B. Kaja (ed.) Wspomaganie rozwoju. Psychostymulacjai psychokorekcja. Tom III [Enhanc<strong>in</strong>g Development. Psycho-stimulation andPsycho-correction, Vol. III] (pp. 7490). Bydgoszcz: Wydawnictwo UczelnianeAkademii Bydgoskiej.Kaja, B. (2001b) Zarys terapii dziecka [An Outl<strong>in</strong>e of Child Therapy]. Bydgoszcz:Wydawnictwo Akademii Bydgoskiej.Kalka, D. (2003) Wartość diagnozy ryzyka dysleksji rozwojowej [The value ofdevelopmental dyslexia assessment of <strong>the</strong> risk for dyslexia]. In B. Kaja (ed.)Diagnoza dysleksji [<strong>Dyslexia</strong> Assessment] (pp. 133146). Bydgoszcz: WydawnictwoAkademii Bydgoskiej im. Kazimierza Wielkiego.Kappers, E.J. (1997) Outpatient treatment of dyslexia through stimulation of <strong>the</strong>cerebral hemispheres. Journal of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 30 (1), 100125.Katz, L. and Frost, R. (1992) The read<strong>in</strong>g process is different for differentorthographies: The orthographic depth hypo<strong>the</strong>sis. In R. Frost and L. Katz(eds) Orthography, Phonology, Morphology, and Mean<strong>in</strong>g (pp. 6784). Amsterdam:Elsevier Science.Katzir, T., Kim, Y-S., Wolf, M., Morris, R. and Lovett, M.W. (2008) The varieties ofpathways to dysfluent read<strong>in</strong>g. Journal of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 41 (1), 4766.Kessler, B. and Treiman, R. (2003) Is English spell<strong>in</strong>g chaotic? Misconceptionsconcern<strong>in</strong>g its regularity. Read<strong>in</strong>g Psychology 24, 267289.Kirk, J., McLoughl<strong>in</strong>, D. and Reid, G. (2001) Identification and <strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>in</strong>adults. In A.J. Fawcett (ed.) <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. Theory and Good Practice (pp. 292308).London: Whurr.Kl<strong>in</strong>gberg, T., Hedehus, M., Temple, E., Salz, T., Gabrieli, J.D.E., Mosely, M.E. andPoldrack, R.A. (2000) Microstructure of temporoparietal white matter as abasis for read<strong>in</strong>g ability; evidence from diffusion tensor magnetic resonanceimag<strong>in</strong>g. Neuron 25, 493500.Knight, D.F. and Hynd, G.W. (2008) Neurobiologia dysleksji [The neurobiology ofdyslexia]. In G. Reid and J. Wearmouth (eds) Dysleksja. Teoria i praktyka[<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and Literacy. Theory and Practice] (pp. 5170). Gdańsk: GdańskieWydawnictwo Psychologiczne.Knobloch-Gala, A. (1995) Asymetria i <strong>in</strong>tegracja pólkulowa a mowa i niektóre jejzaburzenia. Problemy diagnozy psychologicznej dzieci z dysleksją [Asymmetry andHemisphere Integration vs. Speech and Its Selected Disorders. Problems ofPsychological Diagnosis <strong>in</strong> <strong>Dyslexia</strong>]. Kraków: Wydawnictwo UniwersytetuJagiellońskiego.Koda, K. (1992) The effects of lower-level process<strong>in</strong>g skills on foreign languageread<strong>in</strong>g performance: Implications for <strong>in</strong>struction. Modern <strong>Language</strong> Journal 76,502512.Komorowska, H. (1982) Metody badań empirycznych w glottodydaktyce [ResearchMethods <strong>in</strong> Glottodidactics]. Warszawa: Państwowe Wydawnictwo Naukowe.Krasowicz. G. (1997) Język, czytanie i dysleksja [<strong>Language</strong>, Read<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>Dyslexia</strong>].Lubl<strong>in</strong>: Agencja Wydawniczo-Handlowa AD.


References 221Krasowicz-Kupis, G. (1999) Rozwój metajęzykowy a osiągnięcia w czytaniu u dzieci69-letnich [Metal<strong>in</strong>guistic Development vs. Read<strong>in</strong>g Achievement <strong>in</strong> 69-year-olds].Lubl<strong>in</strong>: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Marii Curie-Sklodowskiej.Krasowicz-Kupis, G. (2003) Językowe, ale nie fonologiczne deficyty w dysleksji[L<strong>in</strong>guistic but not phonological deficits <strong>in</strong> dyslexia]. In B. Kaja (ed.) Diagnozadysleksji [<strong>Dyslexia</strong> Assessment] (pp. 95118). Bydgoszcz: Wydawnictwo AkademiiBydgoskiej im. Kazimierza Wielkiego.Krasowicz-Kupis, G. (2004) Rozwój świadomości językowej dziecka. Teoria i praktyka[Development of <strong>Language</strong> Awareness <strong>in</strong> Children. Theory and Practice]. Lubl<strong>in</strong>:Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Marii Curie-Sklodowskiej.Krasowicz-Kupis, G. (2006) Dysleksja a rozwój mowy i języka [<strong>Dyslexia</strong> vs.speech and language development]. In G. Krasowicz-Kupis (ed.) Dysleksjarozwojowa. Perspektywa psychologiczna [Developmental <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. PsychologicalPerspective] (pp. 5369). Gdańsk: Wydawnictwo Harmonia.Krasowicz-Kupis, G. (2008) Psychologia dysleksji [Psychology of <strong>Dyslexia</strong>]. Warszawa:Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN.Krasowicz-Kupis, G. and Bryant, P.E. (2004) Świadomość językowa dziecipolskich i angielskich a czytanie [L<strong>in</strong>guistic awareness of Polish and Englishchildren vs. read<strong>in</strong>g]. In M. Bogdanowicz and M. Smoleń (eds) Dysleksja wkontekście nauczania języków obcych [<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Teach<strong>in</strong>g] (pp.3653). Gdańsk: Wydawnictwo Harmonia.Lavidor, M., Johnson, R. and Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, M.J. (2006) When phonology fails:Orthographic neighbourhood effects <strong>in</strong> dyslexia. Bra<strong>in</strong> and <strong>Language</strong> 96,318329.Ledwoch, B. (1999) Problemy emocjonalne dziecka z dysleksją rozwojową[Emotional problems of dyslexic children]. In A. Herzyk and A. Borkowska(eds) Neuropsychologia emocji. Poglądy. Badania. Kl<strong>in</strong>ika. [Neuropsychology ofEmotions. Beliefs. Research. Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Practice] (pp. 191207). Lubl<strong>in</strong>: WydawnictwoUniwersytetu Marii Curie-Sklodowskiej.Leonard, C., Eckert, M., Given, B., Bern<strong>in</strong>ger, V. and Eden, G. (2006) Individualdifferences <strong>in</strong> anatomy predict read<strong>in</strong>g and oral language impairments <strong>in</strong>children. Bra<strong>in</strong> 129, 33293342.Lev<strong>in</strong>son, H.N. (1980) A Solution to <strong>the</strong> Riddle <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. New York: Spr<strong>in</strong>ger-Verlag.L<strong>in</strong>dsay, G. (2001) Identification and <strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> primary school. InA.J. Fawcett (ed.) <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. Theory and Good Practice (pp. 256280). London:Whurr.L<strong>in</strong>dsay, K., Manis, F. and Baily, C. (2003) Prediction of first-grade read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>Spanish-speak<strong>in</strong>g English-language learners. Journal of Educational Psychology95 (3), 482494.Lovett, M.W., De Palma, M., Frijters, J., Ste<strong>in</strong>bach, K., Temple, M., Benson, N. andLacerenza, L. (2008) Interventions for read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties. A comparison ofresponse to <strong>in</strong>tervention by ELL and EFL struggl<strong>in</strong>g readers. Journal ofLearn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 41 (4), 333352.Lovett, M.W., Ste<strong>in</strong>bach, K.A. and Frijters, J.C. (2000) Remediat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> coredeficits of developmental read<strong>in</strong>g disability: A double deficit perspective.Journal of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 33, 334358.Lundberg, I. (2002) Second language learn<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g with additional load ofdyslexia. Annals of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 52, 165187.Lundberg, I. and Hoien, T. (2001) <strong>Dyslexia</strong> and phonology. In A.J. Fawcett (ed.)<strong>Dyslexia</strong>. Theory and Good Practice (pp. 109123). London: Whurr.


222 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Luszniewicz, A. and Slaby, T. (2001) Statystyka z pakietem komputerowym STATIS-TICA PL [Statistics with a Computer Program STATISTICA PL]. Warszawa:Wydawnictwo C.H. Beck.Lyon, G.R. (1995) Towards a def<strong>in</strong>ition of dyslexia. Annals of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 45, 327.Mabbot, A. (1995) Argu<strong>in</strong>g for multiple perspectives on <strong>the</strong> issue of learn<strong>in</strong>gdisabilities and foreign language acquisition: A response to Sparks, Ganschow,and Javorsky. <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Annals 28 (4), 488494.Mar<strong>in</strong>o, C., Citterio, A., Giorda, R., Facoetti, A., Menozzi, G., Vanz<strong>in</strong>, L., Lorusso,M.L., Nobile, M. and Molteni, M. (2007) Association of short-term memorywith a variant with<strong>in</strong> DYX1C1 <strong>in</strong> developmental dyslexia. Genes, Bra<strong>in</strong> andBehavior 6, 640646.Maruszewski, T. (1996) Psychologia poznawcza [Cognitive Psychology]. Warszawa:Polskie Towarzystwo Semiotyczne.Maurer, A. (ed.) (2003) Dźwięki mowy. Program ksztaltowania świadomości fonologicznejdla dzieci przedszkolnych i szkolnych [Speech Sounds. A Program forDevelop<strong>in</strong>g Phonological Awareness <strong>in</strong> Pre-school and School Children]. Kraków:Oficyna Wydawnicza ‘Impuls’.Merzenich, M.M., Jenk<strong>in</strong>s, W.M, Johnson, P., Schre<strong>in</strong>er, C., Miller, S. and Tallal, P.(1996) Temporal process<strong>in</strong>g deficits of language-learn<strong>in</strong>g impaired childrenameliorated by stretch<strong>in</strong>g speech. Science 271, 7781.Miązek, D. (2001) Scenariusze zajęć do pracy z dziećmi ze specyficznymi trudnościamiw uczeniu się [Lesson Plans for Children with Specific Learn<strong>in</strong>g Difficulties]. Lodź:Wydawnictwo Edukacyjne Res Polona.Mickiewicz, J. (1997) Jedynka z ortografii? Rozpoznawanie dysleksji, dysortografii idysgrafii w starszym wieku szkolnym [Spell<strong>in</strong>g Failed?: Identify<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Dyslexia</strong>,Dysorthography and Dysgraphia <strong>in</strong> Older Children]. Toruń: Dom Organizatora.Miles, E. (2000) <strong>Dyslexia</strong> may show a different face <strong>in</strong> different languages.<strong>Dyslexia</strong> 6, 193201.Miles, T.R. (1996) Do dyslexic children have IQs? <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 2 (3), 175178.Miller, S. and Bussman Gillis, M. (2000) The language puzzle: Connect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>study of l<strong>in</strong>guistics with a multisensory language <strong>in</strong>structional programme <strong>in</strong>foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g. In L. Peer and G. Reid (eds) Multil<strong>in</strong>gualism, Literacyand <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. A Challenge for Educators (pp. 218228). London: David Fulton.Miller-Guron, L. and Lundberg, I. (2000) <strong>Dyslexia</strong> and second language read<strong>in</strong>g:A second bite at <strong>the</strong> apple? Read<strong>in</strong>g and Writ<strong>in</strong>g: An Interdiscipl<strong>in</strong>ary Journal 12,4161.Miller-Guron, L. and Lundberg, I. (2004) Error patterns <strong>in</strong> word read<strong>in</strong>g amongprimary school children: A cross-orthographic study. <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 10 (1), 4460.Morton, J. and Frith, U. (1995) Causal model<strong>in</strong>g: A structural approach todevelopmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti and D.J. Cohen (eds) Manualof Developmental Psychopathology (pp. 357390). New York: Wiley.Nicolson, R.I. (2001) Developmental dyslexia: Into <strong>the</strong> future. In A.J. Fawcett (ed.)<strong>Dyslexia</strong>. Theory and Good Practice (pp. 135). London: Whurr.Nicolson, R.I. and Fawcett, A.J. (1990) Automaticity: A new framework fordyslexia research? Cognition 35 (2), 159182.Nicolson, R.I. and Fawcett, A.J. (1995) <strong>Dyslexia</strong> is more than a phonologicaldisability. <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 1, 1936.Nicolson, R.I. and Fawcett, A.J. (1996) The <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Early Screen<strong>in</strong>g Test. London:The Psychological Corporation.Nicolson, R.I. and Fawcett, A.J. (2001) <strong>Dyslexia</strong> as a learn<strong>in</strong>g disability. InA.J. Fawcett (ed.) <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. Theory and Good Practice (pp. 141159). London:Whurr.


References 223Nicolson, R.I. and Fawcett, A.J. (2006) Do cerebellar deficits underlie phonologicalproblems <strong>in</strong> dyslexia? Developmental Science 9 (3), 259262.Nicolson, R.I., Fawcett, A.J., Berry, E.L., Jenk<strong>in</strong>s, I.H., Dean, P. and Brooks, D.J.(1999) Motor learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties and abnormal cerebellar activation <strong>in</strong>dyslexic adults. Lancet 353, 4347.Nijakowska, J. (2004) Usprawnianie umiejętności odczytywania i zapisywaniawyrazów wjęzyku angielskim mlodzieży z dysleksją rozwojową [Improv<strong>in</strong>gan ability to read and spell English words <strong>in</strong> dyslexic adolescents]. InM. Bodganowicz and M. Smoleń (eds) Dysleksja w kontekście nauczania językówobcych [<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Teach<strong>in</strong>g] (pp. 144155). Gdańsk:Wydawnictwo Harmonia.Nijakowska, J. (2007) Tam<strong>in</strong>g grammar. The Teacher 89 (51), 1925.Nijakowska, J. (2008) An experiment with direct multisensory <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong>teach<strong>in</strong>g word read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g to Polish dyslexic learners of English. InJ. Kormos and E.H. Kontra (eds) <strong>Language</strong> Learners with Special Needs.An International Perspective (pp. 130157). Bristol: Multil<strong>in</strong>gual Matters.Nikolopoulos, D., Goulandris, N., Hulme, C. and Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, M. (2006) Thecognitive basis of learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and spell <strong>in</strong> Greek: Evidence from alongitud<strong>in</strong>al study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 94, 117.Olson, R.K., Wise, B., Conners, F., Rack, J. and Fulker, D. (1989) Specific deficits <strong>in</strong>component read<strong>in</strong>g and language skills genetic and environmental<strong>in</strong>fluences. Journal of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 22, 339348.Opolska, T. (1997) Pokonujemy trudności w czytaniu i pisaniu. Przegląd metod pracykorekcyjno-kompensacyjnej [Deal<strong>in</strong>g with Read<strong>in</strong>g and Spell<strong>in</strong>g Difficulties. AnOverview of Methods of Correction-compensation Work]. Warszawa: CentrumMetodyczne Pomocy Psychologiczno-Pedagogicznej M<strong>in</strong>isterstwa EdukacjiNarodowej.Oren, R. and Breznitz, Z. (2005) Read<strong>in</strong>g processes <strong>in</strong> L1 and L2 among dyslexicscompared to regular bil<strong>in</strong>gual readers: Behavioral and electrophysiologicalevidence. Journal of Neurol<strong>in</strong>guistics 18, 127151.Oszwa, U. (2000) Analiza przejawów agramatyzmu u dzieci z dysleksjąrozwojową [Analysis of agrammatisms <strong>in</strong> dyslexic children]. In A. Borkowskaand E.M. Szepietowska (eds) Diagnoza neuropsychologiczna. Metodologia imetodyka [Neuropsychological Assessment. Methodology and Practice] (pp. 289306). Lubl<strong>in</strong>: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Marii Curie-Sklodowskiej.Ott, P. (1997) How to Detect and Manage <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. A Reference and Resource Manual.Oxford: He<strong>in</strong>emann Educational.Pavlidis, G. (1986) The role of eye movements <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> diagnosis of dyslexia. InG. Pavlidis and D. Fisher (eds) <strong>Dyslexia</strong>: Its Neuropsychology and Treatment (pp.97111). New York: John Wiley & Sons.Payne, J. (1995) English Guides 8: Spell<strong>in</strong>g. London: HarperColl<strong>in</strong>s.Penn<strong>in</strong>gton, B.F. (1989) Us<strong>in</strong>g genetics to understand dyslexia. Annals of <strong>Dyslexia</strong>39, 8193.Penn<strong>in</strong>gton, B.F. and Smith, S.D. (1988) Genetic <strong>in</strong>fluences on learn<strong>in</strong>g disability.Journal of Consult<strong>in</strong>g & Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Psychology 56, 817823.Penn<strong>in</strong>gton, B.F., McCabe, L.L., Smith, S.D., Lefy, D.L., Bookman, M.O.,Kimberl<strong>in</strong>g, W.J. and Lubs, H.A. (1986) Spell<strong>in</strong>g errors <strong>in</strong> adults with a formof familial dyslexia. Child Development 57, 10011013.Petrus, P. and Bogdanowicz, M. (2004) Sprawność fonologiczna dzieci wkontekście nauki języka angielskiego [Children’s phonological ability <strong>in</strong>foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g]. In M. Bogdanowicz and M. Smoleń (eds) Dysleksja


224 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>w kontekście nauczania języków obcych [<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Teach<strong>in</strong>g](pp. 6377). Gdańsk: Wydawnictwo Harmonia.Pętlewska, H. (1999) Przezwyciężanie trudności w czytaniu i pisaniu [Overcom<strong>in</strong>gRead<strong>in</strong>g and Spell<strong>in</strong>g Difficulties]. Kraków: Oficyna Wydawnicza ‘Impuls’.Phillips, S. (1993) Young Learners. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Phillips, B.M., Clancy-Menchetti, J. and Lonigan, C.J. (2008) Successful phonologicalawareness <strong>in</strong>struction with preschool children. Lessons from <strong>the</strong> classroom.Topics <strong>in</strong> Early Childhood Special Education 28 (1), 317.Piechurska-Kuciel, E. (2008) Input, process<strong>in</strong>g and output anxiety <strong>in</strong> studentswith symptoms of developmental dyslexia. In J. Kormos and E.H. Kontra(eds) <strong>Language</strong> Learners with Special Needs (pp. 86109). Bristol: Multil<strong>in</strong>gualMatters.Pietras, I. (2007) O klasifikacji blędów w dysortografii [Classify<strong>in</strong>g errors <strong>in</strong>dysorthography]. In M. Kostka-Szymańska and G. Krasowicz-Kupis (eds)Dysleksja. Problem znany czy nieznany? [<strong>Dyslexia</strong>. Familiar or UnfamiliarProblem?] (pp. 8192). Lubl<strong>in</strong>: Wydawnictwo UMCS.Pietras, I. (2008) Dysortografia uwarunkowania psychologiczne [Dysorthography Psychological Underp<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gs]. Gdańsk: Wydawnictwo Harmonia.Plessen von, K., Lundervold, A., Duta, N., Heiervang, E., Klauschen, F., Smievoll,A.I., Ersland, L. and Hugdahl, K. (2002) Less developed corpus callosum <strong>in</strong>dyslexic subject a structural MRI study. Neuropsychologia 40, 10351044.Porter, J.E. and Rourke, B.P. (1985) Socioemotional function<strong>in</strong>g of learn<strong>in</strong>gdisabled children: A subtypal analysis of personality patterns. In B.P. Rourke(ed.) Neuropsychology of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities (pp. 257277). New York: TheGuilford Press.Preis, S., Ste<strong>in</strong>metz, H., Knorr, U. and Jä¨ncke, L. (2000) Corpus callosum size <strong>in</strong>children with developmental language disorder. Cognitive Bra<strong>in</strong> Research 10,3744.Pugh, K.R., Sandak, R., Frost, S.J., Moore, D. and E<strong>in</strong>ar Mencl, W. (2005)Exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g development and read<strong>in</strong>g disability <strong>in</strong> English languagelearners: Potential contributions from neuroimag<strong>in</strong>g. Learn<strong>in</strong>g DisabilitiesResearch and Practice 20 (1), 2430.Puolakanaho, A., Ahonen, T., Aro, M., Eklund, K., Leppä¨nen, P.H.T., Ppikkeus, A-M.,Tolvanen, A., Torppa, M. and Lyyt<strong>in</strong>en, H. (2008) Developmental l<strong>in</strong>ks ofvery early phonological and language skills to second grade read<strong>in</strong>g outcomes.Strong to accuracy but only m<strong>in</strong>or to fluency. Journal of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 41 (4),353370.Rae, C., Harasty, J.A., Dzendrowskyj, T.E., Talcott, J.B., Simpson, J.M. andBlamire, A.M. (2002) Cerebellar morphology <strong>in</strong> developmental dyslexia.Neuropsychologia 40, 12851292.Ramus, F. (2004) Neurobiology of dyslexia: A re<strong>in</strong>terpretation of <strong>the</strong> data. Trends<strong>in</strong> Neurosciences 27 (12), 720726.Ramus, F. (2006) Genes, bra<strong>in</strong>, and cognition: A roadmap for <strong>the</strong> cognitivescientist. Cognition 101, 247269.Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dak<strong>in</strong>, S.C., Day, B.L., Castellote, J.M., White, S. and Frith, U.(2003) Theories of developmental dyslexia: Insights from a multiple case studyof dyslexic adults. Bra<strong>in</strong> 126, 841865.Ramus, F., White, S. and Frith, U. (2006) Weigh<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> evidence betweencompet<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ories of dyslexia. Developmental Science 9 (3), 265269.Reed, D. and Stansfield, C. (2004) Us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Modern <strong>Language</strong> Aptitude Test toidentify a foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g disability: Is it ethical? <strong>Language</strong>Assessment Quarterly 1, 161176.


References 225Reid, G. (1998) <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. A Practitioner’s Handbook. Chichester: Wiley.Reid, G. and Fawcett, A.J. (2004) An overview of developments <strong>in</strong> dyslexia. InG. Reid and A.J. Fawcett (eds) <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> Context. Research, Policy and Practice(pp. 319). London: Whurr.Richards, T.L. (2001) Functional magnetic resonance imag<strong>in</strong>g and spectroscopicimag<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong>: Application of <strong>the</strong> fMRI and fMRS to read<strong>in</strong>g disabilitiesand education. Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disability Quarterly 24, 189203.Richards, T.L., Cornia, D., Seraf<strong>in</strong>i, S., Steury, K., Echelard, D.R., Dager, S.R.,Marro, K., Abbott, R.D., Maravilla, K.R. and Bern<strong>in</strong>ger, V.W. (2000). The effectsof a phonologically driven treatment for dyslexia on lactate levels as measuresby proton MRSI. American Journal of Neuroradiology 21, 916922.R<strong>in</strong>volucri, M. (1985) Grammar Games. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Ritchey, K.D. and Goeke, J.L. (2006) Orton-Gill<strong>in</strong>gham and Orton-Gill<strong>in</strong>ghambased read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction: A review of literature. The Journal of SpecialEducation 40 (3), 171183.Robertson, J. (2000a) Neuropsychological <strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>in</strong> dyslexia: Two studies onBritish pupils. Journal of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 33 (2), 137149.Robertson, J. (2000b) The neuropsychology of modern foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g.In L. Peer and G. Reid (eds) Multil<strong>in</strong>gualism, Literacy and <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. A Challengefor Educators (pp. 203210). London: David Fulton.Robertson, J. and Bakker, D.J. (2008) Model równowagi pólkulowej i dysleksja[The balance model of read<strong>in</strong>g and dyslexia]. In G. Reid and J. Wearmouth(eds) Dysleksja. Teoria i praktyka [<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and Literacy. Theory and Practice](pp. 141162). Gdańsk: Gdańskie Wydawnictwo Psychologiczne.Rosenthal, J. (1970) A prelim<strong>in</strong>ary psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic study of children withlearn<strong>in</strong>g disabilities. Journal of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 3, 181198.Ryan, M. (1992) The social and emotional effects of dyslexia. Education Digest57 (5), 6871.Saduś, Z. (2000) Analiza blędów ortograficznych popelnianych przez uczniów wstarszym wieku szkolnym oraz sposoby ich przezwyciężania [The analysis oforthographic errors committed by older school children and ways of overcom<strong>in</strong>gspell<strong>in</strong>g problems]. In W. Turewicz (ed.) Jak pomóc dziecku zdysortografią [How to Help a Child with Spell<strong>in</strong>g Difficulties?] (pp. 2536). ZielonaGóra: Ośrodek Doskonalenia Nauczycieli w Zielonej Górze.Saduś, Z. (2003) Jak pomóc uczniowi w nauce ortografii. Poradnik dla nauczycieli irodziców [How to Help Students <strong>in</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g Orthography? A Guide for Teachers andParents]. Opole: Wydawnictwo Oświatowe ‘Promyk’.Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2007) Epil<strong>in</strong>guistic and metal<strong>in</strong>guistic phonological awarenessmay be subject to different constra<strong>in</strong>s: Evidence from Hebrew. First<strong>Language</strong> 27, 385405.Savage, R. (2007) Cerebellar tasks do not dist<strong>in</strong>guish between children withdevelopmental dyslexia and children with <strong>in</strong>tellectual disability. ChildNeuropsychology 13, 389407.Sawa, B. (1999) Jeżeli dziecko źle czyta i pisze [If a Child Reads and Spells Poorly].Warszawa: Wydawnictwa Szkolne i Pedagogiczne.Schneider, E. (1999) Multisensory Structured Metacognitive Instruction. An Approachto Teach<strong>in</strong>g a <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> to at-Risk Students. Frankfurt am Ma<strong>in</strong>: Peter Lang.Schneider, E. and Crombie, M. (2003) <strong>Dyslexia</strong> and <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g.London: David Fulton.Seymour, P.H.K. (2005) Early read<strong>in</strong>g development <strong>in</strong> European orthographies. InM.J. Snowl<strong>in</strong>g and C. Hulme (eds) The Science of Read<strong>in</strong>g: A Handbook (pp. 296315). Malden, MA: Blackwell.


226 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Seymour, P.H.K., Aro, M. and Ersk<strong>in</strong>e, J.M. (2003) Foundation literacy acquisition<strong>in</strong> European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology 94, 143174.Shaw, R. (1999) The case for course substitutions as a reasonable accommodationfor students with foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties. Journal of Learn<strong>in</strong>gDisabilities 32, 320328.Shaywitz, B.A., Shaywitz, S.E., Blachman, B.A., Pugh, K.R., Fulbright, R.K.,Skudlarski, R., E<strong>in</strong>ar Mencl, W., Constable, R.T., Holahan, J.M., Marchione, K.E.,Fletcher, J.M., Reid Lyon, G. and Gore, J.C. (2004) Development of leftoccipitotemporal systems for skilled read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> children after a phonologicallybased<strong>in</strong>tervention. Biological Psychiatry 55, 926933.Shaywitz, S.E. (1997) Dysleksja [<strong>Dyslexia</strong>]. Świat Nauki 1, 5864.Shaywitz, S.E., Shaywitz, B.A., Fulbright, R.K., Skudlarski, P., Mencl, W.E.,Constable, R.T., Pugh, K.R., Holahan, J.M., Marchione, K.E., Fletcher, J.M.,Lyon, G.R. and Gore, J.C. (2003) Neural systems for compensation andpersistence: Young adult outcome of childhood read<strong>in</strong>g disability. BiologicalPsychiatry 54, 2533.Shemesh, R. and Waller, S. (2000) Teach<strong>in</strong>g English Spell<strong>in</strong>g. Cambridge: CambridgeUniversity Press.Shovman, M.M. and Ahissar, M. (2006) Isolat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> impact of visual perceptionon dyslexics’ read<strong>in</strong>g ability. Vision Research 46, 35143525.Siegal, L.S. (1989) IQ is irrelevant to <strong>the</strong> def<strong>in</strong>ition of learn<strong>in</strong>g disabilities. Journalof Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 22, 46978.Silani, G., Frith, U., Demonet, J.F., Fazio, F., Perani, D., Price, C., Frith, C.D. andPaulesu, E. (2005) Bra<strong>in</strong> abnormalities underly<strong>in</strong>g altered activation <strong>in</strong>dyslexia: A voxel based morphometry study. Bra<strong>in</strong> 128, 24532461.Simos, P.G., Fletcher, J.M., Bergman, E., Breier, J.I., Foorman, B.R., Castillo, E.M.,Fitzgerald, M. and Papanicolau, A.C. (2002) <strong>Dyslexia</strong>-specific bra<strong>in</strong> activationprofile becomes normal follow<strong>in</strong>g successful remedial tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g. Neurology 58,12031213.Siok, W.T., Niu, Z., J<strong>in</strong>, Z., Perfetti, C.A. and Tan, L.H. (2008) From <strong>the</strong> cover: Astructural-functional basis for dyslexia <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> cortex of Ch<strong>in</strong>ese readers. PNAS105 (14), 55615566.Skibińska, H. (2001) Praca korekcyjno-kompensacyjna z dziećmi z trudnościami wczytaniu i pisaniu [Correction-compensation Work with Children Encounter<strong>in</strong>gRead<strong>in</strong>g and Spell<strong>in</strong>g Difficulties]. Bydgoszcz: Wydawnictwo Uczelniane AkademiiBydgoskiej im. Kazimierza Wielkiego.Slob<strong>in</strong>, D.I. (1971) Psychol<strong>in</strong>guistics. London: Foresman.Smith, S. (2002) Considerations <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> development of foreign languagesubstitution policies at <strong>the</strong> postsecondary level for students with learn<strong>in</strong>gdisabilities. Association of Department of <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong>s (ADFL) Bullet<strong>in</strong> 33,6167.Smy<strong>the</strong>, I. and Everatt, J. (2000) <strong>Dyslexia</strong> diagnosis <strong>in</strong> different languages. InL. Peer and G. Reid (eds) Multil<strong>in</strong>gualism, Literacy and <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. A Challenge forEducators (pp. 1221). London: David Fulton.Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, M. (1981) Phonemic defects <strong>in</strong> developmental dyslexia. PsychologicalResearch 43, 219234.Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, M.J. (2000) <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. Oxford: Blackwell.Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, M.J. (2001a) From language to read<strong>in</strong>g and dyslexia. <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 7 (1), 3746.Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, M.J. (2001b) Developmental dyslexia. Current Paediatrics 11, 1013.Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, M.J. and Caravolas, M. (2007) Developmental dyslexia. In M.G. Gaskell(ed.) The Oxford Handbook of Psychol<strong>in</strong>guistics (pp. 667683). Oxford: OxfordUniversity Press.


References 227Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, M.J. and Nation, K. (1997) <strong>Language</strong>, phonology and learn<strong>in</strong>g to read.In C. Hulme and M.J. Snowl<strong>in</strong>g (eds) <strong>Dyslexia</strong>: Biology, Cognition andIntervention (pp. 153166). London: Whurr.Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, M.J., Gallagher, A. and Frith, U. (2003) Family risk of dyslexia iscont<strong>in</strong>uous: Individual differences <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> precursors of read<strong>in</strong>g skill. ChildDevelopment 74, 358373.Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, M.J., Muter, V. and Carrol, J. (2007) Children at family risk of dyslexia:A follow-up <strong>in</strong> early adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 48(6), 609618.Sochacka, K. (2004) Rozwój umiejętności czytania [Read<strong>in</strong>g Ability Development].Bialystok: Trans Humana.Sparks, R. (1995) Exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistic Cod<strong>in</strong>g Differences Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis toexpla<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g. Annals of <strong>Dyslexia</strong>45, 187214.Sparks, R. (2001) <strong>Foreign</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g problems of students classified aslearn<strong>in</strong>g disabled and non-learn<strong>in</strong>g disabled: Is <strong>the</strong>re a difference? Topics <strong>in</strong><strong>Language</strong> Disorders 21 (1), 3854.Sparks, R. (2006) Is <strong>the</strong>re a ‘disability’ for learn<strong>in</strong>g a foreign language? Journal ofLearn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 39 (6), 544557.Sparks, R. and Ganschow, L. (1991) <strong>Foreign</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties:Affective or native language aptitude differences? Modern <strong>Language</strong> Journal75, 316.Sparks, R. and Ganschow, L. (1993) The effects of multisensory structuredlanguage <strong>in</strong>struction on native language and foreign language aptitude skillsof at-risk high school foreign language learners: A replication and follow-upstudy. Annals of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 4, 194216.Sparks, R. and Ganschow, L. (1995) Parent perceptions <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> screen<strong>in</strong>g forperformance <strong>in</strong> foreign language courses. <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Annals 28, 371391.Sparks, R. and Ganschow, L. (1996) Teachers’ perceptions of students’ foreignlanguage academic skills and affective characteristics. Journal of EducationalResearch 89, 172185.Sparks, R. and Javorsky, J. (1999) Students classified as learn<strong>in</strong>g disabled and <strong>the</strong>college foreign language requirement: Replication and comparison studies.Journal of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 32, 332349.Sparks, R. and Miller, K.S. (2000) Teach<strong>in</strong>g a foreign language us<strong>in</strong>g multisensorystructured language techniques to at-risk learners: A review. <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 6, 124132.Sparks, R., Ganschow, L. and Pohlman, J. (1989) L<strong>in</strong>guistic cod<strong>in</strong>g deficits <strong>in</strong>foreign language learners. Annals of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 39, 179195.Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., Kenneweg, S. and Miller, K. (1991) Use of an Orton-Gill<strong>in</strong>gham approach to teach a foreign language to dyslexic/learn<strong>in</strong>gdisabledstudents: Explicit teach<strong>in</strong>g of phonology <strong>in</strong> a second language.Annals of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 41, 96118.Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., Javorsky, J., Pohlman, J. and Patton, J. (1992a) Testcomparisons among students identified as high-risk, low-risk, and learn<strong>in</strong>gdisabled <strong>in</strong> high school foreign language courses. Modern <strong>Language</strong> Journal 72,142159.Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., Pohlman, J., Sk<strong>in</strong>ner, S. and Artzer, M. (1992b)The effects of multisensory structured language <strong>in</strong>struction on nativelanguage and foreign language aptitude skills of at-risk high school foreignlanguage learners. Annals of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 42, 2553.


228 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Sparks, R., Ganschow, L. and Javorsky, J. (1993) Perceptions of high- and low-riskstudents and students with learn<strong>in</strong>g disabilities about high-school foreignlanguage courses. <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Annals 26, 491510.Sparks, R., Ganschow, L. and Javorsky, J. (1995a) I know one when I see one (or,I know one because I am one): A response to Mabbot. <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Annals28 (4), 479487.Sparks, R., Ganschow, L. and Patton, J. (1995b) Prediction of performance <strong>in</strong> firstyearforeign language courses: Connections between native and foreignlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g. Journal of Educational Psychology 87 (4), 638655.Sparks, R., Javorsky, J. and Ganschow, L. (1995c) Satiat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> appetite of <strong>the</strong>sociologic sponge: A rejo<strong>in</strong>der to Mabbot. <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Annals 28 (4),495498.Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., Fluharty, K. and Little, S. (1996) An exploratory studyon <strong>the</strong> effects of Lat<strong>in</strong> on <strong>the</strong> native language skills and foreign languageaptitude of students with and without learn<strong>in</strong>g disabilities. The ClassicalJournal 91 (2), 165184.Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., Artzer, M. and Patton, J. (1997a) <strong>Foreign</strong> languageproficiency of at-risk and not-at-risk learners over 2 years of foreign language<strong>in</strong>struction: A follow-up study. Journal of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 30 (1), 9298.Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., Artzer, M., Siebenhar, D. and Plageman, M. (1997b)Anxiety and proficiency <strong>in</strong> a foreign language. Perceptual and Motor Skills 85,559562.Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., Patton, J., Artzer, M., Siebenhar, D. and Plageman, M.(1997c) Prediction of foreign language proficiency. Journal of EducationalPsychology 89 (3), 549561.Sparks, R., Artzer, M., Ganschow, L., Siebenhar, D., Plageman, M. and Patton, J.(1998a) Differences <strong>in</strong> native-language skills, foreign-language aptitude, andforeign-language grades among high-, average-, and low-proficiency foreignlanguagelearners: Two studies. <strong>Language</strong> Test<strong>in</strong>g 15 (2), 181216.Sparks, R., Artzer, M., Javorsky, J., Patton, J., Ganschow, L., Miller, K. andHordubay, D. (1998b) Students classified as learn<strong>in</strong>g disabled and nonlearn<strong>in</strong>gdisabled: Two comparison studies of native language skill, foreignlanguage aptitude, and foreign language proficiency. <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Annals31 (4), 535550.Sparks, R., Artzer, M., Patton, J., Ganschow, L., Miller, K., Hordubay, D. andWalsh, G. (1998c) Benefits of multisensory structured <strong>in</strong>struction for at-riskforeign language learners: A comparison study of high school Spanishstudents. Annals of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 48, 239270.Sparks, R., Philips, L. and Javorsky, J. (2002) Students classified as LD whoreceived course substitutions for <strong>the</strong> college foreign language requirement: Areplication study. Journal of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 35 (6), 482499, 538.Sparks, R., Philips, L. and Javorsky, J. (2003) Students classified as LD whopetitioned for or fulfilled <strong>the</strong> foreign language requirement Are <strong>the</strong>ydifferent? A replication study. Journal of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 36 (4), 348362.Sparks, R., Patton, J., Ganschow, L., Humbach, N. and Javorsky, J. (2006) Nativelanguage predictions of foreign language proficiency and foreign languageaptitude. Annals of <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 56 (1), 129160.Sparks, R., Ganschow, L. and Patton, J. (2008a) L1 and L2 literacy, aptitude andaffective variables as discrim<strong>in</strong>ators among high- and low-achiev<strong>in</strong>g L2learners with special needs. In J. Kormos and E.H. Kontra (eds) <strong>Language</strong>Learners with Special Needs (pp. 1135). Bristol: Multil<strong>in</strong>gual Matters.


References 229Sparks, R., Humbach, N. and Javorsky, J. (2008b) Individual and longitud<strong>in</strong>aldifferences among high- and low-achiev<strong>in</strong>g, LD and ADHD L2 learners.Learn<strong>in</strong>g and Individual Differences 8, 2943.Spencer, K. (2007) Predict<strong>in</strong>g children’s word-spell<strong>in</strong>g difficulty for commonEnglish words from measures of orthographic transparency, phonemic andgraphemic length and word frequency. British Journal of Psychology 98, 305338.Spencer, L.H. and Hanley, J.R. (2003) Effects of orthographic transparency onread<strong>in</strong>g and phoneme awareness <strong>in</strong> children learn<strong>in</strong>g to read <strong>in</strong> Wales. BritishJournal of Psychology 94, 128.Spionek, H. (1985) Zaburzenia rozwoju uczniów a niepowodzenia szkolne [DevelopmentalDisorders vs. Scholastic Failure]. Warszawa: PWN.Spolsky, B. (1989) Conditions for Second <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g. Oxford: OxfordUniversity Press.Stambolitzis, A. and Pumfrey, P. (2000) Text genre, miscue analysis, bil<strong>in</strong>gualismand dyslexia: Teach<strong>in</strong>g strategies with junior school pupils. In L. Peer andG. Reid (eds) Multil<strong>in</strong>gualism, Literacy and <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. A Challenge for Educators(pp. 7079). London: David Fulton.Stanisz, A. (1998) Przystępny kurs statystyki [An Easy Course <strong>in</strong> Statistics]. Kraków:StatSoft.Stanovich, K.E. (1980) Towards an <strong>in</strong>teractive-compensatory model of <strong>in</strong>dividualdifferences <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> development of read<strong>in</strong>g fluency. Read<strong>in</strong>g Research Quarterly16, 3271.Stanovich, K.E. (1986) Mat<strong>the</strong>w effects <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g: Some consequences of<strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> acquisition of literacy. Read<strong>in</strong>g Research Quarterly21, 360407.Stanovich, K.E. (1996) Towards a more <strong>in</strong>clusive def<strong>in</strong>ition of dyslexia. <strong>Dyslexia</strong>2 (3), 154166.Ste<strong>in</strong>, J.F. (2001) The magnocellular <strong>the</strong>ory of developmental dyslexia. <strong>Dyslexia</strong>7 (1), 1236.Ste<strong>in</strong>, J.F. and Talcott, J.B. (1999) The magnocellular <strong>the</strong>ory of dyslexia. <strong>Dyslexia</strong>5 (2), 5978.Ste<strong>in</strong>, J.F., Richardson, A.J. and Fawler, M.S. (2000a) Monocular occlusion canimprove b<strong>in</strong>ocular control and read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> developmental dyslexics. Bra<strong>in</strong> (1), 23.Ste<strong>in</strong>, J.F., Talcott, J.B. and Walsh, V. (2000b) Controversy about <strong>the</strong> visualmagnocellular deficit <strong>in</strong> developmental dyslexics. Trends <strong>in</strong> Cognitive Sciences 4(6), 209211.Ste<strong>in</strong>, J.F., Talcott, J.B. and Witton, C. (2001) The sensorimotor basis ofdevelopmental dyslexia. In A.J. Fawcett (ed.) <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. Theory and Good Practice(pp. 6588). London: Whurr.Stoodley, C.J., Talcott, J.B., Carter, E.L., Witton, C. and Ste<strong>in</strong>, J.F. (2000) Selectivedeficits of vibrotactile sensitivity <strong>in</strong> dyslexic readers. Neuroscience Letters 295,1316.Stoodley, C.J., Fawcett, A.J., Nicolson, R.I. and Ste<strong>in</strong>, J.F. (2006) Balanc<strong>in</strong>g andpo<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g tasks <strong>in</strong> dyslexic and control adults. <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 12, 276288.Szczerbiński, M. (2007) Dysleksja rozwojowa: próba def<strong>in</strong>icji [Developmentaldyslexia: An attempt towards def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> disorder]. In M. Kostka-Szymańskaand G. Krasowicz-Kupis (eds) Dysleksja. Problem znany czy nieznany? [<strong>Dyslexia</strong>.Familiar or Unfamiliar Problem?] (pp. 4870). Lubl<strong>in</strong>: Wydawnictwo UniwersytetuMarii-Sklodowskiej.Szempruch, J. (1997) Umiejętność czytania a osiągnięcia szkolne uczniów klaspoczątkujących [Read<strong>in</strong>g Ability vs. Educational Achievement of First-graders].Rzeszów: Wydawnictwo Wyższej Szkoly Pedagogicznej.


230 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Szenkovits, G. and Ramus, F. (2005) Explor<strong>in</strong>g dyslexics’ phonological deficit I:Lexical vs. sub-lexical and <strong>in</strong>put vs. output processes. <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 11, 253268.Szepietowska, E.M. (2002) Badanie neuropsychologiczne. Procedura i ocena [NeuropsychologicalAssessment. Procedure and Evaluation]. Lubl<strong>in</strong>: WydawnictwoUniwersytetu Marii Curie-Sklodowskiej.Talcott, J.B., Witton, C., McClean, M.F., Hansen, P.C., Rees, A., Green, G.G.R. andSte<strong>in</strong>, J.F. (1999) Can sensitivity to auditory frequency modulation predictchildren’s phonological and read<strong>in</strong>g skills? NeuroReport 10, 20452050.Talcott, J.B., Hansen, P.C., Assoku, E.L. and Ste<strong>in</strong>, J.F. (2000a) Visual motionsensitivity <strong>in</strong> dyslexia: Evidence for temporal and energy <strong>in</strong>tegration deficits.Neuropsychologia 38, 935943.Talcott, J.B., Witton, C., McClean, M.F., Hansen, P.C., Rees, A., Green, G.G.R. andSte<strong>in</strong>, J.F. (2000b) Dynamic sensory sensitivity and children’s word decod<strong>in</strong>gskills. Proceed<strong>in</strong>gs of <strong>the</strong> National Academy of Science 97 (6), 29522957.Tallal, P. (1980) Auditory temporal perception, phonics and read<strong>in</strong>g disabilities <strong>in</strong>dyslexic children. Bra<strong>in</strong> and <strong>Language</strong> 9, 182198.Tallal, P., Miller, S.L., Bedi, G., Byma, G., Wang, X., Nagarajan, S., Schre<strong>in</strong>er, C.,Jenk<strong>in</strong>s, W.M. and Merzenich, M.M. (1996) Fast-element enhanced speechimproves language comprehension <strong>in</strong> language-learn<strong>in</strong>g impaired children.Science 271, 8184.Temple, E. (2002) Bra<strong>in</strong> mechanisms <strong>in</strong> normal and dyslexic readers. CurrentOp<strong>in</strong>ion <strong>in</strong> Neurobiology 12, 178183.Temple, E., Poldrack, R.A., Salidis, J., Deutsch, G.K., Tallal, P., Merzenich, M.M.and Gabrieli, J.D. (2001) Disrupted neural responses to phonological andorthographic process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> dyslexic children: An fMRI study. Neuroreport 12,299307.Thomson, M.E. and Watk<strong>in</strong>s, E.J. (1990) <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. A Teach<strong>in</strong>g Handbook. London:Whurr.Thornbury, S. (1999) How to Teach Grammar. Harlow: Longman.Tod, J. (2008) Indywidualne plany nauczania dla uczniów dyslektycznych[Individual educational plans and dyslexia]. In G. Reid and J. Wearmouth(eds) Dysleksja. Teoria i praktyka [<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and Literacy. Theory and Practice](pp. 355384). Gdańsk: Gdańskie Wydawnictwo Psychologiczne.Tomaszewska, A. (2001) Prawo do nauki dziecka z dysleksją rozwojową wświadomośc<strong>in</strong>auczycieli [Teachers’ Awareness of Dyslexic Children’s Right to Learn]. Kraków:Oficyna Wydawnicza ‘Impuls’.Treiman, R. and Kessler, B. (2007) Learn<strong>in</strong>g to read. In M.G. Gaskell (ed.) TheOxford Handbook of Psychol<strong>in</strong>guistics (pp. 657666). Oxford: Oxford UniversityPress.Turner, M. (2003) Diagnoza dysleksji w Wielkiej Brytanii przegląd testów[Assessment of dyslexia <strong>in</strong> Great Brita<strong>in</strong> review of tests]. In B. Kaja (ed.)Diagnoza dysleksji [<strong>Dyslexia</strong> Assessment] (pp. 3649). Bydgoszcz: WydawnictwoAkademii Bydgoskiej im. Kazimierza Wielkiego.Uppstad, P.H. and Tønnessen, F.E. (2007) The notion of ‘phonology’ <strong>in</strong> dyslexiaresearch: Cognitivism and beyond. <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 13, 154174.Ur, P. (1988) Grammar Practice Activities: A Practical Guide for Teachers. Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.Van der Leij, A. and Morfidi, E. (2006) Core deficit and variable differences<strong>in</strong> Dutch poor readers learn<strong>in</strong>g English. Journal of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 39 (1),7490.Vellut<strong>in</strong>o, F.R. (1979) <strong>Dyslexia</strong>: Theory and Research. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.


References 231Vellut<strong>in</strong>o, F.R., Scanlon, D.M. and Tanzman, M.S. (1998) The case for early<strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>in</strong> diagnos<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g disability. Journal of School Psychology 36 (4),367394.Vellut<strong>in</strong>o, F.R., Fletcher, J.M., Snowl<strong>in</strong>g, M.J. and Scanlon, D.M. (2004) Specificread<strong>in</strong>g disability (dyslexia): What have we learned <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> past four decades?Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 45 (1), 240.Vogel, S.A. (1983) A qualitative analysis of morphological ability <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g andachiev<strong>in</strong>g children. Journal of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 16, 416420.Vousden, J.I. (2008) Units of English spell<strong>in</strong>g-to-sound mapp<strong>in</strong>gs: A rationalapproach to read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction. Applied Cognitive Psychology 22, 247272.Vukovic, L.K. and Siegel, L.S. (2006) The double-deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis: A comprehensiveanalysis of <strong>the</strong> evidence. Journal of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 39 (1), 2547.Wagner, R., Francis, D.J. and Morris, R.D. (2005) Identify<strong>in</strong>g English languagelearners with learn<strong>in</strong>g disabilities: Key challenges and possible approaches.Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities Research & Practice 20 (1), 615.Wearmouth, J. and Reid, G. (2008) Ocena i planowanie nauczania oraz nauki[Issues for assessment of plann<strong>in</strong>g of teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g]. In G. Reid andJ. Wearmouth (eds) Dysleksja. Teoria i praktyka [<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and Literacy. Theory andPractice] (pp. 211234). Gdańsk: Gdańskie Wydawnictwo Psychologiczne.White, S., Milne, E., Rosen, S., Hansen, P., Swettenham, J., Frith, U. and Ramus, F.(2006) The role of sensorimotor impairments <strong>in</strong> dyslexia: A multiple casestudy of dyslexic children. Developmental Science 9 (3), 237255.Whiteley, H.E., Smith, C.D. and Connors, L. (2007) Young children at risk ofliteracy difficulties: Factors predict<strong>in</strong>g recovery from risk follow<strong>in</strong>g phonologicallybased <strong>in</strong>tervention. Journal of Research <strong>in</strong> Read<strong>in</strong>g 30 (3), 249269.Wiig, E.H. and Semel, E.M. (1976) <strong>Language</strong> Disabilities <strong>in</strong> Children and Adolescents.Columbus, OH: Charles E. Merrill.Wiig, E.H. and Semel, E.M. (1984) <strong>Language</strong> Assessment and Intervention for <strong>the</strong>Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabled (2nd edn). Columbus, OH: Charles E. Merrill.Wiig, E.H., Semel, E.M. and Crouse, M.B. (1973) The use of English morphologyby high-risk and learn<strong>in</strong>g disabled children. Journal of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities 7,457465.Wimmer, H. (1993) Characteristics of developmental dyslexia <strong>in</strong> a regular read<strong>in</strong>gsystem. Applied Psychol<strong>in</strong>guistics 14, 133.Wimmer, H., Landerl, K. and Schneider, W. (1994) The role of rime awareness <strong>in</strong>learn<strong>in</strong>g to read a regular orthography. British Journal of DevelopmentalPsychology 12, 469484.Wise, J.C., Sevcik, R.A., Morris, R.D., Lovett, M.W. and Wolf, M. (2007) Thegrowth of phonological awareness by children with read<strong>in</strong>g disabilities: Aresult of semantic knowledge or knowledge of grapheme-phoneme correspondences?Scientific Studies of Read<strong>in</strong>g 11 (2), 151164.Wlodarski, Z. (1998) Psychologia uczenia się [Psychology of Learn<strong>in</strong>g]. Warszawa:Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN.Wolf, M. and Bowers, P.G. (1999) The double-deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis for <strong>the</strong>developmental dyslexia. Journal of Educational Psychology 91, 415438.Wolf, M. and O’Brien, B. (2001) On issue of time, fluency, and <strong>in</strong>tervention. InA.J. Fawcett (ed.) <strong>Dyslexia</strong>. Theory and Good Practice (pp. 124140). London:Whurr.Wolf, M., Vellut<strong>in</strong>o, F. and Berko Gleason, J. (2005) Psychol<strong>in</strong>gwistyczna analizaczytania [Psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic analysis of read<strong>in</strong>g]. In J. Berko Gleanson andN. Bernste<strong>in</strong> Ratner Psychol<strong>in</strong>gwistyka [Psychol<strong>in</strong>guistics] (pp. 439476).Gdańsk: Gdańskie Wydawnictwo Psychologiczne.


232 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Wszeborowska-Lipińska, B. (1996) Dysleksja a badanie rozwoju poziomuumyslowego [<strong>Dyslexia</strong> vs. assessment of <strong>the</strong> level of cognitive development].Psychologia Wychowawcza 2, 126133.Zakrzewska, B. (1999) Trudności w czytaniu i pisaniu. Modele ćwiczeń [Read<strong>in</strong>g andSpell<strong>in</strong>g Difficulties. Types of Exercises]. Warszawa: Wydawnictwa Szkolne iPedagogiczne Spólka Akcyjna.Zelech, W. (1997) Zaburzenia czytania i pisania u dzieci afatycznych, gluchych idyslektycznych [Read<strong>in</strong>g and Writ<strong>in</strong>g Disorders <strong>in</strong> Aphasic, Deaf and DyslexicChildren]. Kraków: Wydawnictwo Naukowe Wyższej Szkoly Pedagogicznej.Ziegler, J.C. and Goswami, U. (2005) Read<strong>in</strong>g acquisition, developmentaldyslexia, and skilled read<strong>in</strong>g across languages: A psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic gra<strong>in</strong> size<strong>the</strong>ory. Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong> 131, 329.Ziegler, J.C. and Goswami, U. (2006) Becom<strong>in</strong>g literate <strong>in</strong> different languages:Similar problems, different solutions. Developmental Science 9 (5), 426453.Ziegler, J., Perry, C., Ma-Wyatt, A., Ladner, D. and Schulte-Korne, G. (2003)Developmental dyslexia <strong>in</strong> different languages: <strong>Language</strong>-specific or universal?Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 86, 169193.


Indexaccommodations vi, ix, 4, 45, 73, 95, 102,117, 121, 126, 131, 145, 146, 147, 184, 217,226accuracy 14, 21, 22, 24, 25, 28, 29, 39, 51, 55,61, 82, 83, 85, 88, 89, 95, 96, 98, 103, 107,109, 110, 116, 119, 193, 224add<strong>in</strong>g 87, 91, 162, 175, 207alliteration 19, 87, 213alphabetic pr<strong>in</strong>ciple 9, 13, 15, 44, 48, 101, 193alphabetic stage 18, 19, 20anatomical magnetic resonance imag<strong>in</strong>g(aMFI) 39, 40anxiety 66, 69, 75, 76, 98, 99, 112, 147, 213,224, 228articulation 12, 16, 62attention span 54, 212auditory perception 12, 91auditory process<strong>in</strong>g 34, 43, 64automatisation viii, 18, 34, 47, 61, 62, 63, 87,103, 116, 179, 181automatisation deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis 61, 62balance 34, 52, 61, 87, 95, 121, 229balance model of read<strong>in</strong>g 18, 20, 118, 119,152, 214, 225bil<strong>in</strong>gual v, vii, 30, 82, 83, 101, 106, 107, 108,109, 113, 117, 118, 191, 194, 195, 214, 215,223, 229b<strong>in</strong>ocular <strong>in</strong>stability 58, 120blend<strong>in</strong>g 14, 45, 46, 87, 153, 156, 157, 161,162, 168, 197, 198, 200, 201bottom-up model/strategy 16, 17bra<strong>in</strong> function 20, 38, 39, 191bra<strong>in</strong> mechanism vii, 34, 35, 65, 152, 230bra<strong>in</strong> structure 37, 39, 40, 43, 63, 65, 190cerebellar v, 42, 52, 61, 62, 63, 65, 113, 215,223, 224, 225cerebellar deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis v, 42, 61, 62, 63,65, 113cerebellum 34, 42, 43, 52, 60, 61, 65, 215colour cod<strong>in</strong>g 148, 149, 157, 168, 169, 180,182corpus callosum 41, 43, 211, 224decod<strong>in</strong>g abilities 104, 107, 117, 195decod<strong>in</strong>g difficulties 52, 104, 217decod<strong>in</strong>g skills 6, 36, 68, 104, 107, 117, 230decod<strong>in</strong>g strategies 19, 154– s<strong>in</strong>gle word decod<strong>in</strong>g 7, 53, 86, 103– word decod<strong>in</strong>g 4, 7, 53, 55, 78, 85, 96, 103,107, 108, 117, 192, 195, 230deletions 29, 56, 87, 90, 162diagnosis x, 2, 6, 8, 15, 32, 37, 68, 71, 72, 100,101, 103, 105, 106, 109, 112, 120, 191, 194,195, 211, 212, 220, 223, 226dictation 88, 91, 103, 138, 174digit span tasks 47, 103, 104double-deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis v, 54, 213, 231dual route model of read<strong>in</strong>g 23dyseidetic 92, 113, 114dysphonetic 92, 113, 114educational accommodations vi, x, 73, 145educational system vii, x, 27, 145, 146, 194,196emotional-motivational problems/disorders v, 7, 12, 32, 89, 97, 122, 123encod<strong>in</strong>g 3, 4, 9, 10, 27, 85, 103, 104, 105,136, 192environment 4, 6, 8, 34, 36, 70, 81, 98, 100,102, 106, 107, 110, 113, 116, 145, 150, 165,190eye movements 58, 59, 223fluency 14, 20, 29, 30, 56, 96, 101, 112, 116,217, 224, 229, 231foreign language ability 77, 79, 132, 194foreign language acquisition 67, 74, 75, 77,194, 222foreign language aptitude 66, 67, 71, 75, 76,77, 78, 79, 80, 83, 84, 11, 128, 129, 130,131, 132, 216, 227, 228foreign language aptitude test 67, 71, 75, 76,77, 129, 130, 132foreign language competence 79, 194foreign language course 66, 72, 74, 76, 77,78, 79, 111, 128, 132, 133, 219, 227, 228foreign language grade 76, 77, 78, 79, 228foreign language learners– at-risk foreign language learners 70, 114,128, 129, 130, 131, 132, 228– good foreign language learners 72, 78, 80,128, 143– LD foreign language learners 71, 76, 77– poor foreign language learners 32, 68, 69,72, 75, 77, 80, 133, 135, 194foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g v, vii, viii, x, 32,233<strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>Index


234 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>66, 67, 68, 69, 70, 71, 72, 74, 75, 76, 77, 78,80, 83, 84, 90, 127, 134, 194, 195, 214, 215,216, 219, 222, 223, 224, 225foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g disability (FLLD)v, 70, 71, 216, 224foreign language performance 111, 128, 129foreign language proficiency viii, 75, 78, 80,132, 194, 228foreign language requirement vii, viii, 66,194, 216, 227, 228foreign language skill 76, 79, 133, 145foreign language study vi, 84, 99, 111, 127,130, 132, 146, 194, 216foreign language teachers viii, ix, 76, 77, 79,102, 128, 133, 195foreign language teach<strong>in</strong>g ix, 127, 128, 129,131, 145, 213, 219, 221, 223, 224functional magnetic resonance imag<strong>in</strong>g(fMRI) 41, 57, 63, 64, 106, 225, 230genes 6, 7, 34, 35, 36, 37, 63, 215, 216, 222,224gra<strong>in</strong> size v, x, 13, 21, 24, 25, 26, 28, 50, 81,84, 113, 114, 154, 213, 215, 232grammar viii, 32, 69, 81, 87, 111, 133, 135,149, 150, 179, 181, 182, 184, 185, 186, 188,223, 225, 230grapheme complexity 22grapheme-phoneme conversion rules 19,21, 24, 114, 134grapheme-phoneme correspondences 14,21, 24, 25, 48, 136, 231grapheme-phoneme relations xi, 22, 46, 50,90, 134, 154, 165handwrit<strong>in</strong>g 62, 88, 92, 95, 138, 147, 206,208, 209heritability 35, 37<strong>in</strong>clusion education/ma<strong>in</strong>stream<strong>in</strong>g 145,196<strong>in</strong>telligence (IQ) 2, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 32, 63, 66, 67,70, 73, 74, 75, 78, 79, 86, 96, 100, 103, 104,135, 138, 215, 222, 226<strong>in</strong>teractive-compensatory model 17, 229<strong>in</strong>tervention xi, 9, , 31, 32, 37, 39, 49, 54, 56,83, 89, 102, 104, 105, 106, 112, 115, 116,117, 118, 119, 121, 122, 134, 142, 144, 146,165, 179, 189, 193, 194, 195, 218, 219, 220,221, 225, 226, 227, 231language anxiety 75, 99learn<strong>in</strong>g disorders ix, x, 2, 4, 52, 100learn<strong>in</strong>g styles 66, 127letter knowledge 13, 14, 103, 117, 153letter-sound correspondences 13, 116, 125,154, 196letter-sound mapp<strong>in</strong>g 18, 29, 116, 193, 196letter-sounds relations 19, 22lexical/visual strategy 15, 49l<strong>in</strong>guistic cod<strong>in</strong>g differences hypo<strong>the</strong>sis(LCDH) v, 68, 69, 71, 74, 78, 80, 82, 227literacy acquisition x, 23, 37, 50, 86, 95, 98,106, 115, 118, 193, 226literacy difficulties 36, 89, 117, 231literacy skills vii, ix, 37, 62, 97, 128literacy tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g 50, 110logographic stage 19L-type dyslexia 20, 21, 118, 119magnetic resonance imag<strong>in</strong>g 39, 40, 63, 220,225magnocellular v, 34, 41, 52, 56, 57, 58, 59, 60,61, 65, 120, 210, 216, 229magnocellular deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis v, 56, 65mapp<strong>in</strong>gs 16, 18, 19, 22, 23, 24, 25, 26, 28, 29,36, 38, 39, 44, 46, 87, 90, 92, 101, 107, 116,193, 196, 200, 231memory 7, 11, 12, 13, 14, 19, 30, 32, 34, 43,44, 47, 48, 50, 56, 58, 66, 67, 78, 83, 87, 92,95, 96, 103, 104, 105, 107, 113, 114, 123,126, 129, 130, 147, 148, 151, 214, 222metacognitive 10, 124, 127, 133, 150, 195, 225m<strong>in</strong>imal bra<strong>in</strong> damage (MBD) 39, 40mnemonics 149, 150, 151monol<strong>in</strong>gual 39, 106, 108, 109, 113, 116, 118,194, 195morphemes 28, 93, 125, 175, 176, 178, 214morphological awareness vi, 175, 214motivation v, 7, 12, 32, 66, 68, 69, 74, 75, 76,89, 97, 106, 121, 122, 123, 148, 151, 179,182motor skill 61, 62, 87, 92, 95, 105, 228movable device xi, 148, 151, 157, 167, 168,169, 170, 171, 176multisensory approach viii, 124, 130multisensory <strong>in</strong>struction viii, xi, 128, 133,134, 143, 144, 145, 165, 223multisensory methods viii, 124, 125multisensory structured learn<strong>in</strong>g (MSL) v,vi, viii, 84, 122, 123, 124, 125, 126, 127,128, 129, 130, 131, 132, 133, 134, 135, 137,138, 142, 143, 144, 145multisensory techniques 126, 151, 179nam<strong>in</strong>g speed 13, 54, 55, 56, 107native language abilities 73, 74, 76, 78, 80,84, 128, 130, 131, 132native language difficulties 69, 75, 83, 111,216native language learn<strong>in</strong>g vii, 67, 75, 76, 124native language phonological measures 129,132native language skills vii, viii, 68, 69, 74, 75,76, 77, 78, 79, 80, 111, 127, 129, 130, 131,132, 133, 135, 145, 194, 216, 228neuroimag<strong>in</strong>g 38, 39, 64, 65, 112, 116, 217,224


Index 235non-word 26, 27, 29, 43, 47, 48, 49, 85, 87,103non-word repetition 47, 103onset 13, 14, 16, 23, 24, 25, 31, 44, 45, 46, 50,51, 62, 86, 97, 142, 154, 157, 158, 165, 192orthographic awareness v, xi, 36, 165orthographic depth v, x, 15, 21, 22, 23, 25,27, 28, 38, 65, 84, 90, 101, 107, 110, 113,114, 216, 220orthographic neighbourhood 25, 26, 29, 221orthographic process<strong>in</strong>g 26, 70, 111, 194,213, 230orthographic stage 18, 19orthography– deep/opaque orthography 21, 22, 23, 27,28, 30, 50, 82, 91, 112, 134– transparent/shallow orthography 14, 22,23, 24, 25, 26, 29, 30, 49, 50, 56, 82, 83,101, 213, 214, 217Orton-Gill<strong>in</strong>gham (OG) 124, 126, 128, 225,227personality disorders 89, 97, 99, 135phonemic awareness 14, 31, 44, 46, 47, 50,69, 111, 121, 122, 153, 210, 229phonics 44, 121, 124, 153, 154, 217, 230phonological abilities 13, 14, 21, 36, 43, 54,60, 63, 79, 223phonological assessment battery 109, 113, 215phonological awareness vi, 9, 13, 14, 23, 31,43, 44, 45, 46, 47, 48, 52, 63, 67, 69, 80, 81,83, 110, 112, 116, 125, 153, 154, 155, 193,196, 218, 222, 224, 225, 231phonological cod<strong>in</strong>g deficit hypo<strong>the</strong>sis v,43, 47, 48, 51, 84, 113, 218phonological competence 31, 45phonological decod<strong>in</strong>g 15, 16, 36, 52, 82, 113phonological deficit 8, 30, 32, 47, 50, 51, 52,53, 55, 80, 101, 103, 110, 121, 193, 218,221, 230phonological development 36, 44, 49, 85, 87phonological difficulties 47, 51, 64phonological dyslexia 49, 52phonological elements 10, 28, 44, 47, 50phonological impairment 29, 51, 54, 101phonological measures 56, 108, 109, 129, 132phonological neighbourhood 49, 50phonological/orthographic awareness 36phonological/orthographic competence128, 131phonological/orthographic measures 130,132phonological/orthographic process<strong>in</strong>g 70,194phonological/orthographic skills 77, 79, 82,83, 143phonological/orthographic system viii, 136,137, 142, 194phonological process<strong>in</strong>g x, 4, 5, 7, 8, 9, 15,30, 31, 32, 34, 37, 38, 40, 43, 47, 48, 49, 50,51, 52, 53, 54, 55, 56, 60, 61, 63, 64, 65, 68,69, 83, 84, 85, 86, 87, 93, 96, 101, 103, 104,105, 107, 108, 114, 120, 129, 192, 193, 195phonological representations 26, 48, 49, 50,51, 54, 85, 87, 96, 193, 217, 218phonological segments 24, 45phonological skills 13, 30, 32, 50, 51, 56, 58,60, 63, 81, 96, 109, 110, 120, 121, 129, 193,194, 213, 217, 219phonological/sublexical/<strong>in</strong>direct strategy15, 16, 27, 49phonological tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g 47, 115, 116, 117, 193,196, 121phonological units 10, 24, 45, 46, 81, 154planum temporal 40, 41, 64, 106, 114, 218pragmatics 10, 85, 103pre-alphabetic stage 19pre-literacy 13, 14, 37, 50predictors x, 13, 14, 30, 47, 56, 69, 78, 79, 82,111pre-school 15, 35, 86, 89, 211, 219, 222pr<strong>in</strong>t process<strong>in</strong>g 4, 6, 85, 104, 108, 124pseudo-word 13, 16, 26, 48, 85, 96, 103, 107,132, 195psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic gra<strong>in</strong> size <strong>the</strong>ory 24, 25, 28,213, 232P-type dyslexia 20, 21, 118, 119, 210rapid serial nam<strong>in</strong>g (RAN) 13, 47, 54, 55,103read<strong>in</strong>ess for learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and spell 11,12read<strong>in</strong>g ability 17, 27, 29, 31, 32, 43, 46, 53,56, 93, 100, 101, 110, 116, 119, 120, 121,192, 194, 196, 220, 226, 227, 229read<strong>in</strong>g development v, 1, 4, 13, 14, 15, 17,18, 25, 29, 30, 35, 47, 51, 55, 65, 113, 152,192, 193, 194, 213, 214, 217, 224, 225read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties 3, 8, 9, 23, 31, 46, 48, 51,52, 53, 54, 55, 56, 59, 61, 81, 82, 85, 86, 89,90, 97, 100, 101, 104, 108, 118, 122, 213,218, 221read<strong>in</strong>g disorder 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 17, 32, 35, 36,38, 48, 56, 62, 100, 109, 190read<strong>in</strong>g errors 53, 90, 119, 217read<strong>in</strong>g impairment vii, 1, 6, 36, 38, 47, 48,55, 57, 61, 86, 89, 102, 217read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction 13, 15, 25, 26, 39, 44, 47,48, 51, 87, 116, 117, 121, 126, 192, 225, 231read<strong>in</strong>g problem 2, 6, 8, 9, 30, 35, 38, 49, 52,53, 62, 81, 86, 89, 97, 101, 107read<strong>in</strong>g skills 2, 7, 8, 10, 13, 14, 17, 18, 27, 32,46, 47, 51, 52, 58, 83, 86, 96, 98, 107, 108,117, 120, 127, 139, 141, 142, 143, 145, 192,193, 213, 217, 227, 230read<strong>in</strong>g strategy v, 13, 15, 16, 24, 26, 27, 65,90, 101, 113, 118


236 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>read<strong>in</strong>g subskills 14, 67restructur<strong>in</strong>g 22, 49, 50retrieval vii, 47, 48, 87, 93, 95, 96, 103, 147,149, 151rewrit<strong>in</strong>g 88, 91, 103, 151, 175rhyme 12, 21, 34, 45, 46, 87, 119, 156, 157,213, 217right ear advantage (REA) 40, 64, 105, 106,114rime 13, 14, 16, 19, 23, 24, 25, 31, 44, 45, 46,50, 62, 81, 154, 157, 158, 165, 231risk for dyslexia 35, 37, 48, 86, 87, 88, 89,112, 113, 122, 211segment<strong>in</strong>g 14, 19, 24, 36, 44, 45, 48, 49, 50,51, 81, 128, 153, 155, 156, 157, 161self-esteem 97, 99, 102, 106, 112self-perception 74, 75, 76, 97semantics 5, 10, 13, 17, 20, 26, 48, 51, 53, 68,69, 72, 75, 77, 79, 80, 81, 85, 88, 94, 103,113, 130, 175, 231sensorimotor impairment 52, 53, 121, 217,229, 231sequenc<strong>in</strong>g 7, 11, 15, 16, 18, 21, 36, 42, 45,46, 47, 56, 87, 91, 95, 96, 106, 109, 120,125, 126, 149, 153, 154, 193short-term memory (STM) 7, 13, 30, 43, 47,48, 129, 222sound-symbol 66, 67, 70, 78, 127, 128special educational needs (SEN) ix, 4, 145,211speech sounds 9, 40, 44, 48, 60, 64, 68, 103,106, 113, 114, 193, 222spell<strong>in</strong>g ability 1, 14, 27, 85, 88, 103spell<strong>in</strong>g choices 11, 22, 49, 90, 133, 137, 149,165, 207spell<strong>in</strong>g difficulties 3, 8, 34, 51, 62, 63, 89, 92,98, 100, 105, 115, 192, 212, 217, 223, 224,225, 226, 229, 232spell<strong>in</strong>g disorder vii, 1, 2, 3, 143, 219spell<strong>in</strong>g impairment 3, 48spell<strong>in</strong>g mistakes 90, 91, 92, 96, 150, 151,208, 223spell<strong>in</strong>g patterns xi, 19, 29, 134, 137, 151, 154,165, 166, 167, 169, 170, 172, 174, 193, 211spell<strong>in</strong>g problem vii, 100, 225spell<strong>in</strong>g skills viii, xi, 2, 10, 86, 90, 97, 113,116, 121, 123, 128, 133, 134, 139, 140, 141,142, 145, 165spell<strong>in</strong>g rules 136, 138, 144, 213spell<strong>in</strong>g-to-sound 22, 25, 231substitutions 29, 73, 88, 90, 113, 153, 162,163, 164, 226, 228syllable 19, 24, 44, 45, 46, 50, 88, 91, 105, 154,156, 165, 166, 167, 170, 218symptoms v, x, 1, 5, 8, 32, 34, 47, 51, 53, 57,85, 86, 88, 89, 90, 91, 93, 95, 96, 97, 98, 99,100, 101, 103, 105, 112, 191, 192, 211, 224– <strong>in</strong>consistent spell<strong>in</strong>g 91, 134, 207– phonetic spell<strong>in</strong>g 91, 207teach<strong>in</strong>g/<strong>in</strong>structional approach xi, 115,124, 126, 191, 194, 195, 196top-down model/strategy 16, 17trac<strong>in</strong>g 88, 125, 148, 149treatment v, x, 39, 56, 105, 106, 109, 112, 115,116, 117, 118, 119, 120, 121, 134, 137, 138,146, 191, 195, 196, 210, 212, 220, 223, 225verbaliz<strong>in</strong>g 149, 150, 151visual motion sensitivity 57, 58, 60, 230visual-motor co-ord<strong>in</strong>ation 87, 88, 105, 114visual perception 12, 58, 104, 113, 168, 169,217, 226visual process<strong>in</strong>g 17, 30, 53, 112, 151, 215visual system 52, 56, 57, 58, 59, 60, 65word family 152, 172, 177word letter-length 22word recognition 15, 17, 23, 24, 26, 27, 28,31, 36, 64, 81, 83, 96, 107, 108, 116, 127,132, 154, 193, 195, 213, 216


236 <strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Classroom</strong>read<strong>in</strong>g subskills 14, 67restructur<strong>in</strong>g 22, 49, 50retrieval vii, 47, 48, 87, 93, 95, 96, 103, 147,149, 151rewrit<strong>in</strong>g 88, 91, 103, 151, 175rhyme 12, 21, 34, 45, 46, 87, 119, 156, 157,213, 217right ear advantage (REA) 40, 64, 105, 106,114rime 13, 14, 16, 19, 23, 24, 25, 31, 44, 45, 46,50, 62, 81, 154, 157, 158, 165, 231risk for dyslexia 35, 37, 48, 86, 87, 88, 89,112, 113, 122, 211segment<strong>in</strong>g 14, 19, 24, 36, 44, 45, 48, 49, 50,51, 81, 128, 153, 155, 156, 157, 161self-esteem 97, 99, 102, 106, 112self-perception 74, 75, 76, 97semantics 5, 10, 13, 17, 20, 26, 48, 51, 53, 68,69, 72, 75, 77, 79, 80, 81, 85, 88, 94, 103,113, 130, 175, 231sensorimotor impairment 52, 53, 121, 217,229, 231sequenc<strong>in</strong>g 7, 11, 15, 16, 18, 21, 36, 42, 45,46, 47, 56, 87, 91, 95, 96, 106, 109, 120,125, 126, 149, 153, 154, 193short-term memory (STM) 7, 13, 30, 43, 47,48, 129, 222sound-symbol 66, 67, 70, 78, 127, 128special educational needs (SEN) ix, 4, 145,211speech sounds 9, 40, 44, 48, 60, 64, 68, 103,106, 113, 114, 193, 222spell<strong>in</strong>g ability 1, 14, 27, 85, 88, 103spell<strong>in</strong>g choices 11, 22, 49, 90, 133, 137, 149,165, 207spell<strong>in</strong>g difficulties 3, 8, 34, 51, 62, 63, 89, 92,98, 100, 105, 115, 192, 212, 217, 223, 224,225, 226, 229, 232spell<strong>in</strong>g disorder vii, 1, 2, 3, 143, 219spell<strong>in</strong>g impairment 3, 48spell<strong>in</strong>g mistakes 90, 91, 92, 96, 150, 151,208, 223spell<strong>in</strong>g patterns xi, 19, 29, 134, 137, 151, 154,165, 166, 167, 169, 170, 172, 174, 193, 211spell<strong>in</strong>g problem vii, 100, 225spell<strong>in</strong>g skills viii, xi, 2, 10, 86, 90, 97, 113,116, 121, 123, 128, 133, 134, 139, 140, 141,142, 145, 165spell<strong>in</strong>g rules 136, 138, 144, 213spell<strong>in</strong>g-to-sound 22, 25, 231substitutions 29, 73, 88, 90, 113, 153, 162,163, 164, 226, 228syllable 19, 24, 44, 45, 46, 50, 88, 91, 105, 154,156, 165, 166, 167, 170, 218symptoms v, x, 1, 5, 8, 32, 34, 47, 51, 53, 57,85, 86, 88, 89, 90, 91, 93, 95, 96, 97, 98, 99,100, 101, 103, 105, 112, 191, 192, 211, 224– <strong>in</strong>consistent spell<strong>in</strong>g 91, 134, 207– phonetic spell<strong>in</strong>g 91, 207teach<strong>in</strong>g/<strong>in</strong>structional approach xi, 115,124, 126, 191, 194, 195, 196top-down model/strategy 16, 17trac<strong>in</strong>g 88, 125, 148, 149treatment v, x, 39, 56, 105, 106, 109, 112, 115,116, 117, 118, 119, 120, 121, 134, 137, 138,146, 191, 195, 196, 210, 212, 220, 223, 225verbaliz<strong>in</strong>g 149, 150, 151visual motion sensitivity 57, 58, 60, 230visual-motor co-ord<strong>in</strong>ation 87, 88, 105, 114visual perception 12, 58, 104, 113, 168, 169,217, 226visual process<strong>in</strong>g 17, 30, 53, 112, 151, 215visual system 52, 56, 57, 58, 59, 60, 65word family 152, 172, 177word letter-length 22word recognition 15, 17, 23, 24, 26, 27, 28,31, 36, 64, 81, 83, 96, 107, 108, 116, 127,132, 154, 193, 195, 213, 216

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!